“To declare that in the administration of the criminal law the end justifies the means … would bring terrible retribution. Against that pernicious doctrine this Court should resolutely set its face.” Justice Louis D. Brandeis, dissenting in Olmstead v. United States, 277 U.S. 438, 485 (1928) “What happens behind doors that are opened and closed at the sole discretion of the police is a black chapter in every country – the free as well as the despotic, the modern as well as the ancient.” Justice William O. Douglas (the longest-serving Supreme Court Justice in U.S. history), concurring in United States v. Carignan, 342 U.S. 36, 46 (1951) “It is a fair summary of history to say that the safeguards of liberty have frequently been forged in controversies involving not very nice people.” Justice Felix Frankfurter, dissenting in United States v. Rabinowitz, 339 U.S. 56, 69 (1950) “If a person is innocent of a crime, then he is not a suspect.” then Attorney General Edwin Meese explaining to the American Bar Association why the Miranda decision enabling those arrested to be advised of their rights was not necessary anymore (From The 776 Stupidest Things Ever Said (p. 100), by R. Petras and K. Petras, 1993, New York: Doubleday.) Clinical Forensic Psychology in Cases involving Disputed Confessions Referral question: To what legal question(s) will your data and opinions be applied? 1. Did the State fail to prove, by a preponderance of the evidence, that the defendant knowingly, intelligently, and voluntarily waived his or her Miranda rights? 2. Did the State fail to prove, by a preponderance of the evidence, that the defendant’s supposed confession was freely and voluntarily made under the totality of the circumstances? 3. Should the Court suppress the defendant’s coerced statements to the police because they are so highly unreliable and virtually uncorroborated? Legal Question #3 Operationalized 3 (a) Did the interrogators use techniques of a type that have contributed to known false confessions in other cases? Did the interrogators use • coercion? • deception? • etc.? 3 (b) Did the interrogators use techniques likely to produce a reliable confession statement? Did the interrogators • record the entire interrogation? • scrupulously avoid contaminating the suspect’s mind? • elicit a detailed postadmission narrative? 3 (c) Does the suspect’s confession statement show guilty knowledge? Each of these three legal questions can be subjected to a person-situation analysis: Who is the person? What are his strengths and weaknesses; his knowledge, skills and abilities? What was the person’s mental and physical state at the relevant time • warning and waiver? • during police pressure? • while confessing? What was the setting – the physical and psychological characteristics of the scene where the interrogation and confession occurred? What techniques did the police use to influence the suspect? Consider the likely impact of police pressure in that setting on that suspect. Clinical Assessment of the Suspect cum Defendant Some psychologists, by virtue of their knowledge, training, and experience, are able to assist the court in each of the following areas: 1. Gather and analyze information regarding “the physical and psychological environment in which the [waiver] was obtained” (Crane v. Kentucky, 1986, p. 684). 2. Assess the defendant’s current mental status, including intelligence, memory, reading comprehension, listening comprehension, and psychopathology. 3. Reconstruct the defendant’s mental state at the time of the waiver (similar to the type of assessment in insanity and other mental-state-at-thetime-of-the-offense evaluations; see, e.g., Rogers & Shuman, 2000). 4. Assist the judge in understanding interactions among the above. For suggestions/guidance on how to conduct the clinical assessment in disputed confession cases, see … DeClue, G. (2005). Interrogations and Disputed Confessions: A Manual for Forensic Psychological Practice. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press. See especially: • Chapter 8 • Assessment Procedures (begins p. 147) • Soddi Jones Sample Report (appendix) What about interrogations by military and intelligence agencies? http://kspope.com/interrogation/index.php False Confessions Introduction Nutshell Some people falsely confess to some crimes some times. Some of those people begin by denying guilt, then during police interrogation say, “I did it,” then afterwards say, “No, I didn’t.” U. S. courts provide two opportunities to challenge disputed confessions. At a suppression hearing the defense can present evidence challenging the voluntariness of the confession and/or whether the defendant gave a knowing, intelligent, and voluntary waiver of his Constitutional rights prior to interrogation. At trial the defense can present evidence challenging the accuracy of the confession. At both stages psychologists are called upon to present expert testimony, either at the request of the defense or the prosecution. Importance of DNA Evidence DNA profiling can identify the guilty and exonerate the innocent 9 a.m. Monday September 15, 1984 Sir Alec Jeffreys He used DNA profiling to establish the identity of a man who raped and killed two 15year-old girls, three years apart, near the village of Narborough, in England. The police collected blood from all the adult males in and around Narborough and Jeffreys successfully identified the culprit. Colin Pitchfork became the first criminal ever apprehended on the basis of DNA profiling When the local police contacted Jeffreys to consult on the Narborough case, they had already arrested a 17year-old kitchen assistant who had a record of minor sex offenses. During their interrogation of him he confessed to one of the murders, but not the other. Jeffreys’ analysis showed that the same man had committed both rapes, but it was not the man who had confessed. “The police subsequently dropped the case against that man,” says Sir Alec, “and he became the first person ever proven innocent by DNA analysis. If we hadn’t developed the technology, I’m confident he would have been gaoled [jailed] for life.” In 1996, the FBI reported that in sexual assault cases in which DNA results could be obtained, the DNA evidence exonerated the primary suspect in 25% of the cases. These days, the use of DNA profiling sees 30 per cent of accused in British rape cases exonerated. Limitations of DNA Evidence Although DNA testing can allow some falsely accused – and some falsely convicted – people to prove their innocence, in most cases there is no biological evidence to test, or none suitable for DNA testing. It is therefore critical to study cases where innocent people have been convicted and to identify what errors led to the wrongful convictions, so that the risk of such errors can be minimized in the future. Psychological Studies of Interrogations and Confessions The psychological study of interrogations and confessions is nearly 100 years old: Hugo Munsterberg (1908). On the Witness Stand. Garden City, New York: Doubleday. Drizin and Leo (2004): “a rich literature and body of knowledge regarding interrogations and confessions.” laboratory experimentation, for example: Kassin, S. & Neumann, K. (1997). On the power of confession evidence: An experimental test of the fundamental difference hypothesis. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 469-484. participant observation, for example: Leo, R. (1996). Inside the interrogation room. Journal of criminal law and criminology, 86, 266-303. interviews, for example: Leo, R. Police interrogation in America: A Study of Violence, Civility, and Social Change. (1994). Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, on file with the North Carolina Law Review. analyses of archival and documentary records, for example: Ofshe, R. & Leo, R. (1997). The decision to confess falsely: Rational choice and irrational action. Denver University Law Review, 74, 9791122. surveys, for example: Leo, R. A., Kassin, S. M., Richman, K. D., Colwell, L. H., Leach, A., La Fon, D., & Meissner, C. Police Interviewing and Interrogation: Toward a National Self-Report Survey of Police Practices & Beliefs. American Psychology-Law Society, March 2006. Frequency of False Confessions in Cases of Wrongful Conviction Bedau and colleagues: 416 cases of wrongful conviction • capital or potentially capital crimes • 40% were sentenced to death Bedau, H. A. & Radelet, M. L. (1987). Miscarriages of justice in potentially capital cases. Stanford Law Review, 41, 21-179. Causes of these miscarriages of justice: • perjury by prosecution witnesses • mistaken eyewitness testimony • false confessions (14%) Subsequent studies have found the percentage of false confessions in the miscarriages-of-justice studies to be even higher (Drizin & Leo, 2004). The Percentage of False Confessions in Prior Studies of Wrongful Convictions Authors and Year Number of False Confessions Percentage of Cases Studied Bedau & Radelet (1987) 49/350 14% Connors et al. (1996) 5/28 18% Scheck, Neufeld, & Dwyer (2000) 15/62 24% Innocence Project (2003) 35/140 25% A study of 125 cases of proven false confessions Drizin, S. & Leo, R. A. (2004). The Problem of False Confessions in the PostDNA World. North Carolina Law Review, 3, 891-1008. How do we know that the wrong person confessed? • crime never happened • physically impossible • true perpetrator identified • scientific evidence, e.g., DNA Type of Exoneration Persons Exonerated Page in Leo & Drizin (2004) No Crime Occurred Dianne Tucker, Medell Banks, Victoria Banks 925 Physical Impossibility Mario Hayes, Miguel Castillo, Peter Williams 925-926 Scientific Evidence Christopher Ochoa 926 True Perpetrator Identified Michael Crowe, Joshua Treadway, Aaron Houser 926 Drizin & Leo (2004) Table 11 Source of Exoneration (N = 125) Type of Exoneration Number of People Percent No Crime Occurred 8 6% Physical Impossibility 11 9% Scientific Evidence 57 46% True Perpetrator Identified 92 74% Age of Proven False Confessors Age Range Number of people percent < 10 2 2% 10-13 5 4% 14-15 15 13% 16-17 18 16% 18-24 31 27% 25-39 34 30% 40-54 7 6% 55 1 1% All Reported Crimes to Which Person Confessed Crime Number of people Percent Murder 101 81% Rape 33 26% Robbery 23 18% Arson 7 6% Assault 4 3% Kidnapping 2 2% Theft 2 2% Routine Police Interrogations • more than 90% last less than two hours • Leo, R. (1996). Inside the interrogation room. Journal of criminal law and criminology, 86, 266-303. In contrast, interrogation-induced false confessions • lengthy interrogations • innocent suspect’s resistance is worn down • coercive techniques • suspect is made to feel hopeless, regardless of innocence Fate of False Confessors (N = 125) Fate Number of people Percent Never Charged 10 8% Charges Dropped 64 51% Acquitted at Trial 7 6% Pled Guilty 14 11% Convicted at Trial 30 24% Risk of a Miscarriage Attributable to a False Confession (N = 125) Outcome Number Released Prior to Decision 74 (59%) Pled Guilty 14 (11%) Acquitted at Trial Convicted at Trial Totals 7 (6%) Risk of a Risk of ConMiscarriage viction at Trial 11% 19% 30 (24%) 24% 125 (100%) 35% 81% Drizin & Leo (2004) Conclusions and Recommendations “Modern psychological interrogation techniques can cause innocent suspects to confess.” Found Guilty: • 81% of the innocent defendants who chose to take their case to trial • 86% of the innocent defendants whose charges were not dropped/confessions were not suppressed Jurors often place almost blind faith in the evidentiary value of confession evidence – even when • no credible corroboration • compelling evidence of the defendant’s factual innocence Recommendations Regarding Interrogations: Electronically record the entire interrogation 3 reasons: First: an objective, comprehensive, and reviewable record of the interrogation This avoids “swearing contests.” The fact-finding process becomes more accurate and reliable. Second: a higher level of scrutiny to deter police misconduct This increases the ability of police to separate the innocent from the guilty. Third, opportunity to identify: • “guilty knowledge” • contamination • coercion The Causes of Police-Induced False Confessions Why do Suspects Confess? “For both innocent and guilty suspects, confessing is something neither would have chosen to do prior to the start of the interrogation and something each would have predicted he would have resisted to his last breath” (Ofshe & Leo, 1997, p. 194). Nevertheless, 40% to 70% of interrogated suspects do confess. Gudjonsson: “The suspects’ behavior during the interrogation is likely to be more influenced by their perceptions, interpretations, and assumptions about what is happening than by the actual behavior of the police. … Gudjonsson: “When the suspect perceives the evidence against him as being strong he is more likely to confess, believing that there is no point in denying the offense.” … For both innocent and guilty suspects, confessing is something neither would have chosen to do prior to the start of the interrogation and something each would have predicted he would have resisted to his last breath” (Ofshe & Leo, 1997, p. 194). How Interrogators Get Suspects to Confess Accusatory interrogation proceeds systematically with one goal: to obtain a confession from whomever has been selected as a suspect. Modern American interrogations employ psychological techniques that “produce false confessions because they convince innocent suspects that their situations are hopeless just as surely as they convince the guilty that they are caught.” (Ofshe & Leo, 1997, pp. 194-195). When there is solid evidence linking the suspect to the crime: • use that evidence • along with additional, fabricated evidence • to persuade the suspect that there is an airtight case. When solid evidence is lacking: • deception • interpersonal dominance Ofshe and Leo (1997) describe two phases of interrogation • pre-admission phase – voluntariness • post-admission narrative – reliability Three steps in the preadmission