LAB 7 (CHAPTER 10): OBJECT PERCEPTION (graded out of 50, worth 5% of final grade) This lab consists of a two parts, which may be done in the lab or at home. Neither part requires the assistance of a partner. Equipment: No special equipment is needed for this lab, just a copy of this hand-out, and a pencil. Handing In: Course website only, no other submission methods accepted. Failure to follow this instruction properly will lead to a 10% mark deduction. General Background: In class we discussed how early visual processing ultimately leads to our ability to perceive and interact with objects. Here we will explore two facets of this topic. One is multistable figures. These were some of the earliest and greatest challenges to Wundt's structuralist approach to object perception. The second concept we will explore is modern structural theories. In particular, we will examine Biederman's "Recognition By Components" model of object recognition, a highly influential and contentious theory about how the brain models objects in the world. Prior to doing this exercise, you should read Chapter 10, esp. section C2. Part 1: Qualitative Observation of a Multistable Figure One way that researchers study perceptual phenomena is that they simply observe them. In the first part of this lab we will practice that method, sometimes referred to a qualitative observation (a.k.a., phenomenology, naturalistic observation). Initial Observations: As a first step, take a look at the figure on the preceding page. Just look at it for a minute or two and record what you see (use objective, scientific language and include technical terms you’ve learned): Having seen this optical illusion before in a previous psychology class, I knew it was a cube with certain lines missing, making the whole picture act as somewhat of an optical illusion. After looking at the shape for a long time, I realized that it was white background composed of lots of black circles with white lines, which when moved together, gave an impression of a partially apparent cube shape. The principle of closure can also be applied to this patterns as when the black circles were moved closer together they made a cube shape, however, when imagined far apart from each other, they just looked like an abstract black geometric pattern of circles on a white background. The law of simplicity can also be present here as one would immediately try to organize the circles in order to find the most concrete or apparent shape, in this case, probably being a 3D cube. The law of fate can also be applied here since we see the circles as moving in uniform parallel/diagonal lines, instead of being mere circles placed against a white background. The Gestalt pattern can also be applied to this shape as, as the cube does not actually exist in the actual image, but it is the human mind attempting to piece apart the illusion and create a common shape that one has perceived before in past experiences, in this case, one would probably try to fill in the spaces between the circles in order to create a 3D cube like shape. I also used the law of proximity while I was analyzing the shape as when I looked at the closely packed circles, I could see a cube like figure. However, when I looked at one circle in the top right corner, the image immediately shifted perspectives as the circles appeared to spread further apart from each other. The law of similarity can also be applied to this analysis as we see the cube figure due to the similar circles sharing common features in shape, colour, and brightness; each circle having a corresponding pair or matching circle in the image. Some Questions to Consider: Now that you’ve had some time to examine the figure in an open-ended way, consider the following: The figure is 2D, but people typically report seeing a cube, which is a 3D structure. In fact, there are at least 4 versions of a cube that people commonly report. That is, people typically see the cube either floating in front of eight black circles, or being seen behind a white wall with eight holes. For either of these interpretations, one can see the cube as going up and to the right or down and to the left, like so: This is what is called a “multistable” figure, in that one’s perceptions can stabilize on one of several interpretations. This is despite the fact that the “sensations” on the retina remain the same. Another interesting thing about the figure is that the lines that connect the corners of the cubes aren’t actually there. They’re only “implied” by the corners, yet people still perceive them. For some, the lines merely “seem to be there”, while others report actually seeing faint illusory lines. Now that you’ve read this over, take another look at the figure on page 1 before answering the questions below. Further Observations 1. How many different three-dimensional interpretations of the figure did you see? Although the figure-ground segregation phenomena in the Gestalt theory states that based on observation, a foreground impression or a strongly apparent pattern tends to stand out and is highly distinguishable from any background stimulation, making many usually see one significant image at first, in this case it would probably be a cube shape made out of white lines. When analyzing the picture from a few angles/perspectives, I saw that there were three different interpretations that really caught my eye. The first time I looked at the figure, I tried to logically make a common shape out of the circles by piecing the invisible lines together to create a normal cube with 12 edges and 8 points. The second time that I attempted to look at the figure, I tried to look outside the supposed cube’s lines and found four pairs of black circles with white lines crossing through them. The third time that I looked at the figure with my head at an angle, I saw that the circles created a 2D hexagonal polygon on top of a series of black circles. 2. If you saw multiple interpretations, did your perception of them shift back and forth on its own? Could you make it shift? Were there any tricks you used to make it shift? I felt that in order to make the figure appear in a certain manner instead of an unclear optical illusion (which is what I saw when I hazily or unconsciously gazed upon the figure), I had to consciously follow certain contours and different angles in order to make the shape shift. In order to see the cube shape I tried to bring my eyes closer towards the center of the image, which simultaneously made the dots unconsciously move closer in my mind, while I tried to differentiate the black shade/circles from the white contrast, in order to make out the shape of a cube. I could make my perception shift back and forth by trying to look for specific shapes in the pattern instead of a cube shape, by paying attention to the outside of the cube shape which immediately made me switch mindsets and see 4 pairs of circles with different patterns of lines in them. I could easily turn the supposed cube shape into a 2D hexagon composed of many different polygons by looking at the picture with a tilted head, which at a specific angle, immediately erased the perception of a 3D cube, leaving me with a whole load of trapezoids, squares and triangles, instead of black circles that are partially cut with white lines in order to make out a distinguishable 3D cube shape. 3. The lines that connect the corners of the cube aren’t actually drawn, but one perceives them. How strong was your impression of the lines? Did you feel like you could actually “see” them, or was there merely a “sense that they were there”? Did the strength of the perception vary depending on whether you saw the cube floating out front vs. behind? Initially, my impression of the lines was actually quite strong as I used the gestalt theory to fill in the void spaces, which would most probably be represented by intersecting the line shapes within the black circles in order to make a 3D cube shape. Relating to the Gestaltian phenomena of holisticism (or the need to create a “whole” instead of separate parts), I initially looked at the shape in order to find a pattern that was rather prominent, in this case, trying to form a geometric pattern out of the lines in the black circles, in my perspective, the most prominent figure was a cube shape. Relating to the Gestaltian theory of figure- ground segregation, the cube was nothing but a subjective impression that tends to stick out from background stimulation when we try and look for a particular shape (in this case we are looking for a common pattern, i.e. a cube, and perhaps our consciousness can make us focus on finding a distinct shape instead of making us solely see a meaningless cluster of patterned circles). I felt as if I could initially sense that the corners/edges of a cube were actually there as I tend to look at things in a “holistic manner” or as a “whole” figure. The strength of the perception actually did vary as it seemed stronger when I viewed at the front of the shape (as it is presented at somewhat of a diagonal angle), than when I looked at the back of the shape. 4. Please note here any other observation(s) you might have had about the stimulus: One other observation that I had about the stimulus, is that the shape tends to shift with the intensity of one’s visual focus. As I opened my eyes wider I began to see a widespread image of 8 circles in a geometric pattern (4 dots sandwiched in between 2 rows of two dots), and when I focused my eyes very carefully by squinting/moving them closer towards the center, I immediately pieced together the outline of a cube shape in between the circles. 5. Did your observations change after you read the background material above? How? Why? My observations did change somewhat after I read that these lines were not actually drawn in, but it is truly our brain and various cortical processing theories that subjectively affect the way we perceive objects. Initially, I thought that the cube shape was drawn in behind some circles in order to test different/subjective views of vision as well as to strategically test if one is vigilant enough to notice the cube figure or not. After reading that this figure is a perception test, I began to approach the figure from different Gestaltian perspectives (e.g. testing the law of similarity as well as the principle of subjective contours which states that one can subjectively see edges and distinct shapes where there is no physical luminance, colour or texture difference), and eventually came to understand that vision and optical images are based off of one’s personal perception instead of generic uniform thoughts about visual phenomena. Instructions for Lab 4, Part 2: In class we discussed Biederman’s RBC structural description model of object recognition. In this part of the lab we will look at those models using a naturalistic observation approach. Data Gathering Pick two objects arbitrarily from your environment. These can be