WG LRA 2012 - Sites@UCI - University of California, Irvine

advertisement
Overview
Paper #1 - An efficacy trial of Word Generation: Results from the first year of a
randomized trial
Joshua F Lawrence (University of California, Irvine), E. Juliana Paré-Blagoev (Strategic Education Research
Partnership), Amy Crosson (LRDC, University of Pittsburgh), David Francis (University of Houston),
Catherine E. Snow (Harvard University)
Paper #2 - Patterns of Students’ Vocabulary Improvement from One-time Instruction
and Review Instruction
Wenliang He (University of California, Irvine), Emily Galloway, Claire White, Catherine Snow (Harvard
University), Judy Hsu (University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign)
Paper #3 Engaging Middle School Students in Classroom Discussion of
Controversial Issues
Alex Lin (University of California, Irvine), Joshua F Lawrence (University of California, Irvine), Patrick Hurley
(Strategic Education Research Partnership)
Paper #4 - Heterogeneous Treatment Effects for Redesignated Fluent English Proficient
Middle School Students
Jin Kyoung Hwang (University of California, Irvine), Joshua F Lawrence (University of California, Irvine), Elaine
Mo (University of the Pacific), Patrick Hurley (Strategic Education Research Partnership)
Academic Vocabulary Instruction
Across the Content Areas: Results
from a Randomized Trial of the
Word Generation Program
Word Generation: Weekly
Schedule
Monday
Paragraph
introduces
words
Tuesday-Thursday
Math-ScienceSocial Studies
Friday
Writing with
focus words
Day 1 - Launch
Introduction to
weekly passage,
containing academic
vocabulary, built
around a question
that can support
discussion and
debate,
(comprehension
questions, student
friendly definitions
included)
Day 2 - Science
Thinking
experiments
to promote
discussion
and
scientific
reasoning
Topic: not directly related to stem cell research but
clearly a link could be made
Target Words: investigate, theory, obtain
Background Information: Countries have different
views about citizens carrying guns. In some countries
the import and export of guns is illegal. Subsequently,
no citizen can obtain a gun in those countries (text
continues).
Questions: Are people more aggressive in countries
that allow handguns?
Hypothesis: Citizens of countries that allow
handguns are more aggressive than citizens of
countries that do not.
Materials:
Procedure:
Data:
Conclusion:
What evidence do you have that supports your
conclusion?
Day 3 - Math
Mathematics
problems using
some of the
target words:
a) Students can work in
pairs
b) Whole class
discussion
c) Open-response
(show/explain how
you got your answer)
1. Some people believe that embryonic
stem cell research is important. They think
this because scientists use these cells to
investigate diseases. Scientists try to find
cures for these diseases, and for conditions
like paralysis. Other people believe that
embryonic stem cell research is wrong.
They think this because scientists must
destroy embryos to obtain these cells. In
a recent poll, 40.75% of people said that
the government should not pay for
embryonic stem cell research. Which
decimal is equivalent to 40.75%?
A) 4.075
B) .4075 *
C) .04075
D) .02
Day 4- Social Studies
Developing positions
on the issue set out in
the passage, to help
the class frame the
debate.
Note: these are
optional. The class
may want to develop
its own positions!
Positions:
1.
Scientists should not be allowed
to investigate cures for disease
using stem cells from embryos.
This is trying to “play God”.
2.
Destroying an embryo to get the
stem cells is murder.
3.
The government should pay for
embryonic stem cell research.
This could lead to cures for many
injuries and diseases.
4.
Scientists should be allowed to
do research on embryonic stem
cells, but the government should
not pay for it because many
taxpayers oppose it.
Day 5 - ELA
Writing Activity:
Should the government pay for stem
cell research?
Give evidence to support your position.
Mon –
Introduce
words in
ELA class
Tues –
Math
activity
with
target
words
Wed –
Social
studies
debate
Thurs –
Science
activity
with
target
words
Friday –
Writing
activity
Focus on classroom
discussion
• There are some studies on talk exposure
and peer interactions and vocabulary for
young children
• Studies on discussion of older children have
examined outcomes like reading
comprehension, math, science, and
philosophy content.
• Connections to ELA and SS student
outcomes is strong.
Research Questions
How engaging were classroom discussions in treatment and
control schools? Were there differences in quality across
content areas? Did schools that participated in the Word
Generation program demonstrate improved classroom
discussion?
Did participating in the Word Generation program impact students’
knowledge of the academic words taught? Did participation
effect students’ general vocabulary knowledge?
Did improved classroom discussion mediate the impact of the Word
Generation program on students’ academic vocabulary
knowledge?
