Prosocial Behavior Social Psychology Chapter 10 November 12, 2004 Class #11 Types of Prosocial Behavior • Prosocial behavior (broadest term) – Action intended to benefit another – Can be done to gain either external or internal reward • Benevolence (slightly narrower term) – Action intended to benefit another, but not to gain external reward) • Pure altruism – Action intended to solely benefit another – No external reward to the helper – No internal reward to the helper • Some argue there is no such thing as pure altruism Defining Prosocial Behavior Type of Behavior Prosocial Behavior Benevolence Pure Altruism Definition Example Any action intended to benefit another (regardless of motive) Giving a large tip to a waiter to impress your boss with your generosity Type of Behavior Prosocial Behavior Benevolence Pure Altruism Definition Example Benefits another intentionally for no external reward Sending $20 to a charity to make yourself feel good inside Defining Prosocial Behavior Type of Behavior Prosocial Behavior Benevolence Pure Altruism Definition Benefits another intentionally for no external or internal reward Example Jumping on a railroad track to help a stranger who has fallen Helping requires investment of time, energy, attention, funds…so why do we do it? • Goals of Prosocial Behavior – Gaining Genetic and Material Benefits • Inclusive Fitness • Reciprocal Aid – Gaining Social Status and Approval – Managing Self-Image – Managing Our Moods and Emotions Evolutionary Factors in Helping: The “Selfish Gene” • What is important is survival of the individual’s genes, not survival of the fittest individual • Kinship selection is the tendency to help genetic relatives – Strongest when biological stakes are particularly high Insights into the Evolution of Help • Inclusive Fitness – The ability of one’s genes to survive in one’s own offspring AND in any relatives one helps – Helping a close relative promotes the survival of those genes Cunningham et al. (1995) 80 60 Percentage Volunteering to Help 40 20 0 High Mod. Low None (parents, siblings, children) (grandparents) (first cousins) (attractive strangers) Degree of Relatedness Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama (1994) • There are three people who need you to run a small errand to the store: – A cousin – A sister – An acquaintance • You have time to help only one… – Whose errand do you run? I made this one up… • If your house is burning down and there are several people asleep • You only have time to save one person – who of the following would you save? – Your Uncle Charlie who owes you $200 – Your step-mother who has raised you since you were two and you love very much – Your adopted son who you have raised since he was 6 months old and you love very much – Your biological son who you haven’t spoken to since you kicked out of the house for smoking pot Who do you help? • Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama (1994) – Participants in this study were asked to imagine scenarios like the following: • There are three people asleep in different rooms of a burning house: – Your 7 year-old female cousin – Your 75 year-old grandfather – A 21 year-old acquaintance • You have time to rescue only one… – Who do you save? Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama (1994) 3.0 For everyday help, people tended to help close relatives more than non-relatives 2.5 Tendency to Help 2.0 1.5 1.0 High Mod. Low (parents, siblings, children) (grandparents) (first cousins) None (acquaintances) Degree of Relatedness Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama (1994) 3.0 The difference became even more pronounced in life-or-death situations 2.5 Tendency to Help 2.0 1.5 1.0 High Mod. Low (parents, siblings, children) (grandparents) (first cousins) None (acquaintances) Degree of Relatedness Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama (1994): Findings • Kin are helped more than non-kin, especially in life-ordeath situations • Females are helped more than males, except elderly females (post- menopausal) • Young are helped more than old • Healthy relatives helped more than non-healthy in lifeor-death situations • In life-or-death helping, relatedness matters (this assures that our genes will continue) • In everyday helping, we may be guided more by social norms and moral rules (e.g., "help the sick") • Scratch my back and I’ll scratch yours… • Reciprocal Aid – Trivers (1971) • The theory of reciprocal altruism predicts that altruistic behaviors will also be a function of beliefs about the recipient's likelihood of reciprocating… • Help that occurs in return for prior help • Animals will help non-relatives if they live in close proximity and can better survive by sharing Evolutionary Factors in Helping: The Cooperative Group • Humans can sometimes increase their reproductive success in two ways: – By protecting their own self-interest in relation to other individuals – By protecting their group’s interests in relation to other groups • Helping based on social connections rather than genetic relationships Rewards of Helping: Helping Others to Help Oneself • More likely to help when the potential rewards of helping seem high relative to the potential costs. • Arousal: Cost-Reward Model – What are the costs and rewards associated with helping? Rewards of Helping: Helping to Feel Good • More likely to help: – If self-esteem has been threatened by failure – Feeling guilty about something • Relationship between helping and feeling better. • Helping others to feel good is often not a conscious decision, but it can be Rewards of Helping: Helping to Be or Appear Good • Social Responsibility Norm – Societal rule that people should help those who need their assistance – May help because motivated to behave in ways that are consistent with moral principles. – Sometimes help for the appearance of morality but really have selfish motives. • Moral hypocrisy • Overhelping Costs of Helping or of Not Helping • Helping has its costs as well as its rewards. • Helping can