phase • the decision to allow questioning • shifting the suspect from confident to despairing • eliciting the confession A skilled interrogator will • carefully conserve his supply of real evidence • introduce something new in response to each objection “airtight case” What effect does this process have for an innocent suspect? “The interrogator will reject the innocent suspect’s denial just as he would if it were made by a guilty suspect—by expressing great confidence in the reliability of the evidence, assuring the suspect that no mistake is possible.” (Ofshe & Leo, 1997, p. 201) When the interrogator perceives that the suspect has reached a point of hopelessness: eliciting the admission Ready to crack? offers reasons why an admission would be to the suspect’s advantage Generally, police use whatever incentives they believe will • get the suspect to confess • and be allowed by the courts Incentives to confess: • doing the right thing • saving face • showing empathy for the victim or the victim’s family • maintaining the good will of the police • showing remorse to look good for the prosecutor, judge, or jury • avoiding a harsh sentence such as a lengthy prison sentence or the death penalty Themes that allow the suspect to account for some of the “facts” without accepting much, if any, responsibility. An ever-popular theme that police suggest: • It was an accident - or • It wasn’t premeditated Eliciting the admission via the accident theme: “We know you did it. The evidence proves that.” “All I’m really interested in is why you did it.” - or “All I’m interested in knowing is whether you planned to do this or whether it was an accident.” The accident technique suggests a version of the facts that • would reduce the moral seriousness of the offense • would drastically lower the charge • would result in a decreased sentence, if any sentence at all Summary regarding the preadmission phase: • Social scientists see some common interrogation techniques as being inherently coercive. • The same techniques that facilitate confessions from guilty suspects can contribute to false confessions from innocent suspects. Post-admission Narrative Social scientists who have analyzed interrogations report that there are no reliable, observable differences between interrogations yielding true or false confessions until after the “I did it” statement. Therefore, police interrogation should never end at the point when the police believe the suspect has made admissions allowing him to be charged with the most serious offense possible. In the next step, the postadmission narrative, interrogators elicit detailed descriptions of events. If the suspect provides accurate details showing special knowledge of the details of the crime, then the confession can be judged as reliable. If the suspect’s postadmission narrative does not match the facts of the case, the reliability of the confession is in doubt. Three ways to determine the reliability of the confession: 1. Does the confession statement lead to the discovery of evidence that is unknown to the police? 2. Does the confession statement include highly unusual features of the crime that have not been made public and have not been told to the suspect during the interrogation? 3. Does the suspect provide accurate descriptions of the mundane crime scene detail, which have not been told to the suspect and have not been made public? Analysis of the details in the post-admission narrative can guide the police whether to • focus their investigation on this suspect or • keep looking for the truly guilty party Every police interrogator in every case should obtain a detailed post-admission narrative. To avoid – actually, to recognize – contamination, the entire interrogation should be electronically recorded. Types of False Confessions Three types of false confessions: 1) voluntary 2) coerced-compliant 3) coerced-internalized Kassin and Wrightsman (1985) voluntary false confessions those purposely offered in the absence of elicitation Lindbergh baby Colorado v. Connelly (1986) coerced-compliant false confessions • The suspect publicly professes guilt in response to extreme methods of interrogation • despite knowing privately that he or she is truly innocent. coerced-internalized false confession – through the fatigue, pressures, and suggestiveness of the interrogation process – actually comes to believe that he or she committed the offense. coerced-internalized false confession dangerous processes: • fatigue • pressures • suggestive questioning coerced-internalized false confession vulnerable suspects: • young • tired • confused • suggestible • exposed to false information coerced-internalized false confession NEVER, NEVER, NEVER: • suggest suspect did it even though he has no such memory • exaggerate/fabricate evidence • provide crime-scene details • suggest internal thoughts, feelings, motivations … coerced-internalized false confession NEVER, NEVER, NEVER (cont.): • show crime-scene photos • exaggerate/fabricate evidence • ask suspect to explain fake evidence • imply leniency for “accident” or “unintended” act coerced-internalized false confession NEVER, NEVER, NEVER: • especially when there is no independent evidence (physical, eyewitness) implicating this suspect in this crime coerced-internalized false confession linguistic chronology: • “I didn’t do it.” • “I know I didn’t do it.” • “But the evidence you’ve told me about means I must have done it.” … coerced-internalized false confession linguistic chronology (cont.): • “But I don’t remember doing it.” • “But I must have done it.” • “Maybe I did do it, but I don’t remember doing it.” … coerced-internalized false confession linguistic chronology (cont.): • “I really don’t think I did do it, but if I did I didn’t mean to.” • “I guess I must have done it.” • “I think I did it like this …” A classic case: 18-year-old Peter Reilly of Falls Village, Connecticut, returned home one night to find that his mother had been murdered. Reilly immediately called the police but he was suspected of matricide. After gaining Reilly's trust, the police used fabricated evidence (a “failed” polygraph test) to convince him that he was guilty even though he had no conscious memory of the event. After hours of interrogation, the audiotape reveals that Reilly underwent a chilling transformation from denial to confusion, self-doubt, conversion ("Well, it really looks like I did it") … … and finally a full confession: "I remember slashing once at my mother's throat with a straight razor I used for model airplanes. ... I also remember jumping on my mother's legs." Two years later independent evidence revealed that Reilly could not possibly have committed the murder. - Kassin & Gudjonsson (June 2005) True Crimes, False Confessions: Why Do Innocent People Confess to Crimes They Did Not Commit? Scientific American Mind. DeClue (2005): self-initiated first response police induced Self-initiated confessions occur when a person initiates contact with a law enforcement officer or other person in authority and declares that he or she is guilty of a crime. First-response confessions occur when the police approach a person and initiate questioning, and the person’s first response is “I did it.” Police-induced confessions occur when the police approach a person and initiate questioning, the person’s first response is something other than “I did it,” (for example, “I didn’t do it”), the police engage in further conversation with the person, and the person subsequently says, “I did it.” This classification scheme avoids legally-charged words, and it is not necessary to delve into the minds of the confessor or the police in order to classify a confession. ?? Who said ?? “Those who cannot remember the past are condemned to repeat it.” In Hopt v. Territory of Utah, 110 U.S. 574 (1884), the U.S. Supreme Court held: A confession, if freely and voluntarily made, is evidence of the most satisfactory character. Such a confession is deserving of the highest credit, because it is presumed to flow from the strongest sense of guilt, and therefore it is admitted as proof of the crime to which it refers. . . . But the presumption upon which weight is given to such evidence, namely, that one who is innocent will not imperil his safety or prejudice his interests by an untrue statement, ceases when the confession appears to have been made [in response to] inducements, threats, or promises. (pp. 584-585, citation omitted) In Brown v. Mississippi, 297 U.S. 278 (1936), the undisputed facts of the case showed that the local sheriff, accompanied by some other men, took one of the defendants to the house of a recently deceased man, and there … “began to accuse the defendant of the crime. Upon his denial they seized him, and with the participation of the deputy they hanged him by a rope to the limb of a tree, and, having let him down, they hung him again, … and when he was let down the second time, and he still protested his innocence, he was tied to a tree and whipped, and, still declining to accede to the demands that he confess, … he was finally released, and he returned with some difficulty to his home, suffering intense pain and agony. The record of the testimony shows that the signs of the rope on his neck were plainly visible during the so-called trial.” (p. 281) The U.S. Supreme Court in Brown ruled that confessions elicited via physical brutality and violence by the police could not be used as evidence. In Chambers v. Florida, 309 U.S. 227 (1940), the facts and findings included: “For five days petitioners were subjected to interrogations culminating in Saturday’s (May 20th) all night examination. … Over a period of five days they steadily refused to confess and disclaimed any guilt. The very circumstances surrounding their confinement and their questioning without any formal charges having been brought, were such as to fill petitioners with terror and frightful misgivings. . . . The haunting fear of mob violence was around them in an atmosphere charged with excitement and public indignation. . . . To permit human lives to be forfeited upon confessions thus obtained would make of the constitutional requirement of due process of law a meaningless symbol” (pp. 239-240). The Court in Chambers noted that “They who have suffered most from secret and dictatorial proceedings have almost always been the poor, the ignorant, the numerically weak, the friendless, and the powerless” (p. 238). December 26, 2007 Boise, Idaho "Don't suffer from PTSD, go out and cause it.” Officers stand by with their armored vehicle in Moscow, Idaho. The academy's director said the class president was ex-military. Arraignment One of your constitutional "due process" rights is the right to be told exactly what you're being accused of doing. That happens at an arraignment ("uh-rainment") - the judge reads the charges filed against you and asks you if you understand what you've been accused of doing. But where does arraignment come from? McNabb v. United States, 318 U.S. 332 (1943) involved members of a clan of Tennessee mountaineers who were in the business of selling whiskey on which federal taxes had not been paid, shots fired in the dark, and a federal agent who died in the McNabb Settlement’s cemetery. The interrogation involved confinement in a “barren cell. . . . For two days they were subjected to unremitting questioning by numerous officers. Benjamin’s confession was secured by detaining him unlawfully and questioning him continuously for five or six hours. The McNabbs had to submit to all this without the aid of friends or the benefit of counsel” (p. 345). Three of the clan confessed. The voluntariness of their confessions was questionable on grounds that it was “secured by protracted and repeated questioning of ignorant and untutored persons in whose minds the power of officers was greatly magnified or who have been unlawfully held incommunicado without advice of friends or counsel” (p. 340). Instead, the confessions were excluded because the federal officers had not promptly taken them to a judicial officer for a hearing, a remedy available to the Court because it was a federal case. The McNabb Court explained the need as follows: “A democratic society, in which respect for the dignity of all men is central, naturally guards against the misuse of the law enforcement process. Zeal in tracking down crime is not in itself an assurance of soberness of judgment. … Disinterestedness in law enforcement does not alone prevent disregard of cherished liberties. Experience has therefore counseled that safeguards must be provided against the dangers of the overzealous as well as the despotic. … The awful instruments of the criminal law cannot be entrusted to a single functionary. The complicated process of criminal justice is therefore divided into different parts, responsibility for which is separately vested in the various participants upon whom the criminal law relies for its vindication. … Legislation such as this, requiring that the police must with reasonable promptness show legal cause for detaining arrested persons, constitutes an important safeguard – not only in assuring protection for the innocent but also in securing conviction of the guilty by methods that commend themselves to a progressive and self-confident society. … For this procedural requirement checks resort to those reprehensible practices known as the “third degree” which, though universally rejected as indefensible, still find their way into use. It aims to avoid all the evil implications of secret interrogation of persons accused of crime. … It reflects not a sentimental but a sturdy view of law enforcement. It outlaws easy but self-defeating ways in which brutality is substituted for brains as an instrument of crime detection (pp. 343-344). June 23, 2003 “Waiver of Arraignment” In Ashcraft v. Tennessee, 322 U.S. 143 (1944), a suspect had been questioned for 36 hours by a team of interrogators. The interrogators had to work in shifts, taking breaks due to their exhaustion, but the pressure on the suspect was unrelenting. The Court noted that there were “disputed questions of fact relating to the details of what transpired within the confession chamber of the jail or whether Ashcraft actually did confess. Such disputes, we may say, are an inescapable consequence of secret inquisitorial practices. … And always evidence concerning the inner details of secret inquisitions is weighted against an accused, particularly where, as here, he is charged with a brutal crime, or where, as in many other cases, his supposed offense bears relation to an unpopular economic, political, or religious cause. . . . We think a situation such as that here shown by uncontradicted evidence is so inherently coercive that its very existence is irreconcilable with the possession of mental freedom by a lone suspect against whom its full coercive force is brought to bear” (pp. 152-153). The dissent in Ashcraft noted “A confession is wholly and incontestably voluntary only if a guilty person gives himself up to the law and becomes his own accuser. The Court bases its decision on the premise that custody and examination of a prisoner for thirtysix hours is ‘inherently coercive.’ … Of course it is. And so is custody and examination for one hour. Arrest itself is inherently coercive, and so is detention. When not justified, infliction of such indignities upon the person is actionable as a tort. Of course such acts put pressure upon the prisoner to answer questions, to answer them truthfully, and to confess if guilty” (pp. 160-161). A Mississippi court held that a defendant who denied that he made a confession was therefore barred from challenging that the disputed confession was coerced. U.S. Supreme Court: “Nah.” Lee v. Mississippi, 332 U.S. 742 (1948). Upshaw v. United States, 335 U.S. 410 (1948): Rather than take Upshaw for arraignment as the law required, the police held him for 30 hours. The purpose for the illegal detention “as stated by the officers themselves, was only to furnish an opportunity for further interrogation” (p. 412). In a 5-4 decision, the majority in Upshaw held that “a confession is inadmissible if made during illegal detention due to failure promptly to carry a prisoner before a committing magistrate, whether or not the confession is the result of torture, physical or psychological” (p. 413, citation omitted). Thus, in 1948 this was a controversial, 5-4 decision: Federal courts cannot admit confessions that were obtained illegally. In Watts v. Indiana, 338 U.S. 49 (1949), the Court ruled involuntary a confession that followed 6 nights of relentless interrogation. The Court reasoned: “To turn the detention of an accused into a process of wrenching from him evidence which could not be extorted in open court with all its safeguards, … is so grave an abuse of the power of arrest as to offend the procedural standards of due process. . . . This is so because it violates the underlying principle in our enforcement of the criminal law. Ours is the accusatorial as opposed to the inquisitorial system. … Such has been the characteristic of Anglo-American criminal justice since it freed itself from practices borrowed by the Star Chamber from the Continent whereby an accused was interrogated in secret for hours on end. Under our system society carries the burden of proving its charge against the accused not out of his own mouth. … [Society] must establish its case, not by interrogation of the accused even under judicial safeguards, but by evidence independently secured through skillful investigation” (p. 54, citations omitted). United States v. Carignan, 342 U.S. 36 (1951) “What happens behind doors that are opened and closed at the sole discretion of the police is a black chapter in every country – the free as well as the despotic, the modern as well as the ancient.” Justice William O. Douglas (the longest-serving Supreme Court Justice in U.S. history), concurring opinion Gallegos v. Nebraska, 342 U.S. 55 (1951) “He was kept in the Texas jail. His detention was incommunicado except for repeated questioning by the deputies. … As is usual in this type of case the deputies say that the confession was wholly ‘voluntary’; petitioner says that it was due to fear engendered by his incarceration and the actions of the deputies” (p. 74). The interrogator, the Chief Deputy Sheriff, said he used no violence, threat of violence, or promises. The Court considered only undisputed evidence, and ruled the confession voluntary. Stroble v. California, 343 U.S. 181 (1952) The entire proceeding was recorded on a recording machine which had been set in operation before petitioner’s arrival. Petitioner stated that on the afternoon of November 14, his victim came to the home of petitioner’s daughter, where petitioner was visiting; … he took his victim into the bedroom and made advances upon her; when she began to scream, he became frightened, got hold of her throat, and squeezed it until she became quiet; she started to squirm again, so he took a necktie from the dresser and tied it around her neck; … when she continued to move, he took her off the bed, wrapped her in a blanket, and hit her on the temple with a hammer which he had obtained from the kitchen drawer; he then dragged her across the back yard to the incinerator, returned to the kitchen to get an ice pick, and pushed the pick into her three times in an effort to reach her heart; … next he got an axe from the garage and hit her on the head and backbone; finally he got a knife from the kitchen and stabbed her in the back of the neck, covered her body with boxes, and left for Ocean Park, a beach resort within the city of Los Angeles, where he remained for the three nights before his apprehension. … Towards the end of the recording petitioner stated that the officers had not threatened or abused him in any way, either in the park foreman’s office or the District Attorney’s office. The recording disclosed no mistreatment at the time of the making of the confession (pp. 186-187).” 2 confessions: “The fact that the accused started talking shortly after he was arrested and prior to the time he was taken before the District Attorney does not save the case. … That talk was accompanied or preceded by blows and kicks of the police; and the Supreme Court of California assumed that it was part and parcel of the first confession obtained through ‘physical abuse or psychological torture or a combination of the two’” (p. 204, citation omitted). (But that’s from the dissent.) Stein v. New York, 346 U.S. 156 (1953) Three men confessed to participation in a murder related to the hijacking of a Reader’s Digest truck near Pleasantville. The three were examined by a prison physician on the morning of June 9, after they had been in custody for 2 to 4 days: “Wissner had a broken rib and various bruises and abrasions on the side, legs, stomach and buttocks; Cooper had bruises on the chest, stomach, right arm, and both buttocks; Stein had a bruise on his right arm. … Counsel for the petitioners, who examined them on the 9th and 10th of June, testified that the injuries sustained by each were more extensive than those described in the doctor’s testimony” (pp. 169170). In preparation for trial, they were faced with a difficult choice. If they testified that their confessions had been coerced, they would be subject to cross-examination that could reveal their prior criminal records. They did not testify at trial. Therefore, on appeal the Court saw no basis to consider that their injuries were the results of police action. In considering whether psychological coercion had been applied, the Court identified the proper test as this: “The limits in any case depend upon a weighing of the circumstances of pressure against the power of resistance of the person confessing. What would be overpowering to the weak of will or mind might be utterly ineffective against an experienced criminal” (p. 185). Has this ever happened to you? Leyra v. Denno, 347 U.S. 556 (1954) A suspect complained of pain and asked for a doctor to treat him for a medical condition. In response to his request, he was introduced to a state-appointed psychiatrist with considerable knowledge of hypnosis. “Instead of giving petitioner the medical advice and treatment he expected, the psychiatrist by subtle and suggestive questions simply continued the police effort of the past days and nights to induce petitioner to admit his guilt. For an hour and a half or more the techniques of a highly trained psychiatrist were used to break petitioner’s will in order to get him to say he had murdered his parents. … Time and time and time again the psychiatrist told petitioner how much he wanted to and could help him, how bad it would be for petitioner if he did not confess, and how much better he would feel, and how much lighter and easier it would be on him if he would just unbosom himself to the doctor. … Yet the doctor was at that very time the paid representative of the state whose prosecuting officials were listening in on every threat made and every promise of leniency given” (pp. 559-560). What’s wrong with that? Greenberg, S. A. & Shuman, D. W. (1997). Irreconcilable conflict between therapeutic and forensic roles. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 28, 50-57. Any relationship to current ethical issues? The Court ruled that Leyra’s statements to the psychiatrist and his subsequent statements (within about 5 hours) to police and to a business associate, should all be considered involuntary. Opper v. United States, 348 U.S. 84 (1954) Smith v. United States, 348 U.S. 147 (1954) “An uncorroborated confession is insufficient to convict someone because the doubt persists that the zeal of the agencies of prosecution to protect the peace, the self-interest of the accomplice, the maliciousness of an enemy or the aberration or weakness of the accused under the strain of suspicion may tinge or warp the facts of the confession” (Opper, pp. 89-90). In Crooker v. California, 357 U.S. 433 (1958), by a 5-4 vote, the Court allowed a confession to be admitted even though the defendant had asked over and over to have his attorney present, and was denied. On the same day, the Court considered similar facts in Cicenia v. Lagay, 357 U.S. 504 (1958) and reached a similar decision. Spano v. New York, 360 U.S. 315 (1959). The Court described its role in disputed confession cases: “We are forced to resolve a conflict between two fundamental interests of society; its interest in prompt and efficient law enforcement, and its interest in preventing the rights of its individual members from being abridged by unconstitutional methods of law enforcement” (p. 315). The police were not therefore merely trying to solve a crime, or even to absolve a suspect. . . . They were rather concerned primarily with securing a statement from defendant on which they could convict him. … The undeviating intent of the officers to extract a confession from petitioner is therefore patent. When such an intent is shown, this Court has held that the confession obtained must be examined with the most careful scrutiny” (321-324). In Rogers v. Richmond, 365 U.S. 534 (1961), the Court ruled that it was a violation of the Fourteenth Amendment for a state court to consider whether a confession was true as an element of whether it was voluntary. Gideon v. Wainwright, 372 U.S. 335 (1963) Every person accused of a crime is entitled to a lawyer at trial. Escobedo v. Illinois, 378 U.S. 478 (1964) “We have learned the lesson of history, ancient and modern, that a system of criminal law enforcement which comes to depend on the “confession” will, in the long run, be less reliable and more subject to abuses than a system which depends on extrinsic evidence independently secured through skillful investigation. . . . We have also learned the companion lesson of history that no system of criminal justice can, or should, survive if it comes to depend for its continued effectiveness on the citizens’ abdication through unawareness of their constitutional rights. … No system worth preserving should have to fear that if an accused is permitted to consult with a lawyer, he will become aware of, and exercise, these rights. If the exercise of constitutional rights will thwart the effectiveness of a system of law enforcement, then there is something very wrong with that system” (pp. 488-489). “We hold, therefore, that where, as here, the investigation is no longer a general inquiry into an unsolved crime but has begun to focus on a particular suspect, the suspect has been taken into police custody, the police carry out a process of interrogations that lends itself to eliciting incriminating statements, … the suspect has requested and been denied an opportunity to consult with his lawyer, and the police have not effectively warned him of his absolute constitutional right to remain silent, … the accused has been denied the Assistance of Counsel in violation of the Sixth Amendment to the Constitution as made obligatory upon the States by the Fourteenth Amendment, … and that no statement elicited by the police during the interrogation may be used against him at a criminal trial” (p. 491, citation omitted). “We hold only that when the process shifts from investigatory to accusatory – when its focus is on the accused and its purpose is to elicit a confession – our adversary system begins to operate, and, under the circumstances here, the accused must be permitted to consult with his lawyer” (p. 492). Miranda v. Arizona, 384 U.S. 436 (1966) The Court noted that, “the modern practice of in-custody interrogation is psychologically, rather than physically, oriented” and “recognized that coercion can be mental as well as physical” (p. 448). The Court noted that “Interrogation still takes place in privacy. Privacy results in secrecy, and this, in turn, results in a gap in our knowledge as to what, in fact, goes on in the interrogation rooms” (p. 448). The current practice of incommunicado