Memory Assessment on an Interdisciplinary Team: Roles and Collaborations Between Neuropsychology and Speech-Language Pathology Angelle M. Sander, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation Baylor College of Medicine Presented at Monthly Meeting of the Houston Neuropsychological Society January 2006 Joint Committee on Interprofessional Relations Between Division 40 (Clinical Neuropsychology) of the American Psychological Association (APA) and the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) ASHA Representatives (2005) Fofi Constantinidou, Ph.D., CCC-SLP Associate Professor & Director of Graduate Studies Director of NeuroCognitive Disorders Laboratory Department of Speech Pathology & Audiology Miami University 2 Bachelor Hall Oxford, OH 45056 Tel 513- 529-2507 Fax 513- 529-2502 Email constaf@muohio.edu Celia R. Hooper, Ph.D., CCC-SLP (Monitoring Vice President) ASHA Vice President for Professional Practices in Speech-Language Pathology (2003-2005) Professor and Department Head, UNC-Greensboro Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders 300 Ferguson Building, P. O. Box 26170 Greensboro, NC 27402-6170 Tel 336- 334-5184 Fax 336-334-4475 Email chooper@uncg.edu Wendy Ellmo, M.S., CCC-SLP, BCNCD Center for Head Injuries Cognitive Rehabilitation Department 2048 Oaktree Road Edison, NJ. 08820 Tel 732-906-2640 ext. 42721 Fax 732-906-9241 Email wellmo@msn.com Diane R. Paul, Ph.D., CCC-SLP (Ex Officio) Director Clinical Issues in Speech-Language Pathology American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 10801 Rockville Pike Rockville, MD 20852 Tel 301-897-5700 ext. 4297 Fax 301-897-7354 Email dpaul@asha.org Stacie Raymer, Ph.D. (ASHA Chair) 110 Child Study Center Old Dominion University Norfolk, VA 23529 Tel 757-683-4522 Fax 757-683-5593 Email sraymer@odu.edu Division 40 Representatives (2005) Robin Hanks, Ph.D., ABPP (Committee Chair) Chief, Rehabilitation Psychology & Neuropsychology Rehabilitation Institute of Michigan 261 Mack Boulevard Detroit, Michigan 48201 Tel 313-745-9763 Fax 313-745-9854 Email rhanks@dmc.org Tessa Hart, Ph.D. Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute (MRRI) Korman Suite 213 1200 West Tabor Road Philadelphia, PA 19141 Tel 215-456-6544 Fax 215-456-5926 Email thart@einstein.edu Angelle Sander, Ph.D. The Institute for Rehabilitation and Research Brain Injury Research Center 2455 South Braeswood Houston, TX 77030 Tel 713 383 5644 Fax 713 668 3695 Email asander@bcm.tmc.edu Risa Nakase-Richardson, Ph.D Neuropsychology Department Methodist Rehabilitation Center 1350 E. Woodrow Wilson Jackson, MS 39216 Tel 601-364-3448 Fax 601-364-3558 Email nakase@aol.com Jeffrey Wertheimer, Ph.D. Brooks Rehabilitation Center 3901 University Blvd., South Jacksonville, Florida 32216 Tel 904-858-7296 Fax 904-858-7255 Email Jeffrey.wertheimer@Brookshealth.org Past Division 40 Committee Members Kenneth Adams Linas Bieliauskas Robert Bornstein Gerald Goldstein Byron Rourke Jill Fischer Sharon Brown Joseph Ricker Doug Johnson-Greene Sanford Pederson Steven Putnam Joint Committee • Established in 1989 • Mission: • improve the clinical care of patients with congenital or acquired brain impairment by identifying and promoting assessment and rehabilitation practices that are both compatible with current neuropsychology knowledge and of demonstrable functional benefit to patients and their families • foster communication and collaborative work between speech-language pathologists and clinical neuropsychologists for the benefit of both professions Joint Committee Documents 1. Interdisciplinary Approaches to Brain Damage - 1989 Position Statement - http://www.asha.org/NR/rdonlyres/4A1C60E7- BC87-49A0-84F4-0E2AA9DED99E/0/ 19051_1.pdf Interdisciplinary Approaches to Brain Damage “Neuropsychology is the scientific study of the relationship between brain function and behavior. As such, neuropsychology, in the generic sense, is an interdisciplinary knowledge area embracing many contributing disciplines and professions. Therefore, it is appropriate that the knowledge base of neuropsychology not be regarded as proprietary by any given discipline or profession.” Interdisciplinary Approaches to Brain Damage “It is acknowledged that this knowledge base may be applied for the betterment of human welfare by different disciplines and professions with different training emphases. It is assumed that such practice will include techniques and procedures included in discipline-specific training and exclude those for which competence has not been established through such training criteria.” Interdisciplinary Approaches to Brain Damage “Individual practice may also be