Aşkar, Petek; B.Akkoyunlu*Kolb Öğrenme Stili Envanteri*Eğitim ve Bilim. 87,1993. 37-47. Title: Are Learning Approaches and Thinking Styles Related? A Study in Two Chinese Populations. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; THOUGHT & thinking -- Psychological aspects Source: Journal of Psychology, Sep2000, Vol. 134 Issue 5, p469, 21p Author(s): Zhang, Li-Fang; Sternberg, Robert J. ARE LEARNING APPROACHES AND THINKING STYLES RELATED? A STUDY IN TWO CHINESE POPULATIONS ABSTRACT. This article presents the results of an investigation of the construct validity of J. B. Biggs's (1987) theory of learning approaches and of R. J. Sternberg's (1988) theory of thinking styles in two Chinese populations. The study is also an examination of the nature of the relations between the two theories. University students from Hong Kong (n = 854) and from Nanjing, mainland China (n = 215), completed the Study Process Questionnaire (J. B. Biggs, 1992) and the Thinking Styles Inventory (R. J. Sternberg & R. K. Wagner, 1992). Results indicated that both inventories were reliable and valid for assessing the constructs underlying their respective theories among both Hong Kong and Nanjing university students. Results also showed that the learning approaches and thinking styles are related in the hypothesized ways: The surface approach was hypothesized to be positively and significantly correlated with styles associated with less complexity, and negatively and significantly correlated with the legislative, judicial, liberal, and hierarchical styles. The deep approach was hypothesized to be positively and significantly correlated with styles associated with more complexity, and negatively and significantly correlated with the executive, conservative, local, and monarchic styles. Implications of these relations are discussed. THINKING AND LEARNING STYLES are sources of individual differences in academic performance that are related not to abilities but to how people prefer to use their abilities. There are alternative theories of thinking and learning styles, all of which share a common goal--that is, to explain individual differences in performance that are not explained by abilities (Sternberg, 1994, 1997). Given the differences among theories of thinking and learning styles, a question that arises is whether such theories relate to different constructs, using a common root word "style," or rather if they are theories of the same construct but have different names for overlapping styles. Psychologists and educators need to understand whether the various theories--and the measures associated with them--provide insights into different constructs or the same constructs under different labels. Following this view, the primary goal of the present study was to verify the nature of the relations between Biggs's (1987, 1992) theory of approaches to learning and Sternberg's (1988, 1990, 1994, 1997) theory of mental self-government, a theory of thinking styles. What, exactly, is a style? How does a style differ is a preferred way of thinking or of doing things. ability, but rather a preference in the use of the It is an interface between ability and personality from an ability? A style A style is not an abilities people have. (Sternberg, 1994, 1997). Since the beginning of the cognitive-styles movement in the 1950s and early 1960s, different theories of thinking styles have been constructed. Because there are many more than we could address here (for more extensive reviews, see Grigorenko & Sternberg, 1995; Kogan, 1983; Sternberg, 1997), we review only selected theories. Myers (1980; Myers & McCaulley, 1988) proposed a series of psychological types based on Jung's (1923) theory of types. According to Myers, there are 16 types, resulting from all possible combinations of (a) two ways of perceiving (sensing vs. intuiting), (b) two ways of judging (thinking vs. feeling), (c) two ways of dealing with self and others (being introverted vs. being extraverted), and (d) two ways of dealing with the outer world (judging vs. perceiving). Gregorc (1985) proposed four main types of styles, based on all possible combinations of two dimensions (concrete vs. abstract and sequential vs. random). Renzulli and Smith (1978) suggested various learning styles, with each corresponding to a method of teaching (e.g., projects, drill and recitation, and discussion), and Holland (1973) proposed six styles (realistic, investigative, artistic, social, enterprising, and conventional) that have been used as a basis for understanding career interests. Some other theories of styles are not general theories; rather, they are theories of specific aspects of cognitive-stylistic functioning (Grigorenko & Sternberg, 1995). For example, Kagan (1976) studied individual differences between impulsive and reflective persons, and Witkin (1978) examined the differences between field-independent and field-dependent individuals. Recently, two theories have been proposed that are fairly general. One is Biggs's (1987, 1992) theory of students' approaches to learning, also known as the 3P model; the other is Sternberg's (1988, 1990, 1997) theory of mental self-government. Biggs's Theory of Approaches to Learning Adapted from Dunkin and Biddle's (1974) presage-process-product model, Biggs's model addresses those three components in the classroom. Presage concerns components before learning takes place; process pertains to components while learning is taking place; product pertains to outcomes after learning has taken place. In the present study we focus on the process of learning. According to the 3P model, there are three common approaches to learning: surface, which involves a reproduction of what is taught to meet the minimum requirements; deep, which involves a real understanding of what is learned; and achieving, which involves using a strategy that will maximize one's grades. Each approach is composed of two elements: motive and strategy (see Biggs, 1987, 1992, for a description of the Study Process Questionnaire [SPQ]). Motive describes why students choose to learn, whereas strategy describes how students go about their learning. An alternative theory is that of Marton (e.g., Marton & Booth, 1997), who proposed surface and deep but not achieving strategies. A question in need of resolution, therefore, is whether the achieving style truly is distinguishable from the other two as a third style, or is a variant of one or both of them. Related work has been done by Entwistle and his colleagues (e.g., Entwistle, 1988, 1990; Entwistle, Koseki, & Politt, 1987; Entwistle & Marton, 1994), who have considered both the two-style and three-style models. One of the instruments used to assess learning approaches among university students is the SPQ (Biggs, 1987, 1992), which was originally designed to assess the learning approaches of Canadian and Australian students. Many studies have been undertaken with the SPQ. Focusing on students' motives and strategies for learning, Biggs (1992) summarized major endeavors regarding the 3P model using the SPQ before 1992. These motives and strategies for learning have been examined in the following contexts: cross-cultural comparisons, the language medium of instruction, teaching/learning environments, student characteristics, professional and staff development, and factor structure and dimensionality of subscales. More recent work examining learning approaches as defined by the 3P model have as their foci the investigation of the relationships between learning approaches and academic achievement (e.g., Albaili, 1995; Rose, Hall, Bolen, & Webster, 1996) and the construction of other versions of the SPQ (e.g., Albaili; Watkins & Murphy, 1994). Investigation of the factorial structure of the SPQ continues to be one of the major approaches to examining the instrument and its underlying 3P model (e.g., Bolen, Wurm, & Hall, 1994; Niles, 1995; O'Neil & Child, 1984). In addition, individual differences based on age and gender (e.g., Sadler-Smith & Tsang, 1998; Watkins & Hattie, 1981; Wilson, Smart, & Watson, 1996) also have been of major interest to scholars using the SPQ in their investigations of student learning. In their study of the relationship between SPQ scores and overall grade point average (GPA) among 202 U.S. undergraduate students, Rose et al. (1996) found that only scores on the achieving approach contributed to prediction (negative correlation) of GPA. Albaili (1995), in his study of 246 United Arab Emirates undergraduate students, found that GPAs were negatively correlated with the surface approach and positively correlated with the deep and achieving approaches. As mentioned earlier, the SPQ was originally constructed to measure Australian and Canadian university students' learning approaches. Other versions of the SPQ, however, also have been constructed. For example, in 1992, a Hong Kong version was established (Biggs, 1992). In 1994, when they studied Brunei university students, Watkins and Murphy came up with a simplified English as a Second Language (ESL) version and a Malay version. In 1995, Albaili established an Arabic version of the SPQ in his study of university students in the United Arab Emirates. All of the versions of the SPQ proved to be reliable and valid measures for assessing students' learning approaches. The study of the validity of the SPQ has taken two forms. One is the examination of its internal structure. The other is the examination of the SPQ compared with other instruments. Many studies have had a focus on the examination of the internal structure of the SPQ. Although some studies supported Biggs's original argument that there are three factors in the SPQ, other studies have shown that there are only two factors. For example, in their study of a sample of U.S. university students' approaches to learning, Bolen et al. (1994) identified three factors--Surface Approach, Deep Approach, and Achieving Approach. Similarly, O'Neil and Child (1984), studying British university students, also identified three factors in the SPQ. However, in a study in Australia by Niles (1995) of overseas and Australian university students, and in Watkins and Dahlin's (1997) study of university students in Sweden, a two-factor model was identified (see also Entwistle, 1981; Marton & Booth, 1997). The two factors were Deep Approach and Surface Approach to learning, and the two Achieving subscales were split between the two factors. Few researchers have investigated the relations between the SPQ and other instruments. We identified three such studies in the literature. A first study was conducted by Kember and Gow (1990), who administered both the SPQ and the Approaches to Studying Inventory (ASI; Entwistle, 1981) to Hong Kong university students. Although all three factors (Surface, Deep, and Achieving) appeared in the SPQ, only two factors (Deep and Achieving) appeared in the ASI. Surface learning was replaced by a factor labeled "narrow orientation" (Harper & Kember, 1989), which has been variously called "operation learning" by Watkins (1982, p. 80) and "disorganized study" by Ramsden and Entwistle (1981, p. 372). A second study was carried out by Murray-Harvey (1994), who conducted a factor analysis on the Productivity Environmental Preference survey and the SPQ data collected from 400 Australian university students. Results indicated that the two inventories measure two quite different conceptualizations of student learning. It was concluded that learning approach is relatively stable over time and that learning style is not quite as stable. A third study was conducted by Wilson et al. (1996), who also studied the relationship between the SPQ and the ASI. Analyzing the data collected from 283 Australian university students, the authors found significant correlations between the scales in the two inventories. They concluded that the two inventories measure similar constructs. Age and gender are two of the variables that scholars have investigated in relation to the SPQ. Findings are, again, varied. For example, Sadler-Smith and Tsang (1998), studying British and Hong Kong university students, did not find any age or gender difference in the British sample; they did, however, observe an interaction of age and gender in their effects on deep and strategic (see Entwistle, 1981) approaches. That is, mature male students reported higher scores on the deep approach than did the nonmature male students; however, for female students, this pattern was reversed. Sadler-Smith and Tsang specified 23 years as the cutoff age between non-mature and mature participants. By the same token, Watkins and Hattie (1981) also observed age and gender differences. They found that male students scored significantly higher on the scales measuring surface learning than did female students, whereas female students scored significantly higher on the scales measuring deep learning than did their male counterparts. They also found that older students scored significantly higher on the scales measuring deep learning than did their younger counterparts. On the contrary, Wilson et al. (1996) found no gender differences. In summary, there is strong evidence that the SPQ is a reliable and valid instrument for assessing the learning approaches of university students, including Chinese university students. Sternberg's Theory of Mental Self-Government Sternberg's (1988, 1990, 1997) theory of mental self-government addresses people's thinking styles, which may be used in many settings, including university, home, and community. At the heart of this theory is the notion that people need somehow to govern or manage their everyday activities. There are many ways of doing so; whenever possible, people choose styles of managing themselves with which they are comfortable. Still, people are at least somewhat flexible in their use of styles and try with varying degrees of success to adapt themselves to the stylistic demands of a given situation. Thus, an individual with one preference in one situation may have a different preference in another situation. Moreover, styles may change with time and with life demands. Thinking styles are at least partly socialized (Sternberg, 1994, 1997), a fact that suggests that, to some extent, they can be modified by the environment in which people reside. As applied to individuals, the theory of mental self-government posits 13 thinking styles that fall along five dimensions of mental self-government: (a) functions, (b) forms, (c) levels, (d) scope, and (e) leanings. Functions As in government, there are three functions in human beings' mental selfgovernment: legislative, executive, and judicial. An individual with a legislative style enjoys being engaged in tasks that require creative strategies. These individuals prefer to choose their own activities, or at least to do the activities chosen for them in their own way. An individual with an executive style is more concerned with implementation of tasks with set guidelines. Such an individual prefers more direction or guidance in structuring tasks. An individual with a judicial style focuses attention on evaluating the products of others' activities. Forms Also as in government, a human being's mental self-government takes four different forms: monarchic, hierarchic, oligarchic, and anarchic. An individual with a monarchic style enjoys being engaged in tasks that allow complete focus on one thing at a time. In contrast, an individual with a hierarchic style prefers to distribute attention to several tasks that are given priority according to their value to the individual in achieving his or her goals. An individual with an oligarchic style also likes to work on multiple activities in the service of multiple objectives, but may not enjoy setting priorities. Finally, an individual with an anarchic style enjoys working on tasks that allow flexibility as to what, where, when, and how one works, but he or she eschews systems of almost any kind. Levels As with governments, human beings' mental self-government functions at two different levels: local and global. An individual with a local style enjoys being engaged in tasks that require working with concrete details. In contrast, an individual with a global style prefers to pay more attention to the overall picture of an issue and to abstract ideas. Scope Mental self-government can deal with internal and external matters. An individual with an internal style enjoys being engaged in tasks that allow that individual to work independently. In contrast, an individual with an external style likes being engaged in tasks that allow for collaborative ventures with other people. Leanings Finally, in mental self-government, there are two leanings: liberal and conservative. An individual with a liberal style enjoys engaging in tasks that involve novelty and ambiguity, whereas an individual with a conservative style prefers adhering to the existing rules and procedures in performing tasks. The theory of mental self-government has been operationalized through inventories, including the Thinking Styles Inventory (TSI; Sternberg & Wagner, 1992), which have been shown to be reliable and valid for U.S. and Hong Kong samples. Furthermore, results from such research have shown some value of the theory and have generated a number of implications for teaching and learning in educational settings. In the United States, Sternberg and Grigorenko conducted a series of studies. In one such study, Sternberg and Grigorenko (1995) reported significant relationships between teaching styles and grade taught, length of teaching experience, and subject area taught. Specifically, teachers teaching at lower grade levels were more legislative than teachers teaching at higher grade levels; complementarily, teachers teaching at lower grade levels were less executive than teachers at higher grade levels. It was shown that teachers with more teaching experience were more executive, local, and conservative than were those teachers with less teaching experience. Furthermore, it was found that humanities teachers were more liberal than were science teachers. A second set of findings indicated significant relationships between students' learning styles and such demographic data as students' socioeconomic status (SES) and birth order (Sternberg & Grigorenko, 1995). Specifically, participants of higher SES status tended to score higher on the legislative style. Likewise, participants who were later-borns in their family scored higher on the legislative style than did participants who were earlier-borns. A third data set indicated that teachers inadvertently favored those students who had thinking styles similar to their own (Sternberg & Grigorenko). In a more recent study, Grigorenko and Sternberg (1997) found that certain thinking styles contribute significantly to prediction of academic performance over and above prediction of scores on ability tests. Their study also indicated that students with particular thinking styles fared better on some forms of evaluation than on others. Three studies concerning the theory of mental self-government have been carried out in Hong Kong (Zhang, 1999; Zhang & Sachs, 1997; Zhang & Sternberg, 1998). These studies indicate that the thinking styles defined by Sternberg's theory also can be identified among university students in Hong Kong. The internal consistency reliabilities and validity data are generally satisfactory (see description in the Method section, under Inventories). Furthermore, results from these studies have suggested that students' thinking styles are statistically different based on such variables as age, sex, college class, teaching experience, college major, school subject taught, and travel experience. For example, male participants scored higher on the global style than did their female counterparts. Participants who had had more teaching experience (as measured by the length of teaching) and those who had had more travel experience scored higher on the creativity-promoting thinking styles, such as legislative and liberal. In our recent study (Zhang & Sternberg, 1998) of 622 Hong Kong university students, we found that thinking styles (as defined by the theory of mental self-government) could serve as reasonable predictors of academic achievement over and above self-rated abilities. For example, higher achievement was positively correlated with the use of conservative, hierarchic, and internal styles of thinking; yet, higher achievement was negatively correlated with the use of the legislative, liberal, and external styles of thinking. Although both the SPQ and the TSI and their underlying theories have been well researched, the present study is the first to investigate the relationships among the scales in the two inventories and the connections between the two theories. In the present study, we examined the relations between the two theories and corresponding measures of styles in two Chinese populations--university students from Nanjing, mainland China, and university students from Hong Kong. The means to achieve this goal was to determine the reliability and validity of the SPQ and of the TSI, to examine the relations between the scales in the two inventories, and to determine whether the hypothesized relationships between the SPQ and the TSI exist among more than one sample. These two inventories were studied together because they are based on similar theoretical constructs. By nature, both Biggs's theory of learning approaches and Sternberg's theory of mental self-government concern two types of mental functioning and thus, two ways of processing information: more simple and more complex. We proposed two sets of hypotheses, drawn in part on past work in the field by Beishuizen, Stoutjesdijk, and Van-Putten (1994), who studied the relation between cognitive levels of task accomplishment and deep versus surface processing of material. Beishuizen et al. expected deep-processing students to benefit from metacognitive support and surface-processing students to benefit from cognitive support. They found that students who processed at a surface level tended to benefit from cognitive support. Students who combined self-regulation with deep processing and students who combined external regulation with surface processing outperformed students who showed the opposite pairings of type of regulation with type of processing. We expected students who take a surface approach to learning and those who use executive, monarchic, local, and conservative styles to be individuals who want to get things done with given structures, who do not want to make mistakes, and who want to "play it safe." We expected students who take a deep approach to learning and those who tend toward legislative, judicial, hierarchic, anarchic, global, and liberal styles to want to make up their own minds and use their own judgments in learning. We expected these students to want to work more in situations in which their creativity and imagination would be allowed free rein. Furthermore, we expected them to be less afraid of making mistakes. Thus, we proposed the following: First, the surface approach should be positively and significantly correlated with styles associated with less complexity--executive, monarchic, local, and conservative styles. Complementarily, this approach should be negatively and significantly correlated with the legislative, judicial, liberal, and hierarchic styles. Second, the deep approach should be positively and significantly correlated with styles associated with more complexityNlegislative, judicial, hierarchic, anarchic, global, and liberal styles. Complementarily, this approach should be negatively and significantly correlated with the executive, conservative, local, and monarchic styles. No specific predictions were made regarding the relations between the achieving approach subscales of the SPQ and the subscales of the TSI, because previous research (e.g., Niles, 1995; Watkins & Dahlin, 1997; Wong, Lin, & Watkins, 1996) has yielded conflicting results. In particular, the achieving motive and strategy subscales of the SPQ (which assess the achieving approach) may be either clustered with one of the two scales (Deep and Surface) or split between the two. In other words, like Marton and Booth's (1997) theory, Biggs's theory conceptually addresses two approaches to learning: deep and surface. Method Participants Hong Kong sample. A total of 854 (362 male and 492 female) students were selected randomly from about 4,000 entering students at the University of Hong Kong during the orientation week of the fall semester of 1997. These participants were from all of the nine faculties (Architecture, Arts, Dentistry, Education, Engineering, Law, Medicine, Science, and Social Sciences) and the School of Business at the university. Of these students, 501 were in social sciences/humanities, 349 were in natural sciences, and 4 were not identifiable. Of all the participants, 702 were undergraduate freshmen, 66 were beginning to pursue their post-graduate certificates, and 86 were starting their education for a master's degree. The average age of the participants was 21 years; 66% were 19 years old or younger, 20% were between the ages of 20 and 25, and 14% were between 26 and 57 years of age. At the time the study was conducted, 535 of the participants were not holding any job, 110 were working full-time, and 198 were working parttime. Eleven did not indicate their employment status. Nanjing sample. A total of 215 (114 male, 101 female) entering freshmen from two big universities in Nanjing, mainland China, participated in the study at the beginning of the fall semester of 1997. Ten teachers were trained in the administration of the questionnaires. Each of the 10 teachers informed his or her class about the nature of the study. Those students who were not willing to participate in the study were not required to participate. Those who volunteered (98% of the students) to participate were from several areas of study, including chemistry, computer science, education, finance, history, law, management, mathematics, medicine, and political science. Classified into the two broad fields of study, 126 were from social sciences/humanities and 89 were from natural sciences. The average age of the participants was 19 years, with a range from 15 to 23. In all, 75% of the participants were 19 years old or younger. Inventories Two inventories and a demographic questionnaire were used in the study. The first inventory was Biggs's SPQ (1992; Chinese version normed on Hong Kong university students). The second was Sternberg and Wagner's (1992) TSI. Both of the inventories were developed originally in English and were later translated and back-translated between Chinese and English. The SPQ is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 42 items. This questionnaire has 6 subscales, with 7 items on each subscale. For each item, the respondents are asked to rate themselves on a 5-point scale anchored by 1 (never or only rarely true of you) and 5 (always or almost always true of you). The 6 subscales are Surface Motive, Surface Strategy, Deep Motive, Deep Strategy, Achieving Motive, and Achieving Strategy. Therefore, the 3 scales based on the three approaches to learning are Surface (Motive and Strategy), Deep (Motive and Strategy), and Achieving (Motive and Strategy). As described earlier, motive describes why students choose to learn, whereas strategy describes how students go about their learning. As mentioned earlier, numerous studies involving the use of the SPQ have been conducted all over the world (e.g., Albaili, 1995; Bolen et al., 1994; Kember & Gow, 1990; Murray-Harvey, 1994; Watkins & Akande, 1992; Watkins & Regmi, 1990). Most of those studies have resulted in internal consistencies ranging from the mid .50s to the low or mid .70s for the 6 subscales and from the low .70s to the low .80s for the three scales (see Albaili, 1995, for details). The TSI (Sternberg & Wagner, 1992) is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 65 items. The inventory has 13 subscales, with 5 items on each subscale. For each item, respondents are asked to rate themselves on a 7-point scale anchored by 1 (does not characterize you at all) and 7 (characterizes you extremely well). These 13 subscales correspond to the 13 thinking styles described in Sternberg's theory of mental self-government. Sternberg and Wagner (1992) collected norms for various age groups on the long version of the TSI (which contains 104 items, 8 for each of the 13 subscales). For Sternberg and Wagner's college sample, subscale reliabilities ranged from .42 (monarchic) to .88 (external), with a median reliability of .78. In another study using the TSI, Sternberg (1994) found a five-factor model corresponding to the five dimensions of mental selfgovernment described in his theory of thinking styles. These five factors accounted for 77% of the variance in the data. The TSI also has been validated against instruments based on other theories of styles (e.g., Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, Gregorc's measure of mind styles), as well as a standard IQ test, the Scholastic Assessment Test (SAT), and GPA. Results from these construct-validity studies indicated that, among U.S. students, the TSI is a reliable and valid instrument for studying thinking styles as defined by the theory of mental selfgovernment. The TSI also has proved to be reliable and valid for identifying thinking styles of university students in Hong Kong. The statistics from three studies (Zhang, 1999; Zhang & Sachs, 1997; Zhang & Sternberg, 1998) conducted in Hong Kong are similar in magnitude to those obtained by Sternberg (1988, 1990, 1994, 1997). For example, the alpha coefficients in Sternberg's (1994) study ranged from .44 to .88; those in Zhang and Sachs's (1997) study ranged from .53 to .87 (from .46 to .89 in Zhang, 1999, and from .43 to .78 in Zhang & Sternberg, 1998). Although Zhang and Sachs's (1997) study extracted only three factors corresponding to the constructs in the theory of mental self-government, both Sternberg's (1994) and Zhang's (1999) studies extracted five factors (the former accounted for 77% of the variance and the latter, 78%). In these studies, each factor roughly corresponded to one of the five dimensions delineated in the theory. In our recent study (Zhang & Sternberg, 1998), the validity of the TSI was tested through an interscale correlation matrix. It was shown that the scales were, in general, correlated in the predicted directions. For example, the correlation between the executive and conservative styles was .63 (p < .001); that between the legislative and liberal styles was .41 (p < .001); and that between the internal and external styles was -.30 (p < .001). Data Analysis The following analyses were conducted both separately for men and women and for the sexes combined. The reliability of each of the 6 subscales in the SPQ and the 13 subscales in the TSI was estimated by Cronbach's alpha. The validity of each of the two inventories was examined through the relations shown among the subscales by its respective intercorrelation matrix. The relations between the two theories were examined via a correlation matrix, with the subscales of the SPQ providing one set of variables and those of the TSI providing another. Results In both the Hong Kong and Nanjing samples, t tests on the 6 subscales of the SPQ and the 13 subscales of the TSI resulted in a few pairs of statistically significant (p < .05) means for men and women. On a 5-point Likert-type scale (of the SPQ), the statistically significant mean differences were (a) .19 on Achieving Motive, (b) .11 on Deep Strategy, and (c) .11 on Surface Motive for the Hong Kong sample; and (a) .24 on Deep Motive and (b) .31 on Deep Strategy for the Nanjing sample. On a 7-point Likert-type scale, the statistically significant mean differences were (a) .12 on the legislative style, (b) .20 on the judicial style, (c) .15 on the global style, (d) .38 on the liberal style, and (e) .23 on the internal style for the Hong Kong sample; and (a) .39 on the legislative style, (b) .74 on the liberal style, and (c) .35 on the internal style for the Nanjing sample. In all cases, men scored higher than women. These differences, although statistically significant, were small in magnitude. Furthermore, none of the remaining statistical analyses conducted for men and women separately indicated significant gender differences. These analyses included (a) a correlational analysis on the 13 subscales of the TSI, (b) a factor analysis on the SPQ, and (c) a correlational analysis between the subscales of the two inventories. Because of the lack of gender differences in the previous three statistical procedures, the results are reported with combined gender analyses. Subscale Reliabilities for the SPQ The alpha estimates of internal consistency for the Deep and Achieving Motive and Strategy subscales for both the Hong Kong and Nanjing samples are in line with those obtained by Biggs (1987) for his Australian norming sample (see Table 1). The findings also are in line with estimates obtained by other authors, such as Watkins and Dahlin (1997), in their study of Swedish university students. However, the alpha coefficients of the Surface Motive and Surface Strategy subscales are higher for the samples in this study (in the mid .60s and low .70s) than for the aforementioned Australian and Swedish samples (low .40s for Surface Motive and mid .50s for Surface Strategy). The alpha coefficients for the 6 subscales ranged from .65 to .80, with a median of .73, for the Hong Kong students, and from .64 to .74, with a median of .70, for the Nanjing students. The alpha coefficients for the Surface, Deep, and Achieving scales were .80, .82, and .83, respectively, for the Hong Kong sample, and .78, .78, and .76, respectively, for the Nanjing sample. These alpha coefficients were considered sufficiently high to allow further statistical analyses. Subscale Reliabilities for the TSI The magnitudes of the estimates of internal consistency for the TSI for the Hong Kong sample and the Nanjing sample were similar (see Table 2). Furthermore, these results are comparable to those obtained by Sternberg (1994) in his study of U.S. participants, by Zhang and Sachs (1997), and by Zhang (1999). Notice that 3 subscales were less internally consistent in those respective studies. These subscales were local, monarchic, and anarchic. Even so, the estimates of internal consistency obtained in the present study were considered to be adequate to allow further statistical analyses. Subscale Intercorrelations for the SPQ In accordance with Biggs's theory, we predicted that the Deep Motive and Deep Strategy subscales would be significantly negatively correlated with the Surface Motive and Surface Strategy subscales. Furthermore, as mentioned earlier, no prediction was made on the Achieving Motive and Achieving Strategy subscales because these subscales may be positively and significantly correlated with either the Deep subscales or the Surface subscales, or split between the two (Watkins & Dahlin, 1997; Wong et al., 1996). The predictions were fully supported by the results from the Nanjing sample. Results from the Hong Kong sample, however, did not support these predictions, in that three of the correlations were in the direction opposite from what was expected from the theory. These correlation coefficients were (a) Surface Motive with Deep Motive (r = .17, p < .01), (b) Surface Motive with Deep Strategy (r = .16, p < .01), and (c) Surface Strategy with Deep Strategy (r = .10, p < .01). These three correlations indicate that students who took a surface approach to learning also tended to take a deep approach, a pattern not consistent with Biggs's theory, according to which surface subscales presumably should be negatively correlated with the deep subscales. Because of the presence of the three unexpected correlations, we conducted a principal-axis factor analysis with a varimax rotation, to examine further the validity of the SPQ for the Hong Kong sample. A scree test (Cattell, 1966) indicated that a two-factor solution would be appropriate. Furthermore, there were two factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. Thus, a two-factor model was retained (see Table 3 for details). The analysis yielded a clear factor for a deep approach (factor loadings: .86 for Deep Motive; .89 for Deep Strategy; .76 for Achieving Strategy) and one for a surface approach (factor loadings: .88 for Surface Motivation; .87 for Surface Strategy; .71 for Achieving Motive). The Achieving Motive and Achieving Strategy subscales thus were split between the Deep and Surface subscales, as expected (Niles, 1995; Watkins & Dahlin, 1997; Wong et al., 1996). A principal-axis factor analysis with a varimax rotation also was conducted with the Nanjing participants' data to confirm the validity of the SPQ for the Nanjing sample. Results from this analysis revealed the same two factors as those for the Hong Kong data (see Table 3). The first factor corresponded to the deep approach (factor loadings: .81 for Deep Motive; .81 for Deep Strategy; .77 for Achieving Strategy). The second factor corresponded to the surface approach (factor loadings: .86 for Surface Motive; .86 for Surface Strategy; .61 for Achieving Motive). Consequently, the SPQ, when conceptualized as a two--rather than threefactor instrument, appeared to be valid for assessing the learning approaches of the two Chinese samples. These results from factor analyses supported not only previous studies using the SPQ (e.g., Niles, 1995; Watkins & Dahlin, 1997; Wong et al., 1996) but also Marton and Booth's (1997) findings regarding learning approaches. Subscale Intercorrelations for the TSI In general, for both the Hong Kong and Nanjing samples, the correlations among the 13 subscales were in the direction predicted by the theory of mental self-government (see Table 4 for details). Some of the examples are (a) Executive with Conservative (r = .65 for Hong Kong; r = .66 for Nanjing), (b) Legislative with Liberal (r = .42 for Hong Kong; r = .50 for Nanjing), (c) Conservative with Liberal (r = -.10 for Hong Kong; r = -.42 for Nanjing), (d) Global with Local (r = .08 for Hong Kong; r = -.35 for Nanjing), and (e) Internal versus External (r = -.23 for Hong Kong; r = .28 for Nanjing). Except for the correlation between Global and Local for Nanjing, these correlations were significant at the .01 level. Furthermore, the magnitudes of these correlations were generally stronger for the Nanjing sample than for the Hong Kong sample. Correlations Among Subscales in the Two Inventories In general, the hypotheses were supported by the data from both samples (see Table 5). The majority of the correlations were in the expected directions. Some of the examples are (a) Surface Motive with executive style (r = .24 for Hong Kong; r = .23 for Nanjing), (b) Surface Strategy with liberal style (r = -.03 for Hong Kong; r = -.31 for Nanjing), (c) Deep Motive with judicial style (r = .40 for Hong Kong; r = .31 for Nanjing), and (d) Surface Strategy with judicial style (r = -.13 for Hong Kong; r = .11 for Nanjing). These correlations varied from being statistically insignificant to being significant at the .01 level. Achieving subscales were inconsistently correlated positively with either the Deep or the Surface subscales. These correlations indicated that students who took a surface approach to learning tended to use an executive thinking style, but not judicial or liberal thinking styles. In addition, students who took a deep approach to learning tended to use the judicial thinking style. There were a few correlations that clearly did not support the predictions. First, for the Hong Kong sample, the correlation between Deep Strategy and executive style was significantly positive (r = .18, p < .001), meaning that the Hong Kong students in this sample who used a deep strategy to learn also preferred using an executive thinking style. Second, our prediction about the relations between learning approach subscales and the global and local styles were only partially supported (see Table 5). Results of this study suggested that regardless of their level of mental functioning (global or local), students could take either a deep or surface approach to learning. Finally, all learning approach subscales were positively and significantly correlated with the monarchic style, which probably means that students with a monarchic thinking style may take either a deep or a surface approach to learning. These unexpected correlations were mostly from the Hong Kong sample, however. These results perhaps can be explained by Pask's (1976) concept of the "versatile learner." For example, the deep learners in Hong Kong may be creative (using the legislative and liberal styles) in their learning; meanwhile, they may also follow closely their teachers' instructions (using the executive and conservative styles). Discussion The major goal of this study was to establish the relations between the constructs in Biggs's theory of learning approaches and Sternberg's theory of thinking styles in two Chinese populations. Results indicated that the two inventories were reliable and valid (there are two factors in the SPQ-Deep Approach and Surface Approach) for assessing the underlying theoretical constructs for these two populations and that the subscales in the two inventories were related in largely predicted ways. Our study suggests that the SPQ and the TSI measure similar constructs. Students who reported taking a surface approach to learning preferred using executive, local, and conservative thinking styles (which are more traditional, norm favoring, and task oriented), whereas students who reported taking a deep approach to learning preferred using legislative, judicial, and liberal thinking styles (which are more creative, norm questioning, and meaning seeking). Although most of the correlations between the scales of the two inventories were low, they were statistically significant. In addition, these results both supported our own hypotheses (based on the study of Beishuizen et al., 1994) about the relationships between the two inventories and confirmed previous research findings of similar studies (e.g., Wilson et al., 1996). Therefore, we believe that these correlations, although weak, revealed true relationships between the two inventories. The contributions of this study may be considered from two perspectives: research and practice. From a research viewpoint, the results of this study have enhanced our knowledge about theories of styles. The question raised earlier was whether theories of styles are different theories of different things, using a common root word ("style") or theories of the same thing but with different names for overlapping styles. Sternberg (1997) suggested that alternative theories of styles cover roughly similar attributes, but with different labels. The relations indicated by the subscales in the two inventories used in this study suggest that Biggs's (1987, 1992) theory of students' approaches to learning and Sternberg's (1988, 1990, 1994, 1997) theory of mental self-government cover similar but not identical ground, with different names for overlapping styles. This finding is also consistent with previous construct-validity studies of measures derived from the theory of mental self-government (e.g., compared with the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and with Gregorc's measures of mind styles; Sternberg, 1994). Of further theoretical importance is the finding that the two-learning-style approach of Marton and Booth (deep and surface; 1997) appears to capture better the structure of the data than does the three learning-style approach of Biggs (deep, surface, achieving). From a practical viewpoint, we believe that there are three implications. First, both teachers and students should be aware that people approach learning differently and use their abilities in a variety of ways. Second, but equally important, teachers and students should understand the relations between learning approaches and thinking styles. An understanding of the existence of different learning approaches and different thinking styles can assist teachers in using several measures to facilitate effective learning. Teachers should try to teach via a variety of styles so that all students, regardless of their preferred ways of dealing with learning tasks, can benefit from teachers' instructions. Alternatively, because learning styles can be modified (Saracho, 1993; Sternberg, 1988, 1990, 1997), awareness of the different learning styles can make students more in tune with how they usually approach their learning tasks and help them identify their preferred, as well as their nonpreferred, learning styles. As a result, students may learn not only how to capitalize on their strengths and compensate for their weaknesses but also how to adapt to those learning environments with which their own styles may not be compatible. Third, a teacher can use different assessment techniques to allow for different learning and thinking styles (Sternberg, 1988, 1990, 1994, 1997). Recognizing this fact, Biggs (1995) coined the term "backwash effect." In particular, he argued that assessment drives the ways in which students learn and think, the content of the curriculum, and how teachers teach. Therefore, among other things, assessment links Biggs's and Sternberg's theories--it has a common impact on both learning approaches and thinking styles. Learning approaches and thinking styles as implemented at a given time may vary as a function of the assessment measures used. For example, if student performance is measured by a multiple-choice test, students may tend to take a surface approach to learning and use executive, conservative, internal, and local thinking styles. In contrast, if student performance is assessed by a group project, it is more likely that students will take a deep approach to learning and use such thinking styles as judicial, legislative, liberal, and external. An awareness of the interrelations between the two theories also can be helpful in teachers' efforts toward the enhancement of effective learning. Each of the learning approaches discussed by Biggs (1987, 1992), as mentioned earlier, contains two concepts, motivation and strategy. Students' learning motivations, learning strategies, and thinking styles are intertwined. Given this intertwining, teachers can facilitate the students' efforts to be flexible in their implementations of styles. For example, teachers may wish to motivate students to take a deep approach to learning more important material, but a surface approach to learning less important material. The significant positive correlations manifested in this study indicate that when students are deeply motivated to learn, they will think critically and creatively, and certainly, also will use a deep strategy in performing their learning tasks. Alternatively, teachers may allow for different thinking styles by using the aforementioned strategies, such as teaching about styles, instructing in different ways, and using varied assessment tools. TABLE 1 Study Process Questionnaire Subscales: Means, Standard Deviations, and Alpha Coefficients Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G - Subscale Items M HK M NJ SD HK SD NJ alpha HK H - alpha NJ A B C F D G E H Achieving Motive 3, 9, 15, 21, 27, 33, 39 3.04 .73 3.51 .78 .74 .72 Achieving Strategy 6, 12, 18, 24, 30, 36, 42 3.16 .66 3.49 .80 .69 .73 Deep Motive 2, 8, 14, 20, 26, 32, 38 3.26 .64 3.42 .65 .58 .64 Deep Strategy 5, 11, 17, 23, 29, 35, 41 3.33 .62 3.60 .75 .58 .74 Surface Motive 1, 7, 13, 19, 25, 31, 37 2.96 .73 2.80 .68 .66 .67 Surface Strategy 4, 10, 16, 22, 28, 34, 40 2.74 .58 2.47 .70 .60 .64 Note. HK = Hong Kong. NJ = Nanjing. Hong Kong n = 854. Nanjing n = 215. TABLE 2 Thinking Styles Inventory Subscales: Means, Standard Deviations, and Alpha Coefficients Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G H - Subscale Items M HK M NJ SD HK SD NJ alpha HK alpha NJ A B C F D G E H Legislative 5, 10, 14, 32, 49 4.91 .86 5.45 .71 .81 .65 Executive 8, 11, 12, 31, 39 4.91 .97 4.68 .66 .79 .61 Judicial 20, 23, 42, 51, 57 4.67 .92 4.87 .72 .85 .62 Global 7, 18, 38, 48, 61 4.28 .95 4.59 .58 .76 .60 Local 1, 6, 24, 44, 62 4.35 .90 4.35 .48 .72 .49 Liberal 45, 53, 58, 64, 65 4.20 1.0 4.74 .80 .94 .81 Conservative 13, 22, 26, 28, 36 4.50 3.96 .86 1.12 .72 .74 Hierarchical 4, 19, 25, 33, 56 4.87 1.06 5.01 .76 .88 .78 Monarchic 2, 43, 50, 54, 60 4.59 .86 4.98 .48 .76 .43 Oligarchic 27, 29, 30, 52, 59 4.57 .95 4.62 .64 .80 .66 Anarchic 16, 21, 35, 40, 47 4.45 .76 4.48 .44 .73 .13 Internal 9, 15, 37, 55, 63 4.35 .97 4.71 .77 .99 .67 External 3, 17, 34, 41, 46 4.83 1.06 5.12 .74 .89 .72 Note. HK = Hong Kong. NJ = Nanjing. Hong Kong n = 854. Nanjing n = 215. TABLE 3 Oblimin-Rotated Two-Factor Model for the Study Process Questionnaire Legend for Chart: A B C D E - Subscale/Item Hong Kong Factor 1 Hong Kong Factor 2 Nanjing Factor 1 Nanjing Factor 2 A Surface Motive Surface Strategy Deep Motive Deep Strategy Achieving Motive Achieving Strategy % of variance Cumulative % Eigenvalue B C D E -.04 -.10 .88 .90 .32 .74 48.2 48.2 2.89 .89 .89 -.07 -.07 .67 .15 24.7 72.9 1.48 -.12 -.10 .81 .82 .50 .77 36.5 36.5 2.19 .86 .86 -.04 -.16 .60 .04 31.0 67.5 1.86 Note. Hong Kong n = 854. Nanjing n = 215. TABLE 4 Interscale Pearson Correlation Matrix for 13 Subscales of the Thinking Styles Inventory Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G H I J - Subscale 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 K L M N - 10 11 12 13 A B 1. Legislative 2. Executive 3. Judicial 4. Global 5. Local 6. Liberal 7. Conservative C I D J E K F L G M -.09 .22 .34 .10 .24 .02 .06 .22 .50 .54 -.14 -.10 .23 .05 .31 .04 .31 .34 .22 -.20 -.01 .66 .29 .20 .37 .27 .18 .11 .13 .29 .51 .20 -.11 .22 .25 .18 .34 .24 .11 -.35 .13 .20 .24 .06 .03 .34 .15 .32 .17 .08 .22 .24 .09 .04 .30 .17 .03 .33 .52 .19 .37 .05 .26 .33 .37 -.42 .07 .65 -.05 .05 .21 .16 .29 .33 .12 -.10 -.08 .15 .33 .44 .36 .22 .42 .23 H N 8. Hierarchical .30 .32 .45 .23 .18 .22 .41 .33 .28 .26 .20 .14 9. Monarchic .40 .42 .30 .28 .34 .31 .35 .31 .25 .20 .41 .13 10. Oligarchic .21 .41 .18 .18 .35 .26 .33 .30 .14 -.06 .44 .37 11. Anarchic .35 .25 .34 .33 .31 .24 .33 .39 .34 .27 .25 .23 12. Internal .64 .21 .23 .34 .34 .28 .05 .27 .23 .40 .16 -.28 13. External .07 .21 .36 .35 .15 .20 .31 .28 .35 .21 -.23 .07 Note. Numbers above the diagonal are for the Nanjing sample. Numbers below the diagonal are for the Hong Kong sample. Hong Kong n = 854. Nanjing n = 215. TABLE 5 Pearson Correlation Matrix for the Subscales in the Study Process Questionnaire and Thinking Styles Inventory Legend for Chart: A B C D E F - Subscale SM HK SM NJ DM HK DM NJ AM HK G H I J K L M - AM SS SS DS DS AS AS NJ HK NJ HK NJ HK NJ A B F J C G K D H L E I M Legislative .05 .21(*) .26(*) -.09 .20 .33(*) .28(*) -.02 .10(*) .24(*) -.12 .02 Executive .24(*) .20(*) .18(*) .23(*) .20 -.04 .17(*) .26(*) .20(*) .08 .34(*) .20 -.00 .17(*) .38(*) -.02 .15 .49(*) .40(*) -.13(*) .26(*) .31(*) -.11 .18 Judicial Global .17(*) .18(*) .25(*) .05 .13 .13 .24(*) .13(*) .13(*) .04 .02 .00 Local .17(*) .21(*) .26(*) .18 .14 .10 .24(*) .17(*) .30(*) .15 .23(*) .23(*) Liberal .07 .20(*) .37(*) -.15 .08 .53(*) .37(*) -.03 .19(*) .31(*) -.31(*) .18 Conservative .25(*) .19(*) .07 .36(*) .19 -.16 .07 .36(*) .19(*) .00 .47(*) .07 Hierarchical -.01 .13(*) .36(*) -.13 .23(*) .39(*) .32(*) -.04 .39(*) .35(*) -.14 .49(*) Monarchic .22(*) .26(*) .24(*) .20 .30(*) .21 .28(*) .22(*) .29(*) .23(*) .18 .31(*) Oligarchic .18(*) .10 .13(*) .23(*) .24(*) .14 .13(*) .19(*) .12(*) .23(*) .23 .25(*) Anarchic .04 .10 .24(*) .14 .28(*) .27(*) .25(*) .08 .18(*) .26(*) .08 .30(*) Internal .07 .24(*) .20(*) -.02 .36(*) .30(*) .24(*) .05 .07 .13 -.02 .10 External .02 -.02 .22(*) .07 .02 .24(*) -.06 .09 -.02 .20(*) .02 .22(*) Note. HK = Hong Kong. NJ = Nanjing. Hong Kong n = 854. Nanjing n = 215. SM = Surface Motivation. DM = Deep Motivation. AM = Achieving Motivation. SS = Surface Strategy. DS = Deep Strategy. AS = Achieving Strategy. (*) p < .01. REFERENCES Albaili, M. A. (1995). An Arabic version of the Study Process Questionnaire: Reliability and validity. Psychological Reports, 77, 10831089. Beishuizen, J., Stoutjesdijk, E., & Van-Putten, K. (1994). Studying textbooks: Effects of learning styles, study tasks, and instructions. Learning and Instruction, 4, 151-174. Biggs, J. B. (1987). Student approaches to learning and studying. Hawthorn, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research. Biggs, J. B. (1992). Why and how do Hong Kong students learn? Using the Learning and Study Process Questionnaires (Education Paper No. 14). Hong Kong: Faculty of Education, The University of Hong Kong. Biggs, J. B. (1995). Assessment of learning. In J. Biggs & D. Watkins (Eds.), Classroom learning: Educational psychology for the Asian teacher (pp. 167-191). Singapore: Prentice-Hall. Bolen, L. M., Wurm, T. R., & Hall, C. W. (1994). Factorial structure of the Study Process Questionnaire. Psychological Reports, 75, 1235-1241. Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 1, 245-276. Dunkin, M. J., & Biddle, B. J. (1974). The study of teaching. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Entwistle, N. (1981). Styles of learning and teaching. New York: Wiley. Entwistle, N. (1988). Motivation and learning strategies. Educational and Child Psychology, 59, 326-339. Entwistle, N. (1990). Student learning and classroom environment. London: Falmer. Entwistle, N., Koseki, B., & Politt, A. (1987). Measuring styles of learning and motivation. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 2, 183-203. Entwistle, N., & Marton, F. (1994). Knowledge objects: Understandings constituted through intensive academic study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 161-178. Gregorc, A. F. (1985). Inside styles: Beyond the basics. Maynard, MA: Gabriel Systems. Grigorenko, E., & Sternberg, R. J. (1995). Thinking styles. In D. Saklosfske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 205-229). New York: Plenum. Grigorenko, E., & Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Styles of thinking, abilities, and academic performance. Exceptional Children, 63, 295-312. Harper, G., & Kember, D. (1989). Interpretation of factor analyses from the Approaches to Studying Inventory. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 59, 66-74. Holland, J. L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of careers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Jung, C. (1923). Psychological types. New York: Harcourt Brace. Kember, D., & Gow, D. (1990). Cultural specificity of approaches to study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 60, 356-363. Kagan, J. (1976). Commentary on reflective and impulsive children: Strategies of information processing underlying differences in problem solving. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41(No. 5, Serial No. 168). Kogan, N. (1983). Stylistic variation in childhood and adolescence: Creativity, metaphor, and cognitive styles. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive development (4th ed., pp. 630-706). New York: Wiley. Marton, F., & Booth, S. A. (1997). Learning and awareness. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Murray-Harvey, R. (1994). Learning styles and approaches to learning: Distinguishing between concept and instruments. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 373-388. Myers, I. B. (1980). Gifts differing. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Myers, I. B., & McCaulley, M. H. (1988). Manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Niles, F. S. (1995). Cultural differences in learning motivation and learning strategies: A comparison of overseas and Australian students at an Australian university. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 19, 369-385. O'Neil, M. J., & Child, D. (1984). Biggs' SPQ: A British study of its internal structure. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 228-234. Pask, G. (1976). Styles and strategies of learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 128-148. Ramsden, P., & Entwistle, N. J. (1981). Effects of academic departments on students' approaches to studying. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 51, 368-383. Renzulli, J. S., & Smith, L. H. (1978). Learning Styles Inventory. Mansfield Center, CT: Creative Learning Press. Rose, R. J., Hall, C. W., Bolen, L. M., & Webster, R. E. (1996). Locus of control and college students' approaches to learning. Psychological Reports, 79, 163-171. Sadler-Smith, E., & Tsang, F. (1998). A comparative study of approaches to studying in Hong Kong and the United Kingdom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 81-93. Saracho, O. N. (1993). Sociocultural perspectives in the cognitive styles of young students and teachers. Early Child Development and Care, 84, 1-17. Sternberg, R. J. (1988). Mental self-government: A theory of intellectual styles and their development. Human Development, 31, 197-224. Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of mind: Conceptions of the nature of intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. Sternberg, R. J. (1994). Thinking styles: Theory and assessment at the interface between intelligence and personality. In R. J. Sternberg & P. Ruzgis (Eds.), Personality and intelligence (pp. 169-187). New York: Cambridge University Press. Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. New York: Cambridge University Press. Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. (1995). Styles of thinking in the school. European Journal for High Ability, 6, 201-219. Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). Thinking Styles Inventory (unpublished test). New Haven, CT: Yale University. Watkins, D. (1982). Identifying the study process dimensions of Australian university students. Australian Journal of Education, 26, 76-85. Watkins, D., & Akande, A. (1992). Assessing the approaches to learning of Nigerian students. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 17, 1120. Watkins, D., & Dahlin, B. (1997). Assessing study approaches in Sweden. Psychological Reports, 81, 131-136. Watkins, D., & Hattie, J. (1981). The learning processes of Australian university students: Investigations of contextual and personological factors. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 51, 384-393. Watkins, D., & Murphy, J. (1994). Modifying the Study Process Questionnaire for students learning English as a second language: Comparisons of reliability and factor structure. Psychological Reports, 74, 1023-1026. Watkins, D., & Regmi, M. (1990). An investigation of the approach to learning of Nepalese tertiary students. Higher Education, 20, 459-469. Wilson, K. L., Smart, R. M., & Watson, R. J. (1996). Gender differences in approaches to learning in first year psychology students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 59-71. Witkin, H. A. (1978). Cognitive styles in personal and cultural adaptation: The 1977 Heinz Werner lectures. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. Wong, N. Y., Lin, W. Y., & Watkins, D. (1996). Cross-cultural validation of models of student learning. Educational Psychology, 16, 317-327. Zhang, L. F. (1999). Further cross-cultural validation of the theory of mental self-government. The Journal of Psychology, 133, 165-181. Zhang, L. F., & Sachs, J. (1997). Assessing thinking styles in the theory of mental self-government: A Hong Kong validity study. Psychological Reports, 81, 915-928. Zhang, L. F., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Thinking styles, abilities, and academic achievement among Hong Kong university students. Educational Research Journal, 13, 41-62. Received July 6, 1999 Research for this project was supported in part by the Committee on Research and Conference Grants as administered by The University of Hong Kong. Preparation of this article was supported in part under the Javits Act Program (Grant No. R206R50001) as administered by the Office of Educational Research and Improvement, U.S. Department of Education. Grantees undertaking such projects are encouraged to express freely their professional judgment. This article, therefore, does not necessarily represent the position or policies of the Office of Educational Research and Improvement or the U.S. Department of Education, and no official endorsement should be inferred. Address correspondence to Li-fang Zhang, Department of Education, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong; lfzhang@hkucc.hku.hk (email). ~~~~~~~~ By Li-Fang Zhang, Department of Education The University of Hong Kong and Robert J. Sternberg, Department of Psychology Yale University ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Journal of Psychology is the property of Heldref Publications and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Journal of Psychology, Sep2000, Vol. 134 Issue 5, p469, 21p. Item Number: 3644717 Result 16 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: The psychometric properties of the Learning Style Inventory and the Learning Style Questionnaire: Two normative measures of learning styles. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; LEARNING ability Source: South African Journal of Psychology, Jun2000, Vol. 30 Issue 2, p44, 9p, 14 charts, 1 diagram Author(s): Pickworth[*], Glynis E.; Schoeman, Willem J. THE PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF THE LEARNING STYLE INVENTORY AND THE LEARNING STYLE QUESTIONNAIRE: TWO NORMATIVE MEASURES OF LEARNING STYLES David Kolb has provided a detailed, useful and widely accepted theory of experiential learning and learning styles. He developed the Learning Styles Inventory (LSI) to assess four learning abilities and four learning styles. Kolb's work is viewed favourably for establishing the existence of individual differences in learning styles, but the major criticism against his work is focused on his method of measuring learning styles and more specifically on the psychometric properties of the LSI. The LSI is an ipsative instrument and the limitations placed on the statistical analysis of data of ipsative measures makes it inappropriate for reliability and validity evaluation of the instrument. In this study the psychometric properties of two normative measures of learning styles, a normative version of the LSI (referred to as the LSI-Likert) and the Learning Style Questionnaire (LSQ), are investigated. A review of the literature on the LSI is presented and the development of normative versions of the LSI is reviewed. First-year university students registered for either a science or human sciences degree completed the two normative instruments. The internal reliability of the four learning ability scales was determined using alpha coefficient. The internal reliability of the LSI-Likert and LSQ was found to be relatively high. The presence of a response bias for both instruments was suspected. It appeared that the LSI-Likert was more successful than the LSQ in differentiating learning abilities and styles in the sample used. Item factor analysis demonstrated two bipolar factors in line with Kolb's theory for the LSQ. The four-factor solution for the LSI-Likert produced four factors which to some extent represented the four learning abilities. * To whom correspondence should be addressed. Kolb's theory of experiential learning The experiential learning movement emerged through the theories and work of John Dewey, Kurt Lewin and Jean Piaget. The work of these three theorists form the foundation of Kolb's theory of experiential learning (Hickcox, 1990). According to Kolb (1984) learning is a continuous process through which knowledge is derived from, and modified through, testing out the experiences of the learner. Kolb also postulated that learning requires the resolution of conflicts between dialectically opposed modes of adaptation to the world. On the prehension (perceiving) dimension the process of apprehension (concrete experience) opposes the process of comprehension (abstract conceptualisation). Kolb referred in this regard to research on brain hemisphere dominance that provides evidence that "there are two distinct, coequal, and dialectically opposed ways of understanding the world" (Kolb, 1984, p. 48), the right-brain mode corresponding to apprehension and the left-brain mode corresponding to comprehension. On the transformation (processing) dimension the process of intention (reflective observation) opposes the process of extension (active experimentation). Kolb stated that Carl Jung's concepts of introversion (intention) and extraversion (extension) best describe this transformation dimension. Learning results from the resolution of conflicts between involvement in new experiences versus conceptualising, and between acting versus reflecting. The process of experiential learning is described as a four-stage cycle involving four learning abilities: Concrete Experience (CE), Reflective Observation (RO), Abstract Conceptualization (AC), and Active Experimentation (AE) (see Figure 1). It is theorized that one learns best by going through the CE, RO, AC, AE sequence of the cycle and that people learn more effectively as they develop learning abilities in their areas of weakness. In the experiential learning process concrete experience is followed by observation and reflection, leading to the formation of abstract concepts that result in hypotheses to be tested in future actions and this in turn leads to new experiences. The learning cycle is continuously recurring and is directed by individual needs and goals (Kolb, Rubin & McIntyre, 1984). The learning abilities are represented on a two dimensional learning styles plane by two bipolar dimensions, one a vertical axis running from CE to AC, and the other a horizontal axis running from AE to RO. The four quadrants formed by the intersection of the two bipolar axes represent the four learning styles derived from the combination of two preferred learning abilities: Diverger (CE and RO), Assimilator (AC and RO), Converger (AC and AE), and Accommodator (CE and AE). (See Figure 1). Kolb (1984) took a contextualist view of learning styles and stated that "psychological types or styles are not fixed traits but stable states" (p. 63). These stable states or enduring patterns of individual human behaviour arise from consistent patterns of transactions between the person and the environment. However, an individual can adjust their learning style according to the demands of the task at hand. Sugarman (1985) pointed out that as Kolb's theory combines a theory of learning and a theory of learning styles there are at least three components that must be addressed in an evaluation of his work: (a) establishing the existence of individual differences in learning styles; (b) effectively measuring these differences, if they are found to exist; and (c) validating the cyclical model of learning. Kolb's work is viewed favourably for aspects (a) and (c), but the major criticism against his work is focused on his method of measuring learning styles and more specifically on the psychometric properties of the Learning Styles inventory (LSI). in the following section literature reporting on the psychometric properties of the LSI is summarized. Assessment of learning styles: The Learning Styles Inventory (LSI) The LSI was developed by Kolb and takes the form of a self-description, self-scoring test that aims to help an individual to identify their relative emphasis on the four learning abilities within the learning cycle (CE, RO, AC and AE) as well as their predominant learning style (Diverger, Assimilator, Converger or Accommodator). The LSI-1976 According to Hickcox (1990) Kolb published the first version of the LSI in 1971. However, the inventory is generally referred to in the literature as the 1976 version. This version will be referred to as the LSI-1976. The LSI-1976 consists of nine sets of words, each set consisting of four words. The four words, each representing one of the four learning abilities, are presented in the same order (CE, RO, AC, AE) throughout so that the words associated with each of the four learning abilities are grouped in columns to facilitate scoring for the self-scoring format of the inventory. A respondent rank orders the four words in each of the nine sets according to how well he/she perceives each word as describing his/her individual learning style. The rankings for only six of the nine items, that is, for only 24 of the 36 words contribute to the scores for the four learning abilities CE, RO, AC and AE. The other twelve words serve as distracters. Two combination scores AC-CE (that indicates the extent to which an individual emphasizes abstractness over concreteness) and AE-RO (the extent that an individual emphasizes action over reflection) are calculated. By plotting these scores on the vertical and horizontal axes respectively, the respondent is positioned in one of the four quadrants representing one of the four learning styles. Due to the ranking format the instrument is an ipsative measure (Kerlinger, 1973). Research results pertaining to the psychometric properties of the LSI-1976 are summarized in Table 1. The LSI-1985 A revised version of the LSI was published in 1985. This version of the LSI will be referred to as the LSI-1985. The format was changed and the LSI1985 consists of 12 sentence-completion items and therefore has more items than the LSI-1976. Each sentence has four word endings corresponding to the four learning abilities. As for the LSI-1976 the four words are presented in the same order (CE, RO, AC, AE) throughout to facilitate the scoring of the self-scoring inventory. A respondent rank orders the four words for each sentence or item. The ratings for all 12 words are summed for each of the learning abilities CE, RO, AC, AE. These scores are used to calculate the combination scores AC-CE and AE-RO and by plotting these two scores on the corresponding bipolar axes, the respondent is assigned to one of the four quadrants, each representing one of the four learning styles. Kolb thus increased the number of items and placed the words in the context of a sentence, but remained committed to the ranking format and the instrument remains an ipsative measure. Research results pertaining to the psychometric properties of the LSI-1985 are summarized in Table 2. The LSI IIA In 1993 a further revised version of the LSI was published. The instrument is called the LSI IIA and the following information is given in the publishers McBer & Company's catalogue: "The LSI IIA has a revised questionnaire format and scoring key. The twelve-question inventory now has scrambled sentence endings and new scoring instructions that have proved to have high test-retest reliability in recent studies." (p. 11). The same 12 items are presented in the same order, but the four word endings for each item have been randomized. Kolb remains committed to the ranking format and the instrument remains an ipsative measure. To date no literature has been found relating to the LSI IIA. Problems associated with ipsative measures Hicks (1970) defined an ipsative measure as follows: "A format in which respondents compare or rank items will always yield purely ipsative scores if respondents rank all alternatives per item, if all these rankings are scored, and if alternatives representing all assessed variables are compared with each other and presented for preferential choice by the respondent." (p. 170). According to this definition the LSI-1985 and LSI IIA are purely ipsative instruments. The LSI-1976 is a partially ipsative instrument as not all alternatives ranked by respondents are scored. Kolb remained committed to the ranking format of the inventory thus making it an ipsative measure. An ipsative measure is designed to measure withinindividual differences, and this creates difficulties when researchers try to make between-subjects analyses. Statistically the ipsative measure results in a between-subjects sum of squares of zero and one individual's preferences cannot be compared with another's (Merritt & Marshall, 1984). Cornwell and Dunlap (1994) stated that ipsative scores cannot be factored and that correlation-based analysis of ipsative data produced uninterpretable and invalid results. As ipsative scores contain only categorical information across individuals multinomial statistical techniques are appropriate. Instead of using the sum of the rank ordered ipsative scores, Cornwell and Dunlap suggested rank ordering the summed responses across the four learning modes for each individual and then applying multinomial techniques to this categorical data. Cornwell, Manfredo and Dunlap (1991) recommended the use of non-ipsative scores for evaluating the construct validity of the LSI. The minimum requirement for an instrument's scores to be amenable to construct interpretations is that the instrument must yield internally consistent scores (Tenopyr, 1988). Tenopyr states that the internal consistencies of scales of ipsative inventories are interdependent and that there is a possibility for artifactual internal consistency to be generated in such inventories. This places limitations on the usefulness of reliability data for ipsative inventories and such instruments are not suitable for psychometric evaluation and should not be used for making important decisions concerning individuals. ipsative scores are also not suitable for theory building (Hicks, 1970). The usual statistics are not applicable to ipsative measures because of the lack of independence and negative correlations among items and analysis of correlations, as in factor analysis, could be seriously distorted by the negative correlations (Kerlinger, 1973). Many of the studies tabled in this article have treated ipsative data normatively and the results of such studies are of little value. Although an ipsative measure is designed to measure intra-individual differences, the limitations placed on the statistical analysis' of data of ipsative measures makes it inappropriate for reliability and validity evaluation of the instrument. Normative versions of the LSI From previous studies using normative forms of the LSI-1976 Marshall and Merritt (1986) concluded that a semantic differential format could be used to develop a reliable and valid normative assessment instrument to assess individual's preferences for ways of learning as proposed by Kolb. They developed the Learning Style Questionnaire (LSQ). In the experimental phase 100 semantic differential word pairs were compiled, with 25 word pairs for each of the four scales. A five-point scale was used by respondents to rate the consistency with which the opposing words characterized their particular learning style. This experimental form of the LSQ was administered to 543 university students from randomly selected classes at two universities. Thirty-seven different majors were represented. About three-fourths of the subjects were under 23 years of age; two-thirds were female and about two-thirds had completed at least two years of college. The 100 items were analyzed and 40 items were selected for the final instrument, 10 items for each of the four scales (CE, RO, AC, AE). The internal consistency reliabilities based on alpha coefficient for the finalized 40-item LSQ were: CE = .78, RO = .86, AC = .85, AE = .88, CE-AC = .90 and RO-AC = .93. Least squares factor analysis of the items was used to examine the construct validity of the instrument. All 40 items loaded on bipolar factors in accordance with Kolb's proposed learning abilities and styles. The authors concluded that the reliability estimates for both bipolar dimensions were very high and that the construct validity for these dimensions had been demonstrated. They recommended that the instrument be used to determine individual learning styles as well as for research purposes. Romero, Tepper and Tetrault (1992) developed a normative, two-dimensional instrument to measure learning style. Rather than construct an instrument that assesses the four learning abilities, the authors constructed an instrument that assessed the two dimensions concreteness/abstractness and reflection/action directly. The instrument consists of 14 pairs of selfdescriptive anchor statements, each pair on a six-point Likert scale. Seven bipolar items assess concreteness versus abstractness, and seven bipolar items assess reflection versus action. The instrument was administered to two independent samples. The one sample consisted of 507 undergraduate students in the fields of liberal arts, business and engineering. The average age was about 21 years and 53% were male. The instrument was administered once to this sample. The second sample consisted of 153 MBA students and the instrument was administered twice with a six week interval. The average age was 28 years and 65% were male. The internal consistency alpha coefficient for the concreteness/abstract scale was .84 for sample I and .78 for sample 2. The alpha coefficient for the reflective/action scale was .86 for sample 1 and .80 for sample 2. The test-retest stability for sample 2 was .75 for the concreteness/abstract scale and .73 for the reflection/action scale. The authors reported that the internal consistency and test-retest stability were acceptable. The two dimensional structure of the instrument was confirmed by factor analysis of both samples using LISREL. Validity support was obtained by comparing student majors with learning style for sample 1. Geiger, Boyle and Pinto (1993) constructed a normative version of the LSI1985 that was scored on n seven-point Likert scale consisting of 48 (12 sentence items X four word endings) separate items randomly presented. The standard LSI-1985 and the normative versions were administered to 455 business administration students (first, second and third year students). The age range was from 18 to 47 years (mean age = 21.4 years) and 281 were male and 174 female. Alpha coefficient internal consistency reliability measures for the ipsative version were as follows: CE = .83, RO = .81, AC = .85 and AE = .84. Alpha coefficient reliabilities for the normative version were as follows: CE = .83, RO = .77, AC = .86 and AE =.84. Correlations of the four scale scores were used to determine the equivalence of the ipsative and normative versions. Correlations ranged from .368 to .526 indicating a moderate amount of agreement. Adjusted scale correlations ranged from .466 to .615 with three of the four coefficients exceeding .50. Separate factor analyses were performed on the two versions. For the ipsative version two strong bipolar dimensions were identified running from CE to RO and from AE to AC. These dimensions are not congruent with Kolb's theorized bipolar dimensions. Analysis of the normative version did not produce any bipolar dimensions, but strong support for the four separate learning abilities was obtained. The comments made in the previous section on the inappropriate use of the statistical analysis of data of ipsative measures pertain to these findings. Method Measures Due to the problems relating to ipsative measures described previously it was decided to investigate the psychometric properties of two normative measures of learning style. The Learning Style Questionnaire (LSQ) developed by Marshall and Merritt (1986), described previously, and the Likert-scale form of the LSI-1985 developed by Geiger et al. (1993), described previously, were used. The five-point Likert-scale version of the LSI used in this study will be referred to as the LSI-Likert. These two instruments were obtained from their American authors and were available only in English. This study can be seen as a pilot study to start investigating the reliability and construct validity of the two instruments in a South African population. Sample First-year students registered for full-time courses presented in English at the University of Pretoria in the fields of science (BSc) and the human sciences (BA) participated in the study. A total of 464 students were tested at the beginning of the 1995 and 1996 academic years. Due to some incomplete answer sheets 419 answer sheets could be scored for the LSILikert and 415 for the LSQ. In scoring the four ability scales (CE, RO, AC, AE) missing or ambiguous responses were substituted with the group average score for an item, for a maximum of two items per questionnaire. For the LSI-Likert the group average score was substituted for one item in 43 cases and for two items in 9 cases. For the LSQ the group average score was substituted for one item in 39 cases and for two items in 4 cases. The allocation of a learning style to a subject is determined by the composite scores ACCE and AE-RO. If a zero score was obtained for either of these composite scores a subject was not allocated a learning style. There was a higher proportion of females than males in the sample with approximately two-thirds females. Regarding home language, 35% of the students were English first language speakers and for the rest English was a second language. The african cultural group comprised 50% of the sample and the white group 38%. The rest were from the Coloured, Indian and Asian cultural groups. The two fields of study (BSc and BA) were fairly evenly represented. The BSc field of study represented 46% of the sample and comprised students studying mainly for degrees in the biological and agricultural sciences, and engineering fields. The BA field of study represented 54% of the sample and comprised first-year Psychology students. The order in which the LSI-Likert and LSQ was completed was varied with 51% of the students completing the LSI-Liken followed by the LSQ, and 49% completing the LSQ followed by the LSI-Likert. Hotteling's T test indicated that the four scales for the LSI-Likert and LSQ did not have equal vector of means for these two test groups and it was concluded that the order in which the LSI-Likert and the LSQ were completed did not affect the scores obtained for the two instruments (Pickworth, 1997). Results and discussion Item analysis and internal reliability Item analysis was done for the LSI-Likert and the LSQ using the ITEMAN Conventional Item analysis Program (Assessment Systems Corporation, 1993). Intercorrelations and alpha coefficient reliabilities for the four scales of the two instruments were also calculated using the ITEMAN program. The LSI-Likert item-scale correlations for the CE scale ranged from .29 to .61 (mean = .47), for the RO scale from .30 to .59 (mean = .46), for the AC scale from .34 to .61 (mean = .52), and for the AE scale from .33 to .62 (mean = .49). Intercorrelations for the four scales are given in Table 3 and the alpha coefficients in Table 4. The LSQ item-scale correlations for the CE scale ranged from .47 to .69 (mean = .58), for the RO scale from .48 to .72 (mean = .59), for the AC scale from .41 to .68 (mean = .56), and for the AE scale from .44 to .70 (mean = .58). Intercorrelations for the four scales are given in Table 3 and the alpha coefficients in Table 4. Response bias A five-point Likert scale was used for the LSI-Likert. Options 1 and 2 (Not at all like me and Somewhat unlike me) were endorsed at most by 35% of respondents. For 28 out of the 48 items options 1 and 2 where used by 10% or less of respondents. Relatively high item means, ranging from 3.0 to 4.7, reflect this. This could indicate a response bias. Each item of the LSQ consists of a word pair on a five-point semantic differential scale. Each of the two words in an item represent opposite learning abilities. In the list of word pairs below the item number is given and the word highlighted was endorsed by less than 20% of the respondents using one of the two response options Generally (Most of the time) or Over half the time: The Abstract Conceptualisation scale 15 consider impulsive 17 reason hunch 26 careful emotional 27 logical sentimental 29 thinking instinctive 34 resolving feeling 36 intellectual emotional The Concrete Experience scale 4 sensing thinking 5 premonition reason 12 perceptual intellectual 18 impulsive planning 25 intuitive reasoning 30 hunch logical The Active Experimentation scale 6 active reserved 23 involved distant 39 solve reflect 40 exercise view The Reflective Observation scale 31 passive active 37 reflective productive The above could reflect a response bias in which "logical" (Abstract Conceptualization) words are favoured over "feelings" (Concrete Experience) words, and "active" (Active Experimentation) words are favoured over "passive/reflective" (Reflective Observation) words. The "logical" and "active" words may be perceived to be more socially correct in a learning context. It must also be remembered that the majority of the students are not English first language speakers and may have experienced difficulty with the meanings of the words. In some cases the words more commonly endorsed may be words they are more familiar with. Learning style frequency The distributions of learning styles for the BSc and BA groups as measured by the LSI-Likert and LSQ were calculated using the FREQ procedure of the SAS statistical package (SAS Institute Inc., 1990). This procedure was also used to calculate the Chi-square test of significance for the frequencies. The frequency distributions of learning styles as measured by the LSILikert are given in Table 5 and as measured by the LSQ in Table 6. For the LSI-Likert the Chi-square statistic had a value of 27.49 with three degrees of freedom which was significant at the 5% level of significance. There was thus a strong association between field of study and learning style as measured by the LSI-Likert. There were more Divergers in the BA group, more Convergers in the BSc group and more Accommodators in the BA group. Assimilators were fairly equally represented in the BSc and BA groups. Except that one would expect more Assimilators in the BSc than the BA group, these results are in line with the descriptions of the learning styles (Kolb, 1984) and thus provide some evidence of construct validity for the learning style constructs for the LSI-Likert. For the LSQ the Chi-square statistic had a value of 7.556 with three degrees of freedom which is not significant at the 5% level of significance. There is thus no strong association between field of study and learning style as measured by the LSQ. Item factor analysis Factor analysis of the items of the LSI-Likert and the LSQ was performed using the principal factor method to extract factors, followed by a direct quartimin (oblique) rotation of factors. The BMDP4M factor analysis statistical package (BMDP Statistical Software Inc., 1993) was used. The factor loadings for the two-factor and four-factor solution for the LSILikert are reported in Tables 7.1 and 7.2. The factor loadings for the twofactor and four-factor solution for the LSQ are reported in Tables 8.1 and 8.2. The two-factor solution was expected to yield the CE -- AC and AE -RO bipolar axes and the four-factor solution was expected to yield the four learning abilities (CE, RO, AC, AE). The first factor for the two-factor solution for the LSI-Likert (see Table 7.1) combines mainly AC and RO items and would appear to represent the Assimilator learning style. The second factor combines mainly CE and AE items and would appear to represent the Accommodator learning style. The anticipated bipolar axes did not emerge and the LSI therefore does not support the bipolar axes theorized by Kolb. For the four-factor solution of the LSI-Likert (see Table 7.2) the four factors appear to represent to some extent each of the four learning abilities with the first factor representing AC, the second AE, the third CE and the fourth RO. However, the first two factors combine items representing other learning abilities. Two bipolar factors, AE -- RO and AC -- CE, emerge for the two-factor solution for the item factor analysis for the LSQ (see Table 8.1). The LSQ therefore supports the bipolar axes as theorized by Kolb. For the four-factor solution of the LSQ (see Table 8.2) the first factor is bipolar representing the AE -- RO axis and supports the construct validity of the AE and RO learning abilities. The second factor appears to represent the CE learning ability, the third factor the AC learning ability and the fourth factor incorporates RO, AC and one CE item. The four learning abilities are therefore supported for the LSQ. Both the delineation of the two bipolar AE -- RO and AC -- CE axes, as well as the four learning abilities (CE, RO, AC, AE) reflects the careful developmental work done by Marshall and Merritt to produce an instrument that measures the constructs proposed by Kolb in his experiential learning theory. Conclusion The results indicate that the internal reliability of the LSQ is somewhat higher than for the LSI-Likert (see Table 4). The presence of a response bias on both instruments is suspected, it would appear that the LSI-Likert was more successful than the LSQ in differentiating learning abilities and styles in the sample used. Frequency distributions of learning style demonstrated more differentiated patterns for the LSI-Likert than for the LSQ (see Tables 5 and 6). The Chi-square statistic was significant only for the LSI-Likert. Except that one would expect a higher percentage of Assimilators in the BSc group than the BA group, the distributions of learning styles as measured by the LSILikert were in accordance with Kolb's theory. Item factor analysis of the LSI-Likert and the LSQ demonstrates that the LSQ produces two bipolar factors in line with Kolb's proposed theoretical constructs whereas the LSI-Likert did not (see Tables 7.1 and 8.1). The four-factor solution for the LSI-Likert and the LSQ produces evidence for the four learning abilities (see Tables 7.2 and 8.2). From the results of this study it would appear that the normative measures of learning style used in this study show promise for use in counselling, academic advising and for research purposes. This study did not make comparisons between gender, different cultural groups and English speaking versus non-English speakers. The effect of these variables needs to be investigated. The reliability and construct validity of the two instruments should also be investigated further. Acknowledgement The financial assistance of the Centre for Science Development of the Human Sciences Research Council towards this research is hereby acknowledged. Opinions expressed in this article and conclusions arrived at, are those of the authors and are not necessarily to be attributed to the Centre for Science Development of the Human Sciences Research Council. Table 1 Summary of reliability and validity findings for the LSI-1976 Legend for Chart: A B C D E - Author(s) N Reliability Bipolar theory Validity A B C D E Plovnick (1974) N = 27 Test-retest (3-4 month interval) Pearson product-moment correlations CE = .48 RO = .73 AC = .65 AE = .64 CE-AC = .61 RO-AE = .71 -Supported 1976 LSI Technical Manual (cited in Geller, 1979) N = 687 Internal consistency Spearman-Brown split-half correlations CE = .55 RO = .62 AC = .75 AE = .66 AC-CE = .74 AE-RO = .82 Test-retest (3, 6 and 7 month intervals) --N = 23 CE = .48 RO = .51 AC = .73 AE = .43 AC-CE = .51 AE-RO = .48 N = 18 CE = .46 RO = .34 AC = .64 AE = .50 AC-CE = .53 AE-RO = .51 N = 42 CE = .49 RO = .40 AC = .40 AE = .33 AC-CE = .30 AE-RO = .43 Freedman & Stumpf (1978, 1980) N = 412 Internal consistency: Alpha coefficient Moderate support Limited support N = 1179 CE = .40 RO = .57 AC = .70 AE = .47 Test-retest (five-week interval) Moderate support Limited support N = 101 Pearson product-moment correlations CE = .39 RO = .49 AC = .63 AE = .47 AC-CE = .58 AE-RO = .51 Whitney & Caplan (1978) N = 111 --Not supported Wunderlich & Gjerder (1978) N = 24 Test-retest (six-week interval) Correlations ranged from .44 to .72 -Not supported Geller (1979) N = 50 Test-retest (31 day interval) Pearson product-moment correlations CE = .56 RO = .52 AC = .59 AE = .61 AC-CE = .70 AE-RO = .55 --- West (1982) N = 42 --Not supported Fox (1984) N = 54 --Not supported Garvey, Bootman, Mc Ghan & Meredith (1984) N = 501 Internal consistency Alpha coefficient CE= .30 RO = .58 AC = .60 AE = .36 Spearman-Brown Prophecy Formula AC-CE = .72 AE-RO = .79 -Partial support Merritt & Marshall (1984) N = 187 Internal consistency: Alpha coefficient CE = .29 RO = .58 AC = .52 AE = .41 Supported -- Sims, Veres, Watson & Buckner (1986) N = 438 Internal consistency: Alpha coefficient CE = .48 RO = .58 AC = .52 AE = .23 -- -N = 309 Test-retest (3 applications at five-week intervals) N = 132 Zero-order correlation coefficients CE = .45 to .60 RO = .46 to .57 AC = .51 to .60 AE = .42 to .46 N = 739 -- Katz (1986) Supported Supported Wilson (1986) N = 102 Internal consistency Split-half correlation coefficients CE = .15 RO = .53 ac = .49 ae = .41 AC-CE = .45 AE-RO = .52 Not supported -- N = 51 Test-retest (six-week interval) CE = .40 RO = .77 AC = .63 AE = .40 AC-CE = .53 AE=RO = .61 Green, Snell & Parimanath (1990) N = 147 --Supported Lashinger (1990) -- --Partial support Review of experiential learning theory research in the nursing profession. Welman & Huysamen (1993) N = 573 Internal consistency: Alpha coefficient AC-CE = .63 AE-RO = .55 -Partial support Table 2 Summary of reliability and validity findings for the LSI-1985 Sims, Veres, Watson & Buckner (1986) N = 181 Internal consistency: Alpha coefficient CE = .76 RO = .84 AC = .85 AE = .82 --- N = 131 Test-retest (3 applications at five-week intervals) N = 94 Zero-order correlation coefficients CE = .24 to .44 RO = .39 to .66 AC = .42 to .50 AE = .56 to .62 Highhouse & Doverspike (1987) N = 111 --Partial support Veres, Sims & Shake (1987) N = 230 Internal consistency: Alpha coefficient CE = .82 RO = .85 AC = .83 AE = .84 --- N = 230 Test-retest (3 applications at three-week intervals) Zero-order correlation coefficients CE = .30 to .52 RO = .36 to .46 AC = .45 to .56 AE = .28 to .44 --- Atkinson (1988) N = 26 Test-retest (nine-day interval) Pearson product-moment coefficient CE = .57 RO = .40 AC = .54 AE = .59 AC-CE = .69 AE-RO = .24 --- Cornwell, Manfredo & Dunlap (1991) N = 317 -Not supported Not supported Ruble & Stout (1991) N = 231 Internal consistency: Alpha coefficient CE = .82 RO = .79 AC = .81 AE = .82 --- N - 139 Test-retest (five-week interval) Pearson product-moment correlations CE = .18 RO = .46 AC = .36 AE = .47 AC-CE = .22 AE-RO = .54 Veres, Sims & Locklear (1991) Random ordering of the four sentence endings of the LSI-1985 -- Internal consistency Mean Alpha coefficients for three applications --- N = 711 CE = .56 RO = .67 AC = .71 AE = .52 N = 1042 CE = .67 RO = .67 AC = .74 AE = .58 Test-retest (3 applications at eight-week intervals) Zero-order correlation coefficients N = 711 CE = .92 to .96 RO = .93 to .97 AC = .94 to .97 AE = .91 to .96 N = 1042 CE = .97 to .99 RO = .97 to .98 AC = .97 to .99 AE = .96 to .99 Geiger, Boyle & Pinto (1992) N = 718 -Not supported Not supported Cornwell & Manfredo (1994) N = 292 --Not supported Table 3 Intercorrelations for the scales of the LSI-Likert and the LSQ CE RO AC AE CE -.254 .335 RO .252 -.411 AC -.424 .033 -AE .077 -.521 .265 Correlations above the diagonal are for the LSI-Likert diagonal are for the LSQ. .454 .305 .439 -and those below the Table 4 Alpha coefficients for the scales of the LSI-Likert and the LSQ LSI-Likert Geiger et al. CE .741 .83 RO .717 .77 AC .799 .86 AE .799 .84 LSQ .801 .812 .823 .839 Marshall & Merritt .78 .86 .85 .88 The alpha coefficients reported by Geiger et al. (1993) and Marshall & Merritt (1986) are included for comparison. Table 5 Frequency of learning styles as measured by the LSI-Likert for the BSc and BA fields of study Legend for Chart: A B C D - Learning style BSc BA Total A B C D 3 1.78% 26 13.47% 29 8.01% Assimilator Frequency Column % 45 26.63% 47 24.35% 92 25.41% Converger Frequency Column % 96 56.80% 72 37.31% 168 46.41% Accommodator Frequency Column % 25 14.79% 48 24.87% 73 20.17% Diverger Frequency Column % Total 169 193 362 46.69% 53.31% 100% The Chi-square has a value of 27.49 with three degrees of freedom which is significant at the 5% level. Table 6 Frequency of learning styles as measured by the LSQ for the BSc and BA fields of study Legend for Chart: A B C D - Learning style BSc BA Total A B C D 8 4.32% 9 4.57% 17 4.45% Assimilator Frequency Column % 42 22.70% 40 20.30% 82 21.47% Converger Frequency Column % 123 66.49% 118 59.90% 241 63.09% 12 6.49% 30 15.23% 42 10.99% Diverger Frequency Column % Accommodator Frequency Column % Total 185 197 382 48.43% 51.57% 100% The Chi-square has a value of 7.556 with three degrees of freedom which is not significant at the 5% level. Table 7.1 Item factor analysis for the LSI-Likert: Oblique rotated factor loadings for a two factor solution Legend for Chart: A B C D - Scale Item Factor 1 Factor 2 A Abstract Conceptualisation B C D 4 6 10 11 19 24 25 26 29 32 43 47 .435 .501 .158 .457 .492 .245 .429 .388 .579 .186 .562 .462 -.045 .026 .130 -.170 .153 .142 -.085 .255 -.008 .358 .166 .235 Concrete Experience 1 7 14 15 18 22 28 31 33 38 42 45 -.028 -.011 -.119 .080 .406 .030 -.047 -.039 0.048 .208 .153 .216 .309 .231 .307 .440 .169 .122 .212 .237 .445 .474 .580 .179 Active Experimentation 5 12 13 17 20 34 35 37 39 41 44 48 .072 .418 -.160 .265 .077 .321 -.093 .048 .104 .137 .040 .291 .441 .243 .553 .102 .330 .294 .561 .458 .423 .512 .446 .279 Reflective Observation 2 3 8 9 16 21 23 27 30 36 40 46 .377 .301 .258 .195 .384 .194 .196 .336 .360 .280 .364 .304 -.046 -.223 .260 .094 -.002 .120 .147 -.079 -.056 .034 -.050 -.204 VP[*] 4.130 3.974 * The VP is the variance explained by the factor. It is computed as the sum of squares for the elements of the factor's column in the factor loading matrix. Table 7.2 Item factor analysis for the LSI-Likert: Oblique rotated factor loadings for a four factor solution Legend for Chart: A B C D E F - Scale Item Factor Factor Factor Factor 1 2 3 4 A B Abstract Conceptualisation C D E 4 6 10 11 19 24 25 26 29 32 43 47 .316 .485 .187 .280 .551 .447 .394 .476 .486 .395 .625 .640 -.081 -.002 -.040 -.172 -.228 -.166 -.247 .167 -.066 .112 .088 .028 .000 -.132 .192 -.038 .037 .165 .073 -.054 -.035 .168 -.135 -.010 .205 .101 .026 .248 .050 -.153 .110 .027 .188 -.143 .074 -.062 .054 -.082 -.127 .212 .432 .029 -.202 .202 -.062 .178 .353 .356 .187 .016 .147 -.039 .210 .051 .037 -.025 -.025 -.066 .069 .351 .454 -.008 .500 .281 .756 .294 .014 .135 .678 .678 .628 .526 .077 .066 .277 -.081 .120 -.025 -.060 .083 .046 .167 .167 .014 -.243 -.037 -.093 .103 .398 .063 .438 .061 .417 .046 .404 .541 .563 .614 .468 .217 -.031 .003 .075 -.048 -.041 .108 .073 .034 .074 .083 .132 -.063 -.084 -.010 -.189 .066 .109 -.099 -.207 .122 .296 .230 .124 -.019 .043 -.195 .130 .069 .045 .035 .093 .023 .062 .057 .041 -.310 2.719 -.087 -.035 .054 -.030 -.025 .029 .177 .111 .020 .074 .026 .139 2.508 .382 .259 .002 .102 .372 .039 .278 .632 .581 .443 .572 .208 2.387 Concrete Experience 1 7 14 15 18 22 28 31 33 38 42 45 Active Experimentation 5 12 13 17 20 34 35 37 39 41 44 48 .240 .546 .093 .279 .060 .506 .188 .031 -.045 .056 .014 .401 Reflective Observation 2 3 8 9 16 21 23 27 30 36 40 46 VP[*] .149 .111 .354 .181 .171 .226 .050 -.087 -.011 .030 .016 .150 4.350 * The VP is the variance explained by the factor. It is computed as the sum of squares for the elements of the factor's column in the factor loading matrix. Table 8.1 Item factor analysis for the LSQ: Oblique rotated factor loadings for a two factor solution Legend for Chart: A B C D - Scale Item Factor 1 Factor 2 A B C D Abstract Conceptualisation 10 15 17 24 26 27 29 34 36 38 .159 .083 -.101 -.059 -.023 -.011 .041 -.099 -.008 .012 -.315 -.414 -.505 -.515 -.533 -.609 -.481 -.425 -.546 -.288 Concrete Experience 1 4 5 12 14 18 21 25 28 30 -.107 -.053 .071 .059 .009 -.081 -.078 .094 .117 .074 .330 .527 .495 .412 .491 .463 .612 .524 .398 .593 Active Experimentation 6 7 11 13 16 19 23 32 39 40 -.602 -.599 -.507 -.399 -.588 -.458 -.520 -.647 -.236 -.441 .003 .075 .138 .268 .075 -.181 -.197 -.035 -.307 -.071 Reflective Observation 2 3 8 9 20 22 31 33 35 37 .611 .512 .667 .666 .363 .338 .549 .349 .503 .434 -.056 -.022 -.102 -.045 .055 .248 .183 -.070 .039 .230 VP[*] 5.388 5.102 * The VP is the variance explained by the factor. It is computed as the sum of squares for the elements of the factor's column in the factor loading matrix. Table 8.2 Item factor analysis for the LSQ: Oblique rotated factor loadings for a four factor solution Legend for Chart: A B C D E F - Scale Item Factor Factor Factor Factor 1 2 3 4 A B C D E -.111 .007 .217 .010 .072 .027 .067 .103 -.003 -.024 -.169 -.276 -.369 -.038 -.149 -.197 -.267 .009 -.104 .130 .163 .137 .136 .707 .486 .563 .216 .569 .620 .536 .171 .296 .371 -.094 .199 .109 .355 .035 .025 -.011 .153 .115 -.020 -.028 .125 .054 .123 -.118 -.126 -.092 .366 .561 .607 .538 .193 .505 .501 .671 .499 .534 -.037 -.122 -.034 -.023 -.586 -.004 -.279 .071 .033 -.154 .089 .097 .075 .035 .315 -.145 .035 -.136 -.060 -.120 -.026 .094 .086 .310 .195 .005 -.063 .128 -.049 .156 -.058 -.028 -.163 -.051 .135 .280 .159 .185 .250 .243 .067 .085 .204 .084 -.045 -.111 .082 -.007 .275 .051 Abstract Conceptualisation 10 15 17 24 26 27 29 34 36 38 Concrete Experience 1 4 5 12 14 18 21 25 28 30 Active Experimentation 6 7 11 13 16 19 23 32 39 40 .638 .649 .603 .450 .597 .425 .552 .668 .331 .470 Reflective Observation 2 -.532 .142 .133 .270 3 -.436 .173 .130 .239 8 -.538 .081 .055 .471 9 -.543 .190 .106 .454 20 -.284 .048 -.104 .227 22 -.358 .316 .053 -.088 31 -.513 .321 .070 .104 33 -.183 -.098 -.196 .513 35 -.459 .171 .083 .137 37 -.375 .194 -.147 .155 VP[*] 5.119 3.608 3.077 1.718 * The VP is the variance explained by the factor. It is computed as the sum of squares for the elements of the factor's column in the factor loading matrix. DIAGRAM: Figure 1 Kolb's model of experiential learning, learning abilities and learning styles. References Assessment Systems Corporation. (1993). User's manual for the ITEMAN Conventional Item Analysis Program. Minnesota: Assessment Systems Corporation. Atkinson, G. (1988). Reliability of the Learning Style Inventory-1985. Psychological Reports, 62, 755-758. BMDP Statistical Software, Inc. (1993). BMDP Manual. Cork, Ireland: BMDP Statistical Software. Cornwell, J.M. & Dunlap, W.P. (1994). On the questionable soundness of factoring ipsative data: a response to Saville & Willson (1991). Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 67, 89-100. Cornwell, J.M., & Manfredo, P.A. (1994). Kolb's learning style theory revisited. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 54(2), 317-327. Cornwell, J.M., Manfredo, P.A. & Dunlap, W.P. (1991). Factor analysis of the 1985 revision of Kolb's Learning Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51, 455-462. Fox, R.D. (1984). Learning styles and instructional preferences in continuing education for health professionals: a validity study of the LSI. Adult Education Quarterly, 35(2), 72-85. Freedman, R.D. & Stumpf, S.A. (1978). What can one learn from the Learning Style Inventory? Academy of Management Journal, 21(2), 275-282. Freedman, R.D. & Stumpf, S.A. (1980). Learning style theory: less than meets the eye. Academy of Management Journal, 5(3), 445-447. Garvey, M, Bootman, J.L., McGhan, W.F., & Meredith, K. (1984). An assessment of learning styles among pharmacy students. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 48, 134-140. Geiger, M.A., Boyle, E.J. & Pinto, J.K. (1992). A factor analysis of Kolb's revised Learning Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 753-759. Geiger, M.A., Boyle, E.J. & Pinto, J.K. (1993). An examination of ipsative and normative versions of Kolb's revised Learning Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53, 717-726. Geller, L.M. (1979). Reliability of the Learning Style Inventory. Psychological Reports, 44, 555-561. Green, D.W., Snell, J.C. & Parimanath, A.R. (1990). Learning styles in assessment of students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 363-369. Hickcox, L.K. (1990). An historical review of Kolb's formulation of experiential learning theory. Unpublished D. Ed. thesis. Oregon State University. Hicks, L.E. (1970). Some properties of ipsative, normative and forcedchoice normative measures. Psychological Bulletin, 74(3), 167-184. Highhouse, S. & Doverspike, D. (1987). The validity of the Learning Style Inventory 1985 as a predictor of cognitive style and occupational preference. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 47, 749-753. Katz, N. (1986). Construct validity of Kolb's Learning Style Inventory, using factor analysis and Guttman's smallest space analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 1323-1326. Kerlinger, F.N. (1973). Foundations of behavioral research (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Kolb, D.A. (1984). Experiential learning: experience as the source of learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. Kolb, D.A., Rubin, I.M. & McIntyre, J.M. (1984). Organizational psychology: an experiential approach to organizational behavior (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. Laschinger, H.K.S. (1990). Review of experiential learning theory research in the nursing profession. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 15, 985-993. Marshall, J.C. & Merritt, S.L. (1986). Reliability and construct validity of the Learning Style Questionnaire. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 46(1), 257-262. Merritt, S.L. & Marshall, J.C. (1984). Reliability and construct validity of ipsative and normative forms of the Learning Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 44(2), 463-472. Pickworth, G.E. (1997). An integration of the theories of JL Holland and DA Kolb: a theoretical and empirical study of vocational personality and learning style types. Unpublished DPhil. thesis. University of Pretoria. Plovnick, M.S. (1974). Individual learning styles and the process of career choice in medical students. Unpublished doctoral thesis. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Plovnick, M.S. (1975). Primary care career choices and medical student learning styles. Journal of Medical Education, 50, 849-855. Romero, J.E., Tepper, B.J. & Tetrault, L.A. (1992). Development and validation of new scales to measure Kolb's (1985) Learning Style dimensions. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 171-180. Ruble, T.L. & Stout, D.E. (1991). Reliability, classification stability and response-set bias of alternate forms of the Learning Style Inventory (LSI1985). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51, 481-489. SAS Institute Inc. (1990). SAS/STAT User's Guide. Carey, N.C.: SAS Institute Inc. Sims, R.R., Veres, J.G., Watson, P. & Buckner, K.E. (1986). The reliability and classification stability of the Learning Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 46, 753-760. Sugarman, L. (1985). Kolb's model of experiential learning: touchstone for trainers, students, counselors and clients, Journal of Counseling and Development, 64(4), 264-268. Tenopyr, M.L. (1988). Artifactual reliability of forced-choice scales. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73(4), 749-751. Veres, J.G., Sims, R.R. & Locklear, T.S. (1991). Improving the reliability of Kolb's revised Learning Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51, 143-150. Veres, J.G., Sims, R.R. & Shake, L.G. (1987). The reliability and classification stability of the Learning Style Inventory in corporate settings. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 47, 1127-1133. Welman, J.C. & Huysamen, G.K. (1993). Die geldigheid van 'n leerstylvraelys in die onderskeiding tussen studierigtings. Suid-Afrikaanse Tydskrif vir Hoer Onderwys, 7(3), 258-269. West, R.F. (1982). A construct validity study of Kolb's learning style types in medical education. Journal of Medical Education, 57. 794-796. Whitney, M.A. & Caplan, R.M. (1978). Learning styles and instructional preferences of family practice physicians. Journal of Medical Education, 53, 684-686. Wilson, D.K. (1986). An investigation of the properties of Kolb's Learning Style Inventory. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 7(3), 3-15. Wunderlich, R. & Gjerde, C.L. (1978). Another look at Learning Style Inventory and medical career choice. Journal of Medical Education, 53, 4554. ~~~~~~~~ By Glynis E. Pickworth[*], Faculty P.O. Box 667, Pretoria 0001, South and Willem J. Schoeman, Department University, P.O. Box 524, Auckland of Medicine, University of Pretoria, Africa, E-mail: glynis@medic.up.ac.za of Psychology, Rand Afrikaans Park 2006, South Africa ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of South African Journal of Psychology is the property of Foundation for Education Science & Technology and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: South African Journal of Psychology, Jun2000, Vol. 30 Issue 2, p44, 9p, 14 charts, 1 diagram. Item Number: 3510857 Result 25 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 28 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: A Strategy For Helping Students Learn How to Learn. Subject(s): LEARNING, Psychology of; LEARNING strategies -- Study & teaching; MARYGROVE College (Detroit, Mich.); MYERS-Briggs Type Indicator; PERSONALITY & academic achievement Source: Education, Spring2000, Vol. 120 Issue 3, p479, 8p, 4 charts Author(s): McClanaghan, Mary Ellen A STRATEGY FOR HELPING STUDENTS LEARN HOW TO LEARN Engaging in the process of learning how to learn must include awareness of how one perceives and processes material to be learned. Instructors can enhance students' awareness by calling their attention to the ways and means by which they are approaching their subject. Varying teaching methods in each component of the instructional cycle on a regular basis and then discussing what each student finds most compelling and most challenging provides opportunities to raise awareness. Introduction Successful people know how to learn. This key to success has never been more important than it is today in our information-saturated society. Marygrove College faculty recognize the importance of learning how to learn and have made "learning to learn," one of seven across the curriculum emphases. This emphasis is begun in the First Year Seminar, an introductory course with the overall goal of student success. To achieve this goal we emphasize self-awareness and learning how to learn. Although there are several approaches to these objectives, one approach is attention to learning styles. In his book Powerful Learning, (1998) Ron Brandt states that attention to individual learning styles is an avenue that leads to learning how to learn. There are several definitions of learning style but basically it is an individual's characteristic means of perceiving and processing information. It is important to first validate a student's dominant means of learning if we hope to challenge them to work in a style in which they feel less competent. Learning Styles The First Year Seminar or Mg 102 as it is referred to at Marygrove introduces students to the theory of learning styles. Students take a short form of the Myers/Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). This is one of the best known psychological instruments in the world today. It is based on the theory of Swiss psychologist, Carl Jung. According to Jung, a persons attitude or readiness to act is determined by a preference for either extraversion, which focuses on the external world, or introversion, which focuses on the internal world. He also identified four behavioral functions that, in various combinations constitute personality type: sensing, intuition, thinking, and feeling. Myers and Briggs built on Jung's theory by adding a judging / perceiving scale. The judging and perceiving scale indicates if a person has a stronger attraction toward one of the perceiving functions (sensing and intuition) or one the judging functions (thinking and feeling). One component of Jung's theory that has a parallel to the teaching and learning process comes from Jung's theory of human development, which identifies two major objectives of psychic development' perfection and completion. The perfection objective involves the human need to develop one's own natural strengths and abilities to the maximum. Completion continues the development process to strengthen also the less dominant but potential abilities (Hanson and Silver, 1996). The value of understanding one's learning style is first to develop one's natural approaches to learning and then to develop the capacity to learn in ways that may require more attention and effort. Learning how to learn in different ways will assist students to be life long learners who are capable of learning in various settings and situations. If students can be successful by learning in ways that are most natural to them they are more likely to take on the challenge to move toward Jung's concept of completion. Action Research The faculty who teach The First Year Seminar, Mg 102, commit to meet together several times during the semester to discuss student progress and to continually seek to improve our service to our new students. As a result of these meetings the idea surfaced to track our students' learning styles in order to offer better instruction and support services. Objectives of the study were to: • work with the Student Support Service tutors and Career Services to empower them to build on the introduction that the students receive regarding learning style theory in the First Year Seminar course. • provide workshops for students with very strong learning preferences to assist them in developing their weaker styles. • study and report the results of the learning style profiles of new Mary grove students to identify any possible clustering of styles in our population. • Offer on-going support to the First Year Seminar faculty to help them make better use of the information to assist their students' in monitoring their own learning style development. Several researchers and educators have adapted the theory of the MBTI and developed instruments for specific uses. One example is "The Thoughtful Education Model"' developed by J. Robert Hanson and Harvey F. Silver. Their work centers on four learning skills identified by Jung: Sensing/Intuition and Thinking/Feeling and offers very valuable application for educators. To test the consistency of the short form of the Myers/Briggs instrument four groups of students completed either "Learning Style Instrument for Adults," developed by Hanson and Silver or the Form G of the MBTI in addition to the short form. The following characteristics of the learning styles is based upon the research of Hanson and Silver (1996) as reported in course materials produced by Canter Educational Associates for Marygrove College's Master in the Art of Teaching Program (1996). ST / Sensing-Thinking Learning Style In the sensing-thinking learning style (ST), students want concrete, specific information and need to know what is right and wrong. They need a structured environment and lose interest if things move too slow or don't seem practical. They learn best from repetition, drill, memorization and actual experience. They need immediate feedback. NT / Intuitive- Thinking Learning Style In the intuitive-thinking learning style (NT), students are skeptical, analytical and logical. They trust hard evidence and reason. They prefer to work independently; they understand things and ideas by breaking them down into their component parts. They want to be challenged and allowed to be creative, and are concerned with relevance and meaning. They have great patience and persistence if their attention is captured. SF / Sensing-Feeling Learning Style In the sensing-feeling learning style (SF), students process information based on their personal experience. They respond to collegiality, trust, respect and learning cooperatively. They view content mastery as secondary to harmonious relationships. They are very sensitive to approval or disapproval. They learn best by talking and like group activities. NF/Intuitive-Feeling Learning Style - In the intuitive-feeling learning style (NF), students are looking for possibilities and patterns, and connections with prior learning. They look for uniqueness, originality and aestheticism. They learn best in a flexible and innovative atmosphere. They have difficulty planning and organizing their time. They need to see the big picture. They are bored by routine and rote assignments. With these categories in mind information was collected on 207 Marygrove students between the years 1995-1998. Of these 207 students 167 were female. Although the age of the students was not documented it should be noted that the average age of Marygrove's undergraduate student is 32. These results are quite different from other studies. According to the research of Hanson and Silver which does not specify age level: Intuitive Feelers make up about 10% of all students. Sensing Feelers make up about 35% of all students. Intuitive Thinkers make up about 20% of all students. Sensing Thinkers make up about 35% of all students, The Marygrove research was compared to another study of college students conducted by Mary Todd and Daniel Robinson at Bunker Hill Community College in Boston, Massachusetts in 1995. Bunker Hill has approximately 6,000 day and evening students. The MBTI preferences of 1007 students were collected over a ten-year period (1985-1995). This research was reported at the Center for Application of Psychological Type Conference held in Orlando, Florida in March of 1998. The study reported scores by racial identification. The racial composition in the Mary grove study was the opposite of the Bunker Hill study. Of 725 students in the Bunker Hill study 63% were Caucasian and 20% were African American. In the Marygrove study, of the 204 students whose racial background was reported 87% were African American and 11% were Caucasian. The most obvious difference in the Marygrove results is in the high percent age of Intuitive Feelers (NF) students. Keirsey and Bates (1984) report that only 12% of the general population are NFs. This is much closer to what Hanson and Silver and Todd and Robinson (Bunker Hill) report. Insight into why Marygrove may attract a higher percentage of NF students may be in the characteristics of the college itself. Marygrove is a small (approximately 1,000 undergraduate students) Catholic liberal arts college. The literature boasts of small class size and a warm and personal atmosphere. Fairhurst and Fairhurst (1999) describe NF students as preferring small group discussions and one on one instruction. They want a personalized learning setting. They seek harmony and demonstrate sensitivity and caring for others. Personal values are very important to them. If we place the stated characteristics of Mary grove College, as described by the mission statement and college catalog, along side the characteristics of the Intuitive Feeler learner the results may not be so surprising. Conclusions and Future Action Adult students bring a consumer mentality to higher education. They will seek out learning environments that offer them the best chance at success. This study pro vides Marygrove faculty and support staff with a closer look at the students who choose Marygrove as their ticket to the future. A great percentage of these students are looking for a personal environment that will allow them to unleash their unique creative potential. Affirming this natural preference for learning provides an important variable that contributes to success. Successful students are more likely to develop abilities that might not have been tapped. These students bring with them many years of life experience in which they have developed habits and attitudes toward learning. Some of these habits and attitudes must be transformed if these students are to graduate and move on to a successful future. This study was undertaken as an action research project that does not seek correlation beyond the population studied. However, faculty at other institutions could easily conduct their own study to ascertain the profile of their student body. The value is in determining if there is a dominant student profile at the institution. If there is, faculty and support staff have a better opportunity to begin working with the students" most natural style. Research has suggested that knowing one's preferred learning style enhances a student's ability to achieve academic success. The knowledge that there are different styles for achieving .success is in itself an eye opener for many students. Some studies have indicated that academically successful students have fewer strong learning style preferences than do low achievers. The challenge is to assist students in perfecting their natural learning style while providing the incentive to develop less dominant styles they will need in the workforce and other areas of their lives. Engaging in the process of learning how to learn must include awareness of how one perceives and processes material to be learned. Instructors can enhance students' awareness by calling their attention to the ways and means by which they are approaching their subject. Varying teaching methods in each component of the instructional cycle on a regular basis and then discussing what each student finds most compelling and most challenging pro vides opportunities to raise awareness. Hanson and Silver offer the following suggestions for what they call teaching around the wheel. Each aspect of instruction offers opportunities to reach the variety of styles by changing teaching methods on a regular basis. Anticipatory Sets (Introductions that prepare for the lesson or a unit) ST Give facts, details NT Raise issues & potential problems SF Relate to students' experiences, feelings & prior knowledge NF Suggest new and original possibilities Questions ST Who, what, where, when NT Explain, compare, identify cause and effect SF Ask: What has been your experience? What do you know about ? NF Ask: What might happen if or ask for an application Tasks ST Organize factual information, practice for recall NT Create a problem solving mode where students must sort out data, analyze and draw conclusions SF Provide for group work or a task that involves the affect NF Provide choices for completing assignments and projects or assign tasks that involve imagination, innovation Setting ST Traditional rows or pairs; teacher at focus NT Teams that will create a debating atmosphere; teacher moves from team to team. SF Groups or pairs for collaboration; teacher meets students at eye level NF Learning centers, student arranged for interest; teacher is a resource Feedback ST Frequent, quick, short/need to know if they are right NT Infrequent but with explanation of why they received the grade they did SF Frequent, quick with an emphasis on the amount of effort that is evidenced NF Infrequent but with emphasis on its value' its uniqueness and creativity Homework ST Provide a model of what a complete and accurate assignment will look like, practice and drill NT Problem solving, analyzing work, it too must be modeled SF Opportunities for articulating ideas, learning from others, develop skills of collaboration designed to convince students they have knowledge NF Projects or opportunities to create new or different ways of looking at material, important to set criteria Assessment ST True and false, fill in the blanks, any measure that allows students to recall factual material NT Critical essays, debates, research projects which mea sure the ability to see relationships SF Interviews in and out of class. Let the students question you NF Anything that can show what the student can do with what they have learned Helping students learn how to learn may be the most important lesson faculty can teach students. Life-long learners, capable of learning and working in diverse settings, are vital to the 21century society. Assisting students in achievement of this goal puts a demand on faculty to take the time to teach around the learning style wheel. The reward for this effort will be more students who are engaged in at least some aspect of the learning process. Going a step further and talking with students about how they experience learning when instruction or tasks call on styles that are not natural for them, raises awareness of their own approach to learning. Students may believe that what comes natural to them is all that they can do well and they are doomed to failure in all other areas. Unless we sup port students to develop under developed aspects of their styles they are unlikely to have lifelong success. An important task of learning how to learn is to develop an awareness of oneself as a learner. Students need to reflect on their experience of learning in order to take charge of the full development of their abilities. The ultimate goal of higher education can not be content learning alone. Content may become obsolete. The U. S. Department of Labor has identified the ability of knowing how to learn as the most fundamental skill for the next century (Camevale, 1988). Selfawareness and then self-monitoring are essentials for learning how to learn. Faculty and support staffs who nurture this type of learning are helping develop tomorrow's workers. The kind of workers who are needed for the learning organizations that will fuel our global economy. Results of Marygrove study Introverts 68% Feelers 66% Intuitive Feelers (NF) 41% Sensing Feelers (SF) 25% Intuitive Thinkers (NT) 18% Sensing Thinkers (ST) 16% Bunker Hill results reported by racial group Style African American (20%) Introvert (I) Feeler (F) Intuitive Feeler (NF) Sensing Feeler (SF) Intuitive Thinker (NT) Sensing Thinker (ST) 63% 29% 07% 21% 12% 60% Marygrove results reported by racial group Style African American (87%) Introvert (I) Feeler (F) Intuitive Feeler (NF) Sensing Feeler (SF) Intuitive Thinker (NT) 69% 63% 40% 23% 19% Caucasian (63%) 46% 50% 24% 26% 19% 31% Caucasian (11%) 43% 78% 39% 39% 13% Sensing Thinker (ST) 18% 09% Comparison of Intuitive Feeler (NF) data Keirsey & Bates Hanson & Silver Bunker Hill 12% 10% African Amer 07% Caucasian 24% Marygrove African Amer 40% Caucasian 39% Marygrove College Characteristics Small, warm and friendly environment Religiously oriented: Catholic and very ecumenical in approach Committed to the liberal arts Especially noted for the helping professions and the arts; committed to valuing diversity Strives to develop graduates who exemplify competence, commitment and compassion Intuitive-Feeler (NF) Learner Learn best in a nurturing environment Have a keen interest in other belief systems and enjoy discussing moral dilemmas Needs to explore creative potential and find ways to express her/his ideas and beliefs and share this inspiration Are inspired be sensitive, supportive, humanistic teachers who show them they care about them as individuals Looks for similarities among people and encourages cooperation and harmony References Brandt, R. (1998). Powerful learning. Alexandria, VA: ASCD. Canter Educational Productions (1996). Learning styles and multiple intelligences. Santa Monica, CA. Carnevale, A., Gainer, L & Meltzer, A. (1988). Workplace basics: the skills employers want. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Labor. Fairhurst, A. & Fairhurst ' L. (1995). Effective teaching and effective learning: making the personality connection in your classroom. Palo Alto, CA: Davis-Black Publishing. Hanson, J.R. & Silver, H.F. (1995). Learning styles & strategies. Princeton, NJ: Hanson Silver Strong & Associates. Keirsey, D. & Bates, M. (1984). Please understand me: character and temperament types. Del Mar, CA: Prometheus Nemesis. Lawrence, G.D. (1979). People types and tiger stripes: a practical guide to learning styles.. Gainsville, FL: Center for Application of Psychological Type. Todd, M & Robinson, D. (1998). "Students of Color at an Urban Community College." Gainsville, FL: Center for Application of Psychological Type. ~~~~~~~~ By Mary Ellen McClanaghan, PH.D., Marygrove College 22461 Revere St. Clair Shores, Michigan 48080 Adapted by PH.D. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Education is the property of Project Innovation and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Education, Spring2000, Vol. 120 Issue 3, p479, 8p, 4 charts. Item Number: 2990111 Result 28 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 30 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Students' Learning Styles in Two Classes. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; EDUCATION Source: College Teaching, Fall99, Vol. 47 Issue 4, p130, 6p, 1 chart, 1 graph Author(s): Diaz, David P.; Cartnal, Ryan B. STUDENTS' LEARNING STYLES IN TWO CLASSES Online Distance Learning and Equivalent On-Campus The idea that people learn differently is venerable and probably had its origin with the ancient Greeks (Wratcher et al. 1997). Educators have, for many years, noticed that some students prefer certain methods of learning more than others. These dispositions, re ferred to as learning styles, form a student's unique learning preference and aid teachers in the planning of small-group and individualized instruction (Kemp, Morrison and Ross 1998, 40). Grasha (1996) has defined learning styles as "personal qualities that influence a student's ability to acquire information, to interact with peers and the teacher, and otherwise to participate in learning experiences" (41). Blackmore (1996) suggested that one of the first things we teachers can do to aid the learning process is simply to be aware that there are diverse learning styles in the student population: There are probably as many ways to "teach" as there are to learn. Perhaps the most important thing is to be aware that people do not all see the world in the same way. They may have very different preferences than you for how, when, where and how often to learn. [online] Although many of us are aware that different learning styles exist, the application of this knowledge is often inconsequential. Some faculty simply opt to use a wide variety of teaching activities, hoping that they will cover most student learning preferences along the way. This method, though expedient, may not be the most effective way to address student learning preferences. Further, many teachers think that the same teaching methods that work in their traditional classes will also work for distance learning. The underlying assumption is that students who enroll in distance education classes will have the same learning preferences as those in traditional classes. Faculty often assume that teaching styles, and accompanying classroom processes, are like a "master key" and thus appropriate for any setting. There is not an overabundance of re search on learning styles and distance education. Most of the studies focus on the discovery of relationships between learn ing styles and specific student achievement outcomes: drop rate, completion rate, attitudes about learning, and predictors of high risk. One of the most popular learning style inventories, which is often used in distance learning research, is the Kolb Learning Style Inventory (LSI) (Kolb 1986). Kolb's LSI measures student learning style preference in two bipolar dimensions. Over time, learners develop a preference for either concrete experiences when learning or a preference for engaging in abstract or conceptual analyses when acquiring skills and knowledge. They also may emphasize interest in turning theory into practice by active experimentation, or they may prefer to think about their experiences by reflective observation (Dille and Mezack 1991, 27). James and Gardner (1995) described Kolb's LSI as a cognitive learning style mode. Cognitive processes include storage and retrieval of information in the brain and represent the learner's ways of perceiving, thinking, problem solving, and remembering (20). Dille and Mezack (1991) used Kolb's LSI to identify predictors of high risk among community college telecourse students. Successful students had lower scores on their preferences for concrete experiences than did the unsuccessful students. Thus, because distance learning courses often lead to social isolation and require greater reliance on independent learning skills, students with less need for concrete experience in learning may be expected to be better suited to the distance format. People with higher scores on concrete experience tend to exhibit a greater sensitivity to feelings and thus would be expected to require more interactions with peers and the teacher. Successful telecourse students also preferred to look for abstract concepts to help explain the concrete experiences associated with their learning. That is, they wanted to know "why" certain things happened in conceptual or theoretical terms. This more abstract approach clearly favored success in the telecourse. Dille and Mezack concluded that students who needed concrete experience and were not able to think abstractly were more highrisk in a telecourse. Gee (1990) studied the impact of learning style variables in a live teleconference distance education class. The study ex amined the influence of learning style preferences of students in an on-campus or remote classroom on their achievement in the following: course content, course completion rates, and attitudes about learning. Both distance and on-campus groups were taught simultaneously by the same teacher, received identical course content, and met weekly. Gee administered the Canfield Learning Styles Inventory (CLSI) (Canfield 1980). Students in the distance learning class who possessed a more independent and conceptual learning style had the highest average scores in all of the student achievement areas. People with the lowest scores in the distance learning course had a more social and conceptual learning style. Students with both a social and applied learning style performed much better in the on-campus class. The outcomes of the Gee study suggested that successful distance education students favored an independent learning environment, and successful on-campus students preferred working with others. The relatively small sample of twenty-six students suggested that additional research is needed. An important question, however, is raised by such research: Are there differences in learning styles between students who enroll in a distance education class and their on-campus counterparts? That question, no matter how it is answered, is vital for anyone interested in students' success. If there are no differences in learning styles, it is likely that faculty can transfer the same types of teaching/learning activities that have worked in the traditional environment into the distance setting with similar success. That is probably true, if enough sensitivity and thought have been given to learning styles and to how these methods will be transferred to the distance education en vironment using current communications technologies. On the other hand, if there are differences in learning styles between groups of students, then faculty must use learning style information to aid their planning and preparation for distance education ac tivities. Sarasin (1998) noted that professors should be willing to change their teaching strategies and techniques based on an appreciation of the variety of student learning styles. "[Teachers] should try to ensure that their methods, materials, and resources fit the ways in which their students learn and maximize the learning potential of each student" (2). If optimal learning is dependent on learning styles, and these styles vary be tween distance and equivalent on-campus students, then faculty should be aware of these differences and alter their preparation and instructional methods accordingly. In any case, the first step in using learning style information in distance education is to determine students' learning styles. Selecting a Learning Style Instrument As educators consider transplanting their traditional courses into distance learning, they should assess the learning styles of the students who enroll. With a variety of learning style instruments in use, it is important to select one according to the unique requirements of the distance learning context. Three important factors to consider when selecting a learning style instrument are defining the intended use of the data to be collected, matching the instrument to the intended use, and finally, selecting the most appropriate instrument (James and Gardner 1995). Other concerns include the underlying concepts and design of the instrument, validity and reliability issues, administration difficulties, and cost (22). One of the distinguishing features of most distance education classes is the ab sence of face-to-face social interaction between students and teacher. Thus, an inventory used in that setting should address the impact of different social dynamics on the learning preferences of the students. An example of this can be seen in Gee (1990), who employed the Canfield Learning Styles Inventory (CLSI). The CLSI demonstrated merit in distance learning studies because it at tempted to measure students' preferences in environmental conditions, such as the need for affiliation with other students and instructor, and for independence or structure. Those varied social dynamics are one of the main differences between distance learning and equivalent on-campus environments. However, in our opinion, both the Canfield Inventory and Kolb's LSI create a narrow range of applicability for learning styles by limiting learning preferences to one or two dimensions. Al though this learning style "stereotyping" may be convenient for statistical analysis, it is less helpful in terms of teaching students about weaker or unused learning preferences. Further, the Kolb LSI, which has been widely used, is primarily a cognitive learning preference instrument, which does not specifically take into a ccount social preferences that are the key distinction between distance and traditional classrooms. Of the different learning style instruments, the Grasha-Reichmann Student Learning Style Scales (GRSLSS) seem ideal for assessing student learning preferences in a college-level distance learning setting. The GRSLSS (Hruska-Riechmann and Grasha 1982; Grasha 1996) was chosen as the tool for determining student learning styles in the present study based on criteria suggested by James and Gardner (1995). First, the GRSLSS is one of the few instruments de signed specifically to be used with senior high school and college students (Hruska-Riechmann and Grasha, 1982). Second, the GRSLSS focuses on how students interact with the instructor, other students, and with learning in general. Thus, the scales address one of the key distinguishing features of a distance class, the relative absence of social interaction between instructor and student and among students. Third, the GRSLSS promotes an optimal teaching/ learning environment by helping faculty design courses and develop sensitivity to students' needs. Finally, the GRSLSS promotes understanding of learning styles in a broad context, spanning six categories. Students possess all six learning styles, to a greater or lesser extent. This type of understanding prevents simplistic views of learning styles and provides a rationale for teachers to encourage students to pursue personal growth and development in their underused learning styles. Only a brief definition of each is provided here in order to assist the reader with the interpretation of the information from this study. 1. Independent students prefer independent study and self-paced instruction and would prefer to work alone rather than with other students on course projects. 2. Dependent learners look to the teacher and to peers as a source of structure and guidance and prefer an authority figure to tell them what to do. 3. Competitive students learn in order to perform better than their peers and to receive recognition for their academic ac complishments. 4. Collaborative learners acquire in formation by sharing and cooperating with teacher and peers. They prefer lectures with small-group discussions and group projects. 5. Avoidant learners are not enthusiastic about attending class or acquiring class content. They are typically uninterested and are sometimes overwhelmed by class activities. 6. Participant learners are interested in class activities and discussion and are eager to do as much class work as possible. They are keenly aware of, and have a desire to meet, the teacher's expectations. The styles described by the GRSLSS refer to a blend of characteristics that apply to all students (Grasha 1996, 127). Each person possesses some of each of the learning styles. Ideally, one would have a balance of all the learning styles; however, most people gravitate toward one or two styles. Learning preferences are likely to change as one matures and encounters new educational experiences. Dowdall (1991) and Grasha (1996) also have suggested that particular teaching styles might encourage students to adopt certain learning styles. Problem and Purpose Students' performance may be related to their learning preferences or styles. Students may also self-select into or away from distance learning classes. As a result, success in distance learning classes may ultimately depend on understanding the learning styles of the students who enroll. Because more online courses will in variably be offered in the future, some as surance must be provided to the college, the faculty, and the students, that distance education will meet expectations for a good education. Not only will students expect an education that is equal in quality to that provided by traditional offerings, they will expect a student-centered learning environment, designed to meet their individual needs. There have been few studies on the relationship of learning styles to student success in a distance learning environment, and none that we are aware of have used the GRSLSS. The purpose of this study was to compare the student learning styles of online and equivalent on-campus, health education classes, by using the GRSLSS. The population for the current study in cluded health education students in a medium-sized (8,000--9,000 enrollment) community college on the central coast of California. The distance education sample included students in two sections of health education offered in an online format (N = 68). The comparison class was selected from four equivalent on-campus sections of health education (N = 40) taught by the lead author. The online distance students were taught according to the same course outline, used the same textbook, covered the same lecture material, and took the same tests as the on-campus students. Three main differences between on-campus and online groups were the delivery mode for the lectures, the mode of teacher/student and student/student communication, and the mode for the assignments. The distance classes reviewed multimedia slides (Power Point presentations converted to HTML) and lecture notes online, while the equivalent classes heard the teacher's lectures and participated in face-to-face discussion. The distance class made heavy use of a class Web site and used a listserv and e-mail for communication/discussion with other students and the instructor. Assignments for the distance class were almost entirely Internet-based and independent, while the equivalent class completed some online assignments but participated most frequently in classroom discussions and other traditional assignments. All 108 participants first reviewed the student cover letter that explained the na ture of the research and provided opportunity for informed consent. Next, the authors distributed the GRSLSS and re viewed the instructions for completion of the inventory. The GRSLSS was administered in a group setting during the second week of classes. Thus, we used the General Class Form to assess the initial learning styles of the students. Students self-scored the inventory, and we obtained raw scores for each of the learning style categories. Inventories were reviewed by the researchers for compliance with di rections and for accuracy of scoring. Research Outcomes The present study compared social learning styles between distance education and equivalent on-campus classes using the GRSLSS. The average or mean scores of the distance learning class and the equivalent health education class on each of the six categories are shown in figure 1. Relatively larger differences in the average scores of the two classrooms oc curred for the independent and the de pendent learning styles. Compared with those students enrolled in the traditional classroom, the students in the distance class had higher scores on the independent learning style scale and lower scores on the dependent scale. A statistical test (a t test) was used to determine if the differences in the scores between the independent and dependent learning styles were due to chance. The variations in average scores between the two styles were found to be statistically significant and thus not likely due to chance (p < .01). The variations in average scores between the two classrooms on the avoidant, competitive, collaborative, and participant learning styles were relatively small, and a statistical analysis using a t test revealed that they were not statistically significant. To ascertain the patterns in the relationships among the learning styles within each class, we examined the associations among different combinations of styles. This was done by calculating the correlation coefficients associated with the combinations of the six learning styles. The outcomes of this analysis are shown in table 1 for the distance learning and traditional classroom groups. For reading this table, we remind the reader that a correlation coefficient varies from -1, 0, to +1, and that the degree to which it deviates from zero in either direction reflects the strength of the relationship between the two variables. The asterisks with some of the values indicate that the size of the correlation was statistically significant and thus not due to chance. Correlational analysis within the on line group showed a negative relationship between the independent learning style and the collaborative and dependent styles. In other words, people who were more independent in their learning styles also tended to be less collaborative and dependent. A second important relationship (positive correlation) was found be tween the collaborative learning style and the dependent and participant learning styles. That is, students who were more collaborative in their learning styles also were more dependent and participatory in their approach to learning. In the equivalent on-campus group, significant positive correlations were found between the collaborative learning style and the competitive and participant styles. That is, on-campus students who were collaborative also tended to be competitive and participatory in the classroom. Finally, a positive correlation be tween the competitive and participant styles of learning also was observed. Students who tended to compete also were "good classroom citizens" and were more willing to do what the teacher wanted them to do. Discussion Gibson (1998) has challenged distance education instructors to "know the learner" (140). She noted that distance learners are a heterogeneous group and that in structors should design learning activities to capitalize on this diversity (141). Be cause the dy namic nature of the distance population pre cludes a "typical" student profile (Thomp son 1998, 9), we should continually assess students' characteristics. A professor using the present data could plan learning opportunities that would emphasize the learning preferences with each of the commonly preferred learning styles (independent, de pendent, collaborative, and participant), thus matching teaching strategies with learning styles. Of particular interest were the significant differences between the groups in the independent and dependent categories. The distance students more strongly favored independent learning styles. It is not surprising that students who prefer independent, self-paced in struction would self-select into an online class. It may be that they are well suited to the relative isolation of the distance learning environment. In his research, Gee (1990) noted that successful telecourse students fa vored an independent learning style. James and Gardner (1995) suggested that students who favored reliance on independent learning skills would be more suited to a distance format. As a result of these significant differences, teaching strategies in the distance class should emphasize relatively more independent and fewer dependent learning opportunities. This approach has practical significance given that professors often complain of too little class time to devote to learning objectives. Armed with learning style data, we can more efficiently allocate instructional time to various learning types. Not only were online students more independent than the on-campus students, but their independent learning preferences were displayed in a way that was negatively related to how dependent and collaborative they were. That is, the independence of online learners was not tied to needs for external structure and guidance from their teacher (dependence) or a need to collaborate with their classmates. The online students can be described as "strongly independent," in that they match the stereotype of the independent learner in terms of autonomy and the ability to be selfdirected. Self-direction and independence were facilitated in the online course by offering students flexible options to shape their learning environment. The lead author, Diaz, used self-paced, independent learning activities that allowed students to choose from a menu of online "cyber as signments" based on their personal interests and the relevance of the assignments. Students completed their chosen assignments by deadlines posted at the class Web site. In contrast, students in the equivalent on-campus class were significantly more dependent learners than the distance group. Because dependent learners prefer structure and guidance, it is not difficult to understand why they might view the isolation and need for self-reliance in a distance education environment with some apprehension. The low level of in dependence displayed by on-campus students was not related to any other aspects of their styles as learners. Thus, independence was clearly a weaker learning preference for traditional class students. The online students also displayed collaborative qualities related to their need for structure (dependence) and their willingness to participate as good class citizens (participant dimension). Thus, although online students prefer independent learning situations, they are willing and able to participate in collaborative work if they have structure from the teacher to initiate it. In his online class, Diaz has used listservs and "threaded discussion" areas to promote collaboration among distance students. In the past, he designed collaborative activities that required students to initiate peer contact and conduct the collaboration with a minimum of structure and support from him. Based on the findings of the current study, it is apparent why this strategy failed: Online students will apparently respond well to collaborative activities, but only if the teacher provides enough structure and guidance. Diaz's mistake was that he assumed that online students would be self-directed, and autonomous, regardless of the type of learning activity. In contrast, the traditional class students had collaborative tendencies related to their needs to be competitive, and good class citizens. In other words, they were interested in collaboration to the extent that it helped them to compete favorably in the class and to meet the expectations of their teachers. Thus, collaboration was tied to obtaining the rewards of the class, not to an inherent interest in collaboration. Average avoidant and competitive learning style scores indicated that these learning preferences were favored to a lesser degree by both groups. It was interesting that, though we live in a highly competitive society, neither the online or equivalent on-campus students really preferred a competitive learning environment. However, the on-campus students ap peared to favor competitiveness if it was clear that it was expected (i.e., thus the relationship of competitive and participant styles). We can also use learning style data to help design "creative mismatches" in which students can experience their less-dominant learning style characteristics in a less-threatening environment (Grasha 1996, 172). Designing collaborative as signments for independent learners, or independent assignments for dependent or collaborative learners, is appropriate and even necessary. Strengthening less-preferred learning styles helps students to expand the scope of their learning, be come more versatile learners, and adapt to the requisites of the real world (Sarasin 1998, 38). Learning styles were not the only differences between the distance and comparison groups in this study. Demographic data indicated that the distance group had a higher percentage of females (59 percent, 49 percent), students currently enrolled in under 12 units (66 percent, 50 percent), students who had completed 60 or more college units (12 percent, 1 percent), students who had completed a degree (12 percent, 7 percent), and students above 26 years of age (36 percent, 6 percent). These characteristics agree with the general profile of distance students as reported by Thompson (1998). Although it is tempting to identify and depend on a "typical" distance student profile, it is likely that the dynamic nature of distance education in general will keep student characteristics fluid. Thus, distance education instructors should continually monitor students' characteristics. Conclusions We have concluded that local health education students enrolled in an online class are likely to have different learning styles than equivalent on-campus students. We found that online students were more independent, and on-campus students were more dependent, in their styles as learners. The on-campus students seemed to match the profile of traditional students who are willing to work in class provided they can obtain rewards for working with others and for meeting teacher expectations. Online students ap peared to be driven more by intrinsic mo tives and clearly not by the reward structure of the class. One of the limitations of this study was the use of a non-probability (convenience) sampling technique. Non-probability sampling is used when it is impossible or impractical to use random sampling techniques. That is the case in a large portion of educational research. Although still valid, the results should not be overgeneralized. We have demonstrated a real and substantial difference in learning styles between distance and equivalent on-campus health education students at our college. Before faculty rush to find out the effects of learning styles on student outcomes, they should first address the issue of whether learning style differences exist at all. The results of this study should send an important notice to faculty who are teaching their traditional courses in a distance mode, that there may be drastic differences in learning styles, as well as other characteristic differences, between distance and traditional students. As the World Wide Web becomes an important medium for education delivery, more and more courses will be offered in an online format. Though faculty may attempt to use the same teaching methods in a distance environment that they would employ in an on-campus class, the data from the current study suggest that faculty will encounter significantly different learning preferences as well as other different student characteristics. Professors may want to employ learning style inventories, as well as collect relevant demographic data, to better prepare for distance classes and to adapt their teaching methods to the preferences of the learners. Faculty should use social learning style inventories and resulting data for help in class preparation, designing class delivery methods, choosing educational technologies, and developing sensitivity to differing student learning preferences within the distance education environment. Future field-based research should replicate the current study in different in stitutions and disciplines. ACKNOWLEDGMENT The authors would like to express their thanks to Tony Grasha, whose encouragement, guidance, and ediorial comments were in strumental in bringing this article to fruition. Table 1.--Intercorrelations between Learning Style Scales for Online and Equivalent On-Campus Students Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G - Scale 1 2 3 4 5 6 A B C D E F G Online students (N = 68) 1. Independent -- -.08 2. Avoidant -- 3. Collaborative -.36(**) -.37(**) .07 -.12 -.03 .12 -.02 -.58(**) -- .37(**) -.04 .28(*) 4. Dependent -- .08 .24 5. Competitive --.12 6. Participant -Equivalent on-campus students (N = 40) 1. Independent 2. Avoidant 3. Collaborative 4. Dependent 5. Competitive -- -.20 -- .10 -.37(*) -- -.12 .13 .09 -.12 -.01 -.67(**) .27 .51(**) .52(**) -- .15 .31 -.46(**) 6. Participant -- Note: (*) p < .05, two-tailed. (**) p < .01, two-tailed. Figure 1. Comparison of Average Group Ratings for Each Learning Style Legend for Chart: B - Control group C - Distance group A Independent Avoidant Collaborative Dependent Competitive Participant B 3.25 2.49 3.80 3.84(*) 2.46 3.79 C 3.56(*) 2.57 3.58 3.55 2.38 3.77 (*) Significant at .01 level Grasha-Riechmann Learning Styles REFERENCES Blackmore, J. 1996. Pedagogy: Learning styles. Retrieved September 10, 1997 from the World Wide Web: http://granite.cyg. net/~jblackmo/diglib/styla.html Canfield, A. 1980. Learning styles inventory manual. Ann Arbor, Mich.: Humanics Media. Dille, B., and M. Mezack. 1991. Identifying predictors of high risk among community college telecourse students. The American Journal of Distance Education, 5(1), 24-35. Dowdall, R. J. 1991. Learning style and the distant learner. Consortium project extending the concept and practice of classroom based research report. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 348 117) Gee, D. G. 1990. The impact of students' preferred learning style variables in a distance education course: A case study. Portales: Eastern New Mexico University. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 358 836) Gibson, C. C. 1998. The distance learners academic self-concept. In Distance learners in higher education: Institutional re sponses for quality outcomes, ed. C. Gibson, 65-76. Madison, Wisc.: Atwood. Grasha, A. F. 1996. Teaching with style. Pittsburgh, Pa.: Alliance. Hruska-Riechmann, S., and A. F. Grasha. 1982. The Grasha-Riechmann student learning style scales. In Student learning styles and brain behavior ed. J. Keefe 81-86. Reston, Va.: National Association of Secondary School Principals. James, W. B. and D. L.Gardner. 1995. Learning styles: Implications for distance learning. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. EJ 514 356) Kemp, J. E., G. R. Morrison, and S. M. Ross. 1998. Designing effective instruction (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. Kolb, D. A. 1986. Learning style inventory: Technical manual (Rev. ed.). Boston, Mass.: McBer. Sarasin, L. C. 1998. Learning style perspectives: Impact in the classroom. Madison, Wisc.: Atwood. Thompson, M. M. 1998. Distance learners in higher education. In Distance learners in higher education: Institutional responses for quality outcomes, ed. C. Gibson, 9-24. Madison, Wisc.: Atwood. Wratcher, M. A., E. E. Morrison, V. L. Riley, and L. S. Scheirton. 1997. Curriculum and program planning: A study guide for the core seminar. Fort Lauderdale, Fla.: Nova Southeastern University. Programs for higher education. ~~~~~~~~ By David P. Diaz and Ryan B. Cartnal David P. Diaz is a professor of health education, and Ryan B. Cartnal is a re search analyst at Cuesta College, San Luis Obispo, California. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of College Teaching is the property of Heldref Publications and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: College Teaching, Fall99, Vol. 47 Issue 4, p130, 6p, 1 chart, 1 graph. Item Number: 2516393 Result 30 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Can computer-aided instruction accommodate all learners equally? Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; HUMAN-computer interaction; EDUCATIONAL technology Source: British Journal of Educational Technology, Jan99, Vol. 30 Issue 1, p5, 20p, 1 graph Author(s): Ross, Jonathan; Schulz, Robert CAN COMPUTER-AIDED INSTRUCTION ACCOMMODATE ALL LEARNERS EQUALLY? Abstract This exploratory study investigated the impact of learning styles on humancomputer interaction. Seventy learners who were enrolled in a large urban post-secondary institution participated in the study. The Gregorc Style Delineator Trademark was used to obtain subjects' dominant learning style scores. Results indicated that patterns of learning indices did not differ significantly based on subjects' dominant learning style. Five of the six measures indicating human-computer interaction behavior were not significant at the p < 0.05 level. However, learning styles significantly affected learning outcomes, as indicated by a significant main effect, as well as an interaction effect between dominant learning style and achievement scores. It would appear that Abstract Random learners may be at-risk for doing poorly with certain forms of computer-aided instruction. Based on the review of literature and results found in this study, it was concluded that computer-aided instruction may not be the most appropriate method of learning for all students. One of the most powerful features of computer-aided instruction (CAI) is its capacity to individualize instruction to meet the specific needs of the learner (Rasmussen and Davidson, 1996). Self-paced instruction, the ability to present content in a variety of ways (eg, text, video, sound, graphics), and features such as hypertext make CAI an effective learning medium. The use of CAI in education has burgeoned in recent years (Price, 1991; Nelson and Palumbo, 1992; Hawkridge, 1995). Faced with increasing class sizes and heavier work loads, teachers are looking towards CAI as a means of supplementing classroom instruction. In addition, CAI software continues to improve in its ability to engage learners and provide realistic and stimulating learning environments (Price, 1991). Learners can now choose from a variety of educational software packages designed to augment the curriculum (Dwyer, 1996). As the use of CAI systems continues to grow, research in the area of humancomputer interaction is becoming increasingly important. Currently, a select few studies examine individual differences and their effects on CAI (Marquez and Lehman, 1992; Nelson and Palumbo, 1992; Reed, 1996). Findings generally indicate that while CAI has tremendous potential to individualize instruction, a number of learner characteristics such as motivation, learning styles, and background knowledge may affect the quality and effectiveness of a CAI instructional session. This exploratory study examines the influences of cognitive learning styles on both achievement levels and human-computer interaction behaviors. Findings from this study indicate that certain forms of CAI may not accommodate all learners equally (see Ross, 1997). Educators should, therefore, remain cautious when using the computer as a learning tool. Just as teachers need to use a variety of approaches to meet the diverse needs of their students, so educators should be aware that CAI may not be the learning medium of choice for all students. Literature review The Gregorc Style Delineator Trademark According to Gregorc (1979): "Learning style consists of distinctive behaviors which serve as indicators of how a person learns from and adapts to his environment. It also gives clues as to how a person's mind operates" (p. 234). Designed to assess learning styles, The Gregorc Style Delineator Trademark is a self-scoring battery which focuses on two types of mediation abilities in adult individuals: perception (the means through which one is able to grasp information), and ordering (the means in which one arranges, systematizes and disposes of information). The two dimensions of ordering are referred to as sequential and random; the two qualities of perception are known as abstractness and concreteness (Gregorc, 1982 a). Abstractness allows the individual to comprehend that which is not visible to the senses. Data can be mentally visualized, grasped, and conceived through the faculty of reason. Individuals who are strong in concreteness use the physical senses to comprehend and mentally register data. Sequential individuals perceive and organize data in a linear, methodical fashion, and can express themselves in a precise manner. Furthermore, discrete pieces of information can be categorized naturally. In contrast, randomness disposes the mind to organize information in a nonlinear and multidimensional fashion. This quality enables individuals to deal with, and process, multiple data simultaneously. Gregorc combines these abilities to create four mediation channels of mind styles: concrete sequential (CS), concrete random (CR), abstract sequential (AS) and abstract random (AR). Gregorc believes that individuals have, to a certain degree, characteristics of each category, but most individuals tend to show a stronger orientation toward specific channels. The inventory's scores are obtained by ranking four words at a time ('1' indicating "least like me", '4' indicating "most like me"). Ten categories of four words determine the scores for each of the four mind-styles. Each word corresponds to a particular mediation channel, and when summed, give a measure of a person's propensity for operating within specific learning channels. Gregorc (1982a) divides the scores received on The Style Delineator Trademark into three levels: 1) Strong orientation towards qualities associated with the particular channel (or pointy-headedness), indicated by a score of 27-40; 2) Moderate ability, indicated by a score range of 16-26 on any one mediation channel: and 3) Minimal capacity (stubby pointedness), indicated by a score of 10-15 in a specific channel. According to Gregorc (1985) approximately 60% of the channel's characteristics are observed in people with a score of 27 or over; hence, 27 has been selected as the cut-off point for "pointyheadedness". Another major cut-off point, 15, has been identified as an indication of "stubby pointedness" because very few of the channel's characteristics are observed in people with scores below 15 (Gregorc, 1982a). Learner characteristics (Unless otherwise stated, information presented in this section is cited from Gregorc's book An Adult's Guide to Style, 1982b) People who are dominant CS are usually practical, thorough, well-organized and prefer quiet, structured environments. CS individuals tend to perceive reality as the concrete world of the physical senses, and think in a sequential and orderly fashion. The CS can detect the most minute details, working with the exactitude of a machine (Gregorc, 1982a). The CS student is a perfectionist and prefers being told what to do. These learners do not like to go against the norm, view work as a job assignment, and enjoy being physically involved and active in lessons. AS people consider themselves as evaluative, analytical, and logical individuals with a preference for mentally stimulating, orderly, and quiet environments. The AS has an academic-type mind which is driven by a thirst for knowledge. To an AS, "knowledge is power", and the ability to synthesize and relate concepts enables the AS to transmit ideas (both through the spoken and written word) intelligibly and eloquently. AS learners thrive on teachers who are experts in their area of interest, learning well through lecture-style teaching. AR individuals are highly focused on the world of feeling and emotion, and are sensitive, spontaneous, attuned, person-oriented people. Thought processes of AR individuals tend to be nonlinear, multidimensional, emotional, perceptive, and critical. AR people prefer active, free, and colorful environments. ARs thrive on building relationships with others and, as learners, dislike extremely structured assignments. Finally, CR individuals process information in three-dimensional patterns and think intuitively, instinctively, impulsively, and independently. CR people prefer competitive, unrestricted, and stimulus-rich environments. CRs can be risk-takers and can easily jump to conclusions, often correctly. Such individuals are divergent thinkers, thriving in environments which engender exploration. CR learners do not need many details to solve a problem, instead operating according to personally constructed standards. Overall, everyone has the capacity to learn within each of the above channels; no one is a "pure type" (Gregorc, 1982b, 41). Therefore, The Style Delineator Trademark is a tool which: "provides an individual with a key to understand better the subtle and potent qualities of the mind, (their) behavior, the behavior of others and the demands placed upon individuals by their environment." (Gregorc, 1982b, 41) Learning styles and CAI: An overview CAI and learner profiles A study conducted by Friend and Cole (1990) discovered that sensingthinking individuals (dimensions correlated with CS and AS) responded more favorably to CAI than did intuitive-feeling types (dimensions which are correlated with AR). Friend and Cole postulated that intuitive-feeling types require more human interaction to achieve desired learning outcomes, and that CAI may not be suitable for all learners. Enochs et al. (1985) found that concrete learners (as determined by Kolb's Learning Style Inventory) learned better from a CAI session than did abstract learners. Pritchard (1982) gives further support for the claim that CAI may not accommodate all learning styles equally. In his article on educational computing, Pritchard explained that CAI is suited best for individuals with an affinity for accuracy and attending to detail. Moreover, the researcher claims individuals with certain learning styles may be more partial to learning from computers than would others, and that people who have a preference for CAI usually enjoy working alone (see also Wood et al., 1996). In keeping with CAI and learner profiles, Hoffman and Waters (1982) stated that CAI is suited best for individuals who: "...have the ability to quietly concentrate, are able to pay attention to details, have an affinity for memorizing facts, and can stay with a single track until completion" (p. 51). Dunn and Dunn (1979) reported that certain students may only achieve through selected instructional methods (eg, CAI, whole-group instruction, etc.), and that matching can significantly improve academic achievement. Dunn and Dunn asserted that students who are motivated, require specific instructions, are sequential, and enjoy frequent feedback generally do well with programmed learning such as CAI. However, students who are kinesthetic, peer-oriented learners (ie, AR learners) may not be engaged adequately by the same method of instruction. The computer as a matching tool Although the idea of matching instruction to students' learning styles has been supported in the literature (eg, Butler, 1984; Hettiger, 1988), it can be difficult for educators to match teaching and learning styles in the traditional classroom. It has been argued that effective CAI can correct for many teachers' inability to meet the needs of all learners (Schlechter, 1991). Yet, CAI may not be the preferred mode of learning for all students. According to Gregorc (1985), sequential students (CS and AS) tend to prefer CAI because the computer is seen as an extension of the sequential person's mind. Random individuals (CR and AR) require environments which are flexible and provide opportunities for multidimensional thinking (Butler, 1984). AR individuals, in particular, are inherently social and enjoy learning with others (Butler, 1984). It is apparent that traditional CAI does not always provide such an environment for this group of learners. Unlike the teacher who may be able to troubleshoot and modify lessons to meet the specific learning needs of the student, the computer is only as good as the program that has been created for it; and, as Gregorc (1985) wrote: "Students who cannot adapt to the demands of the medium are 1) denied access to the content and goals, and 2) are vulnerable to possible psychological damage if they cannot free themselves of the medium .... Children can therefore become victims of a medium which is offensive to them. They are at the mercy of the machine." (p. 168) Moreover, because a computer requires sequential thinking in order to gain access to its content (Gregorc, 1985), many CR and AR individuals may become flustered and agitated when problems arise with the medium. Gregorc (1985) warns that problems such as "burnout" and other mental and physical ailments can arise if individuals are made to accept certain media which are seen as adversive. Butler (1984) claimed that technology, in general, places demands on the learner. The computer is often not inherently flexible, intuitive or adaptive, and may therefore restrict the behaviors and responses of the user. As a result, "learners can master such equipment only when they have mastered its invisible demands" (p. 27). The author concluded that "an all out movement towards computer-aided instruction is bound to leave many students behind" (p. 29). In an effort to ensure that all learners can benefit from computer technology, Gregorc (1985) recommended that leaders (eg, teachers, administrators, employers, professors) provide human mediators who can correct for matching problems that may arise from using an inappropriate and potentially invasive learning medium. Further support for the notion of instructional matching was voiced by Burger (1985). In her opinion, CAI may be overused to a certain degree: "Requiring all students to use [Computer-Aided Instruction[ may not be in the best interest of the student. The matching of the teaching style of the specific computer program and the learning style of the student must be considered." (p. 21) Inasmuch as the computer can be a powerful learning medium, the machine is limited in its capacity to modify instruction to meet individual needs (Enochs et al., 1985; Gregorc, 1985). While there have been advances in the area of intelligent tutoring and adaptive interfaces (see Steinberg and Gitomer, 1992; Mills and Ragan, 1994), some of the software interfaces that are currently available are unintuitive and unnecessarily complex (Mitta and Packebusch, 1995). Wallace and Anderson (1993) explained "designing good computer interfaces has proven a formidable challenge" (p. 259). Hence, many students may be forced to adapt and harmonize with the computer (ie, style flex) in order to attain desired learning goals. "These inanimate objects lack empathy. Machines cannot sense the opportunities, qualifications, fears or problems. Nor can they sense the pressures from the forced intimacy we demand between learners and the media. Without compassion, there are no adjustments or alternative approaches offered. There is no sense of harm or restraint as the frozen medium makes its learning demands for sympathetic resonance. School personnel must recognize these facts when purchasing machines." (Gregorc, 1985; 168) Butler (1984) elucidated the notion of mismatching learning styles and media discussed by Gregorc (1985). "Instructional technology biases the way information is presented, and demands, to varying degrees, that we use certain mediation channels" (p. 237). In other words, the use of technology may systematically discriminate against certain learners who are unable to match learning styles with the medium. Just as the lecture approach in education is best suited to AS learners (Gregorc, 1982b), so the computer may be better suited to certain learning styles. Method Problem Research suggests that CAI may have a limited ability to accommodate users with varying learning styles (eg, Butler, 1984; Gregorc, 1985; Hettiger, 1988; Cordell, 1991). Based on the limited number of studies examining the learning styles and CAI, it would appear that sequential students fare better with most CAI applications than do random students. Yet, in any given classroom, one half of students have a propensity for learning best in the random mediation channel (O'Brien, 1994). When coupled with the fact that the use of hypermedia information systems with little or no teacher guidance is increasing in education (Small and Grabowski, 1992), the need for continuing research in the area becomes apparent. Specifically, further research in the area of learning styles and human- computer interaction is needed in order to understand better the influences of individual differences and CAI. Research questions Since this appears to be the first study to investigate the Gregorc mediation channels and their impact on learning from, and interacting with, a CAI program, no hypotheses have been made. I, instead, explored the following research questions: 1. Will learning outcomes differ significantly based on student cognitive learning styles as measured by The Gregorc Style Delineator Trademark? 2. Will human-computer interaction behaviors (ie, time spent on the program, navigation, events recorded, video, tools and lesson preference) differ significantly based on student cognitive learning styles as measured by The Gregorc Style Delineator Trademark? 3. Will differences in entry level domain knowledge affect learning outcomes above and beyond that of learning style? Subjects Seventy University of Calgary undergraduate volunteers (26 males, 44 females) participated in the study. The following is a breakdown of students by Faculty: Nursing = 18; Kinesiology = 20; Education = 13; Other = 19. Treatment To investigate differences between participants, learning style groups received the same treatment. For the purposes of this study, the onerescuer adult CPR procedure was used to collect data. Content for the CAI program was vetted for accuracy and validated by a three member committee comprised of experienced CPR Instructor Trainers. The entire experimental sessions took two hours to complete for each group of approximately 15 participants. One hour was devoted to assessing and interpreting learning style scores. The second hour was dedicated to the CAI session. Following completion of the workshop, the researcher explained the interface to the participants so that each learner would be familiar with the features and options available to them during the CAI session. Participants then completed the on-line questionnaire (comprised of six demographical questions measuring participants' age, year of program, gender, comfort level with CAI, and CPR confidence level), the 20 question pre-test and the tutorial program. No time restriction was imposed on the learners during the CAI session, as time was a variable under investigation. It was imperative that learners did not feel rushed to complete their learning in a stipulated time limit; similarly, a time restriction may have forced quicker learners to stretch out the CAI session to meet the time restriction. Participants worked independently on the computer, using headphones to listen to audio information. CPR is a psychomotor skill requiring knowledge of theoretical principles, procedural steps and performance principles and practices. The computer program instructs and tests both theory and understanding of procedures necessary to perform CPR, leaving motor performance instruction and evaluation to a certified CPR instructor. Both the pre-test and the post-test were comprised of ten knowledge-type questions, five comprehension questions and five application questions (20 multiple choice questions in total). Questions covered one-rescuer Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation (CPR) guidelines and procedures as stipulated by the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada's Emergency Cardiac Care Committee. Construct validity for the test items was determined by a three member CPR instructor trainer review committee. The test-retest reliability alpha coefficient for the examination was determined to be 0.86 for the pre-test and 0.89 for the post-test. Instrument The Gregorc Style Delineator Trademark is a widely-used measure of assessing cognitive learning styles (O'Brien, 1994). The assessment tool was selected, in part, for the following reasons: • Easy to administer • Easy to interpret • Self-scoring battery • Relatively quick to administer and complete • Inexpensive • Discrete, easily reportable scales • The only inventory available with a technical manual for administrators • Validity and reliability measures have been supported by research (eg, Gregorc, 1982a) Joniak and Isaksen (1988) examined the internal consistency of The Style Delineator Trademark. The data revealed alpha coefficients raging from 0.23 to 0.66, below that which was reported by Gregorc (1982 a). O'Brien (1990) found similar results. Using a sample size of 263 undergraduate students, O'Brien reported alpha coefficients ranging from O. 51 for the AS scale to 0.64 for the CS scale, but concluded that internal consistency scales meet minimal requirements for factor definition (O'Brien, 1990). Gregorc (1982a) reported test-retest alpha coefficients of 0.85 to 0.88. In addition, Gregorc (1982a) published internal consistency reliability coefficients ranging from 0.89 for the AS scale to 0.93 for the AR scale, and predictive validity correlations ranging from 0.55 to 0.76 (all figures significant at the p < 0.001 level). Results were based on a sample size of 110 participants. Quality of material With any CAI program, quality of material presented is always an issue. According to Rushby (1997), three factors are essential to ensuring that a CAI program meets acceptable standards: content is accurate and up to date, the program is rigorously tested to ensure minimal running errors, and the program is free from typographical errors. The program used for the study meets all three quality standards. A CPR committee verified the accuracy of content ensuring that it was up to date and a reflection of current practices in CPR. The program went through a lengthy six month beta testing stage at which time all errors were found by a focus group and addressed by the programmer. To this date, we have had no error reports from any Nursing schools who have purchased the program. In terms of typographical accuracy, an editor with 10 years in the area was used to verify the textual consistency and grammatical structure of the program lessons and narration tracks. Independent measures Learning styles--Subjects' highest learning style scores (as determined by The Style Delineator Trademark) were treated as a measure of dominant learning style. The following is a breakdown of subjects by dominant learning style score: CS = 20; CR = 20; AS = 14; AR = 16. Domain knowledge level--Pre-test scores were used as a measure of domain knowledge levels and for learning outcome achievement analysis. Dependent measures A program audit trail file was created for the purposes of this study to track participants' patterns of learning. Together with the pre-test score, learning style scores, and the preliminary survey information, the audit trail file also stored detailed information (eg, which tools and video options were accessed on which screens, and continuous time reports). The term "patterns of learning", referred to in a study by Liu and Reed (1994), are used in this study to describe human-computer interaction indices. These indicators are listed below: Total time (in minutes) to complete the tutorial--Participants were given no time restriction to move through the tutorial program; hence, time scores varied from subject to subject. Navigation trend--Participants' patterns of movement through the tutorial were determined by a numerical score. The tutorial consisted of 15 instructional screens detailing the discrete steps in performing CPR. The audit trail recorded navigation by assigning a '+1' value when the next screen button was selected, and a '-1' value when the previous screen button was selected. For example, if a subject were to move through the procedures in a linear fashion, a score of 14 would be assigned (14 'next screen' selections x 1). If a subject were to go back three screens while covering all 14 steps, a net score of + 11 would be assigned by the audit trail file ({14 'next screen' selections x 1} + {3 'back screen' selections x -1}). Total number of tools used--The frequency with which the subject accessed the program tools (note pad, search tool, index tool and glossary) was reflected by this measure. Total number of video events--The number of times the user accessed video controls 'Play', 'Pause', 'Rewind', and 'Volume' was indicated by this total. It is important to note that the video, by default, played automatically upon moving to a new screen; hence, the score reflected in this category indicated the number of video events above and beyond the standard score of 15 (or 15 video play options). User preference for instructional sequence--The tutorial program was comprised both of a 15 step tutorial sequence and a video review section. The video review section summarized all video steps covered in the tutorial. Learners could choose to watch the review video prior to, or following, the tutorial. A code of '1' was assigned to those participants who chose the 'Review Video' option first; an indicator of '2' was assigned for learners who chose to move through the tutorial first. Total number of events--This measure indicated the level of user interaction. This number was derived by adding the total number of tools used, videos accessed, and navigational events. A low number reflects user passivity. Post-test results--Learners completed a 20-question multiple-choice posttest. The results from the post-test were used as a dependent variable for the purposes of achievement analysis. Results The alpha level representing statistical significance was set at the p < 0.05 level. Results that have lower or higher p values will be reported as such. Data were analysed using SPSS 6 and BMDP IV. Learning outcomes To explore whether learning outcomes were influenced by dominant learning style groups, a two-way ANOVA (2 x 4 factorial analysis) was conducted. The data revealed a significant main effect for the pre-test and post-test means over time (F[sub (1,66)] = 57.91, p < 0.001). There was also a significant interaction between learning style and learning outcome (F[sub (3.66)] = 20.11, p < 0.001). Figure 1 depicts the interaction between dominant learning style and learning outcome. The mean test scores reveal that three of the four dominant learning style groups showed gains from the pre-test to the post-test. The AS group increased an average of 3.64 points (or 18%), displaying the highest gain of the three groups. CS and CR groups increased an average of about 2 points (or 10%). Interestingly, the AR group decreased from pre-test to post-test by an average of just over 2 points (or 10%). In summary, the results indicate that there were significant differences in achievement between the four dominant learning style groups. Dominant learning styles, it would appear, affected the magnitude and direction of the differences in the pre-test and posttest results. Human-computer interaction To investigate the effects of dominant learning styles on human-computer interaction, a MANOVA was conducted using six patterns of learning as the dependent variables and dominant learning style as the independent variable. Results indicated that there was not a significant effect for patterns of learning by dominant learning style (1 = 0.6 6, F = 1.51, p = 0.09). The data suggest that only one pattern of learning, navigation style, differed significantly at the p < 0.01 level. Results of a post-hoe Scheffe test indicated that the AS and CR group means were significantly different from the AR group mean. It would appear that the AR group was the least linear of the four dominant learning style groups, recording a mean score of just over 10 points. Table 1 delineates the mean scores for the six patterns of learning. Results suggested that AR participants spent less time with the program, used less video and made fewer interactions with the computer than did the other three dominant learning style groups. In contrast, AS subjects tended to spend more time with the program, used a higher number of tools and interacted to a higher degree with the computer than did the other three groups. Although not statistically significant, mean scores do suggest some interesting differences between dominant learning style, to be explored in future studies. The overall lack of significant differences between dominant learning style and patterns of learning measures of time, total events, tools, video and lesson preference suggests that learning styles, as measured by The Gregorc Style Delineator Trademark, did not significantly affect the way in which learners interacted with the computer-aided instructional software. Domain knowledge Examination was also conducted to ascertain whether content knowledge affected learning outcomes above and beyond that of learning styles. Pre-test results indicated that there were disparities domain knowledge participants possessed. An ANCOVA was identify the influences of learning style on post-test controlling for (or equalizing) differences in pretest demonstrated by the four learning style groups. in the entry-level conducted to scores, while knowledge The ANCOVA showed a significant effect for pre1test (b= 0.79; t = 8.4: sig t = 0.001). However, learning styles still retained a significant influence on post-test scores (F[sub 14, 65] = 19.58, p < 0.001). Furthermore, the adjusted r2 value of O. 52 suggested that dominant learning styles alone explained 52% of the variance in post-test scores, after controlling for the influences of pre-test scores. Discussion Learning outcomes In terms of learning outcomes, the data suggests that, as a participants showed an increase from pre-test to post-test, significant at the p < 0.001 level. This would suggest that program led to gains in Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation (CPR) group, statistically the tutorial knowledge. Once subjects were distilled into their dominant learning style groups, however, the data revealed a significant interaction effect between learning style group and achievement levels. In short, learning styles significantly affected both the magnitude and direction of achievement levels. The AS group made a gain of close to four points, while the CS and CR groups made modest gains of about two points from pre-test to post-test. Interestingly, the AR group decreased an average of more than two points from pre-test to post-test, a result which has significant implications for CAI if findings are supported by future studies. The question of why the AR group decreased from pretest to post-test will be discussed further. Theoretical explanations for achievement differences According to Gregorc (1982b), individual learning styles influence preference for method of instruction. Butler (1984) and Gregorc (1985) believe that dominance in CS and AS mediation channels predisposes the individual to having a preference for working with computers (be it in the capacity as a computer programmer, or as a learner using CAI software). Randoms are said to find working with computers frustrating (see Gregorc, 1985, 202,203). In this exploratory study, CR learners did well with the computer program, but ARs did not. Although some of the content covered in the tutorial program required linear processing, CR individuals did well compared with AR learners. Hence, one cannot argue successfully the point that the content, and not the computer medium, was responsible for the differences in learning outcomes. It would appear that possessing the abstract and random qualities together made for a less successful computer-aided learning session. Butler (1984) explained that AR individuals prefer human contact throughout the learning process, and enjoy tasks requiring verbal, multidimensional responses; certain forms of CAI, therefore, may be unsuitable for these learners. Results from the present study are consistent with results reported by Davidson et al. (1992). The researchers found that AR individuals, enrolled in a computer applications university course, showed significantly lower achievement levels than did the other three Style Delineator Trademark groups. AS individuals showed the highest gains in the course, indicating their ability to work well with computer technology. The only significant differences in methodology between the two studies are that Davidson et al. utilized course assignments as a measure of success, whereas this study used pre-tutorial and post-tutorial results as an indicator of achievement levels. Further exploration: differences in pre-test group scores It is interesting to note that the pre-test means were different between the four learning style groups. While the AS group had a mean pre-test score of just 10, the AR group had a mean pre-test score of 15. Such a sharp contrast may be explained by a number of factors. a) Varied CPR background: It appears that the CPR course background varied between the four learning style groups (CS = 2, CR = 1.4, AS = 1.5, and AR = 2.8). An ANOVA was conducted to investigate whether the differences between groups were significant. Results from the ANOVA indicate that differences in CPR course backgrounds were statistically significant (F[sub (3.66)] = 5.16, p < 0.01). A post-hoc Scheffe test was used to ascertain which groups were significantly different. The AR group's mean CPR course background score was deemed significantly different from the CR and AS groups' score. Such contrastingly different group course backgrounds may explain why the AR group had such a high pre-test score. b) CPR confidence: The data suggest that the four dominant learning style groups differed in CPR perceived confidence. As may be recalled, the preliminary survey asked participants to rate their CPR level of confidence (using a Likert-Type Scale; 1 being very confident, 5 being not confident at all). Group mean scores (CS = 3.3, CR = 3.4, AS = 4.1, AR = 2.7) were significantly different (F[sub (3.66)], = 2.71, p < 0.05), indicating differences in CPR confidence by learning style groups. Scheffe post-hoc analysis shows that the AS group mean was significantly different from the three other groups. The majority of CS and All group participants indicated that they were pursuing Nursing degrees; hence, regular CPR certification is required, and may explain the variation in learning style groups' scores. It is not surprising, then, to see disparities in the mean pre-test scores across groups. The AS group, the majority of whom were from Education or other faculties, had taken the least number of CPR courses of the four groups, and had the lowest confidence level in their skills. This group also displayed the lowest pre-test score. Similarly, the All group recorded taking the most number of CPR courses of the four groups, and reported the highest CPR confidence levels. The question of entry level differences in domain knowledge It can be argued that there were obvious background disparities between the four groups upon entering the study. While it is true that groups did differ based on their pre-test scores, the ANCOVA showed that groups still differed significantly on the posttest when controlling for pre-test differences in knowledge. Hence, it would appear that achievement in the CAI session was affected most significantly by cognitive learning styles. Although three of the four dominant learning style groups learned from the CAI lesson, AR learners consistently did not (two dominant AR subjects increased scores from pre-test to post-test, nine decreased scores, and four showed no change). Patterns of learning Patterns of learning, indicating human-computer interaction behaviors, were not significantly different between dominant groups. Although five of six patterns of learning were not significant at the p < 0.05 level, three indices showed some interesting between-group differences. The mean scores revealed in Table I show some interesting differences between groups. It would appear that the All group spent, on average, less time in the program, used less video, and recorded fewer events than did the other three dominant learning style groups. The AS group showed diametrically opposite behaviors, spending more time in the program, interacting to a higher degree, and using more video than did the other three groups. Furthermore, the AR group recorded a significantly different mean navigation value (significant at the p < 0.001 level) from the other groups. AR participants, on average, recorded a mean value of around ten points, indicating some degree of non-linear movement (either moving backward to review previous screens, or using the index tool to jump from step to step). The other learning style groups showed values which hovered around the expected level of 14. Patterns of learning as indicators of achievement Upon closer inspection of the audit trail print-outs, it would appear that many AR participants missed entire screens while traversing from step to step. One participant missed five screens, jumping from step 3 to step 9, moving through the remaining six steps, and then finishing back with step 8. It is not known if the subject knew the content covered by the missed screens; however, it is clear that such an approach to learning CPR--a procedure that requires linear movement through a pre-determined sequence of steps--may interfere with current learning, and may very well interfere with previous learning. One knowledge-type test question, for example, asked participants to put the steps of CPR in order. A correct response for this question required the learner to have moved through the program in a linear fashion. Skipping steps and moving to previous screens may have interfered with the learning required for a correct answer to these types of questions. When teaching CPR in the traditional classroom setting, it would be detrimental for the instructor to move from step 1 to step 12, and then back to step 2. Regardless of the type of learning style one has, certain materials require sequential processing. Excessive and inappropriate use of the index tool--a tool that allows the user to jump from any given step to another--may have contributed to cognitive interference in many of the AR subjects. According to Milheim and Azbell (1988) cited in Small and Grabowski (1992), systems that give the user control over the learning process are empowering for some and destructive for others. Small and Grabowski warn that too much user control can lead to navigation decisions resulting in either skipping pertinent content or leaving the tutorial program before all content has been thoroughly covered (also see Schroeder, 1994). Castelli et al. (1996) discovered that many users of hypermedia "get lost" in hyperspace. The notion of becoming disoriented due to incessant "jumping around" is consistent with findings from Hammond (1989). The overall lack of interaction recorded by AR subjects (based on low events score, video use and time in program) may have resulted from a lack of interest in the CAI session. Attitudes towards computers can be a significant indicator of student achievement with the computer (Brudenell and Stewart, 1990). A breakdown of the computer attitudes survey question by dominant learning styles indicated that close to 60% of AS subjects reported being comfortable with using the computer. In contrast, only 36% of AR subjects felt comfortable with computer technology. Over 50% of CS subjects and 55% of CR subjects felt comfortable with using the computer. Hence, AR subjects were less likely to be comfortable with using the computer than were the other learning style groups. Motivation is also the key to any type of self-paced CAI session, according to findings from Keller (1968). Keller, in his essay on computers in the school, warned of the dangers of leaving important instructional decisions to students. Students may neither have the metacognitive abilities nor the motivation to select appropriate paths for achieving desired learning goals. Small and Grabowski (1992) found that high motivation levels led to subjects spending more time with the computer program, and subsequently contributed to higher learning outcomes. Low motivation levels had an inverse effect. In direct contrast to the AR group, the data revealed that AS subjects were highly engaged in the CAI lesson. Although not statistically significant, all patterns of learning appeared to indicate that these subjects interacted to a high degree with the program. Such enthusiasm and diligence may have contributed to the higher achievement levels observed. In terms of patterns of learning, Liu and Reed (1994) also found that, overall, human-computer interaction measures were not significantly affected by learning styles under investigation in their study. However, field independence (a propensity for thinking analytically and logically) was linked to using the index tool, and field dependence (thinking in a more global way) was correlated with using more video. In addition, field dependent subjects used the courseware significantly more than did field independent participants. (It should be noted that comparisons cannot be made between field dependence/independence and Gregorc's mediation channels. There is no research to support relationships between these dimensions of cognitive learning styles.) Recommendations for the responsible use of technology in education The following recommendations are meant to be used as guidelines for the successful implementation of computer technology, and are based on findings from this study. It remains essential for a clearly stated list of recommendations, outlining proper computer use, to be published. In this way, all individuals are afforded the right to learn in the way that suits them best. 1. Educators should closely monitor--and mediate where necessary--all computer instruction. Students should have clear and identifiable tasks to complete, and learning outcomes should be measured periodically. This is consistent with the views expressed by Greenberg and Pengelby (1989). 2. Students should be asked to express their views towards CAI through the use of a teacher-constructed survey. Furthermore, if teachers have an interest, they should ascertain the learning styles of their students, and provide insight on how learning styles influence students preferences for instruction. Learning style scores could be used in conjunction with preference surveys to identify potential matching problems. 3. Opportunities for group work should be given to those students who are hesitant to work on the computer alone. Research shows that AR students enjoy working with others and sharing ideas during the learning process (Ross, 1998). Since the focus shifts from being intimate with a machine to working collaboratively with a group, the potentially negative effects of CAL for these individuals may be masked and/or lessened (Ross, 1998). 4. Government Departments of Education should remain cautious with sweeping decisions to convert entire curricula onto electronic media (as was mentioned in the article by Dwyer, 1996). The goals of such a process should be weighed against the potential problems (eg, alienating certain learners). 5. To avoid alienating a certain learning style group, educators should continue to incorporate a number of different teaching strategies into their lessons. If a particular student is unable to learn from the computer, instructors should provide alternative ways for content to be delivered. Conclusion CAI is rapidly becoming one of the most influential media of instruction in educational environments. However, findings from this exploratory study indicate that CAI, as an instructional methodology, may not be suitable for all learners. While computer-aided instruction has tremendous potential to provide teachers and industry with a powerful educational tool, educators must be cognizant of inherent differences which exist between learners-differences such as cognitive learning styles. Results from this exploratory study suggest that some learners (AR learners in particular) may have difficulty adapting to certain forms of computer-mediated learning. Studies continue to support the need to critically evaluate this ubiquitous tool which has permeated the classroom and homes more quickly than most other technologies have in the past (see Schlechter, 1991). As more research is conducted in the area of CAI, information regarding appropriate and educationally sound uses for the CAI will become available. It remains essential, then, that the computer continue to be used as a tool for supplementing classroom instruction. Some learners may need greater support and guidance from the teacher, while others may be able to learn from the computer relatively independently. Thus, teachers should not assume every student will automatically benefit from computers in the classroom. There remains the need for interpersonal contact and guidance to ensure that all students attain their learning potential. Limitations of study and opportunities for further research Results from this study have some significant implications for computeraided instruction, if supported by further research. If replicated, a number of considerations should be followed to improve the generalizability of the results. 1. This study used the traditional goals--tutor--test approach to gather data from participants. A study should be conducted with a computer program adhering to a different learning model (eg, discrimination learning, simulation, intelligent tutoring system, etc.). If results prove to be consistent with those of this study, then it can be more conclusively argued that CAI may not sufficiently accommodate all learners equally. 2. It is not clear whether low motivation, as indicated by AR subjects' patterns of learning, was due to the computer or to the content presented in the program. Since the majority of AR subjects were in a Nursing program--and are assumed to enjoy medical procedures and training--it is questionable that the CPR content, in and of itself, led to disparities in pre and post-test scores. However, further research may shed some light on the question of subject matter versus computer instruction. 3. This study used content that was familiar to most, if not all, subjects (as indicated by the relatively high pre-test mean). Inasmuch as it was desirable to have subjects who had varying levels of domain knowledge for the purposes of exploring one research question (namely, domain knowledge as a measure of individual differences), further research should be conducted using a subject area that is unfamiliar to all participants. In this way, learning outcomes could be more accurately measured. 4. Subjects were expected to interact with the CAI tutorial program for a relatively short period of time (about 30-45 minutes). Further research should explore the effects of learning styles and other individual differences on CAI using a one week to one month study time frame. 5. This study used a program requiring the learner to move though content in a somewhat linear way. This study should be replicated using a program with content that can be learned in a non-linear manner. 6. Further research should explore the impact of group learning on learners who may be pre-disposed to encountering difficulty with the computer. AR learners matched with other AR learners may be more successful when using the computer to learn. Research should determine what type of cooperative groupings work best for "imperiled" computer users. The previous recommendations for future research have a common theme: there remains a need for more research in the area of learning styles and humancomputer interaction. The literature suggests that there are definite learning preferences which are consistent with learning style profiles. It follows, then, that CAI may not be suitable for all learners. Unfortunately, the relationship between learning styles and computermediated learning needs to be explored in greater detail before more conclusive statements can be made. Table 1: Mean pattern of learning scores by dominant learning style score Legend for Chart: A B C D E - Learning Style N Mean SD A B C D E CS CR AS AR 20 20 14 16 28.95 26.85 30.64 23.31 7.06 9.27 13.37 9.10 CS CR AS AR 20 20 14 16 11.95 14.05 14.42 10.06 3.56 3.87 4.14 4.74 CS CR AS AR 20 20 14 16 53.70 60.60 60.71 44.06 20.86 32.27 28.00 15.08 CS CR AS AR 20 20 14 16 1.35 1.35 1.64 1.37 0.49 0.49 0.50 0.50 CS CR AS AR 20 20 14 16 5.10 5.80 7.00 4.00 5.92 7.43 8.18 3.06 Time Navigation Events Lesson preference Tools Video CS 20 8.00 8.78 CR 20 16.65 21.64 AS 14 11.57 10.97 AR 16 7.81 6.97 GRAPH: Figure 1: Interaction between tutorial effect and dominant learning style group References Brudenell I and Stewart C (1990) Adult learning styles and attitudes towards computer-assisted instruction Journal of Nursing Education 29 (2) 79-83. Butler K (1984) Learning and teaching styles in theory and practise Gabriel Systems Inc., Maynard, MA. Castelli C, Colazzo L and Molinari A (1996) Getting lost in hyperspace: Lessons learned and future directions CD-ROM Proceedings from the annual ED-MEDIA/ED-TELECOM conference Article No 208. Cordell B J (1991) A study of learning styles and computer-assisted instruction Computers Education 16 (2) 175-183. Davidson G V, Savenye W C and Orr K B (1992) How do learning styles relate to performance in a computer application course? Journal of Research on Computers in Education 24 (3) 349-358. Dunn R D and Dunn K J (1979) Learning/teaching styles: Should they/can they be matched? Educational Leadership 36 (4) 238-244. Dwyer V (1996) Surfing back to school Madean's Magazine 26 August 40-46. Enochs J R, Handley H M and Wollenberg J P (1984) The relationship of learning style, reading vocabulary, reading comprehension and aptitude for learning to achievement in the sell-paced computer-assisted instructional modes of the yeoman "A" school at the Navel Technical Training Center. Meridian. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Mid-South ERA, New Orleans. Friend C L and Cole C L (1990)Learner control in computer-based instruction: A current literature review Educational Technology November 47-49. Greenberg HI J and Pengelby R M (1989) A conceptual basis for the role of the microcomputer in the teaching and learning of college math in Maurer H (ed) Computer-Aided Learning: International Conference ICCAL (second ed) Springer-Verlag. Gregorc A F (1979) Learning/teaching styles: potent forces behind them Educational Leadership 36 (4) 234-236. Gregorc A F (1982a) Gregorc Style Delineator: Development Technical and Administration Manual Gregorc Associates Inc., Columbia, CT. Gregorc A F (1982b) An adult's guide to style Gregorc Associates Inc., Columbia, CT. Gregorc A F (1984) Learning is a matter of style Vocational Education 59 (3) 27-29. Gregorc A F (1985) Inside Style: Beyond the Basics Gregorc Associates Inc., Columbia, CT. Hammond N (1989) Hypermedia and learning: who guides whom? in Maurer H (ed) Computer-Aided Learning: International Conference ICCAL (second ed) Springer-Verlag, 167-181. Hawkridge D (1995) Do companies need technology-based training? in Heap N, Thomas R, Einon G, Mason R and Makay H (eds) Information Technology and Society Sage, London, 182-210. Hettiger G A (1988) Operationalizing cognitive constructs in the design of computer-based instruction Annual Meeting of the Association for Educational Communications and Technology H) 295 645. Hoffman J L and Waters K (1982) Some effects of student personality on success with computer-assisted instruction Educational Technology 47-48. Joniak A J and Isaksen S G (1988) The Gregorc style delineator: Internal consistency and its relationship to Kirton's adaptive-innovative distinction Educational and Psychological Measurement 48 1043-1049. Keller R S (1968) Goodbye teacher Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 1 79-89. Liu M and Reed W M (1994) The relationship between the learning strategies and learning styles in a hypermedia environment Computers in Human Behavior 10 (4) 419-434. Marquez M and Lehman J D (1992) Hypermedia user interface design: The role of individual differences in the placement of icon buttons Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia 1 (4) 417-429. Milheim W D and Azbell J W (1988) How past research on learner control can aid in the design of interactive video materials. Paper presented at the National Conference for the Association for Educational Communications and Technology, New Orleans, LA. Mills S C and Ragan T J (1994) Adapting instruction to individualize learner differences: a research paradigm for computer-based instruction Paper presented at the 1994 National Convention of the Association for Educational Communications and Technology ED 373 740. Mitta D and Packebusch S J (1995) Improving interface quality: an investigation of human-computer interaction task learning Ergonomics 38 (7) 1307-1325. Nelson W A and Palumbo D B (1992) Learning instruction and hypermedia Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia 1 287-299. O'Brien T P (1990) Construct validation of the Gregorc style delineator: an application of LISREL 7 Education and Psychological Measurement 50 631-636. O'Brien T P (1994) Cognitive learning styles and academic achievement in secondary education Journal of Research and Development in Education 28 (1) 11-21. Price R V (1991) Computer-aided Instruction: A Guide Jot Authors Brooks/Cole, Pacific Grove, CA. Rasmussen K and Davidson G V (1996) Dimensions of learning styles and their influence on perform-ante in hypermedia lessons CD-ROM Proceedings from the annual ED-MEDIA/ED-TELECOM conference Article No 385. Reed W M (1996) A review of the research on the effect of learning styles on hypermedia-related performance and attitudes CD-ROM Proceedings from the annual ED-MEDIA/ED-TELECOM conference Article No 491. Ross J L (1997) The effects of cognitive learning styles on human-computer interaction: Implications for computer-aided learning Unpublished Master of Science Thesis, The University of Calgary, Alberta, Canada. Ross J L (1998) On-line but off course: a wish list for distance educators. International Electronic Journal for Leadership in Learning 2 (3). Rushby N J (1997) Quality criteria for multimedia Association for Learning Technology Journal 5(2) 18-30. Schroeder E E (1994) Navigating through hypertext: Navigational techniques, individual differences and learning Proceedings of Selected Research and Development Presentations at the 1994 National Convention of the Association for Educational Communications and Technology ED 373 760. Schlechter T M (1991) Problems and Promises of Computer-based Training Army Research Institute for Behavioral and Social Sciences Ablex Publishing Corporation, Noorwood, NJ. Small R V and Grabowski B L (1992) An exploratory study of informationseeking behaviors and learning with hypermedia information systems Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia 1 (4) 445-464. Steinberg L S and Gitomer D H (1992) Cognitive task analysis interface design and technical troubleshooting Educational Testing Services, Princeton, NJ (Ed 384 677). Wallace M D and Anderson T J (1993) Approaches to interface design Interacting with Computers (3) 259-278. Wood F, Ford IN, Miller D, Sobczyk G and Duffin R (1996) Information skills searching behaviour and cognitive styles for student-centered learning: a computer assisted learning approach Journal of Information Sciences 22 (2) 79-92. ~~~~~~~~ By Jonathan Ross and Robert Schulz Jonathan L. Ross is a doctoral candidate in Educational Technology at the University of Calgary. He is also a senior instructional designer with Media Learning Systems in the Faculty of Education. His web site address is http://www.ucalgary.ca/~jross. Robert Schulz is a Professor in the Faculty of Management at the same university. Address for correspondence: Faculty of Education, The University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive NW, Calgary, Alberta, Canada T2N 1N4. Tel: + 1 403 220 6490; email: jross@acs.ucalgary.ca ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of British Journal of Educational Technology is the property of Blackwell Publishers and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: British Journal of Educational Technology, Jan99, Vol. 30 Issue 1, p5, 20p, 1 graph. Item Number: 3251744 Result 44 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 46 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Intuition-analysis cognitive style and learning preferences of business and management students. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; STUDENTS -- Attitudes; COGNITIVE styles; INDUSTRIAL management Source: Journal of Managerial Psychology, 1999, Vol. 14 Issue 1/2, p26, 13p, 4 charts, 1 diagram Author(s): Sadler-Smith, Eugene Abstract: Studies the cognitive style and learning preferences of business and management education students. Methodology used on the study; Results and discussion. AN: 1675008 ISSN: 0268-3946 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. View Item: Full Page Image View Links: Check linked full text sources ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] INTUITION-ANALYSIS COGNITIVE STYLE AND LEARNING PREFERENCES OF BUSINESS AND MANAGEMENT STUDENTS A UK exploratory study Keywords Human resource developmenl, Learning styles, Managemenl education, Training, United Kingdom Abstract The study is an attempt to provide empirical elaboration, in the context of business and management education, for the "onion" and cognitive control models of cognitive style. Using, a sample of 226 business and management undergraduates the research explored the relationship between cognitive style measured using the cognitive style, index and learning preference. Using principal components analysis, three, categories of learning preference were discerned (active, reflective and individual. Correlational analysis and one way analysis of variance revealed statistically significant relationships between preferences for reflective and individual methods and cognitive style. The results provide some support for the "onion" and cognitive control models; the implications for business and management education, training and development are discussed. Background Introduction Curry (1983), in her "onion" model, argued that learning style and cognitive style constructs may be grouped into three main types or layers resembling the skin of an onion. At the onion's core is the "central personality" dimension, remote from external influences and stable over time. Overlying this central core are: (1) "cognitive personality style": a relatively permanent and stable characteristic measured by instruments such as the embedded figures test (Witkin, 1962); (2) "information processing style"' a relatively stable set of responses to acquiring and assimilating information in a given learning situation (measured by means of instruments such as the learning styles Inventory (Kolb, 1984)); (3) the outer layer of the onion represents the behavioural manifestations of the interaction between these inner layers and the external environment through the expression of, for example, preferences for particular types of teaching and learning methods, such as self-direction, collaboration and dependence (Grasha and Reichmann, 1975) and specific approaches to learning in given environments and within particular assessment regimes, such as deep versus surface approaches to studying (Entwistle, 1988; Marton and Saljo, 1976). Riding (1997) presents a "cognitive control" model (a theoretical elaboration of Curry) consisting of primary sources (knowledge, personality, gender and cognitive history), cognitive control (the wholistanalytical and verbal-imagery dimensions of cognitive style) and cognitive input (perception) and output (learning strategies). Like the onion model, it is an attempt to unify the relationship between apparently similar constructs. The aim of this paper is to examine, in the context of business and management education, the implicit proposition in the onion and cognitive control models, that learning preference is related to cognitive style. This has clear implications for: • the planning and design of business and management education; • training and development in organisational contexts through the matching (or mismatching) of teaching and learning methods to the cognitive style of the learner; • the development of stylistic versatility (by complementing style with strategies). Learning preferences Learning preferences may be defined as an individual's propensity to choose or express a liking for a particular teaching or learning technique or combination of techniques (Sadler-Smith, 1996). From the work of Reichmann and Grasha, (1974) and Renzulli and Smith (1978) it is possible to synthesise three groups of learning preference: (1) dependence: preference for teacher-directed, highly structured programmes with explicit assignments set and assessed by the teacher; (2) collaboration: discussion-orientation and favouring group projects, collaborative assignments and social interaction; (3) independence: preference for exercising an influence on the content and structure of learning programmes within which the teacher or instructor is a resource (Sadler-Smith and Riding, 1999). The learning preference construct has not been as widely researched as learning style, approaches to studying or cognitive style. Learning preferences represent the outer skin of the "onion" and as such they are the most easily accessible but least stable of the constructs and are the interface between the internal world and external learning environment. Like "learning styles" and "approaches" (which may be considered as varieties of learning strategy), preferences are ways of dealing with the external world (see Figure 1). They differ from learning strategies in that the latter are ways in which the individual acquires and assimilates information, whereas the expression and operationalisation of learning preferences are the ways by which the learner attempts (by accommodating her/his preferences) to adapt to or cope with the demands of the external learning environment. Figure 1 Learning preferences, styles, strategies and cognitive style Sadler-Smith (1997) found statistically significant correlations between learning preferences and learning style (learning styles questionnaireHoney and Mumford, 1992) and approaches to studying (revised approaches to studying inventory - Entwistle and Tait, 1994) but not between learning preference and cognitive style (cognitive styles analysis -Riding, 1991). The present study will explore the latter, using an alternative model of cognitive style. Cognitive style Messick (1984, p. 5) described cognitive style as "consistent individual differences in preferred ways of organising and processing information and experience". Steinberg and Grigorenko described it as representing "a bridge between what might seem to be two fairly distinct areas of psychological investigation: cognition and personality" (Steinberg and Grigorenko, 1997, p. 701). A number of assumptions relating to cognitive style may be identified: (1) it is concerned with the form rather than the content of information processing; (2) it is a pervasive dimension that can be assessed using psychometric techniques; (3) it is stable over time; (4) it is bipolar; (5) it may be value differentiated (i.e. styles describe "different" rather than "better" thinking processes) (Sadler-Smith and Badger, 1998). One model of cognitive style which satisfies these criteria for a "cognitive style" and lends itself to research in a business and management context is the intuition-analysis dimension (Allinson and Hayes, 1996). The style models of Allinson and Hayes and Honey and Mumford may be traced back to their origins in Jungian psychological types. Hurst et al. (1989) in a useful, but concise, summary, described the "types" in terms of information gathering modes (intuition versus sensation) and information evaluation modes (feeling versus thinking) to give four basic types (intuitingfeeling; intuiting-thinking; sensing-feeling; sensing-thinking). Intuition was defined by Bunge (1983, p. 2A8) as "that ill-defined ability to spot problems or errors, to "perceive" relations or similarities ... in short to imagine, conceive, reason or act in novel ways". Analysis, on the other hand, is often presented as the antithesis of intuition: "to analyse ... is to exhibit [an object or system's] components, environment (or context) and structure (organisation)" (Bunge, 1983, p. 219). Hurst et al. (1989) went on to speculate that differences in preferences for each type of thinking may be related to hemispherical differences in the brain: "sensing and thinking are left hemisphere related and intuition and feeling right hemisphere related" (Hurst et al., p. 91). This echoed the views of Mintzberg (1976): "in the left hemisphere of most people's brains the logical thinking processes are to be found ... in contrast the right hemisphere is specialised for simultaneous processing; that is it operates in a more holistic ... way". More recently, Mintzberg (1994a, p. 114) has re-stated these ideas in the context of strategic planning, arguing that the planning function in organisations is populated by two types of person: the analytic ("left-brained') thinker and the creative ("right- brained") thinker. He expressed the view that organisations need both types in "appropriate proportions" (see also Leonard and Strauss (1997) on the "whole-brained organisation"). Like Hurst et al. (1989) and Mintzberg (1976), Allinson and Hayes (1996) speculated on hemispherical differences in the brain as a possible basis for cognitive style differences (stemming from the work of Sperry and others - see Nebes and Sperry (1971); they too use the term "intuition" to describe "right brain" thinking (i.e. immediate judgement based on feeling and the adoption of a global perspective) and "analysis" for "left brain" thinking (i.e. judgement based on mental reasoning and a focus on detail). "Style" in this context is the dominance of one mode of thinking over the other and describes "different" rather than "better" approaches to learning, problem solving, etc. It should be noted that the attribution of differences in analytical versus intuitive behaviour to hemispherical differences in brain functioning should, in the absence of firm neuro-physiological evidence, be treated metaphorically rather than literally (see Riding et al. (1997) for a neurophysiological study of cognitive style). Finally, Allinson and Hayes' intuition-analysis dimension of style may be considered to be broadly equivalent to the wholist-analytical dimension (Riding, 1991) and the adaptor-innovator dimension (Kirton, 1994), though there is a pressing need for concurrent validity studies. Style, preferences and performance Hayes and Allinson (1996) reviewed 19 studies which investigated the effects of matching styles to learning method and found that in 12 studies there was some support for the proposition that matching style and method contributed to improved learning performance. Fox (1984, p. 72) argued that "continuing educators must develop programmes that meet the needs of learners" and suggested that some participants do not "fit" with certain activities. Smith and Renzulli (1984) argue that congruence of style and method can have an effect on learner motivation and "investment" in the learning material. Equally important, matching can "help eliminate barriers to learning which arise when we [educators] fail to address the affective response various teaching modalities elicit from students" (Smith and Renzulli p. 74). Dunn (1984) reviewed several studies in which she found that where students were placed in academic situations where they were taught and/or tested in ways that matched or mismatched their self-reported preferences, those who were matched performed better than those who were mismatched. This led her to conclude that "their preferences must be their strength" (Dunn, 1984, p. 13). Miller (1991) took a somewhat different view: he argued that the analytic-holist model of style allows the possibility of individuals who are skilled at both analytical and holistic functioning- referred to as "versatile". He went on to discuss the issues surrounding attempts to engender "versatility" in those already not predisposed towards it. However, his conclusions are that to do so in all students is a waste of time and is potentially damaging and dangerous (given that styles may be forms of psychological defence). He argued that extremely specialised students should be left alone but that teaching should be accommodated to these styles and that versatility is a reasonable goal in those who are already disposed to it. The challenge as far as Miller was concerned was to identify the specialised and the "protoversatile" (Miller, 1991, p. 236). The "versatility" argument (perhaps through the mismatch) is echoed in the pleas from Mintzberg (1994) for balance in strategic planning teams and Leonard and Strauss (1997) to harness the "energy released by the intersection of different thought processes" to propel innovation (p. 121). The challenge, therefore, for business schools and human resource development practitioners, is to acknowledge the differences that exist between individuals and use the differences constructively, for example, by giving careful consideration to when to "match", when to "mismatch" and how to engender cognitive "versatility". At a more superficial level, the onion and cognitive control models suggest that cognitive style may exert some influence over preferences for different learning methods (for example role play versus lectures). Riding (1991) has argued that style may affect social behaviour, which may suggest that intuitives will tend to be dependent and gregarious and prefer collaborative ' learning situations, while analysts may be isolated and self-reliant. Hence, it may be expected that different business and management teaching and learning methods, with their varying degrees of social interaction and autonomy, would be viewed more or less favourably by different cognitive style groups. Similarly, with respect to the cognitive aspects of learning, Allinson and Hayes (1996) argued that analysts may prefer to pay attention to detail, focus on "hard" data, adopt a step-bystep approach to learning and are self-reliant. This suggests that analysts may prefer learning methods which allow opportunities for independent work with the opportunity to analyse data and reflect on information and experiences. Leonard and Strauss (1997) suggested that abstract thinkers (who share some of the attributes of analysts) will prefer to assimilate information from a variety of sources such as books, reports, videos, etc. Conversely, Allinson and Hayes (1996) argued that intuitives are less concerned with detail, adopt a global perspective and take an actionoriented approach to learning and problem solving. These "experiential" individuals will prefer to get information from "direct interaction with people and things" (Leonard and Strauss, 1997, p. 113). This may lead one to suggest that intuitives may prefer learning methods which are active, participatory and gregarious rather than analytical, reflective and selfreferential. Sadler-Smith and Riding (1999) in a study of learning preferences and cognitive style (using the cognitive styles analysis (Riding, 1991)), found that wholists expressed a stronger preference for collaborative methods (role play and discussion groups) than did analytics. They attributed this to the gregarious nature and social dependence of the wholists. Clearly, one challenge for research in this field is to build on a growing empirical base. The study The study aimed to investigate the relationship between learning preferences and the intuition-analysis dimension of cognitive style in the context of business and management education and provide empirical elaboration for the onion and cognitive control models. Sample and data collection The sample consisted of 226 undergraduates studying a range of business and management degree programmes at a university business school in the UK. The sample was an opportunity sample and participation in the research was voluntary. Data were collected by means of a questionnaire which consisted of three sections: (1) the cognitive style index (Allinson and Hayes, 1996); (2) a learning preferences inventory; (3) respondent data. Cognitive style. This was measured by means of the cognitive style index (CSI) (Allinson and Hayes, 1996). The CSI is a paper and pencil inventory consisting of 38 questions scored on a three point "true-uncertain-false" scale. The theoretical maximum score is 76; the higher the score the more analytical is the respondent's style. Learning preferences. Because of the limitations of existing measures a new questionnaire, the learning preferences inventory (LPI), was developed for the purposes of this study and is an extension of exploratory work reported in Sadler-Smith (1997) and Sadler-Smith and Riding (1999). The Reichmann Grasha (1974) instrument, the Rezler and Resmovic (1981) and Dunn et al. (1989) questionnaires appear to conflate notions of style and preference. The LPI consists of 13 items (see Table I); respondents are requested to indicate which teaching and learning methods they prefer in general according to a fivepoint Likert scale ranging from "definitely like" (scored five), through "neither like nor dislike" (scored three) to "definitely dislike" (scored one). The instrument's psychometric properties are discussed below. Respondent data Respondents' were requested to give their age, gender and programme of study and were assured of anonymity and confidentiality. Results Characteristics of the sample The sample consisted of 128 (56.64 percent) males and 98 (43.36 percent) females; the mean age was 21.00. Respondents were a second year cohort in a single higher education institution in the UK; it is acknowledged therefore, that the characteristics of the sample are likely to introduce severe bias. This is compounded from an international perspective since the subjects have in the main experienced the UK's primary and secondary educational systems, which are likely to exert a considerable influence over their learning preferences (see Figure 1). Item and factor analysis The CSI has previously demonstrated construct validity through confirmatory factor analysis and correlational studies (see Allinson and Hayes, 1996). Its level of internal consistency is high, ranging from 0.84 to 0.92 and Allinson and Hayes (1996) report test re-test reliabilities of 0.90. The LPI's factor structure was investigated by mean of a principal components analysis. Examination of the scree plot (Cattell, 1966) suggested that three factors (accounting for 42.2 percent of the variance) should be extracted. The three extracted factors were rotated to simple structure by means of a varimax rotation (the three factors were not intercorrelated). The resultant factor matrix with loadings of less than 0.4 suppressed is shown in Table I. Factor I consists of methods which are active (for example role play exercises, workshops and practical classes) and participatory (for example giving presentations and seminars). Factor I was labelled "active". Factor II consists of methods which are reflective and didactic (for example, lectures) and self-directed (for example computer based and self-study methods). Factor II was labelled "reflective". Two items had high loadings (> 0.5) on Factor III individual work loaded positively and group work loaded negatively. Factor III was labelled "individual". Descriptive statistics Cognitive styles. The level of internal reliability for the CSI was high (see Table m). CSI scores by gender are shown in Table II. Hayes et al. (1998) argue that gendered stereotypic thinking "suggests that intuition is a feminine characteristic whereas analysis is a masculine characteristic" and go on to test this view. In a comparison of style and gender, using a sample of under-graduate business and management students, they found highly significant gender differences (p < 0.001) in cognitive style, with females (43.84; SD, 14.02) being more analytical than males (M, M, 36.33; SD, 15.56). This was the converse of the stereotypical view of "female intuition". Although in the present study females did generally score higher than males the differences were only marginally significant and hence style and gender may be considered independent in this context (see Table II). There is some ambiguity in gender-related style differences. For example, Riding and Rayner (1998) argued that style is independent of gender. Complementary work using the CSI in a professional development context appears to suggest that while style and gender are independent they appear to interact in their effect on learning preferences. There is a need for further research into the relationship between style and gender and their combined effect on learning and workplace behaviours. Learning preferences. The mean scores for each of the three learning preference scales identified were computed and are shown along with their inter-correlations in Table III. The levels of internal consistency (coefficient x) were as follows: (1) active (0.50); (2) reflective (0.59); (3) individual orientation (0.81). While the latter was satisfactory, the x's for active and reflective were low but considered acceptable for use in this exploratory study. The three factors were not correlated among themselves. The general preference was in favour of reflective methods (M = 3.53; SD = 0.63), while individually-oriented methods were least preferred (M = 3.32; SD = 0.74), however, the observed differences were small. Cognitive style and learning preferences The lack of any important differences in the preferences expressed by the sample as a whole compounds the potential importance of any style-related differences, especially from the point of view of the planning and design of business and management education. The relationship between CSI score and learning preferences was explored by means of simple linear correlations. There were statistically significant correlations between CSI score and: (1) reflective methods (r = 0.32; p < 0.001); (2) individually oriented methods (r = 0.25; p < 0.001) see Table III. The effect of style was further investigated by means of a one way analysis of variance in order to test for any non-liner relationships. The sample was divided into three cognitive style groupings: intuitives (0 < CSI < 39); intermediates (39 CSI 48); analysts (48 < CSI < 76). Mean preferences for the three methods for each of the style groups are shown in Table IV. The intuition-analysis model of style leads one to anticipate stronger preferences for active methods on the part of the intuitives. However, there were no significant differences in this regard, therefore the assertion that intuitives will prefer active/participatory methods was not supported. The model also leads one to anticipate that for: (1) reflective methods the analysts would express the strongest preferences and the intuitives the least strongest; (2) individually-oriented methods the analysts would express the strongest preferences and the intuitives the least strongest These data support both of these assertions (see Table IV). A two way analysis of variance (style by gender) did not reveal any statistically significant main effects for gender or interactions of gender and style in their effect on learning preferences. Discussion The onion model and cognitive control models (Curry, 1983; Riding, 1991) infer a relationship between cognitive style and learning preferences, albeit with the latter influenced by the learning environment and context. The present study has lent some support to the notion of learning preference being a correlate of cognitive style. With respect to analysts, the assertion that they would prefer reflective and individually oriented methods received support. With respect to the intuitives the assertion that they would express a dis-preference for reflective and individuallyoriented methods also received support. Therefore, these data would suggest that there is a relationship between cognitive style and preferences for reflective and individually oriented methods. This may suggest that cognitive style manifests itself in learning situations as a preference for those methods which the learner unconsciously or consciously perceives as matching their preferred way of organising and processing information. Under such circumstances the learner may anticipate a benefit which may have a concomitant effect on motivation. The majority of empirical studies (Dunn, 1984; Hayes and Allinson, 1996) present evidence in favour of matching style and method. However, as noted earlier, some have argued that it is beneficial for the learner to consciously expose themselves to methods which do not match their preferred style in order to develop a wider range of learning skills ("learning-to-learn") (Entwistle, 1988; Honey and Mumford, 1992) and gain a "meta-cognitive advantage". The empirical evidence in favour of the mismatch of method and style is less robust than that which supports the concept of matching (Hayes and Allinson, 1996), although the latter is hardly unequivocal. It could be argued that mismatching learner and learning method is potentially valuable in the hands of a skilled facilitator with clearly formulated objectives and is perhaps one way in which learning-to-learn may be engendered. The anticipated preference for participatory/active methods on the part of the intuitives did not receive support. This may suggest that: there is no simple and direct relationship between style and preference with respect to participatory/active methods; there are idiosyncrasies in the participatory/ active methods used in the institution concerned which intervened to confound any relationship with style; the relevant scale of the LPI may be a crude and underdeveloped measure (it had the lowest level of internal reliability) of preference for participatory/active methods. The latter could be improved by the exclusion of those items which loaded ambiguously (i.e. "seminars") or had the lowest factor loadings (i.e. "giving presentations")- see Table I. The relationship between style and preference is worthy of further investigation, using undergraduate samples from a broader range of educational institutions, post-graduate and professional development students and, most importantly, randomly selected work-based samples. The extension of this work into international contexts (given the UK bias in the present study) in order to explore the cross cultural validity of the style and preferences constructs and their interrelationships would also be potentially valuable. Conclusion The aim of this study was to examine the validity of the onion and cognitive control models and it is argued that limited support has been provided. Two central issues may be identified: the status and validity of the "matching hypothesis"; and the notion of learning-to-learn. The two issues are related in that if individuals achieve the latter the former becomes a redundant concept. A key aspect of learning-how-to-learn is strategy development. Riding and Sadler-Smith (1997, pp. 204-5) argued that individuals may adopt a three-stage approach to strategy development based on the fit between their cognitive style and the demands of the learning situation. The first stage is sensing the extent to which the learner feels comfortable with the situation in terms of their own preferences. The second stage involves them, as they become more metacognitively aware, in selecting the most appropriate learning methods. The third stage is strategy development in which individuals attempt to make learning "easier" by translating, adapting or reducing the processing load imposed on them by the situation. This suggests that explicit acknowledgement of cognitive style and learning preferences (along with learning styles and approaches to studying), perhaps through comprehensive "profiling" of these attributes, may be an important step forward in bringing learners and management educators together in an understanding of each other's styles and their mutual interdependence. This is crucial since one of the keys to efficient and effective performance in both the classroom and the workplace is the ability to balance intuition and analysis, since neither is sufficient by itself. Table I. Factor Matrix for the LPI Item Factor I Factor II Group work Factor III -0.72 Role play exercises 0.59 -0.47 Lecturer presenting facts and theories 0.46 Lecturer presenting Examples 0.47 Self-study 0.76 Texts and journals 0.60 Computer-based methods 0.60 Analysis of cases 0.55 Workshops and practical cases 0.78 Problem solving Exercises 0.64 Giving presentations 0.42 Individual work 0.72 Seminars 0.59 0.44 Table II. Cognitive styles scores by gender (*p = 0.05, one tailed test) CSI N 128 Males M 43.27 SD 9.56 n 98 Females M 45.41 SD 9.69 df 224 t -1.67* Table III. Learning preferences means, standard deviations, intercorrelations, reliabilities and relationship with cognitive style CSI CSI 0.89 Active Reflective Individual Active 0.05 0.50 Reflective 0.32*** 0.10 0.59 Individual M SD 0.25*** -0.16* 0.12 0.81 44.25 3.43 3.53 3.32 9.66 0.60 0.62 0.74 Note: Coefficient alphas are shown in bold along the diagonal. 223 [< or equal to] n [< or equal to] 226 ; *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001 Table IV Cognitive style and learning method preferences Intuitives (n=71) M SD Active 3.29 0.64 Reflective 3.31 0.66 Intermediates Analysts (n=65) (n=89) M SD M SD 3.54 0.56 3.44 0.59 3.53 0.56 3.70 0.58 df 221 221 F 2.77 8.32** Individual 3.17 0.76 3.23 0.66 3.49 0.75 221 4.21* Note: *P < 0.05; **p < 0.01 References Allinson, C.W. and Hayes, J. (1996), "The cognitive style index: a measure of intuition-analysis for organisational research", Journal of Management Studies, Vol. 33 No. 1, pp. 119-35. Bunge, M.A. (1983), Exploring the World: Epistemology and Methodology (treatise on basic philosophy), D. Reidel, Dordrecht. Cattell, R.B. (1966), "The scree test for the number of factors", Multivariate Behavioural Research, Vol. 1, pp. 245-76. Curry, L. (1983), Learning Styles in Continuing Medical Education, Canadian Medical Association, Ottowa. Dunn, R. (1984), "Learning style: state of the science", Theory into Practice, Vol. 23 No. 1, pp. 10-19. Dunn, R., Dunn, K. and Price, G.E. (1989), Learning Styles Inventory, Price Systems, Lawrence, KS. Entwistle, NJ. (1988), Styles of Learning and Teaching, David Fulton, London. Entwistle, NJ. and Tait, H. (1994), The Revised Approaches to Studying Inventory, Centre for Research into Learning and Instruction, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh. Fox, R.D. (1984), "Learning styles and instructional preferences in continuing education for health professionals: a validity study of the LSI", Adult Education Quarterly, Vol. 35 No. 2, pp. 72-85. Grasha, A.F. and Reichmann, S.W. (1975), Student Learning Styles Questionnaire, University of Cincinnati Faculty Resource Centre, Cincinnati, OH. Hayes, J. and Allinson, C.W. (1996), "The implications of learning styles for training and development: a discussion of the matching hypothesis", British Journal of Management, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 63-73. Hayes, J., Allinson, C.W. and Taggart, W.M. (1998), "Intuition, women managers and gendered stereotypes" (under review). Honey, P. and Mumford, A. (1992), The Manual of Learning Styles, Peter Honey, Maidenhead. Hurst, D.K., Rush, J.C. and White, J.E. (1989), "Top management teams and organisational renewal", Strategic Management Journal, Vol. 10, pp. 87-105. Kirton, M.J. (1994), Adaptors and Innovators: Styles of Crcativity and Problem Solving. Routledge, London. Kolb, D.A. (1984), Experiental Learning, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliff, NJ. Leonard, D. and Strauss, S. (1997), "Putting your company's whole brain to work", Harvard Business Review, July-August, pp. 111-21. Matron, F. and Saljo, R. (1976), "On qualitative differences in learning. 1' outcomes and processes", British Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 46, pp. 4-11 Miller, A. (1991), "Personality types, learning styles and educational goals", Educational Psychology, Vol. 11 No. 34, pp. 217-37. Mintzberg, H. (1976), "Planning on the left side and managing on ihe right", Harvard Business Review, July-August, pp. 49-58. Mintzberg, H. (1994), "The fall and rise of strategic planning", Harvard Business Review, January-February, pp. 107-14. Messick, S. (1984), "The nature of cognitive styles: problems and promises in educational research", Educational Psychologist, Vol. 19, pp. 59-74. Nebes, R.D. and Sperry, R.W. (1971), "Cerebral dominance in perception", Neurophsychologica, Vol. 9 No. 247, p. 53. Reichmann, S.W. and Grasha, A.F. (1974), "A rational approach to developing and assessing the construct validity ()f a study learning styles scale inventory", Journal of Psychology, Vol. 87, pp. 213-23. Renzulli, J.S. and Smith, L.H. (1978), The Learning Styles Inventory: a Measure of Student Preference for lnstructional Techniques. Creative Learning Press, Mansfield Centre, CT. Rezler, A.G. And Rezmovic, V. (1981), "The learning preferences inventory", Journal of Allied Health, February, pp. 28-34. Riding, R.J. (1991), Cognitive Styles Analysis, Learning and Training Technology, Birmingham. Riding, R.J. (1997), "On the nature of cognitive style", Educational Psychology Vol. 17 Nos 1-2, pp. 29-50. Riding, RJ. and Rayner, S.G. (1998), Cognitive Styles and Learning Strategies, Fulton, London. Riding, R.J. and Sadler-Smith, E. (1997), "Cognitive style and learning strategies: some implications for training design", International Journal of Training and Development, Vol. 1 No. 3, pp. 199-208. Riding, R.J., Glass, A., Butler, S.R. and Pleydell-Pearce, C.W. (1997), "Cognitive style and individual differences in EEG alpha during information processing", Educational Psychology, Vol. 17 Nos 1-2, pp. 219-34. Sadler-Smith, E. (1996), "Learning styles: a holistic approach", Journal of European Industrial Training, Vol. 20 No. 7, pp. 29-36. Sadler-Smith, E. (1997), "Learning style: frameworks and instruments", Educational Psychology, VoI. 17 Nos 1 and 2, pp. 51-63. Sadler-Smith, E. and Badger, B. (1998), "Cognitive style, learning and innovation", Technology Analysis and Strategic Management, Vol. 10 N(). 2, pp. 247-65. Sadler-Smith, E. and Riding, R.J. (1999), "Cognitive style and instructional preferences", lnstructional Science, in press. Smith, L.H. and Renzulli, J.S. (1984), "Learning style preferences: a practical approach for classroom teachers", Theory into Practice, Vol. 23 N(). 1, pp. 44-50. Sternberg, R.J. and Grigorenko, E.L. (1997), "Are cognitive styles still in style?", American Psychologist, July, pp. 700-12. Witkin, H.A. (1962), Psychological Differentiation: Studies of Development, Wiley, New York, NY. ~~~~~~~~ By Eugene, University of Plymouth Business School, Plymouth, UK ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Journal of Managerial Psychology is the property of Emerald and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Journal of Managerial Psychology, 1999, Vol. 14 Issue 1/2, p26, 13p, 4 charts, 1 diagram. Item Number: 1675008 Result 46 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 50 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Can we generalize about the learning style characteristics... Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; GIFTED children -- Education; ACADEMIC achievement Source: Roeper Review, May/Jun98, Vol. 20 Issue 4, p276, 6p, 3 charts Author(s): Burns, Deborah E.; Johnson, Scott E.; et al CAN WE GENERALIZE ABOUT THE LEARNING STYLE CHARACTERISTICS OF HIGH ACADEMIC ACHIEVERS? In 1980 Dunn and Price used their Learning Style Inventory (Dunn, Dunn & Price, 1975) to investigate differences between the learning style preferences of high academic achieving students and the preferences expressed by same-age students with average or below average academic achievement The purpose of the study described in this article was to determine if and how the learning style preferences of a different group of high academic achieving students, inventoried at a later date, but with the same instrument, differed from those identified in the original study. A discriminant function analysis analyzed the learning styles data obtained from 500 students in grades 4 - 8. While significant differences (p <.001) in the preferences distinguished between the average and above average achieving students groups, there was minimal overlap with the preferences identified in original investigation. The authors conclude that the differences within an achievement group may be as great as between groups, and that it is improper to prescribe instructional methods or categorize groups of learners by presuming that they have similar style preferences. During the last 25 years, educators have collected and analyzed a great deal of information about students' learning styles. Researchers such as Barbe and Swassing (1979), Dunn, Dunn and Price (1975), Gregorc: and Ward (1977), Hill (1971), Hunt (1981), Kolb (1978), McCarthy (1980), Myers and Myers (1980), Renzulli and Smith (1978), and Schmeck (1977), developed instructional and theoretical models to explain differences in how students acquire and process information. Although the constructs that underlie these models vary (Ferrell, 1983), each researcher attempted to develop a system that allows teachers to identify, formally or informally, the special learning characteristics of these students and to modify instructional practices accordingly to improve the effectiveness of instruction and to increase academic achievement. Although a number of studies suggest that there may be merit in addressing students' learning styles as a technique for improving achievement and attitude toward school (e. g., Cafferty, 1980; Carbo & Hodges 1988; Domino, 1979; Doyle & Rutherford, 1984; Lynch, 1981; Shands & Brunner, 1989; Shea, 1983), some educators remain skeptical about the value of such information (Kavale & Forness, 1987; Pask, 1988). Weak research designs, the lack of randomly selected samples, and a "premature rush into print and marketing with very early and preliminary indications of factor loadings based on one data set" (Curry, 1990, p. 51) also influence the degree to which findings can be generalized to broad student populations. In addition, the questionable reliability and validity of some learning style instruments (Stahl, 1988), and unanswered questions about the malleability or durability of students' learning style preferences (Davidman, 1981) still plague the field. Despite these problems and a host of competing models and nomenclature within the field of learning style research, educators agree that attention to the individual learning style of a given student holds promise as a technique for improving school performance. This technique, however, may be an inappropriate and ineffective teaching tool if we assume that instruments designed to diagnose students' individual learning style preferences predict the learning styles of specific student groups such as Native American, reading-disabled, middle school, or high achieving students. Learning Styles Research in Gifted Education In the past, investigators conducted a number of studies to address possible learning style differences between groups of students. Researchers in the field of gifted education investigated the degree to which a consistent pattern of learning style preferences distinguish high achieving students from the general population. Most of these studies used one of three diagnostic instruments (Dunn, Dunn & Price, 1975; Barbe & Swassing, 1979; Renzulli & Smith, 1978) appropriate for students in grades K-12. Stewart (1981) administered Renzulli and Smith's Learning Style Inventory to a sample of general education and high achieving students in grades 4, 5, and 6, and found academically able students exhibited preferences for independent study, discussion, and lecture; factors significantly different from the preferences of the general education students. Wasson (1980) used the same instrument and found that the grade 4, 5, and 6 gifted achieving and underachieving students differed in their preferences for teaching games, independent study, peer teaching and programmed instruction. Although both of these studies used the same grade level subjects and instruments, the learning styles identified as significant in the studies differed in three out of four factors. Kreitner (1981) and Kirchoff (1980) administered the Swassing-Barbe Modality Index to assess modality preference (e.g. visual, auditory, tactile kinesthetic) with two different groups of high achieving subjects. Kirchoff concluded that modality strength is not a fixed characteristic. Instead, it usually changes over time, with high academic achievers demonstrating an integration of modalities at an earlier age. Surprisingly enough, Kreitner also found that musically talented adolescents did not demonstrate a preference to learn auditotally, though they did indicate strong perceptual preferences. In addition, his subjects preferred not to team through lecture. Again, neither researcher found similar learning styles when comparing two different groups of high achieving students. Researchers also used the Dunn, Dunn and Price Learning Style Inventory (LSI) (1975, 1979, 1985, 1987, 1989) to examine possible differences between the learning style preferences of academically able students and the general student population. Dunn and Price (1980) conducted a study that involved gifted education students. They compared the learning style preferences of high achieving grade 4 - 8 students with counterparts in the general population. They concluded that high achieving students perceived themselves to be more persistent and had stronger preferences for tactile and kinesthetic learning than students in the general population. In addition, they found that high achieving students preferred a formal classroom design and less structure. These students also saw themselves as less responsible than their peers. Griggs and Price also administered the 1975 version of the Dunn, Dunn and Price LSI Inventory to students who were identified for participation in a gifted education program and to students who were not identified. The LSI scores for the grades 7 - 9 academically able students indicated that these students perceived themselves to be less teacher-motivated. They preferred quiet and a less formal design, perceived themselves to be more persistent, preferred learning through visual, tactile or kinesthetic means rather than through auditory means, and preferred to learn alone rather than with adult or peers. Ewing and Yong's (1992) study used a 1987 version of the LSI with 125 gifted African-American, Mexican-American and Chinese-American students in grades 6 - 8. Preferences for noise, light, visual modality, studying in the afternoon and perceptions of their own persistence significantly differed for students in the three groups. The Need for Additional Studies Although some of the variables associated with the learning style characteristics of high achieving students seem consistent from study to study, no clearly defined pattern has emerged. To some degree, the differences in these studies' findings are related to the differences in the constructs measured by the three instruments; for example, the Barbe and Swassing Inventory analyzes modality preferences, while the Renzulli inventory assesses learner preferences for a variety of instructional strategies. Yet, even within the groups of studies that used the same instrument, conflicting sets of significant variables emerge from the findings. Some of these differences are explained by the fact that the grade levels of the students tested were not identical in all studies, nor were the social, cultural, experiential or economic backgrounds of the students. The results of other learning styles studies, outside the field of gifted education, suggest that preference differences between students may be related to gender, learning disabilities, reading achievement, culture, age, and a host of other factors. Viewed together, the studies in general education and the studies in gifted education bring to question whether differences in learning style preferences are just as great within the gifted education population as they are between gifted education students and students in the general population. Despite the issues raised when generalizing from these studies, some educators suggest that high achieving students do share similar learning style preferences (Barbe & Milone, 1982; Dunn, 1993; Griggs & Price, 1980; Ricca, 1984; Ross & Wright, 1987), and that these preferences may account for differences in their academic achievement. However, before educators can generalize from the work of these researchers to the population of academically able students at large, one must first verify that the variables identified in the above studies apply to additional, but similar groups of high achieving students. Subsequent studies are warranted in light of the many recommendations, presented in print and in oral presentations, that have been made over the years regarding the learning style preferences-of all gifted education students. However, in reviewing more than a dozen studies on the learning style preferences of gifted education students, no such replication or extension studies. For these reasons, we attempted to duplicate, as closely as possible, the sample, the instrument, and the data analysis techniques used in one of these studies; the 1980 research study by Dunn and Price. A Description of the Original Study The Dunn and Price study involved a total of 109 high achieving students and 160 randomly selected students from the general population of grades 4 - 8, in eight different schools in the Eastern portion of the United States. Researchers in the original study classified students as gifted if they possessed an academic aptitude score of 130 or above, or an academic aptitude score between 120 and 129 with scores in the 95th percentile or above on the math and/or reading subtests of standardized norm-referenced achievement tests. The researchers used the Otis-Lennon Academic Aptitude Test to assess academic aptitude, and the 1975 version of the Dunn, Dunn and Price Learning Style Inventory to assess learning style preferences. Step-wise discriminant analysis measured differences in learning style preferences for the gifted and non-gifted education students in their study, with the Learning Style Inventory variables serving as predictor variables. The statistical analysis determined which of the Learning Style Inventory variables discriminated significantly between the two groups of students, minimizing Wilks' lambda coefficient. The F for inclusion and deletion was set at 1.0, with a tolerance level of p < .001. Bartlett's chi square (52.97, 9 df, p < .001) was significant when Wilks' lambda of .817 was tested for one discriminant function. The discriminant analysis identified six variables that significantly differed between the two groups. Gifted students in the Dunn and Price study preferred to learn through their tactile and kinesthetic senses, and indicated less of a preference than the non-gifted students for using their auditory sense of learning. The non-gifted students preferred an informal design, required structure, saw themselves as more responsible, but not as persistent, and preferred to use their tactile and kinesthetic senses less and their auditory senses to a greater extent than the gifted students. On the basis of these six variables it was possible to predict membership in the gifted education group with 53 percent accuracy and predict with 81 percent accuracy membership in the non-gifted group. The authors concluded that gifted education students do possess identifiable learning style preferences that are different from the preferences of students in the general population. The significant variables that differed for the two groups included style preferences related to independence, persistence, perception and motivation. The authors also suggested that the LSI might be used as an alternative identification instrument. Extending the Original Study In reviewing the Dunn and Price findings, the question arose about whether these results would be similar with another sample of gifted education students. The inferred hypothesis was that if these findings were consistent from sample to sample, then recommendations for modifying identification or instructional strategies within gifted education programs might be justified. For this reason, an extension study was conducted in order to investigate the following questions: Do high academic achieving students differ from average and low achieving students with respect to their learning style preferences? If statistically significant discriminating variables do exist, are they the same as the variables identified in Dunn and Price's study? Limitations The sample for this extension study involved 500 students, 54 percent more students than the sample in Dunn and Price's original study. The ratio of high achieving students to students in the general population was 19.8 to 100, as compared to Dunn and Price's 40 to 100 ratio. The procedures used to identify students for the gifted and the non-gifted groups in both studies involved similar, but not identical, criteria. Although the students in both samples were enrolled in grades 4 - 8 in the public schools, they live in different regions of the country. Due to the constraints common to most research studies conducted in the public schools, a random selection of gifted students was not possible in either study. Subjects The sample involved in the extension study included 99 high achieving students and 401 students in the general population of grades 4 8. These students were enrolled in the elementary, junior high, and middle schools of nine public school districts in one Midwestern county of the United States. Communities within the county included metropolitan, agricultural, industrial, and suburban settings. Students categorized as gifted for the purposes of this study had scores at or above the 95th percentile on the Comprehensive Test of Basic Skills or the Iowa Test of Basic Skills or academic aptitude quotients of 125 or above on the California Test of Academic Aptitude. All parents and students received invitations to participate in the study and did so in order to acquire more information about the students' learning style preferences. Three local classroom teachers, two administrators and a gifted education coordinator received training in the administration of the 1975 version of Dunn, Dunn and Price's Learning Styles Inventory prior to using the instrument with students. Instrumentation Although a 1989 edition of the inventory is now available, the goal was to eliminate any problems that might arise from analyzing data from two different versions of the instrument. For this reason, researchers searched for and used student data from 198 1; data that used the same 1975 version of the LSI as Dunn and Price's 1980 study. This version of the LSI assessed grades 3-12 students' beliefs about 34 variables within 13 categories related to learning conditions: noise level - quiet or sound; light - low or bright; temperature - cool or warm; design - informal or formal; motivation - unmotivated or motivated; persistence - impersistent or persistent; responsibility - irresponsible or responsible; structure - more structure or less structure; Sociological - learning with peers, alone, in pairs, in a team, with an authority figure, or varied; perceptual - visual, auditory, tactile, kinesthetic or combined; intake - requires food or does not require food; time - morning, late morning, afternoon, or evening; and mobility - prefers mobility or no mobility. Students completed the inventory in a 30 - 40 minute session and responded to each of the 100 items in the inventory with a dichotomous, true or false, rating. Students rarely exhibit a strong preference for all variables. Instead, the inventory analysis usually identifies the 6 - 14 variables of greatest influence to the learning process. The inventory analysis identifies students' extreme preferences. Scores range from 20 80 with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Scores of 60 or higher indicate a strong preference for a given factor, while scores of 40 or below indicate a negative preference. Principal components analysis with the 1975 instrument identified 32 factors, not 34, each with an eigenvalue greater than 1.0, which collectively explained 62 percent of the variance. The authors report reliability coefficients on the internal consistency for the various scales that range from .40 to .84. Subsequent editions of the instrument used a Likert scale and fewer variables to increase reliability coefficients. Data Analysis Researchers used SPSS step-wise discriminant analysis to find the linear combination of variables that best discriminated between the high achieving and the general sample of students using the 34 variables on the 1975 Learning Style Inventory. The analysis identified which factors on the Learning Style Inventory significantly discriminated between the two groups of students minimizing Wilks' lambda coefficient. The F for inclusion and deletion was 1.0, and the tolerance level was p < .00 1. Prior probability for each group was set at .50. Bartlett's chi square (55.031, 14 df, p < .0001) was significant when Wilks' lambda of .894 was tested for one discriminant function for high achieving students versus students in the general population. Results In addressing the first research question, "Do high academic achieving students differ from average and low achieving students with respect to their learning style preferences?", it was found that of the 34 predictor variables assessed in this study, 14 variables discriminated significantly between the subjects in the high achieving group and students in the general population. Table I summarizes the order in which these variables entered the discriminant equation, the F to enter, and Wilks' lambda. The standardized discriminant function coefficients are indicated in Table 2. The classification procedure correctly classified 68 percent of the students. Based on the 14 variables that significantly discriminated between the two groups, 62 percent of the high ability students and 70 percent of the students in the general population were correctly classified. This yields a discriminant power above chance of 12 and 20 percent, respectively. Table 3 summarizes the classification procedures for these students. It must be noted however, that, as in the Dunn and Price study, the jackknife procedure for classification (Tabachnick and Fidell, 1983) was not used and that bias did enter this classification procedure. The square of the canonical correlation of .326 for the function equation demonstrated that 10.60 percent of the variance between the two groups can be accounted for by the significant variables in the discriminant equation. An analysis of the significant variables and the group means showed that the high achieving group preferred little structure and an informal design, accepted sound, low mobility and bright light in the learning environment, and perceived themselves to be more persistent than their classmates in the general population. The students in the general population preferred a quiet learning environment with low light. They perceived themselves as less responsible, more adult motivated, and preferred to learn alone through auditory means in the late morning or early afternoon. Structure and sound preferences accounted for the greatest differences between the two groups. Discussion Although both studies produced a statistically significant function equation that successfully discriminated between the learning style preferences of high achieving students and those in the general student sample, neither study identified the same set of variables. The only variables consistent for the high achieving students in both studies were the personality trait of persistence and a preference for little structure. Students in the general population of both groups showed preferences for learning through auditory means. More disturbing, however, is the fact that the two studies produced contradictory results in two crucial categories. Dunn and Price's high achieving students preferred a formal class room design, while the students in this study preferred an informal design. Dunn and Price's high achieving students saw themselves as less responsible than their peers in the general population, while this study suggested that the students in the general population, not the high achieving students, saw themselves as less responsible. Seven variables; light, mobility, auditory and tactile/kinesthetic learning, time preferences, adult motivation and the desire to learn alone, were identified in one study, but not the other. The discriminant function equation correctly classified the high achieving students in Dunn and Price's study at a level only 3 percent above chance. The high achieving students in this study were correctly classified, above chance, in only 12 percent of the cases. Even though students in the general population constituted the majority of the research population, and chance classification alone should have worked in their favor, only 21 percent in Dunn and Price's study and 20 percent in this study were correctly classified. In all likelihood this occurred because the magnitude of the difference between the two groups was insufficient to facilitate predictions much greater than chance. On the basis of these findings, the low percentage of shared variance, and the fact that the discriminant equation in this study yielded classifications only 18 percent above chance prediction, it is difficult to accept the idea that the population of academically able students share common learning style preferences. Certainly the current learning environment of the students tested, the quality of their gifted education program, their specific educational experiences, attitudes toward school, and the demographic makeup of their community accounts for some of the discrepancies noted above. These facts weaken the argument that most high achieving students share common learning style preferences. The discriminant function analysis yielded interesting information, but it should not be taken as clear and irrefutable evidence that consistent differences in learning style preferences do or do not exist between achievement groups. Plainly, we are on shaky ground if we continue to assume that certain learning style preferences are associated with achievement test score levels. In the wrong hands, this conclusion might be construed as evidence in favor of a new identification technique, reminiscent of the characteristics checklists that were popular in gifted education just a few years ago. The conflicting data between subgroups suggests at least two possibilities. Either the original instrument was flawed, or, individual differences between students accounts for more variance in style preferences than group differences. Although the 1975 LSI was subsequently revised to improve the factor structure and the reliability of the instrument, no replication or extension studies have been found for either the older or newer versions. At the very least, the findings from this extension study strongly suggest the need to conduct additional studies with the newer instruments. Until such time, researchers should not attempt to synthesize the results of learning styles studies that used more than one version of the LSI instrument. The evidence suggests that the learning style preference differences within an academic achievement group may be as great as the differences between the groups (Barbe & Milone, 1982). All style preferences may be equally appropriate (Fischer & Fischer, 1979), and care must be taken to refrain from placing value judgments on one preference over another. Educators must recognize the emerging nature of learning style preferences (Hunt, 198 1) and come to grips with the seemingly topical and temporal nature of such preferences. Students change. They grow and adapt, and hopefully, become increasingly adept at functioning with a variety of styles. The real issue involves educators' ability to modify the learning environment to deal appropriately with individual preferences. Some researchers believe teachers should consistently teach to a student's preferred learning style. Others believe that such modifications should occur primarily during initial instruction or times of learning difficulty (Barbe & Swassing, 1979). A third point of view advocates teaching all students in all style variables in an attempt to foster independence (Hunt, 198 1; McCarthy, 1980). Perhaps the truth lies somewhere between the extremes of this continuum. Learning style preferences may or may not account for part of what identifies a student as academically superior. However, the interaction of style preferences and the learning environment (Ricca, 1984) precludes a unilateral approach to instructional modification (Stewart, 1982). We recognize the fact that learners are different. We also believe that in general, it is helpful to recognize and accommodate these differences. Based on our findings however, we have concluded that it would not be prudent to prescribe instructional methods or categorize groups of learners by presuming that they have similar style preferences on the basis of singular research studies. After additional replication or extension studies, we may find that learning style inventories should be used as they were originally intended; as informative diagnostic instruments to measure the learning style preferences of an individual student. In other words, the instrument should be used to take a "snapshot" of an individual in a particular situation, at a specific point in time. It should not be used to take a group portrait. Within gifted education, this information can be used during curriculum compacting, content acceleration, or during self-directed student investigations or research; not as an identification device, nor as a blanket recommendation to view or to teach all students with similar achievement levels in the same manner. Table 1 Summary of the Learning Style Variables That Entered the Discriminant Equation Step F to Wilks' Significance number Variable entered enter lambda level 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Little structure Not responsible Late morning Needs quiet Sound acceptable Adult motivated Persistent Bright light Informal design Auditory preference Needs little mobility 11.48 4.87 4.51 8.31 4.28 2.57 2.66 4.50 2.27 2.59 2.28 .966 .951 .941 .934 .928 .923 .918 .914 .909 .905 .902 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 12 13 14 Low light Learn alone Afternoon 1.79 1.55 1.12 .899 .896 .894 .0001 .0001 .0001 Table 2 Standardized Discriminant Function Coefficients for the Learning Style Inventory Learning Style Inventory Factor Name Coefficient Quiet Little Structure Sound Acceptable Bright Light Late Morning Not Very Responsible Low Light Persistent Auditory Adult Motivated No Mobility Informal Design Alone Afternoon -.595 .477 -.437 .430 -.336 .324 .272 .233 .231 .228 .219 .213 -.182 -.149 Table 3 Percentage of Students Properly Classified by Group Using the Function Equation Legend for Table: A B C D E - n Hits Misses Percent of hits using DFA Percent of hits beyond chance Group High achieving students General population students A B C D E 99 61 38 61.6% 11.6% 401 279 122 69.6% 19.6% REFERENCES Barbe, W. B., & Swassing, R. H. (1979). The Swassing-Barbe Modality Index. Columbus, OH: Zaner-Bloser. Barbe, W. B., & Milone, M. N. (1982, January - February). Modality characteristics of gifted children. G/C/T, 5, 2-5. Cafferty, E. (1980). An analysis of student performance based upon the degree of match between the educational cognitive style of the teachers and the educational cognitive style of the students. An unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Nebraska. Carbo, M. and Hodges, H. (1988). Learning styles strategies can help students at risk. Teaching Exceptional Children, 20, 55-58. Curry, L. (1990). A critique of the research on teaming styles. Educational Leadership, 48, 50-55. Davidman, L. (1981). Learning style: The myth, the panacea, the wisdom. Phi Delta Kappan, 62, 641-645. Domino, G. (1970). Interactive effects of achievement orientation and teaching style on academic achievement. ACT Research Report, 39, 1-9. Doyle, W. & Rutherford, B. (1984). Classroom research on matching learning and teaching styles. Theory into Practice, 2, 20-25. Dunn, R. (1993). Learning styles of the multiculturally diverse. Emergency Librarian, 20 (4), 24-32. Dunn, R., Dunn, K., & Price, G. E. (1975). Learning Style Inventory. Lawrence, KS: Price Systems. Dunn, R., & Price, G. E. (1980). The learning style characteristics of gifted students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 24, 33-36. Ewing. N.J., & Yong, F.L. (1992). A comparative study of the learning style preferences among gifted African-American, Mexican-American and American Born Chinese middle grade students. Roeper Review, 14, 120-23. Ferrell, B.G. (1983). A factor analytic comparison of four learning style instruments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 33-39. Fischer, B. B., & Fischer, L. (1979). Styles in teaching and teaming. Educational Leadership, 36, 245-254. Gregorc, A. F., & Ward, H. B. (1977, February). A new definition for individual. NASSP Bulletin, 2. Griggs, S. A., & Price, G. E. (1980). A comparison between the learning styles of gifted versus average suburban junior high school students. Roeper Review, 3, 7-9. Hill, J. (1971). Personalized education programs utilizing cognitive style mapping. Bloomfield Hills, MI: Oakland Community College. Hunt, D. E. (1981). Learning style and the interdependence of practice and theory. Phi Delta Kappan, 62, 647. Kavale, K.A., & Furness, S.R. (1987). Substance over style: Assessing the efficacy of modality testing and teaching. Exceptional Children, 54, 228239. Kirchoff. S. (1980). Modality strengths of gifted students. An unpublished doctoral dissertation, Washington State University. Kolb, D. A. (1978). Learning style inventory technical manual. Boston: McBer & Co. Kreitner, K. R. (1981). Modality strengths and learning styles of musically talented high school students. An unpublished master's thesis, The Ohio State University. Lynch, P. K. (198 1). An analysis of the relationships among academic achievement, attendance and the individual learning style time preferences of eleventh and twelfth grade students identified as initial or chronic truants in a suburban New York school district. An unpublished doctoral dissertation, St. John's University. McCarthy, B. (1980). The 4MAT system: Teaching to learning styles with right/left mode techniques. Barrington, IL: Excel, Inc. Myers, I., & Myers, P. Gifts differing. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Pask, G. (1988). Learning strategies, teaching strategies and conceptual or learning style. In R. R. Schmeck, (Ed.), Learning strategies and learning styles, (pp. 83-100). New York: Plenum Press. Renzulli, J. S., & Smith, L. H. (1978). The learning styles inventory: A measure of student preference for instructional techniques. Mansfield Center, CT: Creative Learning Press. Ricca, J. (1984). Learning styles and preferred instructional strategies of gifted students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 28, 121-126. Ross, E.P., & Wright, J. (1987). Matching teaching strategies to the learning styles of gifted readers. Reading Horizons, 28, 49-56. Shands, R., & Brunner, C. (1989). Providing success through a powerful combination: Mastery learning and learning styles. Perceptions, 25, 6-10. Schmeck, R. R. (1977). Development of a self-report inventory for assessing individual differences in learning process. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 413-431. Shea, T. C. (1983). An investigation of the relationship among preferences for the learning style element of design, selected instructional environments and reading test achievement of ninth grade students to improve administrative determinations concerning effective educational facilities. An unpublished doctoral dissertation, St. John's University. Stahl, S.A. (1988). Evidence to support matching reading styles and initial reading methods? A reply to Carbo. Phi Delta Kappan, 4, 317-22. Stewart, E. D. (1981). Learning styles among gifted/talented students: Instructional technique preferences. Exceptional Children, 48, 134-138. Stewart, E. D. (1982). Myth: One program, indivisible for all. Gifted Child Quarterly, 26, 27-29. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1983). Using multivariate statistics. New York: Harper and Row. Wasson, F. R. (1980). A comparative analysis of learning styles and personality characteristics of achieving and underachieving gifted elementary students. An unpublished doctoral dissertation, Florida State University. ~~~~~~~~ By Deborah E. Burns, Scott E. Johnson, and Robert K. Gable Deborah E. Sums is an associate professor of Educational Psychology and director of the Three Summer Graduate Program in Gifted Education at the University of Connecticut. Scott E. Johnson is a principal of a science, technology and global studies elementary magnet school that serves East Harford and Glastonbury, Connecticut. Robert K. Gable is a professor of educational psychology and associate director in the Bureau of Educational Research at the University of Connecticut. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Roeper Review is the property of Roeper City and Country School and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Roeper Review, May/Jun98, Vol. 20 Issue 4, p276, 6p, 3 charts. Item Number: 745607 Result 50 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 51 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Parenting styles and adolescents' learning strategies in the urban community. Subject(s): PARENTING -- United States; LEARNING strategies -- United States; ADOLESCENT psychology -- United States Source: Journal of Multicultural Counseling & Development, Apr98, Vol. 26 Issue 2, p110, 10p Author(s): Boveja, Marsha E. PARENTING STYLES AND ADOLESCENTS' LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE URBAN COMMUNITY The purpose of this study is to examine the relationship, between perceived parenting styles and urban adolescents' learning and studying strategies. The results revealed that those adolescents who perceived their parents as being authoritative tended to engage in more effective learning and study strategies. Implications are discussed for counselors and teachers using this information as a fostering tool in their work with urban adolescents. The family is generally considered an important system that has a heavy impact on the development of children and adolescents. Some studies have identified child-rearing behaviors as variables that contribute to selfconcept development in children and adolescents (Mboya, 1995). A critical component of parenting style is the way in which parents attempt to control the adolescent. Much research has been conducted with White Americans on this issue. Even though a clear picture has not emerged, researchers have identified critical factors that seem to be significant to all adolescents (Becker, 1994). In studies addressing how parenting practices affect the development of children, findings have shown that parents who are accepting, yet controlling (authoritative), have children who measure high in school-related variables and mental well-being (Dubin, Darling, Steinberg, & Brown, 1993; Hein & Lewko, 1994; Shucksmith, Hendry, & Glendinning, 1995). These variables include the effective use of learning and studying strategies. Other research has shown that one of the key issues of school drop-out rates has to do with parents not expressing love (Reich, 1991). The same research found that adolescent drug abusers came from families in which there was a greater communication gap between parents and youth and either a highly authoritarian or highly permissive disciplinary style, all of which contribute to a student having faulty learning styles and the lack of opportunity to acquire effective studying strategies. In 1996, Laurence Steinberg conducted a study (Salmon, 1996) with 20,000 teenagers and hundreds of parents and educators nationwide and found that children raised by parents who were authoritative did better in school than adolescents from authoritarian or permissive homes. Therefore, counselors, teachers, and parents must consider the impact parenting styles may have on urban adolescents' ability to learn and study, if interventions are to be developed to increase students' academic abilities. However, there has been little research on the possible effects of parenting styles on the academic performance and achievement among racial and ethnic minority urban adolescents and how it relates to learning and studying strategies. In a study done by Reich (1991) with White American adolescents, it was hypothesized and concluded that perceptions of parental love and control may be a significant factor in the teen's general school achievement. There has also been little research on how counselors can enable urban adolescents to become more aware of their learning styles. Weinstein and Palmer (1990), who have done research with minority and majority adolescents, suggested that students can become more aware of their thinking and can comprehend and motivated, have good time management, however, are interventions that allow identify their thought processes that retain more information if they are and can concentrate. What is missing, adolescents to acknowledge and affect their learning. The school and the family provide a network of communication experiences through which the individual learns the arts of speech, interaction, listening, and negotiation, all of which are important in an adolescent's study habits. Urban adolescents, who perceive too much or too little support and control from their parents regarding the basic family functions, are likely to be at risk in their intellectual development, thus reducing their school achievement abilities (Olson, 1981). One recommendation for attending to this problem has been the development of a working alliance between parents and the school. This recommendation is based on the premise that parents play a critical role in the school behavior of their children. Research studies show that adolescents who have parents high in demandingness and responsiveness are more social and have high educational aspirations (Reich, 1991). Similar research results go back more than 20 years. In their research with White Americans, Balswick and Macrides (1975) discovered that a very restrictive (authoritarian) home leads to a cyclical pattern of frustration and aggression. On the other hand, a very permissive home can lead a youth to not know what the parental expectations are, which then leads to aggression in search of norms. If there are no checks on the aggression, an increased Amount of aggression is expressed. Thus, an "authoritative" type of parenting may be beneficial for increasing student achievement. The purpose of this study was to determine if parenting styles affect urban adolescents' learning and studying strategies and if counselors and teachers can set up interventions to work with (a) Parents, to have them incorporate more effective parenting styles, and (b) students, to improve or strengthen their learning and studying strategies. It was hypothesized that perceived authoritative parenting would result in urban adolescents' using more effective learning and studying strategies than would those adolescents with perceived authoritarian or permissive parenting. Finally, the literature review and the findings of the current study should contribute to counselors' knowledge in identifying urban adolescents' unproductive thoughts and behaviors, thus enabling adolescents to become more aware of themselves. Counselors and teachers can also use this information as a tool to help urban adolescents focus on major issues that are important in high school, such as listening and reading comprehension. This tool could also entail the development of a measurement to analyze urban adolescents' learning ability and to see what study strategies work best for them. Finally, these results can be shared with parents as parenting tips to help them work more effectively with their children. METHOD Participants The sample was drawn from a population of currently enrolled high school students (9th to 12th grades) in a large, eastern U.S. city. The total student population of this school was 800, 60% (n = 480) were female students and 40% (n = 320) were male students. The racial and ethnic background of the students was 60% (n = 480) Hispanic American, 20% (n = 160) African American, 15% (n = 120) Asian American, and 5% (n = 40) other. Of the students enrolled, 63% (n = 504) were in the 10th and 11th grades. As reported by the school administration, 90% (n = 432) of the Hispanic students spoke English as a second language (ESOL) and were required to take ESOL classes. The administration also reported that 95% of the students received government assistance for lunch. Finally, all the students attended four 90-minute class periods a day. I randomly selected 127 high school students. The sample was 56% female students and 44% male students. The average age of the students was 16 years with an age range of 14 to 19. All grade levels were represented within the sample: 20.5% (n = 26) were 9th graders, 49.5% (n = 63) were 10th graders, 15.0% (n = 19) were 11th graders, and 15.0% (n = 19) were 12th graders. Of the sample, approximately 60% (n = 76) were Hispanic American, 24% (n = 30), African American, 13% (n = 4) Asian American, and 3% (n = 4) reported being in the "other" category. Similar to the overall high school population, 100% of the sample reported receiving government assistance for lunch. Data was collected in 12 high school classrooms. Before doing this, I obtained informed consent from school administrators to conduct this study in the school to assist the school system in determining how best to attend to and improve the learning and studying strategies among students. Students served as voluntary participants, and the confidentiality of the students, parents, and school identification was assured. Procedure I used a causal-comparative design in this study. Research packets were distributed to all students in each class. Each packet included directions and a three-part booklet to be completed within 1 hour. Part I was a demographic profile sheet. Part 2 was a questionnaire that the researcher modeled after the Perceptions of Parents Actions Questionnaire (PPAQ; Schaefer, 1965; Streit, 1987). Part 3 was the Learning and Studying Strategies Inventory-High School Version (LASSI-HS; Weinstein & Palmer, 1990). All sections of the booklet were in English. Students who had difficulty in understanding questions were assisted by the classroom teacher for translation and given additional time to complete the booklet; furthermore, all teachers were bilingual in English and Spanish. Measures First, the demographic profile sheet was completed by all participants. This measure requested participants to indicate sex, age, race and ethnicity, and level in school. The second measure was a modified version of the Perceptions of Parent's Actions Questionnaire (PPAQ; Schaefer, 1965), a 104-item questionnaire that was designed for adolescents to assess which parent (mother or father) displayed a certain behavior (permissive, authoritative, or authoritarian) in specific situations. The 48-item modified version was designed for adolescents to assess if their parent(s) did or did not display a certain behavior (permissive, authoritative, or authoritarian) in specific situations. These revisions were made for two reasons: (a) I was not concerned about which parent displayed the behavior, but rather, if the behavior was displayed at all, and (b) there would not have been enough time for participants to complete all the questionnaire items, thus the first 16 items related to each parenting style were selected for the modified version. Three parenting style clusters are assessed in the PPAQ: permissive, authoritative, and authoritarian. Permissive parenting is associated with low levels of control, including being neglectful. Authoritative parenting combines reasoned control with support and concern. Authoritarian parenting involves rigidly enforced rules allied to low levels of acceptance. Each questionnaire was divided into the three, predetermined categories of perceived parenting styles. Categories for each student were determined based on the number of their responses of yes, no, or undecided. Schaefer's instrument has been widely used since 1965 and has proven to be a reliable and valid measuring device (Streit, 1981, 1987). The LASSI-HS (Weinstein & Palmer, 1990) measures how students learn and study by presenting statements that fall into 1 of 10 areas: attitude, motivation, time management, anxiety, concentration, information processing, selection of main ideas, study aids, self-tests, and test strategies. The authors provide evidence for the reliability and validity by indicating that when a test-retest reliability study was conducted on a preliminary version of the inventory, a correlation of .88 was obtained for the total instrument. This preliminary version had 130 items; the published version contains 76 items. No other test-retest reliability data are reported (Eldredge, 1990). Each inventory was divided into three, predetermined levels of learning and study strategies: low = 1, average = 2, and high = 3. Categories for each student were determined based on their responses from a 5-point Likert scale (strongly agree, agree, somewhat agree, disagree, and strongly disagree) and from six steps: (a) Each question was categorized in 1 of 10 strategies; (b) each question response was assigned a numerical value from 1 to 5; (c) all numerical values were calculated for each category (values ranged from 0 to 40); (d) a total of 10 numbers (1 for each category) was plotted in a table; (e) each table had three levels of strategies; and (f) depending on the number of scores within a strategy level, a numerical value of I (low), 2 (average), or 3 (high) was assigned. Construct validity has been established by comparing LASSI-HS scale scores with other tests measuring similar learner behaviors, and several of the scales have been validated against performance measures (Eldredge, 1990). RESULTS The analysis of nominal data in this causal-comparative study involved descriptive and inferential statistics. The descriptive statistics used were means and standard deviations. Of all the participants, 69% (n = 87) perceived their parents as being authoritative, 26% (n = 33) of the students perceived their parents as being permissive, and 5% (n = 7) reported a perception of authoritarian parenting. The findings also revealed that 49% (n = 62) of the students reported very limited use of effective learning and study strategies, 43% (n = 55) reported average use of strategies, and 8% (n = 10) reported extensive use of such strategies. The mean score for parenting styles was 2.20 (SD = .52; i.e., authoritarian = 1, authoritative = 2, and permissive = 3). The mean score for the strategies was 1.59 (SD = .63; i.e., low = 1, average = 2, and high = 3). The inferential statistic, chi-square, was used to compare perceived parenting styles (see Table 1) to examine the relationship between group frequencies (permissive, authoritative, authoritarian) in parenting and learning and study strategies. A significant (p < .01) association between perceived parenting styles and learning and study strategies (n = 127, df = 2) was found. Those participants who perceived their parents as being authoritative also engaged most often in effective learning and study strategies. Participants who perceived their parents as permissive were found to engage least often in such strategies. DISCUSSION The results of this study support the initial hypothesis that perceived authoritative parenting style would be significantly associated with urban adolescents' use of effective learning and study strategies. This association has also been documented in other research studies addressing the link between parenting styles and adolescents' academic achievement (Dubin et al., 1993; Hein & Lewko, 1994; Shucksmith et al., 1995). In addition, I believe that it is most important to note the underrepresentation of students who indicated the use of effective learning and study strategies. Data from this study indicated that approximately 50% of the adolescents in this sample reported below average use of effective learning and study strategies. This was found to be the case even though more parents were perceived as being authoritative. Such findings suggest that even though parents are perceived as being high in demandingness and responsiveness (authoritative), a critical representation of urban adolescents continues not to engage in effective learning and study strategies. Other Factors The reason for this below average use of effective learning and study strategies could be due to several factors. First, the relationship between school systems and parents might not result in effective working alliances. If parents are setting standards at home and encouraging their children to do well in school, teachers and counselors might need to be aware of and responsive to these standards. One way of doing this is for educators to accommodate students' learning styles in the classroom by becoming more flexible regarding instruction style (i.e., visual, auditory, hands on, etc.). This would represent educators as not only setting educational standards (like parents) but also encouraging the practice of recognizing student differences. Adolescents can work toward increasing their study habit only if they are made aware of their learning styles. This parent and school alliance, as mentioned before, is important if the community, itself, is going to be authoritative. Another possible reason for the significant number of low learning strategies among urban adolescents could be due to adolescents having learning disabilities. Adolescents with learning disabilities may have different abilities, strengths and weaknesses, and interests. Parents, authoritative or not, need to communicate their needs with school personnel and be decision makers when they participate in the Individual Education Plan (IEP) process. Counselors and teachers need to have positive attitudes and instructional priorities; they should also find out what skills an adolescent will need to function adequately and implement a program for preparing the child to develop these skills. The other problem is that many adolescents with learning disabilities are undiagnosed. Many educators and parents do not recognize the specific learning needs of urban adolescents and therefore cannot design strategies to meet them. Thus, effective academic programs need to mandate a higher awareness of adolescents' learning aptitude variations and to supply educators and parents with comprehensive knowledge of the structure of learning. With such information, low learning and studying strategies of urban adolescents can be changed, so that there is an increase of effective study habits. A third reason for the limited number of adolescents engaging in effective learning and studying strategies is the fact that many urban adolescents may have additional life responsibilities and personal and emotional challenges that compete with academic competence as a priority. For many students, school-related activities are secondary or tertiary to work and family responsibilities. Recommendations and Limitations On the basis of the findings and conclusions of this study, I have made several recommendations for counselors, teachers, and parents of adolescents. First, counselors, teachers, and parents should take advantage of the information they have or can get regarding students' home and school life and should use this information as a fostering tool in working with urban adolescents. An example is for educators to vary their teaching styles to accommodate different learning styles. Second, by having a wellrounded body of knowledge about the urban adolescent population, parents can incorporate or maintain more effective parenting styles. This knowledge would include parents being informed of their child's learning disability and would also include how they can be involved in the IEP process. Third, it is recommended that urban adolescents must first recognize their strengths and weaknesses to improve their learning and study strategies. Perceived parenting styles, alone, may not predict a students' academic success, but an alliance between schools and parents can help these adolescents determine what they need help with and what they can build on. In reference to additional research, I made several recommendations. First, more research needs to be done in the area of how parenting styles affect urban adolescents' learning and studying strategies. As evidenced here, the parenting style variable is not the only variable involved. Collaborative partnership between the school system and parents as well as an awareness of learning disabilities may also contribute. Second, there needs to be more research that looks at the urban adolescent population and how these students compare with other groups of students. Such data could then be compared for possible relationships or lack thereof. This could help determine the cultural influences on study habits of all adolescents. Third, any research with an ESOL population that extends from this study needs to take into account the data gathering instruments to be used. It is recommended that to collect representative responses, the researcher needs to use a "testing language" that is familiar to the sample population. This would include simple translatable questions and items or different versions of the instruments basted on the primary language of the participants. There were two major limitations to this study. First, the sample did not adequately represent all high school students because the sample was drawn from only one high school population. This method of sampling was used because I had difficulty getting permission from other high schools to enter classrooms. Second, the reliability and validity of the PPAQ has not been determined, and more studies need to be done to support the reliability and validity of the LASSIHS. Third, the nature of the data collection using self-report measures limits interpretations to what was perceived by participants. Adolescents may report perceptions that do not always accurately reflect actual parenting styles. Thus, future researchers should be cautioned to attend to these limitations in designing studies addressing this topic. TABLE 1 Two-Way Chi-Square Analysis of Parenting and Strategy Variables Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G - Variable Observed Expected Residual chi[sup 2][a] df Asymp. Sig. A B Parenting Authoritarian -7 C -42.3 D --35.3 E 78.677 -- F 2 -- G .001 -- Authoritative 87 42.3 44.7 Permissive 33 42.3 -9.3 Total 127 126.99 -Strategy 37.622 2 .001 Low 62 42.3 19.7 Average 55 42.3 12.7 High 10 42.3 -32.3 Total 127 126.99 -* 0 cells (0%) have expected frequencies less cell frequency is 42.3. ------------------------than 5. The minimum expected REFERENCES Balswick, J. O., & Macrides, C. (1975). Parental stimulus for adolescent rebellion, Adolescence, 10(38). 53-56. Becker, W. C. (1994). Consequences of different kinds of parental discipline. In M. L. Hoffman & L. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of child development (pp. 51-84). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Dubin, D. A., Darling, N., Steinberg, L., & Brown, B. B. (1993). Parenting style and peer group membership among European-American adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 3(1), 87-100. Eldredge, J. L. (1990). Learning and Study Strategies Inventory--High School Version (Lassi-HS). Journal of Reading, 34(2), 146-149. Hein, C., & Lewko, J. H. (1994). Gender differences in factors related to parenting style: A study of high performing science students, Journal of Adolescent Research, 9(2), 262-281. Mboya, M. M. (1995). A comparative analysis of the relationship between parenting styles and self-concepts of Black and White high school students. School Psychology International, 16, 19-27. Olson, D. H. (1981). Marital and family therapy: A decade review. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42, 973-994. Reich, C. A. (1991). Perceived parental closeness and control in relation to adolescent general expectancy for success in life and school achievement. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland. Salmon, J. L. (1996, November 24). Firm support for stricter upbringing. The Washington Post, pp. B1, B5.S Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children's reports of parental behavior: An inventory. Child Development, 36, 413-424. Shucksmith, J., Hendry, L. B., & Glendinning, A. (1995). Models of parenting: Implications for adolescent well-being within different types of family contexts. Journal of Adolescence, 18, 253-270. Streit, F. (1981). Differences among youthful criminal offenders based on their perceptions of parental behavior. Adolescence, 16 (62), 409-413. Streit, F. (1987). The Epac System manual for professionals (Vol. 1). New Jersey: People Science. Weinstein, C. S., & Palmer, D. R. (1990). Learning and Study Strategies Inventory-High School Version. Clearwater. FL: H & H Publishing. ~~~~~~~~ By Marsha E. Boveja Marsha E. Boveja is a doctoral student of counselor education in the Department of Educational Psychology at the University of South Carolina, Columbia. The study was conducted as a master's thesis in counseling at Bowie State University, Bowie, Maryland. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Marsha E. Boveja, Department of Educational Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC 29208 (e-mail: mboveja@aol.com). ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Journal of Multicultural Counseling & Development is the property of American Counseling Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Journal of Multicultural Counseling & Development, Apr98, Vol. 26 Issue 2, p110, 10p. Item Number: 531030 Result 51 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 52 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Urban adolescents' personality and learning styles: Required knowledge to develop effective... Subject(s): ADOLESCENT psychology -- United States; MULTICULTURAL education -- United States; PERSONALITY in adolescence -- United States; LEARNING strategies -- United States Source: Journal of Multicultural Counseling & Development, Apr98, Vol. 26 Issue 2, p120, 17p, 1 chart Author(s): Peeke, Patricia A.; Steward, Robbie J.; et al URBAN ADOLESCENTS' PERSONALITY AND LEARNING STYLES: REQUIRED KNOWLEDGE TO DEVELOP EFFECTIVE INTERVENTIONS IN SCHOOLS Researchers identified personality typologies (Myers-Briggs Type Indicator; Myers, 1962) among urban African American high school Juniors and seniors (n = 173). Introverted Sensing Thinking Judging (ISTJ) and the Introverted Sensing Thinking Perceptive (ISTP) were the two most represented typologies. Implications for school counselors, teachers, and teacher and counselor educators are discussed. Despite of the shared history of sociopolitical disenfranchisement, discrimination, and limited access to resources and opportunities available to others, the within-group diversity among African Americans in attitudes, interpersonal styles, personal preferences, and worldview (Carter, 1995; Steward, Gimenez, & Jackson, 1995; Steward, Jackson, & Bartell, 1993; Steward, Jackson, & Jackson, 1990) is well documented in the literature. It seems that despite of a common exposure to long-term persisting negative systemic influences, individual differences do occur. Because in addressing within-group diversity among African Americans researchers tend to use samples of convenience, what is known about these differences is somewhat limited to university student populations on predominantly White campuses. However, studies do exist that address the different experiences and outcomes among urban African American adolescents (Barbarin, 1993; Connell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Dryfoos, 1990; Ward, 1995). Attention to this population is particularly important for educators and counselors given that these children not only experience the general challenges related to being African American in this country, but also the day-to-day challenges related to the increased probability of exposure to poverty, hopelessness, crime and violence, and life failure (Barbarin, 1993; Dryfoos, 1990). Nevertheless, even in this population, as in the university population, evidence of within-group diversity has been noted. Despite of the surrounding circumstances and environmental influences, some urban adolescents report positive psychological adjustment, academic competence, optimism about the future, and well-defined vocational and career goals (Ward, 1995). The presence of within-group diversity among adolescents working and playing in reasonably similar environments has also been documented. Perceptions of African Americans as a monolithic group persists despite evidence indicating the contrary. Highlighting the fact of African Americans' shared experiences and identifying clearly defined, unique cultural norms have benefited many in this country. An emphasis on culturespecific knowledge has fueled efforts to heighten multicultural awareness and knowledge of professionals in education and counseling. Disciplines, such as multicultural counseling (Sue & Sue, 1990) and education (AACTE Commission on Multicultural Education, 1973; Banks, 1987; Gay, 1991), currently exist only in response to recommendations to develop culturespecific interventions that would more effectively meet the needs of a population whose experience has been discounted or ignored. To correct the outcome of past and ongoing injustices, practitioners and counselor and teacher educators have altered practices in service delivery and curricula development to accommodate identified cultural differences between African Americans and middle-class Caucasian Americans. However, due to the existence of within-group diversity, the shifts in intervention and training may not always be appropriate for all members of this group. Although some interventions may benefit some African Americans, due to the unknown representation of differences within any given setting, these same interventions may, in fact, impede the well-being and acquisition of knowledge by others. Attending to within-group diversity in the urban school system is particularly important for counselors, given the significantly higher representation of students' academic failure (Barbarin, 1993; Dryfoos, 1990). In this country, academic failure is closely associated with life failure and the inability to become economically self-sufficient. These are the outcomes that have become closely associated with urban America. Identifying the representation of the range and faces of diversity that exist within this population of students can assist in guiding educators and counselors toward the development and implementation of an appropriate number of effective interventions in counseling and teaching, toward a better understanding of the reasons for the failure of current practices and interventions, and toward a better understanding of within-group conflicts that occur due to these differences. More specifically, the identification of personality typologies among urban African Americans, as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI; Myers, 1962), would provide counselors and teachers with information about personality qualities, with information related to students' career and vocational interests, and with assistance in the identification of the most effective strategies or combination of strategies for counseling and teaching. Targeting upper-class high school students can assist university, college, vocational school, and work setting staff to best understand and accommodate the needs and strengths of individuals from the predominantly African American urban community. The purpose of this study is to examine the representation of the within-group diversity among an urban high school population. METHOD Research Setting Community High School is located in a predominantly African American community consisting of Just over 40,000 people on the outskirts of a large urban area in the Midwest. The community is generally of lower socioeconomic status, with a mean household income of $8,200 annually. Nearly 40% of the population lives at or below the poverty level (based on family size and income). The unemployment rate is 27%, and 63% of the households below the poverty level are headed by women and include children under the age of 18 years. The racial composition of the population surrounding the high school includes nearly 85% African Americans, 13% Caucasian Americans, and 2% individuals of other racial or ethnic identification. Community High School serves approximately 1,250 students. Of the student body, 97% is African American. The school's staff consists of 65 teachers and four administrators, most of whom are Caucasian American. The high school's mean grade point average (GPA) is 1.75. The school's daily absenteeism rate is 15%, and the overall attrition rate is 20%. Participants One hundred and seventy-three African American high school juniors and seniors participated in the study. Seventy-two (42%) were seniors and 101 (58%) were Juniors; 98 (57%) were female students and 74 (43%) were male students. Each of the possible 16 MBTI personality profiles was found. Of the participants, 97% identified attitudes reflected within the internalization status of racial identity. The mean age was 17 years. Procedure After reading a brief description of the study, researchers asked participants under the age of 18 to take consent forms home to obtain parents' signature. In addition, all students were asked to provide written consent by completing and signing an information sheet at the time of packet completion. Data collection occurred during normal school hours on 2 consecutive days. Research assistants and teachers instructed in the methods of administering the instruments provided each participant with a packet containing a consent form, a demographic form, the MBTI (Myers, 1962) and the Black Racial Identity Attitude Scale (RIAS-B; Helms, 1990; Helms & Parham, 1985). The RIAS-B was included in this study to best understand the qualities of the population studied. The demographic form included items that addressed participants' age, sex, and grade. During packet completion, participants were separated from those students who did not participate due to grade level, lack of parental consent, or unwillingness to participate. Researchers read aloud all directions and answered individual questions. In return for participation, students were provided with a personality profile and career assessment packet. The career profile and informational packet consisted of an explanation and interpretation of the MBTI and its application to identified career interests. As a result, questionnaire packets were not completed with total anonymity. Although names remained on the instruments, measures were secured in a locked file to ensure confidentiality. Measures The MBTI (Myers, 1962) is a 94-item forced-choice, self-report inventory that measures personality variables proposed by Carl Jung's (1971) theory of conscious psychological type. The MBTI classifies individuals along four theoretically independent dimensions. The measure consists of four bipolar scales: Extroversion-Introversion (E-I), Sensation-Intuition (S-N), Thinking-Feeling (T-F), and Judging-Perceiving (J-P). The dominant process in each pair is the one on which a person relies the most. The E-I scale measures interest in things and people or concepts and ideas; the S-N scale measures tendencies to perceive through direct sensory processes or indirectly influence through the unconscious; the T-F scale involves the style of information organization and the preferred mode of forming judgments; and the J-P scale reflects the dominant preference for dealing with the surrounding environment. Combinations of the four preferences exhibited by each respondent determine the 16 possible personality type combinations. Each type represents qualitatively different patterns of organization of the basic Jungian variables and defines a unique set of characteristics and tendencies in behaviors. Reliability estimates for the individual personality preferences obtained from the MBTI are .70 to .81 (E-I), .82 to .92 (S-N), .66 to .90 (T-F), and .76 to .84 (J-P). Stability in type scores was found to exhibit relatively stable reliabilities over an 8-week period including .69 to .83 (Carlyn, 1975); .86 to .89 (except J-P; Steele & Kelly, 1976). In addition, test- retest reliabilities of .78 to .87 were found in a duplicate study 2 years later. The stability of the MBTI personality type scores has been demonstrated with African American students at a historically Black university (Levy, Murphy, & Carlson, 1972). The test-retest reliability estimates for male students were found to range from .69 to .80, and for female students, from .78 to .83. Therefore, the MBTI has been declared a reliable instrument capable of assessing specific personality traits of African American students. The RIAS-B (Helms, 1990; Helms & Parham, 1985; Parham & Helms, 1981) was developed based on Cross's (1978) assumption that African American individuals, as they move from a position of degrading their racial identity to feeling secure with their racial identity, progress through four identifiable stages: Pre-encounter, Encounter, Immersion, and Internalization. The RIAS-B assesses African American persons' attitudes about themselves. The short form of the RIAS-B consists of 30 attitude statements with a corresponding 5-point Likerttype response format (strongly agree to strongly disagree). The RIASB is scored by averaging ratings for the appropriately keyed items assigned to each of four subscales. Averaged subscale scores range from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating greater endorsement of the attitudes represented by each subscale. The original version was derived from the responses of 54 college students attending a predominantly White midwestern university. Additional normative samples were drawn from both predominantly White and historically Black universities (Pyrant & Yanico, 1991). Internal consistency reliability estimates for the RIAS-B are reported for each stage of racial identity: Pre-encounter .69, Encounter .50, Immersion .67, and Internalization .79. Cronbach's alpha was used again to compute respective reliability coefficients: Pre-encounter, .76; Encounter, .51; Immersion, .69; and Internalization, .80 (Helms & Parham, 1985). RESULTS Table 1 presents the sample's MBTI profile results. The types ISTJ and ISTP were the two most represented typologies: 17.3% and 16.8% of the sample, respectively. The ESTJ personality type accounted for 14.5% of the sample. These findings are consistent with Kaufman, Kaufman, and McLean's (1993) results in which the ESTJ profile was endorsed by 14.1% of the African American male college students and 13.08% of the female college students. Of the participants, 56% indicated that their predominant personality and learning style preference included both a sensing (S) and thinking (T) component. The prevalence of this typology has been noted in previous research findings (Kaufman et al., 1993; Levy et al., 1972). The MBTI personality profiles least represented in the sample were ENFJ (n = 2), INTJ (n = 2), and INFJ (n = 3). A preference for the intuitive (N) characteristic (i.e., learning through intuition and imagination rather than facts) was endorsed by less than 19% of the students. These findings are also consistent with previous research results in which less than 19% of the African American participants endorsed items consistent with the intuitive preference. In summary, 57% indicated a preference for the style of an introversion (I) rather than extroversion (E); 66.6% of the sample indicated a preference for using thinking (T) and logic over feeling (F) and emotions in decision making; and, 52.1% of the sample indicated a stronger tendency toward an organized and predictable judging (J) versus a more spontaneous and carefree perceiving (P) orientation. Eighty-one percent of the sample endorsed the characteristics for sensing (S) over intuition (N). These results support previous findings that indicate African Americans' preference for concrete and logical rather than an intuitive and abstract integrative process (Kaufman et al., 1993; Levy et al., 1972). Overview of Descriptions of the Most Represented Personality Typologies The Sensing-Thinking personality type. The Sensing-Thinking (ST) individual, in general, tends to be present oriented. The individual relies on thinking to make decisions and is concerned more about logical consequences than personal feelings. For the ST individual, perceptions of the world tend to be based on things tangible to the senses rather than on abstract ideas, theories, or models. Members of this typology feel most comfortable in situations wherein personal ideas, plans, and decisions are based on solid facts verified by logic; facts are the only basis for action (Hammer & Macdaid, 1992). The individual with ST preferences tends to be self-sufficient and desires emotional control and treats feelings objectively. Control in self and others is highly respected, and individuals in this type are inclined to be somewhat impatient with ambiguity and uncomfortable with disorder, chaos, and the unfamiliar. For those with ST preferences, consistency is preferred to variation, and seeking and needing early closure to questions or problems are activities of priority. There is great respect for rules, and these individuals take comfort in having and following procedures (Hammer & Macdaid, 1992). The Introverted ST individual is serious, disciplined, reserved, and thorough and has a capacity for facts and details. Planning is a must and decision making is taken seriously by them. The provision of structure with well-defined rules and outcomes is critical. This individual enjoys quantifying information, measuring things, working with data, and listing facts. As an employee, this individual will work for long periods of time, to the extent that the process and outcome make sense. Practical judgment and memory for detail make this individual conservative and consistent (Hammer & Macdaid, 1992). The Extroverted ST is assertive, confident, and energetic. This individual likes to take charge of others and enjoys getting things organized and accomplished. Being action-oriented, the individual recognizes what is necessary and works with speed and economy of effort to complete a task. Work that results in immediate visible and tangible results is that which is most enjoyed. Solving problems through trial and error and application and adaptation of past experiences would be considered to be the most stimulating activities in play and work. The individual has a natural head for business and organization (Hammer & Macdaid, 1992). Overview of Descriptions of the Least Represented Personality Typologies The Intuitive type. The individual whose responses on the MBTI indicated a preference for Intuitive typology, as contrasted to the Sensing, tends to have the following characteristics: interested in ideas; focuses attention on the future and what can be; interested in possibilities beyond what is present, obvious or known; prefers to generate ideas rather than be responsible for putting them into action; is comfortable doing things in their own way; is patient with complicated situations; trusts inspirations, visions, and imagination; prefers elaboration, metaphoric expression, and poetry; works continuously when interested in what they are doing; wants to achieve important new solutions to long range problems; enjoys learning new skills more than using them; and works in bursts of energy, powered by enthusiasm with slack periods in between (Hammer & Maclaid, 1992). The Extroverted Intuitive Thinker (ENT) is highly verbal and believes that words are power and may use them as weapons. This individual takes much satisfaction in conversation and enjoys debating an issue and scoring points. There is a need for intellectual challenge and a tendency to dislike routine, which typically results in loss of interest. These individuals are most comfortable and effective in Jobs that allow tasks related to planning, conceptualizing, and organizing with someone else being responsible for the details. There is a preference for finding solutions to problems rather than carrying out the solutions. The Extroverted Intuitive-Thinker desires power and is competitive and needs to believe that a personal impact has been made on what had been accomplished. The individual is unwilling to accept failure and may be overly critical of self and others (Hammer & Macdaid, 1992). On the other hand, the student who functions as an Introverted IntuitiveThinker (INT) is the most individualistic and most independent of all the types. This individual enjoys dealing with abstract theories and ideas and may be relatively indifferent to the material world. There is a preference for being alone and adhering to a strong sense of principle. Furthermore, there may be a tendency to ignore the views and feelings of those who do not agree, and such individuals may seem to be detached. This individual would be good at scientific research, math, and other abstract or symbolic disciplines. Displaying the characteristics that are associated with this typology, these individuals are quiet, reserved, and skeptical (Hammer & Macdaid, 1992). The introverted intuitive-feeling (INF) person is energetic, enthusiastic, and imaginative. This individual is flexible and fluctuates in mood from one extreme to another. These individuals have a high tolerance for ambiguity and different belief systems. Concerns about the future and the problems of human welfare predominate in the individual's thinking. There is a tendency toward idealism, and often individuals object to things as they are and want to bring about significant changes. Such individuals also tend to believe in entitlements of rights for all individuals and value engagement in activities that attend to issues such as these. Overview of Descriptions of Most Represented Learning Style The Sensing-Thinking Learning Style is characterized by certain attributes: works with and remembers facts and details well; speaks and writes directly to the point; approaches tasks in an organized and sequential manner; adapts to existing procedures and guidelines; is concerned about utility and efficiency; is goal oriented; focuses on immediate, tangible outcomes; knows what needs to be done and follows through; and is concerned about accuracy (Silver & Hanson, 1982). These individuals tend to learn by directly experiencing through the five senses what is being learned; by putting what has been learned into immediate use or practice; by seeing tangible results from efforts; by practicing what has been learned; by following directions one step at a time; by learning in an organized, task-oriented environment; by studying about practical things that have immediate use; by responding to questions for which there are correct answers rather than open-ended questions requiring opinions; by participating in firsthand experiences rather than reading about them or being told about them; by being active rather than passive; by having their work checked immediately upon completion to determine if it has been done correctly; and by knowing exactly what is expected, how well the task is to be done, and why (Silver & Hanson, 1986). This type of learner learns best from the combination of techniques: drill, programmed instruction, demonstration, practice, mastery learning, convergent thinking tasks, and direct, actual experiences. Motivating activities include simple, repetitive learning games, concrete exploration and manipulation, programmed texts, workbooks, making real-life models, dramatizing important events, opportunities to demonstrate what is known, and assignments that have clearly defined conclusions. Such students tend to like doing things that have immediate practical use: being acknowledged for thoroughness and detail; praise for prompt and complete work; and immediate feedback such as rewards, privileges, and so forth. (Silver & Hanson, 1986). Overview of Descriptions of the Least Represented Learning Styles Two learning styles were indicated as being the least represented among members in this sample: the Intuitive-Feeling and Intuitive-Thinking styles. Intuitive-Feeling individuals tend to be good at interpreting facts and details to see the broader picture; to be able to express ideas in new and unusual ways; to approach tasks in a variety of ways or in an exploratory manner; to adapt to new situations and procedures quickly; to be concerned with beauty, symmetry and form; to be process oriented and interested in the future and solving problems of human welfare; to not be confined by convention; and to be concerned with creativity (Silver & Hanson, 1982). On the other hand, the Intuitive Thinker tends to take time to plan and contemplate consequences of actions; to organize and synthesize information; to weigh the evidence and risk Judgment based on logic; to learn vicariously through books and other symbolic forms; to be comfortable with activities requiring logical thinking; to be able to persuade people through logical analysis; to retain and recall large amounts of knowledge and information; and to be interested in Ideas, theories, or concepts (Silver & Hanson, 1982). Students with the Intuitive-Feeling temperament learn best from the use of certain instruction techniques: creative problem-solving activities; fantasizing; creative writing; creative and artistic activities; open-ended discussions of personal and social values; self-discovery; free association; metaphorical thinking; and activities that enlighten and enhance--myths, human achievement dramas, and so forth. Certain activities motivate these individuals: solving problems that require imagination and creativity; solving issues of personal and social importance; expressing themselves through one or more of the arts; expounding on how to improve things; talking about meanings, values, and relationships; engaging in activities that lead to a broader understanding of study material; solving open-ended and challenging problems or questions; and searching for beauty, symmetry, and aesthetics. Individuals Intuitive Feelers also have other preferences: opportunities for contemplation, being allowed to learn through discovery, opportunity to plan and pursue their own interests, recognition for personal insights and discoveries, and praise for unusual solutions to difficult problems (Silver & Hanson, 1986). Intuitive Thinkers learn best from the use of the following instruction techniques: Socratic method, problem-solving techniques, systematic planning, lectures, reading, logical discussions and debates, discovery through use of the scientific method, games of strategy, projects of personal interest, and inductive reasoning. Motivating activities include independent research projects; reading on a topic of personal interest; self-directed activities; puzzles, math problems, and logic problems; debating; open-ended questions; planning their own learning activities; and collecting and interpreting data. Personal preferences include time to plan and organize work, working independently or with other Intuitive-Thinking types, developing opportunities to present personal projects or reports, and working with ideas and things that challenge (Silver & Hanson, 1986). DISCUSSION First, the results of this study support the initial hypothesis of withingroup diversity in personality and learning styles among urban African American adolescents. Even though there tended to be an almost universal shared sense of racial identity within this well-segregated urban district, every possible personality or learning typology was represented. Such findings indicate that even among members who share racial status, developmental stage, and national and community history may simultaneously share some attitudes and maintain unique individual ways of being. Researchers and practitioners should no longer assume points of similarity or points of distinction among racial or ethnic group members. In conjunction with those of previous studies, these findings clearly highlight the importance of assessing and uniquely attending to individual differences in service delivery and teaching. Second, results of this study portray the complexity of providing services to urban adolescent populations and teaching them. Given the limited resources and personnel as well as student and parent populations that are struggling with problems of survival and living, professionals are faced with the challenge of having to effectively manage the heightened withingroup diversity using traditional strategies and models for interventions. Out of desperation, even teachers and counselors in more progressive urban school settings can find themselves shifting to more innovative strategies. In addition, such shifts can often occur first, without assessing student populations; second, without clearly defining criteria as indicators of intervention effectiveness; or third, without evaluating with whom the intervention is effective or ineffective. Attending to each of these is essential in effective program development whether in counseling or academic instruction. With even the possibility of the existence of a wide range of diversity, it becomes apparent that any well-planned theory-based intervention should be effective. However, it is also a given that the same well-planned, theory-based intervention will be ineffective with others in the same student population and in the same school setting and classroom. One simple example of a unidimensional shift is the avoidance of traditional, mainstream practices in individual therapy that tends to focus more on insight, reflection of content and feelings, internal locus of control and responsibility, and ideas and concepts (Sue & Sue, 1990). Avoiding these traditional techniques with most of these adolescents in this setting (i.e., Sensing-Thinking) would be quite appropriate; however, for the more Intuitive-Feeling and other adolescents, doing so may be quite inappropriate. Although the results suggest that psychoeducational interventions might be most appropriate for Sensing-Thinking adolescents who made up a critical representation of this sample, an assumption that this might be a standard form of service delivery would be quite erroneous. Providing multiple alternatives of service delivery and therapeutic interventions and developing a student population of informed consumers would certainly increase the probability of students' unique preferences being matched with the most effective intervention. Another example of ineffective unidimensional shifting is changing the more traditional classroom structural arrangement of individual and separated desks in a row to that of resource tables, with interest centers, and the absence of any permanent arrangement. However, these findings suggest that many students in this setting would benefit most from the more traditional classroom organization. Attending to differences within one classroom can become quite complicated for the classroom teacher. Teachers are confronted daily with the challenge not only of effectively managing conflicts that arise among students due to differences, but also of instructing all students in a manner in which they can most easily learn. Findings may support the notion of the development of criteria for students' contracts for grades. For example, teachers might provide students with options that represent each of the possible preferred learning activities for each letter grade. Classrooms might be structured in a manner that respects this within-group diversity and provides opportunities for and support of each individual's learning preference toward task completion. Having a built-in means of identifying students' success in accomplishing the goals for which they initially contract, this model also fosters strategy evaluation. Those students who are not successful in their first contract selection might select another option the next grading period. Although some schools may choose to group students by learning style to more easily and more systematically attend to differences in learning, we note one critical limitation in doing so. Given that learning styles may result from earlier learning opportunities, experiences, and exposure to various parenting styles, preferences for learning can remain stable to be expanded to include a number of preferences. Urban students' increased awareness of other options and exposure to others as they pursue their preferences allow opportunities for observations that might prove to be useful in addressing future life challenges inside and outside school. Counselors and teachers may work with students to effectively move them toward task completion when they are faced with activities that do not fit their primary learning preference. Third, extensive diversity in already high pressure settings can exacerbate otherwise innocuous events, tensions, and interpersonal conflicts. Relationships among students may be challenged due to the very stylistic differences that were found in this study. Relationships between teachers and students might also become problematic due to students' inability to accommodate teachers' instruction style and teachers' inability or unwillingness to accommodate students' preferences. Counselors might work with teachers in developing a better understanding of how to mediate interpersonal issues related to conflicts due to personality differences and teaching and learning conflicts. Counselors might provide in-service training for teachers' increased understanding of the assets and liabilities of each of the teaching styles. For example, although the Sensitive-Thinking teacher might be most effective for a large percentage of the students represented in this study, there are some concerns that must be monitored with this teacher. For example, this teacher may tend to overlook the needs of Individual learners in the push for content or skills mastery. Detail might be overemphasized to the point that students become bored or discouraged. Concerns with rules could lead to others' perception of the individual as rigid or unfeeling; an emphasis on order may result in such regimentation that students get "turned off." In addition, as the giver of directions, the individual may suppress any natural leadership tendencies among students. Consequently, even though most students may feel comfortable with this teacher's style, the learning of others might be impeded in the teacher's classroom. (Silver & Hanson, 1982) Fourth, findings can guide the investigation of career and vocational decision making among this student population. There are a number of resources that identify a plethora of professions and Jobs associated with each of the Jungian typologies (Hammer & Macdaid, 1992). However, the authors caution against such use of typologies in this setting. In urban communities, adolescents' exposure to a wide range of occupations may be limited and opportunities to engage in a wider range of learning experiences nonexistent. Consequently, typologies might serve only as an indicator of where counselors might begin in the process of increasing adolescents' self-awareness of values, interests, and identification of typology-associated professions. Additional activities that allow students to experience and become knowledgeable about options represented within other students' typologies would provide them with assistance in seeing a more comprehensive vocational map of the full range of possibilities in the world of work. The implementation of the above recommendations for intervention requires professionals who are willing and able to become both cognitively and behaviorally flexible in fulfilling their professional roles (Steward, 1993). It requires creative and responsive individuals whose primary goal is the psychological well-being and academic and life success of all students. Counselor and teacher education academic programs must admit only students who are open to understanding the importance of accommodating learning differences, and willing to accept encouragement, guidance, and challenge to effectively do so. Traditional practice of supporting counseling and teaching interns' adherence to and competence in only one theoretical orientation or teaching style might be expanded to requiring trainees to adopt an eclectic perspective and develop minimal competence in several mainstream orientations. School administrators must have the vision to develop new cultural norms and taboo behaviors associated with the roles of teacher and counselor by (a) providing in-service training to guide, assist, and evaluate staff as they move toward implementing effective building and districtwide interventions (Hopkins, 1990; Leithwood, 1990; Sparks & Loucks-Horsley, 1989); and (b) having the courage to face the challenge of relocating or removing staff members who are not willing to accommodate the needs of the population being served (Jones, 1997). Educational reform will require not only more than marginal individual changes but also the implementation of strategies that result from rethinking the current roles and practices of counselors and teachers in the school and implementing new ways of being (Elmore, 1997). LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY Although we believe that these findings provide a significant contribution to the literature addressing the urban adolescent population, there are some limitations. First, data collection occurred only at one site in one geographical location that might limit the generalizability of the results. Second, the sample included only upperclass high school students. Given the exceptionally high attrition rate that had been noted among 9th graders (45% to 55%), the sample might have represented the learning and personality styles of only those students who persisted up to the 11th and 12th grades. Those typologies that were least represented in this sample, might have had greater representation among those students who had changed schools or who had dropped out from this particular school. Future research is certainly warranted, to examine the effectiveness of recommendations we have suggested. TABLE 1 Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Profile Results (N = 173) Type N ISTJ 30 Percent 17.3 ISFJ 13 7.5 INFJ 3 1.7 INTJ 2 1.2 ISTP 29 16.8 ISFP 12 6.9 INFP 4 2.3 INTP 6 3.5 ESTP 13 7.5 ESFP 9 5.2 ENFP 6 3.5 ENTP 4 2.3 ESTJ 25 14.5 ESFJ 9 5.2 ENFJ 2 1.2 ENTJ 6 3.5 Note. N = Introversion; E = Extroversion; S = Sensation; I = Intuition; T = Thinking; F = Feeling; J = Judging; P = Perceiving. REFERENCES AACTE Commission on Multicultural Education. (1973). No one model American. Journal of Teacher Education, 24, 264-265. Banks, J. A. (1987). Multiethnic education. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Barbarin, O. A. (1993). Coping and resilience: Exploring the inner lives of African American children. Journal of Black Psychology, 19, 478-492. Carlyn, M. (1975). An assessment of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 461-473. Carter, R. T. (1995). The influence of race and racial identity in psychotherapy. New York: Wiley. Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994). Educational risk and resilience in African-American youth: Context, self, action, and outcomes in school. Child Development, 65, 493-506. Cross, W. E. (1978). The Cross and Thomas models of psychological nigrescence. Journal of Black Psychology, 5, 13-19. Dryfoos, J. G. (1990). Adolescents at risk: Prevalence and prevention. New York: Oxford University Press. Elmore, R. F. (1997). Why restructuring alone won't improve teaching. Educational Leadership, April Issues, 44-48. Gay, G. (1991, February). Toward education that is multicultural Keynote address at the National Association for Multicultural Education. New Orleans, LA. Hammer, A. L., & Macdaid, G. P. (1992). MBTI: Career report manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Helms, J. E. (1990). Black and White racial identity: Theory, research and practice. New York: Greenwood Press. Helms, J. E., & Parham, T. A. (1985). Attitudes of racial identity and self-esteem of Black students: An exploratory investigation. Journal of College Student Personnel, 3, 143-147. Hopkins, D. (1990). integrating staff development and school improvement: A study of teacher personality and school climate. In B. Joyce (Ed.), Changing school culture through staff development (pp. 41-67). Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Jones, R. (1997). Showing bad teachers the door. The American School Board Journal, November Issues, 20-24. Jung, C. G. (1971). Psychological types. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Kaufman, A. S., Kaufman, N. L., & McLean, J. E. (1993). Profiles of Hispanic adolescents and adults on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 628-630. Leithwood, K. A. (1990). The principal's role in teacher development. In B. Joyce (Ed.), Changing school culture through staff development (pp. 71-90). Alexandria. VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Levy, N., Murphy, C., & Carlson, R. (1972). Personality types among Negro college students. Educational and Psychological Measurements, 31, 641-653. Myers. I. B. (1962). The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator manual. Princeton. NJ: Educational Testing Service. Parham, T. A., & Helms, J. E. (1981). The influence of Black students' racial identity attitudes on preference of counselor's race. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28, 250-257. Pyrant, C. T., & Yanico, B. J. (1991). Relationship of racial identity and gender-role attitudes to Black women's psychological well-being. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 315-322. Silver, H. F., & Hanson, J. R. (1982). Learning styles and strategies: Who am I as a learner; what are my assets and liabilities; how can I work more effectively with students, teachers, parents, and administrators. Moorestown, NJ: Hanson Silver & Associates. Silver, H. F., & Hanson, J. R. (1986). Teaching styles and strategies: Techniques for meeting the diverse needs and styles of learners. Moorestown, NJ: Hanson Silver & Associates. Sparks, D., & Loucks-Horsley, S. (1989). Five models of staff development for teachers. The Journal of Staff Development, 10(4), 40-57. Steele, R. S., & Kelly, T. J. (1976). Eysenck Personality Questionnaire and Jungian Myers-Briggs Type Indicator correlation of ExtroversionIntroversion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 690-691. Steward, R. J. (1993). Cross-cultural training for professional staff. Journal of College Student Development, 34, 441. Steward, R. J., Gimenez, M. M., & Jackson, J. D. (1995). A study of personal preferences of successful university students as related to race/ethnicity and sex: Implications and recommendations for training, practice, and future research. Journal of College Student Development, 36, 123-131. Steward, R. J., Jackson, J. D., & Bartell, P. A. (1993). Black students' alienation: A study of world view. Journal of College Student Development, 34, 228-229. Steward, R. J., Jackson, M. R, & Jackson, J. D. (1990). Alienation and interactional styles in a predominantly White environment: A study of successful Black students. Journal of College Student Development, 31, 509515. Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (1990). Counseling the culturally different (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Ward, A. J. (1995). Life stress and the development of violence in adolescent males. Presentation at the 103rd American Psychological Association Annual Convention at New York City. ~~~~~~~~ By Patricia A. Peeke, Robbie J. Steward and Judy A. Ruddock Patricia A. Peeke is a counseling psychologist living in Bethlehem, Pennsylvania. Robbie J. Steward is an associate professor in the Department of Counseling and Education at Michigan State University, East Lansing. Judy A. Ruddock is a high school teacher at Northwestern High School in Flint, Michigan. Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Robbie J. Steward, Michigan State University, 436 Erickson Hall, Fast Lansing, MI 48824 (e-mail: devine@msu.edu). ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Journal of Multicultural Counseling & Development is the property of American Counseling Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Journal of Multicultural Counseling & Development, Apr98, Vol. 26 Issue 2, p120, 17p, 1 chart. Item Number: 531031 Result 52 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 53 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: An interview with Rita Dunn about learning styles. Subject(s): DUNN, Rita -- Interviews; LEARNING strategies Source: Clearing House, Jan/Feb98, Vol. 71 Issue 3, p141, 5p Author(s): Shaughnessy, Michael F. AN INTERVIEW WITH RITA DUNN ABOUT LEARNING STYLES What are the main components of a person's learning style? A person's learning style is the way that he or she concentrates on, processes, internalizes, and remembers new and difficult academic information or skills. Styles often vary with age, achievement level, culture, global versus analytic processing preference, and gender. Dunn and Dunn (1992, 1993) describe learning style in terms of individual reactions to twenty-three elements in five basic strands that include each person's environmental, emotional, sociological, physiological, and psychological processing preferences. Do we learn differently or do we process information differently? Human beings process information differently from each other, but information processing is only one of twenty-three elements in the Dunn and Dunn Learning Style Model. How do we know that students achieve more when their teachers teach to the students' learning styles? A meta-analysis of forty-two experimental studies conducted with the Dunn and Dunn model between 1980 and 1990 by thirteen different institutions of higher education revealed that students whose characteristics were accommodated by educational interventions responsive to their learning styles could be expected to achieve 75 percent of a standard deviation higher than students whose styles were not accommodated (Dunn et al. 1995). In addition, practitioners throughout the United States have reported statistically higher test scores and/or grade point averages for students whose teachers changed from traditional teaching to learning-style teaching at all levels--elementary, secondary, and college. Improved achievement was often apparent after only six weeks of learning-style instruction. After one year, teachers reported significantly higher standardized achievement and aptitude test scores for students who had not scored well previously. For example, prior to using learning styles, only 25 percent of the Frontier, New York, school district's special education high school students passed the required local examinations and state competency tests to receive diplomas. In the district's first year of its learning styles program (1987-88) that number increased to 66 percent. During the second year (1988-89) 91 percent of the district's special education population were successful; in the third year (1989-1990) the results remained constant at 90 percent--with a greater ratio of "handicapped" students passing state competency exams than regular education students (Brunner and Majewski 1990). Two North Carolina elementary principals published similarly startling gains with the same learning-styles program. One principal brought a K-6 school, whose students were from poor, minority-group families, that had scored in the 30th percentile on the California Achievement Tests up to the 83rd percentile in a three-year period by responding to students' learning styles (Andrews 1990). The other principal taught highly tactual learning disabled (LD) elementary school students with hands-on resources and allowed them to sit informally in subdued lighting. Based on their learning-style analyses, the children studied alone, with a classmate or two, or with their teacher. Within four months, those LD youngsters showed four months' gain on a standardized achievement test--better than they had previously done and as well as normally achieving children (Stone 1992). Finally, a U.S. Department of Education four-year investigation that included on-site visits, interviews, observations, and examinations of national test data concluded that attending to learning styles was one of the few strategies that had had a positive impact on the achievement of special education students throughout the nation (Alberg et al. 1992). The gains described here were made by using the Dunn and Dunn model, which has been researched at St. John's University and more than 110 other colleges and universities since 1972. Why should we test for children's learning styles? Teachers cannot identify students' learning styles accurately without an instrument (Beaty 1986). Some characteristics are not observable, even to the experienced educator. In addition, teachers often misinterpret students' behaviors and misunderstand their symptoms. For example, it is difficult to determine whether a youngster's hyperactivity is due to a need for mobility, informal seating, kinesthetic resources, or "breaks," or to nonconformity or a lack of discipline. Only a reliable and valid instrument can provide reliable and valid information, and only a comprehensive instrument can diagnose the many learning-style traits that influence individuals. Teachers who use instruments to identify only one or two variables on a bipolar continuum restrict their ability to prescribe for the many elements other than the one or two they identified. Learning style is a multidimensional construct; many variables have an impact on each other and produce unique patterns. Those patterns suggest exactly how each person is likely to concentrate, process, internalize, and retain new and difficult information. The patterns indicate which reading or math method is most likely to be effective with each student. Only three comprehensive models exist, and each has a related instrument designed to reveal individuals' styles based on the traits examined by that model. During the past two decades, the most frequently used instrument in experimental research on learning styles, and the one with the highest reliability and validity, is the Dunn, Dunn, and Price Learning Style Inventory (LSI), with its subtests for students in grades 3-12 and the Productivity Environmental Preference Survey for college students and adults. Tell us about your test for identifying learning styles. The Learning Style Inventory (grades 3-12) was developed through content and factor analysis and is one of the three comprehensive approaches to identifying students' learning styles. Different grade-level forms permit analysis of the specific conditions under which students prefer to learn. This easy-toadminister and interpret inventory uses more than one hundred dichotomous items (e.g., "When I really have a lot of studying to do, I like to work alone" and "I enjoy being with friends when I study") that are rated on a five-point Likert scale and can be completed in approximately thirty to forty minutes. In an analysis of the conceptualizations of learning style and the psychometric standards of nine different instruments that measure learningstyle preference, the LSI was rated as having good or better reliability and validity (Curry 1987). A series of age-appropriate storybooks is available from the Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Styles at St. John's University for primary, elementary, middle school, and secondary students and adults to clarify the concept of style and to demonstrate that there is no bad or better style. Most people can learn anything when they know how to capitalize on their learning-style strengths. Describe what the LSI reveals. The LSI assesses individual preferences in the following areas: (a) immediate environment (sound, light, temperature, and seating design); (b) emotionality (motivation, persistence, responsibility/ conformity, and need for internal or external structure); (c) sociological (learning alone, in a pair, as part of a small group or team, with peers, or with an authoritative or collegial adult; also, in a variety of ways or in a consistent pattern); (d) physiological (auditory, visual, tactual, and/or kinesthetic perceptual preferences; food or liquid intake needs; time-of-day energy levels; mobility needs); and (e) indications of global or analytic processing inclinations (through correlation with sound, light, design, persistence, peer-orientation, and intake scores). How does the LSI affect learning? The LSI does the following: Permits students to identify how they prefer to learn and also indicates the degree to which their responses are consistent Suggests a basis for redesigning the classroom environment to complement students' diverse styles Describes the arrangements in which each student is likely to learn most effectively (e.g. alone, in a pair, with two or more classmates, with a teacher, or, depending on the task, with students with similar interests or talents; it also describes whether all or none of those combinations is acceptable for a particular student) Explains which students should be given options and alternatives and which students need direction and high structure Sequences the perceptual strengths through which individuals should begin studying--and then reinforce--new and difficult information; it explains how each student should study and do homework (Homework Disc 1995) Indicates the methods through which individuals are most likely to achieve (e.g., contracts, programmed learning, multisensory resources, tactual manipulatives, kinesthetic games, or any combination of these) Provides information concerning which children are conforming and which are nonconforming and explains how to work with both types Pinpoints the best time of day for each student to be scheduled for difficult subjects (thus, it shows how to group students for instruction based on their learning-style energy-highs) Identifies those students for whom movement or snacks, while the students are learning, may accelerate learning Suggests those students for whom analytic versus global approaches are likely to be important How can schools order the LSI? Discuss purchasing and cost possibilities with Price Systems in Lawrence, Kansas. When ordering the LSI, stipulate the grade level and total number of students you plan to test; the cost decreases when more students are tested. The LSI is available on IBM and Apple self-scoring discs; if you plan to test three hundred persons or more, the disc may be considerably less expensive. How does learning style influence homework? St. John's University's Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Styles developed IBM and Apple software packages that translate LSI scores into prescriptions for how students should study and do their homework (Homework Disc 1995). Is it possible to identify the styles of children in grades K-2? For young children in K-2, use the Learning Style Inventory: Primary Version (LSI:P) (Perrin 1982), which is obtainable from St. John's University's Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Styles. The LSI:P is a pictorial assessment of young children's learning styles and is accompanied by a research manual that explains how to administer it. Although there are decided advantages to having teachers administer the test on an individual basis--because of all the information each child reveals--the assessment's questions are written so that an intelligent parent can elicit the same information and assist the teacher in compiling the hand-scorable data. How do teachers adapt for each child's style? Teachers do not need to adapt to each child's style. Rather, they need to do the following: Understand the concept, its related practices, and its implementation strategies Explain learning styles to their students so that the youngsters understand that there is no such thing as either a "good" or a "bad" style. Prepare students for taking the LSI (Price Systems interprets the students' print-outs, and the Homework Disc provides their prescriptions) Have alternative instructional methods and resources to teach the identical information differently to students with diverse learning styles St. John's University has many such resources at varied grade levels and subjects. They can be adapted or paralleled for a particular classroom. In addition, many of our books provide directions for developing resources (Dunn and Dunn 1992, 1993; Dunn, Dunn, and Perrin 1994). We also teach students to create their own instructional resources. How do learning-style teachers differ from conventional teachers? Unlike traditional teachers who teach an entire class in the same way with the same methods (or the "brain-based" practices where every student is taught nontraditionally), learning-style teachers actually teach different children differently. Teachers do two important things: Using the resources and methods that best match each child, they teach students (1) to recognize and rely on their personal learning-style strengths and (2) to teach themselves and each other by using those strengths. What is a learning-style school like, and how does it differ from conventional schools? Although students in the same class may be mastering the same information and skills at the same time, in learning-style schools they work in those sections of the classroom that best respond to their environmental and physiological styles. A variety of tactual and kinesthetic resources are available for mastering the curriculum, but children work only with those resources that best complement their own processing, perceptual, emotional, and sociological styles--and students often will have made the materials they use! It would be rare to see whole classes engaged in either teacher-directed instruction or cooperative learning when the students are being introduced to new and difficult material. Instead, children begin learning alone, with a classmate or two, in a small, cooperative or competitive group, or with their teacher through their primary perceptual strengths for the first ten to fifteen minutes. They then reinforce the new information with a different resource through their secondary strengths. Students may vary their choice of resources but are encouraged to begin learning through their strengths whenever the academic material is complex or difficult for them. In learning-style classes, students' strengths are identified and then transferred to a computer software package, the Homework Disc (1995). That package generates a personalized, printed prescription for each child that describes how to study and concentrate through his or her strengths. Gradually, each child learns how to teach him- or herself or how to work with a classmate who learns similarly. Children study, learn, complete inclass assignments, and do their homework through their strengths--instead of as the teacher happens to teach. What happens when teachers teach in a different style from the way in which students learn? When students are unable to learn with complementary resources--such as textbooks, films, or videotapes for visual preferents; manipulatives for tactual preferents; tapes or lectures for auditory preferents; or large floor games for kinesthetic preferents--they do not achieve what they are capable of achieving. Research reveals that the closer the match between students' learning styles and their teachers' teaching styles, the higher the grade point average (Dunn et al. 1995). How do gifted children learn? Although all gifted students do not have the same style, their styles differ significantly from those of underachievers. When comparing the learning styles and multiple intelligences of gifted and talented adolescents in nine different cultures, we found that, regardless of culture, adolescents gifted in a particular domain--athletics, dance, leadership, literature, mathematics, and music--had essentially similar learning styles. Surprisingly, the gifted in each intelligence domain had essentially similar styles--but those were different from the styles of other gifted groups and from the styles of the nongifted (Milgram, Dunn, and Price 1993). Are there perceptual differences between the gifted and nongifted students? Although gifted students prefer kinesthetic (experiential/active) and tactual (hands-on) instruction, many also are able to learn auditorially and/or visually--although not as enjoyably. On the other hand, lowachieving students who prefer kinesthetic and/or tactual learning can only master difficult information through those modalities. In addition, low achievers often have only one perceptual strength, or none, in contrast to the multiperceptual strengths of the gifted. Are there sociological differences between gifted and nongifted students? Gifted adolescents in nine cultures preferred learning either by themselves or with an authoritative teacher. If those students are representative of gifted students across nations, cooperative learning and small-group instructional strategies should not be imposed on them; few wish to learn with classmates. In addition, when permitted to learn alone, with peers, or with a teacher based on their identified learning-style preferences, even gifted first and second graders revealed significantly higher achievement and aptitude test scores through their preferred styles--and few preferred learning either via whole-class instruction or with their nongifted classmates. Are there chronobiological differences between gifted and nongifted students? Although some gifted adolescents learned well in the morning, many more preferred late morning, afternoon, and/or evening as their best times for concentration. At no educational level (K-12) did we find a majority of early-morning students, and this is particularly true for poor achievers. Conventional schooling appears to be unresponsive to the majority of both gifted adolescents and low achievers, whose best time of day rarely is early morning. Are there differences between the processing styles of gifted and nongifted students? Of the gifted and talented students we tested for processing style, 19 percent were analytic, 26 percent were global, and 56 percent were integrated processors who functioned in either style--but only when interested in the content. Both global and analytic students can be gifted, but textbooks and teachers' styles tend to be analytic rather than global. Do the learning styles of able and at-risk students differ? Seven learningstyle traits significantly discriminate between at-risk students and dropouts, and students who perform well in school. A majority of--but not all--low achievers and dropouts need (a) frequent opportunities for mobility, (b) reasonable choices of how, with what, and with whom to learn, (c) a variety of instructional resources, environments, and sociological groupings rather than routines and patterns, (d) opportunities to learn during late morning, afternoon, or evening hours (rarely in the early morning), (e) informal seating--not wooden, steel, or plastic chairs and desks, (f) soft illumination (bright light contributes to their hyperactivity), and (g) either tactual/ visual introductory resources reinforced by kinesthetic/ visual resources, or kinesthetic/visual introductory resources reinforced by tactual/visual resources. Underachievers tend to have poor auditory memory. When they learn visually, it usually is through pictures, drawings, graphs, symbols, comics, and cartoons rather than book text. Although underachievers often want to do well in school, their inability to remember facts through lecture, discussion, or reading contributes to their low performance in conventional schools, where most instruction is delivered by teachers talking and students listening or reading. (Although underachievers learn differently from high achievers and the gifted, it should also be pointed out that they can learn differently from each other.) What role does motivation play in the learning-style construct? Motivation is one of the twenty-three elements of learning style. Unlike at least three-quarters of the remaining elements, motivation is not biologically imposed. Rather it develops as a reaction to each learner's experiences, interest in the content that is being learned, and the ease with which it can be mastered. How does culture contribute to achievement? The Milgram, Dunn, and Price (1993) study of the learning styles of almost 6,000 gifted and nongifted adolescents in nine diverse cultures revealed that opportunity influences individuals' ability to develop specific areas of talents that may eventually lead to giftedness. For example, if access to creative activities, information, or role models was not readily available in a specific culture, few adolescents developed giftedness in that domain. Thus, in cultures that respected art, higher percentages of artistically gifted students were identified. The same finding held firm across other gifted domains--athletics, dance, mathematics, literature, music, and science--across eight countries (Brazil, Canada, Greece, Guatemala, Israel, Korea, the Philippines, and the United States) and the culture of the Maya. It may be important to acknowledge that most communities in the United States financially support athletics regardless of the state of the economy but rarely hesitate to eliminate programs in music, art, or drama. Is it any wonder that most young American boys seem to aspire to becoming baseball, basketball, or football players rather than scientists or artists? How important will learning styles be in the year 2000? Given the statistically higher reading and mathematics standardized achievement test scores of previously failing and poorly achieving students in the United States after their learning styles were addressed, learning styles are likely to become a mandated prerequisite for schooling within the next decade. It will only take one class action suit, led by one small group of angry parent advocates, whose nontraditional children have been demoralized by the imposition of traditional schooling, to cause that change. And it will happen, because learning style is not something that affects other people's children. In every family, mothers' and fathers' learning styles are dramatically different from each other. Siblings do not necessarily reflect their parents' styles, and siblings' styles differ significantly. In most families, one child does extremely well in traditional schooling and another considers academics dull and uninteresting. A third child may be extremely different from the first two; thus, one in three is likely to pursue a path totally different from the parents' and the siblings'. Style affects everyone. Whether or not we acknowledge that we each learn differently, certain resources, approaches, and teachers are right for some--and very wrong for others. REFERENCES Alberg, J., L. Cook, T. Fiore, M. Friend, S. Sano, et. al. 1992. Educational approaches and options for integrating students with disabilities: A decision tool. Triangle Park, N.C.: Research Triangle Institute, P. O. Box 12194, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina 27709. Andrews, R. H. 1990. The development of a learning styles program in a low socioeconomic, underachieving North Carolina elementary school. Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities International 6(3): 307-14. Beaty, S. A. 1986. The effect of inservice training on the ability of teachers to observe learning styles of students. Doctoral diss., Oregon State University. Dissertation Abstracts International 47:1998A. Brunner, C. E., and W. S. Majewski. 1990. Mildly handicapped students can succeed with learning styles. Educational Leadership 48(02): 21-23. Curry, L. 1987. Integrating concepts of cognitive or learning styles: A review with attention to psychometric standards. Ottowa, Ontario: Canadian College of Health Services Executives. Dunn, R., and K. Dunn. 1992. Teaching elementary students through their individual learning styles. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. ------. 1993. Teaching secondary students through their individual learning styles. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Dunn, R., S. A. Griggs, J. Olson, B. Gorman, and M. Beasley. 1995. A metaanalytic validation of the Dunn and Dunn learning styles model. Journal of Educational Research 88(6): 353-61. Dunn, R., K. Dunn, and J. Perrin. 1994. Teaching young children through their individual learning styles. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Dunn, R., K. Dunn, and G. E. Price. 1972, 1975, 1979, 1981, 1984, 1989. Learning Style Inventory. Lawrence, Kan.: Price Systems. Homework Disc. 1995. Jamaica, N. Y.: St. John's University's Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Styles. Milgram, R. M., R. Dunn, and G. E. Price, eds. 1993. Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescents: An international learning style perspective. Westport, Conn.: Praeger. Perrin, J. 1982. Learning Style Inventory: Primary Version. Jamaica, N. Y.: St. John's University's Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Styles. Stone, P. 1992. How we turned around a problem school. The Principal 71(2): 34-36. Editor's Note: Rita Dunn, an authority on learning styles, is a professor in the Division of Administrative and Instructional Leadership and the director of the center of the study of learning and teaching styles at St. John's University Jamaica, New York. She has published more than three hundred articles, chapters, monographs, and research paper on learning styles and on the results of being taught according to one's preffered learning style. She was interviewed by mail by Michael Shaughnessy for this article. ~~~~~~~~ By MICHAEL F. SHAUGHNESSY Michael F. Shaughnessy is a professor at Eastern New Mexico University, Pontales, New Mexico. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Clearing House is the property of Heldref Publications and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Clearing House, Jan/Feb98, Vol. 71 Issue 3, p141, 5p. Item Number: 355784 Result 53 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Learning styles in a technology-rich environment. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies Source: Journal of Research on Computing in Education, Summer97, Vol. 29 Issue 4, p338, 13p, 2 graphs Author(s): Cohen, Vicki L. LEARNING STYLES IN A TECHNOLOGY-RICH ENVIRONMENT Abstract This study investigated whether learning style would change after a year of schooling in a technology-rich educational environment dedicated to a constructivist approach to reaming. The subjects were 15 gifted freshmen who had been accepted into a "magnet" high school. The subjects were given Dunn and Dunn's Learning Style Inventory and a questionnaire before and after the school year. This study could not conclude that learning styles change after one year; however, there are suggestions that learning styles are affected by factors within the environment, such as exposure to technology. Results suggest that a technology-rich environment affects the written and unwritten curriculum within a classroom, especially impacting the social context that exists. The use of computers affected the way the content was explored and presented. A technology-rich environment also seemed to affect the interaction that occurred between students and students, students and teachers, and teachers and teachers. A much more casual social context emerged, which was supportive of exploration and discourse. After one year, some students displayed low preference for learning in this environment; the researcher concluded that instruction must encourage many different forms of learning styles. (Keywords: computers, constructivism, learning styles, secondary education, social context.) The purpose of this article is to document and explain a pilot research project that was undertaken in the 1994-1995 school year to investigate the relationship between student learning style and ability to use the computer as a cognitive tool. The primary objectives of this study were: (a) to investigate whether students' learning styles would change after a year of schooling in a unique technology-rich educational environment dedicated to a constructivist team-oriented approach to learning and (b) to analyze the study's research questions and methodology, pinpointing revisions that would need to be addressed in a more comprehensive study. The study was small in scope, involving a team of 15 freshman from all over Bergen County, New Jersey, who had been accepted into a "magnet" high school that emphasizes science, mathematics, and technology. The high school was conceived in 1990; has approximately 50 students per grade; and, as of 1994-1995 school year, did not have a graduating class yet. The school, the Academy for the Advancement of Science and Technology, is dedicated to a constructivist approach to education. In the constructivist approach, students are encouraged to construct their own knowledge bases, and teachers guide students through the process of obtaining new understandings through the use of discourse, discussion, and questioning. Constructivist teaching practices help learners internalize, reshape, and transform new information (Brooks & Brooks, 1993). The academy, therefore, encourages teachers to keep teacher-directed lectures to a minimum and to emphasize a hands-on discovery-oriented approach to learning. Technology is infused into all classes, and every student and teacher is given a computer to take home. This environment fosters a unique approach to education and supports any research that is conducted on its premises. Within such a rich context, a small exploratory pilot study seemed appropriate for the first year of research. Here, initial research questions could be examined, and instrumentation and methodology could be tried out on a small number of students. It was determined that during the first year of research the researcher would observe one or two classes in depth and gather data on the overall social and educational context of the school. By performing this preliminary research, the author would be better able to determine the best direction for future research. BACKGROUND This study was based on the theoretical assumption that technology is most effectively used in the classroom when students use technology as a cognitive tool. In this way, students must apply problem-solving processes and employ higher order reasoning strategies leading to cognitive growth. As such, the technology becomes a "mind-extension 'cognitive tool'" (Derry & Lajoie, 1993, p. 5). When students use technology as a tool that fosters higher order thinking skills, the ways in which students learn changes; thus, technology has a direct positive impact upon student achievement (Cohen, 1995). Other researchers (Reusser, 1993) maintain that computer technologies can serve as powerful catalysts for facilitating development of generalized self-regulatory skills, provided they are appropriately deployed within a social classroom environment that promotes reflection, discussion, and critique during problem-solving processes. Cognitive instructional tools must be used by mindful teachers and learners in a culture of problem solving in which higher order strategies and control processes are modeled and students are coached by a mentor who gradually phases out support as the student gains independence and expertise in demonstrating how to use these processes (Palincsar, 1986; Reusser, 1993; Vygotsky, 1978). This project used Dunn and Dunn's Learning Style Inventory (LSI; Dunn, Dunn, & Price, 1989) to test learning style. A more complete profile of each student would emerge by using a statistically valid and reliable test such as the LSI. Dunn, Dunn, and Price (1989) state that a learning style is and developmentally imposed set of personal characteristics same teaching method effective for some and ineffective for Beaudry, & Klavas, 1989). The LSI obtains a profile of each following four major areas: a biologically that make the others (Dunn, student in the 1. Environment, including sound, temperature, light, and design. 2. Emotionality, including motivation, responsibility, persistence, and the need for either structure or flexibility. 3. Sociological needs, including learning alone, with peers, with adults, or in a combination of these ways. 4. Physical needs, including perceptual preferences (auditory, visual, tactile, and kinesthetic), time of day one prefers to study, intake, and mobility. This inventory results in an individual profile of a student's preference toward style of learning. Dunn, Dunn, and Price (1989) feel that classrooms need to concentrate more upon individual learning style because students tend to learn and remember better and enjoy learning more when they are taught in a way that takes into account their learning style preferences (Dunn, 1990). By using the LSI inventory, teachers should profile each child's learning style and design instruction based upon individual needs. When permitted to learn difficult academic information or skills through identified preferences, children achieve statistically higher test and attitude scores than when instruction is not supportive of their preferences (Dunn, Beaudry, & Klavas, 1989). Dunn, Dunn, and Price (1989) propose that each student has a specific learning style and that instruction should be designed to best accommodate that unique way of learning. Their model supports the assumption that instruction should address individual styles of learning and that some students learn best through different approaches. The project used this perspective to explore whether learning styles can change within a technology-rich environment that encourages one model of instruction. THE RESEARCH SITE This study took place at a specialized "magnet" high school in Bergen County, New Jersey, the Academy for the Advancement of Science and Technology, which emphasizes science, mathematics, and technology. As an Apple Academy East, the academy infuses technology into all subject areas, and the school is committed to a team-oriented project-based approach to learning. As part of the Coalition of Essential Schools, the academy is dedicated to educational reform and has developed an environment in which students can explore, learn, and work together on projects they might encounter in the real world. One of the academy's goals is to offer interdisciplinary learning with an emphasis on critical analysis and expression of ideas. The academy has many networked classrooms of both Macintosh PowerPCs and IBM compatible PCs. Every classroom has an overhead projection system, at least one scanner, and notebook computers to accommodate any overflow of students. The school also has specialty classrooms equipped for multimedia production with video capture boards, high-capacity storage drives, and VCRs and videodisc players attached to workstations for capture of video images. It also has special PC CAD rooms and specially equipped advanced scientific equipment attached to computers. In addition, there is a robotics area for juniors and seniors to work on special industrial projects. As part of the facility, there is a distance-learning classroom equipped with two-way interactive television that can transmit to a consortium of 14 schools throughout the county. The student body is comprised of those students selected from all over Bergen County who have demonstrated individual initiative; have interests in math, science, and technology; perform in the above average to superior range academically; and have demonstrated a commitment to a longer school day and school year. It attracts a population of students who would be classified as "gifted." The population is also very multicultural, and, with much effort in the area of recruitment, equally divided in male and female representation. Another unique feature of this school is the physical layout of classrooms. There are no "desks" per se; instead there are conference tables, computer workstations, and informal tables. In the interdisciplinary American studies program, which encompasses English and social studies, there is an amphitheater for presentations. This informal design corresponds to the informal atmosphere that pervades this school. Because the academy is a new school, some teachers are not consistently following the approach that the school strongly advocates and still employ lecture and tests as a major part of their classroom instruction. Other teachers are very unstructured and interpret constructivism to be minimal teacher feedback and guidance. Projects are the major focus of the each class but the success of how this approach is implemented varies from class to class. METHOD Sample A team of 15 students was assigned to this researcher by the school administration. The team was chosen based on scheduling arrangements with the school and the researcher. The team was comprised of 12 male students and 3 females. Nine of the students were white, five were Asian, and one was Hispanic. The gender imbalance was seen as a definite disadvantage, but because of scheduling problems and the nature of this exploratory study, this arrangement was accepted. The Measures The LSI (Dunn, Dunn, & Price, 1989) was administered to the sample of students. This inventory obtains a profile of each student in 22 areas that, when identified as relevant areas, represent the way in which that individual prefers to study or concentrate. These 22 areas include the following: 1. Noise level. 2. Light. 3. Temperature. 4. Design of study area. 5. Motivation to achieve academically. 6. Persistence to complete tasks. 7. Responsibility to conform or follow through on assignments. 8. Structure in doing schoolwork or preference for doing an assignment his or her own way. 9. Learning alone or with peers. 10. Preferring to have authority figures present. 11. Preferring to learn in several ways. 12. Auditory preferences. 13. Visual preferences. 14. Tactile preferences. 15. Kinesthetic preferences. 16. Preferring intake while studying. 17. Functions best in evening or morning. 18. Functions best in late morning. 19. Functions best in afternoon. 20. Prefers to be mobile when studying. 21. Parent-figure motivated. 22. Teacher motivated. The test is designed for Grades 5-12. Students respond on a five-point Likert scale ranging from Strongly Disagree to Strongly Agree. There are 105 questions, and an individual profile is calculated from a student's score. The standard score scale ranges from 0 to 80, with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. The standard score is calculated based on the scores of more than 500,000 students who have completed the LSI. Individuals having a standard score of 60 or higher have a high preference for that area when they study. Individuals having a standard score of 40 or lower have a low preference in that area when they study. Individuals having scores that fall between 40 and 60 indicated that their preference is neither high nor low in that area. The inventory has gone through extensive testing and has proven to have high reliability and validity (Dunn, Dunn, & Price, 1989). A questionnaire was also administered to the students immediately following the LSI. The questionnaire surveyed each student's previous knowledge of computers; motivational interest in technology; and preference for working on a team, with a partner, or alone. Procedure The four major methods of gathering data were weekly classroom observations, two interviews with each of the students, administration of the LSI, and administration of the questionnaire. This approach was selected because a flexible exploratory method was needed that combined qualitative analysis of observational reports with quantitative data gathered through the LSI and the questionnaire. I observed primarily one classroom once a week: the American studies class that met 8:00-10:20 a.m. every Friday morning. This class, which was interdisciplinary in approach, was taught by two different teachers: Ms. Cerrato taught the social studies component, and Ms. Lisa taught the English component. Although the two teachers worked closely together on curriculum coordination, these were taught as two separate classes, each with a distinct curriculum and assignments. Ms. Cerrato utilized the computer frequently throughout the social studies curriculum, but Ms. Lisa did not emphasize the use of computers at all, preferring to hold class at a conference table in seminar fashion and to have individual conferences on student writing. Students could use the computer for her projects if they desired, but it was not emphasized. With the approval of the school authorities and with parental consent, the LSI and the questionnaire were administered to the 15 students in the first month of schooling. Questionnaires were given after the LSI. In the final month of schooling, the LSI and the same questionnaire were administered again to 14 students; one of the female students had returned to her district high school. In the third and tenth months of schooling, students were interviewed to assess their perceptions of the academy and to determine the level of satisfaction with the academy's program. The responses were recorded by the researcher and the students were probed if the response was brief. At the three-month interview, students received a written profile of their learning styles to take home. I discussed what each profile meant and how it could be interpreted and used. Data Analysis Descriptive statistics were used to analyze the data quantitatively. After the LSI inventories were scored, Price Systems, Inc., sent two computer printouts for group analysis. These reports summarized the elements by subscale for all individuals in the group having standard scores of 60 or higher or 40 or lower. The printouts indicate frequency of response and percent of the group. Differences in group frequencies and percent that occurred between the two different test administrations were calculated. The responses on the questionnaire were also coded and analyzed, including previous knowledge of computers; motivational interest in technology; and preference for working on a team, with a partner, or alone. The two interviews were carefully studied and the field notes were coded to see if any patterns existed in the American studies classroom. These notes were closely read and coded into three major divisions: (a) social context of classroom, (b) computer-related activities, and (c) learning style of students. FINDINGS The Field Notes and Interviews After analyzing both the field notes taken during the year of observation and the two interviews, a few major points emerge. The first point is that technology seemed to impact the written curriculum in the American studies classroom that I observed. New ways of looking at and exploring the curriculum emerged as these two teachers tried to integrate technology directly into their courses. Computer literacy was no longer left to the jurisdiction of the "computer teacher" who existed in a "computer lab." Each teacher had a unique way of approaching computer literacy as technology became a tool that impacted each subject area in a unique and specific way. Databases, the Internet, scanning images, and HyperCard (1987-1995) were incorporated into the daily routine of the classroom. Teachers and students were exploring how technology could be used as a tool that enhances the subject area. In addition, technology impacted the way the content was presented and discussed. The subject matter was presented with a much more visual representation of the concepts, and computer projects sometimes seemed to determine the direction the class would go. Such a visual emphasis often seemed to help many of the students who had language problems because they were not native speakers or were visual/kinesthetic learners. In the American studies class, one class project was an extensive HyperCard (19871995) stack discussing people who had contributed to or had an effect during the Civil War. Each card had a description of the person, his or her contribution, and a scanned image of the person. Three students were assigned to be project managers; they were responsible for developing a template that the class could use to standardize the stack. The class spent a lot of time discussing the stack, how to set it up, how the template should look and work, and how the buttons should function. Students worked independently on their research, guided by the way the cards would be set up. All work was given to the project managers, who would import the information into the template. One project manager, who had language and social problems, grew a great deal during this project because of his responsibility and leadership role. At another time, the class was involved in a project in which teams were to present each amendment to the U.S. Constitution using ClarisWorks (1995) or HyperCard. I observed an interesting discussion on how to visually depict the 26th amendment: One student raised his hand and said, "I can't find a picture of people voting!" A serious discussion ensued about what this amendment really meant and how it could be depicted. A second major point that emerged from the field notes was that technology seemed to affect the unwritten curriculum as well as the written curriculum. The way the teacher managed the discipline and rules pervading the classroom changed as well. Learning was seen as a much more natural process, which was not disrupted by conversation and discourse. The boundaries and rules of the traditional classroom (e.g., never interrupt the teacher) were replaced by a much more fluid interpretation of how a classroom should function. Much of this could be attributed to the high use of technology within the classroom, as traditional ways of accomplishing tasks, assignments, and lessons were altered. This can be illustrated by examining the social context that existed at the academy. In this type of technology-rich environment, social interaction between students and students, between students and teachers, and between teachers and teachers was different than what you might find in a more traditional high school classroom. In the American studies classroom I observed, the students were given many different types of projects to work on as teams, including designing databases of historical figures using ClarisWorks (1995), developing a timeline of the Civil War using HyperCard (1987-1995) stacks, and developing an interactive game on a novel they had read. During these projects, the students felt free to work where they pleased--at tables, at the computers, or in groups in a corner. During this time I observed them conversing about many things that were on task and off task: how to use the technology, topics concerning the project, and what happened last weekend. I often observed a group of students sitting at a scanner or around a computer, laughing and talking about personal matters for about 10 min with no disciplinary action being taken. The students would naturally refocus on their project and continue talking about more on-task matters. This is not to say disciplinary comments concerning ontask behavior were not made, because, in fact, both Ms. Cerrato and Ms. Lisa continuously urged the students to quiet down and stay focused on the task at hand. However, in this environment, interaction between students was much more frequent, casual, and accepted. Much of the student-student interaction centered around the technology; students discussed new technological discoveries, showed how the content could be covered using the technology, or casually chattered about a new game while sitting informally around a piece of equipment. In addition, teachers and students interacted in a much more casual way. Ms. Cerrato often sat next to a student at the computer and gave individual help with a project. Within this context, students felt comfortable asking questions and seeking help at all times. I continuously saw random students walking into the classroom during the class period and seeking help from Ms. Cerrato or Ms. Lisa; both teachers freely gave help and encouraged this type of interaction. Students would frequently show Ms. Cerrato how to do something, and she often sought help from some of the more capable students. The interaction between teachers and students was not just in one direction and for one purpose--to convey information to students. Rather, the interaction was fluid and fulfilled many purposes. Teacher-teacher interactions were also different than in a regular classroom. Team-teaching facilitated the sharing of ideas, jokes, casual banter, and informal conversations between teachers. Ms. Cerrato and Ms. Lisa were constantly joking with each other and often substituted for each other if one came late or had to leave the room for a meeting. Other teachers frequently walked into the classroom at any time to ask either Ms. Cerrato or Ms. Lisa questions. The only time this changed was during formal student presentations, during which absolute silence and attention were required. In fact, the classroom often seemed chaotic, especially when guests, sometimes numbering 20 or more, would be ushered into the classroom to observe. Despite all this seemingly distracting social interaction, students were actively engaged in learning. Much sharing and discussing took place: Students became teachers and explorers in a classroom culture that was very relaxed. The technology and its use seemed to be the factor that affected the social context and thereby the unwritten curriculum that I observed. A third major point that emerged from the field notes is that these gifted students wanted to be shown how to seek deeper connections when using the computer. Many of the students were not "enamored" with just using the computer; they often became frustrated when they felt the project they were working on was not challenging them to use their minds and seek deeper connections. A student wrote on the questionnaire that one subject that used technology extensively "lacks any content quality. We are told that we are supposed to not learn content but how to think. We have not done either. I want projects with substance!" Many students made comments in a similar vein, saying "Get rid of the busywork." Those teachers who acted as "cognitive mentors" were respected and favored. When the students were not using computers for problem solving and higher reasoning, they became frustrated and bored. As one student said, I like working with the technology [in CAD] and I have a natural talent for designing and I would like to pursue it. [The teacher] is just the most knowledgeable teacher about technology in the school and he's helpful and good humored. Another student said about the same class: I am personally very interested in computer engineering, and love the high level of technology we use to complete various, interesting projects. [The teacher] is kind and humorous, making class extremely fun. As a third student summarized, "I want to be challenged!" The field notes also point out that some students, who preferred working alone and desired a more structured learning environment, may have felt some dissatisfaction with the team-oriented approach used. As one student commented that working in teams "is difficult. If I have a good idea, nobody ever listens to me. I'm never the leader because I'm not the leader type." Another student added a very common remark: "When everyone doesn't pull their own weight, it adds stress." Another said, "I don't like working in teams, and I find it a problem. Everyone is smart, but I like working alone. In teams you can't compromise." These comments illustrate the need to actively teach students how to work on teams effectively. The interviews gave valuable information about the students' perceptions of the academy. During the initial interview in November 1994, most of the students expressed high satisfaction with the program. They were excited and pleased with their progress to date. They loved using the technology. Many also expressed stress over the confusion that pervaded some classes and the fact that some teachers were lecturing. "School could be more organized. Different teachers don't communicate with each other. Many teachers are not experienced in this type of environment and this leads to conflict." Many expressed that they loved working in teams, but others expressed hesitation and even outright dislike for having to work in teams. During the second interview in May 1995, most still expressed high satisfaction with the school. One said "It was the best thing that could have happened to me educationally. The teaching style is excellent in basically all of my classes, and I could not be happier." A few others also expressed disappointment. Almost all expressed deep satisfaction about working with technology. The LSI The results of student responses by subscale on the LSI are summarized in Figures I and 2. Each subscale has been categorized into high, medium and low. In the subscale "Learning Alone or With Peers," high represents preference for working with peers and low represents preference for working alone. There are certain trends indicated by the data that the population of students displays. At the end of the year, the trends showed the following preferences: Students seemed less motivated and, therefore, required shorter and less complicated assignments and more frequent teacher supervision. Students seemed less persistent and, therefore, preferred short-term assignments. Students seemed less responsible and did not seem to want to complete assignments that they did not find worthwhile. Students seemed to prefer structured assignments rather than assignments that allowed them to learn in different ways. Students were less motivated by the teacher and wanted to work independent of teacher and other authority figure input. Students preferred a more formal classroom design, rather than being allowed to sit on the floor, soft chairs, or pillows when studying. Students seemed to prefer perceptual means of learning rather than auditory means of learning. When analyzing these results as a total picture, the LSI has yielded somewhat unexpected, if not startling, results. The results show that the students seemed less motivated, persistent, and responsible toward schoolwork and assignments after a year at the academy. In September, this group showed exceptionally high motivation, persistence, and responsibility. In June, these students showed a marked decrease in these areas. All these students came from traditional eighth-grade classes in which they were perceived as the "smart kids." At the academy, they were suddenly thrust into a much different learning environment, one in which they were among many gifted students working together on teams. Long-range team-oriented projects were frequently used during the year, and students were given independence and latitude in working together on these projects. After a year, the trend in preference toward less motivation, persistence, and responsibility, and the trend toward preferring more structured assignments suggests that this group was reacting to this shift. The students may not have received the structure, feedback, and guidance they needed. In previous studies, high IQ students preferred to learn by themselves rather than with others, tended to be self-motivated, and preferred to receive continuous feedback from authority figures (Dunn, 1993). If this population of students' learning preferences were not accommodated during the year, the trend indicated by the data collected during this study might occur. According to Dunn (1993), "a program designed for gifted and talented youngsters should capitalize on their personal sociological preferences and not be determined by persons who advocate a single approach for all students" (p. 40). This result may indicate that teachers need to address all learning styles in their classrooms and that a curriculum must be designed that develops all forms of intelligence and reaches all types of learning styles throughout the year. If one lesson stresses teamwork, the next should emphasize independent research. Teachers may need to be more sensitive to different learning styles and use many different teaching styles. Students may also need to be trained about engaging in positive social interaction during teamwork so that the social and moral implications of how to work together positively are discussed. If groupwork is a predominant aspect of the curriculum, the process of working together might need to be taught as well. CONCLUSION After completing this study, I could not definitely conclude that learning styles change after one year of immersion in a technology-rich environment dedicated to using a team-oriented, hands-on, and constructivist approach to education. There are suggestions that learning styles are affected by factors within the environment, such as exposure to technology, and that certain areas of preference, such as motivation to succeed, persistence to complete tasks, responsibility to complete assignments, and structure in doing schoolwork, are affected by exposure to specific instructional methodologies. However, this needs to be verified by a larger study. The results obtained during this study suggest that a technology-rich environment seems to affect the written and unwritten curriculum within a classroom. Teachers emphasized the visual nature of the subject matter, and the use of computers affected the way the content was explored, often to the extent of determining the directions in which the assignments and lessons might go. The effect of these changes was that teachers and students learned how to use technology as a tool that enhances each subject area in a different and specific way. The interaction between students and students, students and teachers, and teachers and teachers changed. Learning was seen as a much more natural process whereby conversation does not interfere with acquisition or application of knowledge. A looser, more casual social context that was supportive of exploration and exchange emerged. This social context was not limited to one classroom; it extended throughout the school into almost all classrooms I observed. The results of this study also suggest that the individual learning styles of gifted students must be addressed. Teachers may need to design a curriculum that promotes all types of learning styles throughout the year Gifted students also desire opportunities to use computers as cognitive tools that explore deeper connections in a subject rather than merely using the technology as a vehicle to produce "glitzy" presentations without much depth. They desire to be pushed intellectually and want to be challenged. In summary, a technology-rich environment impacts the written and unwritten curriculum of a school. Schools should be sensitive to students' reaming styles when adopting an instructional methodology that will be used extensively throughout the curriculum. At a time when educators are unilaterally embracing change and reform and claiming that "constructivism" is the panacea for all problems in the classroom, the effects of the instructional approach upon the student population must be analyzed. This study could not definitively prove that learning styles change after one year in a technologically rich environment. A larger, more comprehensive study with control groups must be used to determine this. Contributor Dr. Vicki L. Cohen is an assistant professor in the School of Education at Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, New Jersey. She coordinates the Instructional Technology Certificate Program and teaches graduate classes in reading, evaluation, and instructional technology. Her research interests include technology in education, literacy development, and evaluation and assessment. (Address: Dr. Vicki Cohen, School of Education, Bancroft Hall, Farleigh Dickinson University, 1000 River Road, Teaneck, NJ 07666; cohen @alpha.fdu.edu.) Figure 1. Frequency of responses on selected LSI items, September 1994 Figure 2. Frequency of responses on selected LSI items, June 1995 References Brooks, J., & Brooks, M. (1993). The case for constructivist classrooms. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. ClarisWorks [Computer software]. (1995). Santa Clara, CA: Claris Corporation. Cohen, V. (1995, Fall/Winter). What schools should know about technology: A review of the research. Record, 28-34. Derry, D., & Lajoie, S. (1993). A middle camp for (un)intelligent instructional computing: An introduction. In S. Lajoie & S. Derry (Eds.), Computers as cognitive tools (pp. 1-11). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Dunn, R. (1990). Understanding the Dunn and Dunn learning styles model and the need for individual diagnosis and prescription. Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities, 6, 223-247. Dunn, R. (1993). Teaching gifted adolescents through their learning style strengths. In R. Dunn & G. Price (Eds.), Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescents (pp. 37-67). Westport, CN: Praeger. Dunn, R., Beaudry, J., & Klavas, A. (1989). Survey of research on learning styles. Educational Leadership, 46(6), 50-58. Dunn, R., Dunn, K., & Price, G. (1989). Learning style inventory manual. Lawrence, KS: Price Systems. HyperCard [Computer software]. (1987-1995). Cupertino, CA: Apple Computer, Inc. Palincsar, A. S. (1986). The role of dialogue in providing scaffolded instruction. Educational Psychologist, 21, 73-99. Reusser, K. (1993). Tutoring systems and pedagogical theory: Representational tools for understanding, planning, and reflection in problem solving. In S. Lajoie & S. Derry (Eds.), Computers as cognitive tools (pp. 143-177). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Vygotsky, I. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ~~~~~~~~ By Vicki L. Cohen, Fairleigh Dickinson University ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Journal of Research on Computing in Education is the property of International Society for Technology in Education and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Journal of Research on Computing in Education, Summer97, Vol. 29 Issue 4, p338, 13p, 2 graphs. Item Number: 9709171787 Result 62 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 1 of 2 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Learning Styles and Technology in a Ninth-Grade High School Population. Subject(s): HIGH school environment -- New Jersey -- Bergen County; LEARNING & scholarship -- New Jersey -- Bergen County; HIGH school students -- New Jersey -- Bergen County Source: Journal of Research on Computing in Education, Summer2001, Vol. 33 Issue 4, p355, 12p, 1 chart, 1 graph Author(s): Cohen, Vicki L. Abstract: This study explores whether a technology-rich environment that promotes a constructivist approach to learning has a significant effect on the learning styles of freshmen high school students. Two high school freshmen classes were pre- and posttested on a learning-style inventory. One high school has a technology-rich environment and uses a project-based approach to learning, while the other school has a more traditional curriculum that is not technology rich. Six variables from the inventory were analyzed in this study. The results suggest that a technology-rich environment that promotes collaborative, project-based learning can have an effect on learning style. This study suggests that the environment contributed to the differential in effect size that was found at posttest time. (Keywords: high school and technology, learning style, technology in education.) [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR] AN: 5078025 ISSN: 0888-6504 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. View Item: Full Page Image [Go To Citation] LEARNING STYLES AND TECHNOLOGY IN A NINTH-GRADE HIGH SCHOOL POPULATION Abstract This study explores whether a technology-rich environment that promotes a constructivist approach to learning has a significant effect on the learning styles of freshmen high school students. Two high school freshmen classes were pre- and posttested on a learning-style inventory. One high school has a technology-rich environment and uses a project-based approach to learning, while the other school has a more traditional curriculum that is not technology rich. Six variables from the inventory were analyzed in this study. The results suggest that a technology-rich environment that promotes collaborative, project-based learning can have an effect on learning style. This study suggests that the environment contributed to the differential in effect size that was found at posttest time. (Keywords: high school and technology, learning style, technology in education.) The purpose of this article is to explore the relationship between the learning styles of high school students and their exposure to a technology-rich educational environment in their freshman year of schooling. This article will describe a study that took place in the 1996-1997 school year in which the total high school freshman classes from two different high schools were assessed on learning style. One class was from a more traditional high school that does not infuse technology into its curriculum but is known for its excellence in education, and the other class was from a "magnet" high school that specializes in science and math and infuses technology into every subject area. The objectives of this study were: 1. to determine if there was any significant effect on learning style when freshmen high school students were working in a technology-rich environment that promotes collaborative, project-based learning; 2. to compare two different types of learning environments on high school students' learning styles; and 3. to determine the effect of specific variables in Dunn and Dunn's Learning Style Inventory (LSI) (Dunn, Dunn, & Price, 1989) on freshmen students after a year in two very different high schools. I contend that although Dunn and Dunn claim that learning style is a biologically and developmentally derived set of variables that affect the way one learns and interacts with the surrounding environment (Dunn, 1990), a student's learning style can, in fact, be altered and affected through the external conditions set up in the environment. Therefore, one hypothesis set forth in this study was that a technologically rich environment that supports a constructivist approach to learning would change a student's learning style after a year-long period of exposure. BACKGROUND An initial pilot study was undertaken in the 1994-1995 school year to investigate the relationship of student learning style and ability to use the computer as a cognitive tool (Cohen, 1997). This was a small study of 15 students from all over Bergen County, New Jersey, who had been accepted into the "magnet" high school, the Academy for the Advancement of Science and Technology (AAST). At AAST, teacher-directed lectures were kept to a minimum, technology was infused into each class, and each student and teacher was given a computer to take home. For the pilot study, two classes were observed in depth, field notes were recorded, and a small sample of students were pre- and posttested on the LSI (Dunn, Dunn, et al., 1989) and a questionnaire I developed. This study found that the use of technology affected all aspects of the teaching and learning continuum and demanded new approaches to the curriculum. New ways of looking at and exploring the curriculum emerged as teachers tried to integrate technology directly into the subject matter. However, this pilot study could not definitely conclude that learning styles changed after one year of immersion in a technology-rich environment. There were suggestions that learning styles were affected by factors within the environment, such as exposure to technology, and that certain areas, such as motivation to succeed, persistence to complete tasks, responsibility to complete assignments, and structure in doing schoolwork, were affected by exposure to specific instructional methodologies. However, this pilot study suggested that the findings needed to be verified in a larger study. The present study was designed to address the suggestions of the pilot by pre- and posttesting the freshman classes in two schools--AAST, which comprised approximately 60 students, and Ridgewood High School (RHS), which comprised approximately 100 students. The study was to investigate the relationship between a technology-rich environment that fosters a constructivist approach to instruction and its effect on learning styles. The study used the LSI (Dunn, Dunn, et al., 1989) to assess learning style, which is a biologically and developmentally imposed set of personal characteristics that make the same teaching method effective for some and ineffective for others (Dunn, Beaudry, & Klavas, 1989). The LSI obtains a profile of each student in four major areas: 1. environment, including sound, temperature, light, and design; 2. emotionality, including motivation, responsibility, persistence, and the need for either structure or flexibility; 3. sociological needs, including learning alone, with peers, with adults, and/or in several ways; and 4. physical needs, including perceptual preferences (auditory, visual, tactile and kinesthetic), time of day one prefers to study, intake, and mobility. This inventory results in an individual profile of a student's preference toward style of learning. Dunn and Dunn feel that classrooms need to concentrate more on individual learning style because students tend to learn and remember better and enjoy learning more when they are taught through their learning-style preferences (Dunn, 1990). When permitted to learn difficult academic information or skills through identified preferences, children achieve statistically higher test and attitude scores than instruction that is not supportive of their preferences (Dunn, Beaudry, et al.). The Research Sites The experimental setting for this project was the AAST, a specialized magnet high school in Bergen County that emphasizes science, mathematics, and technology. As an Apple Academy East, AAST infuses technology into all subject areas, and the school is committed to a team-oriented, project-based approach to learning. As part of the Coalition of Essential Schools, AAST is dedicated to educational reform and has developed an environment where students can explore, learn, and work together on projects they might encounter in the real world. One of AAST's goals is to offer interdisciplinary learning with an emphasis on critical analysis and expression of ideas. AAST has many networked classrooms of both Macs and IBM-compatible PCs. Every classroom has an overhead projection system, at least one scanner, and notebook computers to accommodate any overflow of students. The school also has: • specialty classrooms equipped for multimedia production with video capture boards, high-capacity storage drives, and VCRs and videodiscs attached to workstations for capture of video images; • special PC CAD rooms and specially equipped advanced scientific equipment attached to computers; • a robotics area where juniors and seniors can work on special industrial projects; and • a distance-learning classroom equipped with two-way interactive television that can transmit to a consortium of 14 schools throughout the county. The student body comprises students from all over Bergen County who have demonstrated individual initiative; have interest in math, science, and technology; perform in the above-average to superior range academically; and have demonstrated a commitment to a longer school day and school year. It attracts a population of students who would be classified as gifted. The population is also very multicultural and, with much effort in the area of recruitment, is equally divided in male and female representation. Another unique feature of this school is the physical layout of classrooms. There are no desks per se, but conference tables, computer workstations, and informal tables to work at. Numerous computers are noticeable in each classroom, as they dominate the layout of the room, either by circling the circumference of the room or by transforming the room into a lab. This informal design corresponds to the informal atmosphere that pervades this school and supports the instructional goal of the school: to promote a constructivist approach to learning. In the constructivist approach, students are encouraged to construct their own knowledge bases, and teachers guide students through the process of obtaining new understandings through the use of discourse, discussion, and questioning. Constructivist teaching practices help learners internalize and reshape, or transform, new information (Brooks & Brooks, 1993). RHS was the control setting for this study. Located in a suburban, uppermiddle-class neighborhood in Bergen County, RHS has consistently been recognized and honored for its excellence in education. RHS's total enrollment of students in 1997 was 1,446, with 97% of students taking the SATs and averaging 572 in math and 491 in verbal. More than 88% of students at RHS plan to attend college. RHS has a more traditional approach to education, with classes structured in rows of seats and a teacher frequently standing in the front of the room lecturing from an overhead projector. Classes are well behaved, and discipline is not a major problem. Students are given a good deal of freedom, especially during lunch, when they sit in the halls freely talking or wandering about. The atmosphere is relaxed but strict. Technology is not infused regularly into the subject areas, and most classrooms are not equipped with more than a few computers, if any at all. When technology is used, the classes must move to a lab or media center where there are enough computers for all students. Instructional methodology is based on each individual teacher's teaching style, and project-based learning or constructivism is not promoted throughout the school as part of its philosophy. METHOD Samples Sixty-six students (out of a total of 70) in the freshman class at AAST were pre- and posttested; 34 were males, and 32 were females. It was a multicultural group that had been selected to attend through rigorous mathematical testing, references, interviews, and analysis of middle school grades. Ninety-seven students (out of a total of approximately 120) in the freshman class at RHS were pre- and posttested; 43 were male, and 54 were female. The sample included all students whose parents had returned the advised consent form, regardless of the student's educational history or special education classification. The sample reflected a cross-section of the population of students at RHS, approximately 10% multicultural and 90% white, middle-class students. Measures The LSI (Dunn, Dunn, et al., 1989) was administered to the sample of students. This inventory obtains a profile of each student in 22 areas that, when identified as relevant, represent the way in which the individual prefers to study or concentrate: (1) noise level, (2) light, (3) temperature, (4) design of study area, (5) motivation to achieve academically, (6) persistence to complete tasks, (7) responsibility to conform or follow through on assignments, (8) structure in doing schoolwork or preference for doing an assignment his or her own way, (9) learning along or with peers, (10) preferring authority figures present, (11) preferring learning in several ways, (12) auditory preferences, (13) visual preferences, (14) tactile preferences, (15) kinesthetic preferences, (16) prefers intake while studying, (17) functions best in evening or morning, (18) functions best in late morning, (19) functions best in afternoon, (20) prefers to be mobile when studying, (21) parent-figure motivated, and (22) teacher motivated. The test is designed for Grades 5-12. Students respond on a five-point Likert scale ranging from Strongly Disagree to Strongly Agree. The LSI has 104 questions, with approximately four items attributed to each variable. An individual profile is calculated from a student's score. The standard score scale ranges from 0 to 80 with a mean or 50 and a standard deviation of 10. The standard score has been calculated based on the scores of more than 500,000 students who have completed the LSI. Individuals having a standard score of 60 or higher have a high preference for that area when they study. Individuals having a standard score of 40 or lower have a low preference in that area when they study. Individuals having scores that fall between 40 and 60 indicated that their preference is neither high nor low in that area. The inventory has gone through extensive testing and has proven to have high reliability and validity (Dunn, Dunn, et al., 1989). This study focused on six of the variables from the LSI, which were chosen because of the pilot study: motivation, persistence, responsibility, preference for working alone or with peers, parent motivated, and teacher motivated. These six variables, targeted before the study began, seemed to be the most relevant to this study. Many of the variables, such as noise level, light preference, room design, intake, time of day for studying, and others, were not directly relevant to the research question. It also helped to focus the study on six specific areas rather than to look at all 22 factors measured in the LSI. Procedure The two methods of data gathering were administration of the LSI and administration of an interview to a small sample of students at both AAST and RHS. With the approval of the school authorities and with parental consent, the LSI was administered to the two different samples in the first month of schooling. In the final month of schooling, the LSI was administered again to the same two samples of students at AAST and at RHS. In addition, in May, 10 students from each sample were interviewed to determine the level of technology use for that year and the level of satisfaction with their high school's program. I recorded all students' responses. Data Analysis After the LSI inventories are scored, Price Systems, Inc., sends two computer printouts for group analysis. These reports summarize the elements by subscale for all individuals in the group having standard scores of 60 or higher or 40 or lower. The printouts indicate frequency of response and percent of the group. Differences in group frequencies and percent between the two different test administrations were calculated. Descriptive statistics were used to obtain information about the mean and standard deviation. Multivariate tests, Pillai's Trace and Wilks' lambda, were used to obtain information on effect size for all preand posttest measures. Univariate F tests were performed on all variables subsequent to performing the multivariate tests. The data were analyzed and have been presented in Figure 1, which displays graphs showing the relevant sample means along with the size of the standard error of the mean for each variable. The size of the standard errors of the mean provides a direct and intuitive visual measure of how, precisely, the location of the relevant population means--and thereby the general pattern of the population means-can be inferred. RESULTS Multivariate tests, Pillai's Trace and Wilks' lambda, were used to determine whether there was a difference between AAST and RHS at pre-and posttest time. These mulitvariate tests showed that there was a significant difference between schools on the pretest, F(6,144) = 3945.106, p < .002. Effect size, given by eta-squared (Eta[sup 2]), is 0.134. There was a significant difference between schools on the posttest, F(6, 143) = 3653.237, p < .0005. Effect size, given by Eta[sup 2], is .222. The effect size was greater on the posttest time than the pretest. Table 1 shows the mean scores for pre- and posttest scores for each school per variable of the LSI. Figure 1 plots error bars for the results of these selected variables taken at pretest and posttest time intervals from the two samples of students at the two different schools. The error bars represent 95% confidence intervals about the mean. Lack of overlap in the confidence intervals between schools demonstrates significant difference between schools with respect to this measure. Lack of overlap in the confidence intervals from pretest to posttest in one school demonstrates a significant difference between time intervals for that particular school. Motivation (Variable 5) Based on all variables using univariate F tests, AAST and RHS scored significantly different on the pretest, F(1, 140) = 4.820, p = .030. AAST students were significantly higher in the Motivation variable than RHS students on the pretest. Both schools were significantly different on the posttest, F(1,140) = 4.743, p = .031, with AAST students being significantly higher than RHS. In looking at Figure 1, error bars represent 95% confidence intervals about the mean that there was significant difference within subject for each school. Motivation was significantly lower at each school from the pretest to the posttest. In addition, a MANOVA was performed looking at the within-subjects factor of pre- versus posttest scores. Significant differences were found for both AAST, F(1,143) = 452.41, p < .001, and RHS, F(1,143) = 583.68, p < .0005. Persistence (Variable 6) Univariate F tests showed that AAST and RHS did not score significantly different on the pretest, F(1,140) = 1.251, p = .265, or the posttest, F(1,140) = 1.048, p = .308. However, Figure 1 shows a significant difference for AAST at the 95% confidence level between the pretest and the posttest. AAST's scores in this variable were significantly lower on the posttest than the pretest. RHS's scores were not significantly lower, although there are suggestions that the scores did go down. A MANOVA showed a significant difference from pre- to posttest scores at AAST, F(1,143) = 957.67, p < .0005, while no significant difference was found for RHS, F(1,143) = 216.85, p < .050. Responsibility (Variable 7) Univariate F tests showed no significant difference between subjects at the pretest, F(1,140) = .047, p = .829. Both AAST (M = 57.559) and RHS (M = 57.867) scored high in this variable on the pretest. There was a significant difference between subjects on this variable on the postest, F(1,140) = 7.092, p = .009. AAST students scored significantly lower (M = 53.085) than RHS students (M = 56.639) on this variable. Figure 1 shows a significant within-subjects difference for AAST students from pretest to posttest in Responsibility. The MANOVA showed a significant difference from pretest to posttest at AAST, F(1,143) = 607.50, p < .0005, but no significant difference for RHS, F(1,143) = 61.20, p < .156. Working Alone or with Peers (Variable 9) Univariate F tests showed no significant difference between subjects on the pretest, F(1,140) = .099, p = .753. There was a suggestion of a difference on the posttest, although it was not significant at the .05 level, F(1,140) = 1.678, p = .197. The AAST scores suggested that they were higher than RHS's on the posttest. There was no significant difference within subjects for either group, although there is a suggestion that the AAST scores were higher on the posttest. A MANOVA showed no significant difference from pretest to posttest at AAST, F(1,143) = 58.80, p < .214, or at RHS, F(1,143) = 4.61, p < .727. Parent Motivation (Variable 21) Univariate F tests showed significant between-subjects effects on the pretest, F(1, 140) = 5.452, p = .021, with AAST (M = 48.475) being significantly lower in this variable than RHS (M = 52.169). There was no significant difference between subjects on the posttest, F(1, 140) = .634, p = .427, with RHS students becoming less parent motivated at the end of the year. Figure 1 shows significant within-subjects effects for RHS between the pretest and the posttest, with the scores demonstrating less parent motivation at the end of the year so that AAST and RHS scores are within a narrow range at posttest time. The MANOVA showed no significant difference at AAST from pretest to posttest, F(1,143) = 51.93, p < .377, but a significant difference for RHS, F(1, 143) = 435.20, p < .004. Teacher Motivation (Variable 22) Univariate F tests showed no significant difference between subjects on the pretest, F(1, 140) = .212, p = .646. There was a significant difference between AAST and RHS on the posttest, F(1, 140) = 2.791, p = .097, with AAST being much lower than RHS. Figure 1 shows significant within-subjects effects for AAST between the pretest and the posttest, with the scores showing less teacher motivation at the end of the year. There was no significant effect between pretest and posttest for RHS. The MANOVA showed a significant difference at AAST from pretest to posttest, F(1,143) = 452.41, p < .003, but no significant difference at RHS, F(1,143) = 72.48, p < .226. Interviews The following responses were given to interview questions. The responses reflect what a majority of the respondents quoted to me. If there were a variety of responses given, these are included below. How did you like school this year? Both samples said they "liked school a lot." What things did you like about this school? Students at AAST said they liked projects, technology, the material taught, the teachers, the relaxed atmosphere, lots of choices, the social atmosphere, and the challenge. Students at RHS said they liked the rotating schedule so that they do not have classes every day, free periods, the teachers, meeting new people, sports, classes, and the open learning environment. What things did you not like? Students at AAST expressed that they did not like the long hours, the long commute, the workload, not being with friends and losing touch with them, the unstructured environment and not being clear what to do, and the stress over deadlines. Students at RHS expressed that they did not like some of the teachers (inflexible, taught things not relevant, boring, strict, not understanding), they did not like all the lectures, the work was difficult, and the classes were boring. In what areas do you feel that you have grown academically? Students at AAST mentioned math, sciences, and technology. Students at RHS mentioned math, science, English, world history, and foreign languages. In what areas do you feel that you have grown socially? AAST students mentioned making new friends, meeting new people, becoming a more confident speaker, becoming more independent, and working with people better. Students at RHS mentioned making new friends, meeting new people, becoming more confident, and participating in sports. What aspects of this school did you find most difficult? AAST students mentioned the workload, high expectations of teachers, the highly competitive environment, working on teams and often not knowing what to expect, not being able to do sports, and leaving friends. Students at RHS mentioned the workload, time management, expectations of teachers, the rotating schedule, and teacher relationships. On a scale of 1 to 5 with 5 being the highest, to what extent did you use technology in your classes? The AAST average was 4.5. The RHS average was 2. On a scale of 1 to 5 with 5 being the highest, how often did you work in teams? The AAST average was 4.5. The RHS average was 3.5. On a scale of 1 to 5 with 5 being the highest, how stressful was the past year? The AAST average was 3.5, and the average was 3.5. What was stressful? AAST students mentioned finishing projects, working with other people on a team, having all projects due at the same time, the workload, dealing with new teachers, the long school hours (from 8:30 a.m. to 4:00 p.m.), and taking three sciences simultaneously during the year. RHS students mentioned trying to balance schoolwork and playing sports, completing projects, maintaining the workload, being involved in extracurricular activities, adjusting to a new school, and taking midterms and tests. What would you like to change here? AAST students mentioned the overlap of concepts being taught in classes, the heavy emphasis on the sciences with not enough emphasis on the humanities, and teachers needing to coordinate when projects were due. RHS students frequently mentioned more use of technology, or they said, nothing, it's fine here. How relevant did you find school this year? AAST students said that school was very relevant, that it was more like the real world working in teams on projects and learning to apply things. They said e-mail and technology were very helpful in everyday life, and that the heavy integration of technology definitely helped. RHS students said that school "sort of helped for college," but that it was not directly related to day-to-day life. They said they did not know about the relevance to the outside world, because that would depend on which career they will pursue. Summary of Interviews As an overall group, the two samples expressed many of the concerns, frustrations, and insights of typical freshmen in a new high school who are increasingly becoming more social. Both liked their school, although their concerns were different, reflecting major environmental and cultural differences in their schools' climates. AAST students expressed concern over the emphasis of projects and how all the projects were due at the same time. They expressed frustration at the long school days, working in teams, the highly competitive environment that surrounds them, and the high expectations placed on them by teachers, parents, and peers. RHS's concerns were more with the scheduling of the day and the interaction with specific teachers they found boring, dull, or stimulating. They expressed concern with projects, grades, time management, and tests and more tests. An interesting note that emerged in the interviews with the AAST students was the concern regarding lack of structure within specific classes and sometimes within the school itself. Comments were heard about (1) needing to know what to do when working on projects and (2) coordination between teachers and among administrators. This comment was never heard from RHS students. Another interesting point was that AAST students found technology very motivating, exciting, and relevant to their lives. RHS students expressed disappointment over the lack of technology within their subjects. As a result, AAST students saw great relevance of their education to their everyday life, while RHS students perceived their schooling as relevant, but only as it pertained to going to college or pursuing a future career. DISCUSSION The LSI The results from this study suggest that a technology-rich environment that promotes collaborative, project-based learning can have an effect on learning style. The two schools were significantly different in measures of learning style at pretest time; the difference between the schools on these measures was greater at posttest time. AAST showed a greater effect on combined measures from pretest to posttest intervals than RHS did. This suggests that the environment contributed to the differential in effect size. In looking at Variable 5, motivation, both AAST and RHS students' motivation level decreased equally from the pretest to the posttest. It is difficult to assess whether this is a function of being a freshman in each high school studied, a function of the environment, or a common occurrence that is caused by entering any new high school. Nevertheless, in this sample of students, motivation levels decreased at the end of the year. In looking at Variable 6, persistence, the AAST scores were significantly lower on the posttest than the pretest. RHS's scores were not significantly lower, although there are suggestions that the scores went down. Therefore, AAST students had a greater negative effect in the area of persistence in completing a task. In looking at Variable 7, responsibility, both AAST and RHS scored high in this variable at pretest time. However, at posttest time, AAST students scored significantly lower than RHS students on this variable. Looking at these three variables in the total sample of students, motivation, persistence, and responsibility significantly decreased at posttest time for AAST students, while only motivation significantly decreased at posttest for the RHS students. The decrease in motivation, persistence, and responsibility for AAST students could be a direct result of the cultural climate and academic environment within the school. The teachers use a project-based constructivist approach to learning. In student interviews, students responded that working with others on a team and unclear expectations and goals were stressful. Perhaps the very nature of constructivism with its unclear goals and outcomes, and an emphasis on competitive teamwork can have a negative effect on students' motivation, persistence, and responsibility. Variable 9, preference for working alone or with peers, shows no significant difference between groups or within groups for either AAST or RHS students. There is a suggestion that it increased for AAST students from pretest to posttest, but it is not a significant increase. The emphasis on teamwork at AAST would suggest that although students may find it stressful, they would also grow to enjoy and appreciate working with peers. No conclusive statement can be made with regard to this variable except that data suggest this may be so. In looking at Variable 21, parent motivation, there was significant difference between the two schools on the pretest, with RHS students being more parent motivated. There was no significant difference between the two schools on the posttest, with RHS students becoming less parent motivated. There was a significant difference from pre- to posttest scores for RHS. This could be a factor of students maturing from middle school youngsters, who are used to having parents intervene and help with school matters, to more mature young adults, who now take more responsibility for their own learning. AAST students travel from a home district to a distant county school, and they have been preselected as gifted in math and science. These students might already be more independent and less parent motivated. In looking at Variable 22, teacher motivation, the two schools showed very little difference on the pretest. Scores on the posttest showed that AAST students were much less teacher motivated than their RHS peers. This again could be construed as a negative effect of the constructivist, team-oriented approach in which students are "turned off" to teachers, or it could be construed as a positive effect in that AAST students are much more independent at the end of the year than their RHS peers. In looking at all six variables studied in-depth, AAST students' learning styles showed significant change in four of the six variables during the year of study, and RHS students showed significant changes in two of the six variables. This study's results suggest that the school environment can change a student's learning style. This result needs to be investigated further and brings into question whether learning styles are biologically and developmentally set or are, in fact, capable of being manipulated and affected by the external environment. This study also suggests that an environment that is actively engaged in many of the reform efforts promulgated in the literature--such as establishing a technology-rich school, using constructivist methods of instruction, employing project-based teams that solve problems, and discouraging the use of lecture--can have an even greater effect on student learning style. A major question to be explored is if the change is always in the direction that is desired or expected. Unintended outcomes of instituting major reforms in a school need to be examined. Table 1. Mean Scores for Pretest and Posttest Per School Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G H I J K L - Variable Variable Variable Variable Variable Variable Variable Variable Variable Variable Variable Variable 5 Motivation: Pre 5 Motivation: Post 5 Motivation: Pre 6 Persistence: Post 7 Responsibility: Pre 7 Responsibility: Post 9 Alone/Peers: Pre 9 Alone/Peers: Post 21 Parent Motivated: Pre 21 Parent Motivated: Post 22 Teacher Motivated: Pre 22 Teacher Motivated: Post A D G B E H C F I J K L 54.5 49.8 45.8 47.5 50.8 57.6 47.6 49.2 55.3 53.1 48.5 45.7 51.2 51.4 45.4 48.8 47.3 57.9 45.9 49.9 53.8 56.6 52.2 48.4 AAST RHS GRAPHS: Figure 1. Error barter the results of selected variables of the LSI pretest and posttest. References Brooks, J., & Brooks, M. (1993). The case far constructivist classrooms. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Cohen, V. (1997). Learning styles in a technology-rich environment. Journal of Research on Computing in Education, 29(4), 338-350. Dunn, R. (1990). Understanding the Dunn and Dunn learning styles modal and the need for individual diagnosis and prescription. Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities, 6, 223-247. Dunn, R., Beaudry, J., & Klavas, A. (1989). Survey of research on learning styles. Educational Leadership, 46(6), 50-58. Dunn, R., Dunn, K., & Price, G. (1989). Learning style inventory manual. Lawrence, KS: Price Systems. ~~~~~~~~ By Vicki L. Cohen, Fairleigh Dickinson University Dr. Vicki Cohen is an assistant professor in the School of Education at Fairleigh Dickinson University. Her areas of research are in literacy and technology in education. (Address: Dr. Vicki L. Cohen, School of Education, Fairleigh Dickinson University, NJ 07666; cohen@fdu.edu.) Copyright of Journal of Research on Computing in Education is the property of International Society for Technology in Education and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Journal of Research on Computing in Education, Summer2001, Vol. 33 Issue 4, p355, 12p, 1 chart, 1 graph. Item Number: 5078025 Result 1 of 2 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 64 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Temperament-based learning styles as moderators of academic achievement. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; ACADEMIC achievement Source: Adolescence, Spring97, Vol. 32 Issue 125, p131, 11p, 1 chart Author(s): Horton, Connie Burrows; Oakland, Thomas Abstract: Examines the hypothesis that students learn best when taught using strategies consistent with their temperament-based learning style. Definition of learning styles; Use of analyses of covariances in hypothesis examination; Examinations of temperament-based learning styles. AN: 9705295918 ISSN: 0001-8449 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] TEMPERAMENT-BASED LEARNING STYLES AS MODERATORS OF ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT ABSTRACT Considerable interest in applications of temperament theory has led to proposals of four temperament-related learning styles. The hypothesis that achievement is higher when instructional strategies utilize methods consistent with students' preferred learning styles was tested using 417 seventh graders, the majority of whom were from minority and low SES families. The hypothesis was not supported; instead, student achievement was significantly higher with instructional strategies designed to promote personalized learning. The need to extend temperament-based learning styles by considering additional qualities that are important to learning is discussed. Considerable research in education and psychology has been directed toward identifying the effects of individual differences in learning styles. Learning theorists generally agree that curriculum and instructional strategies should be adapted to these aptitudes. Learning styles have been defined as physiological, cognitive, and affective behaviors that serve as relatively stable indicators of how learners perceive, interact with, and respond to learning environments (Keefe, 1987). Thus, learning styles are thought to be stable and enduring personal qualities and not easily acquired (Derry & Murphy, 1986). As noted in Keefe's definition, literature of learning styles has centered on three main qualities thought to be critical: physiology (e.g., Das & Malloy, 1984; Eppele, 1989; Kane, 1984; Keefe, 1987; Levy, 1984; Millard & Nagle, 1986; Polce, 1987; Shannon & Rice, 1983; Sinatra, 1982; Webb, 1983), cognition (e.g., Bertini, 1986; Brennan, 1982; Das & Malloy, 1981; Goodenough, 1986; Kane, 1984; Keefe, 1987; Korchin, 1986; Messick, 1976; Polce, 1987; Witkin, Moore, Goodenough, & Cox, 1977), and affect (e.g., Carrol, 1963; Haring, 1985; Keefe, 1987). Several ways have been proposed that examine learning styles in terms of their conceptualized physiological, cognitive, and affective components. Research designed to study the efficacy of learning style applications generally consider relatively narrow components (e.g., field dependence) within the context of aptitude-treatment interactions (ATI). General support for ATI is lacking (Cronbach & Snow, 1977; Reynolds, 1981; Snider, 1990), and there are few empirically supported guidelines to assist in grouping students for instructional purposes. Moreover, a meta-analysis of studies on learning style applications reports little or no achievement gains when instruction methods match learning modalities (Kavale & Forness, 1987). Despite this somewhat pessimistic view, considerable interest remains in uncovering possible applications of learning styles defined in broader ways. Previous research can be criticized for conceptualizing these styles too narrowly, thus minimizing opportunities to test fully the effects of broader and more encompassing learning styles. Some believe temperament provides this broader perspective. Although the early contributions of Hippocrates and Galen often are cited, modern interest dates to Jung's writings (e.g., Psychological Types (1923). The popularization of temperament type by Myers and Briggs (Myers & McCaulley, 1985) has generated considerable interest among educators and psychologists. Myers and Briggs operationally define temperament through four dichotomous traits: extraversion (E) and introversion (I), sensing (S) and intuition (N), thinking (T) and feeling (F), and judging (J) and perceiving (P). Keirsey and Bates (1984) describe four basic temperaments that can be derived from the interaction of these types of traits, each temperament having its own primary or core value. SJ students primarily value belonging through providing service to others (e.g., they value following traditions and acting responsibly and conservatively). SPs primarily value personal freedom and spontaneity (e.g., to act on their impulses, to play, and to be free of constraints). NTs primarily value competence (e.g., a desire to learn, to know, to predict, and to control). NFs primarily value personal growth (e.g., to develop fully as individuals, to display authentic integrity, and to promote harmony). Golay (1982) and others (e.g., Lawrence, 1982) extended type and Keirseian temperament theory by describing prominent learning styles exhibited by students displaying these four temperament types. SJs were described as learning best when curricular materials were concrete and instruction well planned and routine (e.g., using repetition and drill through step-by-step instructions). SPs were thought to learn best through strategies that highlight variety, action, and entertainment. NT students were described as interested in developing theories and concepts and preferring strategies that promoted discovery and experimentation. NF students were thought to be interested in determining the relevance of learning to their personal lives and the lives of those important to them, and preferred strategies that emphasized cooperation and personalized applications of learning. Despite considerable interest in learning styles derived from temperament, few studies appear in quality refereed journals that examine the efficacy of these applications. The purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis that students learn best when taught using strategies that are consistent with their temperament-based learning style. METHOD Subjects Four hundred seventeen seventh graders enrolled in social studies classes in a large metropolitan district of approximately 65,000 students comprised the sample. Approximately 35% were Mexican-American, 23% were AfricanAmerican, and 42% were Caucasian; approximately 50% were from low-income families and qualified for the free lunch program. Instruments The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI), a 126-item forced choice questionnaire, was used to assess four dichotomous dimensions: Extraversion (E)-Introversion (I); Sensing (S)-Intuition (N); Thinking (T)Feeling (F); Judging (J)-Perceiving (P). The reliability coefficients of the MBTI for middle school students generally is in the high .70s while test-retest studies over 12 months found consistency on each scale also to be in the .70s (Myers & McCaulley, 1985). Temperament classification percentages of students in this study are: 7% NF, 17% NT, 49% SP, 27% SJ) - approximate national estimates as reported by Golay (1982) and Keirsey (1984). Criterion-referenced measures of Texas history also were employed to assess content acquisition from two instructional units. All pre- and post-tests were developed to assess the instructional goals as set forth in the teacher's edition of Texas, Our Texas and were derived from items contained in this volume. Procedure Four teachers received in service training on temperament and temperamentbased learning styles through readings and attending four dydactic training sessions. They also completed the MBTI and received information regarding the implications of their own temperament on their teaching and learning styles. Following training, each teacher was assigned to write lesson plans, along with the senior author, for one of four instructional strategies associated with temperament. The plans were designed to be consistent with instructional strategies and lesson plans that Golay (1982) and Keirsey and Bates (1984) describe for each of four temperaments: Sensing and Judging (SJ), Sensing and Perceiving (SP), Intuitive and Thinking (NT), and Intuitive and Feeling (NF). Instructional strategies important to each of the four types are described below. When possible, teachers were to develop lessons using the instructional strategy which matched their own temperament. The first set of lessons, a six-day unit on Texas explorers was based on Chapter 5 of the Texas, Our Texas social studies text. The second set of lessons, a seven-day unit on Texas colonization, was based on Chapter 9 of the text. SJ lessons were designed to encourage attention to detail, conformity, and obedience. Loss of structure or expectations of spontaneous participation were avoided. Teachers reinforced conventional thinking that was consistent with information presented in the text. SP lessons encouraged performance, playfulness, and fun, avoiding quiet seatwork or boring routines. Teachers using this strategy reinforced participation, involvement, and spontaneity. NT lessons were designed to encourage independent thinking, problem solving, and strategizing. Lessons avoided redundancies, inefficiencies, and an overemphasis on detail. Teachers using this instructional strategy reinforced competence as well as good ingenious ideas. NF lessons were designed to encourage cooperation, personal application, and identification with the historical characters. The lessons avoided competition and overemphasis on detail. Teachers reinforced unique or creative ideas, personal growth, and expression of personal experiences and feelings. The students' social studies grades for the preceding six-week grading period, prior to introducing Chapter 5, were collected. The first phase of the study began at the start of the second six-week grading period. Students were given a pretest on knowledge important to the Chapter 5 social studies lesson regarding Texas explorers. Following the six days of instruction, students were given a posttest on the material. Students remained in their assigned social studies classes and were taught using a single instructional strategy for the six-day unit. Different instructional strategies were used by the teachers for other classes during the school day. One teacher utilized each of the four instructional strategies in each of her four class periods. Two of the teachers utilized each of the four instructional strategies in four classes and repeated one instructional strategy in a fifth class. The fourth teacher had only two seventh grade social studies classes and used two different methods. This process was repeated during the second phase of the study which began at the start of the third six-week session and was based on Chapter 9 of the text. To ensure treatment integrity, the first author completed periodic observations of all four teachers, verifying that their instruction was consistent with the curricula developed for the study. RESULTS The purpose of this study was to determine whether students demonstrate higher levels of achievement when they received social studies instruction through a teaching style designed to match their temperament-based learning styles. Analyses of covariances, using pretests as covariates were used to examine the hypothesis. The type of instructional strategy used by the teachers significantly affected achievement among SJ students during both the first [F (3,106) = 15.53, p < .001] and second [F (3,112) = 4.44, p < .01] units of instruction, among SP students during both the first [F (3,196) = 21.28, p < .001] and second [F (3,102) = 5.95, p < .001] instructional units, among the NT students during the first [F (3,68 = 4.37, p < .01] but not the second [F (3,62) = .51, p = 68, n, s] instructional units, and among NF students during both the first [F (3,29) = 3.60, p < .05] and second [F (3,25) = 5.18, p < .01] units of instruction. Students exhibited significantly higher achievement when NF instructional strategies were used (see Table 1). Teacher Effects The study also explored possible teacher effects, which were examined through ANCOVA. Students' grades for the first six-week grading period and the pretest scores from each unit were used as covariates. Teacher was the independent variable and posttest score was the dependent variable. Results were significant in both the first [F (3,335) = 14.56, p < .001] and second [F (3,350) = 26.16, p < .001] units of instruction. The proportion of variance accounted for by teacher (9% for unit one and 13% for unit two) reveals that, while significant, the teacher effects did not account for a large proportion of the variance even as compared to covariates. Additionally, the implications of teacher effects on the primary hypothesis were minimal in that the teachers whose students demonstrated the highest level of achievement utilized all four instructional strategies. Moreover, all four teachers used the NF method. DISCUSSION This study tested assertions made by Keirsey and Golay about relationships between achievement and learning styles based on student temperament. The findings provide little empirical support for their theory that achievement is improved among students who receive instruction that utilizes teaching strategies which match their temperament-based learning styles. The results of the current study, combined with the lack of empirical support by Keirsey and Golay in their own work, together with the paucity of empirical investigation by others, weakens this assertion. The present results may be explained if one supports the position that temperament is only one personal attribute that influences achievement. Temperament theorists may have become too simplistic in viewing temperament as the basis of learning/teaching styles and have neglected to integrate other important schools of thought including learning and developmental theories, cultural concerns, and cognitive abilities. Learning style should not be the only factor considered in the design of instruction (Doyle & Rutherford, 1984). Other variables, including students' age and stage of development, must be considered (Gregorc, 1979). In the present study, the NF teaching strategy, designed to personalize learning, was superior in facilitating achievement among students of all four temperament types. Current temperament theory cannot fully explain these results. A simple univariable explanation may not be possible due to the multiple factors involved; however, the statistical and practical significance of this finding should not be ignored. Reasons why students learn more when taught with a personal approach may be attributable to many factors. Theories of learning and development as well as acknowledgment of cultural sensitivities may provide useful conceptual frameworks for understanding these findings. The personal teaching strategy employed a variety of techniques designed to enable students to relate to the lessons in personal ways. For example, students completed a visualization exercise in which they imagined having the experiences of an early Texas explorer, including the feelings, motivations, and sensory input the person may have experienced. Students also made diary entries in the first person as if they were a famous person during that historic period. In addition, class discussions focused on relating to the characters, imagining what it would have been like to have had their experiences and the ways students today are similar to those historical figures. Learning theorists also have argued that such personalized approaches can enhance achievement. Schema theory offers some important explanations of these results. "A schema is defined as an abstract data structure which consists of the concepts, relations (conceptual, temporal, and spatial), and related information that apply to a particular concept, event, or other data set" (Siebold, 1989, p. 53). Schema theory suggests that students' understanding of new material is dependent on previous experiences, the extent of their world knowledge, and the way in which these experiences interact with the explicit new information (Smith & Smith, 1986). Prior knowledge and experience with the instructional materials decidedly influence learning and achievement (Cooper, 1989). Personalized lessons used prior knowledge to help students develop schemata at two levels. Lectures and new materials were related to students' previous experiences. For example, students typically were asked to write about personal events in their lives. Thus, when asked to write diaries in the first person as an explorer, the concept was not completely new. In another class exercise, the personalities of historic characters were compared to those of movie stars with whom the students were familiar. Thus, new material was introduced in a way that enabled students to use their prior knowledge to further develop their schema. Further, the class exercises provided additional world experiences through visualization. These experiential activities had a memorable component. Thus, when questions were asked on the test, students may have been better able to draw on their prior knowledge and class experiences; students were likely to be better able to use the schemata they had developed prior to class, to add to that schemata through additional class experiences, and to draw on the more developed schemata when asked to recall information on the posttests. Developmental theory also may provide useful insights into the understanding of these results. Because of their personalizing qualities, the NF lessons may appeal strongly to adolescent narcissism. Since many adolescents are prone to egocentric thinking (Kimmel & Weiner, 1985) lessons which are personally focused may capture and hold their attention during this developmental stage and thus facilitate acquiring and retaining information. Thus, both age and stage of development are critical factors in considering learning styles (Gregorc, 1979). Finally, Trueba (1988) and Rameriz (1982) have underscored the need for more humane learning environments for minority students. As previously noted, the majority of students in this study were minority; half were from economically distressed homes. Personal, feeling-oriented lessons may provide more nurturing qualities which facilitate achievement in minority students. Lessons which encouraged them to relate the new material to personal experiences and feelings also may help those from diverse cultural backgrounds to sustain interest since they were able to relate their personal qualities, history, and backgrounds in ways that valued their diversity. Additionally, the exercises in the NF strategy (e.g., imagine being a Texas explorer or assume the role of an 1830s colonist) may provide a welcome respite for students from families experiencing financial and other stressors. In sum, while some temperament qualities may contribute importantly to how students learn, schema theory, developmental considerations, and cultural sensitivities also should be considered when developing lesson plans designed to optimally reach students. CONCLUSIONS This study provides one of the few empirical examinations of temperamentbased learning styles. While support was not found for using instructional strategies that match students' temperament-based learning styles, results did indicate that a strategy which capitalizes on personalization was superior for students of all types. Thus, it is clear that in addition to temperament, such factors as type of instruction, teachers, learning theory principles, developmental concerns, and cultural issues have an impact on achievement and attitudes. Temperament theorists are therefore encouraged to integrate, or at least acknowledge, these other schools of thought in their conceptualizations. Reprint requests to Connie Burrows Horton, Ph.D., Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Normal, IL 61790. Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations for Achievement Scores for Four Temperament Types by Four Instructional Strategies TEMPERAMENT-BASED INSTRUCTIONAL STRATEGIES Temperament (SJ) (SP) (NT) (NF) N X X SD SD X X SD SD p value SJ Unit 1 112 58 56 22 16 83 54 16 15 <.001 NF>SJ,SP,NT Unit 2 118 75 78 13 12 88 71 13 15 <.001 NF>SJ,SP,NT SP Unit 1 102 67 55 11 22 87 60 15 14 <.001 NF>SJ,SP.NT Unit 2 108 72 76 18 12 88 70 11 14 <.001 NF>SJ,SP,NT NT Unit 1 74 62 63 19 18 86 66 14 18 <.001 NF>SJ,SP,NT Unit 2 68 78 84 19 12 88 79 16 16 NS NF Unit 1 35 52 64 23 19 87 58 17 11 <.001 NF>SJ,SP,NT Unit 2 31 71 85 18 14 96 73 7 16 <.001 NF>SJ,SP,NT SJ: Sensing-Judging; SP: Sensing-Perceiving; NT: Intuitive-Thinking; NF: Intuitive-Feeling Unit 1 refers to the first phase of the study (Chapter 5 of the Texas, Our Texas text) which included 323 students. Unit 2 refers to the replication of the study (Chapter 9 of the Texas, Our Texas text) which included 335 students. Scores are based on a possible 100 and were rounded to the nearest whole numbers. REFERENCES Bertini, M. (1986). Some implications of field dependence for education. In M. Bertini, L. Pizzamiglio, & S. Wapner (Eds.), Field dependence in psychological theory, research, and application (pp. 93-106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brennan, P. (1982). Teaching children globally and analytically. Early Years, 12, 34-35. Carroll, J. B. (1963). A model of school learning. Teachers College Record, 64, 723-733. Cooper, H. M. (1989). Does reducing student-to-instructor ratio affect achievement? Educational Psychologist, 24, 79-98. Cronbach, L. J., & Snow (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook on interactions. New York: Irvington. Das, J.P., & Malloy, G. (1981). Of brain divisions and functions. Academic Therapy, 16, 349-358. Derry, S. J., & Murphy, D. A. (1986). Designing systems that train learning ability: From theory to practice. Review of Educational Research, 1, 1-39. Doyle, W., & Rutherford, B. (1984). Classroom research on matching learning and teaching styles. Theory into Practice, 23, 20--25. Eppele, R. (1989). Left brain/right brain: Research and learning. Bloomington, In ERIC Clearinghouse on Reading and Communication Skills (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 307 567). Golay, K. (1982). Learning patterns and temperament styles. Newport Beach, CA: Manas-Systems. Goodenough, D. R. (1986). History of the field dependence construct. In M. Bertini, L. Pizzamiglio, & S. Wapner (Eds.), Field dependence in psychological theory, research, and application (pp. 5-14). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gregorc, A. F. (1979). Learning/teaching styles: Potent forces behind them. Educational Leadership, 36, 234-237. Haring, E. (1985). Teaching and learning styles. Elgin Community College, Elgin, Indiana. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 258 658.) Kane, M. (1983). Cognitive styles of thinking and learning. Academic Therapy, 19, 527-536. Kane, M. (1984). Cognitive styles of thinking and learning. Academic Therapy, 20, 83-92. Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1987). Substance over style: Assessing the efficacy of modality testing and teaching. Exceptional Children, 54(3), 228-239. Keefe, J. W. (1987). Learning style theory and practice. Reston, VA: NASSP. Keirsey, D., & Bates, M. (1984). Please understand me. (Fourth ed.) Del Mar, CA: Prometheus Nemesis. Kimmel, D.C., & Weiner, I. B. (1985). Adolescence: A developmental transition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Korchin, S. (1986). Field dependence, personality theory, and clinical research. In M. Bertini, L. Pizzamiglio, & S. Wapner (Eds.), Field dependence in psychological theory, research, and application (pp. 45-56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lawrence, G. (1982). People types and tiger stripes: A practical guide to learning styles. (Second ed.). Gainsville, FL: Center for Applications of Psychological Type. Levy, J. (1984). What do brain scientists know about education? Learning Styles Network Newsletter, 3(3), 4-5. Messick, S. (1976). Individuality in learning. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Millard, D. E, & Nagle, S. J. (1986, March). Minds, brains and the language arts: A cautionary note. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Conference on College Composition and Communication, New Orleans, LA (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 283 221.) Myers, I. B., & McCauley, M. H. (1985). Manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, Ca: Consulting Psychologists. Polce, M. E. (1987). Children and learning styles. In A. Thomas, & J. Grimes (Eds), Children's needs: Psychological perspectives (pp. 325-334). Washington, DC: National Association of School Psychologists. Ramirez, M. (1982, March) Cognitive styles and cultural diversity. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 218 380.) Reynolds, C. R. (1981). Neuropsychological assessment and the habilitation of learning: Considering the search for the aptitude X treatment interaction. School Psychology Review, 10(3), 343-349. Shannon, M., & Rice, D. R. (1983). A comparison of hemispheric preference between high ability and low ability elementary children. Education Research Quarterly, 7(3), 7-15. Siebold, B. A. (1989). Effects of schemata and a concept organizer on cognitive learning and acquisition. Journal of Industrial Teacher Education, 26, 53-66. Sinatra, R. (1982). Brainprocessing: Where learning styles begin. Early Years, 12, 49-50. Smith, L. J., & Smith, D. L. (1986). Teaching teachers to transfer their knowledge. Journal of Reading, 29, 342-345. Snider, V. E. (1990). What we know about learning styles from research in special education. Educational Leadership, 48(2), 53. Trueba, H. T. (1988). Culturally based explanations of minority students' academic achievement. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 19, 270-283. Webb, G. M. (1983). Left/right brains, teammates in learning. Exceptional Children, 49(6), 508-515. Witkin, H. A., Moore, C. A., Goodenough, D. R., & Cox, P. W. (1977). Fielddependent and field-independent cognitive styles and their educational implications. Review of Educational Research, 47(1), 1-64. ~~~~~~~~ By Connie Burrows Horton and Thomas Oakland Thomas Oakland, Ph.D., Professor, Department of Educational Psychology. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Adolescence is the property of Libra Publishers Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Adolescence, Spring97, Vol. 32 Issue 125, p131, 11p, 1 chart. Item Number: 9705295918 Result 64 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 66 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Towards a categorisation of cognitive styles and learning sytles. Subject(s): COGNITIVE styles; LEARNING strategies; EDUCATION -- Evaluation Source: Educational Psychology, Mar-Jun97, Vol. 17 Issue 1/2, p5, 24p, 2 charts Author(s): Rayner, Stephen; Riding, Richard Abstract: Editorial. Discusses the origin and elaboration of learning style as a concept tracing the influence of a cognition and a learningcentered approach to the psychology of individual difference. Model of cognitive steel featuring the verbal-imagery cognitive dimension; Model of cognitive style integrating the wholist-analytic and verbal-imagery cognitive dimensions; Application of the learning style in educational practice. AN: 9706014954 ISSN: 0144-3410 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] Section: EDITORIAL ARTICLE TOWARDS A CATEGORISATION OF COGNITIVE STYLES AND LEARNING STYLES ABSTRACT This paper considers the construct, `style', in the study of individual differences and learning. The origin and elaboration of learning style as a concept is discussed, tracing the influence of a cognition and a learningcentred approach to the psychology of individual difference. The authors argue that a contemporary overview of style can contribute to a rationalisation of the theory and facilitate a greater application of learning style in educational practice. A case is made for the need to integrate more fully various models of style into a single construct of learning style. The concept `style' is used in a variety of contexts, in high street fashion, the sports arena, the arts, the media and in many academic disciplines including educational psychology. It has a wide appeal which reflects an enduring versatility, but this same appeal can lead to overuse and often creates a difficulty for definition and understanding. The concept is nevertheless always associated with individuality and is invariably used to describe an individual quality, form, activity or behaviour sustained over time. The term style may be used, for example, to describe the grace of a gymnast, or the game of a football team, the manner and cut of a new fashion on the modelling catwalk, the approach used by a commercial company to organise itself, or even the way a person may think, learn, talk or teach! The concept style represents a distinct notion of coherent singularity--in a variety of context--and might well reflect the need for a sense of identity which is arguably the essence of individuality. The `Style' Construct A `style construct' appears in a number of academic disciplines--in psychology it has been developed in a number of different areas, for example: personality, cognition, communication, motivation, perception, learning and behaviour. Its emergence as a theory entails both separate and related development, reflecting both a philosophical and psychological concern for individuality, but, as a result, reinforcing difficulty in definition or an accepted nomenclature. Several writers have provided an account of the origin of style in cognitive psychology. Martinsen (1994) cited Vernon (1973) when he claimed that antecedents of style can be traced back to classical Greek literature. Martinsen (1994) and Riding (this issue) referred to James' conception of individual differences contributing to the style construct James, 1890). Riding (this issue) also referred to the work of Galton (1883), but more significantly pointed to the work of Bartlett (1932), who continued with research on individual differences in cognition. Riding and Cheema (1991) and Grigerenko and Sternberg (1995) agree that Allport (1937), in work which developed the idea of `life-styles', was probably the first researcher to deliberately use the `style' construct in association with cognition. For a working definition of style, Riding and Cheema (1991), Miller (1987, 1991) and Riding (this issue) cited Tennant's (1988) description of cognitive style as a person's typical or habitual mode of problem solving, thinking, perceiving and remembering. Vernon (1963) provides an early critique of cognitive style, tracing its development from work carried out by German `Gestalt' psychologists. She explained that subsequent work on style flowed from a "considerable number of experiments ... devoted to studying individual differences in perception" (1963, p. 221). Vernon, generally, was critical of style development in the psychology of perception, pointing to a serious problem with the style construct, which many writers were subsequently to repeat. She commented that cognitive style had largely evolved from theories generalised on single experiments and little empirical evidence. One of the aims of this article is to consider such a deficiency. An Overview of Style Development Grigerenko and Sternberg (1995) described three distinct traditions of `style-based work in psychology'. The first is called the `cognitioncentred approach', the second the `personality-centred approach' and the third the `activity-centred approach'. Two of these `traditions' correspond to periods of active development in work on `style'. The first, occurring mostly in a 30-year period beginning in the 1940s, involved the development of `cognitive styles', which reflected the work of experimental psychologists investigating the area of individual differences in cognition and perception. The second began in the 1970s and involved the activitycentred theories of learning style associated with educationists addressing environmental and process-based issues related to meeting individual differences in the classroom. The latter is called the `learning-centred approach' in this discussion to emphasise the educational perspective shared by researchers in this tradition. The personality-centred approach is not considered in this review, partly because there is little evidence of this tradition influencing the general development of style-based theory and, secondly, there exists only the Myers-Briggs style model which clearly and significantly incorporates a personality-centred approach (Myers, 1978). (A) The Cognition-centred Approach An early interest in style amongst cognitive psychologists was given impetus, according to Grigerenko and Sternberg (1995, p. 207) by frustration with research on ability and intelligence which failed to "...elucidate the processes generating individual differences". Research carried out by various workers focused upon cognitive and perceptual functioning, resulting in the identification and description of several `abilities', `styles' and `dimensions' of cognitive processing or cognitive style. Key work in this cognition-centred approach is shown in Table I. The categorisation of the models described below have been made on the basis of an identification of fundamental dimensions of cognitive style which builds upon the work of Riding and Cheema (1991). They proposed an integration of `style' models into two cognitive style families: the Wholist-Analytic and the Verbaliser-Imager. The model of cognitive style proposed by Riding and Cheema (1991) and Riding (1991) argued that two fundamental dimensions of cognitive style structure the way in which people, firstly, process information and take the whole view or see things in parts (WholistAnalytic dimension); and, secondly, represent information or thinking either in pictures or words (Verbal-Imagery dimension). A third group of `learning style' models was identified, some of which were associated with a learning-centred approach to individual differences and considered to describe more properly models or aspects of learning strategies and so left outside the cognitive style construct (Riding & Cheema, 1991, p. 196). If we consider the cognition-centred tradition, it is possible to identify several models of cognitive functioning which appear to stand centrally in the development of a theory of cognitive style. They include the following areas of research and development and are organised into three groups: work which relates to the Wholist-Analytic style dimension; work which relates to the Verbal-Imager dimension of cognitive style; and, finally, more recently developed models which reflect a deliberate attempt to integrate both fundamental dimensions of cognitive style. Models of Cognitive Style Featuring the Wholist-Analytic Cognitive Dimension Perceptual-Functioning Workers led by Witkin and Asch (1948a; b) focused initially on perception, as they identified differences in individuals when locating an upright object in space. Their work reflected earlier research into perception completed by the Gestalt school of German psychology. Further experiments led to the discovery of field-independence and field-dependence as a perceptual style. The early rod and frame test used to measure field dependency was refined and converted into a pencil and paper assessment, the Embedded Figures Test (EFT). This development again reflected earlier work on the discrimination of shape carried out by Thurstone (1944). Assessment of field dependency was further developed to include the Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT). All three tests measured the ability of subjects to `dis-embed' a shape from its surrounding field. The theory was extended to involve a range of functions related to perception called psychological differentiation (Witkin et al., 1962; Witkin, 1964; Witkin et al., 1971; Witkin & Goodenough, 1981). Later studies focused on field dependency in children and learning (Witkin et al., 1977). Field-independent children were found to have a greater capacity than field-dependent children for `active analysis' and perceptual `differentiation'. They were more likely to prefer independent activity, have self-defined goals, respond to intrinsic reinforcement and prefer to structure or restructure their own learning. They were also most likely to develop their own learning strategies. Field-dependent children were found to have a preference for learning in groups, interact more frequently with peers or with the teacher, need higher levels of extrinsic reinforcement and direction, and stated performance goals or established structure in an activity. Numerous experiments replicating an exploration of this `style' of perceptual field response are reported in the literature. A typical example of such research is the use of field dependency as a basis for investigating the effect of matching or mismatching teachers and pupils with specific field-dependent or independent cognitive style (Saracho & Dayton, 1980; Renninger & Snyder, 1983; Saracho, 1991). Impulsivity-Reflectivity This dimension was originally introduced by Kagan and co-workers (Kagan et al., 1964) and measured by the Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT). This style dimension derived from earlier work investigating conceptual tempo which measured the rate at which an individual makes decisions under conditions of uncertainty. Learners fell into two distinct categories: the first were those who reached a decision quickly after a brief review of options and were labelled `cognitively impulsive'; the second were those who would deliberate before making a response, carefully consider all options and were labelled `cognitively reflective'. Implications for the teaching and learning process are immediately obvious, and Riding and Cheema (1991, p. 199) argued that this aspect of cognitive functioning holds for tasks involved in both academic and non-academic learning. Convergent-Divergent Thinking This dimension of the intellect was proposed by Guilford (1967). The dimension reflects a type of thinking and associated strategies for problem solving. The learner will typically attack a problem or task by `thinking' in a way which is either open-ended and exploratory, or close-ended and highly focused. The theory was further developed by Hudson and its implications for the process of teaching and learning explored (Hudson, 1966; 1968). This construct had significant impact upon teacher training throughout the 1970s. Holist-Serialist Thinking This label was introduced by Pask and Scott (1972) as two competencies which reflected an individual tendency to respond to a learning task either with a holistic strategy, which is `hypothesis-led', or a focused strategy which is characterised by a step-by-step process and is `data-led'. This work by Pask led to a development of `conversational theory', which emphasised the utility of the learner to `teach-back' learned material (Pask, 1976). The Style Delineator (Gregorc, 1982) Gregorc's learning style construct maintains that an individual learns through concrete experience and abstraction either randomly or sequentially. Gregorc identified four styles of learning: concrete sequential learners who prefer direct, step-by-step, orderly and sensorybased learning; concrete random learners who rely upon trial and error, intuitive and independent approaches to learning; abstract sequential learners who adopt an analytic, logical approach to learning and prefer verbal instruction; and abstract random learners who approach learning holistically, visually and prefer to learn information in an unstructured experiential way. This model, although placed in the cognition-centred approach because it is likely that Gregorc's construct reflects the Wholist-Analytic dimension of cognitive style, might arguably sit equally well in the learning-centred approach (Curry, 1987; Griggs, 1991). It is interesting to note, too, that Grigerenko and Sternberg (1995) prefer to describe this model as part of a personality-centred approach to style. The Style Delineator is a self-report measure made up of 40 words in which the respondent is asked to rank each time which word best describes their self-perception as a thinker and learner. The measure indicates the position an individual occupies in the `bi-dimensional channels' of "learning preferences for making sense of the world through the perception and ordering of incoming information" (Jonassen & Grabowski, 1993, p. 289). The Assimilator-Explorer (A-E) Cognitive Style (Kaufmann, 1989) Kaufmann's work flowed from an interest in problem solving and creativity. He identified two groups of problem-solvers, assimilators and explorers, and extrapolated an A-E theory of cognitive style to apply to problemsolving behaviour. Kaufmann developed an A-E Inventory, a 32-item forced choice self-reporting questionnaire, in which items described dispositions towards cognitive `novelty-seeking against familiarity-seeking'. Explorers reflected a higher score on the bi-polar continuum. The instrument was organised to reflect three factors: novelty against structure seeking, high against low ideational productivity and opposition against preference for structure. Martinsen (1994) has continued work in this area, specifically with respect to the relationship between cognitive style, insight and motivation in the process of problem solving. The Adaptor-Innovator Cognitive Style (Kirton, 1976; 1994) Kirton argued that style relates to the preferred cognitive strategies involved in personal response to change, and the strategies associated with creativity, problem solving and decision-making. A second key assumption made by Kirton was that these strategies were related to numerous aspects (traits) of personality that appear early in life and were particularly stable, like cognitive style. The dimension, Adaption-Innovation, was understood to exist early in an individual's cognitive development and to be `stable over both time and incident'. The adaptor, therefore, generally has a preference for `doing things better', while the innovator will tend to like `doing things differently'. A useful table, listing the characteristics of each style dimension, is given in Kirton, (1994, pp. 1011). Kirton's A-I theory, in summary, advanced a style construct which is bi-polar and consists of the adaptor-innovator continuum. The assessment instrument developed by Kirton to measure the adaptorinnovator continuum was the Kirton Adaptor-Innovator Inventory (KAI), a self-reporting inventory originally designed for adults with experience in the work-place and life. Kirton provides a summary of studies utilising factor analysis to support the reliability and validity of the instrument (1994, pp. 14-19), which, in turn, is corroborated by other writers (Clapp, 1993; Taylor, 1994; Van der Molen, 1994). The KAI produces a score which Kirton claims is used to identify an individual's preferred cognitive style, that is, as an adaptor or an innovator. The Cognitive Style Index (CSI) (Allinson & Hayes, 1996) The CSI is aimed at the "...generic intuition-analysis dimension of cognitive style" (Allinson & Hayes, 1996, p. 119). The authors have argued that utility of instrument is essential for the operationalisation of cognitive style in a professional context (in this instance, a business management context), and the CSI is designed to further research and development of style in management practice. While the CSI does not purport to produce a `full' measure of cognitive style, it is focused on a single universal dimension, which, Allinson and Hayes (1996) argue, reflects the duality of `human consciousness'--and problem-solving responses which are either intuitive or analytic. The CSI is a self-report questionnaire. It is relatively short and produces a score that reflects an individual's position on an analytic-intuitive continuum, which, the authors argue, reflects the super-ordinate dimension of cognitive style. The construction of the questionnaire is described in some detail by Allinson and Hayes (1996), as part of an attempt to identify a unitary construct of cognitive style and operationalise the same construct in the professional context of business management. A Model of Cognitive Style Featuring the Verbal-Imagery Cognitive Dimension Verbal-Visual Representation An interest in the mode or manner of thinking and knowing has involved a concern for imagery since early work by Galton (1883). Riding and Cheema (1991) described the early work of Bartlett (1932) and the development of Paivio's `dual-coding theory' as the basis for further work investigating the nature of a Verbaliser-Imagery dimension in the cognitive process (Paivio, 1971). Several assessment measures have subsequently been developed which incorporate this feature as a fundamental dimension of cognitive style (Riding & Taylor, 1976; Richardson, 1977; Riding & Calvey, 1981; Kirby et al., 1988; Riding, 1991). A Model of Cognitive Style Integrating the Wholist-Analytic and VerbalImagery Cognitive Dimensions Cognitive Styles Analysis (CSA) and Learning Style (Riding, 1991) Riding's work is dealt with more fully in Riding (1991; this issue) but it is worth noting that its development reflects a synthesis of previous work in cognitive style and it deliberately sets out to integrate fundamental elements of style theory in the development of a learning style model (see Riding & Cheema, 1991; Riding & Rayner, 1995). The Cognitive Styles Analysis is a computerised measure which reveals an individual's tendency to think visually or verbally and to process information wholistically or analytically (Riding, 1991; 1994). Summary Evaluation. The impact of the cognition-centred tradition has varied greatly and much of it attracted a great deal of criticism for a lack of rigour or reliability (Vernon 1963, 1973; Sternberg, 1987). Later commentators have repeated this criticism, questioning in particular the proliferation of style constructs and measures which occurred as part of this movement while offering little or no psychometric rigour (Freedman & Stumpf, 1980; Curry, 1987; Tiedemann, 1989; Grigerenko & Sternberg, 1995). What is significant in more recent work on cognitive style is the attempt to clarify a coherent theory of cognitive style (Curry, 1983, 1987; Miller, 1987; Riding & Cheema, 1991; Grigerenko & Sternberg, 1995). There is also evidence of a growing desire to apply the theory in a variety of professional context and this is reflected in the development of constructs tied to a specific measure forming a basis for its operationalisation. Indeed, it is perhaps the latter trend which led to the emergence of the learning-centred tradition of style theory. (B) The Learning-centred Approach This approach is arguably distinguished by three major features: the first, a greater interest in the impact of individual differences upon pedagogy; the second, the development of new constructs and concepts of learning style; and the third, the presentation of an assessment instrument as a foundation for the exposition of theory. Key work in this learning-centred approach is shown in Table II. It is organised into three style groups which reflect common features pointing to the measurement and conceptualisation of a particular dimension of the learning process. It is important to note at this point that the following models are regarded by the authors as key examples of constructs which might contribute to the clarification and development of learning style theory. The list is therefore not comprehensive and a wider review of learningcentred models of assessment may be found in Curry (1987). Secondly, workers in the learning-centred approach very often use the term `learning style', but this is in a strict sense different to the definition expressed by Tennant (1988) and adopted by other workers in the cognitioncentred approach (Riding & Cheema, 1991; Kirton, 1994). The categorisation of style groups is made on the basis of identifying shared features which may point to additional fundamental dimensions of `learning style' that may be integrated with those dimensions previously described in our review of the cognition-centred approach. Process-based Models of Learning Style Experiential Learning Style (Kolb, 1976; 1984) Kolb's learning style construct consists of two dimensions: perceiving and processing; the first describes concrete and abstract thinking; the second an active or reflective information-processing activity. These dimensions are integrated to form a model describing four types of learning style which are: --divergers: learners who typically perceive information concretely and process it reflectively, and who need to be personally engaged in the learning activity; --convergers: learners who perceive information abstractly and process it reflectively, and who need to follow detailed, sequential steps in thinking in a learning activity; --assimilators: learners who perceive information abstractly and process it actively, and who need to be involved in pragmatic problem solving in a learning activity; --accommodators: learners who perceive information concretely and process it actively, and who need to be involved in risk-taking, making changes experimentation and flexibility in a learning activity. Kolb's theory proposes that learning reflects the structure of a four-stage experiential learning cycle, which, in turn, involves the previously described aspects of learning style. The experiential cycle is used to extrapolate four adaptive learning modes: concrete experience (CE), reflective observation (RO), abstract conceptualisation (AC) and active experimentation (AE). Learning style is therefore construed as an individual's preferred method of `learning'. Interestingly, Kolb's model appears to presuppose a mix of `hard-' and `soft-wiring' in an individual's learning style, but lends greatest weight to the developmental nature of learning ability and style. The model therefore reflects a less stable set of individual differences, which can change over time. This is perhaps not surprising, given Kolb's primary interest in experiential learning and process-bound learning theory. The Learning Style Inventory is a nine-item self-reporting questionnaire which forces the respondent to rank four words, thereby revealing a specific preference in the identified modes of learning. Two scores are calculated, reflecting positions along each of the learning style dimensions: the first is the AC-CE continuum, which shows the degree to which the individual's style is biased toward abstraction or concreteness; the second continuum, RO-AE, shows the degree to which the individual's style is biased towards reflection or activity. Learning Styles (Honey & Mumford, 1986) Kolb's model has attracted considerable interest over the last two decades and has influenced the development of several `new' models of learning style. Honey and Mumford's model (1992) was representative of the work which replicated and attempted to apply Kolb's theory in a commercial context. The pencil and paper Learning Styles Questionnaire (Honey & Mumford, 1986) was devised to attempt a practical application of Kolb's theory in the management of the work-place. The questionnaire is used to explore the implications for management of a four-fold style model consisting of the following types of learner: activists, theorists, pragmatists and reflectors. Approaches to Learning Study (Entwhistle, 1979; 1981) Entwistle's work on style is a continuation of earlier work which looked at the processes of learning undertaken by the learner in a learning situation (Craik & Lockhart, 1972; Marton, 1976). These writers were initially interested in the duality of levels of processing in an approach to learning, which reflected either a surface or deep engagement with the task. The approach also reflects the thinking of Ausubel and Robinson (1966), who identified two principal types of learning process: rotemeaningful learning and passive-active learning. Entwistle attempted to link instructional preference to information processing and developed a model of learning style which consisted of four aspects: meaning orientation, reproducing orientation, achieving orientation and holistic orientation. As part of this model of learning style, Entwistle developed an integrated conception of the learning process, which described a series of learner actions linked to specific learning strategies identified in his original model. Thus, a student engaged in `reproductive learning', who is characterised by `extrinsic motivation', will adopt a style called `surface approach' and achieve a learning outcome which will consist of `surface level understanding' (Entwistle, 1979; 1981). Each of these stages or approaches reflect a range of cognitive control running from deep to surface `thinking' in the individual student. Further refinement of this approach attempted to describe learner orientation and identified specific style features which characterised the `learning interface'. The aim in this work was to provide formative assessment which teachers might use to enhance the pattern of study they require of students in their class (Ramsden, 1979; 1983). The Study Process (Biggs 1978; 1985) Biggs (1978) extended Entwistle's work to develop a new measure of learning strategy. He was interested in the motivation underlying an approach to learning. Curry (1987) describes these features as motive-strategy dimensions involving a `surface', `deep' and `achieving orientation'. Jonassen and Grabowski (1993) described this work as an extension to Entwistle's operationalisation of the holist-serialist theory of cognitive style, with previously identified surface and deep processing activities widened to include motivational factors, which are intrinsic, extrinsic and achievement orientation. Entwistle subsequently developed an empirical model of these processes identified as underlying serialist-holistversatile learning (Entwistle, 1981). Learning Processes (Schmeck et al. 1977) Schmeck and co-workers elaborated a theory of learning which rests upon the notion of quality in thinking. The quality of thinking, they argued, affects the distinctiveness, transferability and durability of memories that result from the learning event (Schmeck, 1988). They further developed this theory to produce a `style' construct which consisted of four subscales, comprising synthesis-analysis, elaborative processing, fact retention and study methods. Curry (1987) and Grigerenko and Sternberg (1995) have both commented on the close relationship between this model and the work of Entwistle (1979), Ramsden (1979) and Biggs (1985). Preference-based Models of Learning Style Learning Style (Dunn et al., 1989) Dunn and Dunn and Price (1989) defined learning style as the manner in which different elements from five basic stimuli affect an individual's ability to perceive, interact with and respond to the learning environment (Dunn et al., 1989). The `learning style' Dunn et al., present is a good example of a construct which more properly describes a learning repertoire rather than a style, and it is a repertoire chiefly made up of learning preferences. The learning style elements identified in this construct are: environmental stimulus (light, sound, temperature, design); emotional stimulus (structure, persistence, motivation, responsibility); sociological stimulus (pairs, peers, adults, self, group, varied); physical stimulus (perceptual strengths, including auditory, visual, tactile, kinaesthetic, mobility, intake, time of day--morning versus afternoon); and psychological stimulus (global/analytic, impulsive/reflective and cerebral dominance). A considerable number of studies have been carried out in the development of the Learning Styles Inventory, investigating and exploring the application of learning style to the school context (Griggs, 1991; Jonassen & Grabowski, 1993). The research has mostly taken the form of doctoral theses. The following are included as a representative sample: investigating the effectiveness of matching versus mismatching learning preferences on learning outcomes (De Bello, 1985; Gianitti, 1988); the identification of developmental patterns (Price et al., 1976; 1977); establishing relationships between variables (Brennan, 1984; Clark-Thayer, 1987; Bruno, 1988) and discriminating preferences between specific subpopulations (Bauer, 1987; Brunner & Majewski, 1990). The Learning Styles Inventory comprises a 104-item self-reporting questionnaire employing a three-choice Likert scale--true, false and unsure. There are several versions of this instrument aimed at the primary and secondary age-range. A third version, developed for use with adults, is called the Productivity Environmental Preference Survey (PEPS). Each version uses self-report methods to measure factors which reflect the key variables identified by the authors as forming an individual's response to the learning task. Each preference factor represents an independent continuum and is not necessarily related to other factors. Examples of factors for the environmental variable include: response to noise level, to light and temperature; for the sociological variable, preference for group learning, response to authority and typical response to adults; for the emotional factor, motivation, responsibility and persistence; for the physical factor, modality preferences, which include auditory, visual, tactile and kinaesthetic, as well as food/fluids intake and time of day. Individual and group profiles are produced from the assessment data and the authors provide guidance for planning style-led instructional method. Style of Learning Interaction (Riechmann & Grasha, 1974) The `style' of learning described by Riechmann and Grasha is very similar to the approach adopted by Dunn et al. (1989) in that it focuses upon an individual's learning preference. Riechmann and Grasha presented a social and affective perspective on patterns of preferred behaviour and attitude which underpin learning in an academic context. They identified three bipolar dimensions in a construct which described an individual's typical approach to the learning situation. These dimensions are: avoidantparticipant, competitive-collaborative and dependent/independent, which, as Jonassen and Grabowski (1993) explained, are related to three classroom dimensions: student attitudes towards learning; view of teachers and peers; and reaction to classroom procedure. Jonassen and Grabowski (1993, p. 281) describe this style construct as a "social interaction scale because it deals with patterns of preferred styles for interacting with teachers and fellow students...". The construct is measured by completing the Student Learning Styles Scale (SLSS), which is a 90-item self-report inventory consisting of six subscales reflecting dimensions of the learning `style'. A composite score is totalled and the respondent's position on the six aspects of this `style' is also recorded. It is worth noting that there are two forms of this measure: one to assess a general class, the second to assess individual style in a specific course. Riechmann and Grasha (1974) expect style to change in different classes and for a different subject. Cognitive Skills-based Models of Learning Style The Child Raring Form (Ramirez & Castenada, 1974) Ramirez and Castenada (1974) described learning style in terms of fielddependency or field-independency, and its interaction with cultural differences. The typical responses of individual students who demonstrated field-independence were identified as learners who often succeeded in the school context but who responded less favourably to social and holistic learning activity. Clearly, this model relates to Witkin's construct and the Wholist-Analytic style dimension, but significantly reflects the attempt to apply the cognition-centred model to the learning environment. The Child Rating Form was a direct observation form yielding frequency of behaviour scales completed by a teacher, or alternatively could be completed by a student as a self-report questionnaire. The results were used to identify style dimensions which relate to field sensitivity and sociological elements involving response to authority and peer orientation. The Edmonds Learning Style Identification Exercise (ELSIE) (Reinert, 1976) Reinert's model was aimed at the identification of an individual's natural `perceptual modality' as they respond to the learning environment. Reinert's work influenced both the development of the Dunn et al. (1989) model, as well as the work of Keefe (1987), in developing the NASSP Learning Style Profile (De Bello, 1990). The ELSE is composed of 50 one-word items which are used to characterise the respondent's reaction on four possible levels: visualisation or creation of a mental picture; alphabetical letters in writing from; sound; activity, that is an emotional or physical feeling about the word. The purpose of this assessment is to provide the teacher with information which will be used to work to the student's strengths or preferred mode of responding to learning stimuli. Cognitive Style Interest Inventory and Style Mapping (Hill, 1976) The work published by Hill while he was Principal at Oaklands Community College makes for fascinating reading (Hill, 1976). Hill's exploration of learning style was part of an ambitious attempt to organise an holistic, college-based approach to learning, which reflected principles of individualised education. The system was called Cognitive Style Mapping. Hill devised the construct Educational Cognitive Style to integrate learning style and curriculum design, as well as the teaching and learning process. Educational Cognitive Style was understood to be the product of an interaction between four variables: symbols and their meanings; cultural determinants; modalities of inference; and educational memory. The construct was used to develop a diagnostic instrument employed to create a personalised education for optimal learning. The Cognitive Style Interest Inventory consists of a three-point scale, self-report questionnaire, made up of 28 variables reflecting Hill's theory of cognitive style. The inventory was organised into three main sections covering symbols and their meaning, cultural determinants and modalities of difference. There are 216 items and the measure is scored by assigning the sum of ratings in each specific `theme'. Jonassen and Grabowski (1993) provide a full discussion of the Hill's theory, but point to the limited research evidence available to support the instrument. This is in spite of a great deal of work, mostly in the form of dissertation studies, conducted in the 1960s and 1970s, examining Hill's work. Perhaps somewhat surprisingly, Jonassen and Grabowski remain very positive about this instrument. Cognitive Style Delineators (Letter, 1980) Letteri described learning as an exercise in information-processing involving the storage and retrieval of information. The process of learning was categorised into six stages ranging from perception reception to longterm memory. Failure to process information in any one of these stages represents a deficit in cognitive skills acquisition. The teaching of cognitive skills, or `augmentation' as Reinert (1976) described the process of cognitive skills training, formed the basis for assessing and developing learning style and intellectual development. Letteri's style construct is significant for the presumption that assessment and style awareness should be used to change a student's cognitive profile and learning style. Letteri integrated the work of several models of cognitive style to create a combined assessment of individual skills on a bi-polar continuum. The assessment identified three types of learner: Type 1 were characterised by reflective, analytical dimensions of learning style; Type 3 were characterised by impulsive, global dimensions of style who were typically non-focused in their learning; Type 2 learners were identified as reflecting a central position in the continuum. The Learning Style Profile (Keefe & Monk, 1986) Keefe's learning style construct describes 24 key elements in learning style, which are grouped together into three areas: the first is `cognitive skills', which embraces information-processing activity, such as analytic, spatial, discrimination, categorisation, sequential processing, simultaneous processing and memory; the second is "perceptual responses", which encompasses perceptual responses to data, including visual, auditory and emotive processing; and the third is `study and instructional preference', which refers to motivational and environmental elements of style, including persistence orientation, verbal risk orientation, manipulative preference, time (early morning-late morning, afternoon, evening), verbal-spatial grouping, posture, mobility, sound, lighting and temperature preferences. The construct, and the rationale for its operationalisation, is based upon the premise that cognitive skills development is a prerequisite for effective learning. In this respect, the approach is very much concerned with `cognitive skills' and reflects an attempt to establish a learning to learn dimension in mainstream secondary schooling in the USA (Keefe & Monk, 1986). Keefe (1987) argued that if an individual cannot process information effectively, ineffective learning will take place, minimising the effect of a positive learning environment. Keefe has produced several monographs providing guidelines for teachers interested in developing programmes of work based on this model (Keefe, 1989; 1990). Summary Evaluation. The learning-centred tradition is by definition concerned with the learning process. This has led to models of style being developed which are `fluid', environmentally orientated and very susceptible to change. Criticism of the approach reflects concern for construct validity, poor verifiability and an uncertainty about the relationship between learning style, learning strategy and cognition. The research continues to be dominated by assessment and with a general approach heavily influenced by experimental psychology. This explains, in part, a prevailing psychometric paradigm in style theory, as well as a continuing focus upon measurement and experimental research design, and a lack of consensual theory. An attempt to integrate aspects or labels in the field of learning style is further discussed in the next section. Learning Style: theory into practice Models, Measures and Meaning A proliferation of models, terms and meaning in the field of learning style seems to increase with each period of new interest and research activity. Many writers have repeated earlier calls for a clarification in `style' terminology (Lewis, 1976; Messick et al., 1976; Curry, 1983; Miller, 1987; Riding & Cheema, 1991; Murray-Harvey, 1994). Curry, rather pointedly, identifies three areas of continuing concern for the operationalisation of learning style: "(1) confusion in definitions; (2) weaknesses in reliability and validity of measurement; (3) identification of the most style relevant characteristics in learners and instructional settings." (Curry, 1991, p. 248). Curry's interest in the `value' of style theory--that is, its application to the work of learning--is crucial to developing an educational perspective on `style'. A tangle of terminology and understanding contributes to this difficulty, which Curry points out is reinforced by the failure of style researchers, who, she explains, have: . . .not yet unequivocally established the reality, utility, reliability or validity of their concepts. Learning/cognitive styles may not exist other than as an insubstantial artefact of the person-environment interaction. Alternatively, learning styles may be real, stable, and potent enough to be useful to educational planners, particularly those with concern for truly individualised educational programming. (Curry, 1987, p. 16). As Curry (1987) rightly argued, these fundamental issues both express the continuing appeal of learning style for educationists, but also serve to challenge any systematic attempt to apply style-based theory in the school classroom. Learning Style: its relevance for education The idea that `style awareness' may help reach the `hard to teach', and perhaps contribute to reducing failure generally by enhancing the learning process, is an elusive but tantalising prospect which clearly merits further attention. The current interest in teaching and learning style is evident not only in schools, but also in higher education, work-place training and professional development. What remains apparently beyond reach is the systematic operationalisation of style in learning, teaching, training or management. Presland (1994) reaches a similar conclusion in his attempt to review and `operationalise' learning style for the continuing professional development of educational psychologists. He stated that there is little guidance for the practitioner interested in implementing style and suggested there is scope for a large research programme to explore the relevance of style in continuing professional development (Presland, 1994). The need, well documented in the literature, for clearer and well grounded development, suggests that putting theory into practice is overdue, should inform the continuing research into style and will need to involve a rationalisation of style as a construct in the psychology of learning. Is it Cognitive Style or Learning Style? Part of the attempt to clarify style theory and make better use of it in professional practice must involve resolving a definition of learning style. The organisation of contemporary `style' theory into a `three-nested model' forming an analogous `onion', devised by Curry (1983), represents a particularly useful effort at relating models in both the cognition- and learning-centred tradition. Curry suggested that an inner core of a `style onion' is made up of personality-centred models, leading to a second strata of informationprocessing models, and then to an outer layer of instructional-preference models of learning style. This model is used by Curry to review style work and consider further clarification of the terminology (Curry, 1987). The style onion usefully offers a model which emphasises the notion of an individual person's psychology and seeks to explain the formation of individual learning behaviour. The value of this effort should be acknowledged, but there is a need to take this further by refining the model. The basic dimensions of learning style, together with associated learning strategies, need to be more clearly identified to enable an elaboration of a personal learning style for the individual learner. As part of this task, contemporary cognitive and learning style models should be examined with an eye to distinguishing and integrating basic dimensions or features of learning style. It is not sufficient, for example, to suggest casually that learning strategies fit neatly into a style dimension, or that a style construct may be regarded as an `umbrella' concept of the kind initially described by Riding and Cheema (1991). The interrelationship between style, strategy and learning behaviour merits more attention, and the question of the exact nature of learning style, an answer. Fundamental Dimensions of Learning Style A way forward is perhaps most usefully summed up by Lewis. He stated, In my opinion, the right thing to do is to focus ... on the search for individual differences which are basic, in the sense that they underlie (and to that extent, explain), a whole range of more readily observable differences. (Lewis, 1976, p. 305). However, Grigerenko and Sternberg (1995) pointed out that several efforts have already been made, albeit unsuccessfully, to integrate the various aspects of style theory. Allinson and Hayes (1994, p. 60), more optimistically, described the work of several writers, including Kogan (1980), Messick (1976; 1984) and Miller (1987), who attempted to produce an integrated model consisting of `super-ordinate' dimensions of cognitive style. Messick's work laid an early foundation for such an approach (Messick, 1976; 1984), in which a typology of cognitive abilities, controls and styles were identified. Significant, in this work, was a distinction drawn between `style' and `strategy'. Messick originally argued for a distinction between Thurstone's work on perceptual domains and style (Thurstone, 1924) and cognitive style as a construct more deliberately used, stating that the former refers to mental abilities linked with intelligence, whereas cognitive `styles', in contract, cut across these domains. Messick et al. (1976) explained that cognitive styles . . . appear to serve as high level heuristics that organise lower-level strategies, operations and propensities--often including abilities--in such complex sequential processes as problem-solving and learning. (Messick et al. 1976; p. 9) Riding and Cheema (1991) take a similar approach in their grouping of dimensions identified in the cognition-centred approach of `style families', which has informed Riding's model of cognitive style (Riding, 1991; 1994; 1996). As previously argued, further work is required if the idea of learning style and learning strategy is to be clarified, so that a definition of learning style and the identification of the "most style relevant characteristics in learners and instructional settings" (Curry, 1987, p. 248) might be realised. There is a need to examine the more recently developed models of learning style with the aim of identifying and integrating additional dimensions of cognitive style as well as establishing linkage with a range of learning strategies which are related to these dimensions of learning style. It seems likely that many of the learning style models developed within the learning-centred approach might offer insights for the development of learning strategies. It is arguably useful to think in terms of cognitive style representing the core of an individual's learning style. The latter will, in turn, consist of a set of `super-ordinate' dimensions of a personal learning style. It is possible that two further aspects of learning might reveal additional dimensions of learning style. The first is the affective aspect of learning, the second is the motivational aspect of learning, forming a third and fourth super-ordinate dimension of learning style. The motivational aspect of learning is perhaps the least `fixed' and might well represent a `bridge' between a person's cognitive style and formation of learning strategy. Both the motivational and the affective dimensions are arguably reflected in several models of learning style associated with the learning-centred tradition and perhaps refer more closely to learning strategy development. There is an urgent need to investigate further the relationship between these models of `learning style' and the fundamental dimensions of an individual's personal learning style. Finally, several multidimensional style models described in this review carry components of all three levels of the learning style `onion', but a distinction is required between the `hard-wiring' of an individual's style and the `soft-wiring' of learning strategies which make up an individual's learning repertoire; and the conceptual validity, reliability and utility of these models must be examined before inclusion into a more fully developed style construct. The National Association of Secondary School Principals (NASSP) model (Keefe, 1989) is one example of an attempt, using profiling, at the educational operationalisation of learning `style'. There is, however, a need for further refinement and synthesis of the style construct as well as a `stream-lining' of the assessment content and methodology which makes the NASSP model rather unwieldy. Secondly, the theory underpinning such a model should be built around the super-ordinate dimensions of an integrated model of learning style. We suggest that Lewis (1976) is right and further development of a `style' model comprised of super-ordinate dimensions is the way forward. Further, if we are to make sense of style, find meaning in theory and realise the `operationalisation' of style in the educational system, the notion that `learning style' is an individual, stable and person-centred construct, needs re-emphasising, with a view to developing a profile for an individual reamer's learning style. This profile should be `basic', containing `primary' features of the individual's learning repertoire which will reflect cognitive style and learning preferences; it should be `manageable', `accessible' and `geared' to the `real' world of education and training; and it should be linked to an assessment procedure which is `user-friendly' for both the teacher and student. The suggestion is then that such a construct will ideally reflect a set of primary individual differences that include cognitive, behavioural and affective features combining to form the learner's learning style. The latter will represent a key consideration in curriculum design, assessmentbased teaching and differentiated learning. The student's role in learning will surely involve the formation and refinement of learning strategies which reflect their own particular learning style and the learning task. The teacher's role in learning must then surely be to incorporate an awareness of style in their approach to the task of teaching and learning. The final purpose of an assessment of learning style will be the enhancement of individuality in the process of teaching and learning. Correspondence: Stephen Rayner, Assessment Research Unit, School of Education, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, B15 2TT, UK. TABLE I. Descriptions and fundamental dimensions of cognitive style Label Description Key Dimension: Wholist-Analytic Constricted-flexible control tendency for distraction or resistance to interference. Broad-narrow preference for broad categories containing many items rather than narrow categories containing few items Analytical-non analytic a conceptual response which differentiates attributes or qualities conceptualising rather than a theme or total effect. Levelling-sharpening tendency to assimilate detail rapidly and lose detail or emphasise detail and changes in new information. Field-dependency/field independency individual dependency on a perceptual field when analysing a structure form which is part of the field. Impulsivity-reflectiveness tendency for quick as against a deliberate response. Cognitive-complexity A tendency for the multidimensional or simplicity or unidimensional processing of information. Automisation-restructuring Preference for simple repetitive tasks rather than re-structuring tasks. Converging-diverging Narrow, focused, logical, deductive thinking rather than broad, open-ended, associational thinking to solve problems. Serialist-holist The tendency to work through learning tasks or problem solving incrementally or globally and assimilate detail. Splitters-lumpers A response to information and interpretation which is either analytical and methodical or global. Adaptors-innovators Adaptors prefer conventional, established procedures and innovators restructuring or new perspectives in problem solving. Concrete concrete abstract abstract The learner learns through concrete experience and abstraction either randomly or sequentially. sequential random/ sequential/ random Reasoning-intuitive active-contemplative Preference for developing understanding through reasoning and/or by spontaneity or insight and learning activity which allows active participation or passive reflection. Key Dimension: Verbal-Imagery Abstract versus concrete Tolerance for unrealistic experiences preferred level and capacity of abstraction. Individual readiness to accept perceptual variance with conventional reality or `truth'. Verbaliser-visualiser The extent to which verbal or visual strategies are used when processing information Key Dimensions: Wholist-Analytic and Verbal-Imagery Analytic-Wholist and Verbal-Imager Tendency for the individual to process information in parts as a whole and think in words or pictures. Label References Key Dimension: Wholist-Analytic Constricted-flexible control Klein (1954) Broad-narrow Pettigrew (1958); Kogan and Wallach (1964) Analytical-non analytic Kaga et al. (1964); Messick and Kogan (1963) Levelling-sharpening Klein (1954); Gardner et al. (1959) Field-dependency/field independency Witkin and Asch (1948a, 1948b); Witkin (1961); Witkin (1971); Witkin et al. (1977); Impulsivity-reflectiveness Kagan et al. (1964); Kagan (1966) Cognitive-complexity Harvey et al. (1961); Gardner and Schoen (1962) Automisation-restructuring Tiedemann (1989) Converging-diverging Hudson (1966; 1968); Guilford (1967) Serialist-holist Pask and Scott (1972); Pask (1976) Splitters-lumpers Cohen (1967) Adaptors-innovators Kirton (1976; 1994) Concrete sequential Gregorc (1982) concrete random/ abstract sequential/ abstract random Reasoning-intuitive active-contemplative Allinson and Hayes (1996) Key Dimension: Verbal-Imagery Abstract versus concrete Tolerance for unrealistic experiences Harvey et al. (1961) Klein et al. (1962) Verbaliser-visualiser Paivio (1971); Riding and Taylor (1976); Richardson (1977) Key Dimensions: Wholist-Analytic and Verbal-Imagery Analytic-Wholist and Verbal-Imager Riding (1991; 1994); Riding and Cheema (1991); Riding and Rayner (1995) TABLE II. Models and fundamental features of learning style Model Description Key Feature: Process-based Meaning orientation/reproducing orientation/achieving orientation/holistic orientation An integration of instructional preference to information processing in the learner's approach to study. Surface-deep--achieving orientation/ intrinsic-extrinsic-achievement orientation An integration of approaches to study with motivational orientation. Synthesis-analysis/elaborative processing/fact retention/ study methods The quality of thinking which occurs during learning relate to the distinctiveness, transferability, and durability of memory and fact retention. Concrete experience/ reflective observation/ abstract conceptualisation/ active experimentation A two-dimensional model comprising perception (concrete/abstract thinking) and processing (active/ reflective information processing). Activist/theorist pragmatist/ reflects learners Preferred modes of learning which shapes an individual approach to learning. Key Feature: Preference-based Environmental/preference-based sociological/emotional/ The learner's response to key stimuli: environmental physical/psychological (light, heat); emotional (structure persistence, motivation); sociological (peers, pairs adults, self); physical (auditory, visual, tactile); psychological (global-analytic, impulsive-reflective). Participant-avoidant/preference based collaborative-competitive independent-dependent A social interaction measure which has been used to develop three bi-polar dimensions in a construct which describes a learner's typical approach to the learning situation. Key Feature: Cognitive-skills-based: Field-dependency/ cultural differences Learning style is defined in terms of field-dependency and cultural differences which produces `bicognitive' and `bicultural' behaviours. Visualisation/verbal symbols/ sounds/emotional feelings Learning style defined in terms of perceptual modality. Linguistic symbols/ cultural determinants/ modalities of interference education memory A model called cognitive style mapping was developed which integrated learning style and pedagogy. The construct was used to create a personalised education for optimal learning. Field dependency/ scanning-focusing/breadth of categorisation/cognitive complexity/reflective-impulsivity /reflective-impulsivity/ levelling-sharpening. total-intolerant. A cognitive profile of three types of learner reflecting their position in a bi-polar analytic-global continuum which reflects an individual's cognitive skills development. Cognitive skills/perceptual responses/study and instructional preferences Model Identifies 24 elements in a learning style construct grouped together into three dimensions. The model presupposes that cognitive skills development is a pre-requisite for effective learning. References Key Feature: Process-based Meaning orientation/reproducing orientation/achieving orientation/holistic Entwistle (1979) orientation Surface-deep--achieving orientation/ intrinsic-extrinsic-achievement orientation Biggs (1978; 1985) Synthesis-analysis/elaborative processing/fact retention/ study methods Schmeck et al. (1977) Concrete experience/ reflective observation/ abstract conceptualisation/ active experimentation Kolb (1976) Activist/theorist pragmatist/ reflects learners Honey and Mumford (1986) Key Feature: Preference-based Environmental/preference-based sociological/emotional/ physical/psychological Price et al. (1976; 1977); Dunn et al. (1989) Participant-avoidant/preference based collaborative-competitive independent-dependent Riechmann-Grasha (1974) Key Feature: Cognitive-skills-based: Field-dependency/ cultural differences Ramirez and Castenada (1974) Visualisation/verbal symbols/ sounds/emotional feelings Reinert (1976) Linguistic symbols/ cultural determinants/ modalities of interference education memory Hill (1976) Field dependency/ scanning-focusing/breadth of categorisation/cognitive complexity/reflective-impulsivity /reflective-impulsivity/ levelling-sharpening. total-intolerant. Letteri (1980) Cognitive skills/perceptual responses/study and instructional preference REFERENCES Keefe and Monk (1986); Keefe (1989; 1990) ALLINSON, J. & HAYES, C. (1994) Cognitive style and its relevance for management practice, British Journal of Management, 5, pp. 53-71. ALLINSON, J. & HAYES, C. (1996) The Cognitive Style Index, a measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research, Journal of Management Studies, Vol. 33, No. 1, pp. 119-135. ALLPORT, G.W. (1937) Personality: a psychological interpretation (New York, Holt & Co.). AUSUBEL, D.P. & ROBINSON, F.G. (1966) School Learning: an introduction to educational psychology (New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston). BARTLETT, F.C. (1932) Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology (Cambridge, Cambridge University Press). BAUER, E. (1987) Learning style and the learning disabled, experimentation with ninth graders. The Clearing House (Washington DC, Heldref Publications, Inc.), Vol. 60, No. 5, pp. 206-208. BIGGS, J.B. (1978) Individual and group differences in study processes, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 48, pp. 266-279. BIGGS, J.B. (1985) The role of metalearning in study processes, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, pp. 185-212. BRENNAN, P. (1984) An analysis of the relationships among hemispheric preference and analytic/global cognitive style, two elements of learning style, method of instruction, gender, and mathematics achievement of tenth grade geometry students, Doctoral dissertation, St. John's University, Jamaica: New York, Dissertation Abstracts International, 45/11, 3271A. BRUNNER, C.E. & MAJEWSKI, W.S. (1990) Mildly handicapped students can succeed with learning styles, Educational Leadership, October, pp. 21-23. BRUNO, J. (1988) An experimental investigation of the relationships between and among hemispheric processing, learning style preferences, instructional strategies, academic achievement, and attitudes of developmental mathematics in students in an urban technical college, Doctoral dissertation, St. John's University, Dissertation Abstracts International, 49, 1066A. CLAPP, R.G. (1993) The stability of cognitive style in adults, a longitudinal study of the KAI, Psychological Reports, 73, pp. 1235-1245. CLARK-THAYER, S. (1987) The relationship of the knowledge of studentperceived learning style preferences, and study habits and attitudes to achievement of college freshmen in a small urban university, Doctoral dissertation, Boston University, Annotated Bibliography, 1991 (New York, The Learning Styles Network). COHEN, R.A. (1967) Primary group structure, conceptual styles and school achievement, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh. CRAIK, F.I.M. & LOCKHART, R.S. (1972) Levels of processing: a framework for memory research, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 11, pp. 671-684. CURRY, L. (1983) An organisation of learning styles theory and constructs, ERIC Document, 235 185. CURRY, L. (1987) Integrating Concepts of Cognitive or Learning Style, a review with attention to psychometric standards (Ottawa, ON, Canadian College of Health Service Executives). CURRY, L. (1991) Patterns of learning style across selected medical specialities, Educational Psychology, 11, pp. 247-278. DEBELLO, T.C. (1985) A critical analysis of the effects on achievement and attitudes of administrative assignments to social studies instruction based on individual, eighth grade students' sociological preferences for learning alone, with peers, or with teachers, Doctoral Dissertation, St. John's University, Annotated Bibliography, 1991 (New York, The Learning Styles Network). DEBELLO, T.C. (1985) Comparison of eleven major learning styles models: variables, appropriate populations, validity of instrumentation, and the research behind them, Reading, Writing and Learning Disabilities, 6, pp. 203-222. DUNN, R., DUNN, K. & PRICE, G.E. (1989) Learning Styles Inventory (Lawrence, KS, Price Systems). ENTWISTLE, N.J. (1979) Motivation, styles of learning and the academic environment. Edinburgh, The University of Edinburgh, ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 190 636. ENTWISTLE, N.J. (1981) Styles of Teaching and Learning: an integrated outline of educational psychology for students, teachers and lecturers (Chichester, Wiley). FREEDMAN, R.D. & STUMPF, S.A. (1980) Learning style theory, less than meets the eye, Academy of Management Review, 5, pp. 445-447. GALTON, F. (1883) Inquiries into Human Faculty and its Development (London, MacMillan). GARDNER, R.W., HOLZMAN, P., KLEIN, G., LINTON, H. & SPENCE, D. (1959) Cognitive Control, a study of individual differences in cognitive behaviour, Psychological Issues, 1, Monograph 4. GARDNER, R.W. & SCHOEN, R.A. (1962) Differentiation and abstraction in concept formation, Psychological Monographs, 76. GIANITTI, M.C. (1988) An experimental investigation of the relationships among the learning style sociological preferences of middle school students (grades 6, 7, 8), their attitudes and achievement in social studies, and selected instructional techniques. Doctoral dissertation, St. John's University, Annotated Bibliography, 1991 (New York, The Learning Styles Network). GREGORC, A.R. (1982) Style Delineator (Maynard, MA, Gabriel Systems). GRIGERENKO, E.L. & STERNBERG, R.J. (1995) Thinking styles, in: D.H. SAKLOFSKE & M. ZEIDNER (Eds) International Handbook of Personality and Intelligence (New York, Plenum Press), pp. 205-230. GRIGGS, S.A. (1991) Learning Styles Counseling (Ann Arbor, MI, ERIC Counselling and Personnel Services Clearinghouse, The University of Michigan). GUILFORD, J.P. (1967) The Nature of Human Intelligence (New York, McGrawHill). HARVEY, O.J., HUNT, D.E. & SCHRODER, H.M. (1961) Conceptual Systems and Personality Organisation (New York, Wiley). HONEY, P. & MUMFORD, A. (1986) Using Your Learning Styles (Maidenhead, Berkshire, Peter Honey). HONEY, P. & MUMFORD, A. (1992) The Manual of Learning Styles, Revised Version (Maidenhead, Berkshire, Peter Honey). HILL, J.E. (1976) The Educational Sciences (Bloomfield Hills, ML, Oakland Community College Press). HUDSON, L. (1966) Contrary Imaginations (Methuen, London). HUDSON, L. (1968) Frames of Mind (Harmondsworth, Penguin Books). JAMES, W. (1890) The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 2 (London, MacMillan). JONASSEN, D.H. & GRABOWSKI, B.L. (1993) Handbook of Individual Differences, Learning and Instruction (Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates). KAGAN, J. (1966) Developmental studies in reflection and analysis, in: A. H. KIDD & J.L. RIVOIRE (Eds) Perceptual Development in Children (New York, International University Press). KAGAN, J., ROSMAN, B.L., DAY, D., ALBERT, J. & PHILIPS, W. (1964) Information processing in the child: significance of analytic and reflective attitudes, Psychological Monographs, 78, (1). KAUFMANN, G. (1989) The Assimilator--Explorer Inventory, in: O. MARTINSEN, Cognitive Style and Insight, Ph.D. thesis, Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen, Norway. KEEFE, J.W. (1987) Learning Style: theory and practice (Reston, VA, National Association of Secondary School Principals). KEEFE, J.W. (1989) Learning Style Profile Handbook: Vol. 11, accommodating perceptual study and instructional preferences (Reston, VA, NASSP). KEEFE, J.W. (1990) Learning Style Profile Handbook: Vol. 1, developing cognitive skills (Reston, VA, NASSP). KEEFE, J.W. & MONK, J.S. (1986) Learning Styles Profile Examiner's Manual (Reston, VA, NASSP). KIRBY, J., MOORE, P. & SCHOFIELD, N. (1988) Verbal and visual learning styles, Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13, pp. 169-184. KIRTON, M.J. (1994) (Ed.) Adaptors and Innovators, 2nd Edn (London, Routledge). KIRTON, M.J. (1976) Adaptors and innovators, a description and measure, Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, pp. 622-629. KLEIN, G.S. (1954) Need and regulation, in: M.P. JONES (Ed.) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Lincoln, NB, University of Nebraska Press). KLEIN, G.S., RILEY, W.G. & SCHLESINGER, H.J. (1962) Tolerance for unrealistic experience: a study of the generality of cognitive control, British Journal of Psychology, 54, pp. 41-55. KOGAN, N. (1980) Cognitive styles and reading performance, Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, pp. 63-78. KOGAN, N. & WALLACH, M.A. (1964) Risk Taking (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston). KOLB, D.A. (1976) Learning Style Inventory: technical manual (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall). KOLB, D.A. (1984) Experiential Learning: experience as a source of learning and development (Englewood Cliffs, NJ., Prentice-Hall). LETTERI, C.A. (1980) Cognitive profile: basic determinant of academic achievement, Journal of Educational Research, 73, pp. 195-199. LEWIS, B.N. (1976) Avoidance of aptitude-treatment trivialities, in: S. MESSICK (Ed.) Individuality in Learning (San Francisco, CA, Jossey-Bass). MARTINSEN, O. (1994) Cognitive style and insight, Ph.D. thesis, Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen, Norway. MARTON, F. (1976) What does it take to learn? Some implications on an alternative view of learning, in: N.J. ENTWHISTLE (Ed.) Strategies for Research and Development in Higher Education, pp. 200-222 (Amsterdam, Swets and Zeitlenger). MESSICK, S. (1976) Personality consistencies in cognition and creativity, in: S. MESSICK and ASSOCIATES (Eds) Individuality in Learning: implications of cognitive styles and creativity for human development, pp. 4-22, (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass). MESSICK, S. (1984) The nature of cognitive styles: problems and promise in educational practice, Educational Psychologist, 19, pp. 59-74. MESSICK, S. & KOGAN, N. (1963) Differentiation and compartmentalisation in object-sorting measures of categorising style, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 16, pp. 47-51. MILLER, A. (1987) Cognitive styles, an integrated model, Educational Psychology, 7, pp. 251-268. MILLER, A. (1991) Personality types, learning styles and educational goals, Educational Psychology, 11, pp. 217-238. MURRAY-HARVEY, R. (1994) Learning styles and approaches to learning, distinguishing between concepts and instruments, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, pp. 373-388. MYERS, I. (1978) Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (Palo Alto, CA, Consulting Psychologists' Press). PAIVIO, A. (1971) Styles and strategies of learning, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, pp. 128-148. PASK, G. (1976) Styles and strategies of learning, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, pp. 128-148. PASK, G. & SCOTT, B.C.E. (1972) Learning strategies and individual competence, International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 4, pp. 217-253. PEETTIGREW, T.G. (1958) The measurement and correlators of category width as a cognitive variable, Journal of Personality, 26, pp. 532-544. PRESLAND, J. (1994) Learning styles and continuous professional development, Educational Psychology in Practice, 10, pp. 179-184. PRICE, C.E., DUNN, R. & DUNN, K. (1976, 1977) Learning Style Inventory Research Report (Lawrence, KS, Price Systems). RAMIREZ, A.I. & CASTENADA, A. (1974) Cultural Democracy: bicognitive development and education (New York, Academic Press). RAMSDEN, P. (1979) Student learning and perceptions of the academic environment, Higher Education, 8, pp. 411-427. RAMSDEN, P. (1983) Context and strategy, situational influences on learning, in: R.R. SCHMECK (Ed.) Learning Strategies and Learning Styles (New York, Plenum Press). REINERT, H. (1976) One picture is worth a thousand words? Not necessarily!, The Modern Language Journal, 60, :pp. 160-168. RENNINGER, K.A. & SNYDER, S.S. (1983) Effects of cognitive style on perceived satisfaction and performance among students and teachers, Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 668-676. RICHARDSON, A. (1977) Verbalizer-Visualizer: a cognitive style dimension, Journal of Mental Imagery, 1, pp. 109-125. RIECHMANN, S. W. & GRASHA, A.F. (1974) A rational approach to developing and assessing the validity of a student learning styles instrument, Journal of Psychology, 87, pp. 213-223. RIDING, RJ. (1991) Cognitive Styles Analysis (Birmingham, Learning and Training Technology). RIDING, R.J. (1994) Personal Style Awareness and Personal Development (Birmingham, Learning and Training Technology). RIDING, R.J. (this issue) On the nature of cognitive style, Educational Psychology, 17. RIDING. R.J. & CALVEY, I. (1981) The assessment of Verbal-Imagery learning styles and their effect on the recall of concrete and abstract prose passages by eleven year old children, British Journal of Psychology, 72, pp. 59-64. RIDING, R.J. & CHEEMA, I. (1991) Cognitive styles--an overview and integration, Educational Psychology, 11, pp. 193-215. RIDING, R.J. & RAYNER, S. (1995) Personal Style and Effective Teaching (Birmingham, Learning and Training Technology). RIDING, R. J. & TAYLOR, E.M. (1976) Imagery performance and prose comprehension in 7 year old children, Educational Studies, 2, pp. 21-27. SARACHO, O. N. (1991) Students' preference for field-dependenceindependence teacher characteristics, Educational Psychology, 11, pp. 323332. SARACHO, O.N. & DAYTON, C.M. (1980) The relationship of teachers' cognitive styles to pupils' academic achievement gains, Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, pp. 544-549. SCHMECK, R.R. (Ed.) (1988) Strategies and Styles of Learning (New York, Plenum Press). SCHMECK, R.R., RIBICH, F.D. & RAMANAIAH, H. (1977) Development of a selfreport inventory for assessing individual differences in learning processes, Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, pp. 413-431. STERNBERG, R.J. (1987) Intelligence and cognitive style, in: O. MARTINSEN (1994) Cognitive style and insight, Ph.D. thesis, Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen, Norway. TAYLOR, J. (1994) The stability of school-children's cognitive style--a longitudinal study of the KAI Inventory, Psychological Reports, 74, 10081010. TENNANT, M. (1988) Psychology and Adult Learning (London, Routledge). TIEDEMANN, J. (1989) Measures of cognitive styles, a critical review, Educational Psychology, 24, pp. 261-265. THURSTONE, J. (1944) A factorial study of perception, Psychological Monographs, No. 4. THURSTONE, T.H. (1924) The Nature of Intelligence (London, Harcoat Brace). VAN DER MOLEN, P.P. (1994) Adaption-innovation and changes in social structure on the anatomy of catastrophe, in: M. KIRTON (Ed.) Adaptors and Innovators, styles of creativity and problem-solving, 2nd Edn (London, Routledge), pp. 137-172. VERNON, M.D. (1963) The Psychology of Perception (Harmondsworth, Penguin Books). VERNON, P.E. (1973) Multivariate approaches to the study of cognitive styles, in: J.R. ROYCE (Ed.), Multivariate Analysis and Psychological Theory (London, Academic Press), pp. 125-148. WITKIN, H.A. (1964) Origins of cognitive style, in: C. SHEERER (Ed.) Cognition, theory, research, promise (New York, Harper and Row). WITKIN, H.A. & ASCH, S.E. (1948a) Studies in space orientation, III. Perception of the upright in the absence of visual field, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, pp. 603-614. WITKIN, H.A. & ASCH, S.E. (1948b) Studies in space orientation, IV. Further experiments on perception of the upright with displaced visual field, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, pp. 762-782. WITKIN, H.A., DYK, R.B., FATERSON, H.F., GOODENOUGH, D.R. & KARP, S.A. (1962) Psychological Differentiation (New York, Wiley). WITKIN, H.A., MOORE, C., GOODENOUGH D. & Cox, P. (1977) Field-dependent and field-independent cognitive styles and their educational implications, Review of Educational Research, 47, pp. 1-64. WITKIN, H.A., OLTMAN, P., RASKIN, E. & KARP, S. (1971) A Manual for Embedded Figures Test (Palo Alto, CA, Consulting Psychologists Press). WITKIN, H.A. & GOODENOUGH, D. (1981) Cognitive Styles, essence and origins: field dependence and field independence (New York, International Universities Press). ~~~~~~~~ By STEPHEN RAYNER & RICHARD RIDING, Assessment Research Unit, University of Birmingham, UK ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Educational Psychology is the property of Carfax Publishing Company and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Educational Psychology, Mar-Jun97, Vol. 17 Issue 1/2, p5, 24p, 2 charts. Item Number: 9706014954 Result 66 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 67 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: `Learning style': Frameworks and instruments. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; EDUCATION -- Evaluation Source: Educational Psychology, Mar-Jun97, Vol. 17 Issue 1/2, p51, 13p, 6 charts Author(s): Sadler-Smith, Eugene Abstract: Investigates several aspects of learning styles and explore their interrelationship. Four broad categories of learning style; Definition of learning preferences, learning style and cognitive style; Relationships between preferences, styles and approaches; Methods used in the study; How the study indicates some overlap between the dimension measured by the Learning Styles Questionnaire and the Revised Approaches to Studying Inventory. AN: 9706014958 ISSN: 0144-3410 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] `LEARNING STYLE': FRAMEWORKS AND INSTRUMENTS ABSTRACT In both education and training an important aspect of the design, development and delivery of learning is the role of individual differences between learners in terms of their `learning styles'. One may identify four broad categories of what have been termed `learning style': (i) `cognitive personality elements' (e.g Witkin et al. 1977; Riding, 1991); (ii) `information-processing style' (e.g Kolb, 1984; Honey & Mumford, 1992); (iii) `approaches to studying' (e.g Entwistle & Tait, 1994); (iv) `instructional preferences' (e.g Riechmann & Grasha, 1974). A study of 245 university undergraduates in business studies aimed to: (i) describe the range of individual differences present within the sample; (ii) investigate the relationship between learners' cognitive styles, learning styles, approaches to studying and learning preferences; (iii) consider the implications of `learning style' for teaching and learning in higher education. The present study suggested some overlap between the dimensions measured by the Learning Styles Questionnaire (Honey & Mumford, 1986; 1992) and the Revised Approaches to Studying Inventory (Entwistle &Tait, 1994). No statistically significant correlations were found between cognitive style, as measured by the Cognitive Styles Analysis (Riding, 1991) and any of the other `style' constructs used. Further research is required to investigate these relationships, as is a large-scale factor analytical study of the Honey and Mumford and Kolb instruments. The notions of whole brain functioning, integrative approaches to studying and degree of learning activity are discussed. Gorham (1986) and Curry (1983) identified three broad categories of `learning style': (i) `cognitive personality elements' such as field dependence and independence (e.g. Witkin et al., 1977); (ii) `informationprocessing style', such as Kolb's model of the experiential learning cycle (Kolb, 1984) and the associated learning styles (converger, diverger, accommodator, assimilator) or the related learning styles suggested by Honey and Mumford (activist style, reflector style, theorist style and pragmatist style; Honey & Mumford, 1992); (iii) `instructional preferences', such as those measured by inventories like the Grasha- Riechmann Student Learning Styles Scales (Riechmann & Grasha, 1974). One may add to this the notion of `approaches to studying' (Marton & Saljo, 1976), which, in terms of function and process, may lie somewhere in between `cognitive personality elements' and `instructional (i.e. learning) preferences'. This array of individual difference constructs suggests a multidimensional, as opposed to bipolar, model of `learning style' (see Murray-Harvey, 1994, p. 374). In order to acknowledge and accommodate this range of aspects of individual difference in an holistic way, cognisance should be taken of learning preferences, learning styles, approaches to studying and cognitive styles (Sadler-Smith, 1996a). The present study aimed to investigate each of these aspects of `learning style' and explore their interrelationships. Learning Preferences Learning preferences may be defined as an individual's propensity to choose or express a liking for a particular instructional technique or combination of techniques. Riechmann and Grasha (1974) identified three learning preference styles or types: (i) dependent learners: prefer teacherdirected, highly structured programmes with explicit assignments set and assessed by the teacher; (ii) collaborative learners: discussion-orientated and favour group projects, collaborative assignments and social interaction; (iii) independent learners: prefer to exercise an influence on the content and structure of learning programmes within which the teacher or instructor is a resource. For the purposes of the present study, a simple paper and pencil inventory of learning methods was developed. The inventory is contained at Appendix 1. Factor analysis of the inventory items (principal components with Varimax rotation) suggested three learning method preference factors: (i) autonomous methods (open/distance/flexible learning, computer-assisted learning); (ii) collaborative methods (roleplay, discussion groups, business games); (iii) dependent methods (lecture, tutorial/surgery). These conceptual grouping corresponded closely to the notions of independence, collaboration and dependence used by Riechmann and Grasha. The individual inventory items were derived from discussions with students and staff at the institution concerned. Learning Style The work of Kolb (1984) in the USA and Honey and Mumford (1986; 1992) in the UK represent two widely used approaches to the identification of `learning style', both of which take as their basis Kolb's model of experiential learning. Kolb (1984) Kolb's model of experiential learning describes learning in terms of processes rather than outcomes and is conceived of as four distinct stages thus (Kolb, 1984, p. 21): [learning is] seen best to be facilitated by an integrated process that begins with here-and-now experiences followed by collections of data and observations about that experience. The data are then analysed and the conclusions of the analysis are fed back to the actors in their experience for their use in the modification of their behaviour and choice of new experiences. On the basis of this model, Kolb argued that learning is a four-stage process consisting of concrete experience, observation and reflection, formation of abstract concepts and generalisations, and the testing of the implications of these concepts in new situations, thus leading to further concrete experiences. An `ideal' learner has the ability to operate with equal facility at all four stages. Such ideal learners are considered rare and most individuals have a preference for one or more stages in the cycle. Kolb suggested that an individual's learning style may be identified by assessing her or his position on each of two bipolar dimensions using a self-report type inventory (the Learning Styles Inventory, Kolb, 1976 [revised 1985]; Kolb et al., 1995). It comprises lists of words (e.g. `analytical', `logical', `receptive', `feeling', `intuitive', etc.) which respondents are required to rank according to how they feel the words best describe their learning style (see Tennant, 1988, p. 101). Learning style scores have been collected for a number of professional groups and it is argued "offer reasonable indications of the learning style orientations that characterise the different professions" (Kolb, 1984, p. 88). The Learning Styles Inventory (LSI) has been criticised for an apparent lack of validity and reliability (Sims et al., 1986). Allinson and Hayes (1988, p. 271), in reviewing the LSI, quoted Freedman and Stumpf's study (1978) which found that the LSI items loaded on two bipolar dimensions, but the factor loadings were low (accounting for only 20.6% of the total variance). Cornwell et al., (1991) analysed the responses of 317 subjects who completed the LSI. Their results "afforded support for only two of the individual ability dimensions and little support for Kolb's two bipolar dimensions" (1991, p. 455) and that the LSI should be used "with some caution as a means to inform adults about how they learn best" (p. 460). Tennant (1988, p. 104) questioned the validity of the experiential learning model, but acknowledged its value as a framework for planning teaching and learning activities (p. 105). He cautioned against its wholesale acceptance lest it leads to misconceptions about learners. Honey and Mumford (1992) The most widely used approach to `learning style' in the UK, and often used as an alternative to Kolb's work, is that of Honey and Mumford (1986; 1992). They used the Kolb model as a basis from which they developed their own Learning Styles Questionnaire (LSQ) (Honey & Mumford, 1986; 1992), an 80-item self-report type questionnaire which has been designed to identify an individual's relative strengths in each of four learning styles (activist style, reflector style, theorist style and pragmatist style). A factor analytical study by Allinson and Hayes (1988) of learning style scores for UK managers questioned the validity of the LSQ. They suggested that confirmation of the LSQ's structure through a factor analysis of the individual questionnaire items rather than the learning styles scores is necessary, as is evidence of its predictive validity, before it can be used with confidence by management educators (Allinson & Hayes, 1988, p. 280). They did go on to suggest a two-factor structure comprising an `analysis' factor and an `action' factor (Allinson & Hayes, 1990). In a factor analytical study of the questionnaire scales with an undergraduate sample, Sadler-Smith and Riding failed to confirm the LSQ's hypothesised structure but did observe the analysis and action factors identified by Allinson and Hayes. Approaches to Studying Marton and Saljo (1976), in a study of how Scandinavian students tackled the task of reading academic articles and texts, identified two contrasting approaches. Students adopting a `deep' approach "started with the intention of understanding the meaning of the article, questioned the author's arguments, and related them to both previous knowledge and personal experience" (Entwistle, 1988a, p. 77). This approach contrasted with that of other students who started with the intention of memorising the important facts and, hence, were described as adopting a `surface' approach. The Revised Approaches to Studying Inventory (RASI) (Entwistle & Tait, 1994) is one of a series of instruments designed to identify these differences in approaches to studying. The RASI is a 38-item self-report type inventory designed to measure an individual's approaches to studying in a higher education context in terms of five `orientations': (i) deep approach; (ii) surface approach; (iii) strategic approach; (iv) lack of direction; (v) academic self-confidence. The first three orientations are each made up of four subscales with between two and four items per subscale. The `lack of direction' and `academic self-confidence' orientations do not themselves have individual subscales but are made up of four items each. Its factor structure appears robust (Sadler-Smith, 1996b). Deep Approach. This is made up of four subscales: (i) looking for meaning; (ii) active interest/critical stance; (ii) relating and organising ideas; (iv) using evidence and logic. Subjects with a deep approach report that they try to work out the meaning of information for themselves, do not accept ideas without critical examination, relate ideas from their studies to a wider context and look for reasoning, justification and logic behind ideas. Surface Approach. The four subscales are: (i) relying on memorising; (ii) difficulty in making sense; (iii) unrelatedness; (iv) concern about coping. Subjects reporting a surface approach would see themselves as relying on rote-learning of material, accepting ideas without necessarily understanding them, emphasising the acquisition of factual information in isolation to a wider picture and express anxiety about their studies in terms of organisation and volume of material. Strategic Approach. The four subscales are: (i) determination to excel; (ii) effort in studying; (iii) organised studying; (iv) time management. Subjects reporting a `strategic approach' perceive themselves as having clear goals related to their studies and being `hard workers', ensuring that they have the appropriate resources and conditions for successful study and feel that they are generally well organised. Lack of Direction. This orientation does not have any separate subscales, but consists of four items which are intended to reflect subjects' lack of clear academic and career direction and goals (e.g. "I rather drifted into higher education without deciding for myself what I really wanted to do"). Academic Self-confidence. Again, this orientation does not have any separate subscales. Subjects scoring highly on the four-item academic selfconfidence orientation typically perceive themselves as able, intelligent and able to cope with the intellectual and academic demands of their studies (e.g. "I seem to be able to grasp things for myself pretty well on the whole") (Entwistle & Tait, 1994). Cognitive Style Cognitive style may be defined as `a distinctive and habitual manner of organising and processing information'. Riding and Cheema (1991), in a survey of a number of different types of cognitive style, suggested that each may be assigned to one of two principal cognitive styles' `families'. They suggested that learners differ in terms of two fundamental styles: Wholist-Analytical Dimension of Cognitive Style. This describes the habitual way in which an individual processes information and is derived from the work of Witkin (see Witkin et al., 1977). Analytics tend to process information into its component parts) wholists tend to retain a global view of a topic. Schmeck (1988, p. 328) concluded that "people with an extreme analytical style ... have focused attention, noticing and remembering details. They have an interest in operations and procedures and proper ways of doing things and prefer step-by-step, sequential organisational schemes ... They are gifted at critical and logical thinking." Similarly, people with a "global [i.e. wholist] style ... [have] an attention toward scanning, leading to the formation of global impressions rather than more precisely articulated codes ... Their thinking is more intuitive than that of an analytic person ... [they] are likely to be more impulsive ... and are more gifted at seeing similarities than differences." (p. 328). Verbaliser-Imager Dimension of Cognitive Style. Verbalisers tend to represent information in memory in `words'; imagers tend to represent information in memory in `pictorial' form (Riding, 1994). These two bipolar dimensions may be considered to be orthogonal. An individual's cognitive style may be assessed using the Cognitive Styles Analysis (Riding, 1991): this is presented and scored by means of a PC. It indicates a subject's position on the Wholist-Analytical and VerbaliserImager dimensions by means of ratios which indicate a his or her: (i) performance in the verbal mode relative to the imagery mode; (ii) balance between seeing the whole and seeing the parts. Norms for data gathered by Riding and his co-workers from over 1400 subjects suggest nine cognitive style types: (i) wholist verbaliser; (ii) wholist bimodal; (iii) wholist imager; (iv) intermediate verbaliser; (v) intermediate bimodal; (vi) intermediate analytic; (vii) analytic verbaliser; (viii) analytic bimodal; (ix) analytic imager. Schmeck (1988) suggested an integration of `global' and `analytical' functioning gives a synthesis of styles producing a single flexible style (referred to by various authors as a `versatile' style or `whole-brain functioning') which takes advantage of both holistic and analytical functioning (1988, p. 8). Hayes and Allinson (1996) have developed a paper and pencil inventory (the Cognitive Styles Index) which assesses a bipolar intuition-analysis dimension of cognitive style. They use the Cognitive Styles Index to assess how far individuals are intuitive or analytic in their cognitive style and they go on to speculate on to whether or not it is possible to integrate these two styles to develop a `whole-brain' approach through training or education of the individual (1996, p. 132). Kirton (1989) described a number of assumptions regarding cognitive style that help to distinguish it from other constructs: (i) it is related to numerous traits of personality that appear early in life and are temporally stable; (ii) it is bipolar, non-pejorative and non-evaluative; (iii) it is conceptually independent of "cognitive capacity, success, cognitive techniques [strategies] and coping behaviour (functioning temporarily outside ones habitual style)" (1989, p. 3). Relationships Between Preferences, Styles and Approaches The present study aimed to: (i) use multiple measures of `learning style' and examine their interrelationships; (ii) examine the relationship between these multiple measures of `learning style' and academic performance. Newstead (1992) examined the relationship between learning styles and approaches to studying and the predictive validity of the short form Approaches to Studying Inventory (ASI). The former is "at first sight an inappropriate question since the two scales [ASI and LSI] are measuring different things: the ASI is looking at learning orientations which are to some extent variable and context-dependent, while the LSI is looking at rather more stable and permanent aspects of learning. Nevertheless, both involve measures of how active a person is as a learner and it is not unreasonable to expect that there will be some connection between these measures" (1992, p. 304). In terms of the relationship between the two instruments, he found statistically significant, but low, correlations between Entwistle's `meaning' (i.e. deep) approach and Kolb's abstract conceptualisation (r = 0.20; p < 0.01) and concrete experience (r = 0.23; p < 0.01) scales, but does not discount the possibility that the obtained correlations are spurious. Positive correlations were found between the achieving orientation and academic performance (r= 0.32; p < 0.01) and between the meaning orientation and academic performance (r = 0.22; p < 0.05). The correlation between the reproducing orientation and academic performance was low (r = - 0.15; p < 0.05). Clarke (1986) examined the predictive validity of the ASI in a medical school. He found that the cognitive aspects of the approaches failed to predict academic success: "a self-reported leaning towards the Meaning Orientation [deep], which subsumes [espoused academic values] ... does not appear to confer any advantage in performance ... but neither does a leaning towards the reproducing orientation [the latter was a negative predictor for final year students]." (1986, p. 318). Based on the rationales outlined in the previous sections it was expected that there would be significant relationships between: (i) learning preferences and learning styles; (ii) learning styles and approaches to studying; (iii) the theorist learning style (which embodies traditional academic values) and academic performance; (iv) deep approaches to studying and academic performance. It was not expected that cognitive style, because of its fundamental nature, would relate in any simple way to the other constructs measured. Method The sample comprised 245 business studies students (130 males and 115 females) aged between 18 and 58 years (mean age 23.81; standard deviation 8.07), who were following undergraduate programmes in business studies, marketing, personnel management, computing and informatics for business, finance and accounting at a university business school in the UK. Subjects were studying a compulsory semester-long module on personnel (employee resourcing, relations and development). For the purposes of the present study, the materials used were: (i) the 38item RASI; (ii) a 23-item Learning Preferences Inventory LPI (see Appendix 1), which consisted of three separate scales: learning method preference (seven items); learning media preference (nine items); and assessment method preference (seven items). The latter two scales were included for exploratory purposes only and will not be considered further; (iii) the Learning Styles Questionnaire (LSQ) (Honey & Mumford, 1992-see above). The LSQ was chosen in preference to the LSI as a result of the criticisms of the latter expressed by Sims et al. (1986), Cornwell et al. (1991), Newstead (1992); (iv) the Cognitive Styles Analysis (CSA) (Riding, 1991). The RASI and LPI were combined into a single questionnaire and prefaced by appropriate prior instructions. The LSQ was administered immediately after the RASI and LPI. Questionnaires were scored by a researcher and results and interpretations of the RASI and LPI scores were not fed back to subjects. The LSQ results were fed back to subjects with an interpretation of their individual scores only after the entire sample had been tested and some weeks had elapsed. The combined RASI and LPI questionnaire was scored as follows: (i) RASI: agree, 5; agree somewhat, 4; unsure, 3; disagree somewhat, 2; disagree 1; (ii) LPI: strong preference, 5; preference, 4; no preference, 3; dislike, 2; strong dislike, 1. In accordance with the instructions accompanying the RASI, subjects were requested not to use `3' unless they really had to or the item could not apply to them. The LSQ was scored as follows: one mark was allocated where a subject agreed that an item applied to them, no mark was allocated if the item did not apply to them. Individuals could score a maximum of 20 on each of the four `learning styles' scales (activist, reflector, theorist and pragmatist). Data were analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (Release 6.1; 1 994). Results The results will be considered as follows: (i) descriptive statistics for LPI, LSQ RASI and CSA; (ii) relationship between LPI, LSQ RASI and CSA; (iii) relationship between LPI, LSQ, RASI, CSA and academic performance. Descriptive Statistics Table I shows the mean scores obtained on the LPI, LSQ and RASI. Relationship Between the LPI, LSQ, RASI and CSA. The relationships between the measures of individual difference were considered by means of the intercorrelations between the various orientations and scales of the respective instruments. Learning Preferences and Learning Styles. The following small, but statistically significant, correlations were observed between the LPI and LSQ scales: (i) between the collaborative scale and the activist scale (r = 0.24; p < 0.01); (ii) between the collaborative scale and the reflector scale (r = -0.15; p < 0.05); (iii) between the autonomous scale and the reflector scale (r = 0.17; p < 0.05)--see Table II. Hence, there did not appear to be any strong relationship between learning style and learning preferences as measured by the LSQ and the LPI. Approaches to Studying and Learning Preferences. The only statistically significant correlations between the RASI orientations and the LPI scales were between the deep orientation and the autonomous (r = 0.21; p < 0.01) and collaborative scales (r = 0. 