Feedback and teaching vocabulary translating academic research findings into classroom practice Alice Gruber International School Stuttgart Aim of this session: - Discuss findings about written and spoken feedback Analyse different types of feedback Share your practice Discuss vocabulary teaching Error Correction (Corrective Feedback - CF) Issues: • which errors • how many errors • how to correct • when to correct (for spoken feedback) Feedback can influence self-efficacy Self-efficacy: people’s perceptions of their ability to perform well on a given task. • more easily destroyed by negative feedback than restored by positive feedback. • twice as much positive feedback as negative feedback (e.g. Voerman et al 2012). • Students are good at recognising formulaic positive comments (Hyland and Hyland 2001). • Language aptitude, learning style, anxiety, and attitudes (Conrad & Goldstein, 1999; Sheen, 2011) • Learners’ beliefs influence: whether or not they retain feedback (Storch &Wigglesworth, 2010) • whether they prefer content over grammar feedback (Hyland, 2011) • Feedback perceived as demotivating: unlikely to attend feedback • Beginners: easily overwhelmed (Bitchener and Ferris 2011) • Comments should be specific rather than formulaic (Hyland and Hyland 2001) Written corrective feedback Students: expect and value teacher feedback (e.g., Eginarlar, 1993; Ferris, 1995;) and prefer the more explicit option. expect their teachers to point out and correct their errors. tend to believe that they cannot possibly correct their own errors without that specific information. Students: feel that teachers should focus their feedback on local issues (such as grammar and mechanics) Direct and indirect feedback Direct WCF: when the teacher provides the correct form • e.g. crossing out of an unnecessary word, the insertion of a missing word, and the provision of the correct form or structure Reasons for giving direct feedback • clear information to help them resolve more complex errors (for example, syntactic structure) • may be appropriate for some error types (e.g. idiomatic lexical issues such as prepositions) • Disadvantages? • In advanced proficiency classroom settings: direct error correction plus oral meta-linguistic explanation (5 minute one-on-one conferences) (Bitchener, Young and Cameron 2005) • Ferris (2010) questions the practicality of this • an instructional emphasis on written errors may neglect other aspects of writing, such as ideas, organization and processes. Indirect WCF Indirect feedback Pointing out errors but without correcting (e.g. underlining or circling an error). • Lalande (1982) argued that indirect CF is preferable because it requires reflection and problem-solving, leading to more long-term growth in writing/selfmonitoring ability. • for lower proficiency writers in language learning classes, indirect feedback tends to be less preferred because they have a more limited linguistic repertoire to draw on (Bitchener and Knoch 2010). Written Feedback • Direct/indirect feedback • Your thoughts/comments/practice? Focus and unfocused feedback focused WCF: teacher targets a small number of specific errors unfocused WCF: all/most errors are corrected Potential issues? students generally prefer unfocused rather than focused correction (Bitchener and Ferris 2011). Some studies: focused WCF is more valuable than unfocused (Ferris et al 2013). But: students might underestimate the amount of effort and attention it takes to self-edit an important piece of writing. Students may be misled by selective error feedback because students may mistakenly think that the rest of their writing is completely right (Lalande 1982) Constraints regarding feedback • Pienemann's Processability Theory (PT) predicts a predictable order of acquisition in language learning. e.g. in ESL question formation: (Pienemann 1998) • Feedback on a particular grammar point: only useful if the learner is at a stage where they are ready to acquire the feature • It is unlikely that teachers will be sufficiently familiar with individual learners’ interlanguages (Hashemnezhad 2012) • consider what the learners expect: will be more likely to engage in the feedback process and be more effective users of the feedback they receive (Bitchener and Ferris 2011) Written Feedback • Focused/unfocused feedback • Your thoughts/comments/practice? focused WCF: teacher targets a small number of specific errors unfocused WCF: all/most errors are corrected Oral Feedback Explicit feedback: Overt error correction (possibly with the provision of a grammatical explanation, metalinguistic information) (1) L2 learner: My father work every day. (a) No, you should say works, not work. (b) The verb needs to agree with the subject. (c) Father is the subject of the verb. So, you should say, “My father