limited by laws or even ethical considerations in a given instance. It is also recognized that clinical practice with individuals who demonstrate impairment of the central nervous system is frequently an interdisciplinary effort which employs the particular strengths and expertise of various professions and disciplines.” “…mutual respect and cooperation between disciplines and professions is an ongoing necessity.” Joint Committee Documents 2. Guidelines for the Structure and Function of an Interdisciplinary Team for Persons With Brain Injury - 2003 Technical Report by Diane R. Paul, Ph.D., & Joseph H. Ricker, Ph.D. - http://www.asha.org/NR/rdonlyres/ 34D07350-A6C0-43DD-A175373B86939A48/0/19110_1.pdf Provides general guidelines for interdisciplinary teams for the clinical management of people with brain injury, with the ultimate goal to improve the quality of service for individuals affected by communication and cognitive disorders. Joint Committee Documents 4. Rehabilitation of Children and Adults With Cognitive-Communication Disorders After Brain Injury - 2002 Technical Report by Mark Ylvisaker, Ph.D., Robin Hanks, Ph.D., & Doug JohnsonGreene, Ph.D. - http://www.asha.org/NR/rdonlyres/7D6D3FD5-9197429E-9CA7-BB31E9C95B26/0/21939_4.pdf Published in Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation. (2002). 17(3), 191-209. The report outlines two paradigms for cognitive Rehabilitation: a traditional discrete approach, and an alternative contextualized approach. Joint Committee Documents 3. Evaluating and Treating Communication and Cognitive Disorders: Approaches to Referral and Collaboration for Speech-Language Pathology and Clinical Neuropsychology (2003) - http://www.asha.org/NR/rdonlyres/ E868544A-0C78-4F90-A5154FA69CE6A708/0/23026_2.pdf Encourages referral and collaboration between speech-language pathologists and clinical neuropsychogists and informs referral sources about the roles of both professions. Survey of Perceived Roles and Collaborations for Neuropsychologists and Speech-Language Pathologists in Rehabilitation • Surveys e-mailed to: – 1,351 SLPs in ASAH Division 2 (Neurophysiology and Neurogenic Speech and Language Disorders): 311 returned (23.2%) – 340 NPs who held joint membership in APA Divisions 40 (Clinical Neuropsychology) and 22 (Rehabilitation Psychology): 77 returned (22.9%) Highlights from Survey • While 88% of NPs practice in settings where an SLP is present, only 60% of SLPs practice in settings where a NP is present. • Many SLPs (46%) view NPs role as consultation only; Few NPs (14%) view SLPs role as consultation only. • Only 29% of SLPs view NPs as assessing language, while 100% of NPs view SLPs as assessing language. Highlights from Survey • 86% of each discipline viewed the other as assessing cognition. • The majority of NPs (>90%) viewed SLPs as treating language and cognition, while only 27% of SLPs viewed NPs as treating cognition and <1% perceived them as treating language. Highlights from Survey • Primary means of collaboration reported by both disciplines was informal consultation. • Most frequent collaborations reported were sharing assessment results and educating patients and families (still only 42% of SLPs and 51% of NPs reported often or always). • Least frequent collaborations were preassessment discussions and orienting medical staff. Highlights from Survey • 59% of SLPs refer to NP for assessment; 37% of NPs refer to SLP for assessment. • While 63% of NPs report referring to SLPs for treatment, only 23% of SLPs refer to NPs for treatment. Impaired memory is a frequently observed occurrence among patients in rehabilitation- both inpatient and outpatient. Diagnoses Commonly Seen on Rehabilitation Unit •Stroke •Traumatic Brain Injury •Anoxia •Multiple Sclerosis •Cerebral Tumors •Dementia (concommitant with deconditioning, orthopedic injuries, etc.) •Encephalitis (e.g., Herpes Simplex) Other Conditions Resulting in Memory Impairment •Epilepsy •Metabolic abnormalities (e.g., NA levels) •Nutritional disorders (e.g., B12 deficiency) •Hematologic Conditions (e.g., chronic anemia) Neuroanatomy of Memory • Temporal lobe and hippocampus important for storage of new memories and retrieval of existing memories • Frontal lobe and subcortical structures important for encoding and retrieving through their role in “executive” or “supervisory” functions (e.g., attention, organization, temporal memory) • Memory can be impacted by lesions anywhere in the brain (e.g., language issues impacting verbal memory; parietal lobe lesions impacting visual memory. Neuroanatomy of Memory • Modality specificity – Left hemisphere verbal memory] – Right hemisphere visual memory This only holds true with relatively circumscribed lesions. Furthermore, most visual memory tests include materials that can be verbalized. Memory Assessment is an Important Part of the Rehab Process • To guide implementation of treatment goals by the team (e.g., learning of strategies; assimilating safety practices) • To guide development of compensatory strategies • To guide discussions with patients and their family members regarding challenges after discharge • To serve as an anchor point for future changes Memory is assessed by multiple disciplines, in a variety of ways, both formally and informally, raising the potential for disparate messages to be communicated to patients, family members, and other rehabilitation staff. Purpose • To provide some guidelines to improve clarity and consistency with regard to the communication of memory impairments – Presentation of a theoretical model based in cognitive neuroscience – Discussion of some frequently used memory measures and their relation to the model – Presentation of a case to illustrate assessment issues and treatment implications Theoretical Model Early Stage Models • Encoding • Storage • Retrieval Encoding • Early processing of material to be learned • Involves strategies such as rehearsal and organization • Quality determines how well info is stored and later retrieved (e.g., depth of encoding, organization of material) Storage • Holding of information in the memory system for future use • Short-term store temporary unless transferred to long-term store • Encoding processes occur during short-term storage • Long-term store considered to be permanent unless disrupted by pathological process Retrieval • Pulling information from storage (long-term store) in order to use it • Delayed recall on memory tests • May be facilitated by presentation of information in recognition formats (e.g., multiple-choice; yes-no) Interaction Between Encoding, Storage, and Retrieval • Quality of encoding impacts storage and retrieval • Information is better recalled under conditions that are similar to when it was learned (context-dependent memory) • Repeated retrieval of information can increase the probability of it being retrieved at a later time Systems Models of Memory • Evolved from concerns that stage models were simplistic and could not explain complexities of memory process • Breakdowns can occur in one component of the system, while others are preserved (e.g., severe amnestics can have preserved digit span and recall of recent items, but be unable to learn new material • Memory is comprised of a set of interrelated systems and subsystems Model of Working Memory (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) Visuospatial Sketchpad Central Executive Phonological Loop Model of Working Memory (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) • Two “slave systems” serve long-term memory: phonological loop and visuo-spatial sketchpad. • The systems temporarily store information, as well as perform operations (such as rehearsal) that would maintain information and eventually transfer it to long-term memory; also holds information that has been temporarily pulled from long-term store (e.g., multiplication tables) Model of Working Memory (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) • Central executive: – Interfaces between phonological loop, visuospatial sketchpad, and long-term memory – Traditional “frontal lobe functions” – Allocates attention to different processes; chooses and carries out different activities, such as organization Model of Long-Term Memory (Tulving, 1985; Squire, 1992) Long-term Memory Store Declarative (Explicit) Semantic Episodic Non-Declarative (Implicit) Skills & Habits Priming Long-Term Memory (Tulving, 1985; Squire, 1992) • Declarative Memory – Semantic: knowledge of facts (e.g., multiplication tables, historical facts) – Episodic: knowledge regarding personal experiences (e.g., college graduation; what you had for breakfast) – Episodic memory is most typically disrupted by damage to the brain, while semantic is typically relatively preserved. Long-Term Memory (Tulving, 1985; Squire, 1992) • Non-Declarative – Implicit memory in amnestic patients (primingpreserved learning even when they cannot recall the learning episode) – Preserved learning of procedural skills and perceptual skills in amnestic patients Table 1. Testing Tasks and Their Relationship to Components of the Theoretical Memory Model SHORT-TERM STORE LONG-TERM STORE Working Memory Testing Task Visual Phonological Central Executive + + Declarative Memory List Learning Memory Supraspan Lists (>than 9 words per list) Immediate Recall (IR) Delayed Recall (DR) + + Recognition (Rec) + + Forced Choice (FC) + + Subspan Lists (<than 7 words per list; typically single presentation) Immediate Recall + + Delayed Recall + + Recognition + + Paragraph Memory Immediate Recall + + Delayed Recall + + Recognition + + Paired Associates Learning Immediate Recall + + Delayed Recall + + Recognition + + Non-declarative Memory SHORT-TERM STORE LONG-TERM STORE Working Memory Testing Task Visual Phonological Central Executive + + Declarative Memory Non-declarative Memory Digit Span Task or Serial Recall Task - Backward (verbal) Immediate Recall Picture Recall Immediate Recall + + Delayed Recall + + Recognition + + Figure Recall Immediate Recall + + Delayed Recall + + Recognition + + Digit Span Task or Serial Recall Task - Backward (visual) Immediate Recall + + Procedural Memory + Visual-Auditory Learning Immediate Recall Delayed Recall + + + + + Case Study Background • • • • • • • • 58 year-old, right-handed, Hispanic female 3 years of education Sustained a right subcortical stroke Symptom presentation: left hemiparesis and mild left inattention Employment history: housewife for most of her adult life Psychiatric history: none Substance abuse history: none Learning disability history: none Neuroimaging Findings Intracranial hemorrhage in the right internal capsule (part of the basal ganglia) Memory Tests Administered • Ross Information Processing Assessment-2 • Digit Span (Forward and Backward) from WAIS-III • California Verbal Learning Test-2 • Logical Memory I & II from WMS-III • Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure TestImmediate and Delayed Recall Test Results • RIPA-II – Within normal limits on items assessing orientation, memory for recent events (e.g., “What is the first thing you did this morning?) and memory for remotely learned information (e.g., “In what month is Christmas?”) – Correctly repeated 6 digits in forward sequence – Repeated a 15-word sentence – Couldn’t repeat a more complex sentence with 3 ideas – Recalled 2 of 3 words after a 10-minute delay Test Results CVLT-2 •Intrusion errors on most trials •Benefited somewhat from semantic cueing based on category •Auditory recognition impaired due to a high number of false alarm errors 16 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 Trial 5 List B Immediate Recall Delayed Recall Test Results • Logical Memory – Within normal limits for number of details recalled for immediate and 30-minute delayed recall – Qualitatively, she recalled details in a piecemeal, disorganized fashion • Rey-Osterrieth Figure – Impaired (partially due to impairment of copy secondary to left neglect) • Digit Span – Forward=6; Backward=3 Behavioral Observations • • • • Distractibility Motor restlessness Impulsive responding Reduced awareness of errors Conclusions • Immediate attention was within normal limits • Working memory impaired • Problems with organization and selective attention (screening out irrelevant information) resulted in impaired learning and recall) • May recall details, but may recall them out of sequence, resulting in errors on everyday tasks (e.g., medication management) Functional Recommendations • • • • • Supervision for most of each day Assistance with making important decisions Home safety evaluation Supervision for medication management Restriction from using potentially dangerous appliances • Cueing by family members to reduce impulsive behavior • Training in compensatory organizational and memory strategies Discussion Points • Memory was sufficient for functional communication skills. • Use of screening measures alone (e.g., RIPA-II) would have overestimated the patient’s memory abilities. • Use of raw scores and percentiles alone would have underestimated functional problems (importance of qualitative analysis and behavioral observations) Relation to Theoretical Model • Able to access information in the long-term store relatively well – Semantic (“In what month is Christmas?”_ – Episodic (what she did yesterday or what she has for breakfast)- encoded in an organized way with personal meaning/significance • Impaired working memory • Impairment in Central Executive system (organization and selective attention) led to trouble encoding information in a way that would enhance recall) Relation to Theoretical Model • Able to recall sentences and stories because they were organized in a manner that allowed for ease of encoding in the episodic store • Unable to impose organization on unstructured material, like word lists • Impairment in allocation of attention by Central Executive system led to false positive errors during auditory recognition memory performance