also be more sustained and deliberate. – Courageous resistance • Helping can have negative health effects if involves constant and exhausting demands. Altruism or Egoism: The Great Debate • Is helping motivated by altruistic or egoistic concerns? – Altruistic: Motivated by the desire to increase another’s welfare – Egoistic: Motivated by the desire to increase one’s own welfare • Batson: The motivation behind some helpful actions is truly altruistic The Empathy-Altruism Hypothesis From C.D. Batson, The Altruism Question, 1991. Reprinted with permission of Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. C. D. Batson, The Altruism Question. Copyright © 1991 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Reprinted by permission. Telling the Difference Between Egoistic and Altruistic Motives • How easy it to escape from a helping situation? – If egoistic motive, helping should decline when escape from the situation is easy – If altruistic motive, help is given regardless of ease of escape Batson et al. (1981) • Students in one experiment were given the opportunity to help a suffering student (Elaine) by taking her place in an electric shock experiment – Half could easily escape the whole situation by leaving immediately – The other half would have to stay and watch as she received the remaining shocks Batson et al. (1981) • Students given the easy opportunity to escape usually took it… • But if they felt high empathy for Elaine they were more likely to help her out • Dan Batson and colleagues argue that empathy engages pure altruism, and overrides selfish motivations Egoistic Alternatives • Empathy encourages helping because of concern about the costs to the self of not helping • Empathy highlights the potential rewards for helping others – Negative state relief model • Helper experiences empathic joy by helping another person Altruism vs. Egoism: Limits and Convergence • Strong evidence for the empathy-altruism hypothesis • Limitations to empathy-altruism hypothesis: – Not all helping is altruistically motivated – Motives do not guarantee behavior – Is the assumption that there is a clear divide between the self and the other a valid one? An egoistic alternative? • Cialdini and his colleagues argue that there is an egoistic explanation of these findings: – Empathy causes an observer to feel kinship with the victim, thus tapping into a basic selfish motivation – to serve myself by serving those who share my genes “I didn’t want to get involved…” • Kitty Genovese Tragedy (March 13, 1964) – We watched video clips on this last week… – Kew Gardens apartment in Queens, NYC where Kitty lived Winston Moseley , mugshot She might still be alive today… Bystander Effect • The tragic story of Kitty Genovese. – Why did no one help? – Latané & Darley: • Were social psychological processes at work? Diffusion of Responsibility • “Bystander Effect” – Tendency for each group member to dilute personal responsibility for acting by spreading it among all other group members Taking Responsibility • Moriarty (1975) – Jones Beach study - confederate steals a radio • DV: % who intervene to stop theft • IV1: question asked – “Would you watch my things?” or “ Do you have a light?” • IV2: sex – Results: • A percentage of subjects who intervened to stop a thief at Jones Beach, New York, varied according to whether the subjects felt they had a responsibility to stop the thief • Asking subjects to ''watch my things" proved very effective, although certainly not by reducing fear of the thief Latané & Darley (1970) • Helping is the last step of a process involving multiple decisions: – Helping Decision Tree • 1. Notice the incident • 2. Interpret incident as emergency • 3. Assume responsibility • 4. Know the appropriate response • 5. Implement decision to help Bystander Helping on Computer From P.M. Markey, Computers in Human Behavior, 16 (2002) 183-188. Time Costs… • Darley and Batson (1973) – 40 students from Princeton Theological Seminary • Researchers asked half of the subjects to prepare themselves for a brief talk on the parable of the Good Samaritan and half were told that they would be asked questions concerning employment prospects for seminary students IV’s • Some subjects were put in the “early” condition… – “It will be a few minutes before they are ready for you but why don’t you head over there anyway…” • Some subjects were put in the “on time” condition… – “They’re just about ready for you…if you leave now you’ll be right on time” • Some subjects were put in the “late” condition… – “They were expecting you a few minutes ago, better hurry…” Personality Measure was also included… • Before all this was done, subjects were given a questionnaire intended to measure their basis for interest in religion… • They were asked: Did their religious interest primarily have to do with: – assuring their personal salvation or – their concern for helping others Is there time enough to be a Good Samaritan? • On their way to the next building they passed a poorly dressed man slumped in a doorway, head down, eyes closed, not moving… • As the subject moved past him, the man coughed twice and began groaning Pitting Dispositions Against Situations • Darley and Batson (1973) compared the strength of a seemingly important dispositional factor (primarily altruistic or not) with a relatively small situational variable (early or late)… Darley & Batson’s (1973) Results 70 60 50 Percentage 40 who helped 30 20 10 0 Ahead of Schedule On Time Late Pluralistic Ignorance • Phenomenon that occurs when bystanders to an emergency, trying to look poised, give misleading cues to others that no help is needed Bystanders as sources of information about helping • Results suggest that people look to others to provide information… – If no one else seems upset, that suggests this isn’t an emergency Latane & Darley (1968): Smoke study • In this study, researchers pumped smoke into a lab while students filled out a questionnaire… – Some students were left alone – Some with 2 other real participants – Some