works, instead of my father work.” • drawback it is more obtrusive than other types of correction and could disrupt the flow of communication. • If metalinguistic information is provided: this kind of feedback promotes not only noticing but also understanding, which can lead to deeper learning (Sheen 2010) Implicit feedback: • signals incomprehensible language or a misformulation without interrupting the flow of the interaction. • attempt to clarify meaning and prevent a breakdown in communication. • Negotiation strategies: confirmation checks, clarification requests and comprehension checks. NNS: I bought a basen of flowers. NS: a basin? (Confirmation check) NNS: a base NS: a what? (Clarification request) NNS: a base …. a glass of flowers NS: oh, a vase NNS: yes, a vase of flowers Elicitation Eliciting completion of students’ own utterance by strategically pausing to allow students to fill in the blank No, not that. It‘s a . . .. How do we say X in French? students are asked to reformulate their utterance (Faqeih 2012) Opportunities for self-repair, promoting and automatizing the retrieval of the forms learners are familiar with Opportunity for hypothesis testing: may have a facilitative effect for foreign language development (Nassaji 2009). Your thoughts/comments/practice regarding these types of feedback explicit and implicit feedback elicitation Recasts Recasts are “utterances that repeat a learner’s incorrect utterance, making only the changes necessary to produce a correct utterance, without changing the meaning” (Nicholas, Lightbown and Spada (2001: 733). NNS: I don’t know much people here. NS: many people here recasts are the most common form of error correction employed by language teachers. (Oliver and Mackey (2003), Sheen (2004)) Lyster (1998): recasts are ambiguous to learners because they occur alongside other types of feedback, such as non-corrective repetitions Lyster and Ranta’s (1997) study showed that nearly 70% of recasts went unnoticed by the students. efficacy of recasts is constrained by a number of factors, for example: --learners’ proficiency (Panova and Lyster, 2002; Mackey and Philp, 1998), -choice of target form (Carpenter et al., 2006; Kim and Han, 2007), • recasts of morphosyntactic errors are less noticeable than recasts of phonological and lexical errors, and longer recasts are less effective than shorter recasts • In order for recasts to be effective, the focus needs to be on one error and it needs to be clearly signalled as a recast. Can be an effective form of feedback: they may be an opportunity for learners to modify their output and it may increase the salience of a particular form. Can raise learners‘ awareness of what they are unable to say in the target language or that there is a difference between their version and the version of the target-language. Your thoughts/comments/practice regarding recasts Recast: correct the mistake by repeating a learner’s utterance without the mistakes. Self-Correction In general, the advice given to teachers is to try to elicit self-correction from the learner rather than to correct directly (Erlam et al 2013) Problems with self-correction: • Learners prefer the teacher to do the correction for them. • Only possible if learners possess the necessary linguistic knowledge. • Depends on students’ analytical ability and accuracy What to correct • The gravity of an error - a matter of personal opinion (Ellis 2009). • Some teachers were inclined to view all errors as equally serious—“an error is an error.” (Vann, Meyer, and Lorenz (1984)) • Treat those errors which are regularly repeated by one or more students (Moss 2000) • correct frequent, serious and stigmatising mistakes (Bitchener and Ferris (2011)) • Not been exposed to a language form or are not ready to learn a particular structure: there is little point in intervening and giving feedback. Delayed or immediate CF? • Hedge (2000) noted that teacher guides accompanying course books frequently instruct teachers to leave correction until the end of fluency activities. • There is no research evidence to show that immediate correction is any more effective than delayed. Your thoughts/comments/practice regarding delayed/immediate CF and what to correct Vocabulary learning and teaching • Krashen (1993) points out, "When students travel, they don't carry grammar books, they carry dictionaries” • Vocabulary knowledge correlates highly with performance in listening, reading and writing. • Learning a word involves a receptive-productive continuum: receptive processing (for comprehension) and productive processing (for production) • active and passive vocabulary Learning a new word - the chance of a new word being stored in long-term memory Involvement load hypothesis (Hulstijn and Laufer 2001) need: depends on whether the word