Strategy Flexibility Matters for Student Mathematics Achievement: A Meta-Analysis Kelley Durkin Bethany Rittle-Johnson Vanderbilt University, United States Jon R. Star Harvard University, United States Defining Strategy Flexibility • Simplest definition: Knowing more than one strategy for solving a particular type of problem (e.g., Heirdsfield & Cooper, 2002) • Most complex definition: Being able to use a variety of strategies and information from the problem context, the learner’s environment, and the sociocultural context to select the most appropriate problem solving procedure (e.g., Verschaffel, Luwel, Torbeyns, & Van Dooren, 2007) 2 Recent Focus on Strategy Flexibility • Previously, flexibility rarely measured as an instructional outcome (Star, 2005). • Standardized tests in the U.S. include sections on: – Concepts – Procedures – Problem solving – But not flexibility • Recently, flexibility examined as a separate outcome (Star, 2007; Verschaffel et al., 2007). 3 Importance of Strategy Flexibility • Helps adapt existing procedures to unfamiliar problems (e.g., Blöte, Van der Burg, & Klein, 2001) • Greater understanding of domain concepts (e.g., Hiebert & Wearne, 1996) • Crucial component of expertise in problem solving (Dowker, 1992; Dowker, Flood, Griffiths, Harris, & Hook, 1996; Star & Newton, 2009) 4 Current Study • Is strategy flexibility related to other mathematical constructs? – Conceptual Knowledge • Success recognizing and explaining key domain concepts (Carpenter et al., 1998; Hiebert & Wearne, 1996) – Procedural Knowledge • Success executing action sequences to solve problems (Hiebert & Wearne, 1996; Rittle-Johnson, Siegler, & Alibali, 2001) – General Mathematics Achievement • Meta-analysis of our past work 5 Our Definition of Strategy Flexibility • Knowing multiple strategies and their relative efficiencies (Flexibility Knowledge) AND • Adapting strategy choice to specific problem features (Flexible Use) (e.g., Blöte et al., 2001; National Research Council, 2001; Rittle-Johnson & Star, 2007) 6 Method Overview • Selected Studies • Measures • Analysis Strategies 7 Included Studies Study Authors Rittle-Johnson & Star Year 2007 Topic N Equation Solving 70 Grade 7 Star & Rittle-Johnson 2008 Equation Solving 155 6 Rittle-Johnson & Star 2009 Equation Solving 162 7&8 Rittle-Johnson, Star, & Durkin Star et al. 2009 Equation Solving 236 7&8 2009 Estimation 65 5 Star & Rittle-Johnson 2009 Estimation 157 5&6 Rittle-Johnson, Star, & Durkin Schneider, Rittle-Johnson & Star 2011 Equation Solving 198 8 2011 Equation Solving 293 7&8 8 Measures • • • • • Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Procedural Knowledge Standardized Tests 9 Measures • Flexibility Knowledge – Knowing multiple procedures and the relative efficiency of the procedures 5(x + 3) + 6 = 5(x + 3) + 2x 6 = 2x a. What step did the student use to get from the first line to the second line? b. Do you think that this is a good way to start this problem? Circle One: (a) a very good way (b) OK to do, but not a very good way (c) Not OK to do c. Explain your reasoning. 10 Measures • Flexible Use – Students using the most appropriate strategy depending on problem features 3(h + 2) + 4(h + 2) = 35 7(h + 2) = 35 • Sometimes know a more appropriate strategy for solving a problem before actually use it (Blöte et al., 2001; Siegler & Crowley, 1994) 11 Measures • Conceptual Knowledge – Ability to recognize and explain key domain concepts Which of the following is a like term to (could be combined with) 7(j + 4)? (a) 7(j + 10) (b) 7(p + 4) (c) j (d) 2(j + 4) (e) a and d • Procedural Knowledge – Ability to execute action sequences to solve problems 3(h + 2) + 4(h + 2) = 35 12 Measures • Standardized Tests National Tests • Comprehensive Testing Program (CTP) • Measures of Academic Progress (MAP) State Tests • Massachusetts Comprehensive Assessment System (MCAS) • Tennessee Comprehensive Assessment Program (TCAP) • Collected scores from school records 13 Coding and Analysis Strategies • Calculated correlation between each pair of outcomes for each study • Fischer’s z to transform correlations to get effect sizes, ESr, for each study (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). • The mean correlation effect size was calculated using a random effects model. 14 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Use Knowledge Procedural Standardized Knowledge Test 1 .635 .563 .610 .535 1 .541 .627 .404 1 .544 .520 1 .475 Procedural Knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) 15 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Use Knowledge Procedural Standardized Knowledge Test 1 .635 .563 .610 .535 1 .541 .627 .404 1 .544 .520 1 .475 Procedural Knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) 16 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Use Knowledge 1 .635 .563Flexibility knowledge .610 .535 and flexible use strongly related 1 .541 .627 .404 1 .544 .520 1 .475 Procedural Knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) Procedural Standardized Knowledge Test 17 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized Use Knowledge Knowledge Test 1 .635 .563 .610 .535 1 .541 .627 .404 1 .544 .520 1 .475 Procedural Knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) 18 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized Use Knowledge Knowledge Test 1 Conceptual .635 knowledge had moderately strong relationships to 1 flexibility .563 .610 .535 .541 .627 .404 1 .544 .520 1 .475 Procedural Knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) 19 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Procedural Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized Use Knowledge Knowledge Test 1 Conceptual .635 knowledge had moderately strong relationships to 1 flexibility Procedural knowledge had moderately strong relationships to flexibility .563 .610 .535 .541 .627 .404 1 .544 .520 1 .475 Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) 20 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Procedural Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized Use Knowledge Knowledge Test 1 .635 .563 .610 .535 1 .541 .627 .404 .544 .520 1 .475 Similar to correlation 1 between conceptual and procedural knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) 21 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized Use Knowledge Knowledge Test 1 .635 .563 .610 .535 1 .541 .627 .404 1 .544 .520 1 .475 Procedural Knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) 22 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized Use Knowledge Knowledge Test 1 .635 .563test measures .610 Standardized