this presentation - Harvard Graduate School of Education

advertisement
Strategy Flexibility Matters for
Student Mathematics
Achievement: A Meta-Analysis
Kelley Durkin
Bethany Rittle-Johnson
Vanderbilt University, United States
Jon R. Star
Harvard University, United States
Defining Strategy Flexibility
• Simplest definition:
Knowing more than one strategy for solving a
particular type of problem (e.g., Heirdsfield & Cooper, 2002)
• Most complex definition:
Being able to use a variety of strategies and
information from the problem context, the
learner’s environment, and the sociocultural
context to select the most appropriate problem
solving procedure (e.g., Verschaffel, Luwel, Torbeyns, & Van
Dooren, 2007)
2
Recent Focus on Strategy Flexibility
• Previously, flexibility rarely measured as an
instructional outcome (Star, 2005).
• Standardized tests in the U.S. include sections on:
– Concepts
– Procedures
– Problem solving
– But not flexibility
• Recently, flexibility examined as a separate
outcome (Star, 2007; Verschaffel et al., 2007).
3
Importance of Strategy Flexibility
• Helps adapt existing procedures to unfamiliar
problems (e.g., Blöte, Van der Burg, & Klein, 2001)
• Greater understanding of domain concepts (e.g.,
Hiebert & Wearne, 1996)
• Crucial component of expertise in problem
solving (Dowker, 1992; Dowker, Flood, Griffiths, Harris, & Hook, 1996;
Star & Newton, 2009)
4
Current Study
• Is strategy flexibility related to other
mathematical constructs?
– Conceptual Knowledge
• Success recognizing and explaining key domain
concepts (Carpenter et al., 1998; Hiebert & Wearne, 1996)
– Procedural Knowledge
• Success executing action sequences to solve problems
(Hiebert & Wearne, 1996; Rittle-Johnson, Siegler, & Alibali, 2001)
– General Mathematics Achievement
• Meta-analysis of our past work
5
Our Definition of Strategy
Flexibility
• Knowing multiple strategies and their relative
efficiencies (Flexibility Knowledge)
AND
• Adapting strategy choice to specific problem
features (Flexible Use)
(e.g., Blöte et al., 2001; National Research Council, 2001; Rittle-Johnson &
Star, 2007)
6
Method Overview
• Selected Studies
• Measures
• Analysis Strategies
7
Included Studies
Study Authors
Rittle-Johnson & Star
Year
2007
Topic
N
Equation Solving 70
Grade
7
Star & Rittle-Johnson
2008
Equation Solving 155
6
Rittle-Johnson & Star
2009
Equation Solving 162
7&8
Rittle-Johnson, Star, &
Durkin
Star et al.
2009
Equation Solving 236
7&8
2009
Estimation
65
5
Star & Rittle-Johnson
2009
Estimation
157
5&6
Rittle-Johnson, Star, &
Durkin
Schneider, Rittle-Johnson &
Star
2011
Equation Solving 198
8
2011
Equation Solving 293
7&8
8
Measures
•
•
•
•
•
Flexibility Knowledge
Flexible Use
Conceptual Knowledge
Procedural Knowledge
Standardized Tests
9
Measures
• Flexibility Knowledge
– Knowing multiple procedures and the relative
efficiency of the procedures
5(x + 3) + 6 = 5(x + 3) + 2x
6 = 2x
a. What step did the student use to get from the first line to
the second line?
b. Do you think that this is a good way to start this problem?
Circle One:
(a) a very good way
(b) OK to do, but not a very good way
(c) Not OK to do
c. Explain your reasoning.
10
Measures
• Flexible Use
– Students using the most appropriate strategy
depending on problem features
3(h + 2) + 4(h + 2) = 35
7(h + 2) = 35
• Sometimes know a more appropriate strategy
for solving a problem before actually use it
(Blöte et al., 2001; Siegler & Crowley, 1994)
11
Measures
• Conceptual Knowledge
– Ability to recognize and explain key domain concepts
Which of the following is a like term to (could be
combined with) 7(j + 4)?
(a) 7(j + 10) (b) 7(p + 4) (c) j (d) 2(j + 4) (e) a and d
• Procedural Knowledge
– Ability to execute action sequences to solve problems
3(h + 2) + 4(h + 2) = 35
12
Measures
• Standardized Tests
National Tests
• Comprehensive Testing Program (CTP)
• Measures of Academic Progress (MAP)
State Tests
• Massachusetts Comprehensive Assessment System (MCAS)
• Tennessee Comprehensive Assessment Program (TCAP)
• Collected scores from school records
13
Coding and Analysis Strategies
• Calculated correlation between each pair of
outcomes for each study
• Fischer’s z to transform correlations to get effect
sizes, ESr, for each study (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
• The mean correlation effect size was calculated using
a random effects model.
14
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual
Use
Knowledge
Procedural Standardized
Knowledge Test
1
.635
.563
.610
.535
1
.541
.627
.404
1
.544
.520
1
.475
Procedural
Knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
15
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual
Use
Knowledge
Procedural Standardized
Knowledge Test
1
.635
.563
.610
.535
1
.541
.627
.404
1
.544
.520
1
.475
Procedural
Knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
16
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual
Use
Knowledge
1
.635
.563Flexibility knowledge
.610
.535
and
flexible
use strongly related
1
.541
.627
.404
1
.544
.520
1
.475
Procedural
Knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
Procedural Standardized
Knowledge Test
17
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized
Use
Knowledge Knowledge Test
1
.635
.563
.610
.535
1
.541
.627
.404
1
.544
.520
1
.475
Procedural
Knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
18
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized
Use
Knowledge Knowledge Test
1 Conceptual .635
knowledge had
moderately strong
relationships to
1
flexibility
.563
.610
.535
.541
.627
.404
1
.544
.520
1
.475
Procedural
Knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
19
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Procedural
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized
Use
Knowledge Knowledge Test
1 Conceptual .635
knowledge had
moderately strong
relationships to
1
flexibility
Procedural
knowledge had
moderately strong
relationships to
flexibility
.563
.610
.535
.541
.627
.404
1
.544
.520
1
.475
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
20
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Procedural
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized
Use
Knowledge Knowledge Test
1
.635
.563
.610
.535
1
.541
.627
.404
.544
.520
1
.475
Similar to correlation 1
between conceptual
and procedural
knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
21
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized
Use
Knowledge Knowledge Test
1
.635
.563
.610
.535
1
.541
.627
.404
1
.544
.520
1
.475
Procedural
Knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
22
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized
Use
Knowledge Knowledge Test
1
.635
.563test measures
.610
Standardized
significantly correlated with
flexibility
1
.541
.627
1
Procedural
Knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
.535
.404
.544
.520
1
.475
23
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized
Use
Knowledge Knowledge Test
1
.635
.563test measures
.610
Standardized
significantly correlated with
flexibility
1
.541
.627
Standardized test measures
significantly correlated with
other outcomes
1
.544
Procedural
Knowledge
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
1
.535
.404
.520
.475
24
Results
• Mean correlations between outcomes
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible
Use
Conceptual
Knowledge
Procedural
Knowledge
Flexibility
Knowledge
Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized
Use
Knowledge Knowledge Test
1
.635
.563test measures
.610
Standardized
significantly correlated with
flexibility
1
.541
.627
Standardized test measures
significantly correlated with
other outcomes
1
.544
Correlations between flexibility
and standardized tests 1similar
to other correlations
Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001)
.535
.404
.520
.475
25
3 Main Findings
• Flexibility knowledge and flexible use are
separate constructs
• Flexibility is related to other constructs
• Standardized tests relate to flexibility as well
as they relate to other constructs
26
The Construct of Strategy Flexibility
• May be important to measure flexible use and
flexibility knowledge separately.
– Appears measures of knowledge and use are
tapping different aspects of flexibility.