is necessary to complete a given task search: refers to the learner trying to find the meaning of a L2 word, using a dictionary or other sources, for instance teachers or peers. evaluation: decision-making during tasks, for instance a comparison of a given word with other words • The higher the level of the involvement load, the more effective the task is in promoting vocabulary learning • For example: flashcards to teach vocabulary versus exploring vocabulary via reading texts • Introducing vocabulary in lexical sets, i.e. word class sets, does not necessarily facilitate learning. • A thematic group containing the words sweater, changing room, try on, wool, striped may be more effective than the words scarf, tie, coat, pants and skirt, which are a semantically related set (Erten and Tekin 2008) • L1 translations: more effective than providing L2-based meanings (Ramachandran and Rahim (2004) • For relatively experienced L2 learners, L2–L1 word pairs may be more effective than L2-picture pairs (Lotto and de Groot 1998) • Learning lists of vocabulary with translations is an efficient way to learn the large volumes a language requires (Fitzpatrick, Al-Qarni and Meara 2008) Your thoughts/comments/practice regarding vocabulary teaching Summary - Variables influence feedback uptake (e.g. learners’ beliefs, whether students are ready…..) - Feedback influences self-efficacy (give twice as much positive feedback) - Consider what learners expect when giving CF - Recasts: potentially ambiguous – one at a time and clearly signalled. - Active vs. passive vocabulary - Involvement load: need, search, evaluation - Teach different word classes together, in a thematic group Thank you for listening! grubera@issev.de Bibliography Bitchener, J. 2008. Evidence in support of written corrective feedback. Journal of Second Language Writing, 17, 102–118. Bitchener, J.,& Ferris, D. 2011. Written corrective feedbackin second language acquisition and writing. NewYork: Routledge. Busse, V. How do students of German perceive feedback practices at university? A motivational exploration. Journal of Second Language Writing Volume 22, Issue 4, December 2013, Pages 406–424 Black, P.,Harrison,C.,Lee,C.,Marshall,B.,&Wiliam,D.2002.Working inside the blackbox: Assessment for learning in the classroom. London. Carroll, S., & Swain, M. 1993. Explicit and implicit negative feedback: An empirical study of the learning of linguistic generalizations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 357–386. Conrad, S.M.,&Goldstein, L.M.1999. ESL studentrevisionafterteacherwrittencomments:Text,contexts,andindividuals. Journal of Second Language Writing, 8, 147–179. Duijnhouwer,H.,Prins,F.J.,& Stokking,K.M. 2010. Progress feedback effects on students’writing mastery goal,self-efficacy beliefs,and performance. Educational Research and Evaluation,16, 53–74. Eginarlar,H.1993. Student response to teacher feedback in EFLwriting. System, 21, 193– 204. Ellis, R., Loewen, S., & Erlam, R. 2006. Implicit and explicit corrective feedback and the acquisition of L2 grammar. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 339–368. Ferris, D.R.1995. Student reactions to teacher response in multiple-draft composition classrooms. TESOL Quarterly,29, 33–53. Ferris, D. R. 2006. Does error feedback help student writers? New evidence on the short- and long-term effects of written error correction. In K. Hyland & F. Hyland (Eds.), Feedback in second language writing: Contexts and issues (pp. 81–104). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Hattie, J.A.1987. Identifying the salient facets of a model of student learning: A synthesis of meta-analyses. International Journal of Educational Research,11, 187– 212. Hattie, J.A. 2009. Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800meta-analyses relating to achievement. NewYork: Routledge. Hyland, F.2011. The language learning potential of form-focused feedback on writing:Students’ and teachers ’perceptions. InR.M.Mancho´n (Ed.), Learning-towrite and writing-to-learn in an additional language (pp. 159–180). Amsterdam:JohnBenjamins. Hyland, K.,&Hyland,F. 2006. Feedback in second languag ewriting: Contexts and issues. Cambridge :Cambridge University Press. Lalande, J. F., II 1982. Reducing composition errors: An experiment. Modern Language Journal, 66, 140–149. Sheen, Y.(2011). Corrective feedback, individual differences and second language learning. New York: Springer. Storch, N.,& Wigglesworth,G.(2010). Learners’ processing,uptake,and retention of corrective feedback on writing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32, 303– 334. Voerman,L.,Meijer,P.C.,Korthagen,F.A.J.,&Simons,R.J.2012. Types and frequencies of feedback interventions in classroom interaction in secondary education. Teaching and Teacher Education,28, 1107–1115.