HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 1 PSYCHOLOGY 3050: Infant Perception and Cognition Ch 4 Dr. Jamie Drover SN-3094, 864-8383 e-mail – jrdrover@mun.ca Winter Semester, 2015 Basic Perceptual Abilities of Young Infants • It was once thought that infants were born deaf and blind and with limited pain sensitivity. • Although far from mature, all of the infant’s senses are functioning. – They even prefer some sights, smell, and sounds over others. • Infants are also sensitive to pain – ELBW infants’ response to pain is affected by repeated painful episodes. Basic Perceptual Abilities of Young Infants • Vision and hearing develop rapidly over the first year. • Tactile stimulation is very important to infants. – Pre-term infants who receive massage gain more weight, spend more time awake, and display more advanced cognitive and motor skills than do normal treated preterm babies (Schanber & Field, 1987). Basic Perceptual Abilities of Young Infants • The chemical senses (taste and smell) develop early. • Six-day olds prefer the scent of their mother’s breast pads over those of a stranger (Macfarlane, 1975). Methodologies of Infant Perception • To assess infant perception, we must observe a behavior that an infant can control and use that to infer perception. Sucking Response • Researchers often use infants’ sucking rates. • DeCasper and Spence (1986) had pregnant women read one of three passages aloud twice per day. Methodologies of Infant Perception • Shortly after birth, headphones were placed on the infants. • One of several passages could be played over the headphones based on sucking rates. • Infants would alter their sucking rates in order to hear the reading of familiar passages. Methodologies of Infant Perception Visual Preference Paradigm • Fantz (1958, 1961) placed babies in a looking chamber and presented them several visual stimuli. • If they spend more time gazing at one pattern more than another, it is assumed they can discriminate between them. Habituation/Dishabituation • Habituation: the decrease in response to a stimulus as a result of repeated presentations of that stimulus. • Infants can habituate to a visual stimulus. • The longer infants are exposed to a stimulus, the less time they will spend looking at it. • Habituation occurs when there is a substantial decrease in looking time following repeated presentation. Habituation/Dishabituation • Often defined as when fixation to the stimulus is 50% of what it was initially. • If a new stimulus is then presented, the infant may show a sudden increase in looking time. – This is dishabituation. • Thus, the infant can discriminate between the two stimuli. • Also indicates that infants can remember the earlier stimulus. Habituation/Dishabituation Habituation/Dishabituation Habituation/Dishabituation • Using this paradigm, Friedman (1972) found evidence that 1- to 3-day-old infants will habituate and dishabituate to visual stimuli. • Newborns are capable of visual memory. The Development of Visual Perception • Infants can perceive light (pupillary reflex) but because of poor accommodation, much of what they see is blurry. – Accommodation is adult-like at 3 months. • Newborns can track a moving object, but the eyes do not always move in harmony. • Convergence and coordination are adult-like by 6 months. Testing Infants’ Visual Acuity • Visual acuity of newborns is 20/600 to 20/400. • Can be tested using forcedchoice preferential looking. • Infants are presented with rectangular cards that contain black and white stripes on one side of a central peephole, while the other side is blank. – Teller Acuity Cards Testing Infants’ Visual Acuity • Given an infant’s preference for patterned stimuli over unpatterned stimuli, if he/she can detect the stimulus, he/she will fixate it. • A naïve observer must determine the location of the stripes based on the fixation of the child. Testing Infants’ Visual Acuity • The thinnest stripewidth at which the observer can determine the location of the stripes provides a measure of visual acuity. The Development of Visual Perception • Vision is poor at birth because the fovea is underdeveloped. – Point on the retina where vision is sharpest. Packed with cones. • The infant fovea contains large, less densely packed cones. • Newborns can discriminate between red and white, but can not differentiate blue, green, and yellow from white (Adams et al.1994). The Development of Visual Preferences • Until 2 months of age, an infant’s visual preferences are affected by physical properties of the stimulus. • Babies prefer moving stimuli over stationary stimuli. – See Haith (1966) p. 193. • Infants prefer high contrast stimuli over low contrast stimuli. – See Salapatek and Kessen (1966) p. 193. The Development of Visual Preferences The Development of Visual Preferences • Infants at 1 month of age focus their attention primarily on the outside of a figure. • Externality effect. The Development of Visual Preferences • At 4 months, infants start to show a preference for vertical symmetry. • They prefer to process stimuli that are vertical and symmetrical as opposed to asymmetrical and horizontal stimuli. • Curvature, or curvilinearity is a also important to infants. – Fantz demonstrated that infants sometimes prefer cuved stimuli over linear stimuli. The Development of Visual Preferences • Infants as young as 3 to 4 months of age prefer the curvilinear and concentric stimuli (Ruff & Birch, 1974). • Even newborns prefer curvature. Psychological Stimulus Characteristics • At around 2 to 4 months, psychological characteristics of a stimulus become important to infants. – Eg., familiarity and novelty • Kagan (1971) proposed that at 2 months, infants form schemas. – Sensory representations of a stimulus. – The similarity of a stimulus to a previously determined stimulus will determine attention. Psychological Stimulus Characteristics • Infants are most attentive to stimuli that are moderately discrepant from a schema. – Discrepancy principle. – They are less attentive to stimuli that are highly familiar, or highly discrepant. • McCall et al. (1977) familiarized 24 month-old infants to stimuli and later showed them stimuli that varied in their similarity with the original. Psychological Stimulus Characteristics • Stimuli that were highly similar, or highly discrepant to the original received less attention than those that were moderately similar or moderately discrepant. • There are instances when infants prefer similar, not novel, stimuli. Psychological Stimulus Characteristics • Generally, young infants prefer familiar stimuli, then show no preference, then they prefer novel stimuli. • Takes time to create schemas. The Development of Face Processing • Infants prefer vertical, symmetrical stimuli with curved lines making them well-suited to attend to faces. • A bias to human faces would make evolutionary sense and would facilitate attachment. • Johnson et al. (1991) showed newborns paddle stimuli which resembled faces, did not resemble faces, or were blank. The Development of Face Processing • They presented these stimuli and moved them. • Measured how much infants followed the stimuli by moving their eyes and head. • Following this paradigm, infants will show a preference for face-like stimuli as early as 5 days of age. The Development of Face Processing • There is evidence that newborns may be able to make discriminations between faces. – Look longer at photos of their mothers than at photos of other women (Bushnell et al., 1989) . – They will alter sucking rate to see a photo of their mother over another woman (Walton et al., 1992). • Babies also show a preference for attractive faces over unattractive faces (Langlois, 1987). – Infants as young as two months will look longer at attractive faces as opposed to unattractive faces. The Development of Face Processing • This might be because infants prefer upright, curvilinear, symmetrical stimuli. – It might be evolutionary since symmetry is a sign of health. – Important for mate selection. Auditory Development • Hearing develops substantially in the first year, but is not adultlike until about 10 years of age. • Auditory perception is well-developed in the newborns, particularly at high frequencies. • DeCasper and Fifer (1980) found that 1- to 3-day-old infants will alter their sucking rates to hear a tape recording of their mothers as opposed to that of a stranger. Auditory Development • DeCasper’s and Spence’s (1986) earlier research on infants and sucking rate shows that they’re capable of auditory learning prenatally. • Studies measuring heart rate in response to familiar and novel passages during the third trimester reveals similar findings. Speech Perception • Infants can perceive most and perhaps all phonemes found in all human languages. – Phonemes: the basic units of speech. • Eimas et al. (1971) repeatedly presented 1-montholds with a phoneme along the ba/pa continuum until they showed a decrease in sucking rate (i.e., habituation). Speech Perception • Infants would show an increase in sucking rate (dishabituate) if a phoneme was presented on the other side of the ba/pa continuum. • They use the same dividing line as older children and adults. Speech Perception • Infants can make phoneme discriminations that adults can not make. • They can make discriminations in foreign languages that adults can not make. • However, this ability is quickly lost. • At the same time, they are able to make increasingly fine discriminations between phonemes in their mother tongue (Kuhl et al., 2006). Speech Perception • This language flexibility is probably not adaptive after a certain age. – The brain should dedicate neurons to processing sounds in the language its exposed to. • Infants are able to recognize frequently heard sound patterns at least by 4.5 months of age. Violation-of-Expectation Method • An infant’s reaction to an unexpected event is used to infer what he/she knows. • Uses infants’ looking behavior along with preference-fornovelty and habituation/dishabituation procedures. • If what they see differs from what they expect, they should look longer at this event. Core Knowledge • Infants possess core knowledge. • They are born with a small set of distinct systems of knowledge that have been shaped by natural selection and upon which new skills and belief systems are built. • According to Spelke, there are three core knowledge systems in infancy. – Object representation, knowledge of people and actions, ability to represent numbers or quantities. Object Representation • What infants know about the nature of objects. • Object Constancy: Knowledge that an object remains the same despite changes in how it is viewed. – As an object gets closer, its retinal image gets larger – We perceive it as getting “closer” rather than “larger” • Newborns possess object constancy (Slater, Mattock, & Brown, 1990). • Infants were habituated to an object of a particular size. Object Constancy • Then shown one of two objects. – Same object as seen at a different distance – New object of a different size (yet retinal image is the same). – Infants remain habituated to the first object Object Continuity and Cohesion • Individual objects are seen as cohesive wholes with distinct boundaries. • Kellman and Spelke (1983) tested this with 4 montholds. – Habituated to a stick moving back and forth underneath a bar – Tested with one stick or two stick displays Condition 1 Habituation + 2 Object Test Condition 1 Habituation + 2 Object Test Condition 1 Habituation + 2 Object Test Condition 2 Habituation + 1 Object Test Condition 1 Habituation + 2 Object Test Recover (dishabituate): unexpected Condition 2 Habituation + 1 Object Test Don’t recover (dishabituate): expected Object Continuity and Cohesion • 4-mo dishabituate to condition 1 but not condition 2 – But only if the object was moving – Not if stationary • 2- and 4-month-olds infer object unity in some situations, but not in others. • Newborns don’t appear to be born with continuation. Objects Continuity and Cohesion • Baillargeon et al. (1995) investigated whether infants understood the notion of support. Objects Continuity and Cohesion • 3-month-old infants weren’t surprised with the impossible outcome. • 6.5-month-olds expect the box to fall unless a large portion maintains contact with the platform. Objects Continuity and Cohesion • Initially, infants believe that any contact between two objects is enough for one to support the other. • They progress until they reach an adult-like concept of support. • However, 2-year-old children will fail on a similar task (see Hood et al., 2000; p. 215). Objects Continuity and Cohesion • Baillargeon proposed that infants possess the principle of persistence. • Objects exist continuously, remain cohesive, and retain their individual properties. • Yet this notion is underdeveloped at first, and later becomes enriched with experience. Objects Continuity and Cohesion Familiarize: (a) (b) (c) Hood et al. (2000) study with 2- and 2.5-yr-olds Test event: Where is The ball? Objects are Continuous, Solid, and Require Support • Perhaps the difference in the nature of the tasks explain these findings. • That is, 2-year-olds need to demonstrate an explicit understanding of spatial relations, whereas infants need only display an implicit understanding. Object Permanence • The understanding that objects continue to exist when they can not be perceived. • According to Piaget, until 4 months of age objects exist merely as extensions of their perceptions or actions upon them. • At 4 months, they will retrieve an object that is partly hidden. – Will not search for a completely hidden toy. Object Permanence • At 8 months, infants will retrieve a completely hidden toy. • Yet, they can not solve the A not B task. • At 12 months, they can solve A not B, but cannot do invisible displacement. – Object is hidden in one container and then in another container out of the vision of the observer. – Need to mentally represent objects. A New Look at Object Permanence • Infants as young as 3.5 months possess more knowledge than Piaget proposed. – Some believe object permanence is innate. • Baillargeon (1987) studied this using the violation-ofexpectation method (p 140). • 3.5- and 4.5-month-old infants were habituated to a screen moving 180° in space. • A block was then placed behind the screen to produce an impossible event. A New Look at Object Permanence A New Look at Object Permanence • No differences were found for the control condition. • In the experimental condition, infants looked longer at the impossible event. • They believed the block continued to exist when out of their sight and were surprised when the screen dropped. • They have knowledge of object permanence and that one solid can’t pass through another. A New Look at Object Permanence • Newcombe et al. (1999) buried a toy in a sandbox in front of 5-month-olds, and then dug it out 10 sec later. • After multiple trials, the object was dug out from 6 inches away. • Infants looked significantly longer at these trick trials. • They weren’t surprised when a different object was dug out of this location. A New Look at Object Permanence • Even when using the traditional A not B task, infants as young as 7.5 months will search correctly on B trials. – The delay must be short. – Thus, inhibition is likely involved. Early Number Concepts • Numerosity: the ability to determine the number of items in a set without counting. • Ordinality: a basic understanding of more than or less than. • Infants as young as 10 months can distinguish between two boxes containing different amounts of crackers. Early Number Concepts • Infants as young as 6 months of age can tell the difference between arrays, but the ratio must be relatively large. Simple Arithmetic • Wynn has used the violation-ofexpectations method to show that infants can add and subtract small quantities. Simple Arithmetic Simple Arithmetic • But, it’s not clear exactly what’s going on here. • Maybe infants are not responding to number, but the total amount of substance present. Arguments Against Core Knowledge • Bogartz et al (1998) argue that it is not necessary to use innate knowledge of objects to explain infants’ looking behavior. • Infants acquire object knowledge through perceptual experience. Arguments Against Core Knowledge • This perceptual processing produces the looking patterns others interpret as innate knowledge. • Perceptual processing takes time. • Novel events (impossible events) simply take longer to process because initial encoding of the stimuli needs to be done. • This takes time and results in longer looking times. Category Representation • Babies show preferences indicating that they have some form of visual memory. • This implies that they form categories. • Items from similar categories are treated as being the same. Measuring Categorization • Often measured using habituation/dishabituation. • Stimuli are varied slightly during habituation trials. • Eg., when habituating infants to faces, multiple faces can be used. – Infants will still show decrease in looking times. • Cohen and Strauss (1979) did this with 30-week-olds. – Infants can form a human face category. – Human face is distinct from other perceptual categories. Measuring Categorization • Infants as young as 3 months can form categories (see Eimas and Quinn, pp 147-8). • But do these categories exist when infants enter the lab? • Or can infants merely distinguish the physical features necessary to discover the categories in the experimenters’ minds? – It’s probably a little of both. The Structure of Infants Categories • Category Prototype: An abstract representation of a category. A best example of a category. • Infants appear to form category prototypes – See Roberts and Horowitz, p 149. – Infants will form a category for the concept of birds when habituated to highly typical exemplars. – The same is not the case when atypical exemplars are used.