District / School
Control Schools
School
Code
District
Grade 6
Contribution
Grade 7
Contribution
Grade 8
Contribution
Total
Contribution
1
3
5
6
11
13
20
35
36
30
31
32
33
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
2
2
2
2
11
24
24
38
17
27
11
21
69
7
6
12
14
281
17
15
19
17
25
43
12
14
74
0
4
0
10
250
0
0
0
0
15
59
0
17
0
0
0
0
0
91
28
39
43
55
57
129
23
52
143
7
10
12
24
622
8
9
10
12
15
16
17
18
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
2
2
2
2
2
2
2
0
39
41
32
0
54
5
43
0
0
18
0
0
0
0
232
29
28
41
33
60
87
0
29
5
0
13
0
45
60
0
430
0
11
26
23
0
115
0
0
0
12
17
15
0
0
51
270
29
78
108
88
60
256
5
72
5
12
48
15
45
60
51
932
Total
Word Generation
Schools
Total
Support for Participation (1 – 3)
the teacher created a well-ordered and respectful
environment that enabled engagement with lesson
content and participation in the discussion
• 1 was reserved for classrooms that were chaotic or
there was student hostility or lack of participation
• 3 points were scored if nearly all students appeared
consistently engaged with minimal side talk and
distractions
Student Engagement (1 – 3)
percent of students participating or attending to
the classroom discussion.
• 1 was awarded if around a quarter of the
students participated in discussion during the
observation period
• 3 was awarded when 50% to 100% of
students participated in discussion
Teacher Talk Moves (1 – 5)
Teachers’ use of open-ended questions and follow-up
questions asking students to explain their thinking or
provide evidence for their ideas.
• 1 was given to classroom discussions in which all
teacher questions had single, known answers (closed
questions).
• 5 reserved for classrooms where the teacher initiated
a range of question types including open-ended
questions and also asked students to provide
evidence or explain their ideas more clearly.
Substantive contributions (1 – 5)
The level of students’ contributions to the discussion
was rated on a five point scale.
• 1 perfunctory answers were given the lowest ratings
• 5 multiple students elaborated ideas and explaining
their thinking while providing evidence. (In these
classrooms students also asked each other to explain
their thinking or explicitly link their own to others’
contributions.)
Composite Scores
Composite Discussion Quality Rating (1 – 4) – Overall
quality score for each class.
Weighted School Level Discussion Quality Ratings (z
score
Control Schools
Word Generation Schools
30
20
10
0
Feb
Mar
Apr
May
Jun
Feb
Mar
Observation Date
Apr
May
Jun
Academic Vocabulary
Knowledge
• 36-item multiple-choice test
General Vocabulary
Knowledge
Participants completed level 6 or level 7/9 of the Gates-MacGinitie
vocabulary assessment (depending on their grade level)..
Covariates
Grade-level proficiency scores
School percent free and reduced lunch
School percent special education
Student grade level
RQ1
How engaging were classroom
discussions in treatment and control
schools? Were there differences in
quality across content areas? Did
schools that participated in the Word
Generation program demonstrate
improved classroom discussion?
Control Schools
Word Generation Schools
30
20
10
0
1
2
3
4
1
2
Composite Discussion Quality Rating
3
4
Content-Area
Observed
Math
(n = 48)
Word
Generation
Schools
Control
Schools
n
Mean
n
Mean
n
1.85
25
2.90
23
2.35
48
+ 1.05
1.08
47
+ 0.43
0.47
54
+ 0.35
0.41
64
+ 0.29
0.35
213
+ 0.52
0.58
1.85
(0.84)
27
(0.85)
Social Studies
(n = 54)
2.24
Total (n = 213 )
2.26
2.28
(0.97)
20
(0.93)
24
(0.83)
ELA (n = 63)
Total
Mean
(0.80)
Science
(n = 47)
Difference Effect
(WG size
Control (Cohen's
Schools)
d)
2.59
(0.90)
30
(0.86)
36
2.55
(0.89)
(0.73)
2.07
113 2.59
(0.86)
(0.85)
2.03
2.43
(0.86)
27
2.39
(0.82)
100
2.31
(0.89)
RQ2
Did participating in the Word Generation
program impact students’ knowledge of
the academic words taught? Did
participation affect students’ general
vocabulary knowledge?