anything, natural or artificial, big or small, animate or not. 1. Sketch out the geon structural description of the two objects below. That is, sketch out the object as a set of geons such as cubes, cones, spheres, bent cylinders, etc (see page 346 of the text). Don’t worry, this is not an art class, but do try to keep things tidy. Label each of the objects and each of its geons. Bent cylindrical figure Thin rectangular prism shape Cylinder Rectangular Prism Coffee Mug Cash Box Bent cylindrical figure All of the separate Geons 2. How easy or hard was it to break the two objects down into geons? Did this differ between the two objects? Why? It was rather easy to break both my cash box and my coffee mug down into their primitive geons or basic shapes. As the vessel of the mug is nevertheless a normal stout cylinder with a semicircular/bent cylindrical handle, I immediately broke it down into its basic two geons or geometric shapes. However, for my cash box, it was slightly harder as the box contained more certain 3D shapes on one of its faces (represented by the cylindrical handle and the prominent rectangle or the lock that is right under the handle). Previous experience seeing and using a lot of coffee mugs also aided with my perception and processing speed, as I only occasionally see and use cash boxes, compared to the amount of time I spend using and seeing mugs in everyday life. 3. Do you think the two objects could be differentiated based on their geon structural description? Could they be recognized from the geons alone? Why or why not? Since the cash box is rather similar to a normal rectangular prism shaped box with a lid on it, and a distinct line (representing the two halves of the box) that runs around it, one can probably distinguish that it is a container used for holding something. In most cases as people would observe the geonic prism shape alongside its cylindrical handle on top of a thin rectangular prism (the common shape of a keepsake/money box closure/lock) it can be easily implied that it is a cash box, and can somewhat be easily recognized by its distinct geons. However, when one sees the mug with its distinct geometric/cylindrical components, they can easily label and differentiate it from another item, signifying that it can be recognized solely by its geons. 4. Based on their geons alone, would the two objects both be recognizable from various angles? The coffee mug is rather recognizable from the majority of angles since it is a rather generic shape, composed of a short rotund cylinder connected by a thinner curved cylindrical shape for a handle. View invariance, or the angle and altitude that the object is looked at, can also have a profound effect on one’s recognition process/object differentiation time. From a horizontal distance at eye level, if the round handle of the coffee mug is not showing alongside the cylinder, one may have trouble differentiating it from a similar object (e.g. a cylindrical coffee canister). One can notice that the coffee mug is a hollow cylindrical shape with a bent cylinder on its side for a handle, heavily signifying (based on geonic properties/personal opinions due to previous experience), that this is infact a coffee/drinking mug. However, the cash box can easily be pieced apart into geonic shapes and recognized to be a container at different angles (e.g. when viewed at an elevated height and from a diagonal perspective). The easiest angle or perspective of view invariance used to clearly piece apart the all of the box’s geonic shapes would be when looking at it from the front as one can notice the specific geonic shapes of the cylindrical handle and the rectangular lock base, signifying that it is most likely a container for money/keepsakes. 5. Based on your observations here, and your personal experience with object recognition, what are your thoughts about Biederman’s geon structural description model? I actually agree that Biederman’s geon model is frequently used when trying to differentiate objects from one another and to piece apart the exact shape of certain objects (e.g. a briefcase which is usually composed of two geons, a semicircular cylinder which is connected to a prism shaped box). Whenever I approach a new object, I try to identify it in a top down process, which is similar to Biederman’s theory, as I take the whole object and break it down into smaller shapes, in order to aid with the recognition process. View invariance is also another key factor in my mental processes, as I view and try and identify the whole object from different angles (e.g., looking at the cup from the front where its handle cannot be seen as it is usually on one side of the mug, then turning it around to see if there is a handle or cylindrical figure on the side when looking from a different angle, I also to see if the cylinder is hollow [marking if it is a mug or not] by looking at it from an elevated angle), before I make up my mind and come to a conclusion what something really is. In this lab, I found Biederman’s model to be quite effective in recognizing/recalling what my chosen objects were, even though I possess them and have used them multiple times in the past. Breaking things down into smaller components, (in this case 3D shapes), may seem like a rather slow and unnecessary process; however, it initially helps clarify and recall past memories about similar items used and their specific purposes, making one have a higher chance of identifying the item correctly.