16; p < 0.05)--see Table III. There did not appear to be any strong relationship between approaches to studying and learning method preferences as measured by the RASI and LPI. Approaches to Studying and Learning Styles. A number of statistically significant correlations were observed between subjects' approaches to studying and their scores on the learning styles scales: (i) the deep orientation correlated positively with the reflector (r = 0.25; p < 0.01), theorist (r = 0.39; p < 0.01) and pragmatist scales (r = 0.33; p < 0.01); (ii) there was a small positive correlation between the surface orientation and the reflector scale (r = 0.22; p < 0.01); (iii) the strategic orientation correlated negatively with the activist scale (r = -0.18; p < 0.01) and positively with the reflector (r = 0.35; p < 0.01), theorist (r = 0.42; p < 0.01) and pragmatist scales (r = 0.20; p < 0.01); (iv) there was a small negative correlation between the lack of direction orientation and the theorist scale (r = - 0. 16; p < 0.05); (v) there was a small positive correlation between the academic self-confidence orientation and the pragmatist scale (r = 0.22; p < 0.01)--see Table IV. A similar result was obtained by Newstead (1992). He observed a correlation of 0.20 between the deep orientation and abstract conceptualisation (the Kolb equivalent of the theorist style). He also speculated on the notion of a learner's degree of `activity'. In this sense, the sum of an individual's scores on each of the learning style scales is a measure of how `active' a learner is: a score of 20 on each of the scales (giving a maximum of 80) would represent a person who reports themselves as being highly active at each stage of the experiential learning cycle. The correlations between this `activity' score and the RASI orientations were computed. The following significant correlations between the various RASI orientations and the `activity' score were observed: (i) deep approach (r = 0.43; p < 0.01); (ii) strategic (r = 0.35; p < 0.01); (iii) lack of direction (r = 0.14; p < 0.05). Examination of the scatter-plots did not reveal any outlying values or non-linear relationships. Hence there does appear to be some relationship between subjects' approaches to studying as measured by the RASI and their learning styles as measured by the LSQ. However, the possibility that a high `activity' score indicates a subject's propensity to agree with questionnaire items should not be overlooked. LPI, LSQ, RASI and CSA. In terms of linear correlations, no significant relationships were detected between cognitive styles and learning preferences, learning styles or approaches to studying. Sadler-Smith and Riding (in press b) reported a number of significant interactions between cognitive style, academic ability and sex in their effects on learning preference. LPI, LSQ, RASI, CSA and Academic Performance. In order to investigate the predictive validity of the LPI, LSQ, RASI and the CSA, the linear correlations between each of these and a single measure of academic performance were calculated. The academic performance measurement consisted of the aggregate percentage score for each individual across 12 undergraduate modules (mean score, 58.89%; SD, 5.76). The correlations are shown in Table V. Statistically significant correlations were observed between academic performance and the: (i) deep approach (r = 0.25; p < 0.01), academic selfconfidence (r = 0.17; p < 0.05) and strategic approach (r = 0.14; p < 0.05); (ii) theorist style (r = 0.17; p < 0.05); (iii) autonomous preference (r = 0.13; p < 0.05). There was a very low correlation between the `activity' measure (see above) and academic performance (r = 0.14; p < 0.05). Discussion As a number of the LSQ items invite respondents to express preferences for particular types of learning situations characterised by, amongst other things, varying degrees of autonomy, collaboration and dependence, it was anticipated that the correlations between the LPI and LSQ scales would have been stronger. For instance, whilst it was expected that the activist scale would have correlated positively with the collaborative preference, stronger negative correlations with the autonomous preference were anticipated. One may also have expected a strong positive relationship between the reflector scale and the dependent preference. This suggests that either the underlying dimensions that each instrument is measuring are not the same or that the psychometric properties of the LPI and LSQ are themselves questionable. The LPI was derived by the author for the purposes of the present study, hence the factor structure revealed therein has yet to be confirmed by reference to larger samples. The factor structure of the LSQ, especially at the item level, remains unclear. Entwistle (1988b) describes the deep, surface and strategic approaches to studying in terms of predominant motivations (e.g. interest in the subject matter, fear of failure, competition, etc.) and intentions (e.g. to fulfil assessment requirements by reproduction). Individuals satisfy their motivations and intentions by means of specific learning processes, for example, rote-learning (Entwistle, 1988b, pp. 46-47). Individual learning preferences, one could speculate, relate to several factors including: (i) personality; (ii) needs; (iii) context; (iv) experience. One could anticipate relationships between `deepness' of approach and autonomy of process (pursuit of subject through intrinsic motivation) and between a `surface' approach and a dependence of process (pursuit of assessment goals as a result of, perhaps, extrinsic motivation). The former was observed to a limited extent. The correlation between the deep approach and the autonomous preference could lead one to speculate regarding the extent to which a deep approach (as a result of interest in the subject, vocational relevance, etc.) leads a learner to adopt an autonomous approach, as a process, in the pursuit of particular learning outcomes (deep level of understanding, integration, etc.). Even if one acknowledges the inevitable weaknesses in the LPI, the poor relationships between the RASI and LPI would, on the whole, suggest that they do not measure similar dimensions. The moderate correlations between the deep approach of the RASI and the theorist and pragmatist scales of the LSQ suggests some overlap in the dimension which their respective instruments purport to assess. Similarly, the correlations between the strategic approach and the reflector and theorist scales suggests some commonality. Further analysis of the intercorrelations between the items of the respective scales of the LSQ and RASI would help to clarify the nature of the underlying dimension which they may independently be measuring. It should be noted that both Kolb's and Entwistle's models put considerable emphasis on process. Entwistle's `to reach a personal understanding' intention and the associated processes of operation, versatile and comprehension learning (Entwistle, 1988b) may have some equivalence with Honey and Mumford's `theorising' and, ultimately, with Kolb's `abstract conceptualisation'. Investigation of the relationships between the RASI and LSI may prove to be instructive in this regard. Newstead (1992) speculated that the extent to which a person is `active' as a learner may link the underlying constructs of the LSI and ASI. The present study demonstrated moderate correlations between the deep and strategic approaches of the RASI and the total score on the LSQ (labelled `activity' by the present author). This could suggest that those individuals adopting deep and strategic approaches are, in terms of Kolb's model, `rounded' (or versatile) as learners, that is, have some proficiency at most of the stages of the experiential learning cycle. One should not lose sight of the possibility that: (i) in correlational studies of this nature, some of the observed correlations may, in fact, be spurious; (ii) subjects may be exhibiting acquiescence in their responses to the LSQ. The observation that the CSA did not correlate with any of the other instruments used in the present study is evidence that the underlying dimensions that it measures are quite different to the motivation, process and activity constructs which may underlie the LPI, LSQ and RASI. Riding (this issue) suggests that the Verbaliser-Imager and Wholist-Analytical dimensions of style are quite fundamental and that they may reflect activities in different hemispheres of the brain. These activities may be mediated through experience, context and motivation to affect, for example, learning preferences and performance under specific instructional treatments. The issues discussed here raise a number of questions. Does `whole-brain functioning' occur, as some authors suggest, in those individuals who have strengths in both analytical and global thinking (i.e. wholist verbalisers and analytic imagers) and does this equate to Entwistle's and Pask's concept of `versatile' learning (Pask, 1976), that is, relating evidence to ideas and integrating principles with facts? Can this whole-brain, integrative approach, once it has been identified, be taught and/or facilitated through the use of particular teaching and learning strategies? Can a knowledge of cognitive style facilitate a deep, all-round approach to learning and teaching and, hence, improve the efficiency and effectiveness of learning? Conclusion The present study may indicate some overlap between the dimensions measured by the Learning Styles Questionnaire (Honey & Mumford, 1986; 1992) and the Revised Approaches to Studying Inventory (Entwistle & Tait, 1994) which one could speculate include constructs such as motivation, learning process and degree of learning activity. Further research is required to investigate these relationships. As a precursor to this, a large-scale factor analytical study of the Honey and Mumford and Kolb instruments is required to explore their factor structure at the item level and their interrelationships. The value of the former as a method of raising awareness is widely accepted (see Tennant, 1988; Presland, 1994); the question of its diagnostic and predictive capabilities remains unresolved. The present study adds further support to Riding's contention that the Wholist-Analytical and Verbal-Imagery dimensions of cognitive style are quite fundamental and independent of learning `styles' and strategies per se. The notions of whole-brain functioning, integrative approaches to studying and degree of learning activity are of considerable potential significance and warrant further investigations of a multidimensional nature. Correspondence: Eugene Sadler-Smith, Plymouth Business School, University of Plymouth, Drake Circus, Plymouth, Devon, PL4 8AA, UK. TABLE I. Mean scores for the LPI, LSQ and RASI (standard deviation in brackets) Construct Scale Mean score Preferences Autonomous Collaborative Dependent Learning style Activist Reflector Theorist Pragmatist Approaches Deep Surface Strategic Self-confidence Lack of direction 3.07 (0.95) 3.22 (0.90) 3.92 (0.85) 10.09 14.07 11.80 12.26 (3.79) (3.46) (3.37) (2.97) 3.77 3.17 3.79 3.69 1.65 (0.61) (0.79) (0.68) (0.62) (0.81) TABLE II. Correlations between the LPI and LSQ Autonomous Collaborative Dependent Activist Reflector Theorist Pragmatist -0.06 0.17[*] 0.07 0.00 0.24[**] -0.15[*] -0.04 0.07 -0.08 0.05 0.04 0.08 [*] p < 0.05; [**] p < 0.01. TABLE III. Correlations between RASI orientations and LPI scales Legend for Table: A B C D E - Deep Surface Strategic Lack of direction Academic self-confidence A Autonomous Collaborative Dependent 0.21[**] 0.16[*] 0.07 B C D E -0.10 -0.05 0.05 0.08 -0.10 0.03 -0.05 -0.07 -0.05 -0.01 0.01 -0.02 [*] p < 0.05; [**] p < 0.01. TABLE IV. Correlations between RASI and LSQ Legend for Table: A B C D - Activist Reflector Theorist Pragmatist A Deep Surface Strategic Lack of direction Academic self-confidence -0.03 -0.08 -0.18[***] 0.04 0.05 B 0.25[**] 0.22[**] 0.35[**] -0.08 -0.04 C 0.39[**] 0.07 0.42[**] -0.16[*] D 0.33[**] -0.09 0.20[**] -0.10 0.01 0.22[**] [*] p < 0.05; [**] p < 0.01. TABLE V. Correlation of LPI, LSQ, RASI, CSA with academic performance Correlation with academic Construct Scale performance Collaborative Dependent -0.13 0.03 Learning style Activist Reflector Theorist Pragmatist -0.05 0.08 0.17[*] 0.11 Approaches Deep Surface Strategic Lack of direction Self-confidence 0.26[**] -0.11 0.14[*] -0.11 0.17[*] Cognitive style VI ratio WA ratio -0.01 0.04 [*] p < 0.05; [**] p < 0.01. REFERENCES ALLINSON, C.W. & HAYES, J. (1988) The Learning Styles Questionnaire: an alternative to Kolb's inventory? Journal of Management Studies, 25, pp. 269-281. ALLINSON, C.W. & HAYES, J. (1990) The validity of the Learning Styles Questionnaire, Psychological Reports, 67, pp. 859-866. ALLINSON, C.W. & HAYES, J. (1996) The Cognitive Styles Index: a measure of intuition-analysis for organisational research, Journal of Management Studies, 33, pp. 119-135. CLARKE, RM. (1986) Students' approaches to studying in an innovative medical school: a crosssectional study, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, pp. 309-321. CORNWELL, J.M., MANFREDO, P.A. & DUNLAP, W.P. (1991) Factor analysis of the 1985 revision of Kolb's Learning Styles Inventory, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51, pp. 455-462. CURRY, L. (1983) Learning Styles in Continuing Medical Education (Ottawa, Canadian Medical Association). ENTWISTLE, N.J. (1988a). Styles of Learning and Teaching (London, David Fulton). ENTWISTLE, N.J. (1988b) Motivational factors in approaches to learning, in: R.R. SCHMECK (Ed.) Styles and Strategies of Learning (New York, Plenum). ENTWISTLE, N.J. & TAIT, H. (1994) The Revised Approaches to Studying Inventory (University of Edinburgh, Centre for Research into Learning and Instruction). FREEDMAN, R.D. & STUMPF, S.A. (1978) What can one learn from the Learning Styles Inventory?, Academy of Management Journal, 21, pp. 275-282. GORHAM, J. (1986) Assessment classification and implication of learning styles as instructional interactions, Communication Education, ERIC Reports, 35, pp. 411-417. HAYES,J. & ALLINSON, C. (1996) Journal of Management Studies. HONEY, P. & MUMFORD, A. (1986, 1992) The Manual of Learning Styles (Maidenhead, Peter Honey). KIRTON, M.J. (Ed.) (1989) Adaptors and Innovators: styles of creativity and problem solving (London, Routledge). KOLB, D.A. (1976) Learning Style Inventory: technical manual (Boston, MA, McBer). KOLB, D.A. (1984) Experiential Learning (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall). KOLB, D.A. (1985) Learning Style Inventory (Revised, 1981, 1985) (Boston, MA, McBer). KOLB, D.A., OSLAND, J.S. & RUBIN, I.W. (1995) Organisational Behaviour: an experiential approach (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall). MARTON, F. & SALJO, R. (1976) On qualitative differences in learning: I. outcome and process, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, pp. 411. MURRAY-HARVEY, R. (1994) Learning styles and approaches to learning: distinguishing between concepts and instruments, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, pp. 373-388. NEWSTEAD, S.E. (1992) A study of two `quick and easy' methods of assessing individual differences in student learning, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, pp. 299-312. PASK, G. (1976) Styles and strategies of learning, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, pp. 128-148. PRESLAND, J. (1994) Learning styles and continuous professional development, Educational Psychology in Practice, 10, pp. 179-184. RIDING, R.J. (1991) Cognitive Styles Analysis (Birmingham, Learning and Training Technology). RIDING, R.J. (1994) Personal Style Awareness and Personal Development (Birmingham, Learning and Training Technology) RIDING, R.J. (this issue) On the nature of cognitive style, Educational Psychology, 17. RIDING, R.J. & CHEEMA, I. (1991) Cognitive styles--an overview and integration, Educational Psychology, 11, pp. 193-215. RIECHMANN, S.W. & GRASHA, A.F. (1974) A rational approach to developing and assessing the construct validity of a student learning styles scale instrument, Journal of Psychology, 87, pp. 213-223. SADLER-SMITH, E. (1996a) Learning styles: a holistic approach, Journal of European Industrial Training, 20, 8, pp. 29-36. SADLER-SMITH, E. (1996b) Approaches to studying: age, gender and academic performance, Educational Studies, 22(3), pp. 367-380. SADLER-SMITH, E. & RIDING, R.J. (in press a) A Study of the Properties of the Learning Styles Questionnaire. SADLER-SMITH, E. & RIDING, R.J. (in press b) Learning Preferences and Cognitive Styles in Business Studies Students. SCHMECK, R.R (1988) (Ed.) Styles and Strategies of Learning (New York, Plenum). SIMS, R.R., VERES, J.G., WATSON, P. & BUCKNER, K.E. (1986) The reliability and classification stability of the Learning Styles Inventory, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 46, pp. 753-760. TENNANT, M. (1988) Psychology and Adult Learning (London, Routledge). WITKIN, H.A., MOORE, C.A., GOODENOUGH, D.R. & Cox, P.W. (1977) Fielddependent and field-independent cognitive styles and their educational implications, Review of Educational Research, 47, pp. 1-64 WITKIN, H.A., OTTMAN, P.K., RASKIN, E. & KARP, S.A. (1971) A Manual for the Embedded Figures Test (Palo Alto, CA, Consulting Psychologists Press). APPENDIX 1 Learning preferences inventory Please indicate your preference for each of the items listed below. Respond according to the following scheme: (5) Strong preference; (4) Preference; (3) No preference; (2) Dislike; (1) Strong dislike. Legend for Table: A B C D E - Strong preference Preference No preference Dislike Strong dislike Teaching Methods Lecture Tutorial/surgery Role-play Open/Distance/Flexible learning Discussion groups Computer-assisted learning Business games ~~~~~~~~ A B C D E 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 By EUGENE SADLER-SMITH, Plymouth Business School, University of Plymouth, UK ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Educational Psychology is the property of Carfax Publishing Company and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Educational Psychology, Mar-Jun97, Vol. 17 Issue 1/2, p51, 13p, 6 charts. Item Number: 9706014958 Result 67 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 71 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Understanding the effects of a process-orientated... Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; EDUCATION -- Evaluation Source: Educational Psychology, Mar-Jun97, Vol. 17 Issue 1/2, p111, 15p, 8 charts Author(s): Schatteman, A.; Carette, E. Abstract: Investigates the effect of Interactive Working Group in learning styles. background and goal of the study; Theoretical framework of learning styles; Method used in the study; Results of the study; Conclusions. AN: 9706014971 ISSN: 0144-3410 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] UNDERSTANDING THE EFFECTS OF A PROCESS-ORIENTATED INSTRUCTION IN THE FIRST YEAR OF UNIVERSITY BY INVESTIGATING LEARNING STYLE CHARACTERISTICS ABSTRACT In an attempt to remedy transition problems from secondary education to university, the Learning and Guidance Centre of the Faculty of Sciences at the Vrije Universiteit Brussel has developed a process-orientated instruction, called Interactive Working Groups (IWG). The major goal is to promote in-depth learning by the training of general and specific learning skills in a content-specific context. Previous quantitative analyses have shown that participation in IWG leads to better performance in examinations and induces positive effects on the learning approach. Therefore, we considered separately variables such as prior knowledge test results, examination scores and learning style characteristics. The goal of the present research is to understand these effects by analysing quantitatively the interdependency of previous variables. The results show, for instance, that the IWG enhance precisely those changes in learning approach and regulation which induce an increase in performance in examinations. The results were confirmed by qualitative analyses through interviews. Introduction Background and Goal Due to the fact that students seem to be unprepared for the study skills that are necessary to succeed in university education (especially in the first year) (Entwistle & Tait, 1993), the Learning and Guidance Centre of the Faculty of Sciences at the Vrije Universiteit Brussel (Eisendrath & D'Haese, 1989) has developed for first year students a process-orientated instruction called Interactive Working Groups (IWG). The major goal of this supervision is to promote in-depth learning by the training of general and specific learning skills (Entwisde, 1993; 1994a) in a content-specific context (in science) and to encourage a scientific attitude. Evaluations in the past (Crabbe et al., 1993; Schatteman et al., 1993) have shown that IWG participation may lead to better scores in examinations and that it also induces positive effects on the learning approach. The main questions we address in this study are: --whether changes in learning style are fostered by IWG; --whether, due to participation in IWG, changes in learning style can improve the students' performance in examinations. The variables considered here are: prior knowledge, examination scores, learning approach, regulation activities, motivation and beliefs towards learning and instruction. This paper has to be seen as a first step in this research goal: based on quantitative analyses of pre-test, post-test and learning style inquiry results, rather coherent changes in learning approach and regulation activities are detected; moreover, they are found to affect positively performance in examinations. In a qualitative way, these findings were confirmed through interviews. Interactive Working Groups The Learning and Guidance Centre wanted to approach the deficiencies in the way first year students learn. Their learning is very often limited to a surface approach, characterised by memorising and reproduction. IWG are created with the following two aims in mind: (1) to train and to stimulate students in: --activities which are fundamental in the process of knowledge assimilation: relating, structuring, analysing; --activities to control the processing of learning: planning, diagnosing, evaluating, reflecting; (2) to change, if necessary, the cognitive structure of the student in order to build new and correct concept images. IWG are student- instead of teacher-orientated and the method induces the active participation of the student in the regulation of his or her own learning process. The premise of IWG is that the efficiency of a study-method course increases when study skills are embedded in a content-dependent context. Therefore, IWG are organised in parallel with, but closely related, to the regular courses of physics, mathematics, chemistry and biology. In this way, students experience very directly the benefit of an appropriate study method and supplementary study load is minimised. The instructor interacts on a metacognitive level, although manipulating contentdependent concepts and tasks (Vermunt, 1992). The ultimate goal is a shift from external regulating activities (by the instructor) to individual internal regulating activities (by the students). Theoretical Framework The key words and ideas related to this study are constructivism, processorientated instruction, learning process, learning style, interaction between instruction and learning, in the sense used by Vermunt (1992). Constructivism The main idea is that learning is an active and constructive process (Paris & Byrne, 1989). Understanding is more complex than the mere intake of knowledge: an active integration process occurs; all previous knowledge, competencies and experiences are important for the learner to construct new internal representations of information. Learners gather, (re)organise and generalise information; their mental representations are in this way elaborated and modified over time. Contrary to the static intake model, understanding is seen to be subject to progressive refinement and it never actually finishes. One of the paradigms of the constructivist theory is `situated cognition', meaning that knowledge and competencies can only acquire their full significance for the learner in a domain-specific context. It entails that `transfer' is not assumed to occur spontaneously (Duffy & Jonassen, 1991). Learning Styles Students differ in the way they learn. We follow Vermunt when he states that the individual learning style is determined by the different learning activities (assimilation, regulation) they use during the learning process, and by their motivations towards learning and opinions about learning and instruction (Simons, 1982; Vermunt, 1992). The learning styles are rather stable, but they are not unchangeable. The learning styles of the students can be diagnosed by reliable and valid questionnaires such as the Inventory of Learning Styles (ILS) of Vermunt (1992) and others. The ILS contains more than 120 expressions about learning and instruction, classified in 16 main scales (possibly with subscales), in their turn sorted into four principal categories: learning approach, regulation of learning, learning motivations, and beliefs on learning and instruction. According to this inquiry, four prototype learning styles can be distinguished at university level: meaning-directed, reproductiondirected, concrete-directed and problematic (lack of regulation) styles (Vermunt, 1996). We can reasonably assume that the meaning- and concrete-directed learning style prototypes (or, more realistically, a mixture of them) offer the best perspectives for success in higher education (Vermunt, 1995). Table I displays the meaning of the different learning style scales. Table II shows the correlation of these scales with each of the four prototype learning styles. Process-orientated Instruction In contrast to the classical instructional concepts, instruction can be set up according to the learning process of the learner. Here, the instructor insures that the learners perform the proper thinking activities to build, change and make use of the mental models within a specific domain. The issue here is that instruction can initiate, guide and influence the learning process in order to increase the efficiency of learning. Conceptions and learning skills are therefore taught in total coherence. It is important that instruction is adapted to the learning styles of the learners, in order to avoid destructive frictions. Links with IWG The IWG meet the properties of a process-orientated instruction in the way that this instruction is set up according to the learning process: --thinking skills and conceptions are diagnosed before the instruction takes place; --conceptions and thinking skills are treated interdependently; --students are active participants; they are trained, on the one hand, in activities which are fundamental in the process of assimilation of knowledge and, on the other, in activities that control the processing of learning. In this research, we investigate the interaction of the IWG instruction with the students' learning activities and performance. Method Subjects The student group we investigated included medicine, dentistry and biomedical students. This choice was made only for practical reasons, but some results (for instance motivational aspects) have to be interpreted in this context. We used the following experimental design: --the experimental group (n = 15): students who participated in IWG mechanics frequently (at least four of the eight sessions), --the control group (n = 46): students who did not participate in IWG mechanics at all. Data and Tests For both groups, we considered following data: Quantitative Data. Pre-test results: scores on a prior knowledge test of mathematics administered at the start of the first semester; post-test results: scores on a preliminary exam (at the end of the first semester) in mechanics; learning style data: based on the ILS questionnaire developed by Vermunt, administered at the start of the academic year (ILS1) as well as at the end of the first semester (ILS2), just after the preliminary exams. The total scores obtained by the students on each scale of ILS were transformed into ordinal scale scores (from 1 to 5) by normalisation, after considering the frequency distributions. Qualitative Data. We realise that possible quantitative effects have to be interpreted carefully (Entwistle, 1994b) as they could be attributed to factors independent of the IWG. Additional qualitative analyses are needed to clarify these effects and to make the conclusions of the quantitative analysis more reliable. In order to reveal their learning experiences, 16 students were interviewed (eight students from the experimental group and eight students from the control group) at the end of the first semester, after the learning style inquiry was administered for the second time. They were all interviewed individually for about 1 hour in an open way: the interview schedule specified themes rather than detailed questions. The goal was to gather information on their learning activities their motivation towards and beliefs on learning and instruction, before entering university as well as throughout this first year. Results Research Question 1 In what way does the experimental group show different learning style characteristics from the control group, at the start and at the end of the first semester? Frequency distributions for the ILS scores were considered. Table III shows the learning style characteristics and changes during the first semester for both student groups. Globally, it is striking that the learning style of the control group was rather stable throughout the first semester; more pronounced changes can be observed for the experimental group. The figures in bold in Table III suggest positive effects possibly caused by the intervention through IWG. Learning Approach and Regulation --an increase in higher scores for the experimental group and a status quo for the control group for deep approach learning activities and selfregulating activities; --a higher increase for elaborate approach activities for the experimental group; --a decrease in higher scores for external regulation for the experimental group. Motivation/Conceptions --a decrease of certificate and self-test orientation for the experimental group; --a greater increase of personal interest for the experimental group; --an increase of vocational orientation for the experimental group, a decrease for the control group; --an increase in higher scores for use of knowledge for the experimental group and a status quo for the control group; --almost no effects were observed on intake of knowledge and a slight decrease was observed in construction of knowledge. Globally, based on these figures, one can say that the general learning style profile of the experimental group at the beginning of the academic year was not very promising compared to that of the control group. Initially, the experimental group showed a larger tendency towards surface approach, external regulation and self-test orientation and less tendency towards deep approach. No strong effects were expected on self-regulation, as the scores were already higher at the start in the experimental than in the control group. At this stage of the research, effects on deep approach and self-regulation are important considering the goals of IWG. The status quo for intake and construction of knowledge give rise to concern. Nevertheless, one has to be careful with these conclusions, because, for instance, a status quo in percentages can conceal an internal shift of scores within a group. Research Question 2 In what way do the learning style characteristics of each student group change during one semester? The cross-tabulation in Table IV confirmed the positive effects on deep approach. Comparing the areas III and III' of both cross-tabulations, the progress in deep approach activities was clearly larger for the experimental group. Similar results were obtained for self-regulation (more progress for the experimental group), external regulation (greater decrease for the experimental group) and personal interest (greater increase for the experimental group). It is positive to note that the scores on intake of knowledge were rather low (for both groups); unfortunately, no better results were obtained for the experimental group. Nevertheless, the relative low scores on construction of knowledge confirm our concern: more than 60% of the experimental group preserved or reinforced the belief that construction of knowledge is not one of their tasks, although they improved in deep approach learning activities. Overall, we can say that the effects of IWG on the learning style are rather satisfactory. Research Question 3 How is progress in performance related to changes in learning style? Considering Learning Activities. For the entire population of students compared to their evolution in deep learning approach, we investigated those students performing well on the pre-test and those performing well on the preliminary exam. This is shown in Table V. The group of students who improved considerably (Area III) for the deep learning approach seemed to have the greatest chance of success in the preliminary exam, although they did not perform so well on the pre-test. Relating this result with the positive effect of IWG on deep approach (see Table IV), we conclude that IWG succeeded in enhancing the students' chances of success by improving their deep learning approach. Similar relations were found between an increase in self-regulation and improvement of performance. Combined with the previously mentioned positive impact of IWG on self-regulation, we again can conclude that selfregulation is one of the factors explaining the positive effects of IWG on performance. For external regulation, we detected an interesting subtle distinction: (a) in the experimental group, a decrease in external regulation went together with an increase in study success; (b) in the control group, on the contrary, an increase in external regulation enhanced the chances of success. This last point was explained through the interviews with the students of the control group: lack of time was such a crucial problem that making use of external regulation (any advice of the teacher) could help and save time. Considering Motivations. Here, we notice that an increase in vocational orientation motivated the students of the experimental group to progress in performance; the contrary was true for the control group. Moreover, an increase for self-test orientation was a greater stimulant of good performance in the control group than in the experimental group. Research Question 4 How are changes of learning style characteristics interrelated in each student group? It is striking that for the experimental group the correlations between differences in scale scores (ILS2-ILS1) were much more pronounced than for the control group. Shifts in learning scales for the experimental group seem to be less coincidental, but instead more related to the entire learning style, as shown in Table VI. For instance, a shift in scores for deep approach was more positively correlated with a shift in personal interest and more negatively correlated with a shift in self-test orientation for the experimental group than for the control group. In the latter group, this deep approach shift was positively correlated with a surface approach shift, which was not the case for the experimental group. Striking are also the shifts in external regulation which were more for the experimental group than for the control group correlated with shifts in surface approach, self-test orientation, ambivalence, intake of knowledge and lack of regulation. Research Question 5 Which learning style characteristics are indicators of progress in performance or of an evolution to a more appropriate learning style, or which students would benefit the most of IWG intervention? Progress in Performance. We simultaneously analysed the results on ILS1 and shifts (positive or negative) in performance from pre-test to preliminary exam, for the experimental group as well as for the control group. The results are displayed in Table VII. A necessary condition at the start to participate in IWG appears to be that students are personally interested (low personal interest is fatal both for the experimental as for the control group), that they are not too ambivalent and that they have a rather strong belief about construction of knowledge (IWG affect neither ambivalence, nor this belief about construction of knowledge). A low deep approach tendency (according to the positive effect of IWG on this learning activity) and a high need for external regulation did not turn out to be negative characteristics at the start. Changes in Deep Approach. As we noticed before that an increase of deep approach activities enhances the chances of success in examinations, we additionally investigated in Table VIII if predictions were possible for those shifts in deep approach. Students of the control group who increased their deep approach learning activities or preserved a good deep approach tendency are characterised by low lack of regulation and low ambivalence at the start. High lack of regulation and high ambivalence appear not to hinder students of the experimental group to evolve positively in deep approach learning activities. The previously mentioned favourable starting characteristics according to performance (high personal interest, low ambivalence, high construction of knowledge) seem also important at the start in order to improve deep approach activities. It is now impossible to define which starting characteristics of the learning style can be indicators of improvement in learning style or in performance. Previous observations suggest that motivational aspects and beliefs at the start are more important to enhance success by IWG than favourable learning and regulation activities, except for external regulation. However, previous research on effects of IWG (Crabbe et al., 1993) showed that effects on performance were still significant after considering motivation as a co-factor. We want to remark that through the interviews we understood that most of the students participated in IWG at the start due to a need for external regulation. Interview Findings In general, the results on the ILS were confirmed by the interviews. However, the fact that the ILS investigates the global learning style while our research is focused on the IWG and performance in physics should not be overlooked. For instance, a high memorising activity (according to ILS2) may be explained by the high amount of memorising needed to study biology or chemistry. Students with a good result in their preliminary exam often voice that the IWG were a good help in preparing for the exam and that IWG motivate them to study. The good result in their exam was also considered a reward for their efforts and as a motivation to carry on. Students from the experimental group said they understand the material if they can rephrase it in their own words, if they are able to help other students solving their problems, or if they can apply their knowledge. It is striking from the interviews that students of the control group, on the contrary, use external factors to confirm their knowledge. They evaluate themselves by computer tests or by the scores on the preliminary exams. Some students participate in IWG without knowing what IWG is all about. They do not participate in IWG with the intention to change their learning methods. Some participate to understand the material better. Once students have participated a few times, the IWG become an important factor of their external regulation. They said that they learned to work with the material in a broader and deeper way, and that they learned to find connections. As IWG are very interactive, students voice that they learn a lot from each others' mistakes. In general, IWG are considered to be a fruitful support and a motivation to study. However, some students get frightened when they find out that a whole story lies behind the taught facts. Before attending an IWG, students are asked to prepare a particular part of the material. As a consequence, students of the experimental group keep up with the study material. Some students of the control group revealed that they had consulted the general counselling office of our institution. We realise that this may influence some positive evolutions of this group on the level of their learning style. Moreover, the fact that students of the control group have been subjected to the learning style inquiry may have influenced their way of learning by becoming conscious of some defects. Conclusion The analyses revealed that IWG enhance deep approach learning activities and selfregulation, which, in their turn, increase the chances of success in examinations. This was precisely one of the main goals of IWG. Moreover, we noticed that IWG affected the learning styles more globally, in a coherent way. The interviews revealed that the scores on the ILS closely corresponded to the real activities and beliefs of the students, and therefore it is a reliable instrument for our research goals. The result of this investigation will have effects on the IWG methodology; though we noticed that IWG enhance deep learning approach and do not really affect the belief on construction of knowledge. Special tasks to approach this deficiency have to be developed. This looks comparable with the tackling of misconceptions: one cannot change the concept image by only correcting the process; students have to be confronted with problem situations in such a way that the erroneous concept is not sufficient to solve the problem and that only a correct concept can offer this opportunity. On the level of selection criteria for participation in IWG, more learning style characteristics will be taken into account. Much further investigation has to be done; firstly, to determine areas in some subspaces of the multidimensional state space which will correspond to `good' or `bad' learning style profiles (related with performance on examinations) and, later, to adjust IWG so that `bad' profiles can evolve to become `better' ones. Acknowledgements We want to thank EIs Robbrecht for her advice about the interviews and Telidja Klai for her advice, for her concrete participation in the interviews, as well as for her critical analyses of the transcriptions. Both psychologists are working at the study counselling office at our university. We are also very grateful to Nicole Fux for her detailed analysis of the interviews. Correspondence: Anne Schatteman, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Zelfstudiecentrum B, Pleinlaan 2, B-1050-Brussels, Belgium. TABLE I. Learning style: scales and categories Learning approach learner performs cognitive processing activities such as: Deep approach --relating structuring, critically analysing, selecting Surface approach --analysing step-by-step, memorising, reiterating Elaborate approach --concretising, applying knowledge Regulation of Learning Self-regulation the learner performs metacognitive regulation activities such as: --orientating oneself before tackling a learning task, --planning, process evaluation, self-testing, diagnosing, evaluating, reflecting External regulation Lack of regulation the instructor is being called upon for the activation and execution of regulation activities any attempt by the learner to regulate the learning process fails Study Orientations Certificate-orientated motivation by the future obtaining of a degree or certificate Self-test orientated the learner is motivated by testing her/his learning abilities, proving what she/he is capable of Personally interested motivation stems from geniune interest in a specific domain or the opportunities to enrich oneself Vocation-orientated the prime goal is skilling oneself for a specific vocation Ambivalent insecure, doubtful attitude towards learning Conceptions Intake of knowledge Construction of knowledge Use of knowledge Stimulating education Co-operation performing learning activities is considered to be the task of the instruction; the learner views her-/himself as a passive absorber of knowledge the learner assumes her-/himself the responsibility of performing learning activities; learning is viewed as gaining insight in the learning material and grasping the relations between the components of it emphasis is put on the expected practical use of the learning material; concretising activities are seen as an important task of learning and instruction the learner accepts her/his task as processor of learning material, but expects the instruction to provide the necessary stimuli with fellow students and sharing learning tasks is highly valued TABLE II. Correlation of the learning style scales with each of the four prototype learning styles[*] Legend for Table: A B C D - meaning reproduction application problematic Learning Style Learning Approach Deep approach Surface approach Elaborate approach A B C X X X X D Regulation of Learning Self-regulation External regulation Lack of regulation Study Orientations Certificate-orientated Self-test-orientated Personally interested Vocation-orientated Ambivalent Conceptions Intake of knowledge Construction of knowledge Use of knowledge Stimulating education Co-operation X X X X X X (X) (X) (X) X X X X (X) (X) X X X [*] X = high positive correlation; (X) = still correlating but less important. TABLE III. Learning style characteristics and changes during the first semester for experimental and control groups Experimental group ILS1 I (%) ILS2 II (%) I (%) II (%) 20 13.3 53.3 13.3 46.7 20 20 33.3 33.3 53.5 46.7 40 Regulation of Learning Self-regulation External regulation Lack of regulation 26.7 20 26.7 40 53.3 46.6 26.7 26.7 33.3 53.3 40 40 Study Orientations Certificate-orientated Self-test-orientated Personally interested Vocation orientated Ambivalent 40 13.3 33.3 46.7 33.3 33.3 53.4 26.7 40 33.3 40 33.3 26.7 20 20 20 33.3 46.7 60 40 Conceptions Intake of knowledge Construction of knowledge Use of knowledge Stimulating education Co-operation 6.7 53.3 26.7 26.7 33.3 33.4 33.4 20 26.7 26.7 20 66.7 33.3 40 40 33.3 20 40 40 26.7 Learning Approach Deep approach Surface approach Elaborate approach Control group ILS1 I (%) Learning Approach Deep approach 37 ILS2 II (%) I (%) II (%) 34.8 34.8 39 Surface approach Elaborate approach 37 34.8 32.6 28.2 39.1 34.8 37 41.3 Regulation of Learning Self-regulation External regulation Lack of regulation 43.5 37 43.5 21.7 39.1 13 32.6 37 45.7 28.2 41.3 19.5 Study Orientations Certificate-orientated Self-test-orientated Personally interested Vocation orientated Ambivalent 21.7 21.7 50 26.1 41.3 47.8 41.3 32.6 47.8 19.6 32.6 28.3 37 26.1 45.7 43.5 40 41.3 39.2 26.1 Conceptions Intake of knowledge Construction of knowledge Use of knowledge Stimulating education Co-operation 34.8 52.2 26.1 39.1 30.4 23.9 24.9 21.7 21.7 39.1 34.8 50 23.9 37 34.8 17.4 23.9 23.9 26 37 Frequency distributions of the scores for the ILS were considered. Displayed are, for both student groups and for each scale of the ILS, relative frequencies of lower scores (column I: scores = 1 or 2) and of higher scores (column II: scores 4 or 5), for the two administrations of the ILS (ILS1: start of first semester; ILS2: end of first semester). TABLE IV. Change of deep approach during the first semester TABLE V. Evolution in deep approach versus performance for the total population of students. TABLE VI. Intercorrelations between the changes of the learning scales after the first semester vdeep vsurf vselfr vext vlackr vcer vdeep vsurf vselfr vext vlackr vcer vvoc vselft vpers vamb vint vconstrk X 0.1076 0.7431 -0.0653 0.4071 X 0.6973 X 0.5377 -0.2893 0.1467 -0.5036 0.4305 -0.3876 -0.4729 0.1977 0.2183 -0.4729 0.4572 0.2869 0.3570 -0.0157 X 0.4150 0.3869 0.3876 0.4489 0.5457 0.6172 0.1313 -0.0698 -0.1164 X 0.1511 X 0.6847 0.1436 -0.2276 vdeep vsurf vselfr vext vlackr vcer vvoc vselft vpers vamb vint vconstrk vvoc vselft vpers 0.3346 0.2505 0.1946 0.3281 0.4668 0.0672 0.2104 -0.0768 X -0.3258 -0.0359 X vamb vint 0.4412 0.4331 -0.2353 0.1477 0.1037 0.3568 0.0629 0.3069 0.3912 0.1361 0.0255 0.0963 0.3894 X 0.3347 0.1130 X X 0.4038 0.3142 -0.4060 0.1131 vconstrk X vdeep = `deep ILS2' minus `deep ILS1' for the deep approach learning scale. The figures below (above) the diagonal represent the correlations between the learning scales for the experimental (control) group. Figures in bold show differences between the two student groups. TABLE VII. Initial learning style indicators for progress in performance TABLE VIII. Initial learning style indicators for changes on the deep approach learning scale for experimental (Exp.) and control (Contr.) groups REFERENCES CRABBE, C., SCHATTEMAN, A. & EISENDRATH, H. (1993) Interactive Working Groups and their effect on the performance of first year students, in: J.K. KOPPEN & W.D. WEBLER (Eds) Strategies for Increasing Access and Performance in Higher Education, (Thesis Publishers, Amsterdam) pp. 157-171. DUFFY, T.M. & JONASSEN, D.H. (1991) Constructivism: new implications for instructional technology, Educational Technology, 31, pp. 7-12. EISENDRATH, H. & D'HAESE, I. (1989) The learning and guidance centre as an instructional forum for a new strategy of learning and instruction at the university, Persoon en gemeenschap, 41, pp. 367-387. ENTWISTLE, N.J. (1993) Influences of the learning environment on the quality of learning, in: T.H. JOOSTENS, G.W.H. HEYNEN & A.J. HEEVEL (Eds) Doability of Curricula (Amsterdam, Swets & Zeitlinger) pp. 69-87. ENTWISTLE, N..J. (1994a) Teaching for Understanding, Seminar at the LUC (Limburgs Universitair Centrum), Belgium. ENTWISTLE, N.J. (1994b) Student Interviews as Feedback and Research Data. Seminar at the LUC (Limburgs Universitair Centrum), Belgium. ENTWISTLE, N.J. & TAIT, H. (1993) Identifying Students at Risk Through Ineffective Study Strategies, Paper presented at the 5th Conference of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, Aix-enProvence. PARIS, S.G. & BYRNE, J.P. (1989) The constructivist approach to selfregulation and learning in the classroom, in: B.J. ZIMMERMAN & H. SCHUNK (Eds) Self-regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: theory, research and practice, pp. 169-200 (New York, Springer). SCHATTEMAN, Training in University, Association A., CRABBE, C. & EISENDRATH, H. (1993) The Evaluation of a General and Specific Learning Skills in the First Year of Paper presented at the 5th Conference of the European for Research on Learning and Instruction, Aix-en-Provence. SIMONS, P.RJ. (1982) Learning strategies and learning styles: an introduction, in: J. LODEWIJKS & P. SIMONS (Eds) Strategieen in leren en ontwikkeling, pp. 105-111 (Amsterdam, Swets & Zeitlinger). VERMUNT, J. (1992) Leerstijlen en sturen van leerprocessen in het hoger onderwijs--Naar procesgerichte instructie in zelfstandig denken [Learning styles and regulation of learning in higher education--Towards processorientated instruction in autonomous thinking] (Amsterdam/Lisse, Swets & Zeitlinger). VERMUNT, J. (1995) Process-oriented instruction in learning and thinking strategies, European Journal of Psychology of Education, 10, pp. 325-349. VERMUNT, J. (1996) Metacognitive, cognitive and affective aspects of learning styles and strategies: a phenomenoeraphic analysis, Higher Education, 31, pp. 25-50. ~~~~~~~~ By A. SCHATTEMAN, E. CARETTE, J. COUDER & H. EISENDRATH, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Educational Psychology is the property of Carfax Publishing Company and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Educational Psychology, Mar-Jun97, Vol. 17 Issue 1/2, p111, 15p, 8 charts. Item Number: 9706014971 Result 71 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 72 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Secondary school teachers and learning style preferences... Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; EDUCATION -- Evaluation Source: Educational Psychology, Mar-Jun97, Vol. 17 Issue 1/2, p157, 14p, 1 chart, 7 diagrams Author(s): Lawrence, M. Veronica M. Abstract: Investigates the preferred learning styles of secondary school teachers and managers using the Honey and Mumford model of learning styles. Overview of the Honey and Mumford learning style model; Learning style in an educational context; Rationale for research; Data collection and analysis; Method used in the research; Discussion of the research findings; Further research. AN: 9706014982 ISSN: 0144-3410 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] SECONDARY SCHOOL TEACHERS AND LEARNING STYLE PREFERENCES: ACTION OR WATCHING IN THE CLASSROOM? ABSTRACT The problematic issue in education is in applying learning styles research to classroom settings in schools where a range of teacher and student learning style preferences operate simultaneously. The preferred learning styles of secondary school teachers and managers were investigated using the Honey and Mumford model of learning styles. The Learning Styles Questionnaire (LSQ) developed by Honey and Mumford (1986) was used in this research. The LSQ identifies four learning style preferences: Activist, Reflector, Theorist, Pragmatist. Data was collected (1989 1992) from a random sample of 353 Main Professional Grade (MPG) (now known as the Common Pay Spine [CPS]) teachers and 47 senior managers working in Local Education Authority (LEA) maintained secondary schools. Findings indicated that in the sample: (i) Teachers tended to have similar learning style preferences, namely, Reflector with a back-up preference for Theorist. Their least preferred style was Pragmatist. (ii) Where learning style preferences differ between teachers, these could be accounted for by differences in subject specialism. Using a two-way analysis of variance, a highly significant interaction was found between subject taught (12 subject specialism) and teachers' learning style preference; (iii) Significant differences in learning style preferences were found between MPG teachers and senior managers in schools. The paper finishes with a brief description of the focus of the research into the role of learning style preferences in secondary school teachers' classroom management. Variables being investigated are teaching style, including teachers' beliefs and values surrounding learning and teaching (evidenced by their language patterns); observable teacher behaviour; and students' altitude to learning. It has been suggested that learning styles are a permanent part of human behaviour (Kolb, 1976, 1984; McCarthy, 1982; Honey & Mumford, 1986, 1992; Curry, 1983). They are, however, considered to be a flexible structure, rather than set, unchangeable personality traits (Fielding, 1994). There is a question about the extent to which a person's learning styles can be changed. Curry's description of the `onion model' of learning styles goes some way towards clarifying the level of malleability of learning styles. Curry proposed that measures for identifying learning styles could be grouped into three main types which varied in the extent to which they were likely to be influenced by extraneous factors, such as environment. Curry described an `onion model' of learning styles with cognitive personality style as the innermost layer and virtually unchangeable. The second layer of the onion model was the `information-processing style'. This was the way in which a person processed information from the environment, adapting it in accordance with their underlying personality. It was at this level of interaction, between personal predisposition and how a person processes the information bombarding them from their environment, that this research was concerned. The Honey and Mumford Learning Styles Questionnaire (LSQ) (1986), selected for this research, falls into the second layer of the Curry `onion model'. The LSQ identifies style preferences which are compatible with an individual's personality predisposition and which are still open to change and development. The outermost layer, `instructional preference', referred to an individual's choice of learning environment. It was the layer most likely to change and to be influenced by external factors. The importance of the model has been in helping to group, according to function, the range of instruments associated with learning styles. Much of the learning styles research developed within the context of experiential learning. Many of the instruments developed to elicit learning styles use experiential learning as a framework. The notion of experiential learning was initially developed by Kolb. Sequential stages through which a person has to go in order to learn effectively was the key to the `experiential learning model' proposed by Kolb. It is suggested that effective learners are competent in each of the four stages of the learning cycle (Kolb et al., 1974; Honey & Mumford, 1986, 1990b, 1992). Four stages in the experiential learning cycle were described by Kolb through which an individual will pass sequentially, beginning with having an experience, through to reflecting upon that experience, prior to increasing their understanding of that learning by placing it within a theoretical framework. The final stage in the cycle is one whereby the individual will test out the experience in a practical setting. Learning has occurred only when the sequence is followed through, according to Kolb. People may vary in relation to where they begin the sequence, some, for example, preferring to start by thoroughly reviewing relevant data rather than direct associated experiences. The two continua functioning in learning styles identification, as described by Kolb-namely, `doing and watching' and `thinking and feeling'--hold a relative emphasis with the four stages of learning (see Fig. 1). The four learning style preferences identified by Honey and Mumford are matched to the four stages of the Kolb experiential learning cycle in Fig. 1. Honey and Mumford Learning Style Model Honey and Mumford developed the notion of Kolb's learning cycle and designed an 80-item, forced choice (agree/disagree) questionnaire. The Learning Styles Questionnaire (LSQ) can elicit a person's learning style preference by focusing on their behaviour. The four preferred styles of learning were identified as: Activist, Reflector, Theorist and Pragmatist, and match the four stages of the Kolb experiential learning cycle. People with a style preference of Activist gain from learning which is action based and immediately experienced. People with a Reflector learning style preference opt for work which involves data gathering and analysis. The Theorist preferred learning style focuses on analysing and synthesising information, while people with a Pragmatist learning style preference need to see the direct application of their learning in helping plan practical solutions to their problems. The Honey and Mumford model has been applied to the educational sector with school managers in eliciting their learning style preference, with a follow-up study using a diagnostic questionnaire to clarify subjects' learning orientation (Seymour & West-Burnham, 1989, 1990; Kelly, 1995). Honey and Mumford's work on preferred learning styles (1986,1992), which is developed from the Kolb model, was selected for use with teachers in this study. It has easily understood language by focusing on behaviours, is applicable to school environments and holds a developmental focus. The Learning Styles Questionnaire (LSQ) and associated materials proved a powerful motivator in encouraging teachers to review their own learning. For pupils and students in schools, the outcome was one of adjusted classroom practice towards a more learner-centred approach. Learning Styles in an Education Context In some education circles, it is suggested that an awareness and understanding of learning styles may provide another dimension to our understanding of, for example, individual differences in learners in schools, colleges and universities (Entwhistle & Ramsden, 1983; Gibbs, 1992; Fielding, 1994; FEDA, 1995). Indeed, Fielding forcefully argues that an understanding of learning styles should be `a student entitlement and an institutional necessity' (1994, p. 393). Research into learning styles has primarily attended to students in higher education (Kolb; Gibbs; Entwhistle & Ramsden; Biggs, 1993) and adults in the business and commercial environment (Kolb, 1979; Honey & Mumford, 1990a; Mumford, 1982, 1987). Much of the published research has been on the psychometric properties of the various instruments designed to elicit people's learning style (Freedman & Strumpf, 1978; Allinson & Hayes, 1988, 1990; Fung et al., 1993). The work of McCarthy (1987, 1990) is possibly the most well known in illustrating how the curriculum can be designed to reflect the four stages of experiential learning as described by Kolb. Evidence of its application in British schools and universities, however, is scarce. The number of studies focusing on the effect of learning style on classroom practice in schools in Britain, or in management training, is comparatively small (Kelly, 1995). Indeed, the possibility of a link between a teacher's own learning style preference and the way they manage their classroom has yet to be investigated. Observation and anecdotal evidence suggest a link (Lawrence, 1992). In the secondary school arena, teaching pupils and students has focused on the specifics of teaching itself, such as: lesson planning; depth of subject knowledge; types of teaching techniques; resource issues; and, ironically perhaps, disaffected learners. Application of knowledge about how people learn, the dynamics involved and how this might affect classroom learning has taken a back seat. More so, perhaps, since the advent of the National Curriculum, GNVQs and NVQs. Teachers are expected to make radical adjustments to the way they manage their classrooms to meet the requirements of the National Curriculum, changing expectations of pupils and prospective employers. Discovering ways whereby teachers' attention could be directly focused on aspects of learning, therefore, is important. A learning styles approach enabled teachers to focus on how they teach, without implying deficiency in their present activity. Instead, it simply provided a welcome alternative for further understanding of what was happening in the process of learning between learner and learning, teacher and taught, and how teachers managed their classrooms. Experiential learning offered a way of establishing learning as a process. Learning styles offered teachers an insight into individual differences in learning. The attraction of a learning styles approach lay in it being both directly applicable to teachers' own experience of learning as learners and to their experience as teachers in schools teaching others. By drawing teachers' attention to their own learning patterns, two things can be established: (1) that learning is a process; and (2) people learn in different ways. The suggestion is that within a class a teacher could expect to have a range of pupil and student learning styles represented. If the goal is to meet the range of learning style preferences, using predominantly one teaching method, it is therefore not likely to do this. Furthermore, it may be that teachers are influenced by their own learning style preferences. It would appear reasonable to suggest that a person is more likely to work in a way which matches their own learning style preference. In teachers, this would suggest development of a particular style of classroom management over and above others. If teachers teach and manage their classrooms according to their learning style preference, pupils' learning will be affected. For example, a science teacher with a learning style preference of Activist and back-up Reflector style, rearranged the laboratory so that pupils sat in a semi-circle, in contrast to colleagues who retained the separate rows. The interaction and dynamics were different in the laboratories. Though there is no conclusive evidence to suggest that matching learning style preferences in learners significantly enhances their learning (Davidson, 1990), anecdotal evidence suggests that continued mismatching will detrimentally affect motivation and attitudes to learning. This becomes an even greater issue if, as a group, teachers have similar learning style preferences which are different to their pupils' learning style preferences. Add to this a possibility of any differences in learning style preferences between teachers being due to different subject specialism and the need to identify whether a link between classroom management and learning style becomes apparent. The secondary school curriculum in England and Wales is structured according to separate subject specialisms. Pupils in any one day will regularly be taught by different teachers, probably with different learning styles. Learning styles research suggests a range of things for classroom teachers. Firstly, there is likely to be a diversity in learning style preferences in any one classroom (Lawrence, 1992). The question arises as to whether one particular teaching strategy or set of strategies will suit all learners. The answer is that it will not. Unless the teaching methodology bridges each of the four stages in the learning cycle (Kolb, 1984) and can embrace a wide variety of techniques, any pupil/student whose style does not match is likely to be disadvantaged. There is an issue for teachers as to whether to teach to existing style preference or to enable learners to develop lesser preferred styles. The answer would seem to lie in a balance. An implicit suggestion in learning styles research is that being faced with learning opportunities which consistently mismatch a person's preference is likely to lead to disinterest, lessening in motivation and a decrease in the likelihood of learning. The reverse is also suggested. However, the debate, on matching style preference to teaching method, is inconclusive. The evidence supporting the notion that matching reaming styles with teaching styles will improve performance is mainly anecdotal and widely generalised (Davidson, 1990). Rationale for Research From the training events run over a period of 3 years (1989-1992), patterns of learning style preferences seemed to emerge among teachers and their managers. Moreover, the groupings which emerged appeared to reflect differences in how teachers and senior managers solved tasks set in the training programmes, classroom management and differences between teachers and their managers in their attitude towards change. This prompted the systematic collection of the information relating to learning style preferences of the teachers and senior managers attending professional development sessions. Were there similarities between teachers' learning style preferences? What effect did this have on the way they tackled tasks in professional development sessions? What impact did this have on the way they approached their jobs as teachers? Was there a link between learning style preferences and classroom management and their selection of teaching approaches? Where there were differences in learning style preferences between teachers, were these due to differences in subject specialism? Data Collection and Analysis Details collected from teachers in the sample included: school and LEA name, gender, initial qualification, subject taught, raw scores from the LSQ, learning style preferences using the Honey and Mumford norms, and status (i.e. MPG teacher or senior manager). Teachers and managers were told that all information would be regarded as confidential. The percentiles were categorised by Honey and Mumford's norms for the raw scores of the 400 respondents: very strong preference, strong preference, moderate, low preference and very low preference. The dominant learning style preference is signified by a score at strong or very strong preference level. For the purpose of the analysis, each of the learning styles was rated on a separate scale so that any pattern of preferences was possible. Learning style preferences were therefore dealt with as separate variables in the two-way analysis of variance, which are used throughout. Table I provides the information relating to teachers' and managers' learning style preferences by subject specialism and status. Method Subjects Learning style preferences of 353 secondary school teachers and lecturers in further education colleges were collected over a period of 3 years (1989-1992). A sample of 47 school senior managers was taken. All were from the maintained sector. The opportunity for taking the sample arose from training programmes in `learning and teaching styles and environments'. Some were organised as discrete 4-day courses, others were whole-school training days. All participants in the programmes agreed that the data could be used for research purposes. Instrument A number of self-administered inventories is available by which teachers could discover their own learning style preference. The Learning Styles Questionnaire (LSQ) developed for managers by Honey and Mumford (1986) was selected rather than either Kolb's Learning Style Inventory (1976) or the Myers-Briggs schedule Myers (1962). The LSQ focuses on what a person does, rather than asking direct questions about their approach to learning. The LSQ shows a reliability correlation of 0.89, with individual styles correlating between 0.80 and 0.95 (Honey & Mumford, 1992). Its face validity has a degree of accuracy, in that the results match consistently respondents' self-perceptions. However, its technical validity and predictability are more difficult to ascertain (Allinson & Hayes, 1988, 1990; Fung et al., 1993; Sadler-Smith, 1996). The LSQ is contained in the `Manual of Learning Styles' (Honey & Munford, 1986; 1992). The questionnaire was used to elicit teachers' own learning style preferences. Gender No significant differences were found between men and women in the sample and learning style preferences (F = 0.772; df = 3,1212; p < 0.51). This reflected the findings by Honey and Mumford (1992) which indicated that gender differences were minimal. Senior Managers and MPG Teachers The two-way analysis of variance between MPG (now, CPS) teachers and senior managers (heads and deputies, not heads of department) and learning style preference found a significant interaction (F = 2.208; df = 12,1146; p < 0.01). The findings are illustrated in Fig. 2. The interaction suggests a difference in learning style preference probably linked to differences in role and function in school for senior managers and teachers. The dominant style preference for teachers was that of Reflector with a back-up style preference of Theorist. The combination suggests a preference for carefully gathering data, conceptual analysis and synthesis of the data in making decisions and solving problems (their least preferred style was Pragmatist). Teachers with this combination of learning style preference, when convinced by the evidence, will work hard at making the identified adjustments. Their focus would be on observation with a people focus rather than a task focus. One of the difficulties is that they often tend towards perfectionism with a strong value of `right and wrong' and low risk-taking behaviour. Their preparation and rigour can lead to, for example, procrastination, inflexibility and delays in implementing change. The dominant learning style preference for senior managers was for Theorist typified by behaviours associated with analysing, synthesising and conceptualising from which to plan strategies for action and decisions. Having a clear focus, underpinned by supporting evidence, would be influential in their decision-making process. Their lesser preferred styles were that of Activist and Pragmatist, both on the `doing' side of the continuum. This suggests a disinclination for getting immediately involved (distancing themselves from the experience) and for planning practical solutions. Recent research by Kelly (1995) suggests there is a shift in learning style preference of managers in schools to a more Activist preference suggesting `action' and `doing' in preference to previously described behaviours of `watching' and `thinking' (Honey & Mumford, 1992; Lawrence, 1992). The relevance of the earlier findings for schools and how they may function as institutions is the fact that Reflector and Theorist learning style preferences sit within the `watching' and `thinking' sections of the continua: `watching' and `doing'; `feeling' and `thinking' (Fig. 1, cf. Kolb). Anecdotally, schools in the mid-1990s appear to be much more actionbased, moving from one new initiative to the next, frequently before consolidating the learning. Subject Specialism and Teachers' Learning Style Preference A question regarding the learning style preferences of the teachers in the sample arose. Were there any differences in learning style preference? To what extent did the differences group according to the different subjects being taught in secondary schools? The subjects represented in the sample were: mathematics (n = 33), physics (n = 25), chemistry (n = 25), biology (n = 28), English and drama (grouped together, n = 59), social sciences (n = 22), geography (n = 16), history (n = 38), art and music (grouped together, n = 14), modern foreign languages (n = 25), physical education (n = 19), technology (n = 29), business studies and IT (grouped together, n = 20).The sample included heads of department as well as MPG teachers. A two-way analysis of variance was run between preferred learning styles and subject specialism. English and drama teachers were not included in the analysis because the number of teachers in the sample was much larger than the other subjects (n = 59). A highly significant interaction was found between the subject taught and the teacher's learning style preference (F = 2.018; df = 33,846; p < 0.001). For the majority of subject specialism (69% or (9/13), the dominant style preference of Reflector, or Reflector/Theorist combination remained. These were: business studies and IT, social sciences, geography, modern foreign languages, technology, mathematics, physics, chemistry and biology. The least preferred learning style for the majority of subject specialism was pragmatist (54% or (7/13) (see Figs 3, 4 and 5). The learning style preferences profile for chemistry, physics, technology and geography teachers is identical (Fig. 3). Art and music teachers and physical education teachers had a dominant style preference of Activist with a back-up style of Pragmatist (Fig. 6). This suggests that these teachers function at the practical planning section of the `doing-watching' continuum and the direct experience section of the `feeling-thinking' continuum (Fig. 1). This is in contrast to the majority of their colleagues who operate within the `watching' and `thinking' sections of the continua. History teachers and English and drama teachers reflected the stages of the learning cycle with a dominant preference for Activist, a back-up style of reflector, following the stages in sequence with a least preferred style of Pragmatist (Fig. 7). The learning style preferences profile for history and English and drama teachers is identical. Most of the teachers were teaching the history project, a structured approach which partially reflected the learning cycle. This could, in part, explain their learning style preference profile. Discussion The importance of the research findings is in the degree to which learning style preferences affect the way in which teachers manage their classrooms, including their choice of teaching methodologies. The majority of the teachers in the sample had a learning style preference for Reflector with Theorist back-up. This learning style preference falls within the watching and thinking sections of the continua (Fig. 1). In managing their classrooms, many felt most comfortable teaching in a way which meant that they controlled both the information and the way in which pupils would be expected to learn. Watching and listening were the behaviours expected by teachers of the pupils. This was interesting in that it reflected their own approach to learning evidenced by their learning style preference and how they tackled curriculum change. They expressed a need to know that the pupils had all the information and the only way for this to happen was if they were to give it to them. Thorough preparation and tight schedules often meant that there was little room for manoeuvre within the lesson plan. A certainty that they themselves had covered all the options appeared as a consistent characteristic among the sample teachers (Lawrence, 1992). Their need for thorough data collection was advantageous in encouraging them to look at the learning styles research and experiential learning cycle as another way of managing their classrooms. As suggested earlier, when people with this learning style combination are convinced by the evidence, they will work hard at making the adjustments. Their challenge lay in increasing the risk factor by enabling pupils to learn in different ways. An example of this was an A-level chemistry teacher, who taught two parallel groups the same topic using different methods. Her role as teacher was also different. Both groups had the same end test. Her experimental group, where she had designed the topic using a greater range of teaching methods, reflecting more closely the four stages of the learning cycle, performed better on the end test than the group she had taught using her usual information-giving and lecture modes of delivery. This example illustrates how this teacher was prepared to review her classroom management in light of her own learning about learning styles. She explained that she would never have considered changing her classroom approach had it not been for the learning styles research. By linking the classroom practice to the experiential learning cycle, teachers could adjust the emphasis of their lessons. In contrast to the majority of subject specialists, the following group of teachers preferred to learn from settings which offered action and involvement; doing and feeling sections of the continua (Fig. 1). Art teachers in the sample expected pupils to experiment, to immerse themselves in doing the activity and to experience using the materials. Trying out an idea, seeing what it looks like and then reflecting on the experience and the product would be a commonly described pattern of activity. The teachers explained how they would structure their lessons to reflect this. In discussion, they agreed that this way of approaching felt familiar and comfortable. This reflected their own approach to learning and their learning style preference. They could see that pupils who wanted to watch before being involved, or find out more about the detail before starting could become frustrated. The Activist preferred learning style lends itself to becoming directly involved with what is happening, being prepared to take risks and embracing new ideas. It is interesting to note the match between learning style preference and how art teachers described their approach to managing the classroom. PE teachers in the sample had a similar learning style preference of Activist with a back-up Pragmatist style preference. Getting on with the task, practicing specific activities and involvement are expectations described by the PE teachers. On the whole, PE teachers became frustrated with what they saw as unnecessary discussion and examination of the curriculum, especially if it occurred without concrete examples which they could use in their lessons. Being able to demonstrate in practice what was discussed held a high premium with them. This, too, was reflected in the way in which many managed their classes. The profile of the sample history teachers was interesting in that it reflected the distinct way in which the history project was taught. Teachers would use artefacts in their lessons, encourage pupils to empathise with the people they were studying and use an extensive range of teaching approaches which reflected, in part, the experiential learning cycle. This demanded of many teachers a role change as well as familiarisation with different methodologies. A question arises as to whether it was the demands of a new curriculum which has influenced the teachers' learning style preference, or whether teachers with this learning style preference were more likely to teach history. The teachers of English, in contrast to the history teachers teaching the history project course, did not necessarily organise their classes to reflect the four stages of the experiential learning cycle. Rather, the profile suggests behaviours which encourage pupils to be involved through structured discussion, using first-hand experience, trying out unfamiliar territory and using a variety of immediate experiences. The gap often lay in providing sufficient space for review so that learners with a more Reflector style preference felt hurried. When teachers in the sample became aware of their own learning style preferences, many commented on the way this helped them understand better the variety of behaviours within their classes; for example, why the same task might motivate some and not others. Many teachers decided to organise their curriculum according to the four stages in the cycle of learning (Kolb). This would mean that, at some time, each of the four learning style preferences would be catered for. Other teachers described the learning cycle and learning style preferences with pupils. They anticipated that alerting pupils to the differences in learning style preferences might positively affect motivation. This proved to be the case (Lawrence, 1992). A finding which caused some concern was the negligible number of teachers with a Pragmatist learning style preference in schools. In classrooms, for example, this often meant little opportunity for drawing links between learning and practical examples, or how to plan practical solutions to problems. On a whole-school level it could lead to inertia: discussion about, rather than doing, something to change things! Differences between teachers attributable to learning style preferences may account for differences in versatility of teachers in relation to being prepared to employ, for example, a range of teaching methodologies. One may begin to see a connection between learning style preference and how teachers teach. Whether it is the teacher's own learning style preference which influences the way they teach or the nature of the subject itself which influences decisions over the curriculum remains an important and as yet unresolved question. This is a matter for further investigation as it fell outside the parameters of this study. Conclusion The connection between learning style preference and what happens in schools is beginning to become clearer. What is apparent is that there is a place for learning styles research in clarifying our understanding of classroom and whole-school dynamics. The differences in learning style preferences found between senior managers in schools and teachers in this study may be linked to different roles and functions. The extent to which a person's learning style preference is influenced by their job is not yet clear. Kelly (1995) suggests there may be a causal relationship between role and learning style preference in school managers as he identified a change in the profile of senior managers in schools towards a much more Activist learning style. The Honey and Mumford model of learning style (1986, 1992) operates at the informationprocessing style layer cf. Curry (1983). It may be that a person's learning style preference may shift to accommodate the role they undertake. The research findings identify that there is an interaction between a teacher's own learning style preference and subject specialism. Anecdotal evidence suggests a connection between a teacher's own learning style preference and their approach to classroom management. One could speculate as to the likelihood of being able to predict a teacher's learning style preference from their subject specialism and then their approach to classroom management. However, the research evidence, as yet, is inconclusive about the exact nature of the relationship between learning style preference in teachers and classroom management, or subject specialism. Further Research The focus of the present research is to analyse the role of learning style preferences on teachers' classroom management. There is little research into the relationship between learning style preferences and the way in which teachers organise their classrooms. Most of the research has concentrated on education managers' and students' learning style preference. The focus of the research is towards investigating the processes involved in how teachers manage their classrooms using learning style preferences as a framework to analyse what is happening. What happens between teachers and pupils/students appears further influenced by the teacher's understanding of, and beliefs about, learning, teaching and their interrelation, if any. Variables to be investigated are teaching style, including: teachers' beliefs and values about teaching and learning using their language patterns; observable teacher behaviour; and students' attitude to learning. The case study will combine qualitative and quantitative research methods. Correspondence: M. Veronica M. Lawrence, Institute of Education, University of Warwick Coventry, CV4 7AL, UK. email <m.v.m.lawrence@warwick.ac.uk> TABLE 1. Learning style preferences by subject specialism and status Activist Reflector Theorist Pragmatist PE Music and Art English History Chemistry Physics Technology Geography Business Biology Maths Languages Social sciences 4 4 4 4 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 3 1 2 3 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 3 3 4 2 1 2 2 3 3 3 3 4 3 4 4 2 3 3 1 1 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 1 Senior Managers Head of Department Teachers 1 1 2 3 4 4 4 2 3 2 3 1 4 = Dominant learning style; 1 = least preferred style. FIG. 1. The learning cycle (Kolb, 1984) and the learning style preferences (Honey & Mumford, 1986). FIG. 2. Learning styles. Teachers and managers. FIG. 3. Learning styles. Chemistry; physics; technology; geography. DIAGRAM: FIG. 4. Learning styles. Business; biology; maths. DIAGRAM: FIG. 5. Learning styles. Languages; social sciences. DIAGRAM: FIG. 6. Learning styles. PE; music and art. DIAGRAM: FIG. 7. Learning styles. English; history. REFERENCES ALLINSON, C.W. & HAYES, J. (1988) The Learning Style Questionnaire: an alternative to Kolb's Inventory?, Journal of Management Studies, 25, pp. 269-281. ALLINSON, C.W. & HAYES,J (1990) Validity of the Learning Styles Questionnaire, Psychological Reports, 67, pp. 859-866. BIGGS, J. (1993) What do inventories of students' learning processes really measure? A theoretical review and clarification, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, pp. 3-19. CURRY, L. (1983) An organisation of learning styles theory and constructs, Microfiche ED, 235 185. DAVIDSON, G.V. (1990) Matching learning styles with teaching styles: is it a useful concept in instruction? Performance & Instruction, 29, pp. 36-38. ENTWHISTLE, N.J. & RAMSDEN, P. (1983) Understanding Student Learning (London, Croom Helm). FEDA (1995) Learning Styles (Peterborough, Potters, Meridan House). FIELDING, M. (1994) Valuing difference in teachers and learners: building on Kolb's learning styles to develop a language of teaching and learning, The Curriculum Journal, 5, pp. 393-417. FREEDMAN, R.D. & STRUMPF, S.A. (1978) What can one learn from the Learning Style Inventory?, Academy of Management Journal, 21, pp. 275-282. FUNG, H., Ho, A.S.P. & KWAN, K.P. (1993) Reliability and validity of the Learning Styles Questionnaire, Journal of Educational Technology, 24, pp. 12-21. GIBBS, G. (1992) Improving the Quality of Student Learning (Bristol Technical and Education Services). HONEY, P. & MUMFORD, A. (1986) The Manual of Learning Styles, 2nd Edn (Maidenhead, Honey). HONEY, P. & MUMFORD, A. (1990a) The Manual of Learning Opportunities (Maidenhead, Honey). HONEY, P. & MUMFORD, A. (1990b) The Opportunist Learner (Maidenhead, Honey). HONEY, P. & MUMFORD, A. (1992) The Manual of Learning Styles, 3rd Edn (Maidenhead, Honey). KELLY, M. (1995) Turning heads: changes in the preferred learning styles of school leaders and managers in the 1990s, School Organisation, 15, pp. 189201. KOLB, D.A. (1976) Learning Style Inventory (Boston, McBer). KOLB, D.A. (1984) Experiential Learning Experience as the Source of Learning and Development (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall). KOLB, D.A., RUBIN, I.M. & MCINTYRE, F.M. (1974) Organizational Psychology: an experiential approach (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall). KOLB, D.A., RUBIN, M.I. & MCINTYRE, J.M. (1979) Organizational Psychology. a book of readings (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall). LAWRENCE, M.V.M. (1992) Do the preferred learning styles of secondary school teachers show similarity across different schools, or do they homogenise within particular organisations?, Unpublished Masters Thesis, Nottingham University. MCCARTHY, B. (1987) The 4MAT System (Barrington, Excel). MCCARTHY, B. (1990) Using the 4MAT system to bring learning styles to schools, Educational Leadership, 48, pp. 31-37. MUMFORD, A. (1982) Learning styles and learning skills, Journal of Management Development, 1(2), pp. 55-65. MUMFORD, A. (1987) Learning styles and learning, Personnel Review, 16, pp. 20-23. MYERS, I.B. (1962) Introduction to Type, Pala Alto, CA: Consulting & Psychologists Press. SADLER-SMITH, E. (1996) `Learning Style': frameworks and Instruments, Paper presented at the Learning Style Conference, University of Birmingham, 19-21 April. SEYMOUR, R. & WEST-BURNHAM, J. (1989) Learning styles and education management: part 1, International Journal of Educational Management, 3, pp. 19-25. SEYMOUR, R. & WEST-BURNHAM, J. (1990) Learning styles and education management: part 2, International Journal of Educational Management, 4, pp. 22-26. ~~~~~~~~ By M. VERONICA M. LAWRENCE, Institute of Education, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Educational Psychology is the property of Carfax Publishing Company and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Educational Psychology, Mar-Jun97, Vol. 17 Issue 1/2, p157, 14p, 1 chart, 7 diagrams. Item Number: 9706014982 Result 72 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 79 of 127 [Tips] Title: The learning styles of medical students: an annotated bibliography of twenty years of research. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies; MEDICAL students Source: Perceptual & Motor Skills, Dec96 Part2, Vol. 83 Issue 3, p1411, 12p Author(s): Hylton, Jaime; Hartman, Steve E. Abstract: Presents an annotated bibliography of 25 papers on the learning styles of medical students from 1975 to 1995. Specific inventories used to measure individual styles; Learning Style Inventory. AN: 9711040270 ISSN: 0031-5125 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. Result 79 of 127 [Tips] Result 80 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: The learning styles of community college art students. Subject(s): ART students -- Psychology; COMMUNITY college students -Psychology; LEARNING strategies Source: Community College Review, Winter96, Vol. 24 Issue 3, p17, 10p, 2 charts, 1 diagram Author(s): Gusentine, Stephen D.; Keim, Marybelle C. Abstract: Determines the demographic profile and the learning styles of community college art students. Assimilation as the most dominant learning style; Differences in learning styles between art majors and nonmajors; Impact of age and gender on learning approach. AN: 9703240670 ISSN: 0091-5521 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] THE LEARNING STYLES OF COMMUNITY COLLEGE ART STUDENTS Community colleges have witnessed a changing student body during the past several decades (Cohen & Brawer, 1996). While traditional students, 18 to 22 years of age, still account for a large percentage of two-year college students, nontraditional students have flocked to community colleges in record numbers. The nontraditional students, also known as new students (Cross, 1971), include women, minorities, older adults, the academically underprepared, and those in lower socioeconomic strata. To accommodate the distinctiveness of new students, community colleges will need to assess the instructional techniques of their faculty to determine the congruence between teaching strategies and students' learning styles. One of the most promising answers to more effective teaching is research on student learning styles (McCarthy, 1980). Matching learning styles with teaching styles is particularly appropriate in working with poorly prepared students (Claxton & Murrell, 1987). Learning styles can be defined in many ways; aspects of learning style include personality, information processing, social interaction, and instructional preference. Keefe (1982) defined learning style as "the cognitive, affective, and physiological traits that serve as relatively stable indicators of how learners perceive, interact with, and respond to the learning environment" (p. 44). Schmeck (1983) thought of learning styles as a predisposition to adopt a particular learning strategy regardless of the specific demands of the learning task. Several instruments have been designed to identify learning styles, including the Canfield Learning Styles Inventory, the Productivity Environmental Preference Survey, the Adult Learning Needs Survey, the Gregorc Style Delineator, Your Style of Learning and Thinking, the Schmeck Inventory of Learning Processes, and the Kolb Learning Style Inventor. All have been thoroughly validated and used in numerous studies. The Kolb Learning Style Inventory (1985) was the instrument selected for this study because of its readability and ease of scoring; thus, only the Kolb theoretical framework is detailed in the following paragraphs. Kolb's (1984) theory of experiential learning was based on the work of Dewey, Lewin, and Piaget. Kolb posited that learners must involve themselves in learning for the experience to be educative. He maintained that knowledge is acquired either by concrete experience or abstract conceptualization and that knowledge is processed through reflective observation or active experimentation. Kolb theorized that a person first has a concrete experience (CE), and then makes reflective observations (RO) about it. Then these reflective observations will form the basis of abstract conceptualizations (AC) as the individual fits the observations into generalized theories. A person will then test these theories through active experimentation (AE). This theory forms the basis of Kolb's model, which is a circle with two bipolar constructs (Figure 1). The constructs describe two ways of grasping knowledge through either concrete experience (CE) or abstract conceptualization (AC) and two ways of transforming knowledge, namely active experimentation (AE) or reflective observation (RO). The model is divided into quadrants between the vertical axis of the grasping dimension and the horizontal axis of the transforming dimension. The grasping dimension represents two dialectically opposed modes of acquiring knowledge. The first, concrete experience, involves the learner acquiring knowledge through direct contact with the experience. The second, abstract conceptualization, involves using conceptual interpretation and symbolic representation to acquire knowledge. The transforming dimension (the horizontal axis) represents two dialectically opposed modes of processing knowledge. The first mode, reflective observation, involves the learner processing information internally by reflecting on it. The emphasis of this mode of transformation is on dealing with the information rather than trying to manipulate it. The second mode of transforming knowledge, active experimentation, involves manipulating information and testing it in new situations. According to the model, a person's learning style is determined by his or her preference for a particular phase of the cycle indicated by one of the quadrants. Each quadrant represents one of four learning style modes, which are labeled Divergers, Assimilators, Convergers, and Accommodators. Divergers grasp knowledge through concrete experience and transform it through reflective observation. Those falling into this quadrant are imaginative, good at generating alternative ideas, and good in situations that call for brainstorming. Divergers tend to be emotional, peopleoriented, and aware of meanings and values. They tend to specialize in the humanities and liberal arts. Assimilators grasp knowledge through abstract conceptualization and transform it through reflective observation. Emphasis in this quadrant is on inductive reasoning and creating theoretical models in order to assimilate their observations into an integrated explanation. Assimilators favor abstract concepts rather than people and tend to specialize in areas such as mathematics and science. Convergers grasp information through abstract conceptualization and process it through active experimentation. Those with this learning style are interested in the practical application of theories. Their greatest strengths lie in problem-solving and decision-making. People with this learning style tend to be unemotional and concern themselves with things rather than people. Areas of study tend to be technical, such as engineering. Accommodators grasp information through concrete experience and process it through active experimentation. Accommodators' greatest strength lies in doing things, carrying out plans and experiments, and involving themselves in new experiences. They are best suited for situations where one must adapt to immediate circumstances. In organizations these people often have action-oriented jobs and are sometimes seen as aggressive. Areas of study tend to favor marketing and sales. Method The purpose of the study described in this report was to determine a demographic profile and the learning styles of community college art students. Two hundred students from five community colleges in southern Illinois constituted the sample for the research. A demographic survey and the Kolb Learning Style Inventory (LSI) were administered by the first researcher during regularly scheduled two-dimensional studio and art history/appreciation classes. One hundred students were enrolled in transfer courses, and 100 were in noncredit community education classes. All students who attended art classes on the days that the researcher visited the community colleges were surveyed. After students completed the instruments, the scoring procedure for the LSI was demonstrated to them so that they could score their own inventories and ascertain their learning styles. Students were permitted to keep the grid on which they had plotted their scores, as well as a description of the learning styles. The researcher then collected the completed LSIs and the supplementary materials. Kolb's model of learning formed the basis of his Learning Style Inventory. The LSI is a 12-item, self-report questionnaire that asks respondents to rank order four sentence endings with each item. The sentence endings are ranked, 4 to 1, with 4 being the most characteristic of the person's learning style and 1 the least characteristic. The LSI generates four raw scores emphasizing the person's preference for CE, RO, AC, and AE, plus two combination scores that indicate the extent to which the person emphasizes abstractness over concreteness (AC-CE) and the extent to which a person emphasizes action over reflection (AE-RO). A positive score on the AC-CE scale indicates a more abstract score, and a negative score is more concrete. On the AE-RO scale, scores are either more active or more reflective. By plotting the two combination scores on a grid, an individual learns which of the four learning styles he or she prefers. Results and Conclusions Demographic Profile Age. The ages of the students varied a great deal (Table 1). The age categories with the most students were 60 and older (n=59, 29.5%) and 22 and younger (n=53, 26.5%). Fewer numbers and percentages were found in the other age ranges. In the transfer courses, the predominant age was 22 or younger (n=53, 53%), followed by 18 (18%) in the 23-29 group, 12 (12%) between 30 and 39, 11 (11%) between 40 and 59, and 6 (6%) who were 60 or older. In the community education courses (noncredit), the predominant age was 60 and older (n=53, 53%). There were no students 22 and younger enrolled in the noncredit courses, seven (7%) were between 23-29, 14 (14%) between 3039, 13 (13%) between 40 and 49, and 13 (13%) between 50-59. Gender. Women were overwhelmingly represented in the sample. Of the 200 students, nearly 72% (n=143) were women and 28% (n=56) were men (Table 2). One student did not furnish this information. Men accounted for 44% (n=44) in the transfer category and 12% (n=12) in the community education group; there were 56 women (56%) in the transfer category and 87 (87%) in the community education group. Age and Gender. The male community college art students were considerably younger than the women (Table 2). Of the 56 men in the study, 28 (50%) were 22 or younger. Of the 143 women, 50 (35%) were 60 or older. Gender and Category. Women were more likely to be enrolled in community education courses than in transfer courses. Sixty-one percent (n=87) of the women were in the noncredit classes and 39% (n=56) were in the transfer classes. Men were more likely to be enrolled in transfer courses than in community education courses. Seventy-nine percent (n=44) of the men were in transfer classes, and 21% (n= 12) were in the community education classes. Art Majors. More men than women were planning to be art majors. Thirty-four percent (n=19) of the men and 9% (n=13) of the women intended to major in art. Concentration. Art studio was more popular than art history. Twenty-nine (91%) of the prospective art majors planned to concentrate in art studio and 3 (9%) were more interested in art history. Age of Art Majors. Most of the students who were preparing to major in art were youthful in age. Nineteen (59%) were 22 or younger. Six (19%) were 2329, 5 (16%) were 30 to 39, and 2 (6%) were 40-49. Reason for Taking Course. The predominant reason for art majors to take the course in which they were enrolled was because it was a requirement. Nearly two-thirds (n=21, 66%) indicated that the course was required, 5 (16%) were taking it as an elective, and the remaining 6 gave other reasons. Of the community education students, nearly all were taking the course for personal interest, listing reasons such as enjoyment, pleasure, or fun. Learning Styles Dominant Learning Style of Students Planning to Major in Art. It was hypothesized that the dominant learning style of students who planned to major in art would be in the Diverger quadrant. According to Kolb's Learning Style theory, students in the arts use the learning modes of reflective observation and concrete experience. However, the largest number of art majors in this study preferred the Assimilator learning style (n=14, 44%), followed by Accommodator (n=7, 22%), Converger (n=6, 19%), and Diverger (n=5, 16%). Because of the small percentage of students preferring the Diverger style, the hypothesis was rejected. Differences in Learning Styles between Art Majors and Nonmajors. Both art majors and nonmajors preferred the Assimilator learning style. The preferences of the transfer students who were nonmajors were Assimilator (n = 28, 44%), Accommodator (n = 17, 27 %), Diverger (n=13, 20%), and Converger (n=6, 9%). A chi-square analysis (chi[sup 2]= 1.937, df=3, p=.585) revealed no statistically significant differences between art majors and nonmajors in their learning style preferences. Differences Between Traditional Age and Nontraditional Age Students in Approaching Learning. A t test was used to compare the combination scores (AC-CE and AE-RO) between traditional and nontraditional age students (23 years of age or older). The AC-CE showed no statistically significant differences (t=963, df= 197, p=.337), but the AE-RO was significant at the .034 level (t=-2.138, df=197). This indicated that traditionally aged college students processed information through reflective observation, while the nontraditionally aged students processed information through active experimentation. Relationship Between Demographic Age and Learning Styles. A frequency analysis was first prepared for the youngest students (22 years and younger; n=53) and for the oldest students (60 and older; n=59). The dominant learning styles for the younger students were Assimilator (47.2%), followed by Diverger (22.6%), Accommodator (18.9%), and Converger (11.3%). The dominant learning styles for the older students were Accommodator (33.9%), followed by Diverger (32.2%), Converger (18.6%), and Assimilator (15.3%). To determine whether there was a statistically significant difference between age and learning style, a chi-square analysis was performed. The results indicated that there was a highly significant difference (chi[sup 2]=13.63, df=3, p=.003). The older students preferred to learn by concrete experience and to transform it into knowledge by active experimentation. The younger students preferred to learn using abstract conceptualization and processing it through reflective observation. Relationship Between Gender and Learning Styles. A chi-square analysis was performed to determine if there were significant differences between gender and learning styles. The results indicated that there were no significant differences (chi[sup 2]=5.77, df=3, p=. 