Treatment Effect
βˆ†=
𝛿𝑇 − 𝛿𝐢 =
πœ‡π‘‡,π‘π‘œπ‘ π‘‘ − πœ‡π‘‡,π‘π‘Ÿπ‘’ − πœ‡πΆ,π‘π‘œπ‘ π‘‘ − πœ‡πΆ,π‘π‘Ÿπ‘’
𝜎
Effect Sizes (Student Level)
Outcome Overall Sample
Measure
Mean
Academic
Vocabulary
GatesMacGinitie
Vocabulary
Pre
Post
18.58
19.94
(6.21)
(7.08)
505.26
510.9
(32.46) (35.0)
n
Control
Pre
1540 18.04
Post
18.8
n
Word Generation
δC
Pre
δT
δT δC
Effect
Size
0.16
1400 501.54 508.44 570 6.9 507.5 512.59 830 5.05 -1.85
-0.06
(33.49) (36.37)
20.7
n
922 1.76 1.00
(6.02) (6.83)
618 0.76 18.94
Post
(6.21) (7.14)
(31.6) (33.87)
Basic Model
𝑉𝑂𝐢𝐴𝐡𝑖𝑗
= 𝛽0𝑗 + 𝛽1𝑗 𝐢𝐸𝑁𝑇𝐸𝑅𝐸𝐷_𝑉𝑂𝐢𝑖𝑗 + 𝐺𝑅𝐴𝐷𝐸𝑖𝑗 + πœ€π‘–π‘—
𝛽0𝑗 = 𝛾00 + 𝛾01 𝑀𝐸𝐴𝑁_𝑉𝑂𝐢𝑗 + TREAT𝑗 + COVARIATE𝑗 + 𝑒𝑗
Academic
Vocabulary
Model B
With co variates
Academic
Vocabulary
0.837***
0.840***
(0.097)
(0.145)
0.697***
0.697***
(0.024)
1.264*
(0.500)
3.397
(1.757)
(0.024)
1.551**
(0.501)
4.581
(4.342)
28.29***
28.24***
Model A
Outcome
Academic
Vocabulary Teaching
Team Mean
Academic
Vocabulary Teaching
Team Mean
Centered
Treatment
Intercept
Level 2 Variance
(Teaching Team)
(0.517)
Residual
1.775
(0.313)
N
1554
-2LL
9654.227
Standard errors in parentheses
*
p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
(0.516)
1.555
(0.283)
1554
9647.524
HLM
Predicting
Student
Knowledge
of
Academic
Words
Outcome
Gates MacGinitie
Vocabulary Teaching
Team Mean
Gates MacGinitie
Vocabulary Teaching
Team Mean Centered
Treatment
Intercept
Level 2 Variance
(Teaching Team)
Residual
N
-2LL
Model C
Model D
With Covariates
Gates MacGinitie
Vocabulary
Gates MacGinitie
Vocabulary
0.691***
0.580***
(0.070)
(0.086)
0.798***
0.798***
(0.022)
0.0151
(2.323)
160.9***
(34.920)
(0.022)
-0.373
(2.167)
235.2***
(43.480)
530.7***
530.7***
(10.180)
40.88***
(6.785)
1416
12956.545
(10.180)
27.25***
(5.360)
1416
12944.513
Standard errors in parentheses
HLM
Predicting
Student
knowledge
of Gates
Vocab
RQ3
Did improved classroom discussion
mediate the impact of the Word
Generation program on students’
academic vocabulary knowledge?
c prime path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.700***
(0.024)
1.633**
(0.530)
a path
Composite
Discussion
Quality Rating
0.138***
(0.031)
0.396*
(0.167)
Intercept
5.812***
(0.567)
-2.789***
(0.567)
Level 2 Variance (Teaching
Team)
2.108*
1.775
(0.375)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
(0.334)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
Outcome
Academic Vocabulary (Pre)
Treatment
Composite Discussion Score
Residual
N
1446
b path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.696***
(0.024)
1.271*
(0.560)
0.533
(0.376)
6.090***
(0.586)
c prime path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.700***
(0.024)
1.633**
(0.530)
a path
Composite
Discussion
Quality Rating
0.138***
(0.031)
0.396*
(0.167)
Intercept
5.812***
(0.567)
-2.789***
(0.567)
Level 2 Variance (Teaching
Team)
2.108*
1.775
(0.375)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
(0.334)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
Outcome
Academic Vocabulary (Pre)
Treatment
Composite Discussion Score
Residual
N
1446
b path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.696***
(0.024)
1.271*
(0.560)
0.533
(0.376)
6.090***
(0.586)
c prime path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.700***
(0.024)
1.633**
(0.530)
a path
Composite
Discussion
Quality Rating
0.138***
(0.031)
0.396*
(0.167)
Intercept
5.812***
(0.567)
-2.789***
(0.567)
Level 2 Variance (Teaching
Team)
2.108*
1.775
(0.375)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
(0.334)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
Outcome
Academic Vocabulary (Pre)
Treatment
Composite Discussion Score
Residual
N
1446
b path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.696***
(0.024)
1.271*
(0.560)
0.533
(0.376)
6.090***
(0.586)
c prime path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.700***
(0.024)
1.633**
(0.530)
a path
Composite
Discussion
Quality Rating
0.138***
(0.031)
0.396*
(0.167)
Intercept
5.812***
(0.567)
-2.789***
(0.567)
Level 2 Variance (Teaching
Team)
2.108*
1.775
(0.375)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
(0.334)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
Outcome
Academic Vocabulary (Pre)
Treatment
Composite Discussion Score
Residual
N
1446
b path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.696***
(0.024)
1.271*
(0.560)
0.533
(0.376)
6.090***
(0.586)
c prime path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.700***
(0.024)
1.633**
(0.530)
a path
Composite
Discussion
Quality Rating
0.138***
(0.031)
0.396*
(0.167)
Intercept
5.812***
(0.567)
-2.789***
(0.567)
Level 2 Variance (Teaching
Team)
2.108*
1.775
(0.375)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
(0.334)
28.38***
(0.538)
1446
Outcome
Academic Vocabulary (Pre)
Treatment
Composite Discussion Score
Residual
N
1446
b path
Academic
Vocabulary
(Post)
0.696***
(0.024)
1.271*
(0.560)
0.533
(0.376)
6.090***
(0.586)
Multilevel Mediation Model
π‘«π’Šπ’”π’„π’–π’”π’”π’Šπ’π’π’‹
𝑻𝑹𝑬𝑨𝑻𝒋
Multilevel Mediation Model
π‘«π’Šπ’”π’„π’–π’”π’”π’Šπ’π’π’‹
𝑻𝑹𝑬𝑨𝑻𝒋
1.633**
𝑽𝑢π‘ͺπ‘¨π‘©π’Šπ’‹
Multilevel Mediation Model
π‘«π’Šπ’”π’„π’–π’”π’”π’Šπ’π’π’‹
𝑻𝑹𝑬𝑨𝑻𝒋
1.633**
𝑽𝑢π‘ͺπ‘¨π‘©π’Šπ’‹
Multilevel Mediation Model
π‘«π’Šπ’”π’„π’–π’”π’”π’Šπ’π’π’‹
𝑻𝑹𝑬𝑨𝑻𝒋
1.633**
𝑽𝑢π‘ͺπ‘¨π‘©π’Šπ’‹
indirect effect = .396*.533 = .211
total effect = indirect effect + direct effect = 0.211 + 1.28 =
1.48
Bootstrapping
Coefficient Standard Error
z
p value
95% Confidence
Interval
Indirect Effect
0.21
0.10
2.22
0.03
0.02
0.40
Direct Effect
1.28
0.31
4.06
> 0.001
0.66
1.90
Total Effect
1.49
0.30
4.99
> 0.001
0.90
2.07
command: ml_mediation, dv(WGV_TCTR_W2) iv(TREAT)
mv(wrub_tot_SM_Y1) cv(WGV_TCTR) l2id(unit)
Conclusions
Changes in curricular materials can
improve discussion.
A free, low “implementation cost” program
can have credible effects.
The proportion of total effect of Word
Generation mediated through improved
discussion is 0.14.
Limitation and questions
How can we further improve discussion?
Does improved discussion transfer?
What are the other effective pathways for
student learning from WG?
Patterns of Students’ Vocabulary
Improvement from One-time
Instruction and Review Instruction
Wenliang He, Joshua Lawrence (University of California, Irvine)
Emily Galloway, Claire White, Catherine Snow (Harvard University)
Judy Hsu (University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign)
Background
 Vocabulary knowledge is positively correlated with reading skills.
 Prior research has focused on studies of factors that impact the learning
of academic vocabulary (e.g. frequency, exposure, prior knowledge)
and effective vocabulary intervention techniques (e.g. multimodal
enhancement, active processing, mnemonics).
 Regardless of the importance and the prevalence of vocabulary
instruction in schools, few classroom-based intervention studies have
been conducted on learning the effect of review (second-time)
instruction.
Research Questions
 What factors predict students’ improvement in vocabulary knowledge
from first-time instruction in the WG program?
 Can review (second-time instruction) significantly contribute to
additional improvement in vocabulary knowledge to first-time
instruction?
