“You’ve always got to wash your hands before you eat food” – Complex sentences in English child-directed speech Young children appear to have difficulty in comprehending and producing complex sentences (e.g., temporal, causal or conditional sentences). In experimental settings, children between three and five years misinterpret sentences like “Before the girl jumped the gate, she patted the horse” to mean that the jumping occurred first (e.g., Clark, 1971). They also reverse cause and effect in causal sentences (e.g., Emerson, 1979). Different factors have been suggested to influence children’s performance, such as iconicity (e.g., Clark, 1971) or memory limitations (e.g., Blything, Davies, & Cain, 2015). It has also been claimed that main-subordinate clause orders should be easier to process and to produce in general (Diessel, 2005). One potential factor that has received relatively little attention is the language that children actually hear as they grow up. How often do children encounter these sentences? What are the properties of those sentences? Do certain clause orders occur more often than others? Information about the input is necessary to discern the relative contribution of semantic, syntactic and processing factors to the acquisition process on the one hand and familiarity/frequency effects on the other. I extracted all occurrences of the prepositions/conjunctions after, before, because and if in about 93 hours of child-directed speech (CDS) from two dense (British) English corpora of parent-child interaction (Lieven, Salomo, & Tomasello, 2009), starting at the third birthday and covering six weeks. I found that temporal terms (before, after) were relatively infrequent, and occurred primarily in other constructions (e.g., in phrasal verbs). For all sentences types, there appeared to be clear preferences in clause order. I also found that a large number of complex sentences contained some sort of additional syntactic complexity (e.g., being embedded in other subordinate clauses). Furthermore, subjects were often either pronouns or null forms; definite noun phrases, which often feature in experiments with children, were relatively rare. In my talk, I will present these and other analyses of the complex sentences in our data set and discuss the results in connection with existing findings as well as their implications for experiments that we are currently planning. References: Blything, Liam, Davies, Robert, & Cain, Kate. (2015). Young children’s comprehension of temporal relations in complex sentences: the influence of memory on performance. Child Development 86(6), 1922-1934. Clark, Eve V. (1971). On the acquisition of the meaning of before and after. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 10(3). 266-275. Diessel, Holger. (2005). Competing motivations for the ordering of main and adverbial clauses. Linguistics 43(3). 449-470. Emerson, Harriet F. (1979). Children’s comprehension of because in reversible and nonreversible sentences. Journal of Child Language 6(2). 279-300. Lieven, Elena, Salomo, Dorothé, & Tomasello, Michael. (2009). Two-year-old children's production of multiword utterances: A usage-based analysis. Cognitive Linguistics 20(3). 481-507.