Coping with Achievement-Related Failure:
An Examination of Conversations Between Friends
Ellen Rydell Altermatt, Elizabeth Broady,
& Taryn Bellgard
Hanover College
Funded by National Science Foundation Grant BCS-0236678
Responses to Achievement-Related Failure
Mastery-oriented approach (Dweck, 1986)
Maintain high expectations for future success
Report positive affect
Demonstrate persistence in the face of challenge
Learned helpless approach (Dweck, 1986)
Have diminished expectations for future success
Report negative affect
Fail to persist in the face of challenge
What Role Do Social Interactions Play?
Hokoda and Fincham (1995)
Mothers of mastery-oriented children were more likely to offer assistance when their children requested it.
Mothers of mastery-oriented children were less likely to respond to self-critical statements (e.g., “I can’t do it.”) by suggesting that their children discontinue the activity.
Why Friends?
Children spend a substantial amount of time with friends
(e.g., Larson & Richards, 1991)
Children seek the support and advice of friends during times of stress (e.g., Causey & Dubow, 1992)
Research Questions
What are the features of children’s conversations with friends following achievement-related failure?
Are the features of children’s conversations related to changes in their responses to failure over time?
Participants
Fourth, fifth, and sixth grade students
116 friendship dyads
40 male dyads, 76 female dyads
70% Caucasian, 14% African-American, 7% Latino
Procedure
Ice-breaker activity
Children worked on puzzles in separate rooms
Focal child received unsolvable puzzles
Friend received either solvable (success condition) or unsolvable puzzles (failure condition)
Children were reunited to discuss the task
Children work on a final set of solvable problems
Questionnaires
Mastery-Orientated Beliefs
Baseline, Post-Failure, Post-discussion
Sample Items
“I want to do the puzzles.”
“I am confident that I will do well on the puzzles.”
Reliability: αs = .81 to .91
Coding Children’s Conversations
Overview
17,000 statements (κs = .71 to .99)
8,441 focal child statements
8,559 friend statements
75% of statements were on-task
Statement Types
Performance Checks (e.g., How many [puzzles] did you solve?)
Positive Performance Statements (e.g., I got them all.)
Negative Performance Statements (e.g., I didn’t get any of mine.)
Positive Self-Evaluative Statements (e.g. I’m really good at puzzles.)
Negative Self-Evaluative Statements (e.g., I stink at puzzles.)
Discounting Statements (e.g., I’m used to doing puzzles that attach.)
Help Seeking (e.g., How do you make the diamond?)
Help Giving (e.g., Okay. Well, you just need to look for a green one.)
Descriptive Analyses
Mastery-oriented beliefs
Features of conversations
4
3.5
3
2.5
2
1.5
1
Mastery-Orientated Beliefs
Baseline Post-Failure Post-discussion
Male Focal Child Female Focal Child
Descriptive Analyses
Mastery-oriented beliefs
Features of conversations
Gender Differences in Performance Checks
2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
4
3.5
3
Male Focal Child Female Focal Child
Predicting Mastery-Oriented Beliefs
Hierarchical regression analyses
Dependent variable
Mastery-oriented beliefs at post-discussion
Control variable
Mastery-oriented beliefs at post-failure
Predictor variables
Statement type
Gender (male, female)
Condition (friend success, friend failure)
Predicting Mastery-Oriented Beliefs
Statement Type Main Effect
Discounting statements (FC), β = -.15, p < .01
Help-giving statements (Friend), β = .12, p < .01
Predicting Mastery Orientation
Statement Type x Gender Interactions
Negative performance statements (FC), β= -.28, p < .001
Negative self-evaluative statements (FC), β= -.19, p < .01
Negative Performance Statements
3.5
3.25
3
2.75
2.5
2.25
2
Low (-1 SD) High (+1 SD)
% of Negative Performance Statements
Male Female
Negative Self-Evaluative Statements
3.5
3.25
3
2.75
2.5
2.25
2
Low (-1 SD) High (+1 SD)
% of Negative Self-Evaluative Statements
Male Female
Why the gender difference?
Sequential analyses
What happens immediately after each statement type?
Are particular sequences of statements more likely to occur with boys than with girls?
Sequential Analyses
Negative Performance Statements
20
16
12
8
4
0
Performance Check Positive Performance
Statement
Negative Performance
Statement
Male Friend Female Friend
Sequential Analyses
Negative Self-Evaluative Statements
8
4
0
20
16
12
Negative Self-Evaluative Statement
Male Friend Female Friend
Sample Conversation Between Girls
FC: I can’t put puzzles together. As a matter of fact I think that I may need to practice a little more.
FR: I know… This was hard.
FC: [Laughs] …. I mean, I hated it. I was like, ok do this, do this, and then she was like, ‘I’m sorry, but your time is up.’
FR: I know, she was like, ‘It’s time for the next one.’ I was like, um.
FC: [Laughs]. Shoot!
FR: And then …. it was time for the next one.
FC: …I’m still shaking from doing it.
Co-Rumination
Rose (2002)
Co-rumination is characterized by
repeated discussion of the same problem
mutual encouragement of discussing problem
Girls are more likely to co-ruminate than are boys
Co-rumination has tradeoffs