Research Design
Pre-test
Review
Post-test
Group A
Group B
151 students
159 students
Right before program started
5 words/week
5 words/week
for 7 weeks
for 7 weeks
7 Yellow Words
7 Red Words
5 words/week
5 words/week
for 4 weeks
for 4 weeks
By the end of the 12th week
Research Design
Overview of Yellow, Red and Core Instructional Words
Count
Word
List
Yellow Words
Red Words
7
adequate
(1)
relevant
(2)
capacity
(3)
Contrast
(4)
assess
(5)
paralyzed
(6)
emphasize
(7)
7
formulate
(8)
retain
(9)
distribute
(10)
logical
(11)
disproportionate
(12)
obtain
(13)
restrict
(14)
Core Instruction Words
recite
(15)
cohesion
(20)
constrain
(25)
amended
(30)
generate
(35)
incentives
(40)
enable
(45)
34
invasion
conclusion
components
(16)
(18)
(17)
equity
documented interaction
(22)
(23)
(21)
eligible
complex
contaminated
(26)
(27)
(28)
apathy
Concept
maintained
(32)
(31)
(33)
enforced
perceived
perspective
(37)
(38)
(36)
amnesty
acquired
prescribed
(43)
(41)
(42)
excluded
aptitude
assumed
(46)
(47)
(48)
critical
(19)
altered
(24)
distinct
(29)
Attributes
(34)
conserve
(39)
proceeded
(44)
Analysis & Results (Q1)
 What factors predict students’ improvement in vocabulary knowledge
from first-time instruction in the WG program?
Correlations between Measures from One-time Instruction Data
1. Percent Correct in Pretest
2. Percent Correct in Posttest
3. Word Frequency
4. Week of Instruction
5. Improvement
1
2
3
4
1
0.90***
1
0.34*
0.30*
1
-0.02
-0.04
0.09
1
-0.23
0.2
-0.09
-0.03
(p=0.11) (p=0.17) (p=0.53) (p=0.86)
5
1
Note. * p < .05. *** p < .001. (two-tailed t-tests)
Percent Correct is the percentage of students who are correct in a multiple choice question, testing on a
specific word in either pre or post test.
Analysis & Results (Q1)
Percent Correct
at Pretest (%)
0 ≦ P+ ≦ 100
Below Average
Above Average
Detailed Breakdown
10-20
20-30
30-40
40-50
50-60
60-70
70-80
80-90
Word
Count
48
26
22
Mean Percent
Improvement (%)
10.59
11.06
10.04
SD
(%)
8.56
10.61
5.43
Min
(%)
-4.77
-4.77
0.59
Max
(%)
46.00
46.00
20.16
4
5
7
11
9
4
5
3
18.37
11.60
7.11
11.49
12.40
8.63
8.83
3.46
19.35
13.43
3.14
8.17
3.92
6.55
3.74
2.57
0.89
-4.77
3.22
-0.16
5.01
1.10
5.76
0.59
46.00
30.85
12.56
22.65
17.21
16.29
14.70
5.56
50
Analysis & Results (Q1)
40
41 amnesty
10
20
30
contrast
0
20
18
21
capacities
restricted 27
34
32
disproportionately
46 relevant
40
adequate
35
28
assess
25
43
45
3848
26
2433
paralyzed
23
17
42
30
37
47
emphasized
15
19 36
retain
29
16
distribute
logical
4439
22
formulated
obtain
31
10
20
.
30
40
50
60
70
Percentage of Students Correct at Pretest
Yellow Words
Red Words
80
90
Fitted values
Analysis & Results (Q1)
Improvement for words with above average difficulties
20
β = -0.61, p < 0.01
capacities
eligible
attributes
maintained
excluded
relevant
15
prescribed
assumed
perspective
10
conclusion
enable
distribute
contaminated
acquired
5
perceived
logical
proceeded
conserve
retain
obtain
0
50
60
documented
formulated
70
80
Percentage of Students Correct at Pretest
Fitted values
90
Analysis & Results (Q1)
Improvement for words with below average difficulties
50
amnesty
β = -0.20, p = 0.32
40
contrast
30
components
equity
20
incentives
adequate
disproportionately
constrain
10
interaction
emphasized
cohesion
0
restricted
generate
complex
assess
altered
concept
paralyzed
invasion
amended
aptitude
recite
critical enforced
distinct
apathy
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
Percentage of Students Correct at Pretest
Fitted values
45
50
Analysis & Results (Q2)
 Can review significantly contribute to additional improvement in
vocabulary knowledge to first-time instruction?
35
Instruction
30
25
Review
Instruction
20
Review
15
10
5
0
Red Words
Yellow Words
Group A
Group B
Analysis & Results (Q2)
Overview of Review Impacts by Word Groups
Word Groups Word Count Review Impact (%) S.E.(%) p (two-tailed)
All Words
14
2.58
1.71
0.16
Hard Words
7
-1.36
2.32
0.58
Easy Words
7
6.51
1.47
0.004
Words below the 48% cut-off were recoded as Hard Words, and those above were recoded as Easy Words.
Conclusion
 The effects of one-time instruction and review instruction depend
on students’ prior word knowledge, reflected by the percentage of
student correct at pretest.
 For moderately difficult words, i.e. percent correct values greater
than 48% in this study, improvement from one-time instruction is
stronger for harder words than for easier words. However, if words
are exceedingly difficulty, none of the measure in this study can
effectively predict improvement from one-time instruction.