• Conceptual and procedural knowledge are
related to flexibility (Schneider et al., 2011).
27
Relation to Standardized Tests
• Standardized test scores relate to flexibility just as
well as they relate to conceptual and procedural
knowledge.
• Teachers can feel pressured to teach to the test,
and the lack of flexibility items on assessments
could lead to less time on flexibility in the
classroom.
• Push for standardized tests to include items that
assess flexibility.
• Flexibility a valued outcome when evaluating
interventions.
28
Conclusion
• Strategy flexibility is important for developing
expertise and efficient problem solving
• Need to measure and encourage students’
strategy flexibility in the future
29
Acknowledgements
• E-mail: kelley.durkin@vanderbilt.edu
• Visit our Contrasting Cases Website at
http://gseacademic.harvard.edu/contrastingcases/index
.html for more information
• Thanks to the Children’s Learning Lab at Vanderbilt
University
• Funded by a grant from the Institute for Education
Sciences, U.S. Department of Education
– The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not
represent views of the U.S. Department of Education.
30
30
References
Blöte, A. W., Van der Burg, E., & Klein, A. S. (2001). Students' flexibility in
solving two-digit addition and subtraction problems: Instruction effects.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 627-638.
Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., Jacobs, V. R., Fennema, E., & Empson, S. B.
(1998). A longitudinal study of invention and understanding in children's
multidigit addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 29(1), 3-20.
Dowker, A. (1992). Computational estimation strategies of professional
mathematicians. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23(1),
45-55.
Dowker, A., Flood, A., Griffiths, H., Harris, L., & Hook, L. (1996). Estimation
strategies of four groups. Mathematical Cognition, 2(2), 113-135.
Heirdsfield, A. M., & Cooper, T. J. (2002). Flexibility and inflexibility in accurate
mental addition and subtraction: Two case studies. The Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 21, 57-74.
31
References
Hiebert, J., & Wearne, D. (1996). Instruction, understanding and skill in
multidigit addition and subtraction. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 251283.
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical Meta-Analysis (Vol. 49).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up: Helping children learn
mathematics. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing
conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An
iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 346-362.
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solution methods
facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study
on learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3),
561-574.
32
References
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2009). Compared with what? The effects of
different comparisons on conceptual knowledge and procedural flexibility
for equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 529-544.
Rittle-Johnson, B., Star, J. R., & Durkin, K. (2009). The importance of prior
knowledge when comparing examples: Influences on conceptual and
procedural knowledge of equation solving. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 101(4), 836-852.
Rittle-Johnson, B., Star, J. R., & Durkin, K. (2011, June 28). Developing
procedural flexibility: Are novices prepared to learn from comparing
procedures? British Journal of Educational Psychology. Advance online
publication.
Schneider, M., Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011, August 8). Relations
among conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural
flexibility in two samples differing in prior knowledge. Developmental
Psychology. Advance online publication.
33
References
Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1994). Constraints on learning in nonprivileged
domains. Cognitive Psychology, 27(2), 194-226.
Star, J. R. (2005). Reconceptualizing procedural knowledge. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 36, 404-411.
Star, J. R. (2007). Foregrounding Procedural Knowledge. [Peer Reviewed].
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 132-135.
Star, J. R., & Newton, K. J. (2009). The nature and development of experts’
strategy flexibility for solving equations. ZDM-International Journal on
Mathematics Education, 41, 557-567.
Star, J. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2008). Flexibility in problem solving: The case
of equation solving. Learning and Instruction, 18, 565 - 579.
Star, J. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2009). It pays to compare: An experimental
study on computational estimation. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 102, 408 - 426.
34
References
Star, J. R., Rittle-Johnson, B., Lynch, K., & Perova, N. (2009). The role of prior
knowledge in the development of strategy flexibility: The case of
computational estimation. ZDM, 41(5), 569-579.
Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., & Van Dooren, W. (2007). Developing
adaptive expertise: A feasible and valuable goal for (elementary)
mathematics education? Ciencias Psicologicas, 2007(1), 27-35.
35
Download