interrogation is at odds with one of our Nation’s most cherished principles – that the individual may not be compelled to incriminate himself. Unless adequate protective devices are employed to dispel the compulsion inherent in custodial surroundings, no statement obtained from the defendant can truly be the product of his free choice” (pp. 457-458). “In sum, the privilege is fulfilled only when the person is guaranteed the right ‘to remain silent unless he chooses to speak in the unfettered exercise of his own will.’” (p. 460, citations omitted) The Court ruled that these rights must be afforded people in police custody, not just at court hearings and trials, because “As a practical matter, the compulsion to speak in the isolated setting of the police station may well be greater than in courts or other official investigations, where there are often impartial observers to guard against intimidation or trickery” (p. 461). “We deal with the admissibility of statements obtained from an individual who is subjected to custodial police interrogation and the necessity for procedures which assure that the individual is accorded his privilege under the Fifth Amendment to the Constitution not to be compelled to incriminate himself” (p. 439). The Court recognized “precious rights [that] were fixed in our Constitution only after centuries of persecution and struggle” (p. 442): “that ‘No person . . . shall be compelled in any criminal case to be a witness against himself,’ and that ‘the accused shall . . . have the Assistance of Counsel’” (p. 442). The Court held: “The prosecution may not use statements, whether exculpatory or inculpatory, stemming from custodial interrogation of the defendant unless it demonstrates the use of procedural safeguards effective to secure the privilege against self-incrimination. … By custodial interrogation, we mean questioning initiated by law enforcement officers after a person has been taken into custody or otherwise deprived of his freedom of action in any significant way. … As for the procedural safeguards to be employed, unless other fully effective means are devised to inform accused persons of their right of silence and to assure a continuous opportunity to exercise it, the following measures are required. … Prior to any questioning, the person must be warned that he has a right to remain silent, that any statement he does make may be used as evidence against him, and that he has a right to the presence of an attorney, either retained or appointed. … The defendant may waive effectuation of these rights, provided the waiver is made voluntarily, knowingly and intelligently. If, however, he indicates in any manner and at any stage of the process that he wishes to consult with an attorney before speaking, there can be no questioning. … Likewise, if the individual is alone and indicates in any manner that he does not wish to be interrogated, the police may not question him. … The mere fact that he may have answered some questions or volunteered some statements on his own does not deprive him of the right to refrain from answering any further inquiries until he has consulted with an attorney and thereafter consents to be questioned” (pp. 444-445). Of particular note to consulting psychologists is the following: “If the interrogation continues without the presence of an attorney and a statement is taken, a heavy burden rests on the government to demonstrate that the defendant knowingly and intelligently waived his privilege against selfincrimination and his right to retained or appointed counsel. … This Court has always set high standards of proof for the waiver of constitutional rights, and we reassert these standards as applied to in-custody interrogation. … Since the State is responsible for establishing the isolated circumstances under which the interrogation takes place, and has the only means of making available corroborated evidence of warnings given during incommunicado interrogation, the burden is rightly on its shoulders” (p. 475, citations omitted). Miranda in Context In Berkemer v. McCarty (1984), the U.S. Supreme Court explained that the purposes of the safeguards prescribed by Miranda are threefold: 1. to ensure that police do not coerce or trick captive suspects into confessing; 2. to relieve the inherently compelling pressures generated by the custodial setting itself, which work to undermine the individual’s will to resist; and 3. as much as possible to free courts from the task of scrutinizing individual cases to try to determine, after the fact, whether particular confessions were voluntary. How well have these goals been met? The first two goals were met to some extent in the first decade after Miranda, but police have developed effective ways to get around Miranda, and courts have allowed it. The third goal has not been met. More importantly, because the first two goals have not been met, the third goal should not be met. Leo, R. (2001). Questioning the relevance of Miranda in the twentyfirst century. Michigan Law Review, 99(5), 1000-1029. “Contrary to ... dire predictions, ... police have successfully adapted to Miranda in the last four decades. … Following an initial adjustment period, police have learned how to comply with Miranda, or at least how to create the appearance of compliance with Miranda, and still successfully elicit a high percentage of incriminating statements, admissions, and confessions from criminal suspects” (p. 1016). “Miranda has exercised a long term impact on police behavior, court cases, and popular consciousness in at least four ways. … First, Miranda increased the professionalism of police detectives, removing the last entrenched vestiges of the third degree. Second, Miranda has transformed the culture of police detecting in America by fundamentally reframing how police talk and think about the process of custodial interrogation. … Third, Miranda has increased public awareness of constitutional rights. … And fourth, Miranda has inspired police to develop more specialized, more sophisticated, and seemingly more effective interrogation techniques with which to elicit inculpatory statements from custodial suspects” (p. 1026). “Contrary to the visions of its creators, Miranda does not meaningfully dispel compulsion inside the interrogation room. Miranda has not changed the psychological interrogation process it excoriated, but has only motivated police to develop more subtle and sophisticated – and arguably more compelling – interrogation strategies. … How police ‘work’ Miranda in practice makes a mockery of the notion that a suspect is effectively apprised of his rights and has a continuous opportunity to exercise them. … Miranda offers no protection against traditionally coercive interrogation techniques, but may have, instead, weakened existing legal safeguards in this area. … And Miranda offers suspects little, if any, protection against the elicitation and admission into evidence of false confessions. As a safeguard, Miranda produces very few benefits” (p. 1027). In an individual case there is no reason to treat proper Miranda warnings as a proxy for a detailed analysis of whether the confession was made voluntarily. Post-Miranda, advising a suspect of his rights is a necessary, but not a sufficient, factor in determining that a confession was made voluntarily. Assessment regarding waiver of Miranda rights What is the court looking for? In Moran v. Burbine, 475 U.S. 412 (1986) the Supreme Court wrote that when a court is to decide whether a waiver was made voluntarily, knowingly, and intelligently, … “The inquiry has two distinct dimensions. First, the relinquishment of the right must have been voluntary in the sense that it was the product of a free and deliberate choice rather than intimidation, coercion, or deception. … Second, the waiver must have been made with a full awareness of both the nature of the right being abandoned and the consequences of the decision to abandon it. … Only if the totality of the circumstances surrounding the interrogation reveals both an uncoerced choice and the requisite level of comprehension may a court properly conclude that the Miranda rights have been waived” (p. 421, citations omitted). The Court did not define the “requisite level of comprehension.” Psychologists’ role - Miranda waiver - Does Miranda apply in the following situation? Some psychologists, by virtue of their knowledge, training, and experience, are able to assist the court in each of the following areas: 1. Gather and analyze information regarding “the physical and psychological environment in which the [waiver] was obtained” Crane v. Kentucky, 476 U.S. 683, 684 (1986). 2. Assess the defendant’s current mental status, including intelligence, memory, reading comprehension, listening comprehension, and psychopathology. 3. Reconstruct the defendant’s mental state at the time of the waiver (similar to the type of assessment in insanity and other mental-state-at-the-time-of-theoffense evaluations; see, e.g., Rogers, R., & Shuman, D. (2000). Conducting Insanity Evaluations (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. 4. Assist the judge in understanding interactions among the above. ASSESSMENT PROCEDURES First, the psychologist reviews all available information regarding the events that occurred immediately before, during, and after the waiver. Prior to any contact with the defendant, the expert should develop as clear an understanding of the circumstances and events as possible, from points of view other than that of the defendant. Also prior to the face-to-face evaluation, the attorney should provide the expert with records (e.g., school records, medical and psychological reports, etc.) regarding the defendant. The second step is to conduct a current psychological evaluation of the defendant. Clinical Forensic Psychological Assessment As with other forensic psychological assessments, a history and mental status provide useful information … which can be supplemented by a psychiatric screening instrument such as the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) or the Symptom Checklist 90-R (SCL-90-R) … [The BSI and SCLR-90-R are both available via http://assessments.ncspearson.com/as sessments/] by a structured diagnostic interview … see Rogers, R. (2001). Handbook of Diagnostic and Structured Interviewing. New York: Guilford. and/or (if the subject’s reading comprehension level is sufficient) by an objective test of psychopathology such as the Personality Assessment Inventory or the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2. When is a good time to use an oral administration of a test like the PAI or MMPI-2? [If the subject’s reading comprehension level is below the reading level of a significant number of the test items, and if the subject’s listening comprehension level is at or above the level of the test items, then it may be useful to employ an oral administration of an objective test of psychopathology.] How do we test for exaggeration or feigning of psychopathology in a forensic psychological evaluation? SIRS Rogers, R., Bagby, R. M., & Dickens, S. E. (1992). Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. www.parinc.com How do we assess for intelligence in a forensic psychological evaluation? Wechsler tests http://harcourtassessment.com Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales (RIAS) www.parinc.com How do we assess for memory in a forensic psychological evaluation? Wechsler Memory Test-III http://harcourtassessment.com How do we screen for neuropsychological impairment in a forensic psychological evaluation? Screening Test for the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery Golden, C. J. (1987). Screening Test for the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. www.wpspublish.com How do we assess effort in a forensic psychological evaluation? Word Memory Test Green, P. (2003). Green’s Word Memory Test for Windows: User’s Manual. Edmonton, Canada: Green’s Publishing. www.wordmemorytest.com/ Test of Memory Malingering Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM). North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems. www.parinc.com How do we test for reading comprehension, listening comprehension, etc.? Wechsler Individual Achievement Test – Second Edition San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2001) Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement (WJ-III) Maher, N., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement Examiner’s Manual. Itasca, IL: Riverside. http://www.riverpub.com/products/wjIIIA chievement/index.html WJ-III subtests Letter-Word Identification Reading Fluency Passage Comprehension Reading Vocabulary … WJ-III subtests (cont.) Story Recall Understanding Directions Story Recall-Delayed Oral Comprehension WJ-III clusters Oral Language Listening Comprehension Broad Reading Reading Comprehension If the defendant wrote out a statement/confession: subtests: Writing Fluency Writing Samples cluster: Written Expression admirable elocution? “He has the IQ of a nine-year old.” “His reading and oral comprehension skills are at a thirdgrade level.” How do we assess a person’s current functional abilities relevant to Miranda waiver? 1. Forensic Assessment Instruments (FAIs) 2. direct questioning of the defendant’s understanding of the rights listed on the local waiver form What are Forensic Assessment Instruments (FAIs)? Forensic assessment instruments are specialized assessment tools that have been developed in response to the demands of assessments for legal competencies. They are designed to provide standardized, quantitative methods for observing and describing behaviors that are directly relevant to legal questions about human competencies and capacities (Grisso, 2003). Grisso, T. (2003). Evaluating Competencies: Forensic Assessments and Instruments (2nd ed.). New York: Plenum. Currently, what are the best FAIs for psychological assessment relevant to waiving Miranda rights? Instruments for Assessing Understanding and Appreciation of Miranda Rights (IAUAMR; Grisso, 1998) Grisso, T. (1998). Instruments for Assessing Understanding and Appreciation of Miranda Rights (Manual). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press. www.prpress.com The IAUAMR are psychological tests that are directly relevant to the legal question but are not tests of the legal question. What is the legal question RE Miranda? The legal question is whether the defendant gave a knowing, intelligent, and voluntary waiver of his or her Miranda rights prior to questioning by the police. What do the IAUAMR assess? The IAUAMR assess understanding and appreciation at a different (typically later) time and in a different interpersonal situation (a psychological evaluation, not an interrogation). The IAUAMR are useful tools when administered along with tests of intelligence, achievement, etc. People v. Hernandez 846 N.Y.S.2d 371 N.Y.A.D. 2 Dept, 2007 People v. Cole 24 A.D.3d 1021, 807 N.Y.S.2d 166 N.Y.A.D. 3 Dept, 2005 State v. Griffin 273 Conn. 266, 869 A.2d 640 Conn, 2005 Carter v. State 697 So.2d 529 Fla.App. 1 Dist.,1997 At the conclusion of the face-to-face evaluation (which might involve more than one session), the psychologist should have a clear assessment of … 1) the person’s current mental state; 2) a detailed account of the person’s recollection of events occurring before, during, and after the waiver; 3) the defendant’s description of how and why his or her mental state may have been different at the time of the waiver; … 4) objective measurements of the defendant’s current understanding of his or her rights; 5) the defendant’s description regarding what he or she understood at the time of the waiver; and 6) the defendant’s description of why he or she waived his or her rights. After completing the face-to-face evaluation of the defendant, the psychologist can address the fourth and final task of the evaluation. The psychologist can now consider interactions among these factors in an attempt to reconstruct the defendant’s mental state at the time of the waiver. Based on knowledge, training, and experience, the psychologist can help the judge understand the defendant’s mental state at the time of the waiver, which aids the judge in determining whether the defendant made a knowing, intelligent, and voluntary waiver. 1. Did the State fail to prove, by a preponderance of the evidence, that the defendant knowingly, intelligently, and voluntarily waived his or her Miranda rights? DeClue, G. (in press). Oral Miranda Warnings: A Checklist and a Model Presentation. Journal of Psychiatry and Law. Miranda v. Arizona, 384 U.S. 436 (1966). What does Miranda require? Police must • advise suspect • warn suspect Miranda requires that the contents of the warnings be stated in “clear and unambiguous language” (p. 468) lest the process devolve into “empty formalities.” Requirements for a valid waiver of Miranda rights are described in Colorado v. Spring, 479 U.S. 564 573 (1987): First, the relinquishment of the right must have been voluntary in the sense that it was the product of a free and deliberate choice rather than intimidation, coercion, or deception. Second, the waiver must have been made with a full awareness both of the nature of the right being abandoned and the consequences of the decision to abandon it. Only if the “totality of the circumstances surrounding the interrogation” reveals both an uncoerced choice and the requisite level of comprehension may a court properly conclude that the Miranda rights have been waived. Where an interrogation is conducted without the presence of an attorney and a statement is taken, a heavy burden rests on the Government to demonstrate that the defendant knowingly and intelligently waived his right to counsel. Miranda, p. 475. What does it take for the prosecution to show that a suspect understood his rights at the time that he waived them? What does it take for the prosecution to show that a suspect understood his rights at the time that he waived them? Psychologists can play an important role in gathering evidence regarding a defendant’s current understanding of Miranda rights, along with current intelligence, achievement, and various personality test scores. But more and more interrogations are being recorded, allowing an opportunity for detectives to create a record that clearly shows whether and to what extent a suspect understands his or her Miranda rights. Three cases C L T C 43-year-old male suspected of capital sexual battery Excerpt from transcript of the interrogation of C Detective A:All right, we’ll go ahead and get started. … C, raise your right hand. You swear the statement you’re about to give is gonna be the truth, nothing but the truth? C: Yes. Detective A: Okay. (to Detective B) I got him to sign here. He signed his Miranda. So that’s good. Can you witness this for me real quick? Detective B: (to C) This is your signature right here? C: Yes. T 16-year-old male suspected of homicide Excerpt from transcript of the interrogation of T Detective: I am just going to explain this, this rights waiver form to you and your folks. We kinda talked about it before. But, um, you know I want you to know, now that I mean we read you your rights so people understand your rights and so you know anytime you are interviewed by the police for the most part and you come down to the station or interview room here, um, people sometimes get the impression that maybe they are in custody and they are not free to leave, so it’s a good time to give you your rights so you understand you know your rights are per Miranda. I’m going to go ahead and read them to you. If you have any questions, just go ahead and let me know. It says before you are asked any questions, you must understand your rights, okay? You have the right to remain silent, however, anything you do say can and will be used against you in court, okay? You have the right to talk to a lawyer for advice before you are asked any questions and have him with you during questioning, okay? You have this right to the advice and presence of a lawyer even if you can’t afford to hire one. That means, you know, if you can’t afford to hire one, that you get a public defender, that’s what that means, okay, so one will be appointed to you. If you wish to answer questions or make a statement at this time without a lawyer being present, you have the right to refuse to answer any questions, okay, and to have this interview terminated at any time, okay. Do you understand those rights and in a nutshell it means that you understand anything you say can and will be used against you. At the same token, if we ask you something you don’t like, you don’t, you’re not being forced to answer any questions, okay? T: Okay. Detective: The second part of this is just merely a waiver and the waiver says that I read you the form, that I have read you the statement of your rights, and I have shown you, um, which, and I have told you what your rights are, okay? I desire to answer questions and to make a statement without first consulting an attorney, which I think you have today, and without having a lawyer present at this time, okay? But you have your parents here because you are a juvenile and you know they have rights over you there. This decision is voluntary on your part and your parents’, right, and no promises and threats of force of any nature have been made against you to get you to come in here and talk, okay? T: Okay. Detective: So again it’s voluntary, it’s totally on your own free will and we are just going to sit and it will be, you know, basically five people in here talking and you can just sign it right there, it’s just your signature that you understand your rights. Detective: Okay, I’ll just sign it. And you witness it here. That’s all set now. That’s that! Rogers, R., Harrison, K. S., Shuman, D. W., Sewell, K. W., & Hazelwood, L. L. (2007). An analysis of Miranda warnings and waivers: Comprehension and coverage. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 177-192. Rogers et al. (2007) • 560 different (written) versions of Miranda warnings • reading levels varied: elementary to post-graduate • easiest to read: 2.8 Rogers et al. (2007, p. 190) report, anecdotally, that “college students do not understand the term ‘right’ as a protection. Instead, the large majority of students construed ‘right’ as simply an option, but an option for which they will be severely penalized (i.e., their non-cooperation will be used in court as incriminating evidence).” They note (p. 186): “The Miranda decision articulates several mechanisms to protect the Constitutional privilege against selfincrimination including (a) the assertion of rights will stop further interrogation and (b) the exercising of rights cannot be used as incriminating evidence. The Supreme Court did not specify whether these protections needed to be expressed to custodial suspects. We found that they remain unexplained in almost all Miranda warnings (98.2%).” Missing in 98% of Miranda warnings! (a) the assertion of rights will stop further interrogation and (b) the exercising of rights cannot be used as incriminating evidence. Oral Miranda Warning Checklist Did the suspect show, in his or her own words, understanding of the following (if so, list page and line numbers from the transcript): 1) I am/am not free to leave. 2) I do not have to talk to the police. 3) If I do talk to the police, anything I say can be used against me in court. 4) If I do not talk to the police, my choice not to do so cannot be used against me in court. 5) I can talk to an attorney. 6) If I cannot afford an attorney, an attorney will be provided for free. 7) I can talk to an attorney before I decide whether to talk to the police. 8) If I decide to talk to the police, I can talk to an attorney before talking to the police. 9) If I decide to talk to the police, I can talk to an attorney while I talk to the police. 10) If I decide to talk to the police, I do not have to answer every question. I can choose not to answer any question. If I choose not to answer a question, that cannot be used against me in court. 11) If I decide to talk to the police, I can decide at any time to stop talking to the police, and the decision to stop talking cannot be used against me in court. 12) If I say, “I do not want to talk to you anymore,” the police will stop asking me questions and the interview is over. 13) If I say, “I want a lawyer,” the police will stop asking me questions and the interview is over. A) Did the police make any statements before, during, or after advising the suspect of Miranda warnings that directly contradict any of the above? (If so, list page and line numbers from the transcript.) B) Did the police make any statements before, during, or after advising the suspect of Miranda warnings that (perhaps implicitly) may contradict any of the above? (If so, list page and line numbers from the transcript.) Model Oral Miranda Warning Can an oral Miranda warning do all of the following? • Use clear and unambiguous language, as required by Miranda • Include clear promises that exercising rights is not being uncooperative? • And that exercising rights cannot be used against suspect? • all in easy, understandable language Yes! reading level = 2.6 We would like to talk to you today. We would like to ask you some questions. You do not have to talk to us. You do not have to be here today. You do not have to stay here. You can leave if you want. You can leave any time you want. If you do not talk to us, that cannot be used against you in court. If you do talk to us, anything you say can be used against you in court. Now, I’m going to read you your rights. These are important rights. The U.S. Supreme Court says that these apply to every suspect in a criminal case. Right now you are a suspect in a criminal case, and that’s why I’m going to read you your rights. It is important that you understand your rights. I know you’re probably feeling nervous right now. I’m going to read these to you slowly and carefully. I’m going to ask you to tell me in your own words what each right means. So I’ll read each right to you. And then I would like you to show me whether you understand or not. Tell me in your own words what the right is. Ready? You have the right to remain silent. Tell me in your own words what that means. … And being silent is your right. You don’t have to talk to us. And if you don’t talk to us we can’t hold that against you. We can’t use it against you in any way. You can say no right now, and that’s it. We’ll stop. We will not hold it against you that you chose not to talk to us. If you do choose to talk to us, at any time you can say the magic words. “Stop, I don’t want to talk anymore.” And that’s it. We’ll stop. And we won’t hold that against you. Anything you say can and will be used against you in court. Tell me in your own words what that means. … So if you do talk to us, anything you say can be used against you in court. You have the right to talk to a lawyer for advice before you are asked any questions. Tell me in your own words what that means. … So you could say, “Stop, I want to talk to a lawyer.” Those are magic words, too. And if you say those magic words, “Stop, I want to talk to a lawyer,” we will stop. We won’t ask you any more questions. We won’t say or do anything to try to get you to talk more. And the fact that you told us to stop cannot be used against you. You can say that before we ever start. If you do, we won’t ask you any questions. You can say that right now, and we will stop right now. Or if you do agree to start answering questions, it is up to you when we stop. All you have to do is say those magic words. “Stop, I want to talk to a lawyer.” Also, you have the right to have a lawyer present with you during questioning. Tell me in your own words what that means. … So, if you want to have a lawyer present right now while we talk, that’s fine. Or if you want to talk to a lawyer first, and then also have a