Conclusion
 Review instruction is most effective (p = 0.004) when students are
reasonably familiar with the words, i.e. percent correct values
greater than 48%. In this case, review contributes an extra 6.51%
improvement compared with the 10.04% improvement from firsttime instruction.
 Learning overly unfamiliar words twice, however, does not have
any statistically significant effect over one-time instruction (p =
0.58).
Implications
 This study indicates that
(a) effects of vocabulary instruction is most predictable with
Goldilocks words, as students do not benefit as much with either
overly simple or exceedingly hard words;
(b) prior knowledge is crucial in explicit vocabulary instruction
(c) teaching difficult words may necessitate more effective
instruction techniques.
Thank You
Engaging Middle School
Students in Classroom
Discussion of Controversial
Issues
Alex Lin
Dr. Joshua Lawrence
Civic engagement
Declining voting turnout
Declining rate of civic knowledge
Importance of stimulating news media use and political
discussion at home
Discussion of Controversial
Issues
One way of improving civic knowledge is using controversial issues as
topics for classroom discussion
McDevitt and Chaffee (2000) found that enrollment in Kids Vote was
positively related to students’ habit of initiating political discussion at
home
Hess and Posselt (2002) found that enrollment in the Public Issues was
related to students making 70% more contributions in classroom
discussion.
What about middle school students?
Program Features
In the Word Generation program:
Students are exposed to daily discussions of controversial topics
Different topic each week
Students take a position on an issue
Teacher facilitates classroom debate
Breakdown of Controversial Topics (Year 1 and Year 2) in
Two Years of WG Program
Grade 6
Grade 7
Grade 8
YEAR 1 TAUGHT
-
Should colleges use Affirmative Action?
Should the government fund stem cell research?
Should Creation be taught in school?
Should English be the official language of the United States?
Is nuclear power a danger to society?
(Fall 2010 – Spring 2011)
YEAR 2 TAUGHT
-
Should there be more strict dress codes at schools?
Is the death penalty justified?
Does rap music have a negative impact on students?
What should be done about global warming?
Should the government allow animal testing?
NON-TAUGHT TOPICS
-
Should schools protect students from cyberbullying?
Should schools be a place for debates?
Should secret wiretapping be legal?
Should schools have a vocational track?
(Fall 2011 – Spring 2012)
Research Questions:
(1) In the First Year Sample (6th Graders only)
How do students enrolled in Word Generation compare with control students,
in terms of their confidence to participate in a discussion of controversial
topics?
(2) In the Second Year Sample (7th and 8th Graders)
How do students enrolled in Word Generation compare with control students,
in terms of their confidence to participate in a discussion of controversial
topics?
Methods
Surveys administered in May 2012 (2nd year of the study)
Students were assessed the question:
How confident are you in being able to participate in a discussion
about the following topics?
-
14 topic questions from the year 1, year 2 and non-taught topics
-
Students responded about their discussion confidence level from (1)
“not at all” to (5) “extremely”
Methods: Participants
Grade Level Contributions by Schools
Word Generation
Control
TOTAL
6
7
8
TOTAL
1,163
1,167
1,121
3,430
617
482
694
1,793
1,780
1,649
1,815
5,223
Analysis Plan
For each sample (6th) and (7th + 8th) graders:
Compare discussion confidence levels on the 14 controversial topics between
students enrolled in the WG program and control groups
Compare discussion confidence levels between students enrolled in the WG
program with control groups on TAUGHT and NON-TAUGHT topics
Confidence to Discuss Comparison by Schools (6th Grade Sample)
Results
4.00
4.12 4.10
3.88
3.67
3.50
Confidence to
Participate in
Discussion
3.31 3.26
3.15 3.19
3.00
3.27
2.95
2.80
2.50
2.62
2.56
2.53 2.56
2.40
2.28
2.00
1.99
2.86 2.84
2.73 2.68 2.76 2.69
2.40 2.35
2.36
2.06
1.50
TAUGHT
Control School
WG School
NON-TAUGHT
Control School
WG School
Results
1st Year Students: Comparison of Students’ Self-Reports on
Discussion Confidence Level Between Schools
Confidence to Participate in Discussion
3.00
0.01
t(1857)= -0.41, ns
2.94
0.13 ***
2.95
t(1850)= -3.41, p < 0.001
2.75
2.72
2.50
2.59
2.25
2.00
CONTROL
WORD
CONTROL
WORD
GENERATION
GENERATION
Taught Effect size: (sd=.82) = 0.16
Non-taught
TAUGHT
Control School
WG School
NON-TAUGHT
Control School
WG School
Confidence to Participate in Discussion Comparison (7th & 8th
Graders)
Results
4.00
3.82 3.82
3.66 3.69
3.50
Confidence to
Participate in
Discussion
3.00
3.13
2.98
2.64
2.50
2.72
2.69 2.71
2.47
2.36
2.00
2.21
2.46
2.30
3.01
3.11
3.03
3.09
3.18
2.79
2.76
2.68
2.55
2.33 2.37
2.03
1.50
TAUGHT
Control School
WG School
NON-TAUGHT
Control School
WG School
2.70
Results
2nd Year Students: Comparison of Students’ Self-Reports on Discussion
Confidence Level Between Schools
Confidence to Participate in Discussion
3.00
0.08 ***
0.02
t(3615)= -3.47, p < 0.001
t(1200)= -10.93, ns
2.84
2.75
2.88
2.90
CONTROL
WORD GENERATION
2.76
2.50
2.25
2.00
CONTROL
WORD GENERATION
Taught
Effect size: (sd=0.70) 0.11
TAUGHT
Control School
WG School
Non-taught
NON-TAUGHT
Control School
WG School
Results
How do students enrolled in Word Generation compare with control
students, in terms of their confidence to participate in classroom
discussion of controversial topics?