123). A t test was conducted to determine if there were gender differences in students' approaches to the learning dimensions. There was no significance on the AC-CE dimension (t= 1.89, df= 196, p=.059), but there was a significant difference on the AE-RO dimension (t=-1.98, df=196, p=.049). The results indicated that the women preferred to transform information into knowledge through active experimentation and that the men preferred to transform it using reflective observation. Discussion Analysis of the demographic data revealed two distinct groups of community college art students. Those planning to major in art were predominantly young and male. Those taking the noncredit community education classes were overwhelmingly older women. The findings about art majors in this study are similar to those of Cohen (1988), who found that students who anticipated earning a significant portion of their income from art careers tended "to be younger, more likely male, full-time students, enrolled because of the faculty's reputation and availability, and planning on further study in more specialized programs" (p. 255). Differences in learning styles according to gender were consistent with those found by Brainard and Ommen (1977). They found that men preferred instruction that allows for greater independence and that men like to work with inanimate objects. Women preferred a more structured learning environment with well-organized and adequately detailed material. Conclusions about the learning styles of older students were not substantiated in this study because the 60 and older group preferred the Accommodator grouping, characterized by concrete experience and active experimentation. McCarthy (1980), Keefe (1982), and Kolb (1984) contended that as a person ages he or she will move toward greater levels of abstraction. The older women in this research resembled those studied by Cohen (1988), who found that students over the age of 35 took art classes to satisfy personal interest. The older women in this study apparently took art courses not only for the instruction, but also for the social interaction. Canfield's (in Claxton & Murrell, 1987) suggestions that older students prefer traditional instructional formats--listening, reading, well- organized and detailed materials and less independence--are probably still appropriate. Based on the characteristics of the students planning to transfer to a four-year institution and to major in art, the following recommendations are made for instructing them: (1) create an independent learning environment using individualized learning programs; (2) allow students to work on subject matter of their own choosing; (3) give assignments that are conceptual in nature; (4) teach the theory behind the different media; (5) teach art history by emphasizing the driving conceptual forces behind different schools of art and during different periods of time; (6) employ the Socratic method by allowing these students to arrive at their own solutions; (7) give individual critiques in addition to group critiques; and (8) provide students with individual reading lists and examples in other disciplines such as philosophy, poetry, and dance that both support and challenge their personal systems of belief and aesthetics. For older students who prefer the Accommodator learning style, these suggestions are offered: (1) create a structured learning environment using well defined instructional goals; (2) encourage students to work on group still lifes at least for the first painting; (3) encourage learning groups with group critiques after each painting technique has been introduced and dealt with by the students; (4) give assignments that are specific in nature, detailed, and well-organized; (5) create and distribute detailed handouts and provide reading lists; and (6) give clearly outlined demonstrations carefully explaining each step. Art instructors who want to accommodate the learning styles and personal characteristics of their students are encouraged to experiment with the instructional recommendations offered by the researchers, modifying them where appropriate. Other community colleges may wish to replicate this study to determine whether or not their art students also fall into two distinct categories. It may be that students studying art in other locations are altogether different from those in rural Illinois community colleges. However, it may be that art students at many two-year colleges are the young male and the older woman. Although the numbers of students studying art in the two-year college are relatively small, the finding that older students in this study preferred the Accommodator learning style has implications for further research. Among community college students who are majoring in other disciplines, is it possible that older students in other majors have similar learning styles to the art students in this study? Or do older students in other fields prefer more abstract learning styles? Only further studies using the Kolb Learning So, le Inventory can answer these questions. Table 1 Subjects by Age Groupings Age 22 23 30 40 50 60 Frequency and younger - 29 - 39 - 49 - 59 and older 53 25 26 18 19 59 ----200 Percent 26.5 12.5 13.0 9.0 9.5 29.5 ----100.0 Table 2 Subjects by Age and Gender Women -------------------Frequency Percent Age 22 23 30 40 50 60 Men -------------------Frequency Percent and younger - 29 - 39 - 49 - 59 and older 25 17.5 28 15 10.5 10 22 15.4 4 18 12.6 0 13 9.1 5 50 34.9 9 ------------143 100.0 56 DIAGRAM: Figure 1. - Kolb's Cycle of Learning 50.0 17.9 7.1 0 8.9 16.1 ----100.0 References Brainard, S.R., & Ommen, J.L. (1977). Men, women, and learning styles. Community College Frontiers, 5(3), 32-36. Claxton, D., & Murrell, P. (1987). Learning styles: Implications for improving educational practices. Washington, DC: ASHE/ERIC Higher Education Report. Cohen, A.M. (1988). Art education in community colleges. Studies in Art Education, 29(4), 250-256. Cohen, A.M., & Brawer, F.B. (1996). The American community college. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Cross, K.P. (1971). Beyond the open door. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Keefe, J.W. (1982). Assessing student learning styles: An overview. In Student learning styles and brain behavior (pp. 43-53). Reston, VA: National Association of Secondary School Principals.d Kolb, D.A. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of Development. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. Kolb, D.A. (1985). Learning Style Inventory technical manual (Rev. ed.). Boston: McBer & Co. McCarthy, B. (1980). The 4 MAT system: Teaching learning styles with right/left mode techniques. Barrington, IL: Excel Inc. Schmeck, R.R. (1983). Learning styles of college students. In N. R. F. Dillon and R. R. Schmeck (Eds.), Individual differences in cognition, vol. 1 (pp. 233-279). New York: Academic Press. ~~~~~~~~ By Stephen D. Gusentine and Marybelle C. Keim Stephen D. Gusentine teaches art at Southeastern Illinois College in Harrisburg, Illinois and works as a self-employed artist Marybelle C. Keim is a professor in the Department of Educational Administration and Higher Education at Southern Illinois University in Carbondale, Illinois ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Community College Review is the property of North Carolina State University and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Community College Review, Winter96, Vol. 24 Issue 3, p17, 10p, 2 charts, 1 diagram. Item Number: 9703240670 Result 80 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 81 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Factors affecting college students' learning styles: Family characteristics which contribute to... Subject(s): COLLEGE students -- Family relationships; LEARNING strategies Source: College Student Journal, Dec96, Vol. 30 Issue 4, p542, 5p, 1 chart Author(s): Schmeck, Ronald Ray; Nguyen, Thuhien Abstract: Examines factors affecting college students' learning styles focusing on the effect of family characteristics on career choice and on attitudes toward education and learning strategies. Effect of directive family influence on college students; Effect of families emphasizing mercenary motives for going to college; Effect of authoritarian families on students. AN: 9707062840 ISSN: 0146-3934 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] FACTORS AFFECTING COLLEGE STUDENTS' LEARNING STYLES: FAMILY CHARACTERISTICS WHICH CONTRIBUTE TO COLLEGE STUDENTS ATTITUDES TOWARD EDUCATION AND PREFERENCES FOR LEARNING STRATEGIES Although the literature contains information regarding the influence of family characteristics on career choice, it contains little information regarding the effect of this variable on attitudes toward education and on learning styles of college students. The present study focused upon these dimensions using the Family Characteristics Questionnaire and the Inventory of Learning Processes. Results suggested that directive family influence lowered the efficacy and assertion of college students. The reverse was true of nondirective families. Also, families which emphasize mercenary motives for going to college lowered academic interest and raised the agentic (task focused) behavior of college students. Authoritarian families lowered the students' concern with form and appearance and raised their tendency to process elaboratively (self actualizing) while studying, perhaps indicating an attempt to achieve independence through rebellion. The influence of parents and family on career development of college students has received considerable attention (e.g. Cochran, 1985; PaLmer and Cochran, 1988; Rodriguez and Blocker, 1988: Young, Friesen, and Pearson, 1988). However, little attention has been given to the specific mechanisms by which family characteristics influence individual differences in attitudes, motivation, and learning styles of college students. A notable exception is the work of Zirkel and Cantor (1988). Zirkel and Cantor (1988) interviewed college students as they began there education and then repeatedly during their four years in college. They noted that they could reliably classify them according to their concerns about being away from their families. All students agreed it was a challenging task. However, the group that had difficulty separating from family described the separation in "serious" terms such as deprivation of guidance and touching (e.g. hugs) and in terms of developing adult identity, while the other group simply described it in terms of practical tasks such as having to do their own laundry, their own cooking, and so on. The interesting point is that, throughout their four years in college, reliable differences appeared between these two groups with regard to their attitudes toward the role of being a college student in general and toward academic performance in particular. The first group (the one concerned most about separation) tended to perform as well as the second, but they tended to underestimate their level of performance. They also showed more stress, more concern about competition, and more general dissatisfaction with their performance. Also, independence was a definite issue with them. For example, they were most in need of family support but least likely to return home to live after college. When the two groups were asked about the meaning of grades, the first group was concerned about disappointing their parents and about the stress of competition. The second group merely mentioned practical aspects such as completing class assignments on schedule. It appeared that their previous family environment was affecting the attitudes and learning styles of both groups in college. The present study was concerned with relationships between a recently constructed measure of college students' family background and an elaborate, widely used measure of their current college learning styles. The measure of family background was developed by Nugyen (1993). The instrument assesses six dimensions of family background. The six characteristics are distinguished by an emphasis on one of the following: (Family A) effort and work; (Family B) family cohesion; (Family C) nondirective, practical support (e.g. "I'm here if you need me; I know you will do free"); (Family D) directive support (e.g. "I want you to major in X; your career will be Y"); (Family E) mercenary motives for going to college; (Family F) general obedience to family demands (authoritarian child rearing). This retrospective assessment of characteristics of family background was treated as a potential predictor of the dimensions of the students' current learning styles (the Inventory of Learning Processes: Revised). An attempt was made to describe prior family influences that contribute to current learning style in the college setting. Method Subjects. Eighty three students at a large midwestern university completed the assessments. Their participation was voluntary, and they were currently enrolled in various small classes representing varied majors and grade levels. Instruments. The Inventory of Learning Processes-Revised (ILP-R; Geisler-Brenstein & Schmeck, 1995) is a revised and expanded 150-item version of the original 62-item Inventory of Learning Processes (Schmeck, Ribich, & Ramanaiah, 1977). The major scales measure motivational and attitudinal aspects of school learning: Academic Self-Efficacy (SE), Academic Motivation(M), Academic Self-Esteem (ES), and Academic Self-Assertion (SA). They also assess general preferences for learning strategies: Methodical Study (MS; concerned with appearance and form), Deep Processing (DP; concern with ideas and theory), Elaborative Processing (EP; concern with selfactualization and personal experience), and Agentic Processing (AP; concern with task analysis, task completion, and sequencing of tasks). The ILP-R takes 20-30 minutes to complete. Responses are recorded on custom computer answer sheets (using a 6-point Likert scale format). Internal consistency reliabilities (Cronbach alpha) range from .70 to .93 for the major scales used in the present study. The Family Characteristics Questionnaire was originally developed by Nguyen (1993) while exploring the nature of the family setting in VietnameseAmerican students. This unpublished Masters Thesis involved factor analyzing 64 items relating to family background. Thirty two items were retained and subsequently keyed on 6 different subscales: (Family A) effort and work; (Family B) family cohesion; (Family C) nondirective, practical support (e.g. "I'm here if you need me; I know you will do fine"); (Family D) directive support (e.g. "I want you to major in X; your career will be Y"); (Family E) mercenary motives for going to college; (Family F) general obedience to family demands (authoritarian child rearing styles). Cronbach Alpha internal consistency reliabilities range from .69 to .82. Results The Pearson product-moment correlational analysis summarized in Table 1 was the first step in the description of the relationship between students' family backgrounds and their approaches to learning in college. Table 1 also presents the intercorrelations among the six Family scales themselves. The intercorrelations among the ILP-R scales were ritually identical to those reported by Geisler-Brenstein & Schmeck (1995) and can be obtained from that source. In addition, we examined the data using step-wise regression analyses. In each case, one of the ILP-R scales served as a dependent variable and the six Family scales were entered as independent variables. This was done to provide, for each learning style, information regarding the independence of the contribution of each of the 6 family scales in predicting college learning styles. In general, the stepwise regression analyses duplicated the results in Table 1, but there were two equations which yielded more than one significant predictor and one equation that entered only one predictor although the correlation table suggested that there were two. These regression analyses are presented in Footnotes 3, 4, and 5 at the bottom of Table 1. It can be seen in Table 1 that the Family C scale (nondirective support) contributed positively and the Family D scale (directive support) contributed negatively to the prediction of self-efficacy as measured by the ILP-R Academic Self-Efficacy scale. Although the correlation coefficient for Family D fell short of significance, the stepwise regression analysis indicated both C and D made significant independent contributions to the prediction of self-efficacy (cf. Footnote 3). With regard to ILP-R Academic Motivation (assessing academic interest, effort, and responsibility), the analyses suggested that such motivation is lower in students from families which emphasize mercenary motives for going to college (Family E). With regard to ILP-R Academic Self-Assertion, results suggest that Family D (directive child rearing) may lower the assertiveness of the son or daughter in college. With regard to ILP-R Methodical Study, Family A (effort and work) and Family F (obedience) were negatively related to this concern with form and appearance, but the stepwise regression analysis entered only Family A into the equation predicting Methodical Study (cf. Footnote 4). With regard to ILP-R Elaborative Processing, the findings indicate that Family F(obedience) related positively to this learning style. In spite of the fact that the correlation coefficient in Table 1 fell short of statistical significance, the stepwise regression analysis also entered Family D (negatively) into the prediction equation suggesting a significant independent contribution from this scale (cf. Footnote 5). And finally regarding the ILP-R Agentic Processing scale, scores on Family E (mercenary motives) predicted high scores on Agentic Processing. Discussion The Family C scale contributed positively and the Family D scale contributed negatively to the prediction of self-efficacy in college as measured by the ILP-R Academic Self-Efficacy scale. Family C reflects an emphasis upon nondirective, practical support for children (e.g. "I'm here if you need me"). Family D reflects parental directive support for the student to the point of informing the son or daughter what subject to major in and what career to plan for. Thus, in general, students reared in families which emphasize parental direction also have lower self-efficacy with regard to their potential to succeed in college. There is no way of knowing whether the family environment "caused" the current state of the students' efficacy or whether students with low efficacy simply perceive their families as being directive or even coerce them into being directive, i.e. the direction of causality is in need of further study. With regard to ILP-R Academic Motivation (including academic interest, acceptance of personal responsibility, and effort), the analyses suggest that academic motivation is lower in students from families that emphasize mercenary motives for going to college (Family E). This is not surprising since such students would probably view college as a means to an end rather than as an end in itself. Such students would tolerate the process, but not enjoy it. With regard to ILP-R Academic Self-Assertion, our results suggest that the family which emphasizes parental direction (Family D; e.g. informing the son or daughter what to choose as a major) also may lower the assertiveness of the son or daughter in college. Once again with regard to the direction of causality, it may be the case that students low on assertiveness look to their families to provide direction and perceive them as doing so. In other words, the direction of causality is open to further study. With regard to ILP-R Methodical Study (measuring impression management or a concern with "looking like" a good student), the student from a family that emphasizes effort and work as the road to success (Family A) demonstrates less concern with appearances (lower Methodical Study). Although Table 1 suggests that the authoritarian family (Family F) is similarly related to Methodical Study, the stepwise analysis indicated that only Family A (work and effort) contributed independently to the prediction. With regard to ILP-R Elaborative Processing (measuring the tendency to personalize one's studying, and valuing personal experience which relate to studies), the findings indicate that students who perceive their families as being authoritarian (Family F) tend to engage in elaborative processing when studying in college. This may reflect a rebellion or search for independent identity on the part of these individuals. On the other hand, the results also suggest that those who report a supportive but directive approach on the part of their parents (Family D; e.g. telling them what major to choose) tend to avoid elaborative processing. The latter students perhaps are not rebelling but rather willingly accepting parental direction, perhaps even eagerly requesting it. The price the latter students pay may be a reduction in elaborative processing, one of the more beneficial learning strategies. It should be noted that this is consistent with the lower self-efficacy and self-assertion demonstrated by these same students. And finally, regarding the ILP-R Agentic Processing scale (measuring an emphasis on task analysis and task completion in college studies), high scores on Family E (measuring an emphasis upon mercenary motives for going to college) predicted high scores on Agentic Processing. This is consistent with the interpretation of the Family E scale as a measure of the pragmatic philosophy that college education is a means to an end rather than an end in itself. It is the way to get a job. To summarize, the family emphasizing effort and work seems to give rise to a student who is less concerned with form and appearance. The nondirective family seems to raise the student's efficacy. The directive family lowers the student's efficacy. In addition, the latter family contributes to lowered assertion and elaborative processing (all having to do with selfexpression and self-discovery. The mercenary family contributes to a lowering of academic motivation but also raises pragmatic work habits (many teachers would consider these to be "good students" yet it turns out that they aren't really interested in learning for the sake of learning). The authoritarian family lowers methodical study and raises elaborative processing. The contrast between the effects of directive and authoritarian families upon elaborative processing may seem perplexing, but it is likely the case that there are drastic differences between providing friendly direction and giving orders. It is the difference between "you need me to tell you what to do; I'm willing to take that responsibility for you," versus "you'll do it because I say so." When the student willingly relinquishes responsibility (directive support), self-expression and self-discovery suffer. When the family simply takes responsibility in authoritarian fashion, rebellion seems to occur in college as indicated by lowered methodical study and increased elaborative processing. Table 1 Intercorrelations among Family Scales and the scales of the Inventory of Learning Processes (N = 83). Legend for Chart: A B C D E F G H I J K L M - Inventory of Learning Inventory of Learning Inventory of Learning Inventory of Learning Inventory of Learning Inventory of Learning Inventory of Learning Inventory of Learning Family Scales[2]: A Family Scales[2]: B Family Scales[2]: C Family Scales[2]: D Family Scales[2]: E Processes: Processes: Processes: Processes: Processes: Processes: Processes: Processes: Revised[1]: Revised[1]: Revised[1]: Revised[1]: Revised[1]: Revised[1]: Revised[1]: Revised[1]: SE[3] M ES SA MS[4] DP EP[5] AP A F B G C H K D I L E J M Fam. A .16 .06 .02 .14 .05 .06 -- -.03 --- -.30[*] --- Fam. B .19 -.05 .11 .05 -.03 -.11 -- -.07 .22 -- .08 --- Fam. C .33[*] .00 .09 .09 .15 -.16 -- .01 .27[*] -- -.14 .54[*] -- Fam. D -.22 -.05 -.19 -.18 -.14 .13 .27[*] -.28[*] .08 -- .08 .25[*] -- Fam. E -.13 -.02 -.24[*] -.16 -.13 .25[*] .13 -.24 .24[*] .52[*] -.11 .16 -- Fam. F .10 .01 -.16 .50[*] -.11 -.01 -.27[*] .07 .28[*] .31[*] .28[*] .31[*] .28[*] 1 Scales: SE = Academic Self Efficacy; M = Academic Motivation: ES = Academic Self Esteem; SA = Academic Self Assertion; MS = Methodical Study; DP = Deep Processing; EP = Elaborative Processing; AP = Agentic Processing. 2 Scales: A = effort and work; B = family cohesion; C = nondirective, practical support; D = directive support; E = mercenary motives for education; F = general obedience, authoritarian. 3 Stepwise regression (SE): Variable Entered Beta R[sup 2] Family C Step 1 +.33 -Family D Step 2 -.33 .21 4 Although both Family A and Family F were significantly and negatively correlated with MS, only Family A was entered in the stepwise analysis (cf. the intercorrelation among Family scales). 5 Stepwise regression (EP): Variable Entered Beta R[sup 2] Family F Family D Step 1 Step 2 +.27 -.28 -.14 References Cochran, L. (1985). Parent career guidance manual. Richmond, British Columbia, Canada: Buchanan-Kells. Geisler-Brenstein, E. & Schmeck, R. (1995). The Revised Inventory of Learning Processes: A multifaceted perspective on individual differences in learning. In Birenbaum, M. & Dochy, F. (Eds.), Alternatives in assessment of achievements, learning processes and prior knowledge. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Nguyen, T. (1993). Vietnamese-American students perceptions of family environments and parental attitudes concerning academic performance and career choice (Instrument Development). Unpublished Master's thesis, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, Ill. Palmer, S. & Cochran, L. (1988). Parents as agents of career development. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 71-76. Rodriguez, M. & Blocker, D. (1988). A comparison of two approaches to enhancing career maturity in Puerto Rican college women. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 275-280. Schmeck, R. R., Ribich, F., & Ramanaiah, N. (1977). Development of a self report inventory for using individual differences in learning processes. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 413-431. Young, R. A, Friesen, J. D. & Pearson, H. M. (1988). Activities and interpersonal relations as dimensions of parental behavior in the career development of adolescents. Youth and Society, 20, 29-45. Zirkel, S. & Cantor, N. (1990). Personal construal of life tasks: Those who struggle for independence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 172-185. ~~~~~~~~ By RONALD RAY SCHMECK AND THUHIEN NGUYEN, Southern Illinois University ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of College Student Journal is the property of Project Innovation, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: College Student Journal, Dec96, Vol. 30 Issue 4, p542, 5p, 1 chart. Item Number: 9707062840 Result 81 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 103 of 127 [Tips] Title: Learning strategies, styles and approaches: An analysis of their interrelationships. Subject(s): LEARNING -- Technique; RESEARCH -- Management Source: Higher Education, Mar94, Vol. 27 Issue 2, p239, 22p, 7 charts Author(s): Cano-Garcia, Francisco; Justicia-Justicia, Fernando Abstract: Examines the interdependence among research tools for measuring learning from different theoretical bases. Testing of university students; Existence of three dimensions or paths involved in learning; Motivational and approach elements; Separation between Deep Processing and Deep Approach; Principal-factor analyses of ASI and LASSI. AN: 9501185823 ISSN: 0018-1560 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. Result 103 of 127 [Tips] Result 84 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Differences in learning styles of low socioeconomic status for low and high achievers. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies Source: Education, Fall96, Vol. 117 Issue 1, p141, 7p, 1 chart Author(s): Caldwell, Ganel P.; Ginther, Dean W. Abstract: Investigates differences in the learning style of elementary aged low socioeconomic status, low and high achievers. Administration of the Learning Styles Inventory; Methods and procedures; Instrumentation. AN: 9611212691 ISSN: 0013-1172 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING STYLES OF LOW SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS FOR LOW AND HIGH ACHIEVERS Differences in the learning style of elementary aged low socioeconomic status, low and high achievers were investigated. Eighty-two subjects, drawn from a pool of 194 third and fourth grade students in 2 low socioeconomic elementary schools were administered the Learning Styles Inventory (LSI) (Dunn, Dunn & Price, 1989). Variables on the LSI differentiating low from high achievers (p < .05), in math and reading, were used as predictor variables in a linear discriminate analysis. Predictor variables correctly classified 78.83% of the students in reading achievement and 80% in math achievement. All significant variables were related to motivation. The findings indicated that, for low SES elementary students, motivational (internal) rather than environmental (external) factors predicted achievement. At present, estimates of the percentage of students who are at-risk of dropping out of school range from 15% in rural communities to 66% in some urban populations (Cairns, Cairns, & Neckerman, 1989). Since they have increased economic, legal and psychological problems, at-risk students who eventually drop out of school create numerous problems for society. This may be due, in part, to inadequate skills and limited earning potential (Steinberg, Blind, & Chan, 1984). Students from a low socioeconomic (SES) background constitute the largest population of individuals considered to be at-risk of not graduating from high school (Tuma, 1989; Hobbs, 1990). Through research, educators and cognitive psychologists are investigating ways to reduce the number of at-risk students. Students cite numerous reasons for dropping out of school. These reasons are frequently complex and involve several factors. Research (Dunn & Griggs, 1988) has grouped these factors into four general categories: familial factors, personal characteristics, socioeconomic factors, and educational achievement and school behaviors. Of these factors, educational achievement and school behaviors are the only factors that can be altered by educators. Other studies (Texas Education Agency, 1986; Hahn, 1987) reveal that lack of academic achievement is the single best predictor of dropping out of school. Thus, if dropout rates are to be significantly lowered, strategies to improve the academic achievement of at-risk students must be developed. Research (Hobbs, 1990) asserts that socioeconomic status (SES) is the single best predictor of academic achievement; low SES predicts low achievement. Programs such as Title I, which began in the 1960s, have attempted to remediate the problems associated with being economically disadvantaged. Studies (Hubbell, 1983; White, 1985) indicate that participation in these programs initially increases achievement, but that these academic gains fade over time. This suggests that the methods currently being used to remediate the educational deficits associated with being economically disadvantaged should be revised. The question then becomes, how should these methods or techniques be changed? A significant body of research (Dunn & Dunn, 1992; Dunn, Krimsky, Murray, & Quinn, 1985; Hodges, 1985; Lemmon, 1985; Pizzo, 1981) indicates that the achievement of all students could be improved by providing initial instruction in a manner consistent with each student's learning style. Based on studies (Johnson, 1984; Thraser, 1984; Gadwa & Griggs, 1985) using the Dunn and Dunn model of learning styles and the Learning Style Inventory (Dunn, Dunn & Price, 1989), many high school dropouts have learning styles that are mismatched with the traditional instructional mode. While low SES is highly correlated with low achievement, some low SES students are academically successful. These differences in achievement may be associated with differences in learning styles. Since both low SES and learning styles incompatible with traditional instruction are highly associated with school dropouts (Dunn & Griggs, 1988), it would seem that the low SES, nontraditional learner is in double jeopardy of dropping out. This raises the question: Does the learning style of the low SES high achiever differ from the learning style of the low SES low achiever? The purpose of this study is to determine if there is a difference in the learning style of low SES high achievers and low SES low achievers, in math and reading. Method Subjects One hundred nineteen subjects, enrolled in two elementary schools in Texas, were drawn from a pool of 194 third and fourth grade students in eleven classes. Subjects were selected based on their participation in the free lunch program (Federal REgulation 7CFR245, Garner & Cole, 1986). Thus, the entire sample contained 119 students, of which there were 22 AfricanAmerican males, 24 African-American females, 31 Caucasian males and 42 Caucasian females. Further selection resulted in a final sample of 82 subjects. Instrument The Learning Style Inventory (LSI) by Dunn, Dunn and Price (1989) was used to assess the learning styles of the subjects. The LSI (Dunn, Dunn & Price, 1989) is based on the Dunn and Dunn model of learning style and is the most reliable instrument for use with elementary grade students. Currently the Dunn and Dunn model is divided into five broad categories and includes 21 elements that demonstrate how learners are affected by their (a) immediate environment, (b) own emotionality, (c) sociological preferences; (d) physiological characteristics; and (e) processing inclinations (1992, p. 3). Procedures Using cumulative records, the following data were compiled for each subject: (a) ethnicity, (b) gender, (c) IQ (Otis-Lennon School Abilities Test), (d) and the Texas Learning Index (TLI) in reading and math from the 1994 Texas Assessment of Academic Skills (TAAS) Test (eighth edition). Prior to testing, the classroom teacher read a short story explaining learning styles to all third and fourth grade students in their respective classrooms. The story was titled, "Mission From Nostyle: Wonder and Joy Meet the Space Children" (Braio, 1987). After the story, students were allowed to ask questions and to participate in a discussion of learning styles. After the discussion the LSI was administered to all students in the classroom. Each student received a copy of the LSI. Test items were read aloud by the teacher. Students were allowed 10 seconds to fill in their response. The LSI was machine scored by Price System, Inc. Subjects included in this study met three criteria: (a) Their Learning Style Profile had a consistency score of 70 or higher, (b) they had no missing data, and (c) they participated in the free lunch program. Using the mean Texas Learning Index (TLI), from the 1994 Texas Assessment of Academic Skills (TAAS) in reading and math, subjects were classified as either low achievers or high achievers in reading and low achievers or high achievers in math. Subjects selected were the 30 with the highest scores and the 30 with the lowest scores in reading achievement, as well as the 30 with the highest scores and the 30 with the lowest scores in math achievement. Fifty of the 119 subjects were subsequently selected for both reading and math, and 32 subjects were selected only for either reading or math. A total of 82 subjects were used for the final data analysis. Gender and race were controlled for in both the low and high groups in math and reading. In math there were 14 males and 16 females in the low group and 14 males and 16 females in the high group. There were 10 African-Americans and 20 Caucasians in the low math achievement group and 10 African-Americans and 20 Caucasians in the high math achievement group. There were 15 males and 15 females in the low reading achievement group and 15 males and 15 females in the high reading achievement group. Of these 60 students, there were 11 African-American and 19 Caucasians in the low reading achievement group and 11 African-American and 19 Caucasians in the high reading achievement group. Results Two separate direct discriminat function analysis were performed to predict membership in one of two groups, high and low achievers in reading and high and low achievers in math. Predictor variables for the discriminate analysis were those variables which indicated a significant difference in independent group means (p < .05). Screening of the data showed no multivariate outliers. Evaluation of assumptions of linearity, normality, multicollinearity and homogeneity of variance-covariance matrices revealed no compromises to multivariate analysis. Selected predictor variables in reading were motivation, persistence, responsible, kinesthetic, and teacher motivated. One direct discriminat function was calculated for reading with a X[sup 2](5) = 22.58, p < .004. The cannonical correlation indicated that 33.6% of the variance in the discriminant function for reading (R = .58) can be attributed to these five variables. This combination of variables accurately predicted group membership in 78.83% of the 60 cases in reading and misclassified 21.17% of the 60 cases (See Table 1). Selected predictor variables in math were motivation, persistence, responsible and teacher motivated. One direct discriminat function was calculated for math with a X[sup 2](4) = 15.81. p </= .003. In math, 24% of the variance was attributed to the discriminant function (R = .49). Based on math achievement these variables accurately predicted group membership in 80.00% of the 60 cases and misclassified 20.00% of the 60 cases (See Table 2). The results indicated that a combination of learning styles variables (motivation, persistence, responsible, kinesthetic and teacher motivated) discriminated between low achievers and high achievers in reading by placing 78.83% of the cases in the correct group. Twenty-two of 30 low achievers were correctly classified (73.3%) and 8 were incorrectly classified (26.7%). Twenty-five of 30 high achievers were correctly classified (83.3 %) and 5 were incorrectly classified (16.7%) (See Table 1). This is considerably better than would be expected by chance alone when both groups are evenly divided. In math, the predictor variables (motivation, persistence, responsible and teacher motivated) placed low and high achievers in the correct group in 80.00% of the cases. Twenty-two of 30 low achievers were correctly classified (73.3%) and 8 were incorrectly classified (26.7%). Twenty-six of 30 high achievers were correctly classified (86.7%) and four were misclassified (13.3%)(See Table 2). This is considerably better than would be expected by chance alone with equal groups. Discussion These results indicated that high achievers, in both reading and math, are characterized as being highly motivated, persistent, responsible (conforming), and teacher motivated. An evaluation of these results indicated that variables related to motivation are the common construct among the predictor variables included in the discriminat analysis. For the purposes of the following discussion, the variables on the LSI are categorized as either (a) environmental factors or (b) internal factors. Studies based on other populations (Dunn & Griggs, 1985; Dunn & Dunn, 1992) found differences in the learning style preferences of low achieving students and high achieving students on the environmental variables of lighting, mobility, design, learning with others, and tactile/kinesthetic preferences vs. auditory/visual preferences. In addition, there were differences in the learning style preferences of low and high achievers on internal variables associated with motivation, and persistence. However, the low SES low and high achievers in this study differed only on variables associated with internal factors. This raises two primary questions: a) What factors might account for differences in levels of motivation, persistence, and responsibility between low SES low achieving and high achieving students? b) How can the school environment be changed to increase the motivation of low achieving students? Clearly, motivation is a complex phenomenon. For this reason, multiple factors may account for the motivational levels of low SES students. Moreover, the influence of any one factor may differ from individual to individual and from situation to situation. Brophy (1988) defines motivation to learn as" . . . a student tendency to find academic activities meaningful and worthwhile and to try to derive the intended academic benefits from them" (pp. 205-206). The motivation to learn is governed by cognitive and affective components which guide and direct behavior (Ames, 1992). Within this framework, the motivation to learn can be described in terms of achievement goals. Achievement goals can be divided into two contrasting constructs: (a) performance goals or performance-oriented behavior and (b) mastery goals or mastery-oriented behavior. Dweck and Leggett (1988) characterize students who exhibit performance oriented behavior in three ways: (a) they view difficulties as failures and future effort is considered to be pointless, (b) they exhibit negative self-cognitions and performance when faced with failures, and (c) they pursue performance goals. Individuals pursuing performance goals are concerned with receiving positive judgement of their ability. The thinking processes of children described as performance-oriented indicate they attribute success to factors outside of themselves, such as, "the task was easy, the teacher likes me, or I was lucky". Failure is attributed to lack of ability. In both cases the child believes he/she has no control over the outcome, thus, he/she has an extrinsic locus of control. This frequently leads to lowered motivation (Licht & Dweck, 1984; Weisz, 1981). In contrast, students high in motivation, called mastery-oriented students, pursue learning goals directed toward increasing their competence (Dweck, 1975). Mastery-oriented students associate success with effort. They provide self-praise and encouragement, they accept responsibility for failure, but do not tend to blame themselves. Failure is attributed to lack of effort. These individuals are said to have an intrinsic locus of control (Dweck, 1975). A key component in attribution theory is the issue of control. Performanceoriented individuals see themselves as having little or no control over the events in their lives. Whereas, mastery-oriented individuals see themselves as having a high level of control. Research by Nolen and Haladyna (1990) indicated that the perception of control appears to be a significant factor affecting children's task involvement and the quality of their learning. It could be argued that the low socioeconomic child may have an increased risk for exhibiting performance-oriented behavior. Research (Garner & Cole, 1986) indicates that students from a low SES background exhibit lowered expectancy for success and lower intrinsic motivation. In addition, Schultz (1993) found that "socioeconomic advantage and achievement motivation are important mediators of academic performance" (p. 229). Based on these data it can be hypothesized that low socioeconomic background and low motivation may interact in such a way that each compounds the effects of the other. Yet, some low SES students are successful. For successful students, high levels of motivation may counterbalance many of the negative effects of low SES on achievement. The possible interaction of low socioeconomic status with an extrinsic locus of control and performance-orientation as opposed to an intrinsic locus of control and a mastery-orientation, make the subject of motivation a critical issue for teachers in schools which teach low SES students. Learning environments must be structured to achieve the highest level of internal motivation from all students. Assuming that individual control is a critical component of internal motivation, classrooms which allow for and encourage personal control will be effective. Numerous studies (Ames, 1992; Boggiano & Katz, 1991; Boggiano, Main, & Katz, 1990) suggest that classrooms which are less competitive and more autonomy-inducing increase the perceived level of individual control. In the autonomy-inducing classroom, the teacher is less controlling. Flink, Boggiano, and Barrett (1990) found that the controlling behaviors of teachers negatively affected performance. Students who had more controlling teachers performed lower than students of less controlling teachers. It appears that competitive classrooms and controlling teachers both contribute to the students perception of little or no control, since all control is external. (Boggiano & Katz, 1991). In the autonomy-inducing classroom, students are active participants in setting goals for their own learning through the use of contracts, selfmonitoring of progress, cooperative group leaning and task choice (Stipek & Kowalski, 1989). Recognition of differing learning styles could be implemented easily into the autonomy inducing classroom, becoming another tool which could further increase the student's sense of autonomy and control. This tool would allow students to learn in different ways and to maintain a high level of control over their immediate learning environment. In conclusion, low motivation is a critical factor in student achievement, especially for the low socioeconomic student. Enhancing motivation requires that students become active participants in their own learning with teachers assuming a less controlling role. This enhanced motivation would lead the student to value effort and would increase the individual's commitment to effort based strategies. This study showed no significant difference in the environmental learning style needs of these low SES students. The critical differences between low and high achievers were internal variables related to motivation. Instructional methods and strategies which encourage students to become active participants in their own learning would help to develop autonomy for the individual student, thereby, increasing motivation and achievement. Table 1. Classification of Low and High Achievers in Reading Based on the Predictor Variables of Motivation, Persistent, Responsible, Kinesthetic, and Teacher Motivated. Actual Group n Low Achievers Percent Correctly Classified 30 High Achievers Percent Correctly Classified 30 Predicted Group Low High 22 73.3% 5 16.7% 8 26.7% 25 83.3% Total percent of cases correctly classified 78.83% Table 2. Classification of Low and High Achievers in Math Based on the Predictor Variables of Motivation, Persistent, Responsible, and Teacher Motivated. Actual Group n Low Achievers Percent Correctly Classified High Achievers Percent Correctly Classified 30 30 Predicted Group Low High 22 8 73.3% 4 26.7% 26 13.3% 86.7% Total percent of cases correctly classified References 80.00% Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(3), 261-271. Boggiano, A.K. & Katz, P.A. (1991). Maladaptive achievement patterns in students: The role of teachers' controlling strategies. Journal of Social Issues, 47(4), 35-51. Boggiano, A.K., Main, D.S., & Katz, P.A. (1990). Children's preference for challenge. The role of perceived competence and control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 134-141. Braio, A.C. (1987). Mission from nostyle: Wonder and joy meet the space children. Jamaica, MR, St. John's University. Brophy, J.E. (1988). Synthesis of research on strategies for motivating students to learn. Educational Leadership, 44, 40-48. Cairns, R.B., Cairns, B.D. & Neckerman, H.J. (1989). Early school dropout: Configurations and determinants. Child Development, 60, 1437-1452. Dunn, R. & Dunn, K. (1992) Teaching elementary students through their individual learning styles. Needham Heights MA: Allyn and Bacon. Dunn, R., Dunn, K., & Price, G.E. (1989). Learning style inventory. Lawrence, KS: Price Systems, Inc. Dunn, R., & Griggs, S.A. (1988). High school dropouts: Do they learn differently from those who remain in school. The Principal, 34, 1-8. Dunn, R., Krimsky, J., Murray, J., & Quinn, P. (1985) Light up their lives: A review of research on the effects of lighting on children's achievement. The Reading Teacher, 38(9), 863-869. Dweck, C.S. (1975). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040-1048. Dweck, C.S. & Leggert, E.L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273. Fink, C., Boggiano, A.K., & Barrett, M. (1990). Controlling teaching strategies: Undermining children's self-determination and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 916-924. Gadwa, K., & Griggs, S. 91985). The school dropout: Implications for counselors. The School Counselor. 33, 9-17. Garner, C.W. & Cole, E.G. (1986). The achievement of students in low-ses settings: An investigation of the relationship between locus and control and field dependence. Urban Education, 21(2), 189-206. Hahn, A. (1987). Reaching out to America's dropouts: What to do? Phi Delta Kappan, 73(4), 290-94. Hobbs, D. (1990). School based community development: Making connections for improved learning. In S. Raferty & D. Mulkey (Ed). The Role of Rural Schools in Community Development (pp. 57-64). Mississippi State, MS. Southern Rural Development Center. Hodges, H. (1985). An analysis of the relationships among preferences for a formal/informal design, one element of learning style, academic achievement, and attitudes of seventh and eighth grade students in remedial mathematics classes in a New York City junior high school. (Doctoral dissertation, St. John's Univ.) Dissertation Abstracts International, 45, 2791A. Hubbell, R. 91983). A Review of Head Start Since 1970. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Johnson, C.D. (1984). Identifying potential school dropouts. (Doctoral dissertation, United States International University.) Dissertation Abstracts International, 45, 2397A. Lemmon, P. (1985). A school where learning styles makes a difference. Principal, 64, 7. Licht, B.G. & Dweck, C.S. (1984). Determinants of academic achievement: The interaction of children's achievement orientations with skill areas. Developmental Psychology, 20, 628-636. Nolen, S.B. & Haladyna, T.M. (1990). Motivation and studying in high school science. Journal of Research on Science Teaching. Pizzo, J. (1981). An investigation of the relationships between selected acoustic environments and sound, an element of learning style, as they affect sixth grade students' reading achievement and attitudes. (Doctoral dissertation, St. John's University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 42, 2475A. Schultz, G.F. (1993). Socioeconomic advantage and achievement motivation: Important mediators of academic performance in minority children in urban schools. The Urban Review, 25(3), 221-232. Steinberg, L., Blind, P.L. & Chun, K.S. (1984). Dropping out among language minority youth. Review of Educational Research, 54, 113-132. Stipek, D.J. & Kowalski, P.S. 91989). Learned helplessness in taskorienting versus performance-orienting testing conditions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 384-391. Texas Education Agency. (1986). Characteristics of at-risk youth. Practitioner's Guide, Series Number One. 26. Thrasher, R., (1984). A study of the learning style preferences of at-risk sixth and ninth graders. Pompano Beach, FL: Florida Association of Alternative School Education. Tuma, J.M. (1989). Mental health services for children: The state of the art. American Psychologist, 44, 188-199. Weisz, J.R. (1981). Perceived control and learned helplessness among mentally retarded and nonretarded children: A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychology, 15, 311-319. White, K.R. (1985). Efficacy of early intervention. Journal of Special Education, 19, 401-416. Authors Note Ganel P. Caldwell and Dean W. Ginther, Department of Psychology and Special Education, East Texas State University, Commerce Texas. The authors wish to thank the administrators, teachers, and students of the Denison Independent School District who participated in this study. We also thank Bernadette Gudzella and Harry Fullwood for serving as members of the thesis committee. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dean W. Ginther, East Texas State University, Department of Psychology and Special Education, East Texas State University, Commerce, TX 75489. ~~~~~~~~ By GANEL P. CALDWELL AND DEAN W. GINTHER, East Texas State University, Commerce, Texas 75428 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Education is the property of Project Innovation and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Education, Fall96, Vol. 117 Issue 1, p141, 7p, 1 chart. Item Number: 9611212691 Result 84 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 110 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Learning style characteristics: An introductory workshop. Subject(s): INSTRUCTIONAL systems -- Design -- Congresses; LEARNING strategies Source: Clearing House, Nov/Dec92, Vol. 66 Issue 2, p122, 5p, 2 diagrams Author(s): Reynolds, Jim; Gerstein, Martin Abstract: Presents the outline and rationale for a three-hour workshop on learning styles designed for use with teachers, counselors and administrators. Learning style characteristics; Learning style workshop; Conclusions. AN: 9705041279 ISSN: 0009-8655 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] Section: SPEAKING OUT LEARNING STYLE CHARACTERISTICS: AN INTRODUCTORY WORKSHOP The topic of learning style characteristics is of widespread interest in the educational community, especially on the middle and secondary school level (Dunn and Griggs 1989; Orsak 1990; Sinatra 1990). Teachers and administrators have found that when they are aware of their own learning styles and the styles of their students they can improve the quality of instruction in their schools. This article presents the outline and rationale for a three-hour workshop on learning styles designed for use with teachers, counselors, and administrators. The workshop has been conducted with secondary school and college students; secondary school counselors, teachers, and administrators; and college counselors, faculty, and administrators. Learning Style Characteristics Learning style characteristics are preferences that people have for the way they learn. Individual differences in the way a learner approaches the task of learning is called his or her learning style. A learner's preference for the visual (seeing) sensory mode over the auditory (hearing) sensory mode is an example of a preferred learning style characteristic. The preference for visual stimulation suggests that the learner may need to study and learn by techniques that provide visual representation of the material being learned, such as graphs, charts, and drawings. Experts, however, do not seem to agree on how to define learning style or on the number and type of characteristics that make up one's learning style. This confusion over learning style terminology and concepts has been addressed by Bonham (1988), Curry (1990), and Reynolds (1991). Although writers define learning styles somewhat differently, the importance of the concept of individual learning style characteristics is recognized by many writers and researchers (Claxton and Murrell 1987; Cornett 1983; DeBello 1990; Hill 1976; Keefe 1987; Kolb 1984; McCarthy 1980; Price 1987; Smith 1982; Witkin and Goodenough 1981). It is helpful to look at the different ways that writers distinguish characteristics related to learning styles. Cornett (1983) and Keefe (1987) identified three major categories of learning style characteristics as cognitive, affective, and physiological. Price, Dunn, and Dunn (1982) identified four major categories with four to six elements in each for a total of twenty characteristics that affect the learning process. DeBello (1990) suggested that there may be "as many definitions of learning styles as there are theorists" (203). But most experts would probably agree that the concept of learning style should be viewed as multidimensional. Figure 1 presents a conceptual model for categorizing learning style characteristics as multidimensional. This conceptual model of one's unique pattern of learning style characteristics includes, but may not be limited to, the six categories of perceptual preference, physical environment, social environment, cognitive style, time of day, and motivation/values. This categorization is a modification of the one used by Price, Dunn, and Dunn (1982). They placed perceptual preference and time of day into one physical-needs category; also, the term psychological is used by Dunn (1984) to categorize cognitive styles. Similar categorization of learning style characteristics can be found in the work of Hill (1976) and his model of cognitive mapping. Learning Style Workshop Design and Rationale Our workshop is designed to introduce participants to the concept of learning style characteristics. The objectives of the workshop are to encourage the participants to describe in general terms the idea or concept of learning style characteristics; identify some of their own learning style characteristics; and think about how they might use their own learning style characteristics to make a difference in their learning. Kolb's (1984) model of experiential learning was selected as the model around which to design the workshop. Kolb's four learning styles are examples of learning style characteristics associated with the category of cognitive styles as seen in figure 1. The four elements of the workshop were developed to use instructional techniques associated with Kolb's four learning styles. Kolb's Learning Style Model Kolb's (1984) model of learning styles is based on the idea that learners use the following four learning modes: (a) concrete experience (feeling); (b) reflective observation (watching); (c) abstract conceptualization (thinking); and (d) active experimentation (doing). He sees the four learning modes as making up two dimensions that are "polar opposites" (Kolb 1984). One dimension is represented at one end by concrete experience (feeling) and at the other end by abstract conceptualization (thinking). The other dimension is made up of active experimentation (doing) on one end and reflective observation (watching) at the other end. The two dimensions of concrete/ abstract involvement and active/reflective participation can be combined to produce the four quadrant learning style model that is presented in figure 2. The four quadrants represent the four learning styles: (a) diverger, (b) assimilator, (c) converger, and (d) accommodator. Kolb (1985) developed a Learning Style Inventory (LSI) that can be used to assess the four learning styles of individuals. Workshop Elements The first element of the workshop is designed to support the instructional/learning strategies associated with Kolb's diverger learning style. This learning style is best characterized as one in which the learner is concerned with divergent ideas and is usually considered an imaginative learner (Kolb 1985). Brainstorming and discussion groups are effective instructional/learning strategies for this style (Svinicki and Dixon 1987). This element consists of a small group exercise in which the participants are asked to identify some positive learning experiences from their past. Each group makes a list of positive learning environment characteristics and the lists are posted on the was. The workshop leader then leads a discussion about the similarities and differences of the lists. Each list usually contains such items as group study sessions, hands-on experience, quiet place to study, discussion in groups or classrooms, and use of audiovisual aids. This workshop element, as well as the others, takes between thirty and forty-five minutes so the whole workshop can be completed in two to three hours. In order to make the small groups workable, it is suggested that the total number of workshop participants be no more than twenty-five. The second element of the workshop deals with modes of reflective observation and abstract conceptualization, which Kolb (1984) called the assimilator learning style. This learning style is best described as one in which the learner responds to abstract ideas and/or concepts. This type of person is viewed as a rational or logical learner, and lecture is seen as an appropriate instructional activity for this learning style. This element of the workshop consists of a brief lecture on learning style characteristics with the leader using an overhead transparency of the categories of learning style characteristics (see figure 1) to show how characteristics can be grouped into six major categories. The workshop leader reviews each of the six categories and tells how the characteristics in each of the categories are defined. The leader points out that one might find other categories and/or other characteristics in these six categories. For example, some researchers might include kinesthetic as a characteristic under perceptual preference (James and Galbraith 1985; Price 1987). Others might define left/right brain dominance as a unique category, but figure I would categorize brain hemispheric dominance as just another type of cognitive style. The third element of the workshop deals with abstract conceptualization and active experimentation, which Kolb (1984) called the converger learning style. This learning style is best characterized as the theory-intopractice stage in which the learner starts to relate theory to practical application. This element of the workshop allows participants to start to identify their own learning style characteristics through the use of two inventories. The first is a thirty-item self-assessment perceptual preference inventory. Participants are asked to identify items that help them learn. The self-assessment perceptual preference inventory has ten items that are visual activities such as "reading assignments/books," ten auditory items such as "hearing recitations by others," and ten tactile/kinesthetic items such as "drawing pictures." The idea for this inventory was adapted from the work of James and Galbraith (1985). The participants score their inventories and end up with three scores--one score for each of the three areas of visual, auditory, and tactile/ kinesthetic. The workshop leader should use his or her own scores to explain the selfassessment perceptual preference inventory. The numbers that reflect the senior author's results would be a high number on the visual items (7 to 8), a low number on the auditory items (3 to 4), and a midrange number for tactile/kinesthetic (around 5). These results identify a strong preference for visual learning, weak preference for auditory, and an average response for tactile/kinesthetic learning. The senior author has learned, from other learning style inventories and from his own learning experience, that having a preference for visual learning means, for example, looking at a class roster to learn student's names and drawing diagrams to learn concepts. Other workshop participants are asked to share their scores and to self-validate those scores using their own learning experiences. The second instrument used is Kolb's (1985) Learning Style Inventory. The LSI consists of twelve sentences with four different endings for each sentence. Respondents are asked to rank the endings to each sentence as the endings match with their own self-description of how they learn. The LSI is self-scoring and produces four raw scores that can then be plotted to identify one of Kolb's four learning styles as presented in figure 2. Kolb's (1985) LSI includes a section that can be used to interpret the results for the workshop participants. Murrell's (1987) Learning-Model Instrument has also been used for this part of the workshop. This instrument takes less time to score but does not have the LSI research data base. This instrument was published by University Associates so it can be used in an educational setting without charge. The Murrell instrument is based in large part on Kolb's model. The use of the two learning style characteristics inventories allows the workshop participants to start to identify some of their own learning style characteristics. The participants are asked to validate the results of each inventory against their own learning experiences and behavior in order to change the placements if they do not seem to fit. Most participants seem able to self-validate the results of the inventories used in the workshop. The fourth element of the workshop deals with what Kolb (1984) called an accommodator learning style. This learning style is characterized by an emphasis on active experimentation (doing) and concrete experience (feeling). Laboratory and field work are examples of the instructional activities associated with this learning style (Svinicki and Dixon 1987). This last part of the workshop has the participants return to their small groups. This time the group is asked to develop a list of learning strategies that might help each group member to become a more productive learner. Each group is asked to share their list of learning strategies with the rest of the workshop participants. At this point in the workshop, the leader discusses the idea that Kolb's styles might be used as a model for instruction and learning. Each of the four workshop elements are reviewed and their relationship with the Kolb (1984) model are discussed. The four learning styles of Kolb can be viewed as a learning cycle. In the first stage, the learner becomes aware of the need to learn and to seek meaning by asking the question "why." In the second stage, the learner asks "what do I need to know" and seeks content information. In the third stage, the learner deals with the theory-intopractice process of using the new knowledge, while the fourth stage allows the learner to apply this new knowledge to real life situations. McCarthy (1980) suggested that all students be taught using instructional/learning techniques that apply to each of the four Kolb (1985) learning styles. Conclusion The commitment and practices needed to help produce more effective learners can start at a very early age. As students move up through the middle and secondary levels, more emphasis and time can be placed on developing learning skills/strategies based on individual learning style characteristics. The learning process is lifelong, and when individuals receive insight into their unique patterns of learning style characteristics, they are empowered as learners. A major objective of our educational system should be to empower learners to direct and take charge of their own learning. DIAGRAM: FIGURE 1; Categories of Learning Style Characteristics DIAGRAM: FIGURE 2; Kolb's Learning Styles REFERENCES Bonham, L. A. 1988. Learning style use: In need of perspective. Lifelong Learning 11(5):14-17. Claxton, C. S., and P. H. Murrell. 1987. Learning styles: Implications for improving educational practices. ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report No. 4. Washington, D.C.: Association for the Study of Higher Education. Cornett, C. E. 1983. What you should know about teaching and learning styles. Bloomington, Ind.: Phi Delta Kappa Educational Foundation. Curry, L. 1990. A critique of the research on learning styles. Educational Leadership 48(2):50-56. DeBello, T. C. 1990. Comparison of eleven major learning styles models: Variables, appropriate populations, validity of instrumentation, and the research behind them. Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities: International 6(3):203-22. Dunn, R. 1984. Learning style: State of the science. Theory Into Practice 23(1):10-19. Dunn, R., and S. A. Griggs. 1989. Learning Styles: Quiet revolution in American secondary schools. Clearing House 65(1):40-42. Hill, J. E. 1976. The educational science. Bloomfield Hills, Mich.: Oakland Community College. James, W. B., and M. W. Galbraith. 1985. Perceptual learning styles: Implications and techniques for the practitioner. Lifelong Learning 8(4):20-23. Keefe, J. W. 1987. Learning style theory and practice. Reston, Va.: National Association of Secondary School Principals. Kolb, D. A. 1984. Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. Kolb, D. A. 1985. Learning-style inventory: Self-scoring inventory and interpretation booklet. 2nd ed. Boston: McBer. McCarthy, B. 1980. The 4MAT system: Teaching to learning styles with right/left mode techniques. Barrington, Ill.: EXCEL. Murrell, K. L. 1987. The learning-model instrument: An instrument based on the learning model for managers. In The 1987 annual: Developing human resources, edited by J. W. Pfeiffer, 109-119. San Diego, Calif.: University Associates. Orsak, L. 1990. Learning styles versus the Rip Van Winkle syndrome. Educational Leadership 48(2):19-21. Price, G. E. 1987. Changes in learning style for a random sample of individuals ages 18 and older who responded to the productivity environmental preference survey. Paper presented at the 1987 Annual Convention of the AACD, New Orleans. ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 283 112. Price, G. E., R. Dunn, and K. Dunn. 1982. Productivity environmental preference survey: PEPS manual. Lawrence, Kan.: Price Systems. Reynolds, J. 1991. Learning and cognitive styles: Confusion over definitions and terminology. Virginia Counselors Journal 19(1):22-26, Sinatra, C. 1990. Five diverse secondary schools where learning style instruction works. Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities: International 6(3):323-34. Smith R. M. 1982. Learning how to learn: Applied theory for adults. Chicago: Follett. Svinicki, M. D., and N. M. Dixon. 1987. The Kolb model modified for classroom activities. College Teaching 35:141-46. Witkin, H. A., and D. R. Goodenough. 1981. Cognitive styles: Essence and origins. New York: International Universities Press. ~~~~~~~~ By JIM REYNOLDS and MARTIN GERSTEIN Jim Reynolds is an associate professor and counselor at Northern Virginia Community College, Alexandria, Virginia. Martin Gerstein is an associate professor of education at Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, Virginia. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Clearing House is the property of Heldref Publications and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Clearing House, Nov/Dec92, Vol. 66 Issue 2, p122, 5p, 2 diagrams. Item Number: 9705041279 Result 110 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 89 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Psychometric properties of the revised Grasha Riechmann Student Learning Style Scales. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies Source: Educational & Psychological Measurement, Feb96, Vol. 56 Issue 1, p166, 7p, 2 charts Author(s): Ferrari, Joseph R.; Wesley, Joseph C. Abstract: Examines the psychometric properties of the revised GrashaRiechmann's Student Learning Style Scales. Factor analyses of items and scales; Analyses of the student subjects learning styles. AN: 9603202453 ISSN: 0013-1644 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF THE REVISED GRASHA-RIECHMANN STUDENT LEARNING STYLE SCALES The 60-item version of Grasha and Riechmann's Student Learning Style Scales (six scales, 10 items per scale) was administered to a large sample of college freshmen on each of three campuses (total N = 870) in the northeast. The Participative, Avoidant, and Collaborative scales showed acceptable internal consistency, but the Dependent, Independent, and Competitive scales did not. Factor analyses of items and scales produced no solution approximating simple structure in any sample. Neither items nor scales yielded a factor pattern resembling the theoretical structure postulated by Grasha and Riechmann in any sample, although scale scores in two samples yielded a Participative-Avoidant factor that is one of the theoretical dimensions. Properties of the 60-item version are thus very similar to those reported for an earlier 90-item version. In the 1960s and 1970s, aptitude-treatment interaction captured the imagination of many educational psychologists. With aptitude variously defined as abilities, cognitive/developmental readinesses, conceptual levels, personality traits, and learning styles, aptitude-treatment experimentation flourished (for interesting summaries of this body of work, see Keefe, 1987, and Schmeck, 1983). Compared to other constructs, learning styles fared poorly in these studies (Curry, 1990; Goldstein & Bokoros, 1992; Moran, 1991; Westman, 1993). As advocates of learning styles attempted to bolster their position (e.g., Dunn, 1987; Keefe & Ferrell, 1990), critics such as Moran (1991) called for more adequate theory and measurement: "There is a need for rigorous conceptual and empirical analysis (including psychometric validation) of the construct of learning style to avoid the danger that over-extension of this term will weaken its theoretical foundations" (p. 239). This report focuses on the current version of a measure first developed around 1970, namely Grasha and Riechmann's (Riechmann & Grasha, 1974) rationally constructed inventory of learning styles. Proceeding on the assumption that learning styles were describable in terms of three bipolar dimensions--dependent rs. independent, participative vs. avoidant, and collaborative rs. competitive--Grasha and Riechmann devised items to assess each pole of each dimension, six scales in all. At first they used eight items per scale, but soon developed a 90-item inventory (with 15 items per scale and a 5-point Likert response format) (Riechmann & Grasha, 1974; see also Hruska & Grasha, 1982). Ferrell (1983) administered the 90-item inventory along with other objective self-report measures of learning style to a sample of high school seniors (N = 471) and a sample of community college students (N = 260). Her data showed clearly that the instruments had one shortcoming in common: None of them was able to produce a factor structure corresponding to the dimensions it was supposed to measure. Grasha and Riechmann then abbreviated their inventory to the 60-item form assessed here (10 items per scale; the form can be obtained from A. F. Grasha, Dept. of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH 45221). Representative items are, for Independent, "I like to develop my own ideas about course content"; for Avoidant, "I often daydream during class"; for Collaborative, "Working with other students on class projects is something I enjoy"; for Dependent, "Teachers should state exactly what they expect from students"; for Competitive, "To stand out in my classes, I try to do assignments better than other students"; and for Participative, "Classroom activities generally are worthwhile." The purpose of the present study was to examine the factor structure and other psychometric features of the 60-item version. Method Three campuses provided subjects; designated A, B, and C, they differed appreciably in size and selectivity (Kelly & Quinlan, 1993). Sampling and recruiting of subjects, and the inducements offered for their participation, were dissimilar on the three campuses. Time and circumstances of data collection differed as well. Such dissimilarities render between-campus comparisons equivocal, but are advantageous for psychometric analysis. For example, if converging patterns are found in disparate samples from the target population, inferences are strengthened accordingly. Subjects From each campus, one sample of students was obtained whose college career began in September 1992. The Campus A sample consisted of 375 students (14% of entering freshmen), the Campus B sample had 171 students (15%), and the Campus C sample had 324 students (57%). Women outnumbered men by at least 2:1 in the student body at all three campuses (Kelly & Quinlan, 1993) and in each of these samples outnumbered men by approximately 3:1. In work with the 90-item version of their instrument, Hruska and Grasha (1982) reported "little or no sex differences in styles" (p. 83). Responses from the women and men were therefore combined in the analysis of results. Procedure Sampling and recruiting. At Campus A, freshmen enrolled in introductory psychology were invited to fill out a questionnaire containing the Grasha- Riechmann items as an alternative way to earn optional extra credit in the course. At Campus B, 555 freshmen who had expressed an interest in research participation received a cover letter, a statement of consent, which included a waiver granting the investigators access to the registrar's records, and the questionnaire. The letter promised confidentiality and a written report of results, and asked the prospective subject to complete the questionnaire and return it via campus mail within 3 weeks. At Campus C, freshmen participating in an orientation program were asked to fill out the questionnaire. Data collection. All subjects signed a consent form approved by the respective campus human subjects committee, and all completed the same questionnaire, which also contained Berzonsky's (1989) measure of identity styles and Solomon and Rothblum's (1984) measures of academic procrastination and reasons for procrastinating. At Campus C, subjects' grade point averages were provided by college officials at the end of the academic year. Statistical analysis. The learning styles data were analyzed using SPSS programs: internal consistency data were obtained via the RELIABILITY package, factor patterns via the FACTOR package (SPSS, 1990, chapters 26 and 21, respectively). Results Reliability data are summarized in Table 1. The Avoidant, Collaborative, and Participative scales showed marginally acceptable estimates of reliability (seven of the nine values of Cronbach's alpha are above .70, and negative interitem rs are rare), whereas the other scales yielded inadequate estimates (all nine values of coefficient alpha are .70 or less, and together the three scales yield 62 negative interitem rs). When Briggs and Cheek's (1986) rule is applied so that items measuring the same construct should intercorrelate >.20, the Avoidant, Collaborative, and Participative scales taken together meet this criterion about twice as often (59% of rs) as the other three scales taken together (28% of rs). Factor analysis of items was attempted via direct oblimin rotation (which allows for correlated factors) for data from each campus. No approximation to simple structure emerged in data from any campus; in each case, the program (SPSS, 1990, pp. 335-336) terminated after 23 or more iterations. Factor analysis of scale scores with varimax rotation (SPSS, 1990, pp. 331334) was attempted twice, once seeking a six-factor solution and once seeking simple structure. The six-factor solution was uninterpretable because of multiple salient loadings exhibited by one or more scales in each sample. Results of the simple structure analyses were not much better; they are shown in Table 2. Simple structure was not produced in any sample. A bipolar Participative vs. Avoidant factor did emerge in the data from Campuses A and B, but not in data from Campus C. The Grasha-Riechmann rationale specifies two other bipolar factors as well (i.e., Dependent rs. Independent and Collaborative vs. Competitive) (Riechmann & Grasha, 1974), but these factors did not appear. In data from Campus C, scale scores were correlated with grade point average earned during the freshman year. Pearson rs were significant only for Participative (r = .23, p < .01) and Avoidant (r = -.27, p < .01) scales. Discussion Generic caveats regarding learning styles have been issued by Curry (1990), Moran (1991), and others; the results obtained in the current investigation bear them out, at least as far as the Grasha-Riechmann scales (Riechmann & Grasha, 1974) are concerned. Three of the six scales appear to be defective because of low reliability estimates. Consequently, the 60-item form examined here exhibits the same serious shortcoming Ferrell (1983) found in the 90-item form--that is, the scales do not yield a clear, stable factor structure congruent with the theoretical structure (Hruska & Grasha, 1982; Riechmann & Grasha, 1974). Another similarity between Ferrell's (1983) findings and the present results is the bipolar Participative vs. Avoidant factor in data from both of her samples, and from Campuses A and B here. This factor shows some stability (it has appeared in data from at least four samples) and, at the campus where scholastic records were available, both of its poles correlate significantly and in the expected direction with grade point average for the freshman year. Nevertheless, some reservations concerning the Participative vs. Avoidant factor are in order. First, it ought to appear in the data from Campus C, but does not. Instead, as Table 2 shows, the Participative, Collaborative, and Dependent scales all load at the same pole of Factor 1--an outcome contrary to Grasha and Riechmann's (1974) theory. Second, although both Participative and Avoidant styles correlate significantly with grade point average (Hruska & Grasha, 1982, found the same pattern using the 90-item version), measures of other nonintellective constructs yield slightly larger than those observed here. Wolfe and Johnson (in press), for example, obtained rs of .34 for conscientiousness and .38 for self-control, and Schuerger and Kuna (1987) reported that conscientiousness measured during high school correlates .28 with cumulative grade point average subsequently earned in college. Furnham (1992) presented evidence that learning styles are entirely reducible to personality traits and recommended that educational psychologists abandon learning styles in favor of trait theories. His point has received support both from data arrays (including the present one), showing that certain measures of learning style lack psychometric adequacy, and from studies demonstrating that traits do a better job of predicting performance. Although we agree with Furnham, it is possible that the Grasha-Riechmann scales (Riechmann & Grasha, 1974) can be "salvaged" by improving their technical adequacy. Revision of the Dependent, Independent, and Competitive scales to make them internally consistent would have to be the first step. The constraints that these scales must not overlap with the Participative, Avoidant, and Collaborative scales, and must not overlap with each other, make this a difficult step. The next, and larger, challenge is to demonstrate that the six scales yield the three bipolar factors specified by Grasha and Riechmann's theory. Finally, if congruence is achieved, it has to be shown that the factors are stable and capable of withstanding multitrait-multimethod assessment. Until such thoroughgoing refinement is carried out, conventional measures of ability and personality will continue to have more utility than learning style inventories. This report was presented at the 1994 Eastern Psychological Association convention in Providence, RI. Correspondence about this article should be directed to Joseph R. Ferrari, Department of Psychology, DePaul University, 2219 North Kenmore Avenue, Chicago, IL 60614-3504. The data described here can be made available via Internet. Contact Raymond Wolfe, Department of Psychology, SUNY College, Geneseo, NY 14454-1401 (e-mail: Wolfe @UNO.CC. geneseo.edu). Table 1 Internal Consistency Estimates for Learning Style Scales in Data From Three Samples: Coefficient Alpha and Summary of Interitem rs Learning styles Independent Coefficient alpha Number of negative interitem rs Range of interitem rs Number of interitem rs > .2 Avoidant Coefficient alpha Number of negative interitem rs Range of interitem rs Number of interitem rs >.2 Collaborative Coefficient alpha Number of negative interitem rs Range of interitem rs Number of interitem rs > .2 Dependent Coefficient alpha Number of negative interitem rs Range of interitem rs Number of interitem rs > .2 Competitive Coefficient alpha Number of negative interitem rs Range of interitem rs Number of interitem rs > .2 Participative Coefficient alpha Number of negative interitem rs Range of interitem rs Number of interitem rs > .2 Table 2 Campus A (n = 375) .51 7 -.07 to .29 Campus B (n = 171) Campus C (n = 324) .57 7 -.23 to .40 .55 8 -.07 to .31 4 10 11 .73 .77 .68 0 .01 to .46 1 -.13 to .58 0 .04 to .38 24 31 20 .75 .73 .77 0 .04 to .44 1 -.01 to .67 0 .09 to .48 33 25 31 .55 .44 .60 10 -.14 to .36 7 -.11 to.35 5 -.14 to.40 12 1 13 .70 .70 .70 4 -.07 to .42 3 -.09 to .50 4 -.04 to .47 21 19 21 .73 .69 .76 0 .06 to .45 1 -.00 to .41 0 .13 to .44 25 19 31 Factor Analysis of Learning Style Scale Scores in Three Samples: Varimax Rotation Learning styles Independent Avoidant Collaborative Dependent Competitive Participative Campus A (n = 375) Factors 1 2 3 -.10 -.03 .71 .84 .34 .54 -.18 .91 -.24 .05 .35 -.71 .88 .07 .03 .03 .64 .20 Campus B (n = 171) Factors 1 2 .23 -.86 .48 .06 .01 .87 -.10 .03 -.28 .78 .75 .22 3 .85 -.10 -.44 -.26 .18 .16 Campus C (n = 324) Factors 1 2 .36 -.42 .80 .73 .16 .88 .57 .63 .10 .23 .75 -.05 References Berzonsky, M.D. (1989). Identity style: Conceptualization and measurement. Journal of Adolescent Research, 4, 268-282. Briggs, S. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1986). The role of factor analysis in the development and evaluation of personality scales. Journal of Personality, 54, 106-148. Curry, L. (1990). A critique of the research on learning styles. Educational Leadership, 48(2), 50-55. Dunn, R. (1987). Research on instructional environments: Implications for student achievement and attitudes. Professional School Psychology, 21, 4352. Ferrell, B. G. (1983). A factor analytic comparison of four learning-styles instruments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 33-39. Furnham, A. (1992). Personality and learning style: A study of three instruments. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 429-438. Goldstein, M. B., & Bokoros, M. A. (1992). Tilting at windmills: Comparing the Learning Style Inventory and the Learning Style Questionnaire. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 701-708. Hroska, S. R., & Grasha, A. F. (1982). The Grasha-Riechmann Student Learning Style Scales. In J. Keefe (Ed.), Student learning styles and brain behavior (pp. 81-86). Reston, VA: National Association of Secondary School Principals. Keefe, J. W. (1987). Learning style: Theory and practice. Reston, VA: National Association of Secondary School Principals. Keefe, L W., & Ferrell, B. G. (1990). Developing a defensible learning style paradigm. Educational Leadership, 48(2), 57-6L Kelly, M, & Quinlan, L. (1993). College admissions data handbook, 19931994: Northeast region. Concord, MA: Orchard House. Moran, A. (1991). What can learning styles research learn from cognitive psychology? Educational Psychology, 11,239-245. Riechmann, S. W., & Grasha, A. F. (1974). A rational approach to the construct validity of a student learning style scales instrument. Journal of Psychology, 87, 213-223. Schuerger, J. M, & Kuna, D. L. (1987). Adolescent personality and school and college performance: A follow-up study. Psychology in the Schools, 24, 281-285. Schmeck, R. R. (1983). Learning styles of college students. In R. F. Dillon & R. R. Schmeck (Eds.), Individual differences in cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 233-279). New York: Academic Press. Solomon, L. J., & Rothblum, E. D. (1984). Academic procrastination: Frequency and cognitive-behavioral correlates. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31,503-509. SPSS, Inc. (1990). SPSS base system user's guide. Chicago: Author. Westman, A. S. (1993). Learning styles are content specific and probably influenced by content areas studied. Psychological Reports, 73, 512-514. Wolfe, R. N, & Johnson, S. D. (in press). Personality as a predictor of college performance. Educational and Psychological Measurement. ~~~~~~~~ By JOSEPH R. FERRARI, DePaul University , SUZANNE M, BAMONTO, University of Oregon and BRETI L. BECK, Bloomsburg University By JOSEPH C. WESLEY, RAYMOND N. WOLFE, AND CARRIE N. ERWIN, State University of New York, College at Geneseo ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Educational & Psychological Measurement is the property of Sage Publications Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Educational & Psychological Measurement, Feb96, Vol. 56 Issue 1, p166, 7p, 2 charts. Item Number: 9603202453 Result 89 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 115 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Personality types, learning styles and educational goals. Subject(s): LEARNING -- Technique; LEARNING strategies Source: Educational Psychology, 1991, Vol. 11 Issue 3/4, p217, 22p, 4 charts, 5 diagrams Author(s): Miller, Alan Abstract: Outlines a personality typology, provides a coherent system within which to construe and conduct research upon learning styles and the implications of theory for educational goals couched in terms of learning styles. Learning styles; `New' personality model; Cognitive dimension; Affective dimension; Conative dimension; Model of personality types; Versatility and personality dynamics; Implications for educational goals. AN: 9707160494 ISSN: 0144-3410 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] PERSONALITY TYPES, LEARNING STYLES AND EDUCATIONAL GOALS ABSTRACT Attempts to broaden conceptions of learning styles to represent more adequately individual differences in motivation/emotion, as well as cognition, are limited by a paucity of relevant theory. Personality theories should, but do not, provide a satisfactory conceptual framework for this endeavour. In an attempt to remedy this situation, a new personality typology is outlined which, it is argued, provides a coherent system within which to construe and conduct research upon learning styles. The implications of the theory for educational goals, couched in teens of learning styles, also are discussed. One limitation of education, especially higher education, is that it overemphasises analytical, intellectual (cognitive) training at the expense of affective and conative development (Collier, 1988). Similarly, many well-established conceptions of `learning styles', such as Pask's serialist (operation)-holist (comprehension) distinction, reflect this cognitive emphasis (Entwistle, 1981). However, there is more to learning, both as a process and as a goal, than mere cognition. At the very least, one needs to consider the effect of motivation and emotion on cognitive development. Or, to take the matter further, perhaps there is a need to give equal weight to affective and conative, as well as cognitive, development in formulating educational goals. In other words, it could be argued that some form of comprehensive personality development should be the central focus of education (Rauste-Von Wright, 1986). If one accepts this line of argument, however, one is left with the problem of, amongst other things, reconceptualising the notion of `learning style' to reflect these broader goals. Whilst the idea of a personality-based learning style is not new, I shall argue here that those that have been developed are, in various ways, inadequate to the task. The paper begins, therefore, with a brief review of such learning styles, followed by an outline of a new personality model which, I believe, provides a more useful conceptual basis for understanding individual differences in learning. A more detailed discussion of this theory, along with its conceptual and empirical roots, is available in Miller (1988, 1991). Learning Styles Carl Jung's personality theory is commonly used as a conceptual basis for learning styles, either directly, through the use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, or indirectly, as a source of insights about individual differences (Huff et al., 1986). It is appropriate, therefore, to start with an evaluation of this theory. Jung's typology (1923) is derived from three dimensions; a basic attitude (extraversion-introversion) and two functional (sensation-intuition, thinking-feeling) dimensions. Unfortunately, the rambling nature of Jung's writing lends itself to various interpretations, or multilations, as Caine et al. (1981) put it. For instance, the extraversion-introversion dimension has been depicted, on the one hand, as a direction-of-interest concept, one that contrasts an external with an internal orientation to life. Thus the extravert is said to subordinate inner life to external necessity with attention being directed at `objective' happenings. The extravert responds to external demands rather than inner promptings. Introverts, however, are depicted by Jung as being quite the opposite, concerned primarily with their own subjective reality, rather than with objective reality. On the other hand, some elements of Jung's writing on the matter have led others to interpret extraversion-introversion in terms of individual differences in surgency and impulsiveness' more a question of temperament than interest. Attempts to disentangle this conceptual confusion have led to the conclusion that the predilection of individual theorists for their particular interpretation of the dimension precludes any consensus on the matter (Caine et al., 1981). Depending on one's usage, therefore, extraversion-introversion can refer to individual differences in `social extraversion' (temperament/emotionality) or `direction-of-interest' (motivation). In constructing `learning styles', it is important to recognise the difference between these psychological domains, especially if one wishes to modify preferred learning styles in some way. A temperamental interpretation implies a more genetically-based and, hence, less modifiable, personality characteristic than does the motivational interpretation. In my own theorizing, to be discussed shortly, I have separated the two interpretations, locating them within different personality domains. Jung's two functional dimensions represent attempts to account for individual differences in perception and judgment, with unsatisfactory results it seems. Thus, the perceptual dimension contrasts sensation (perception by means of the senses, preference for realism, concern with objective factual detail, acute observation), with intuition (perception by means of insight, preference for patterns and wholes, and imagination over facts). Similarly, the judgment dimension contrasts thinking (analytic, logical, objective judgments; a concern with value-free `truth') with feeling, (value judgments predominate; sensitivity to explanations in value terms) (McCaulley, 1981). Unfortunately, the complex nature of Jung's `functions' has created a number of problems (Storr, 1973). For instance, the separation of irrational (perception) from rational (judgment) functions seems odd in light of modern views on information processing. As I've argued in Miller (1991), one would expect, for instance, that someone who is analytic in perceptual style would be analytic in memory and thought. In other words, there appears to be little ground for assuming, as does Jung, that individual differences in perception and thought lie on orthogonal dimensions. A more reasonable formulation, therefore, would be to contrast sensation/thinking (akin to an analytical style) with intuition/feeling (holistic style). Such a re-organisation seems to `work' reasonably well for sensation/thinking since both functions emphasise an analytic concern for detail and logical thought, yet they do not refer to a `pure' cognitive style since both include conative factors. Thus, sensing is said to involve a preference for hard realistic facts while thinking is associated with objectivity and tough-mindedness. Similarly, while intuition, clearly, is couched in terms of holistic perception, feeling is virtually a pure description of subjectivity. In other words, Jung fails to separate his three dimensions, confounding his `functions' with extraversion-introversion, the basic attitudes to life. Empirical support for this latter contention comes, for instance, from Coan's (1979) finding of a significant correlation between objectivity-subjectivity and thinkingfeeling, as well as Forisha's (1983) conclusion that sensation/ thinking is correlated with objectivity, while subjectivity is related to intuition/feeling. It follows that, without considerable modification, Jung's theory does not serve as a coherent basis for conceptualising learning styles. As an example of a learning style model that has been influenced by Jung, I shall focus on Kolb (1984) who recognises two orthogonal, `adaptive orientations': prehension (the grasping or salting hold of experience) and transformation (the manipulation of experience). Both dimensions reflect a cognitive emphasis, corresponding as they do to Piaget's figurative (prehension) and operative (transformation) aspects of thought (Kolb, 1984, p. 41). In turn, the prehension dimension contrasts apprehension and comprehension modes, while reflective and active modes of transformation are recognised. Combinations of these adaptive modes or learning styles are said to result in the acquisition of different forms of knowledge (Fig. 1, Table 1). Kolb's concrete experience (CE)--abstract conceptualization (AC) dimension appears to be internally consistent as a cognitive style that contrasts analytical-verbal (AC) with holistic-concrete (CE) modes. Thus, Kolb's association of AC with Jung's sensing and thinking, and CE with feeling and intuition, is consistent with my earlier comments on Jung. However, reference to the valuing of relationships to others (CE) and the preference for scientific rigour (AC), which are conative elements, suggests that Kolb, too, tends to confound conation with cognition (Table I). When we turn to the active experimentation (AE)-reflective observation (RO) dimension, it is evident that Kolb wishes to make an external-internal distinction, akin to Jung's direction-ofinterest conception of introversion-extraversion. In fact, Kolb takes great pains in stressing his interest in Jung's epistemological distinctions, rather than the later interpretations of Jung's work that focus on social extraversion (Kolb, 1984, p. 53). Thus, AE is said to reflect a practical, pragmatic, activism, one that emphasises doing rather than observing. In contrast, the RO mode is one that favours understanding over action, and truth over utility. The fact that the RO mode is said to enjoy intuitive thought, and to rely on one's own thoughts and feelings in forming opinions (qualities also ascribed to the CE mode) is, perhaps, a minor quibble with Kolb's conception. I conclude, therefore, that Kolb's model is a good reading of recent notions of the analytic-holistic cognitive style and the extraceptive-intraceptive version of Jung's extraversion-introversion. There are, however, two deficiencies in Kolb's model and similar models of learning styles currently in vogue (Huff et al., 1986). First, the descriptions of each dimension have very little syntax. What I mean by this is that the dimensions lack theoretical development, especially in light of the massive empirical literature pertinent to them. Secondly, Kolb's model, as well as others, omit the darker, more intractable elements of human personality found, for instance, in Eysenck's conceptions of neuroticism and psychoticism (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985), both of which are important in the educational process. In my own personality model, a description of which follows, I have tried to deal with both of these issues. A 'New' Personality Model The search for adequate conceptions of personality is an ancient one. However, until recently, many observers of the scene found the end-products of all this effort profoundly disappointing. Millon (1981), for instance, observed that little consensus had been achieved about the number or kind of dimensions needed to represent personality adequately, a consequence of the excessive subjectivity of researchers whom, he suggested, simply invent traits to suit their predilections. The net result is that "catalogs of convenience have replaced meaningful taxonomies of personality traits amongst most of the current generation of social and personality researchers" (McCrae & Costa, 1987). One interesting feature of this research, however, is that some traits seem to recur as their authors discover ancient precedent or simply reinvent notions temporarily forgotten. The possibility that these recurring traits may represent basic or fundamental human characteristics is recognised in the substantial degree of unanimity emerging recently around what have become known as the `robust' personality traits. Conley (1985), Digman and Inouye (1986), and McCrae and Costa (1985, 1987) summarise extensive recent work in the United States that has confirmed the earlier findings of, amongst others, Norman (1963) that the personality domain can be represented adequately by five robust factors (Table II). In an independent review originating in Britain, of work along the same lines, Brand (1984) comes to similar conclusions (Table II). The difference between the two models is due to the preference of some researchers for splitting the energy surgency/extraversion factor into sociability and willfulness components (McCrae & Costa, 1987). What I have done in my own theorising is to adopt these five (or six) factors as hypotheses about the nature of basic (genotypic) traits and use some of them in developing a three dimensional typology. One problem with trait lists, such as those in Table 2, is that there is no indication of meaningful relationships amongst the traits listed. In constructing a personality model one has to find some way to depict such relationships without making the typology too complex, and thereby impractical, or too simplistic. In seeking an organisational principle that would accommodate those demands, I'm attracted, along with Buss and Finn (1987), to the old tripartite system that has informed psychology over the centuries, one that recognised three psychological domains; cognition, affection and conation. The use of this system and the selection of one genotypic dimension to represent each domain results in a relatively simple, but comprehensive threedimensional typology. There are those who argue that the recognition of cognition/ affection/conation as separate processes seriously misrepresents the unity of psychological processes and, worse, once you have broken the individual into pieces you are faced with the unenviable task of putting Humpty Dumpty back together again (Bruner, 1986; Revelle, 1983; Santostefano, 1986). Rather than reverting to an outmoded way of thinking, they say, it would be better to find other ways of dealing with the multiple processes within each of us. While this is an admirable sentiment, until other ways are found, one has recourse only to more traditional approaches which involve the analytic separation of processes followed by attempts to describe their many interactions. My personality typology, therefore, is composed of cognitive, affective and conative dimensions. The Cognitive Dimension The identification of an intellectual factor as an important genotypic trait (Table II) underscored the need to include a cognitive dimension in the typology. Unfortunately, the precise composition of this factor remains controversial. From Brand's (1984) British perspective, it is depicted as general intelligence, g and assessed in terms of mental ability. In contrast, the U.S. work on the intellect factor is based, not on ability measures, but on reports of the qualities thought to be associated with intelligence, which include artistic and intellectual interests, cultural sophistication, inquiring intellect, and openness to experience. As a result, American factor analysts have had great difficulty in achieving consensus on the nature of this fifth factor, although there is agreement that it is not a matter of ability. We are presented with something of a dilemma, therefore, whether to represent the cognitive dimension as general intelligence or in terms of the non-intellective correlates of intelligence. I chose to do neither, preferring, instead, to think of the dimension in terms of cognitive styles. There are two reasons for this. First, ability and intellective conceptions of `intelligence' refer to what people are capable of or what they prefer to `cognise'. In other words, they are content aspects of personality. Unfortunately, the content domain is so large that it is difficult to decide how best to represent it. For instance, there are many kinds of `intelligence' and much dispute over the most suitable ways in which to assess each kind (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). In contrast, the stylistic aspects of behaviour (i.e. how people behave) can be represented parsimoniously by relatively few variables (Royce & Powell, 1983). Secondly, the available evidence appears to indicate that cognitive styles exhibit strong cross-situational consistency and are (according to Bem, 1983) among the most promising genotypic traits one might include in a personality typology. The most suitable cognitive style dimension almost selects itself, for as Brand (1984) notes: "A serious possibility is that there are omnipresent differences between people in whether they attend narrowly to (self-) selected aspects of reality or whether they are more broadly attentive" (p. 195). The distinction alluded to here, between cognitive narrowness and broadness, is ancient and is one that has not only recurred over the centuries, but also continues to play a major role in the way that cognitive differences are depicted (Coan, 1979). Of the many labels that could be used for this style dimension, I prefer analytic-holistic, c, a distinction also recognised by Kolb in his CE-AC dimension. In developing this analytic-holistic conception further, I have attempted to pull together many disparate empirical and conceptual elements from both the cognitive style and cognitive science literatures into a model of individual differences in cognitive processing (Miller, 1987, 1991). The latter proposes that, at each stage of cognition (Fig. 2), one can identify different cognitive styles (Fig. 3). In other words, the analytic-holistic dimension is comprised of a set of cognitive styles, each of which contributes to a consistent individual difference in cognitive processing (Table III). I should point out that there have been several attempts at this kind of analysis in the recent past, with varying degrees of success (Fowler, 1977; Kagan & Kogan, 1970; Royce & Powell, 1983). However, I believe that the model offered here is plausible as well as being useful in organising research on cognitive styles. The Affective Dimension Two of the robust traits (Table II) have an emotional flavour, namely: surgency/ energy and emotional stability/neuroticism. Indeed, they are identical to Eysenck's Extraversion and Neuroticism, respectively, which are considered to be dimensions of temperament or emotional style. The possibility of representing the affective dimension of the model in terms of emotional styles is appealing for, like cognitive styles, they offer a parsimonious way of depicting a specific personality domain. Given that two generic traits have been identified, however, the question is which should be selected for inclusion in the model. My decision has been to use both. Since the reasoning behind this is too convoluted to summarise more than briefly here, the reader is directed to Miller (1991) for more adequate details. As noted earlier, the trait dimension of `neuroticism' is considered to be an important component of personality. It appears, in the research literature, under a number of guises including `emotional instability', `anxiety' and, as I prefer to regard it, `emotionality' (Brand, 1984). Although the dimension is evidently an `emotional' one, we need to be more precise about its relationship to the concept of `emotion'. Although there are many definitions of the term `emotion' (Epstein, 1984), there is some agreement that emotions can be conceived of as having at least three components: physiological mobilisation (arousal), subjective experience (feeling) and behavioural expression (affect). One can ask two different questions about these: what combinations of arousal, feeling and affect occur, and how do people differ in the way in which these components function? The first of these questions has to do with emotional content and is about what kinds of emotions occur. The term `emotionality', however, is clearly not a matter of what emotion is being experienced, but is more closely linked to the second question: How are emotions experienced and expressed? Thus, `emotionality' might properly be seen as an emotional style. Emotional styles have been studied under the rubric of temperament, where there is some agreement that one of its major aspects is the intensity of emotional response (Goldsmith & Campos, 1982; Lerner & Lerner, 1983; Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981; Thomas, 1985). This emphasis stems, no doubt, from Allport's (1961) pre-feminist conception of temperament as: " . . . the characteristic phenomena of an individual's emotional nature, including his susceptibility of emotional stimulation, his customary strength and speed of response, the quality of his prevailing mood, and all peculiarities of fluctuation and intensity of mood . . . " (p. 34). Emotionality, therefore, may be seen as a temperamental or emotional style that reflects the intensity of emotional experience. It follows that this intensity should cut across emotional content. That is, it should be associated with a variety of emotions, both positive and negative. However, there is a tendency, in the literature, to equate `emotionality with `neuroticism'. The latter reflecting an intense experience of the more negative emotions (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). For example, Buss & Plomin (1984) regard emotionality as primordial distress, defining it as: " . . . the tendency to become upset easily and intensely. Compared to unemotional people, emotional people become distressed when confronted with emotionladen stimuli--the stresses of everyday life--and they react with higher levels of emotional arousal. It follows that they should be harder to soothe" (p. 54). Why emotionality should be conceptualised primarily in terms of distress puzzles Zuckerman (1985) who reflects that most of our theories do not account for positive emotions, but focus, instead, on what he calls the unhappy triad of fear, anxiety and depression (FAD). The reason for this is not clear, for, as Rothbart and Derryberry (1981) comment, a style definition implies that those who show intense distress would also be expected, at some other time, to show intense positive emotion. Evidence in favour of this latter assumption has been marshalled by Larsen and Diener (1987) who, in proposing the stable individual difference of affective intensity, argue that " . . . the intensity of an individual's affective responsiveness generalizes across specific emotion categories implying a general temperament dimension of emotional reactivity and variability" (p. 1). The affective dimension of my personality model, therefore, combines elements of social extraversion and neuroticism into an affective intensity dimension labelled emotional stability-instability. The Conative Dimension The term conation appears to have dropped out of common usage along with the demise of faculty psychology, being replaced by the more familiar term motivation. However, conation and conative carry implications of an effortful, striving, self-willing form of behaviour rather than the expression of some biological urge or a mechanical response to situational pressures. In other words, conation has to do with volition, a psychological concept that has languished in the back-waters of psychological theory for decades and is only now beginning to re-surface (Westcott, 1985). The selection of a conative dimension for my model is aided by Brand's (1984) observation of what he calls a certain negative tension between his will and affection factors which suggests the possibility of a higher-order will-affection dimension that is clearly conative in nature. The contrast he has in mind is between striving for autonomy/power/masculinity (will) and dependence/co-operation/femininity (affection). One of the attractive features of a will-affection dimension is that it bears a striking resemblance to William James' (1907) toughtender mindedness, a dichotomy that is said to date back to the SocraticSophist dialogues of ancient times and continues to the present day in various guises (Kimble, 1984). In conceptualising the conative dimension of the present model. I set myself the task of developing a more modern version of this grand polarity. Rather than using James' term tough-minded v. tender-minded as a label for the dimension, one that has accumulated an excess of pejorative baggage, I prefer Coan (1979) and