lawyer present while we talk, that’s fine, too. And if you choose to talk to a lawyer or to have a lawyer present while we talk, that’s fine. That’s a fine way for you to cooperate with us in the investigation. There is nothing uncooperative about talking with a lawyer. There is nothing uncooperative about having a lawyer present while you talk to us. If you’d like to have a lawyer present, we won’t hold that against you in any way. You have the right to the advice and presence of a lawyer even if you cannot afford to hire one. Tell me in your own words what that means. … So if you do not have the money to pay for a lawyer, you can still say, “Stop, I want a lawyer.” And we stop. And you get a lawyer for free. And you can talk to the lawyer and decide whether you want to talk to us. And if you do decide to talk to us, you can have a lawyer present, even if you don’t have the money to pay for a lawyer. If you talk to me, you do not have to answer every question. Tell me in your own words what that means. … So if I ask you something that you don’t want to answer, all you have to say is, “I don’t want to answer that.” Or “I don’t want to talk about that.” And we won’t hold it against you. You have the right to stop this interview at any time. Tell me in your own words what that means. … Like I said, just say the magic words. “Stop, I don’t want to talk anymore.” Or “Stop, I want a lawyer.” And we’ll stop. And we won’t hold it against you. Now, do you understand all of those rights? Do you have any questions? … Like I said, you don’t have to talk to us. And we won’t hold it against you if you don’t talk to us. Do you want to talk to us now? [If yes] If you understand each of these rights, please put your initials next to each right. But listen, if you put your initials there, that means that we went over these rights, and you’re saying that you understand the right. So, here’s the first one. You have the right to remain silent. If you understand that, please put your initials here, next to that one. [Continue for each of the rights.] And now I’m asking you, having these rights in mind, do you want to talk to us? … Do you have any more questions? Okay, then, if you want to talk to us, then sign here. Your signature here means that you understand the rights, and you are choosing to talk to us. … Okay, now remember, you can talk to us as long as you want. But any time you want to stop, all you have to do is say the magic words. Okay, here we go. L 17-year-old female suspected of homicide Excerpt from transcript of the interrogation of L Detective G: There’s a couple things that we want you to know. I understand that since you’ve been here you’ve been great. You’ve been talking to everybody and trying to tell your side of the story. Our job is to gather all of the facts, okay, and try to put this whole picture together. It’s kind of like a big jigsaw puzzle. We try to put it together. We had to talk to a bunch of people and get a whole bunch of information and you’re kind of the last person on the list to talk to, so we can get your side. But there’s some things I want to go over first before we talk about any of that stuff. How old are you? L: Seventeen. Detective G: Okay, um, do you go to school? L: No. Detective G: … How far did you go in school? … What kind of grades did you get? … Do you drive? … Did you ever get a driver’s license? … Have you ever been in trouble with the police before? … Have you ever been to court before? … Do you think you understand the court system a little bit? … I’m sure you’ve watched television and seen different things. When somebody gets arrested for a crime there’s certain rights that they have. I’m gonna go over those rights with you because I want to make sure that you understand them. The first right that they talk about is: I understand that I have a right to remain silent. Do you understand that? L: Mm-hm [yes]. Detective G: What does that mean? L: I’m not s’pose to say anything. Detective G: Is it you’re not supposed to say anything or you don’t have to say anything? L: I don’t have to say anything. Detective G: Okay. So if you want to say something you could, but if you didn’t want to, you also have that right. L: Okay. Detective G: I understand that anything I say can be used against me in a court of law. Do you understand that? L: Mm-hm [yes]. Detective G: What does that mean? L: That mean anything I say, that could be brought up again in court. Detective G:Correct. I understand that I have a right to talk to an attorney and have him or her present with me while I’m being questioned. Do you understand that? L: Mm-hm [yes]. Detective G: What does that mean to you? L: That I could hire a lawyer and that, um, discussing it, he be right there. Detective G: He could be with you, or she could be with you, when you’re talking. L: Mm-hm [yes]. Detective G: Okay. I understand that if I want an attorney and cannot afford one that an attorney will be appointed to represent me free of charge before any questioning. Do you understand that? L: Mm-hm [yes]. Detective G: What does that mean? L: Like a public defender. Detective G: Okay, um, if you came in here today and you had no money to afford, to pay for an attorney, would you still have the right to have one before we talked? L: Mm. I don’t know. Yeah. I don’t know. Detective G: Okay. Let’s go over that. It says [pointing to the page] if I want an attorney and cannot afford one that an attorney will be appointed to represent me free of charge before any questioning. L: Okay. Detective G: Okay. So in other words if you came in here and you didn’t have the money for an attorney but you wanted one, you could get one before you talked. Is that right or wrong? L: Right. Detective G: Okay. And feel free to correct me if I say something that’s not correct. Okay. I understand that at any time I can decide to exercise these rights and not answer any questions or make any statements. Do you understand that? L: Yeah. Detective G: What does that mean? L: If you ask me a question, that I don’t have to answer it. Detective G: Correct. If we talked for however long we talked and all of a sudden you decided, you know what, I don’t want to talk anymore, do you have that right? L: Mm-hm [yes]. Conclusions Regarding Oral Miranda Warnings Miranda warnings can be presented orally in clear and unambiguous language, understandable at a secondgrade level, even when including warnings that (a) the assertion of rights will stop further interrogation and (b) the exercising of rights cannot be used as incriminating evidence. The Oral Miranda Warning Checklist should assist in addressing whether the record clearly shows a knowing, intelligent, and voluntary waiver of Miranda rights. go to El Paso presentation Addressing the Voluntariness of a Confession One of the purposes of the safeguards prescribed by the Miranda decision was to free courts from the task of scrutinizing individual cases to try to determine, after the fact, whether particular confessions were voluntary Berkemer v. McCarty, 468 U.S. 420 (1984). But a signed Miranda waiver is no guarantee that a confession was given voluntarily, in large part because police use trickery to extract confessions from at-leastinitially unwilling suspects (Leo, 1992, 1996a, 1996b, 2001b). Leo, R. A. (1992). From coercion to deception: The changing nature of police interrogation in America. Crime, Law, and Social Change, 18, 35-59. Leo, R. (1996a). Inside the interrogation room. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 86, 266-303. Leo, R. (1996b). Miranda’s revenge: Police interrogation as a confidence game. Law and Society Review, 30, 259-288. Leo, R. (2001b). Questioning the relevance of Miranda in the TwentyFirst Century. Michigan Law Review, 99(5), 1000-1029. Therefore, some courts, including Massey v. State 820 So.2d 1003 (Fla.App. 4 Dist. 2003), have determined that a defendant’s claim that his or her confession was induced by police trickery is sufficient legal grounds to require an evidentiary hearing to determine whether the confession should be suppressed. The legal issue to be considered by the judge at the suppression hearing might be presented as follows: Did the State fail to prove, by a preponderance of the evidence, that the Defendant’s supposed confession was freely and voluntarily made under the totality of the circumstances? In a particular case, who frames the legal question? Legal Context “It is now axiomatic that a defendant in a criminal case is deprived of due process of law if his conviction is founded, in whole or in part, upon an involuntary confession, without regard for the truth or falsity of the confession, and even though there is ample evidence aside from the confession to support the conviction. … Equally clear is the defendant’s constitutional right at some stage in the proceedings to object to the use of the confession and to have a fair hearing and a reliable determination on the issue of voluntariness, a determination uninfluenced by the truth or falsity of the confession” (pp. 376-377). Jackson v. Denno, 378 U.S. 368 (1964) Sims v. Georgia, 385 U.S. 538 (1967) The Court clarified that the voluntariness of a confession must be decided by the trial judge prior to submitting the confession to the jury. What is the standard of proof? How certain must the evidence be that the defendant’s statements were made voluntarily? “The prosecution must prove at least by a preponderance of the evidence that the confession was voluntary. Of course, the States are free, pursuant to their own law, to adopt a higher standard.” Lego v. Twomey, 404 U.S. 477, 489 (1972). Does the defense get a second crack at it? If the Court has already deemed the defendant’s statement to have been voluntary, can the defense nevertheless challenge the voluntariness of the defendant’s statement at jury trial? Yes (unanimous) Crane v. Kentucky, 476 U.S. 683 (1986) To do: 1. Read the Crane case. 2. Note what was in the proffer. 3. Note that the Supreme Court ruled that such testimony must be admitted at jury trial. 4. So include that in your testimony - and in your report. “The manner in which a statement was extracted is, of course, relevant to the purely legal question of its voluntariness, a question most, but not all, States assign to the trial judge alone to resolve. … But the physical and psychological environment that yielded the confession can also be of substantial relevance to the ultimate factual issue of the defendant’s guilt or innocence. … Confessions, even those that have been found to be voluntary, are not conclusive of guilt. And, as with any other part of the prosecutor’s case, a confession may be shown to be insufficiently corroborated or otherwise . . . unworthy of belief. … Indeed, stripped of the power to describe to the jury the circumstances that prompted his confession, the defendant is effectively disabled from answering the one question every rational juror needs answered: If the defendant is innocent, why did he previously admit his guilt? … Accordingly, regardless of whether the defendant marshaled the same evidence earlier in support of an unsuccessful motion to suppress, and entirely independent of any question of voluntariness, a defendant’s case may stand or fall on his ability to convince the jury that the manner in which the confession was obtained casts doubt on its credibility.” Crane v. Kentucky, 476 U.S. 683, 688-689 (1986). There must be some element of police coercion for a confession to be ruled involuntary. Colorado v. Connelly, 479 U.S. 157 (1986). What do courts consider in the totality-of-the-circumstances analysis? Courts determine the factual circumstances surrounding the confession, assess the psychological impact on the accused, and evaluate the legal significance of how the accused reacted. The decisions do not turn on the presence or absence of a single controlling criterion, but reflect a careful scrutiny of all the surrounding circumstances, including both the characteristics of the accused and the details of the interrogation. Schneckloth v. Bustamonte 412 U.S. 218, 226 (1973): - youth of the accused, - lack of education, - low intelligence, - lack of any advice to the accused of his constitutional rights, - length of detention, - repeated and prolonged nature of the questioning, - use of physical punishment such as the deprivation of food or sleep The ultimate issue has been defined in psychological terms in Culombe v. Connecticut, 367 U.S. 568, 602 (1961): “Is the confession the product of an essentially free and unconstrained choice by its maker? If it is, if he has willed to confess, it may be used against him. If it is not, if his will has been overborne and his capacity for self determination critically impaired, the use of his confession offends due process.” What is the problem with the Culombe conceptualization of whether a confession was voluntary? Courts do not look to experts to decide whether a particular defendant’s will was overborne, due to the inherent subjectivity of such a question. Rather, psychologists can provide useful information about the person, the situation, and the person-situation interaction. The psychologist can assist the Court in the following ways: 1. Gather and analyze information regarding “the physical and psychological environment in which the confession was obtained” - Crane v. Kentucky, 476 U.S. 683 (1986). 2. Gather and analyze information about the interrogation techniques employed by the police. 3. Assess the defendant’s current mental status, including intelligence, memory, reading comprehension, listening comprehension, personality, and psychopathology. 4. Reconstruct the defendant’s mental state during the confession. 5. Assist the judge in understanding the effect of the interrogation techniques on the defendant throughout the interrogation. How do we do those things? 1. Gather and analyze information regarding “the physical and psychological environment in which the confession was obtained.” In Crane v. Kentucky (1986, p. 686) the trial court did not allow the defense to present evidence to the jury about “the size and other physical characteristics of the interrogation room, the length of the interview, and various other details about the taking of the confession.” The U.S. Supreme Court reversed and remanded the case, noting that evidence surrounding a confession bears on both the confession’s voluntariness and its credibility. Courts must allow such evidence to be presented to the judge to determine voluntariness, and to the jury to determine its credibility. Some relevant questions: Who initiated contact? Did the police approach the suspect, or did the suspect approach them? At what point did the police tell, show, or take actions to suggest that the suspect was not free to go? What conversations, if any, took place at the location where the police and suspect first came into contact, during transport to the police station, in the hallway, and so on? How much time elapsed between the time when the police and suspect first came into contact and when the Miranda waiver form was signed? What happened during that time? Sources of information: Information about the physical and psychological environment can be gleaned from the recording of the interrogation, if it was recorded; from the interrogators via their reports, notes, and depositions; and from questioning the defendant. Discrepancies? In your jurisdiction, are you allowed to interview the law-enforcement officers? Construct a time line? 2. Gather and analyze information about the interrogation techniques employed by the police. Do you know what it is that you seek? (What are you looking for?) Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions: A Handbook. West Sussex, England: Wiley Leo, R. A. (1992). From coercion to deception: The changing nature of police interrogation in America. Crime, Law, and Social Change, 18, 35-59. Ofshe, R., & Leo, R. (1997). The social psychology of police interrogation: The theory and classification of true and false confessions. Studies in Law, Politics, and Society, 16, 189-251. Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., & Jayne, B. C. (2004). Criminal Interrogation and Confessions (4th ed.). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen. Drizin, S. A. & Leo, R. A. (2004). The Problem of False Confessions in the Post-DNA World, 82 N.C. L. Rev. 891, 894-900. 3. Assess the defendant’s current mental status, including intelligence, memory, reading comprehension, listening comprehension, personality, and psychopathology. Many of the same procedures mentioned previously What FAIs are useful for addressing whether an admission or confession was obtained voluntarily? In considering a person’s vulnerability to the pressure of interrogation, three personality constructs have been considered to be directly relevant: interrogative suggestibility, compliance, and acquiescence. Interrogative suggestibility is defined as “the extent to which, within a closed social interaction, people come to accept messages communicated during formal questioning, as the result of which their subsequent behavioral response is affected.” This definition comprises five interrelated components which form an integrated part of the interrogative process: 1. a social interaction; 2. a questioning procedure; 3. a suggestive stimulus; 4. acceptance of the stimulus; and 5. a behavioral response” (Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 345). People with low intelligence or memory problems are generally more uncertain about the answer to interrogators’ questions and are therefore more prone to change their answers in response to negative feedback. If an interrogator succeeds in getting a suspect to doubt his or her memories, that enhances the likelihood that the suspect will change his or her answers to the interrogator’s questions. Suspects who generally have greater interpersonal trust are more prone to believe that the interrogators’ intentions are genuine and that there is no trickery involved in the questioning. Interrogators who promote trust and use subtle leading questions are more likely to succeed in getting an uncertain suspect to change his or her responses. People are more likely to accept the interrogator’s cues to change their answers, if they believe that: 1. they must provide a definite answer, 2. they should know the answer to the question, and 3. they are expected to know the answer and be able to give it. Can individual differences in interrogative suggestibility be measured reliably? Yes Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scales Susannah.Brown@informa.com Compliance According to the Gudjonsson-Clark model, suggestibility implies personal acceptance of the information provided, but compliance does not. Broadly, compliance refers to the tendency of a person to go along with – comply with – propositions, requests, or instructions, in order to achieve some immediate interpersonal gain. The compliant person is fully aware that his or her responses are being influenced. The person may disagree with the proposition or request made, but he or she nevertheless reacts in a compliant way. Are there any FAIs to measure compliance? Qualified yes Gudjonsson Compliance Scale (GCS) Acquiescence Acquiescence refers to the tendency of a person to answer questions in the affirmative, regardless of the content. Are there any FAIs to measure compliance? No test score, and no combination of test scores, tells whether a given defendant gave a true or a false confession (or a partially true and partially false confession), or whether a given confession should be deemed to have been voluntary or coerced. While scores on tests measuring interrogative suggestibility, compliance, and acquiescence are all potentially helpful for understanding a person’s vulnerability to interrogation procedures, such scores should not be interpreted or conveyed as if they had talismanic significance. low IQ = incompetence? schizophrenia = insane? suggestible = false confession? 4. Reconstruct the defendant’s mental state during the confession. similar to the process of reconstructing a defendant’s mental state at the time of an alleged offense Rogers, R., & Shuman, D. (2000). Conducting Insanity Evaluations (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. Relevant information comes from 1. an assessment of the person’s current mental state, 2. data regarding the person’s mental state at other points in time (e.g., previous test scores, results of previous psychological assessments), … 3. data about the setting and the interrogation procedures used, and 4. data about other factors that would be likely to affect a person’s mental state (e.g., alcohol or drug use, medication, sleep deprivation, family stress, fear, etc.). As Gudjonsson (2003, p. 314) notes, “Any information obtained from the accused must, whenever possible, be supported or corroborated by other evidence, because it is essentially selfserving.” Are there any FAIs useful in reconstructing a defendant’s mental state at the time of an interrogation? Yes Gudjonsson Confession Questionnaire – Revised (GCQ-R) Use of the GCQ-R can help a defendant articulate why he or she confessed, although, as it is when simply asking the defendant why he or she confessed, there are no guarantees that the defendant’s responses will be truthful. 5. Assist the judge in understanding the effect of the interrogation techniques on the defendant throughout the interrogation. As Gudjonsson (2003, p. 315) notes, “When leading questions have been asked by the interrogators and persuasive manipulation and pressure [have been] employed, then these have to be related to the accused’s personality and mental state, as well as to the circumstances of the situation.” Focusing on confessions generally (not just false confessions), Gudjonsson (2003, p. 157) summarizes research into why subjects confess as follows: “The available evidence indicates that suspects confess due to a combination of factors, rather than to one factor alone. Three general factors appear to be relevant, in varying degree, to most suspects. … These relate to an internal pressure (e.g., feelings of remorse, the need to talk about the offense), external pressure (e.g., fear of confinement, police persuasiveness), and perception of proof (e.g., the suspects’ perceptions of the strength of evidence against them). After psychologists identify the defendant’s vulnerabilities, they can help the judge recognize how interrogators have exploited those weaknesses, if they have; … how interrogators have manipulated information to alter the suspect’s perceptions, if they have; and how interrogators have manipulated external pressures, if they have. Of course, it is up to the judge to render the ultimate decision about whether a confession was illegally coerced. Addressing the Reliability of a Confession As used here, “unreliable” does not necessarily means false. In simple terms, it means “You can’t count on it” or “You don’t know what it means” or “uncertain.” Richard A. Leo, Steven A. Drizin, Peter J. Neufeld, Bradley R. Hall & Amy Vatner Bringing Reliability Back In: False Confessions and Legal Safeguards in the Twenty-First Century Wisconsin Law Review, Volume 2006 (2), 479-538. http://hosted.law.wisc.edu/lawrevie w/issues/2006-2/ “Regrettably, neither the Fifth Amendment privilege against selfincrimination nor the prophylactic Miranda rules offer any significant protection against the elicitation or admission of false and unreliable confessions” (Leo et al., 2006, 498). Two Doctrines of Confession Admissibility A. The Voluntariness Rule B. The Corroboration Rule A. The Voluntariness Rule Common Law Voluntariness The King v. Warickshall, 168 Eng. Rep. 234, 234-35 (K.B. 1783). “Confessions are received in evidence, or rejected as inadmissible, under a consideration whether they are or are not intitled [sic] to credit. A free and voluntary confession is deserving of the highest credit, because it is presumed to flow from the strongest sense of guilt . . . but a confession forced from the mind by the flattery of hope, or by the torture of fear, comes in so questionable a shape . . . that no credit ought to be given to it; and therefore it is rejected. Warickshall was adopted in the U.S. in Hopt v. Utah, 110 U.S. 574 (1884). In the U.S. the constitutional protection against involuntary confessions is based upon both the Fifth Amendment’s privilege against compelled self-incrimination and the Fourteenth Amendment’s due process clause. See Leo et al. (2006) for details. “The Supreme Court relied on different and sometimes conflicting rationales, and the due process voluntariness decisions evolved throughout the twentieth century. … However, the 1930s and 1940s saw the ascendance of another idea— that courts should only admit confessions into evidence that were the product of a free and independent will. … A third but subordinate rationale underlying the voluntariness test was that confessions elicited through fundamentally unfair police methods should be excluded so as to deter offensive police behavior, regardless of whether the suspect confessed involuntarily or his statements were likely to be trustworthy. … These underlying purposes— reliability, protecting free will, and fundamental fairness—roughly correspond to the three goals of the adversary system: promoting truthfinding, protecting individual rights, and checking state power” (Leo et al., 2006, p. 494). The template of the due process voluntariness test became a balancing analysis of whether the pressures and police techniques of the interrogation, as they interacted with the interrogated suspect’s personal susceptibilities, were sufficient to render his confession involuntary. In Rogers v. Richmond, 365 U.S. 534 (1961): A confession’s admissibility must be determined by whether the police interrogation methods were such “as to overbear petitioner’s will to resist and bring about confessions not freely self-determined—a question to be answered with complete disregard of whether or not petitioner in fact spoke the truth.” The “overbearing of the will” standard became the primary consideration of the modern due process voluntariness test. Miranda v. Arizona, 384 U.S. 436 (1966) introduced a new, easily administered test of admissibility for confession evidence. In practice, Miranda offers little or no protection against eliciting false or unreliable confessions from innocent suspects or their admission into evidence, for two reasons: 1. 80% or more of criminal suspects waive their Miranda rights and submit to police interrogation. Leo, R. A. (1966). Inside the Interrogation Room, 86 J. Crim. L. & Criminology, 266. 2. “Once the rights have been issued and waived, Miranda does not restrict deceptive or suggestive police tactics, manipulative interrogation strategies, hostile or overbearing questioning styles, lengthy confinement, or any of the inherently stressful conditions of modern accusatorial interrogation that may lead the innocent to confess. … Once police issue warnings and obtain a waiver, Miranda is virtually irrelevant to the subsequent interrogation process or as a safeguard against false confessions. … Not surprisingly, in virtually all of the documented false confessions cases, the innocent suspects either explicitly or implicitly waived their Miranda rights” (Leo et al., 2006, 497-498). Meanwhile, “Miranda … has effectively displaced the due process voluntariness standard as the primary test of a confession’s admissibility. The courts’ analyses have shifted from the voluntariness of a confession to the voluntariness of the Miranda waiver. … Regrettably, neither the Fifth Amendment privilege against selfincrimination nor the prophylactic Miranda rules offer any significant protection against the elicitation or admission of false and unreliable confessions” (Leo et al., 2006, p. 498). Colorado v. Connelly, 479 U.S. 157 (1986). “The Death Knell of the Reliability Rationale for the Voluntariness Rule” The Supreme Court ruled Connelly’s confession admissible, despite its apparent untrustworthiness, because there was no evidence of police coercion. “A statement rendered by one in the condition of respondent might be proved to be quite unreliable, but this is a matter to be governed by the evidentiary laws of the forum ... and not by the Due Process Clause of the Fourteenth Amendment” Colorado v. Connelly, p. 160. B. The Corroboration Rule The corroboration rule requires that additional evidence, other than the confession, exists to support the crime. the rule does not add to procedural and constitutional safeguards considered necessary to prevent coerced confessions. C. The Trustworthiness Standard This new rule of corroboration was announced by the Supreme Court in two cases issued on the same day, Opper v. United States, 348 U.S. 84 (1954) and Smith v. United States, 348 U.S. 147 (1954). Applies in: - most federal courts - many state courts The trustworthiness standard requires corroboration of the confession itself. “Under the trustworthiness standard, before the state may introduce a confession, it ‘must introduce substantial independent evidence which would tend to establish the trustworthiness of the [confession].’ … In effect, the trial court judge acts as a gatekeeper and must determine, as a matter of law, that a confession is trustworthy before it can be admitted. In making the trustworthiness determination, the trial court judge is to consider ‘the totality of the circumstances.’ … Only after a confession is deemed trustworthy by a preponderance of the evidence may it be admitted into evidence. … The Utah Supreme Court described this gatekeeping function as similar to a determination about the voluntariness of a confession” (Leo et al., 2006, p. 508). State v. Lucas, 152 A.2d 50 (N.J. 1959), stated that “No greater burden should be required of the State than independent corroborative proof tending to establish that when the defendant confessed he was telling the truth, plus independent proof of the loss or injury.” See State of New Mexico v. Weisser, Opinion Number: 2007-NMCA-015 Filing Date: December 22, 2006 Docket No. 25,079 at http://www.supremecourt.nm.org/op inions/VIEW/07ca-015.html D. A New Reliability Test 1. Reliability Test for Recorded Interrogations and Confessions 2. Stricter Reliability Test for Unrecorded Interrogations and Confessions 1. Reliability Test for Recorded Interrogations and Confessions Judges evaluating the reliability of confessions that are the product of a recorded interrogation should weigh three factors: 1) whether the confession contains nonpublic information that can be independently verified, would only be known by the true perpetrator or an accomplice, and cannot likely be guessed by chance; … 2) whether the suspect’s confession led the police to new evidence about the crime; and 3) whether the suspect’s postadmission narrative “fits” (or fails to fit) with the crime facts and existing objective evidence. “As in the case of voluntariness hearings, challenges to the reliability of confession evidence should commence upon filing a motion to exclude by the defense. The motion can be styled as a motion in limine under local rules of evidence that track Federal Rule of Evidence 403. … Although confession evidence failing to meet one or more of the factors in our test is clearly relevant under the Federal Rules of Evidence, it may see confession evidence as dispositive of guilt, even when it is false, its prejudicial effect can be devastating to an innocent defendant. … This is the reason Rule 403 allows judges to exclude unreliable evidence on the ground that its probative value is outweighed by its prejudicial effect” (pp. 530-531). Federal Rules of Evidence: http://www.law.cornell.edu/rules/fre/ rules.htm ARTICLE IV. RELEVANCY AND ITS LIMITS Rule 401 Definition of "Relevant Evidence" "Relevant evidence" means evidence having any tendency to make the existence of any fact that is of consequence to the determination of the action more probable or less probable than it would be without the evidence. Rule 402 All relevant evidence is admissible, except as otherwise provided by the Constitution of the United States, by Act of Congress, by these rules, or by other rules prescribed by the Supreme Court pursuant to statutory authority. Evidence which is not relevant is not admissible. Rule 403. Although relevant, evidence may be excluded if its probative value is substantially outweighed by the danger of unfair prejudice, confusion of the issues, or misleading the jury, or by considerations of undue delay, waste of time, or needless presentation of cumulative evidence. State Rules of Evidence: http://expertpages.com/news/state_ rules_of_evidence.htm Example: Florida: http://tinyurl.com/35rybk 90.403 Exclusion on grounds of prejudice or confusion. (FL) Relevant evidence is inadmissible if its probative value is substantially outweighed by the danger of unfair prejudice, confusion of issues, misleading the jury, or needless presentation of cumulative evidence. RULE 403 (TX) Although relevant, evidence may be excluded if its probative value is substantially outweighed by the danger of unfair prejudice, confusion of the issues, or misleading the jury, or by considerations of undue delay, or needless presentation of cumulative evidence. Yes, the suspect confessed. How do we know it was not a false confession? (tripartite test) What would have caused this suspect to confess if he is not guilty? “In the last two decades, social scientists and legal scholars have published hundreds of empirical studies on police interrogations, false confessions, and related issues. … These studies have extensively documented the existence of false confessions, offered an empirically informed psychological analysis of interrogation techniques and the influence process that leads to confessions, … suggested an empirical analysis of the causes and risk factors for false confessions, analyzed the impact of confession evidence and its consequences in the criminal justice system, and provided an analysis of the indicia of false and unreliable confessions” (Leo et al., 2006, p. 514). “There is now no question that false confessions occur with sufficient regularity to warrant the imposition of legal safeguards such as electronic recording requirements and meaningful corroboration rules in order to minimize the wrongful conviction of the innocent” (p. 515). The same techniques that police use to get guilty suspects to confess, can and do lead some innocent suspects to confess. (If applicable) Such techniques were used in this case. Here’s what I mean … “Psychological research has shown that interrogation is a sequenced, multistep influence process through which detectives seek to persuade a suspect that he or she is indisputably caught, and that the most viable way to mitigate punishment or to escape the situation is to agree with the interrogator’s proposed scenario and confess. … Interrogators try to break down a suspect’s anticipated resistance by: repeatedly accusing the suspect of committing the crime and lying about it; cutting off and interrupting denials; attacking alibis or assertions of innocence as illogical, implausible, or untrue; insisting that no one will believe the suspect’s protestations of innocence; and, most importantly, accumulating real or fabricated evidence said to prove the suspect’s guilt incontrovertibly. These ‘negative incentives’ are intended to convince the suspect that it is futile to deny the crime and that he or she will be successfully prosecuted and convicted if such denials continue. … In addition to these techniques, detectives also use ‘positive incentives’ or inducements to motivate the suspect to believe that it is in his or her self-interest to comply with the interrogator’s demand to confess. … These inducements range from intangible suggestions that the suspect will feel better if he or she confesses, to the implication that the interrogator or system will favor or help a suspect who confesses, to various forms of promises and threats” (Leo et al., 2006, pp. 515-516). “Empirical research has also considerably advanced our understanding of the psychological causes of, and risk factors for, police-induced false confessions. … The primary cause of false confession is the interrogator’s use of psychologically coercive interrogation techniques such as implicit or explicit promises of leniency in exchange for confession and threats of differential punishment in the absence of confession. … Other coercive techniques include lengthy or incommunicado interrogation; depriving essential necessities such as food, sleep, water, or access to bathroom facilities; refusing to honor a suspect’s request to terminate interrogation; and inducing extreme exhaustion and fatigue. … Some researchers have argued that additional situational risk factors that may cause innocent people to confess falsely include physical custody and isolation, confrontation, and minimization techniques” (pp. 516-517). “Even though psychological coercion is the primary cause of police-induced false confessions, individuals differ in their ability to withstand interrogative pressure, and thus in their susceptibility to confess falsely. … Individuals who are highly suggestible or highly compliant—all other things being equal—are more likely to confess in response to police interrogation pressure. … Mentally handicapped or cognitively impaired individuals, children, juveniles, and the mentally ill are also unusually vulnerable to police interrogation pressure and are more likely to confess falsely as a result” (p. 517). For an overlapping list of personal factors that make some people more vulnerable to police influence than others, see DeClue, 2005, pp. 173-174. For a lengthy list of interrogation procedures expected to increase the risk of false confessions, see DeClue, 2005, pp. 169-172. Summary regarding the probative value of the defendant’s statement: 1) Reliability of the process of interrogation 2) Reliability of the content of the admission statement So, if a confession’s veracity is in doubt, is that prejudicial to the defendant? Leo et al., 2006: “Social scientists and legal scholars have also empirically studied the impact of confession evidence on triers of fact and the consequences of false confessions for the American criminal justice system. A suspect’s confession sets in motion a seemingly irrefutable presumption of guilt among justice officials, the media, the public, and lay jurors. … Taken together, these studies converge on the same conclusion: as the U.S. Supreme Court stated in Arizona v. Fulminante, 499 U.S. 279, 296 (1991), ‘a confession is like no other evidence.’ It is ‘uniquely potent’ and ‘profoundly prejudicial’ in its ability to bias the trier of fact in favor of the prosecution, overwrite contradictory or exculpatory case evidence, and lead to the wrongful conviction of the innocent” (518-519). More prejudicial than probative? “By definition, an unreliable confession can have little probative value. Given juror overreliance on confession evidence, unreliable confessions will be unfairly prejudicial once entered into evidence against the accused” (p. 524). “Rule 403 (and its state analogues) directs trial judges to conduct a general balancing test to ensure that weakly probative or highly prejudicial evidence is not introduced at trial if the potential harm from doing so exceeds the potential benefit” (p. 524). “Because juries often see confession evidence as dispositive of guilt, even when it is false, its prejudicial effect can be devastating to an innocent defendant. This is the reason Rule 403 allows judges to exclude unreliable evidence on the ground that its probative value is outweighed by its prejudicial effect” (p. 531). Case Example: Central Park Jogger In April 1989, a young woman was attacked while jogging in New York City’s Central Park. She was dragged into a wooded area, beaten within an inch of her life, and raped. When her body was finally discovered, she had been beaten so severely that she had lost nearly 80 percent of her blood. Three boys had been arrested for creating mayhem in the park that night. Their statements during interrogation led to three more boys being arrested. The police obtained confessions from five of the boys. All five were convicted of participating in the rape, in spite of the fact that DNA from a cervical swab and from a sock excluded all five boys. The admission of the confessions into evidence, and the convictions and sentences, were upheld on appeal. In January 2002, nearly thirteen years after the attack on the jogger, a convict named Matias Reyes contacted authorities and informed them that he, acting alone, had raped the Central Park Jogger. Reyes’s DNA matched DNA taken from semen recovered from the Central Park Jogger crime scene. On December 19, 2002, Judge Charles Tejada granted the motion and vacated all of the convictions of the original Central Park Jogger defendants. June 11, 1989 Reyes raped, robbed, stabbed, and beat a twenty-four-year-old woman. June 14, 1989 Reyes raped, robbed, and stabbed to death another twenty-four-yearold woman. July 19, 1989 Reyes raped, robbed, and cut a twenty-year-old woman. July 27, 1989 Reyes robbed and punched a twenty-eight-year-old woman whom he had intended to rape before neighbors interrupted the crime. August 5, 1989 Reyes raped and robbed a twentyfour-year-old woman. “The pretrial reliability test that we propose … will prevent judges from admitting false confessions into evidence, thus preventing juries from wrongfully convicting the innocent. In so doing, it will also force police to focus on gathering reliable evidence so that true perpetrators, such as Matias Reyes, are no longer free to continue inflicting their violent crimes on innocent victims.” Happy Trails Greg DeClue Sarasota, FL http://gregdeclue.myakkatech.com/ gregdeclue@mailmt.com