In both (6th grade) and (7th + 8th grade) samples, students in the
Word Generation program report more confidence in being able to
have a classroom discussion of controversial topics that are
covered in class
Discussion
Consistent with past studies that curricular exposure to
controversial topics can improve students’ confidence to
participate in discussion
Students’ abilities to participate effectively in discussion of
controversial issues can improve as a result of being enrolled in a
course that places primacy on such discussions (Hess & Posselt, 2002)
Young people draw more attention to news information when that
information is perceived to be useful for school assignments and peer
conversations (Atkin, 1981; Kanihan & Chaffee, 1996; McDevitt and Chaffee
(2000)
.
Future Work
- Examine students’ perception of openness in classroom
discussion
- Freedom to disagree in class
- Teacher’s skill in facilitating discussion
- Compare students’ perception of openness in
classroom discussion between WG and CO schools
- Student level
- Classroom level
- Examine students’ habit of extending discussion of
controversial issues outside of home and their news
media use (newspaper and Internet)
.
Works Cited
Atkin, C. (1972). Anticipated communication and mass media information
seeking. Public Opinion Quarterly, 36, 188-199.
Hess, D., & Posselt, J. (2002). How high school students experience and learn
from the discussion of controversial public issues. Journal of
Curriculum and Supervision, 17(4), 283-314.
Kanihan, S., & Chaffee, S. H. (1996). Situational influence of political
involvement on information seeking: A field experiment. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Education in
Journalism and Mass Communication, Anaheim, CA
McDevitt, M., & Chaffee, S. (2000). Closing gaps in political communication and
knowledge effects of a school intervention. Communication Research,
27(3), 259-292.
Appendix
1ST Year Students: Comparison of Students'
Discussion Ability Between Schools (6th Graders)
Non-taught
2.95
-0.23 ***
Taught
2.72
t(1200)= -10.93, p < 0.001
Non-taught
2.94
-0.35 ***
t(650)= -12.22, p < 0.001
Taught
2.59
2.00
2.25
2.50
2.75
Confidence to Participate in Classroom Discussion
3.00
Results
2ND Year Students: Comparison of Students'
Discussion Ability Between Schools (7th & 8th Graders)
Non-taught
2.90
-0.06 ***
t(2319)= -15.20 p < 0.001
Taught
2.84
Non-taught
2.88
-0.12 ***
t(1283)= -15.20, p < 0.001
Taught
2.76
2.00
2.25
2.50
2.75
Confidence to Participate in Classroom Discussion
3.00
Heterogeneous Effects of
Word Generation on
Students with Differing Levels
of English Proficiency
Jin Kyoung Hwang, Joshua Lawrence
(University of California, Irvine)
Elaine Mo
(University of the Pacific)
Patrick Hurley
(Strategic Education Research Partnership)
Language Minority (LM) Learners
• School-aged students in the US who
hear and/or speak a language other
than English at home (August & Shanahan, 2006)
• Large population
• Over 11 million students, about 5 million
are classified as limited English proficient
(Aud et al., 2012)
• Largest growth
• One of the fastest growing groups among the
school-aged population in the U.S. (Aud et al., 2011)
Language Minority (LM) Learners
• Differing Levels of English Proficiency
• Initially fluent English proficient (IFEP)
• Students with full English proficiency by the time they
enter school
• Limited English proficient (LEP)
• Students who still need English language support
• Redesignated fluent English proficient (RFEP)
• Students who used to be LEP but attained sufficient
English proficiency to be reclassified
Vocabulary Intervention & LM Learners
•
Quasi-experiments of WG (Lawrence, Capotosto, Branum-Martin, White, &
Snow, 2012; Snow, Lawrence, & White, 2009)
•
•
•
Vocabulary Improvement Program (Carlo et al., 2004)
•
•
No language by treatment interaction
Quality English and Science Teaching (August et al., 2009)
•
•
ELLs in treatment schools improved more than EO
students on polysemy task
Improving Comprehension Online (Proctor et al., 2009/2011)
•
•
LM students with full English proficiency benefitted more
LM students with limited English proficiency did not
No language by treatment interaction
Language Workshop (Townsend & Collins, 2009)
•
All participants were Spanish-English speakers
Research Questions
• Is there a heterogeneous effect of
Word Generation for EO, IFEP, and
RFEP students?
• Is there a heterogeneous effect of
Word Generation for RFEP students
according to the number of years they
have been redesignated?
Methods: Participants
•
•
•
13 middle schools: 7 treatment & 6 control
N = 6,193 students
Language Status
•
•
•
•
•
Initially Fluent English Proficient (IFEP, n = 632)
Redesignated Fluent English Proficient (RFEP, n = 2,515)
Limited English Proficient (LEP, n = 1,011)
English only (EO, n = 2,035)
Years since Redesignation
•
•
•
•
0-1 year: n = 419
1-2 years: n = 754
2-3 years: n = 419
> 3 years: n = 661
Methods: Measures
• Dependent Variable:
• Academic Vocabulary
•
Word Generation Vocabulary Posttest (50 items total; 40 items were taught throughout
the program)
• Independent Variables:
•
•
•
•
Intervention
Language Proficiency Groups (EO, IFEP, RFEP; RQ1)
Years since Redesignation (for RFEP students only; RQ2)
Academic Vocabulary
•
Word Generation Vocabulary Pretest (School mean & School centered mean)
• Reading Comprehension
•
Gates-MacGinitie Comprehension Test (School mean & School centered mean)
• Grade Level
• School of Attendance
• Gifted and Talented & Special Education
Methods: Analysis Plan
RQ 1: Heterogeneous Effect of WG for
EO, IFEP, and RFEP students?
RQ1: Heterogeneous Effect of WG
for EO, IFEP, and RFEP students?
Academic Vocabulary:
WG Posttest
29
28.5
28
27.5
27
26.5
Treatment
Control
26
25.5
25
24.5
24
EO
IFEP
RFEP
Language Proficiency Groups
RQ2: Heterogeneous Effects of WG
within RFEP students?
Academic Vocabulary:
WG Posttest
RQ2: RFEP Students with Different Years
since Redesignation
30
29
28
27
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
Treatment
Control
0-1 years
1-2 years
2-3 years
>3 years
Years since Redesignation
Conclusion
• Heterogeneous effects of Word Generation
•
Replicated and extended findings from previous
Word Generation studies
• RFEP students are a heterogenous group
References
Aud, S., Hussar, W., Kena, G., Bianco, K., Frohlich, L., Kemp, J., Tahan, K. (2011).
The Condition of Education 2011 (NCES 2011-033). U.S. Department of Education,
National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Aud, S., Hussar, W., Johnson, F., Kana, G., Roth, E., Manning, E., . . . Zhang, J.
(2012). The Condition of Education 2012 (NCES 2012-045). U.S. Department of
Education, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners:
Report of the National Literacy Panel on language minority chidren and youth.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
August, D., Branum-Martin, L., Cardenas-Hagan, E., & Francis, D.J. (2009). The
impact of an instructional intervention on the science and language learning of
middle grade English language learners. Journal of Research on Educational
Effectiveness, 2(4), 345-376.
References
Carlo, M.S., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C.E., Dressler, C., Lippman, D.N., . . . White,
C.E. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English language
learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(2),
188-215.
Kieffer, M.J. (2008). Catching up or falling behind? Initial English proficiency, concentrated
poverty, and the reading growth of language minority learners in the United States.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 851-868. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.4.851
Lawrence, J., Capotosto, L., Branum-Martin, L., White, C., & Snow, C. (2012). Language
proficiency, home-language status, and English vocabulary development: A longitudinal
follow-up of the Word Generation program. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University.
Proctor, C.P., Dalton, B., Uccelli, P., Biancarosa, G., Mo, E., Snow, C., & Neugebauer, S.
(2011). Improving comprehension online: Effects of deep vocabulary instruction with
bilingual and monolingual fifth graders. Reading and Writing, 24(5), 517-544.
Townsend, D., & Collins, P. (2009). Academic vocabulary and middle school English learners:
An intervention study. Reading and Writing, 22(9), 993-1019.
Download