IN TIME PERSPECTIVE, INDIVIDUALS’ DIFFERENT FLOW EXPERIENCE, ENJOYMENT, AND THE PERCEPTION OF QUALITY OF LIFE IN LEISURE TIME PHYSICAL ACTIVITY Hyoung-Kil (H.K.) Kang Recreation Leisure Services Administration Southern Wesleyan University 214 Folger, 907 Wesleyan Drive, Central, SC 29630-1020 PH: (864) 644-5292 E-mail: hkkang@swu.edu Keith C. Russell Department of Physical Education, Health and Recreation Western Washington University Carver 102-MS 9067 Bellingham, WA 98225-9067 PH: (360) 650-3529 E-mail: Keith.Russell@wwu.edu 1 Introduction Time Perspective (TP) is defined as “the totality of the individual’s views of his psychological future and psychological past existing at a given time” (Lewin, 1951, p. 75). TP has been considered an important psychological trait that influences attitudes and behaviors (Anderson & Golden, 1989; Boyd & Zimbardo, 1997; Cotte & Ratneshwar, 2001; Denton, 1994; Zimbardo, 2002; Zimbardo, Keough, & Boyd, 1997). A literature search revealed only four studies about TP within the leisure studies literature. Philipp (1992) stated that time orientation significantly influences participation in leisure activities, and a future time orientation plays an important role in understanding the participants. Cotte and Ratneshwar (2001) suggested that people choose a leisure activity over another by relying on the previous experience with the leisure activity and categorical knowledge about leisure time (i.e., every Saturday night watching a movie, every Wednesday playing a card game). Shores’ dissertation (2005) proposed that individual time perspective make significant differences in people’s time allocation, recreation experience preferences, and wellness. A subsequent Shores and Scott’s study (2007) further confirmed their previous findings, using more inclusive subjects in age. These four studies focused primarily on how TP affects individuals’ decision on their leisure participation. However, no studies have addressed the relationship between TP and leisure-related psychological variables in leisure-related literature. Therefore, the pupose of this study was to examine how leisure participants differently experience Flow, Enjoyment, and perception of Quality of Life (QOL) according to respondents’ TP. Methodology Seven hundred ninety seven subjects were recruited from nine universities: a) University of Minnesota, b) University of Toledo, c) University of Georgia, d), University of Marlyland, e) 2 Kent State University, f) University of Kansas, g) University of Akron, h) Texas A&M, and i) University of Tennessee. Most (89%) were students in classes taught by the primary investigator or colleagues (reference Table 1). Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval for this study was obtained for recruiting subjects inside and outside the universities. Every subject was older than 17 and was participating in a Leisure Time Physical Activity. For the data analysis, respondents’ characteristics were described using descriptive statistics and frequencies. All the subjects were clustered into one of TP variables utilizing a Kmeans cluster analysis. ANCOVA was used to examine associations between the TP variables (i.e., Past-Positive, Past-Negative, Present-Fatalistic, Present-Hedonistic, and Future TP, reference Table. 2 for further information) and Flow, Enjoyment, and the perception of Quality of Life (QOL). Significant findings between the TP variables and Flow, Enjoyment, and QOL were described using Boferroni Post Hoc test. Results This study found that four TP most clearly categorized the datasets. 223 (28.7%) respondents were grouped in Future (F) Time Perspective. 163 (21.0%) respondents were in Present Fatalistic (PF) Time Perspective. 127 (16.3%) respondents were clustered in Past Positive (PP) Time Perspective. 264 (34%) respondents, the largest of the four clusters, were labeled Past Negative/Present Hedonistic (PN/PH) Time Perspective. The respondents with PP showed significant higher levels of Flow experience than those with PF. Higher levels of Enjoyment were experienced by the respondents with F than those with PN/PH. The respondents with F experienced higher levels of perception of QOL than those with PF and PN/PH. The respondents with PP also showed significantly higher perception of QOL than those with PF and PN/PH. 3 Table 3 Table 4 Discussion According to TP, different levels of Flow, Enjoyment, and QOL were shown. PP and F are considered “good TP” given that respondents with those TP categories showed significantly higher levels of Flow, Enjoyment, and the perception of QOL. Conversely, PN/PF is considered “bad TP” in LTPA contexts because of significantly lower levels of those psychological variables with this TP. These findings are consistent with the findings of Shores and Scott (2007), where learning, spirituality, family togetherness, competence testing, and physical fitness are considered “good recreation” and respondents with PP and F showed higher levels of those variables, whereas PN and PF showed lower levels of the variables. If the findings of this study and Shores and Scott’s study are not coincidence, PP and F can be regarded as good TP, while PN and PF cannot. From these findings, two important questions are raised for leisure researchers and educators: How are people educated into having PP or F?, and How are people with PN or PF educated into becoming those with PP or F? Further investigations into the two questions are needed. 4 References Anderson, W. T., & Golden, L. L. (1989). The five faces of Eve: Women's timestyle typologies. Advances in Consumer Research, 16, 346-354. Boyd, J., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1997). Constructing time after death: The transcendental-future time perspective. Time & Society, 6, 35-54. Cotte, J., & Ratneshwar, S. (2001). Timestyle and leisure decisions. Journal of Leisure Research, 33, 396-409. Denton, F. (1994). The dynamism of personal timestyle: How we do more in less time. Advances in Consumer Research, 21, 132-137. Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in the social sciences: Selected theoretical papers. New York: Harper. Philipp, S. F. (1992). Time orientation and participation in leisure activities. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 659-664. Shores, K. A. (2005). The relationship of time perspective to time allocation, recreation experience preferences, and wellness. Texas A&M University, College Station. Shores, K. A., & Scott, D. (2007). The relationship of individual time perspective and recreation experience preferences. Journal of Leisure Research, 39(1), 28-59. Zimbardo, P. G. (2002, Mar/Apr). Time to take our time. Psychology Today, 62. Zimbardo, P. G., Keough, K. A., & Boyd, J. N. (1997). Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personality & Individual Differences, 23, 1007-1023. 5 Table 1. Respondent Characteristics Characteristics Gender ( N = 778) Male Female Other Age of Respondent (N = 758) < 19.9 20 thru 20.9 21 thru 21.9 22 thru 22.9 23 thru 24.9 25 thru 29.9 30 thru 39.9 40 < Education (N = 775) High School Diploma 2 year Degree 4 year Degree Master’s Degree Doctoral Degree Ethnicity (N = 780) American Indian or Alaska Native Asian Black or African American White Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander Other Income (N = 682) < $20,000 $21,000 < $40,000 $41,000 < $60,000 $61,000 < $80,000 $81,000 < $100,000 > $1000,000 Region (N = 788) Minnesota Out of Minnesota (OH, MD, KS, TN, TX) N (Respondents %) Cumulative % 374 (48.1) 400 (51.4) 2 (.3) 48.1% 99.6% 99.9% 100 (13.2) 126 (16.6) 179 (23.6) 144 (19.0) 83 (10.9) 64 (8.4) 54 (7.1) 8 (1.1) 13.2 29.8 53.4 72.4 83.4 91.8 98.9 100 368 (47.5) 38 (4.9) 287 (37.0) 56 (7.2) 24 (3.1) 47.6 52.5 89.5 96.8 99.9 8 (1.0) 209 (26.8) 34 (4.4) 507 (65.0) 1.0 27.8 32.2 97.2 2 (.3) 97.4 14 (1.8) 99.2 207 (30.4) 109 (16.0) 93 (13.6) 65 (9.5) 85 (12.5) 122 (17.9) 30.5 46.5 60.1 69.6 82.1 100.0 529 (67.1) 67.1 259 (32.9) 100.0 6 Table 2. The Characteristics of Five Time Perspectives (Shores & Scott, 2007, p. 30) Time Perspective Description Past-negative A bias to think about and interpret the present in light of a generally unhappy, aversive view of the past Past-positive A bias to think about and interpret the present in light of a warm, sentimental attitude toward the past Present-fatalistic A bias to think about and interpret the present in light of a helpless and hopeless attitude toward life that is related to an external locus of control Present-hedonistic A bias to spend most time thinking about and interpreting the present in light of a hedonistic, risk-taking, “devil may care” attitude toward life Future A bias to think about and interpret the present in light of anticipated goals and rewards Table 3. Analysis of Covariance of Time Perspective, Flow, Enjoyment, the Perception of QOL Marital Status Education Ethnicity Age Gender Income Time Perspective Main Effect (F) 1.470 2.639 2.426 6.804 11.567 .006 2.696 Sig. N.S. N.S. N.S. .009 .001 N.S. .045 Main Effect (F) 1.868 7.898 1.279 24.301 30.009 1.080 4.276 Sig. N.S. .005 N.S. .000 .000 N.S. .005 Main Effect (F) .150 .678 20.530 1.296 3.036 3.438 24.010 Sig. N.S. N.S. .000 N.S. N.S. N.S. .000 Variables Flow Enjoyment QOL *Main effects and Sig. between TP and the variables are highlighted. Table 4. Resutls of Bonferroni Post Hoc Test between TP and the Variables Variables TP F PF PP PN/PH Mean 3.738 3.582 3.821 3.657 SD .692 .653 .661 .691 Mean 3.581 3.352 3.561 3.334 SD .847 .895 .930 .953 Mean 4.048 3.714 4.003 3.640 SD .469 .559 .427 .552 Flow Enjoyment QOL F Sig. Bonferroni 3.501 .015 PP > PF* 4.208 .006 F > PN/PH* .000 F > PF***; F > PN/PH***; PP > PF***; PP > PN/PH*** 32.595 * Significant at p. 05, ** Significant at p .01, ***Significant at p .001