with 2 other confederates who pretended nothing was wrong Latane & Darley (1968) 80 60 Percentage Reporting Smoke 40 20 0 Alone With 2 other real subjects With 2 calm confederates Informational influence? • Being around others made people less likely to interpret smoke as an emergency Location and Culture • Do individuals have a worse chance of being helped in an emergency in a big city than in a small town? • Greater population density is associated with less helping. Evans & Lepore, 1993 Milgram, 1970 • Helping is less likely in densely packed cities… • Part of the problem is that people living in dense urban environments pay less attention to those around them Helping in the U.S.A. From "Helpfulness Index: How U.S. Cities Rank," The Boston Globe, July 7, 1994. Managing Our Moods and Emotions Dovidio, Piliavin, Gaertner, Schroeder, & Clark (1991) • Arousal/Cost-reward model – View that observers of suffering help to relieve their own personal distress – We will help in emergency if: • We feel negative arousal, and other conditions suggest that helping will alleviate the unpleasant feeling Cialdini et al. (1982) Schaller & Cialdini (1990) • Mood management hypothesis – Idea that people use helping tactically to manage their moods – Throughout life, we learn that helping others can lead to rewards – This reward makes us feel good, and we learn to use helping to manage our mood Scents and Sensibilities From R.A. Baron, "The Sweet Smell of ...Helping: Effects of Pleasant Ambient Fragrance on Prosocial Behavior in Shopping Malls," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 23, pp. 498503. Copyright (c) 1997 by Sage Publications, Inc. Reprinted by permission of Sage Publications, Inc. Good Moods Lead to Helping: Reasons • Why feeling good leads to doing good: – – – – Desire to maintain one’s good mood Positive expectations about helping Positive thoughts Positive thoughts and expectations about social activities Good Moods Lead to Helping: Limitations • Why feeling good might not lead to doing good: – Costs of helping are high – Positive thoughts about other social activities that conflict with helping Bad Moods and Helping • When negative moods make us more likely to help others: – If we take responsibility for what caused our bad mood (i.e., feel guilty) – If we focus on other people – If we are made to think about our personal values that promote helping Cunningham et al. (1980) 80 70 60 50 Percentage who helped 40 next person 30 20 10 0 Believed Broke Camera No Broken Camera Best to wait until after confession… • Harris, Benson, & Hall (1975) – Higher percentage of Catholic subjects donated money to a charity after confession – Guilt seems to lead to altruism Bad Moods and Helping • When negative moods make us less likely to help others: – If we blame others for our bad mood – If we become very self-focused – If we are made to think about our personal values that do not promote helping Effects of Those Around Us • Observing another giving help can teach young children about the helping norm • For adults, a model can remind them of the helping norm Awareness of the Helping Norm • Froming, Allen, & Jensen (1985) – Very young children are not aware of the norm requiring people to help those in need – Between 6 and 9 years of age, they become aware of the norm – At this age, they will help if an adult is present Essential Qualities for an Altruistic Personality • Empathy • Internalized and advanced moral reasoning • Attractiveness – Physical Appearance • I think we’ve talked enough about that effect already – Charisma • Penner and Fritzsche (1993) – Example: Earvin Johnson Magic’s Influence on Helping Others Penner & Fritzsche, 1993. Attributions of Responsibility • Beliefs about the needy person’s responsibility influences helping • Effect particularly strong among those who believe in a just world Does it matter if he’s drunk? • Piliavin (1981): The New York Subway Experiment – Teams of 4 traveling on NY subways (Victim/model/2 observers) – Recorded number of people in car, gender, race, etc. – Victim: always male, either white or AfricanAmerican/sometimes with a cane, sometimes drunk – Victim collapses/model instructed to help after exactly 70 seconds • Results??? Similarity and Familiarity • Dovidio (1984) – In 34 studies, 29 found significantly higher helping for similar over dissimilar others • Gaertner & Dovidio (1986) – However, people will often help people of other races but only if not helping might make them appear prejudiced Similarity and Familiarity • Familiarity may also be a cue to genetic relatedness – Our ancestors encountered their relatives on a daily basis – In animal and human societies, familiarity increases helping • Schroeder, Penner, Dovidio, & Piliavin, 1995 – If familiarity is associated with shared genes, helping familiar others would have generally helped relatives Gender and Helping • Classic male-helper scenario: “Knight in shining armor” • Classic female-helper scenario: “Social support” • Gender differences in willingness to seek help Gender and Help • Williams & Best (1990) – Women are universally perceived as kinder, more soft-hearted, and more helpful – But over 90% of Carnegie Hero awards go to men (for saving, or attempting to save, the life of another) • Why? Threat-to-Self-Esteem Model • Help is experienced as self-supportive when recipient feels appreciated and cared for • Help is experienced as self-threatening when recipient feels inferior and overly dependent When Is Receiving Help Perceived as Threatening? • Those with high self-esteem tend to react more negatively than those with low self-esteem • Being helped by a similar other may imply that recipient is inferior • Help from a significant other on an ego-relevant task can threaten one’s self-esteem Shotland & Straw (1976) • Sometimes people assume help would be seen as an unwelcome intrusion… – When a woman fighting with a man shouted: “I don’t even know you!” - help was more likely than if she shouted: “I don’t know why I married you!”