Cotgrove's (1982) more neutral term objective-subjective. My conception of objectivity-subjectivity is firmly rooted in the intrapsychic conflict tradition in personality theory, the most notable versions of which are associated with Angyal and Bakan (Maddi, 1976). Both theorists consider that we are riven by a persistent conflict between two inherent and opposing sets of drives (what Maddi calls core tendencies). On the one hand, we are compelled to assert our individuality, to separate ourselves from others and to curtail our dependence on them (autonomy, agency). On the other hand, we experience the urge to join with others in co-operative, intimate and, often, dependent relationships (surrender, communion). In other words, human behaviour is seen to result from a continuing conflict between innate sets of self-assertive and selfabnegating drives, all of which operate at an unconscious level. Maddi explains further that, while the two great forces may seem to be irrevocably opposed, psychological health or integrity requires that some compromise be found. The most successful integration would seem to be one in which both core tendencies are represented as much as possible in living one's life. What I shall assume, therefore, is that objectivity and subjectivity represent concrete expressions of the more abstract tendencies toward agency and communion. There have been many empirical studies of the 'concrete expressions', resulting in a profusion of findings under various rubrics. The problem we face in attempting to discuss this information is finding some systematic way of doing so. For instance, amongst the terms that have been used to label motivational dispositions, one finds: motive, goal, interest, value, expectation, purpose, plan, desire, wish and intention, to name but a few. To cut a long story short (see Miller, 1991, for details), I propose that objectivity-subjectivity be seen as a motive dimension, one that is comprised of three subdimensions: power-love, emotional detachment-empathy and extraception-intraception. Thus, the objective individual is conceptualised as power-seeking, emotionally detached and prone to imposing his/her frame of reference on events. Taken to an extreme, objectivity reflects Eysenck's Psychoticism, a tough-minded, often cruel, impersonal orientation to others. In contrast, the subjective individual seeks loving, more empathetic relationships with others, an orientation aided by his/her intraceptive perspective on life. This too can be taken to an extreme, one that exhibits itself as an excessive dependency on others. The implications of these value orientations for educational methods and goals will be discussed shortly. A Model of Personality Types The three generic dimensions outlined above have been incorporated into the personality model shown in Fig. 4. In describing the personality types so generated, I find it convenient, following Cotgrove (1982), to define main types in terms of the analytic holistic and objective-subjective dimensions (Fig. 5), assuming further, that each of these main types exhibits stable and unstable variants. A brief description of the main types follows. The objective-analytic type (OA) The objective aspect of this type is reflected in the adoption of a toughminded orientation to life in which the central concern is the achievement of a sense of agency, a sense of control over oneself and one's immediate environment. This goal may be pursued by seeking power over others, or by resisting the attempts by others to exert power over oneself. In either case, the attainment of power is facilitated by establishing a degree of control over one's emotional and cognitive interactions with other people. Typically, the OA type achieves this by a process of distancing. Thus, emotional control is attained by maintaining emotional detachment, the advantage of which is that limited emotional involvement reduces the likelihood that one will be bothered by the vagaries of one's own emotions or by emotional pressure exerted by others. Similarly, cognitive control is sought through an extraspective stance. The potential confusion engendered by taking another person's viewpoint into account is thereby avoided, as are the upsetting consequences of introspection, the exploration of one's subjective world. An illusion of control is sustained, therefore, by focusing on the exterior world of objective certainty, the world of outward appearance and physical reality. As a consequence, a mechanistic world view is developed in which simple cause-effect relationships are sought as a means of understanding and control. The analytic component of the OA type can be viewed as a strategy for achieving objective ends. If one holds a mechanistic view of life, in which both people and things are subject to simple rules of physical causality, then one way to ensure the control of events is to pay painstaking attention to the minutiae of external experience. This goal is facilitated by an analytical style in which factual detail is sought in a relatively circumscribed field. Thus 'objective' facts are thought to provide the key to understanding and control, whereas intuitions and impressions are not. When these emotional and cognitive control strategies show signs of failing, and the emotional/subjective world begins to intrude, then defensive coping is brought to bear. Typically, the OA type would be inclined toward the use of articulated defenses, after the fashion of a surgeon's scalpel, carefully separating unruly emotions from thoughts so that disturbing events are made emotionally bland while remaining cognitively amenable. Characteristic forms of defence are, therefore, emotional isolation, intellectualisation and rationalisation. In summary, the OA type is empirical, reductionist, impersonal and obsessive. It follows that members of this category might be labelled reductionists, a prototypical example being the analytical scientist, one who seeks understanding and control through the collection of "objective data" in a narrowly defined segment of the external, physical world. The objective-holistic type (OH) The OH and OA types share the same objective, impersonal manipulative orientation, but power/control is achieved using different strategies. Rather than an attempt to understand and control reality by seeking factual information (as one finds in the OA type), the illusion of control is achieved by the development of schemes, theories, systems of thought and/or fantasies, all of which serve to organise and control 'reality'. In contrast to the OA type who seeks to document reality, therefore, the OH type may seek to impose a system of order on to it, or to seek to force the surrounding environment to comply with the fit into his/her model of how things should be. When the mismatch between fantasy and reality becomes too great and the OH type's conceptual schemes are threatened, then defences of a global nature, such as denial and repression, are used to suppress this unpleasant truth. Prototypical examples of this schematist category are the intellectual model builders, rationalists and philosophers who weave speculative tales about physical reality. A case in point is the philosopher-novelist Ayn Rand, famous for her philosophical system of 'objectivism' a homage to the power of will, striving and personal accomplishment. Yet, in her private life, Ayn Rand was, apparently, either incapable or unwilling to engage in introspection with the result that she understood little of her own behaviour and its effects on others. Nor was she sensitive to the emotional states of others, unless these were brought to her attention forcefully (Brander, 1986). The subjective-holistic type (SH) The subjective aspect of this type is reflected in a primary concern with establishing communion through intimate, nurturing relationships with other people and with the surrounding environment. Given this urge to blend and join, and the implication that the self of the SH type is relatively 'permeable', then there is less concern with protecting the self from the influence of others. Indeed, SH types seek to establish a sense of self by joining with others in what the more objective types derisorily refer to as 'dependent' relationships. Regardless of how one might label this behaviour, it does seem that SH types, rather than seeking power over others, strive to empower others through nurturing behaviour. This is facilitated by a well-developed cognitive and emotional empathy, all of which implies a main interest in subjective experience be that of one's own inner reality or the inner, psychological world of others. Feelings and personal impressions are given priority over the details of 'objective reality'. When subjectivity is coupled with an holistic style, then one finds a lack of interest in the analysis of personal experience and a concomitant preference for experiencing subjective reality intuitively or globally. It is possible that SH types view analysis as another form of separation, an alienating experience that they prefer to avoid. As a result, introspection (in the sense of analysis of subjective experience) is kept to a minimum, although intraception remains a major orientation. The absence of analysis and the lack of concern about emotional control results in personal reactions intruding into thought, making the latter evaluative, emotionally-tinged and intensely subjective. Thus, the romantic lives in an impressionistic, often imaginative, world of personal anecdote and unanalysed subjective experience. Given this interest in communion, and the dislike of separation, it follows that the primary fear for the SH type would be separation anxiety which, in adults, would be generated by an inability to establish intimate contact with others, especially loved ones. The defensive reaction to separation anxiety, and the unwarranted intrusion of the impersonal, objective world, would be massive repression and denial, the use of unarticulated defences so characteristic of all holistic types. The subjective-analytic type (SA) The SA type shares with the preceding SH type a primary concern with establishing communion, a focus on subjective experience and a lack of interest in, or distaste for, the objectified, impersonal world. The difference between the two types lies, however, in the strategy used to achieve communion and in their level of tolerance of separation anxiety. Thus, the adoption of an analytical style by the SA type appears to presume that contact with others is best achieved through understanding and knowledge. It is as if the subjective-holist emphasises emotional empathy while the subjective-analytist emphasises cognitive empathy. It follows that the SA type engages in the analysis of personal experience, a psychologically-minded search for the source of one's inner life and that of others. As a consequence, there is a tendency to withdraw into a reflective, narrowly preoccupied world of introspective thought at the expense of engagement in the broader reality. Since analysis has the effect of distancing oneself from the thing being analysed, I would presume that the SA type has a greater tolerance of separation anxiety than one would find in the SH type, although such tolerance would be much less than that found in objective individuals. Where intimacy is frustrated and objective reality intrudes into the introspective mindscape, then the SA type has a particular problem. Of all four types, the SA person has the greatest difficulty in summoning effective defensive coping. Their commitment to communion, with its implication for the integration of parts of the self into an homogeneous unit, mitigates against the deployment of articulated defences, while their inclination to introspective analysis prevents the use of global defenses. Thus, the SA type has difficulty in protecting himself/herself from what Smail (1984) calls the horrors of psychological honesty. Subtypes Within each of the four main types, at least two subtypes can be formed from extreme positions on the emotionality dimension. Thus, one would see, for example, emotionally stable and unstable forms of the analytic/objective (reductionist) type, and so on. The distinction afforded by these subtypes draws attention to the relative intensity of people's lives. As mentioned earlier, the emotionally unstable individual is prone to distress, reacting to the world with fear and anxiety, the implication being that defence mechanisms would play a major role in keeping this within bounds. For those who are unable to use defence mechanisms effectively, one would expect to find lives of great distress and unhappiness, as Smail (1984) has described at some length. At the opposite extreme are those who appear to react little to the world around them, an unresponsiveness that may verge on apathy. I would speculate that their problem is not so much to allay anxiety, but to convince those around them that they are actually emotionally alive, assuming of course, that they would wish to do so. Recognition of these subtypes allows us to distinguish between, for example, the emotionally unstable and defensive reductionist (the classical obsessive), and a more bland, emotionally inert reductionist. Something of the sort has been recognised in Maslow's (1966) contrast between safety science and growth science. I would speculate that safety science emanates from emotionally unstable reductionists. While growth science is associated with the emotionally stable reductionist (and others). Versatile types The present model allows one to recognise what might be called versatile types, individuals who have achieved some harmonious balance between the conflicting motives underlying objectivity-subjectivity; who are not excessively emotionally stable or unstable, and who are capable of employing both analytical and holistic styles where appropriate. Although such psychic efficiency may be rare (Hudson, 1968), it could be argued that versatility is a desirable educational goal. Whether this is, in fact, a practical ideal is a matter to which we now turn. Versatility and Personality Dynamics Versatility, the ability to adapt flexibly to life's demands, is a common theme in conceptions of the optimal personality (Coan, 1974). Intrapsychic theorists, for instance, extol the virtues of some judicious mix of agency and communion as an ideal compromise in life (Maddi, 1976). A similar sentiment is to be found in the cognitive and learning style literatures, where stylistic versatility is lauded (Entwistle, 1981).It follows that many style-based systems of teaching encourage students to make more use of styles other than those they normally prefer (Huff et al., 1986). There are claims of success in such endeavours (Kolb, 1984, p.206), but I remain skeptical. If learning styles are defined more comprehensively as personality styles (or types), then formidable obstacles stand in the way of change. It may be possible to achieve some superficial behavioural changes amongst most students, but I doubt that these would be anything other than ephemeral. This conclusion is based on what is known about the relationships between styles and personality dynamics. Far from being simple habits that can be changed at will, some believe learning styles to be complex adjustments to life that are learned early in life and remain held in place, as it were, by demands of psychodynamics (Hudson, 1968, 1970; Witkin & Goodenough, 1981). Hudson, for instance, depicts convergent and divergent styles as forms of psychological defence. If this is so, then it is likely that attempts to modify an individual's style could generate varying degrees of distress and/or hostility. The reason for this is clear enough. The control of anxiety within tolerable limits is a central feature of human adjustment (Maddi, 1976). One strategy for achieving this end is to screen everyday events for their `threat' value, using selective inattention to avoid anything troublesome that promises to disturb our peace of mind (Goleman, 1985). Over time, we develop characteristic styles of selective inattention (defences) which, in turn, form the bases of personality styles. For instance, to reiterate some earlier points, the psychodynamic thrust of each of my four main polarities appears to be: (1) the analytic style is a way of seeking certainty through the pursuit of detail within a circumscribed domain, thereby avoiding the uncertainty and attendant anxiety generated by the larger reality. (2) the holistic style seeks certainty in flights of fancy, elaborate schemes which provide an illusion of control and an escape from troublesome empirical reality; (3) the objective style focuses on the material, impersonal world thereby avoiding the anxiety created by the irrational, unpredictable world of emotion and subjectivity; (4) the subjective style, in contrast, avoids the harshness of objective, material reality in favour of the security and warmth of personal relationships. This notion of style-as-defence and the particular interpretation offered above is afforded some degree of support by research on personality disorders. (For more details on the empirical and conceptual structure of personality disorders and the reasoning behind the arguments offered here the reader is directed to Miller, 1991). For instance, many of the personality disorders recognised in DSM-III can be construed as extreme forms of my four main types (Fig. 6, Table IV). In other words, personality disorders appear to arise in cases of stylistic `specialisation' where individuals appear to have difficulty in switching styles to accommodate changing circumstances. They suffer, it seems, from an inadvertent excess of a dominant style. It is interesting to note that descriptions of each disorder are consistent with conceptions of my main types. Thus, the compulsive disorder (OA) exhibits an excessive and persistent concern with factual detail and routine coupled with a rejection of emotional involvement with others, presumably to avoid a feeling of loss of control over life events (Millon, 1981; Pollak, 1979; Shapiro' 1965). The histrionic disorder (OH), on the other hand, shows an emotionally labile, shallow, exhibitionism, one that carefully avoids the chastening influence of factual reality (Millon, 1981; Pollak, 1981; Shapiro, 1965). Recently referred to as a masochistic type, the dependent disorder (SH) couples ingenuous docility with excessive needs for affection and nurturance. Refuge from the difficulties presented by the material world is sought within a dependent relationship (Millon, 1981). Finally, the avoidant disorder (SA) is one of social anxiety, sensitive perceptiveness and excessive rumination. Such individuals seek, but do not find, security through emotional contact with others. I he harshness of the objective world is avoided through withdrawal into an introspective realm (Millon, 1981; Smail, 1984). In summary, the more stylistically `specialised' an individual, the more difficult will it be to encourage versatility. This is because specialisation serves a defensive function in protecting the individual from anxiety. Since stylistic specialisation is common amongst students, it is unlikely that many will welcome concerted efforts to modify their entrenched styles. This will be particularly true amongst those emotionally unstable students who are, according to my model, easily distressed and, therefore, heavily defended. What, then, are the implications of all this for educational goals? Implications for Educational Goals Let me begin this section with a personal anecdote. Some years ago, I had the dubious pleasure of teaching at a small agricultural college. Along with other members of my department, I shared the task of developing some psychosocial understanding amongst agricultural students destined to become advisors within the government system. The material I covered in lectures was, to my mind, relatively innocuous, a smattering of ideas from psychology. Nothing prepared me for their reaction to this modest endeavour. A small minority, less than a third, showed interest, while a middle group were quite indifferent. It was the remaining third or so who were the most interesting and disturbing, for their reaction was one of sullen hostility. To say the least, I was startled by the viciousness of it all and only later came to realise that what to me was relatively innocuous material was, to them, profoundly disturbing. It seems I was asking them to delve into the subjective-emotional realm, something that was anathema to them. I suspect that if I had been foolish enough to try to modify their personal styles, rather than simply presenting some ideas for discussion, the reaction would have been even more negative. This experience, which has been repeated in other situations, together with the implications of the above model, leads me to the following conclusions. First, I believe that wholesale attempts to encourage stylistic versatility in all students is not only a waste of time and resources, but also can be psychologically damaging. Extremely specialised students should be left alone, secure within the confines of their dominant mode. Certainly, attempts should be made to adjust teaching to suit these styles, but not to change them. It follows that versatility is a reasonable goal for those who are already predisposed to it. In other words, to those that hath shall be given. The agenda for research, in such circumstances, would be to find ways of identifying the specialised and the proto-versatile, thereby determining who should be left alone. Secondly, it would seem that treating learning styles as cognitive styles, bereft of affective, motivational and defensive implications, is naive. Teaching systems based on such assumptions, therefore, are likely to be ingenuous and, possibly, dangerous. Thirdly, many ethical questions are raised by attempts to modify styles, personality or otherwise. Separating out relatively flexible students for special treatment smacks of elitism and would be controversial. Similarly, any tinkering with personality styles would require informed consent, some ongoing discussion between teacher and learner about the purpose and methods of education. The last time I heard of this happening on any scale was in 1968. Finally, a genuine concern for personality development as a goal of education would require that teaching becomes a form of counselling over and above the mere transmission of information. This is unlikely to happen since the traditional separation of `intellect' from `personality' is too entrenched in academic circles. However, versatility is, I believe, an eminently sensible educational goal, one that is achievable, perhaps, in isolated pockets where there are teachers who have the necessary understanding and commitment to their students. Correspondence: A. Miller, Psychology Department, University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, NB, Canada E3B 6E4. TABLE I. Kolb's learning orientations[1] 1. An orientation toward concrete experience focuses on being involved in experiences and healing with immediate human situation in a personal way. It emphasises feeling as opposed to thinking; as a concern with the uniqueness and complexity of present reality opposed to theories and generalisations; an intuitive, 'artistic' approach as opposed to the systematic, scientific approach to problems. People with concreteexperience orientation enjoy and are good at relating to others. They are often good intuitive decision makers and function well in unstructured situations. The person with this orientation values relating to people and being involved in real situations, and has an open-minded approach to life. 2. An orientation toward reflective observation focuses on understanding the meaning of ideas and situations by carefully observing and impartially describing them. It emphasises understanding as opposed to practical application; a concern with what is true or how things happen as opposed to what will work; an emphasis on reflection as opposed to action. People with a reflective orientation enjoy intuiting the meaning of situations and ideas, and are god at seeing their implications. They are good at looking at things from different perspective and at appreciating different points of view. They like to rely on their own thoughts and feelings to form opinions. People with this orientation value patience, impartiality, and considered, thoughtful judgment. 3. An orientation toward abstract conceptualisation focuses on using logic, ideas and concepts. It emphasises thinking as opposed to feeling: a concern with building general theories as opposed to intuitively understanding unique, specific areas; as opposed to an artistic approach to problems. A person with an abstract-conceptual orientation enjoys and is good at systematic planning, manipulation of abstract symbols, and quantitative analysis. People with this orientation value precision, the rigor and discipline of analysis ideas, and the aesthetic quality of a neat conceptual system. 4. An orientation toward active experimentation focuses on actively influencing people and changing situations. It emphasises practical applications as opposed to reflective understanding; a pragmatic concern with what works as opposed to what is absolute truth; an emphasis on doing as opposed to observing. People with an active-experimentation enjoy and are good at getting things accomplished. They also value having an influence on the environment around them and like to see results. [1] From Kolb (1984). TABLE II. The robust personality traits[*] USA UK Surgency talkative-silent sociable-reclusive adventurous- cautions Energy talkative-silent sociable-unsociable adventurous- cautions Agreeableness good natured-irritable mild-headstrong co-operative-negativistic Affection trusting-suspicious affectionate-hostile co-operative-uncooperative Conscientiousness responsible-undependable persevering-quitting tidy-careless Conscience responsible-irresponsible persistence-quitting order-disorder Emotional stability calm-anxious composed-excitable poised-nervous Neuroticism calm-anxious composed-excitable poised-nervous Intellect intellectual-non-reflective imaginative-simple artistically sensitive-insensitive Intelligence general intelligence ('g') cognitive ability analytical capacity Will independent-dependent dominating-submissive strong willed-weak [*] Modified from Digman & Inouye (1986) and Brand (1984). TABLE III. Relationship between cognitive styles Cognitive process Analytic style Pattern recognition Selective attention Analytic Field independence Representation Organisation Verbal/analytic Conceptual differentiation Retrieval Classification Analogical reasoning Judgment Convergence Serial Tight Actuarial Holistic Field dependence Visual/analog Conceptual holism Divergence Holistic Loose Intuitive TABLE IV. Personality disorders Disorder Objective/subjective Compulsive objective: preoccupied with self-control; avoids introspection and attains little self-insight; seeks emotional detachment; little ability to express warmth and tenderness; rigidly structures environment; overly concerned with rules, procedures and formalities. Antisocial objective: power-oriented, tough, unsentimental; obtains gratification by humiliating and dominating others, callous, insensitive and vindictive; absence of self-insight; contemptuous of intimacy, compassion, emotional warmth. Paranoid objective: inordinate fear of losing independence and power to shape events in accord with grandiose sense of self; mistrusts others, seeks to avoid entrapment by becoming hard, obdurate, vigilant; lacks self-insight, compassion, warmth. Narcissistic objective: shows interpersonal exploitativeness, uses others to indulge self; emotional detachment, low empathy, lack of regard for others; avoids introspection and lacks self-insight. Histrionic objective: avoids introspection; experiences a barren intrapsychic world, an inner emptiness; compensates by actively seeking attention, reassurance; manipulative, seductive; intensely extraceptive. Dependent subjective: overly strong needs for affection and nurturance; non-competitive, avoids autonomy; subordinates own needs to those of others; emotionally warm, tender, considerate; friendly, obliging, generous, obsequious. Avoidant subjective: desires affection and acceptance but socially anxious; empathetic; intensely sensitive to rejection, humiliation; uncertain about to the introspective world of thoughts and feelings. Disorder Analytic/holistic Compulsive analytic: exhibits a narrow, small-minded outlook; a preoccupation with trivial detail and objective 'facts', all of which preclude the possibility of developing a broader perspective. Antisocial analytic and holistic: most exhibit clarity and logic in their thinking (implying analytic capacity) but rarely exhibit foresight; the success of some variants implies a versatile cognitive style. Paranoid analytic and holistic: lives in a world of fantasy and delusion composed of fixed beliefs, and unrealistic perceptions (holistic); hypervigilant, intense and narrow search for confirmation of expectations (analytic). Narcissistic holistic: preoccupied with pretentious, unrealistic fantasies; takes liberties with the 'facts' in refashioning 'reality' to his/her own liking. Imaginative, cognitively expansive. Histrionic holistic: prone to flights of (romantic) fantasy; thought processes scattered; little interest in careful analysis; pays fleeting attention to detail; inability t think in a concentrated, logical fashion. Dependent holistic: tends to be naive, unperceptive, uncritical; inclined to see only the pleasant side of troubling events; minimally introspective, a pollyanna perspective on life. Avoidant analytic: sensitive, acutely perceptive observer; hyperalert to feelings and intentions of others; vigilant scanning for signs of rejection; tends to be excessively introspective and self-conscious. Disorder Emotional stability/instability Compulsive unstable: sits stop a powder-keg of inner turmoil, his/her greatest task being to control the intense feelings that lurk below a cloak of respectability. Commonly prone to anxiety disorders. Antisocial unstable: frequent signs of emotional distress and dysphoria; an irrascible temper that flares easily into fury and vindictiveness. Paranoid unstable: finds it difficult to relax; appears tense, edgy, irritable, disputations, factious abrasive; prone to extremes of mood and general anxiety disorders. Narcissistic stable: affect is generally relaxed; a pervasive sense of well-being; a buoyancy of mood; does not characteristically develop anxiety disorders. Histrionic unstable: lively, dramatic and exhibitionistic; highly labile emotions, overly reactive, easily excitable, capricious and given to angry outbursts or tantrums; intensely expressive. Dependent stable: a pacific temperament; docile, friendly, but with a tendency to maudlin sentimentality. Avoidant unstable: experiences recurrent anxiety and mood disharmonies, affective dysphoria, easily distressed by rejection; upset by lack of social ease; prone to anxiety disorders. Note: based on description from Disorders of Personality: DSM--III Axis II by T, Millon, 1981 (New York, Wiley). FIG. 1. Kolb's model of learning styles (from Kolb, 1984). FIG. 2. An information processing model of cognition. FIG. 3. A model of cognitive styles and cognitive processes (from Miller, 1987). DIAGRAM: FIG. 4. A three-dimensional model of personality. DIAGRAM: FIG. 5. The four main types (A = analytic; H = holistic; O = objective; S = subjective). DIAGRAM: FIG. 6. Personality types nd disorders. REFERENCES ALLPORT, G. (1961) Pattern and Growth in Personality (New York, Holt Rinehart & Winston) BEM, D. (1983) Toward a response style theory of persons in situations, in: M. PAGE (Ed.) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, pp. 201-231 (Lincoln, Nebraska, University of Nebraska Press). BRAND, C. (1984) Personality dimensions: an overview of modern trait psychology, in J. NICHOLSON & H. BELOFF, (Eds) Psychology Survey 5, pp. 175-209 (Leicester, British Psychological Society). BRANDEN, B. (1986) The Passion of Ayn Rand (New York, Doubleday). BRUNER, J. (1986) Thought and emotion: can Humpty Dumpty be put back together again, in: D. BEARISON & H. ZIMILES (Eds) Thought and Emotion: developmental perspectives (New Jersey, Erlbaum). Buss, A. & FINN, S. (1987) Classification of personality traits, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, pp. 432-444. Buss, A. & PLOMIN, R. (1984) Temperament: Early Developing Personality Traits (Hillsdale, New Jersey, Erlbaum). CAINE, T., WIJENSINGHE, O. & WINTER, D. (1981) Personal Styles in Neurosis: Implications for Small Group Psychotherapy and Behaviour Therapy (London, Routledge & Kegan Paul). COAN, R. (1974) The Optimal Personality: An Empirical and Theoretical Analysis (New York, Columbia University Press). COAN, R. (1979) Psychologists: Personal and Theoretical Pathways (New York, Irvington). COLLIER, G. (1988) Higher education and the critique of values, Journal of Moral Education, 17, pp. 21-26. CONLEY, J. (1985) Longitudinal stability of personality traits: a multitrait-multimethod-multioccasion analysis, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, pp. 1266-1282. COTGROVE, S. (1982) Catastrophe or Cornucopia (Chichester, Wiley). DIGMAN, J. & INOUYE, J. (1986) Further specification of the five robust factors of personality, Journal of Personality and Social Pychology, 50, pp. 116-123. ENTWISTLE, N. (1981) Styles of Learning and Teaching (Chichester, Wiley). EPSTEIN, S. (1984) Controversial issues in emotion theory, in: P. SHAVER (Ed.) Review of Personality and Social Pychology, Volume 5, pp. 64-88 (Beverley Hills, Sage). EYSENCK, H. & EYSENCK, M. (1985) Personality and Individual Differences (New York, Plenum). FORISHA, B. (1983) Relationship between creativity and mental imagery; a question of cognitive styles? in: A. SHEIKH (Ed.) Imagery: Current Theory, Research and Application, pp. 310-339 (New York, Wiley). FOWLER, W. (1977) Sequence and styles in cognitive development, in: F. WEIZMANN & I. UZGIRIS (Eds) The Structuring of Experience, pp. 265-295 (New York, Plenum). GOLDSMITH, H. & CAMPOS, J. (1982) Toward a theory of infant temperament, in: R. EMDE & R. HARMON (Eds) The Development of Attachment and Affiliative Systems, pp. 161-193 (New York, Plenum). GOLEMAN, D. (1985) Vital Lies, Simple Truths: The Psychology of SelfDeception (New York, Simon & Schuster). HUDSON, L. (1968) Contrary Imaginations (Harmondsworth, Penguin). HUDSON, L. (1970) Frames of Mind (Harmondsworth, Penguin). HUFF, P., SNIDER, R. & STEPHENSON, S. (1986) Teaching & Learning Styles (Toronto, Ontario Secondary School Teachers' Federation). JAMES, W. (1907) Pragmatism (New York, Longmans, Green). JUNG, C. (1923) Psychological Types (Princeton, Princeton University Press). KIMBLE, G. (1984) Psychology's two cultures, American Pychologist, 39, pp. 833-839. KAGAN, J. & KOGAN, N. (1970) Individual variation in cognitive processes, in: P. MUSSEN (Ed.) Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology, Volume 1, pp. 1273-1365 (New York, Wiley). KOLB, D. (1984) Experiential Learning: Experience as the Source of Learning and Development (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, Prentice-Hall). LARSEN, R. & DIENER, E. (1987) Affect intensity as an individual difference characteristic: a review, Journal of Research in Personality, 21, pp. 1-39. LERNER, J. & LEANER, R. (1983) Temperament and adaptation across life: theoretical and empirical issues, in: P. BALTES & O. BRIM (Eds) Life-span Development and Behaviour, Volume 5, pp. 197-231 (New York, Academic Press). MADDI, S. (1976) Personality Theories: A Comparative Analysis, 3rd edn (Homewood, Illinois, Dorsey). MASLOW, A. (1966) The Pychology of Science: a Reconnaisance (Indiana, Gateway). MCCAULLEY, M. (1981) Jung's theory of psychological types and the MyersBriggs Type indicator, in: P. McREYNOLDS (Ed.) Advances in Pychological Assessment, Volume 5, pp. 294-352 (San Francisco, Jossey Bass). MCCRAE, R. & COSTA, P. (1985) Updating Norman's `adequate taxonomy': intelligence and personality dimensions in natural language and in questionnaires, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, pp. 710721. MCCRAE, R. & COSTA, P. (1987) Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers, Journal of Personality and Social Pychology, 52, pp. 81-90. MILLER, A. (1987) Cognitive styles: an integrated model, Educational Pychology, 7, pp. 251-268. MILLER, A. (1988) Toward a typology of personality styles, Canadian Psychology, 29, pp. 263-283. MILLER, A. (1991) Personality Types: a Modern Synthesis (Calgary, University of Calgary Press). MILLON, T. (1981) Disorders of Personality: DSM III, Axis II (New York, Wiley). NORMAN, W. (1963) Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: Replicated factor structure in peer nomination personality rating, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, pp. 574-583. POLLACK, J. (1979) Obsessive-compulsive personality: A review, Psychological Bulletin, 86, pp. 225-241. POLLAK, J. (1981) Hysterical personality: An appraisal in light of empirical research, Genetic Psychology Monographs, 104, pp. 71-105. RAUSTE-VON WRIGHT, M. (1986) On personality and educational psychology, Human Development, 29, pp. 328-340. REVELLE, W. (1983) Factors or fictions, and other comments on individuality theory, Journal of Personality, 51, pp. 707-714. ROTHBART, M. & DERRYBERRY, D. (1981) Development of individual differences in temperament, in: M. LAMB & A. BROWN (Eds) Advances in Developmental Psychology, Volume 1, pp. 37-86 (Hillsdale, New Jersey, Erlbaum). ROYCE, J. & POWELL, A. (1983) Theory of Personality and Individual Differences: Factors, Systems and Processes (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, Prentice-Hall). SANTOSTEFANO, S. (1986) Cognitive controls, metaphors and contexts: an approach to cognition and emotion, in: D. BEARISON & H. ZIMILES (Eds) Thought and Emotion: Developmental Perspectives (Hillsdale, New Jersey, Erlbaum). SHAPIRO, D. (1965) Neurotic Styles (New York, Basic Books). SMAIL, D. (1984) elusion and Reality: The Meaning of Anxiety (London, Dent). STORR, A. (1973) Jung (London, Fontana/Collins). THOMAS, A. (1985) Affective response tendency, in: E. GORDON, (Ed.) Review of Research in Education, Volume 12, pp. 197-227 (Washington, D.C., American Educational Research Association). WESTCOTT, M. (1985) Volition as a nag, in: F. BRUSH & J. OVERMIER (Eds) Affect, Conditioning and Cognition, pp. 353-367 (Hillsdale, New Jersey, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates). WITKIN, H. & GOODENOUGH, D. (1981) Cognitive Styles: Essence and Origins (New York, International Universities Press). ZUCKERMAN, M. (1985) A critical look at three arousal constructs in personality theories, in: J. SPENCE & C. IZARD (Eds) Motivation, Emotion and Personality (Amsterdam, Elsevier). ~~~~~~~~ By ALAN MILLER, Psychology Department, University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, NB, Canada E3B 6E4 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Educational Psychology is the property of Carfax Publishing Company and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Educational Psychology, 1991, Vol. 11 Issue 3/4, p217, 22p, 4 charts, 5 diagrams. Item Number: 9707160494 Result 115 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 96 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Effects of a learning styles and strategies intervention upon atrisk middle school students... Subject(s): ACADEMIC achievement -- Psychological aspects; LOCUS of control; LEARNING, Psychology of Source: Journal of Instructional Psychology, Mar95, Vol. 22 Issue 1, p34, 6p, 1 chart, 1 diagram, 2 graphs Author(s): Nunn, Gerald D. Abstract: Examines the effects of a year-long learning styles/strategies intervention course on achievement and locus of control of at-risk middleschool students. Characteristics of at-risk students; Significant improvements within the at-risk intervention group; Implications for educators. AN: 9505042097 ISSN: 0094-1956 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] EFFECTS OF A LEARNING STYLES AND STRATEGIES INTERVENTION UPON AT-RISK MIDDLE SCHOOL STUDENTS ACHIEVEMENT AND LOCUS OF CONTROL The present paper examines effects upon achievement and locus of control of at-risk middle school students (N = 103) enrolled in a year long learning styles/strategies intervention course. Results indicated significant improvement within the at-risk intervention group in grade point average and locus of control, i.e., decreased externality. Implications and further research is discussed. American education is currently addressing the needs of students termed "at-risk", and faces ongoing challenges to enrich school experiences for these students to capture talents human resources (Rumberger, 1987). In 1985-86, there were more than 600,000 youth who dropped out of school with an anticipated cost of $120 billion in lost revenue during theft lifetimes (Hamby, 1989). Within this context, schools have been increasingly called upon to provide for a continuum of student needs which exceed traditional educational parameters. Students considered to be at-risk may be considered so for a variety of reasons which may include issues of learning problems, academic achievement, motivation, cultural differences, mental health issues, teenage pregnancy, and drug abuse which make adjustment to the school environment difficult (Mills, Dunham, & Alpert, 1988). Ehly & Perish (1990) have, therefore, considered "at-risk" a concept which includes a variety of factors detrimental to educational opportunity and school completion. Observers of the educational system call for substantive restructuring of how education is provided for students, and in particular, how education addresses needs of students already at-risk for serious school problems (Raebeck, 1990). Research into effective educational practices lends support for strategies that promote active involvement, encourage selfmanagement of learning, enable insight regarding student strengths, nurture internal locus of control and strengthen individual differences as they relate to the school experience (Lindgren, 1980; Stipek & Weisz, 1981). The concepts of learning style and learning strategies have shown promise in addressing these areas of educational need. According to Keefe (1988), learning style is "...an umbrella term encompassing cognitive, affective, and physiological/environmental dimensions." (pg. 5). Learning style research has as it's goal a "...more personalized and effective system of education" (Keefe, 1982, pg. 2). As Brandt (1990) has pointed out, the last ten years have witnessed considerable experimentation with learning styles and their relation to student lemming. Proponents of this approach believe that, through the process of exploring learning styles, positive effects upon student motivation and achievement are produced. Advocates also believe that learning style intervention exemplifies the concept of individualization by providing a unique glimpse into how a student learns. The approach is further seen as proactive in nature rather than diagnostic-remedial, by emphasizing the importance of working with student strengths rather than weaknesses. Derry (1989) defines learning strategies as "A for accomplishing a learning goal" (pg. 5). As can be thought of as specific tactics by which manipulate, and perform in relation to defined complete plan one formulates such, learning strategies students organize, retrieve, learning outcomes. Learning strategies form discrete instructional methods by which a student can take control over their learning and performance. Research in this area has attempted to bridge the gap between what is known about effective teaching/learning practices and student application of strategies. Proponents of learning strategy models point to the benefits of teaching specific ways students may learn more effectively rather than the traditional focus upon instructional process and content (Cook, 1983; Derry, Jacobs, & Murphy, 1987). Such learning may have greater relevance for students, and teaches them that learning is something in which they have a definite role and purpose. As the Strategies Intervention Model (SIM) in Figure 1 illustrates, learning strategies may be thought of as critical themes which can lead to specific skills in acquisition, storage, motivation, and expression/demonstration of competence regarding knowledge obtained in school (Deshler & Shumaker, 1987). The present study has, therefore, examined effects of utilizing these approaches with students who have had problematic histories of school failure which places them "at-risk" for continued difficulties as well as dropping out in the future. The goal of this study was to determine how systematic application of learning styles and strategies instruction affects student success in school as measured by school achievement and perceived student locus of control. Students. In all, 103 students (59 males and 44 females), in grades 7 & 8 voluntarily participated in this study. These students were selected randomly from school rosters to represent at-risk and nonat-risk populations. Racial background of the total group consisted of: White = 93.2%; Black = 5.83%; Native American = 0%; Asian = 0%; Hispanic = .97%. Design. A Nonequivalent Control-Group Design was used in this study (Borg and Gall, 1989). Students representative of at-risk and non at-risk populations were assigned to three comparison groups: At-Risk Intervention (i.e. problematic school performance with intervention); At-Risk Nonintervention (i.e. problematic school performance without intervention), and a General Education Control (i.e. students demonstrating average academic performance without intervention). Measures. Locus of control was assessed with the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control Scale (Nowicki & Strickland, 1973). This instrument is a well researched locus of control instrument, and has demonstrated satisfactory reliability and validity (Nunn, 1986; Nunn, 1987; Nunn, 1988; Nunn, 1989). Measures of achievement were taken from current and previous grade reports kept in student files. Procedure. The Learning Styles/Strategies Intervention Course was staffed by experienced teachers at the middle school, and met for one class period every other day during the school 6-day cycle for the school year. The primary focus of this course was to help students apply learning styles and strategies to facilitate positive adjustment to school. All students in the intervention course had their learning styles assessed, profiled, and interpreted for them. The Learning Styles Inventory (Canfield, 1988) was used as a measure of learning style. The instrument is an easily administered, self-scoring inventory which yields comparative scales related to: Conditions of Learning, e.g. peer, goal setting, independence, competition; Area of Interest, e.g. numeric, qualitative, people, inanimate; Mode of Learning, e.g. listening, reading, direct experience; Expectation of Grade, e.g. A, B, C,D; and Learner Typology, e.g. Social, Independent, Applied, Neutral. The Strategies Intervention Model (SIM) (Deshler & Shumaker, 1987) was used to systematically focus upon strategies which would compliment learning styles and improve performance. Strategies which focused upon acquisition, storage, motivation, and expression of competence made up the primary curriculum. Also, throughout the year, students were encouraged to conference with teachers to set goals and problem-solve ways to improve their performance by using strategies and styles reinforced in the intervention course. Results Pre-post outcomes were analyzed using a two-factor repeated measures ANOVA with comparison groups of At-Risk Intervention (AR/); At-Risk Nonintervention (ARN); and General Education Control (GEC). With regard to effect upon Grade Point Average, a significant treatment effect was obtained, F(2,88) = 43.14, p<.0001, as well as a significant treatment X repeated measure interaction effect F(2,88) = 4.79, p<.01. With respect to the interaction effect, students in the ARI group significantly improved grade point averages (90-91 GPA = 1.78 vs. 91-92 GPA = 1.95), while students in the ARN group significantly decreased their performance (90-91 GPA = 2.09 vs. 91-92 GPA = 1.8), with no significant change in the GEC group (90-91 that the ARI group decreased externality scores (Mean Fall 91 = 16.85 vs. Mean Spring 92 = 12.96) while the ARN group increased externality scores (Mean Fall 91 = 16.14 vs. Mean Spring 92 = 19.79), and the GEC group did not change significantly (Mean Fall 91 = 12.65 vs. Mean Spring 92 = 13.06). Discussion The present analysis has provided tentative support for the effectiveness of this intervention in significantly improving the school GPA = 3.18 vs. 91-92 GPA = 3.17). Locus of control also revealed significant main effects for Treatment F(2,80) = 3.12, p <.05; for the Repeated Measure F(1,80) = 3.99, p <.05, and was significant for Treatment X Repeated Measure interaction F(2,80) = 4.49, p<.05). Mean comparisons indicated adjustment of at-risk students by increasing grade point averages and decreasing external locus of control. It appears that, in this instance, a combined learning styles and strategies approach demonstrated salutory effects upon students who might otherwise decrease their performance in school. Further replication of this research is needed to verify its utility in promoting educational success with at-risk students, as well as to determine the degree to which these effects are retained and generalized after the student no longer receives such intervention. Also, knowing the importance of early intervention in the lives of at-risk students, studies which modify and attempt to implement a similar approach at the elementary school level could provide educational researchers with even more proactive interventions which may help to establish effective learning strategies and perceptions of personal control earlier in the school curriculum. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gerald D. Nunn, Area Education Agency 6, 909 South 12th Street, Marshalltown, Iowa 50158. Figure 1. Strategies Intervention Model: Learning Strategies Curriculum Acquisition e.g. Work Identification Skills, Paraphrasing, Visual Storage e.g. Mnemonic skills, Paired Associates, Listening/Notetaking Motivation e.g. I-Plan, set goals, conference, share strategies Expression and Demonstration of Competence e.g. Writing Skills, Test Taking Skills, Error Monitoring Adapted from: Deschler, D. & Schumaker, I. An instructional model for teaching students how to learn. In J.L. Graden, J.E. Zins, & M.J. Curtis (Eds.). Alternative educational delivery systems: Enhancing instructional options for all students. Unpublished manuscript. Figure 2. Learning Styles and Learning Strategies Intervention Course Legend for chart: A - Learning Styles/Strategies B - Learning Styles/Strategies C - Outcomes A B C Conditions Interests Expectations Typology Attention focusing Schema builing Idea elaboration Practice Self-monitoring Conferencing SIM-Model Focus Acquisition, School Adjustments My Style? My Goals My Progress Grades Locus of control Expression, & Storage of Knowledge via Strategies Figure 3. Effects of intervention upon grade point averages. Legend for chart: A - Pre GPA B - Post GPA A Group ARI ARN GEC B Grade Point Average 1.78 2.09 3.18 1.95 1.8 3.17 Figure 4. Effects of intervention upon locus of control Legend for chart: A - Pre LOC B - Post LOC A Group ARI ARN GEC B Grade Point Average 16.85 16.14 12.65 12.96 19.79 13.06 References Brandt, R. (1990). On learning styles: A conversation with Pat Guild. Educational Leadership, 48, 10-13. Borg, W. & Gall, M. (1989). Educational research: An introduction. New York: Longman. Burks, H.F. (1977). Manual for the Burks Behavior Rating Scales. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Canfield, A. (1988). Manual for Learning Styles Inventory. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Cook, L.K. (1983). Reading strategies training for meaningful learning from prose. In Cognitive Strategy Research: Educational Applications In, M. Pressley & J.R. Levin (Eds.). New York: Springer-Verlag. Derry, S. (1989). Putting learning strategies to work. Educational Leadership, 46, 4-10. Derry, S.J., Jacobs, J. & Murphy, D.A. (1987). The JSEP Learning Skills Training System. Journal of Educational Technology Systems, 15, 273-284. Deshler, D. & Shumaker, J. (1987). An instructional model for teaching students how to learn. In J.E. Graden, et. al., Alternative educational delivery systems: Enhancing instructional options for all students. Unpublished manuscript. Ehly, S. & Retish, P. (1990). Children at risk: A review of the literature. Unpublished manuscript. Hamby, J. (1989). How to get an "A" on your dropout prevention report card. Educational Leadership, 46, 21-27. Keefe, J.W. (1988). Learning Style Profile: Technical Manual. Reston, VA: National Association of Secondary School Principals. Lindgren (1980). Educational psychology in the classroom (6th Ed). New York: Oxford University Press. Mills, R., Dunham, R., & Alpert, G. (1988) Working with high-risk youth in prevention and early intervention programs: Toward a comprehensive wellness model. Adolescence, 91, 643-660. Nowicki, S. & Strickland, B. (1973). Alocus of control scale for children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40, 148-154. Nunn, G. (1986). Criterion-related validity of the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control Scale with academic achievement. Psychology: A Quarterly Journal of Human Behavior, 23, 9-11. Nunn, G. (1987). Concurrent validity between children's locus of control and attitudes toward home, school, and peers. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 47, 1087-1089. Nunn, G. (1988). Concurrent validity between the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control Scale and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 48, 435-438. Nunn, G. (1989). Concurrent validity between the Personal Attribute Inventory for Children and the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control Scale. Journal of Human Behavior and Learning, 6, 4647. Raebeck, B. (1990). Transformation of a middle school. Educational Leadership, 47, 1821. Rumberger, R. (1987). High school dropouts: A review of issues and evidence. Review of Educational Research, 57, 101-121. Stipek, D. & Weisz, J. Perceived personal control and academic achievement. Review of Educational Research, 51, 101-137. ~~~~~~~~ By Gerald D. Nunn Gerald D. Nunn, Area Education Agency 6, Marshalltown, Iowa. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of Journal of Instructional Psychology is the property of Instructional Innovation and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Journal of Instructional Psychology, Mar95, Vol. 22 Issue 1, p34, 6p, 1 chart, 1 diagram, 2 graphs. Item Number: 9505042097 [Tips] Result 96 of 127 [Go To Full Text] Result 98 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: An investigation of students' learning styles in various disciplines in colleges and universities. Subject(s): COLLEGE students -- Education -- United States Source: Journal of Humanistic Counseling, Education & Development, Dec94, Vol. 33 Issue 2, p65, 10p, 2 charts Author(s): Matthews, Doris B. Abstract: Studies the learning style of college majors by the use of the Canfield model as a discipline. Information on use of other strategies for selection of students; Examination of individual learning styles by researchers; Discussion of demographic characteristics provided in Learning Styles Inventory Manual; Demonstration study which consisted of selection of 2,332, four year college and university students. AN: 9708111815 ISSN: 0735-6846 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] Section: RESEARCH AN INVESTIGATION OF STUDENTS' LEARNING STYLES IN VARIOUS DISCIPLINES IN COLLEGES AND UNIVERSITIES This study investigates the learning styles of college majors using the Canfield model. Disciplines differed singificantly regarding style. Students majoring in mathematics and science fell into the applied categories more often than those students majoring in humanities, social science, and education, who fell mainly into conceptual categories. Within majors, there were also sex and race differences. The intensifying need for balancing high educational standards and equity among diverse student populations remains a concrete challenge to educators in higher education. Sliding enrollments and tight economic constraints force educators to look for alternatives in the guidance services that they provide as well as for innovative approaches to classroom instruction (Claxton & Murrell, 1987; Robbins & Smith, 1993). A traditional approach to maintaining such high standards has been to guide students to pursue majors in which they were predicted to be successful based on high school grades and standardized test scores, as well as other variables (Chartrand, 1991; Holland, 1985). Although this approach protects the projected academic standards of the college or university, it does not take into consideration the possibility that predicted success is as much a matter of inflexible instructional approaches as it is sets of characteristics and conditions in the history of students (Bonham, 1989; Grasha, 1984; Lowman, 1993; Matthews, 1991; Miller, Alway, & McKinley, 1987). Educators (Banks, 1988; Claxton & Murrell, 1987; Hoyt, 1989) have contended that many more students may be successful in majors that traditionally would have excluded them if those students were admitted and the teachers in those majors provided more flexible, innovative instructional approaches that more nearly matched learner typologies. Such matching of course content, assignments, and methods of presentation to specific learning styles of students, would enhance the achievement and degree of perceived program satisfaction of students and, therefore, retention in college. "The term 'learning style' refers to the affective component of the educational experience which motivates a student to choose, attend to, and perform well in a course or training exercise" (Canfield, 1988). It is a personal trait that develops from inherited characteristics, previous experience, and the demands of the present environment (Kolb, 1981, 1984). Although widespread agreement supports the notion of the existence of individual learning styles, learning style researchers often define the concept differently. Gregorc (1984) emphasized distinctive behaviors and dualities. Kolb (1984) specified hereditary equipment, past experience, and the environment. Canfield (1988) discussed conditions, content, modes, and expectations. A style is a fairly stable, consistent way of learning across a variety of activities, experiences, and demands of the present environment (Kolb, 1981, 1984). According to Dunn, DeBello, Brennan, Krimsky, and Murrain (1981), models have different characteristics, but tend to overlap in many aspects. The Canfield model is an instructional preference approach, based on components of Maslow's hierarchy of needs and McClelland's theory of achievement motivation (Claxton & Murrell, 1987). This approach served as the model for this research. Therefore, relevant research discussed in the following section uses Canfield's Learning Styles Inventory to measure students' learning style. One of the most popular suggestions to come out of learning style research is to use information derived from the Learning Styles Inventory as a tool for faculty in modifying classroom instruction and conditions to bring instructional style more in line with preferences of students. According to Holland (1985), people tend to be happy in occupations and stay in positions longer when job skills match personal traits. Likewise, proponents of learning style (Claxton & Murrell, 1987) contended that students are happier and stay in school longer when instructional requirements match their learning styles. The practice of matching college teaching conditions to the learning styles of students, however, is a complicated affair and raises several questions. Do learning styles of students differ across academic majors? Do men and women differ in learning styles? Do African Americans and Caucasian Americans differ in leaming styles? Research on learning styles of students in various disciplines has been reported previously in the literature. Canfield (1988) reported significant differences among groups of students enrolled in various majors in collegiate settings. Biberman and Buchanan (1986) examined learning styles within the area of business and found that the styles of majors in accounting and economics/finance varied from majors in marketing and management. Wunderlich and Gjerde (1978) studied learning styles and career choice in medicine and found that no association existed between the two in the medical field. When nontraditional nursing students (RN baccalaureate) and traditional learners (basic baccalaureate) were compared, Merritt (1983) found that the preferences of basic students were significantly higher than those of RN students on several scales of the learning style instrument used to assess them. Walker, Merryman, and Staszkiewicz (1984) found that undergraduate majors in vocational education needed more external motivation, direction and structure, time limits, and instruction by the auditory mode than that needed by graduate students. Pettigrew and Zakrajsek (1984) found that physical education majors preferred authority, iconics, and direct experience compared to other majors in education. In describing demographic characteristics in the Learning Styles Inventory manual, Canfield (1988) reported that the Learning Styles Inventory has been normed separately for men and women because they tend to express substantial differences in preferences on all the scales of his instrument. For example, in an early study using the Learning Styles Inventory, Brainard and Oremen (1977) reported significant differences between men and women within several majors in a community college. To date, no research using the Learning Styles Inventory has been reported that compares the learning style of African Americans and Caucasian Americans in different college disciplines. Furthermore, all previous studies have compared students on each of the 21 scales independently rather than using learner typologies or the two major learning style dimensions. Typologies and the two dimensions to the learning style instrument are a recent addition to the model (Canfield & Knight, 1983). Therefore, research comparing differences across college disciplines using the learning styles or typologies of students derived specifically from the Canfield Learning Styles Inventory is unavailable in the literature. The purpose of this study then was to examine the learning styles of students in various disciplines in several colleges and universities in a Southern state. The following research questions were addressed: 1. Do students in academic disciplines differ in their style of learning? 2. Do male and female students differ in learning styles within the disciplines? 3. Do African American and Caucasian American students differ in learning styles within the disciplines? METHOD Sample The sample for this study consisted of 2,332 students in selected 4-year colleges and universities in a Southern state. Students came from three state-supported institutions and two private schools. There were 1,055 men and 1,277 women. African Americans numbered 1,114 and Caucasian Americans numbered 1,218. Although there were some younger (673) and older (157) students, the majority (1,502) were in the age category of 19 to 24. Of those in the sample, 41% of the students had fathers with a high school education or less, whereas 59% of the students' fathers had some college or degrees in higher education. A similar pattern was true for the educational level of mothers. Of those sampled, 42% of their mothers had a high school education or less and 58% had degrees in higher education or some college education. The majority of students came from homes in rural areas (42%) or small towns (26%) with a population of less than 20,000. The design used cluster sampling with classes selected from the courses being offered on the schedule within majors for the spring semester at the five institutions. The sample represented approximately 9% of the 27,000 students in the institutions. The number of students in the sample by discipline was 381 (Education), 510 (Mathematics), 450 (Science), 499 (Business), 178 (Humanities), and 314 (Social Science). Table 1 presents the numbers and percentages of students in each discipline by race and sex. Instrumentation Learning Styles Inventory. The Learning Styles Inventory, developed by Canfield and Knight (1983), is a self-report questionnaire of 30 items that allows students to describe features of their educational experience that they most prefer. Each item has four choices that the student ranks on a 4point scale with 1 = most-liked and 4 = least-liked choice. The choices that students make on the instrument are summed to form 21 scales. The scales include 8 preferred Conditions for Learning (peer, organization, goal setting, competition, instructor, detail, independence, and authority); 4 preferred Areas of Interest (numeric, qualitative, inanimate, and people); 4 preferred Modes of Learning (listening, reading, iconic, and direct experience); and 5 preferred Expectations for Course Grades (A, B, C, D, and total expectation). Learner typologies, or styles, are computed in three steps (Canfield, 1988; Gruber & Carriuolo, 1991). First, raw scores on the 21 scales of the Learning Styles Inventory are converted to T scores. Second, by using T scores from 10 of the scales, a score is computed for each of two continua or dimensions: the Applied Conceptual continuum and the Independent-Social continuum. The Applied-Conceptual continuum comprises the horizontal axis and a score for this continuum is computed by the formula: Total Score = organization + qualitative + reading direct experience - inanimate iconic. The Independent-Social continuum makes up the vertical axis and its score is computed by the formula: Total Score = peer + instructor - goal setting - independence. Third, a specific learner typology is determined by the intersection of the two continua. That is, by using the two scores generated on the Applied-Conceptual and Independent-Social dimensions, a learning style can be located in one of nine categories: social (likes to learn with people), independent (likes to learn alone), applied (likes to learn by making theories operational), conceptual (likes to learn with language-oriented experiences), social/applied (likes to learn with people using hands-on experiences), social/conceptual (likes to learn with people using language-oriented experiences), independent/applied (likes to learn with hands-on experiences alone), independent/conceptual (likes to learn with language-oriented experiences alone), and neutral preference (no preference of style). Canfield (1988) discussed validity and reliability in the manual. The Learning Styles Inventory has high reliability. A study of internal consistency using item analysis from a sample of 1,397 college students produced correlations ranging from .87 to .97. Split-half reliability results using this same sample produced values ranging from .96 to .99. The validity of the Learning Styles Inventory has been determined in two ways. The first is the power of the Learning Styles Inventory to discriminate meaningful group differences in learning style preferences. Canfield (1988) reported that "hundreds of administrations of the Learning Styles Inventory...give solid preliminary evidence that the preferences discriminated by scales and sets of scales do relate to the academic and career choices of those tested" (p. 38). The second is whether teaching a student with techniques that match his or her learning style improves achievement and satisfaction with learning. Again, Canfield (1988) reported a variety of studies that supported this assumption. Student Demographic Questionnaire. The researcher constructed a demographic questionnaire to obtain pertinent information such as sex, age, race, educational level of mother and father, and hometown size. Procedure During the spring semester of 2 succeeding years, students at the private and public institutions answered the Student Demographic Questionnaire and the Learning Styles Inventory. Each institution had a facilitator who assumed the responsibility for coordinating with faculty and then administering the instruments to select classes. When the facilitators completed the administration of instruments, they returned them to the researcher for scoring and data analysis. Inasmuch as enrollment numbers in specific majors were small, similar majors were combined into six academic disciplines for purposes of analysis. General mathematics, engineering, computer science, and architecture were combined into a discipline called Mathematics. The discipline of Science consisted of majors in biology, chemistry, nursing, pharmacy, forestry, and agricultural science. Majors in economics, agribusiness, management, banking and finance, marketing, accounting, office occupations, and home economics made up the discipline of Business. Humanities was composed of the majors in art, music, English, foreign language, history, and drama. Education consisted of majors in early childhood, elementary, secondary, special, and physical education. Majors in psychology, sociology, social welfare, political science, and criminal justice were combined into the discipline of Social Science. Analysis of Data Selected statistical procedures were used to analyze the data. Percentages were computed to show the proportion of students in the nine learning typologies within the disciplines among the six categories. Likewise, percentages determined the proportion of students in the three categories along the two continua. The chi-square test was used to determine if significant differences existed between the proportion of students in the learner typologies within disciplines for sex and race, as well as among disciplines. To explore more fully the differences among disciplines, the researcher studied categories along each continuum. By using the directions by Canfield (1988) to determine placement on the two continua, three categories emerged on each continuum. The two continua categories are: Applied-Neutral-Conceptual and Independent-Neutral-Social. Total Scores on each dimension were used to place students into these categories as follows: Applied-Neutral-Conceptual continuum 1. Applied: a score of less than -15 2. Neutral: a score from -15 to +15 3. Conceptual: a score greater than +15 Independent-Neutral-Social continuum 1. Independent: a score of less than -10 2. Neutral: a score from - 10 to + 10 3. Social: a score greater than +10. FINDINGS The first research question asked if students were different among the six academic disciplines in terms of nine learner typologies identified by the Learning Styles Inventory. Table 2 shows the number and percentage of students in each category of learner typology across disciplines. A visual inspection of Table 2 shows that differences exist in proportion of students in the various categories for the disciplines. The chi-square statistical test showed differences to be significant, X(2) - 190.08, p <. 001. Another way of examining these differences is to compare the six disciplines on the two learning style dimensions. The chi-square test showed that the differences were significant (X(2) 161.41, p <.001) among the proportion of students in the three categories on the AppliedConceptual continuum across the six disciplines. Students majoring in mathematics and science tended to fall into the applied category more often than those majoring in humanities, social science, and education who tended to fall more frequently into the conceptual category. Differences among the proportion of students in the three categories on the Independent-Social continuum were not significant. The second research question asked if there were differences in style of learning between men and women within disciplines. There were significant differences in mathematics (X(2)- 16.25, p < .05), business (X2 -- 29.56, p < .001); social science (X(2) 16.80, p < .05), and education (X(2)- 16.31, p < .05). Although, both men and women in mathematics tended to be applied, women were more independent in their style of learning than were their male counterparts. In business, both groups tended to be conceptual in style, but more men fell in the social category than did women. In social science and education, high proportions of men and women were in the social or social combination categories, but women tended to select the conceptual and independent categories more frequently than did men. Young men and women who selected science or humanities as their area of concentration differed little in learning style. The third research question asked if there were race differences in learning styles within the disciplines. When comparisons were made, African Americans and Caucasian Americans tended to differ significantly in mathematics (X(2) 46.01,p < .001) science (X(2)- 21.75, p < .01), business (X(2) 33.85, p < .001); and social science ( X(2) - 17.57, p < .05). In mathematics and science, African Americans had more conceptual styles of learning than did Caucasian Americans who favored the applied styles. Majors in both races in business and social science preferred social and conceptual styles, but African Americans had higher percentages in the two aforementioned categories. There were no significant differences between the races in humanities or education. DISCUSSION Research verified that academic disciplines composed of a variety of college majors have different proportions of students with markedly different learning styles. For instance, students with majors in humanities such as art, music, drama, dance, and English tended to reveal themselves as conceptual learners. On the other hand, students in mathematics courses such as computer science, engineering, and mathematics tended to exhibit a learning style described as more applied than those in the humanities. Although approximately one quarter to one third of students in every major tended to fall into the social category on the social to independent continuum, the majority of students in these majors preferred the independent style of learning. Men and women tended to differ in their learning styles within several disciplines. For example, differences existed in mathematics, business, social science, and education. Race differences occurred within disciplines, also. African Americans and Caucasian Americans differed significantly in mathematics, science, business, and social science. The findings of this study have several important implications. Differences in learning styles across academic disciplines suggest that students select majors that match their learning styles and enhance their perceived potential for success. A first reaction might be to use this finding to guide students into majors whose content, conditions, and instructional approaches match the learning styles of students. This solution might ameliorate the issue of academic quality, because students would be expected to have greater success under these conditions. It would likely increase problems of equity, however, when educators also examine the differences within certain disciplines among men and women and African Americans and Caucasian Americans. For example, men enrolled in mathematics at a rate of almost 21/2 times that of women (32% vs. 13%). On the other hand, women were enrolled in education at a rate almost four times that of men (23% vs. 8%). Important differences existed when comparisons were made of enrollments of Caucasian Americans and African Americans in various disciplines. Caucasian Americans were enrolled in mathematics at twice the rate of African Americans (28% vs. 15%), whereas African Americans were enrolled in business at more than twice the rate of Caucasian Americans (30% vs. 13%). If one of the goals of education is to provide greater opportunities to a wider diversity of students (for example, by getting more women and African Americans into mathematics-oriented majors and more men into education majors), then recruitment, selection, and retention approaches using learning style as one variable would likely be appropriate. Career and self-development counselors at colleges and universities can effect change on campuses by using information from the Learning Styles Inventory. They can help students understand the relationship of individual learning styles and academic majors in terms of their organization, content, and instructional approaches. Rather than guiding a student away from a major if a mismatch is apparent, the counselor simply gives the student an opportunity to make a more intelligent choice about various courses of study. If the student chooses a major that is to a degree not congruent with his or her learning style, the counselor can help the student identify ways to strengthen those areas in which learning styles information predicts that a problem might arise. The counselor can also take a consulting role by facilitating faculty in modifying their instructional delivery systems to address student differences in learning styles. Wholesale modifications would not be necessary because faculty could address the differences in student preferences by using a variety of presentation formats, assignments, and student participation approaches. Canfield (1988) provided many practical suggestions to aid faculty in making these modifications. Addressing differences in learning styles of students has several advantages for meeting standards of high quality and equity among students of diversity. First, if universities and colleges improve the academic achievement and program satisfaction of students so that graduation rates increase, then all of higher education will benefit. Second, if these graduates are distributed across disciplines with more nearly equal proportions of men and women and majority and minority groups than is the case today, and if these graduates are better prepared for the demands of the future world of work, then all of society will benefit. Finally, if differences in learning and attitudes are addressed in all instructional programs so that students have greater opportunity to choose appropriately challenging and rewarding courses of study, every individual will benefit. REFERENCES Banks, J. A. (1988). Multi-ethnic education: Theory and practice. Needham Heights, MD: Allyn & Bacon. Biberman, G., & Buchanan, J. (1986). Learning style and study skills differences across business and other academic majors. Journal of Education for Business, 61, 303-307. Bonham, L. A. (1989). Using learning style information, too. In E. R. Hayes (Ed.), Effective teaching styles. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Brainard, S. R, & Ommen, J. L. (1977). Men, women, and learning styles. Community College Frontiers, 5(3), 32-36. Canfield, A. A., & Knight, W. (1983). Learning Styles Inventory. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Canfield, A. (1988). Learning Styles Inventory Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Claxton, C., & Murrell, P. H. (1987). Learning styles: Implications for improving education practices. (ASHE-ER1C Higher Education Report No. 4). Washington, DC: Association for the Study of Higher Education. Chartrand, L M. (1991). The evolution of trait-and-factor career counseling: A person x environment fit approach. Journal of Counseling & Development, 69, 518-524. Dunn, R., DeBello, T., Brennan, P., Krimsky, J., & Murrain, P. (1981). Learning style researchers define differences differently. Educational Leadership, 38(5), 372-375. Grasha, A. F. (1984). Learning styles: The journey from Greenwich Observatory (1796) to the college classroom (1984). Improving College and University Teaching, 32(1), 46-53. Gregorc, A. F. (1984). Style as a symptom: A phenomenological perspective. Theory into Practice, 23(1), 51-55. Gruber, C. P., & Carriuolo, N. (1991). Construction and preliminary validation of a learner typology for the Canfield Learning Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51(4), 29-36. Holland, J. L. (1985). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments. Englewood Cliffs, N J: Prentice-Hall. Hoyt, K. B. (1989). The career status of women and minority persons: A 20year retrospective. The Career Development Quarterly, 37, 202-212. Kolb, D. A. (1981). Learning styles and disciplinary differences. In W. Chickering and Associates (Eds.), The modern American college. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning. Englewood Cliffs, N J: PrenticeHall. Lowman, R. (1993). The inter-domain model of career assessment and counseling. Journal of Counseling & Development, 71, 549-553. Matthews, D. B. (1991 ). The effects of learning style on grades of firstyear college students. Research in Higher Education, 32(3), 253-267. Merritt, S. L. (1983). Learning style preferences of baccalaureate nursing students. Nursing Research, 32(6), 367-372. Miller, C. D., Alway, M., & McKinley, D. L. (1987). Effects of learning styles and strategies on academic success. Journal of College Student Personnel, 28(5), 400-404. Pettigrew, F, & Zakrajsek, D. (1984). A profile of learning style preferences among physical education majors. Physical Educator, 4(2), 8589. Robbins, S. B, & Smith, L. C. (1993). Enhancement programs for entering university majority and minority freshmen. Journal of Counseling & Development, 71, 510-514. Walker, T. L, Merryman, J. E., & Staszkiewicz, M. (1984). Identifying learning styles to increase cognitive achievement in a vocational teacher education program. Journal oflndustrial Teacher Education, 22(I), 27-40. Wunderlich, R., & Gjerde, L. (1978). Another look at learning style inventory and medical career choice. Journal of Medical Education. 53.4554. TABLE 1 Number and Percentage of Students in Each Discipline Race Caucasian American Discipline Mathematics Science Business Humanities Social Science Education African American Men N N 242 102 153 35 95 131 42 38 55 78 29 55 Women % % 47 20 34 8 19 26 24 21 17 25 8 14 Men N N 100 66 169 93 67 206 46 52 68 113 152 145 Women % % 20 13 38 20 14 41 25 30 22 36 40 38 TABLE 2 Number and Percentage of Students in Learner Typologlea by Major Typology Social/Conceptual Social Social/Applied Conceptual Neutral Preference Applied Independent/Conceptual Independent Independent/Applied N 49 72 88 27 63 65 51 60 54 Mathematics % 13.4 22.1 37.1 8.1 24.1 33.0 17.6 26.3 37.0 N 56 63 54 65 44 46 55 49 24 Science % 15.3 19.3 22.8 19.6 16.9 23.4 19.0 21.5 16.4 Business Humanities Typology Social/Conceptual Social Social/Applied Conceptual Neutral Preference Applied Independent/Conceptual Independent Independent/Applied N 93 77 42 88 50 31 55 43 26 Typology Social/Conceptual Social Social/Applied Conceptual Neutral Preference Applied Independent/Conceptual Independent Independent/Applied N 63 45 14 57 36 16 46 30 16 % 25.4 23.6 17.7 26.5 19.2 15.7 19.0 18.9 17.8 Social Studies % 17.2 13.8 5.9 17.2 13.8 8.1 15.9 13.2 11.0 N 41 17 10 42 16 4 33 14 5 Education N 64 52 29 53 52 35 49 32 21 % 11.2 5.2 4.2 12.7 6.1 2.0 11.4 6.1 3.4 % 17.5 16.0 12.2 16.0 19.9 17.8 17.0 14.0 14.4 REFERENCES Banks, J. A. (1988). Multi-ethnic education: Theory and practice. Needham Heights, MD: Allyn & Bacon. Biberman, G., & Buchanan, J. (1986). Learning style and study skills differences across business and other academic majors. Journal of Education for Business, 61, 303-307. Bonham, L. A. (1989). Using learning style information, too. In E. R. Hayes (Ed.), Effective teaching styles. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Brainard, S. R., & Oremen, J. L. (1977). Men, women, and learning styles. Community College Frontiers, 5(3), 32-36. Canfield, A. A., & Knight, W. (1983). Learning Styles Inventory. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Canfield, A. (1988). Learning Styles Inventory Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Claxton, C., & Murrell, P. H. (1987). Learning styles: Implications for improving education practices. (ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report No. 4). Washington, DC: Association for the Study of Higher Education. Chartrand, J. M. (1991). The evolution of trait-and-factor career counseling: A person x environment fit approach. Journal of Counseling & Development, 69, 518-524. Dunn, R., DeBello, T., Brennan, P., Krimsky, J., & Murrain, P. (1981). Learning style researchers define differences differently. Educational Leadership, 38(5), 372-375. Grasha, A. F. (1984). Learning styles: The journey from Greenwich Observatory (1796) to the college classroom (1984). Improving College and University Teaching, 32(1), 46-53. Gregorc, A. F. (1984). Style as a symptom: A phenomenological perspective. Theory into Practice, 23(1), 51-55. Gruber, C. P., & Carriuolo, N. (1991). Construction and preliminary validation of a learner typology for the Canfield Learning Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51(4), 29-36. Holland, J. L. (1985). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments. Englewood Cliffs, N J: Prentice-Hall. Hoyt, K. B. (1989). The career status of women and minority persons: A 20year retrospective. The Career Development Quarterly, 37, 202-212. Kolb, D. A. (1981). Learning styles and disciplinary differences. In W. Chickering and Associates (Eds.), The modern American college. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning. Englewood Cliffs, N J: PrenticeHall. Lowman, R. (1993). The inter-domain model of career assessment and counseling. Journal of Counseling & Development, 71, 549-553. Matthews, D. B. (1991). The effects of learning style on grades of firstyear college students. Research in Higher Education, 32(3), 253-267. Merritt, S. L. (1983). Learning style preferences of baccalaureate nursing students. Nursing Research, 32(6), 367-372. Miller, C. D., Alway, M., & McKinley, D. L. (1987). Effects of learning styles and strategies on academic success. Journal of College Student Personnel, 28(5), 400-404. Pettigrew, F., & Zakrajsek, D. (1984). A profile of learning style preferences among physical education majors. Physical Educator, 4(2), 8589. Robbins, S. B., & Smith, L. C. (1993). Enhancement programs for entering university majority and minority freshmen. Journal of Counseling & Development, 71, 510-514. Walker, T. J., Merryman, J. E., & Staszkiewicz, M. (1984). Identifying learning styles to increase cognitive achievement in a vocational teacher education program. Journal oflndustrial Teacher Education, 22(1), 27-40. Wunderlich, R., & Gjerde, L. (1978). Another look at learning style inventory and medical career choice. Journal of Medical Education, 53, 4554. ~~~~~~~~ By Doris B. Matthews Doris B. Matthews is a professor in the Department of Counselor Education and Psychological Foundations at South Carolina State University, P. 0. Box 7215, Orangeburg, SC 29117. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------- Copyright of Journal of Humanistic Counseling, Education & Development is the property of American Counseling Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Journal of Humanistic Counseling, Education & Development, Dec94, Vol. 33 Issue 2, p65, 10p, 2 charts. Item Number: 9708111815 Result 98 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 123 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: The Naturalistic approach to learning styles. Subject(s): LEARNING strategies Source: College Teaching, Summer90, Vol. 38 Issue 3, p106, 8p, 5 charts Author(s): Grasha, Tony Abstract: Focuses on the author's research regarding students' learning styles. How he conducted his research; Observational methods; In-depth interviews; Analyzing learning projects; Choosing learning experiences; Using metaphors or `practical poetry.' AN: 9709144349 ISSN: 8756-7555 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------[Go To Citation] THE NATURALISTIC APPROACH TO LEARNING STYLES Learning styles are the preferences that students have for thinking, relating to others, and for various classroom environments and experiences. A large number of such preferences have been identified (Claxton and Murrell 1987; Fuhrmann and Grasha 1983; Keefe 1982), and a comprehensive literature exists on the uses of learning style information for diagnosing students and designing instructional environments. Much of this work employs self-report personality tests, opinion and attitude surveys, and questionnaires designed to elicit preferences students have for particular instructional procedures. Over the past eighteen years, I have conducted research on the GrashaRiechmann Student Learning Style Scales, which is a questionnaire designed to elicit information about student tendencies toward competition, collaboration, independence, dependence, participation, and avoidance. About five years ago, I began seriously to question the utility of these questionnaires and tests. Some of my reasons involved the inadequate reliability and validity of many instruments, the failure of some authors to identify clear instructional procedures that would enhance certain styles, and the relatively small effects in student achievement and satisfaction that learning style information produced in many studies (Grasha 1984; Grasha 1989). A related factor in my growing disenchantment was the comments I would receive from a minority of participants in my seminar and workshop sessions on learning styles. In particular, those outside the fields of education and the social sciences frequently would comment on their discomfort with such approaches. To quote one participant from the English department of a four-year college, "I can't relate well to those categories and the numbers associated with them. They do not describe people as I know them. You are putting students into little boxes and missing the essential qualities of what makes someone a dynamic human being. It's too sterile an approach for me." My initial reaction was to acknowledge the comment, but after the seminar or workshop I largely dismissed it. After all, personality profiles were a legitimate part of my discipline, and if someone could not appreciate them, so be it. The problem was that every time I heard a similar comment, it became much more difficult to ignore. "Perhaps they have a point," I began to say to myself. If what people were telling me was accurate, I was faced with another dilemma: how to capture the "dynamic quality of what makes a student a fully functioning human being." My approach was based on quantitative measures of student learning styles. My assessment of the situation suggested that to overcome my reservations about current approaches I had to look for relatively more qualitative methods for capturing the learning styles of students. Fortunately, I was able to find some that already existed and was able to devise a couple of additional procedures for achieving the latter goal. My search began with the goal of finding procedures for assessing learning styles that were not grounded in the responses to items on a personality test. I was interested in approaches to assessing learning styles that were grounded in the daily experiences of students and labeled such approaches "naturalistic." I was able to identify four methods that met the latter criterion. They were direct observations of student behavior, in-depth interviews with students, the analysis of self-directed learning projects, and the analysis of the guiding metaphors students used when describing the teaching-learning process. Each led to valuable insights on the types of learning styles students employed and to ways instructors could adapt to them. Observational Methods The behaviors of students in a classroom can tell us a considerable amount about their learning styles. Such observations can be made by participant observers and by using audio or video tape recordings of the interactions that occur. The content of the observations can be analyzed and the underlying themes identified. One such approach was taken by Richard Mann and his colleagues (Mann et al. 1970). They audiotaped the interactions in classrooms and analyzed the content for the presence of consistent themes in how students dealt with teachers. A brief description of the learning styles identified appears in Figure 1. In examining Mann's learning styles to prepare myself for discussing them in a series of workshops and seminars, I was struck by the presence of three learning needs that underlie his typologies: (1) a need for structure and dependency (e.g., as seen in the compliant and anxious-dependent clusters), (2) an independent orientation or need to be away from the influence of others (e.g., as seen in the independent, hero, and discouraged worker clusters), and (3) a need to have the attention of others, suggesting somewhat of a cooperative orientation (e.g., as seen in the attention seekers). Given the three orientations, it is possible to design course goals that help students meet their goals. One suggestion I have made in those seminars and workshop settings is for instructors to select the major goals that they have for a course. Next, they must think of a way that each goal could be achieved, employing instructional processes that emphasize either dependency, independence, or cooperation among peers. For example, one of my goals in introductory psychology is to have students describe the three parts of our personalities (i.e., the id, ego, superego) as developed by Sigmund Freud. They could learn about them through a lecture (dependent orientation), by going to the library to search for descriptions of each part in books written about or by Freud (independent orientation), or by having a small group of peers read about Freud and share their learnings with one another (cooperative orientation). (During a term, instructors might alternate how they achieve various goals so that the three orientations are represented in their teaching styles.) In-Depth Interviews Interviews are an excellent way to have students talk about their experiences as learners. The narratives they generate are a rich source of information about their attitudes about teaching and learning, the way they learn, and the preferences they have for instructional techniques. Although somewhat time-consuming, interviews yield a rich source of qualitative data about the preferences students have for processing and acquiring new information. (See Figure 2 for a format for lunchtime interviews.) William Perry and his colleagues, for example, employed in-depth interviews to assess the intellectual and ethical development of students (Perry 1970). In the process, they were able to categorize several patterns in the cognitive components of students' learning styles. Probing interviews asked questions such as: How has being in college changed the way you think about yourself or the world? When learning about something you want to know, whom do you rely upon for information? Can you describe what a really powerful learning experience you had was like? The latter questions are only samples of the types of things that are asked. For a more detailed description, see Perry (1970) and the recent work Woman's Ways of Knowing (Belenky et al. 1986) for more specific examples of interview protocols. Through interviews, Perry identified patterns in the cognitive components of students' learning styles and the complexities involved in the way people think. Perry's work illustrated how individuals moved from a simplistic, categorical view of the world (e.g., wethey, right-wrong, goodbad) to a realization of the contingent nature of knowledge, relative values, and the formation of lifetime commitments. Three styles of thinking in college students emerged from the interviews and were labeled in order of complexity as dualism (either-or thinking), multiplicity (acknowledging multiple perspectives), and relativism (knowledge is situational). Students in the classroom with a dualist orientation tend to ask questions such as: Why do we have to learn so many different points of view? Why don't you teach us the right ones? What is the correct answer? Those who see the world more broadly acknowledge that there are multiple perspectives, but they lack the capacity to apply criteria other than personal beliefs to assess alternative points of view. Students who reason in relative terms recognize that knowledge is contextual and relative to a certain time and place. They are able to apply disciplinary-related and other types of criteria to make such judgments. In terms of teacher-student interactions, such styles have implications for how information is taught. The majority of college students have a dualist orientation or weakly developed abilities to assume different perspectives on issues. Thus, they value concrete, specific, and less abstract presentations of information. In addition, because faculty tend to think in multiple and relativist terms, dualist-oriented students do not think the faculty know very much. After all, how could these instructors know anything if they are so fuzzy in their thinking and do not have the right answers? A challenge for faculty is how to encourage such students to expand their ability to think in other modes. Exposing them to more abstract conceptual lectures is unlikely to be effective, although it may make some faculty members believe they are pursuing a correct course of action. Most students are likely to become confused and to miss the point of a highly conceptual argument. Instead, those with a dualist/ orientation need concrete examples of abstract issues and to be gently challenged to expand their horizons. They also need a forum to test new ways of thinking in an active manner, and thus the classroom environment must include much more interaction than it normally does. Craig Nelson (1989), a biologist who has spent time integrating Perry's styles into courses, suggests that instructors take the time gradually to encourage the development of alternative modes of thinking. The process involves introducing students to the uncertainties about knowledge in a discipline and having them actively engage such ambiguities. He suggests that students need frequent reminders that facts change over time, that there are different ways to examine an issue, and that biases inherent in theoretical perspectives color our views of the world. Students must, he argues, discover such things as they struggle with uncertainties, and teachers must facilitate this process of discovery. One of the procedures Nelson suggests is to devote a considerable amount of classroom time to structured smallgroup discussions designed to encourage the development of alternative perspectives. One technique is to have students prepare a worksheet that will form the basis for such small-group discussions after completing an assigned reading. The worksheet asks them to do the following: (a) Summarize the author's overall argument and list each of the major points. (b) List what criteria should be used to evaluate the adequacy of the arguments made. (c) Evaluate the overall argument and the major points against the criteria. (d) Decide whether to accept, reject, or withhold judgment on the adequacy of each major point and the overall argument. Small groups of five-to-seven students use the worksheet to structure their discussions. About 50-60 percent of a class session is devoted to such group interactions. The remainder of the time is used to clarify points, to expand the students' analyses, and to present new information about a topic. Evaluations of discussion assignments cover preparation and participation, thus emphasizing participation and understanding. Nelson argues that it is possible to encourage students to think in other than a "black-and-white" fashion and to go beyond their tendency to use personal opinion as evidence to support an argument. Research using similar schemes to apply Perry's model in classroom settings generally supports such thinking. In a review of the literature, King (1978) concludes that the complexity of students' thinking can be affected by discussion and problem-solving procedures that encourage active learning. Analyzing Learning Projects In his book, The Adult's Learning Projects, Allen Tough (1979) identified the activities adults engaged in when they needed to learn something. The types of learning projects might include learning how to run a computer, fixing a leaky faucet, learning to play the piano, tennis, or golf, or acquiring information about interior design. Tough's interest was in the types of things adults wanted to learn and in the processes that they used to acquire information and skills. Based on interviews, Tough suggested that adults were more self-directed as learners than teachers gave them credit for being. Also, personal recognition and satisfaction were important motivators of that learning. He argued that teachers of adult learners needed to take such tendencies into account. I was intrigued by his findings for two reasons. One was my suspicion that they also were applicable to younger students. The other was that examining how people designed learning projects would be a useful way to develop information about their learning styles. The data could provide the basis for classroom procedures. I decided to test whether an analysis of learning projects could yield information about learning styles. To do so, I developed a ninety-item Learning Projects Checklist based in part on a shorter checklist employed by Ann Davis Toppins (1987). A sample of fifty freshmen from a large introductory psychology class was recruited to complete the checklist. To begin, participants first listed and described three important skills or domains of knowledge that they had learned on their own. They then checked whether certain aspects of how people learn were a component of their own learning processes. After completing the checklist, each wrote a narrative describing themselves as learners based on information in the checklist. A summary of the data from this study appears in Table 1. The self-directed nature of the learning styles of college freshmen is quite clear as well as the tendency to do other than take courses as the primary way to learn. Their learning was primarily self-directed, related to personal growth, and motivated by enhancing personal growth, satisfying their curiosity and interest, and their desires to be successful. The primary rewards were not grades but self-satisfaction and recognition by others. Asking students to develop a narrative description of the information in the checklist also provides a summary of how individual students see themselves as self-directed learners. Such narratives, as the sample shown at the bottom of Table 1 illustrates, provide a more qualitative description of their learning styles and can quickly give an instructor an overview of how one or more students in a course prefer to learn. In general, the information in Table 1 was in line with Allen Tough's findings with adult learners and with data contained in a recent report by Ann Davis Toppins (1987) on the implications of learning projects for teaching graduate students. It was also compatible with a sample of older undergraduate students (juniors, seniors, and adult students enrolled in continuing education classes). It is safe to conclude that the preferences for self-directed learning are not just a characteristic of "adult learners" and are not only useful in the context of "adult education." I believe that instructors teaching younger students also must consider such tendencies in designing instructional processes. An important challenge is how to allow students in the context of traditional settings to select learning experiences for themselves and thus allow them to meet their needs for curiosity, personal growth, and recognition. I have tried to do this in several ways. Choosing Learning Experiences One is the initiation of personal growth contracts for graduate students in my department's Social Psychology Graduate Program. Each graduate student plans his or her course work and other learning experiences with a committee of two faculty members and two advanced students. The written contracts that result are re-viewed and modified regularly as needed. This insures that students meet departmental requirements, but the contracts also allow for students to design a broad range of educational experiences beyond traditional course work (e.g., internships, off-site workshops, consultantships, research projects, and keeping journals on significant learnings during the year). In my courses, I now make one-third of a student's course grade depend on a learning project related broadly to course content that he or she designs. For example, in my undergraduate applied psychology class, students must find a social problem and work on solving it. The current academic year projects found students setting up car pools for peers who live off campus, helping international students become integrated into campus life, and organizing a campaign to protest perceived inequities in student loan procedures. The students contract with me in writing for the type of project that they want to pursue, what principles of psychology will be employed, and how they will evaluate their efforts. In my graduate seminar on teaching processes, students must identify an area of teaching that they want to explore in detail and contract for a broad range of learning opportunities that will help them achieve their goals. Outcomes of this learning are presented to the class in the form of case studies, discussions that employ audio-visual materials, and a variety of non-traditional discussion procedures. The mode of presenting is something students also select, which has the added benefit of helping them learn alternative methods for sharing information with others. Using Metaphors or "Practical Poetry" One of the essential differences between creative problem solvers and those who are less creative is the use of metaphor (Grouchy 1987a, 1987b). Using metaphors to describe a problem, creative thinkers are able to identify the elements of a problem and to work on them in ways that are efficient, unusual, and appropriate for the task at hand. Indeed, many discoveries in science, technology, and other areas of daily life began with generating a metaphor. Albert Einstein, for example, used to remark that before he could develop theoretical equations for his theory of relativity, he had to have a visual image of the concept. Thus, his imagination had him riding beams of light surveying objects below him and speculating on how observers from other vantage points might view the same event. Sonar was developed during World War II when naval researchers realized that a ship on the surface was vulnerable to submarine attacks because it was "blind as a bat." Bats, of course, locate their prey with sound waves, and thus the use of sound waves as an object-detection device began. In sports, swimmers' times began to drop dramatically twenty years ago. Then the primary metaphor was viewing a swimmer's movements like an oar pushing and pulling boat. The change involved conceptualizing a swimmer as an airplane with propellers that move it through the air. Thus, strokes became much more graceful and involved underwater hand movements that to some extent mimic the motions of a propeller. In general, metaphors tend to organize our thoughts and provide directions for our actions in a variety of settings (Lakoff and Johnson 1976). I have labeled such metaphors "guiding metaphors," and they play an important role in understanding why certain teaching-learning processes are employed (Grasha 1987a). In classroom settings, faculty members often employ three metaphors to describe the teaching-learning process (Pollio 1986). They are the container model, "teaching fills student with knowledge"; the journey-guide model, "faculty lead students on a journey through their courses"; and the master-disciple model, "the master drills students in relevant skills and they become willing apprentices." Each has advantages and disadvantages, but when employed they provide a certain amount of direction and purpose to the process, whether or not the outcomes are always desirable. (See Gregory 11987] and Kloss [1987] for specific examples of the implications of common metaphors for teaching and learning.) Students also have metaphors for how they perceive the teaching-learning process and their roles in it. I have found that students are often very articulate about their metaphors. In a recent study to test the generality of a metaphor-generating process to assess curriculum and organizational issues, students listed their metaphors and their implications for teaching-learning. They did this in four stages. First, eighty undergraduate students were randomly divided into two groups of forty each and asked to think of a recent course that was judged to be effective or ineffective. Second, each group was then asked to list the words, images, and feelings that they had about their effective or ineffective class. Some people have a difficult time generating metaphors, and making a list helps them to organize their thinking. Third, participants then developed a "guiding metaphor" that would summarize the words, images, and feelings that they had generated. Fourth, students listed specific classroom procedures associated with their guiding metaphors. Finally, they were instructed to list changes in the words, images, feelings, and guiding metaphors that they would like to see made in their courses and the instructional implications of those changes. In the latter stage, students revealed through metaphors what their needs and preferences were as learners. In the process, they were stating something about their learning styles. A thematic analysis of these words, images, feelings, and guiding metaphors provided a qualitative summary of the student learning styles and needs as expressed through figurative language. A description of the results of this process for twenty students selected from a sample of eighty individuals who participated in the initial study appears in Figures 3 and 4. (See pages 112-13.) The metaphor process described in Figures 3 and 4 can be adapted for assessing the learning styles of students enrolled in a particular class. I have used the process as a course evaluation device and as a tool to examine the learning styles that my teaching methods generated. To do this, students were asked to use my class as a frame of reference. The effective and ineffective class instruction segment was not employed, but all other features of the process were used. Both sets of instructions provide valuable insights into students' perceptions of the teaching-learning process as expressed through figurative language. They also provide instructors with concrete information about specific classroom procedures that students prefer and those that match their learning styles. This is something that traditional approaches to learning styles do not do directly. Users of such instruments must often make educated guesses about what students would like. The metaphor process outlined in Figures 3 and 4 allows students directly to express their preferences for particular classroom experiences that are in line with their guiding metaphors. Thus far, my examination of naturalistic approaches to learning styles suggests several things about their use in instructional processes. Although the data are much more qualitative in nature than traditional approaches, they provide useful insights into the thoughts and behaviors of students. Indeed, the descriptions that they provide are much richer and suggest a depth to student behavior that is largely absent from traditional measurement techniques. Just as there are different learning styles, I suspect that there are also different preferences for how to measure them. At a recent workshop where both approaches were presented, I asked participants to indicate which one they most preferred. Most liked the naturalistic approaches, but 40 percent were inclined to stick with traditional methods. "I'm more comfortable with the objectivity they provide," one participant noted. The underlying factor that explains such preferences may be whether or not one tends to be a right- or left-brain thinker. Naturalistic approaches tend to rely on making qualitative and somewhat intuitive judgments and do not necessarily rely as much on what have been described as left-brain capabilities, that is, the use of orderly and logical thought processes. The issue is not which side of the brain or what approach to learning styles is better. Both cerebral hemispheres are needed for people to function, and both quantitative and qualitative assessment procedures provide information about students that teachers sensitive to students' needs cannot afford to ignore. Figure 1. Learning Styles Identified by Richard Mann Style Description Compliant Typical student of the traditional classroom. Conventional, trusting to authorities, willing to go along with what the teacher wants. Focuses on understanding material rather than criticizing it or formulating own ideas. Self-image is not well fined. Anxiousdependent Concerned about what authorities think of them. Low self-esteem and doubtful of own intellectual abilities and competence. Anxious about exams and grades. