Revoicing in Clinical Interviewing: Interaction Meets Knowledge Andy diSessa, Jim Greeno, Sarah Michaels, Cathy O’Connor Work supported, in part, with a Conference Grant from American Educational Research Association (AERA) Larger Framing Analysis of Knowledge and Analysis of Interaction are often positioned as adversaries. The KAIA Project To investigate possible synergistic relationship between knowledge analysis (KA) and interaction analysis (IA) Are they: 1. Contradictory, Competitors? 2. Distinct and Complementary? 3. Deeply Synergistic: Mutually Constitutive? Edited Volume This and other studies will appear in: diSessa, A., Levin, M, & Brown, N. (Eds.): (2015) Knowledge and Interaction in Learning: Theories in Collision. Taylor and Francis. Clinical Interviewing Manifestly a distinctive interactional form, used for studying students’ knowledge and learning at least since Piaget • Contested: Characterized as unnatural, interactionally suspect, ecologically invalid • Sometimes a locus for contesting the viability of “knowledge,” per se IA Enters via: Revoicing O’Connor and Michaels, 1993, 1996. Grounded in discourse analysis, conversation analysis, ethnography of speaking Structure and Function Starting with structure: Structure of a Prototypical Revoicing 1. 2. 3. 4. “So” marker Verb of thinking or saying (attribution) Reformulation Solicitation of validation So, you’re saying energy is not conserved in chemistry. Do I have that right? Functions of Revoicing • Validating students as sources of valuable knowledge and legitimate adjudicators of their own ideas. • Positioning students as participants in a scientific enterprise • “Bending” toward normative science – Carrying “productive” student ideas to the floor – Clarifying what they have said for other students – Introducing technical vocabulary Method: Developed a Ground-up Coding Scheme • Goal or function: clarify, refine, promote rethinking (12 categories +) • Formulation: – Focus: what’s said, thought, infered, chain of reasoning, other, extended explanation of focus (4 categories +) – Anticipation Frame: (A-Frame): yes/no, extended explanation (2 categories +) • Interviewer response: yes/no, rejectattribution, rethink&affirm/reject/refine (7 categories +) • … Interview • “Why is it hotter in the summer and colder in the winter?” • Subject is “J” • Interviewer (I) is me. Baseline About a third, 30 / ~100 turns of the interviewer were classified as revoicing!! Examples I: So you’ve decided now that probably // J: // I haven't decided anything yet. I'm just thinking; see if it makes more sense. I: I understand. That’s great. I: So the problem that you are having now is that you are thinking that this part of the world is just going to get cooked? Is that // J: Right. … well … yeah. Yeah, because …. Examples: Following a Chain of Reasoning I: Let me try to follow your reasoning. You decided that // It sounded like in the beginning you weren't quite sure if it was cold in Australia when it was hot here or not. [J: Right.] I: Then you decided you did know that it’s wintertime there, it’s cold season there when it is warm here. Did I // [J: Yeah.] I: So you decided that you knew that. That was pretty certain. [J: Right. Definitely.] I: OK. Definitely. I: Now that’s a problem because your little theory says that if it’s closer // [to the sun, then it is hotter.] [J: Right.] Analysis of Function In common: + Bringing student/interviewee ideas to the floor Mutatis mutandis: • Validate students as sources of (scientifically) valuable ideas Analysis of Function In common: + Bringing student/interviewee ideas to the floor Mutatis mutandis: • Validate students as sources of (scientifically) valuable ideas Analysis of Function In common: + Bringing student/interviewee ideas to the floor Mutatis mutandis: • Reinforce that interviewee ideas are the focus NOT in clinical context: X Framing interviewee as scientist/inquirer X Bending toward the normative X Carrying forward “productive” ideas X Clarifying for others X Introducing normative terms Analysis of Function New (newly important): ! Clarifying (for the interviewer) ! Asking for refinement ! Promoting reconsideration Synthesis of Function Classroom Context: • Moving students along toward normative understanding and participation Clinical Context: • “Making data appear” about focused aspects of interviewee’s thinking Analysis of Structure 1. 2. 3. 4. “So” marker Verb of thinking or saying (attribution) Reformulation Solicitation of validation Structural Anomalies? Relatively few revoicings had the conventional structural earmarks. A kind of uniformity can be recovered by “functional abstraction,” observing more general purposes served by the earmarks. Functional Abstaction (anticipated in the grounded codes) ATTRIBUTION (verb of thinking or saying) subsumed under “FOCUS” – specifying what, precisely, is the focus of attention. SOLICITATION OF VALIDATION subsumed under providing an “A-FRAME” (anticipation of answer frame), a partial specification of what kind of an answer is sought. Structural Anomalies? 1. “So”: only 14/30. To the extent that “so” serves to identify that the relevant idea is inferred from what the interviewee says or thinks, this is a partial specification of FOCUS. Diversity: “so” means: - I infer from what you say… - You inferred… - You MIGHT have inferred… And there are a host of other, more extended ways of specifying focus. Structural Anomalies? 2. Attribution (“verb of thinking or saying”): only 11/30. Can be read as FOCUS. Diversity: – – – – – – – The words said The gist of what was said An inference that was made An inference that MIGHT have made The inferential link, WHY an inference was made Epistemic attitude: “That seemed obvious to you.” … Structural Anomalies? 3. Reformulation - Seldom actual reforumlations (no bending) - More literal repetitions or subspecification of aspect of interest. Diversity: The coding scheme did not pick up a lot of interest, here that was not captured under focus. Structural Anomalies 4. Soliciting validation: Only 4/30. Might be read as a particular “low-resolution” aframe, guidance on what kind of answer is hoped for (agree or disagree). Mostly the interviewer and interviewee seemed to think, and assumed, they understood each other well. Diversity in a-frame: – Anticipated “yes or no” about an interviewee’s interpretation – “Is that the idea? (rough agreement) – “I’m trying to understand exactly …” (anticipating more detail) Knowledge and Interaction? • Hugely diverse practice, intimately connected to interviewer focus and intent. • How is it that strategies align so well with goals? (consider the clinical vs. classroom cases) – Strategized! (involving “problem solving” concerning goals, resources, and context) – Evidence of strategizing, and interviewer/teacher report – Reflective practitioners! – Knowledge of Interaction, need not be conscious • Theory of knowledge is strongly implicated – VERY few questions about about the normative list of ideas, no assessment of right and wrong – VERY strong attention to diversity and contextuality Conclusions: “The Big Enchilada” • You cannot understand revoicing without understanding the interviewer’s knowledge, i.e., without understanding the functions it serves for those who do it, and how they imagine that what they do might bring about what they want. • This is particularly true in knowledge-oriented enterprises like clinical interviewing, when diverse assumptions about knowledge are involved, and practices will track them. Conclusions: “The Big Enchilada” Knowledge and Interaction 1. Contradictory, Competitors 2. Distinct and Complementary 3. Deeply Synergistic: Mutually Constitutive Conclusions: “The Big Enchilada” Knowledge and Interaction 1. Contradictory, Competitors 2. Distinct and Complementary 3. Deeply Synergistic: Mutually Constitutive Knowledge and interaction are so tightly interconnected here that we cannot understand one without the other. End