significantly correlated with flexibility 1 .541 .627 1 Procedural Knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) .535 .404 .544 .520 1 .475 23 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized Use Knowledge Knowledge Test 1 .635 .563test measures .610 Standardized significantly correlated with flexibility 1 .541 .627 Standardized test measures significantly correlated with other outcomes 1 .544 Procedural Knowledge Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) 1 .535 .404 .520 .475 24 Results • Mean correlations between outcomes Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Use Conceptual Knowledge Procedural Knowledge Flexibility Knowledge Flexible Conceptual Procedural Standardized Use Knowledge Knowledge Test 1 .635 .563test measures .610 Standardized significantly correlated with flexibility 1 .541 .627 Standardized test measures significantly correlated with other outcomes 1 .544 Correlations between flexibility and standardized tests 1similar to other correlations Note: All correlations were significant (p < .001) .535 .404 .520 .475 25 3 Main Findings • Flexibility knowledge and flexible use are separate constructs • Flexibility is related to other constructs • Standardized tests relate to flexibility as well as they relate to other constructs 26 The Construct of Strategy Flexibility • May be important to measure flexible use and flexibility knowledge separately. – Appears measures of knowledge and use are tapping different aspects of flexibility. • Conceptual and procedural knowledge are related to flexibility (Schneider et al., 2011). 27 Relation to Standardized Tests • Standardized test scores relate to flexibility just as well as they relate to conceptual and procedural knowledge. • Teachers can feel pressured to teach to the test, and the lack of flexibility items on assessments could lead to less time on flexibility in the classroom. • Push for standardized tests to include items that assess flexibility. • Flexibility a valued outcome when evaluating interventions. 28 Conclusion • Strategy flexibility is important for developing expertise and efficient problem solving • Need to measure and encourage students’ strategy flexibility in the future 29 Acknowledgements • E-mail: kelley.durkin@vanderbilt.edu • Visit our Contrasting Cases Website at http://gseacademic.harvard.edu/contrastingcases/index .html for more information • Thanks to the Children’s Learning Lab at Vanderbilt University • Funded by a grant from the Institute for Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education – The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the U.S. Department of Education. 30 30 References Blöte, A. W., Van der Burg, E., & Klein, A. S. (2001). Students' flexibility in solving two-digit addition and subtraction problems: Instruction effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 627-638. Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., Jacobs, V. R., Fennema, E., & Empson, S. B. (1998). A longitudinal study of invention and understanding in children's multidigit addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29(1), 3-20. Dowker, A. (1992). Computational estimation strategies of professional mathematicians. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23(1), 45-55. Dowker, A., Flood, A., Griffiths, H., Harris, L., & Hook, L. (1996). Estimation strategies of four groups. Mathematical Cognition, 2(2), 113-135. Heirdsfield, A. M., & Cooper, T. J. (2002). Flexibility and inflexibility in accurate mental addition and subtraction: Two case studies. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 21, 57-74. 31 References Hiebert, J., & Wearne, D. (1996). Instruction, understanding and skill in multidigit addition and subtraction. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 251283. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical Meta-Analysis (Vol. 49). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up: Helping children learn mathematics. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 346-362. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solution methods facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study on learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 561-574. 32 References Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2009). Compared with what? The effects of different comparisons on conceptual knowledge and procedural flexibility for equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 529-544. Rittle-Johnson, B., Star, J. R., & Durkin, K. (2009). The importance of prior knowledge when comparing examples: Influences on conceptual and procedural knowledge of equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 836-852. Rittle-Johnson, B., Star, J. R., & Durkin, K. (2011, June 28). Developing procedural flexibility: Are novices prepared to learn from comparing procedures? British Journal of Educational Psychology. Advance online publication. Schneider, M., Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011, August 8). Relations among conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flexibility in two samples differing in prior knowledge. Developmental Psychology. Advance online publication. 33 References Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1994). Constraints on learning in nonprivileged domains. Cognitive Psychology, 27(2), 194-226. Star, J. R. (2005). Reconceptualizing procedural knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 36, 404-411. Star, J. R. (2007). Foregrounding Procedural Knowledge. [Peer Reviewed]. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 132-135. Star, J. R., & Newton, K. J. (2009). The nature and development of experts’ strategy flexibility for solving equations. ZDM-International Journal on Mathematics Education, 41, 557-567. Star, J. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2008). Flexibility in problem solving: The case of equation solving. Learning and Instruction, 18, 565 - 579. Star, J. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2009). It pays to compare: An experimental study on computational estimation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 408 - 426. 34 References Star, J. R., Rittle-Johnson, B., Lynch, K., & Perova, N. (2009). The role of prior knowledge in the development of strategy flexibility: The case of computational estimation. ZDM, 41(5), 569-579. Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., & Van Dooren, W. (2007). Developing adaptive expertise: A feasible and valuable goal for (elementary) mathematics education? Ciencias Psicologicas, 2007(1), 27-35. 35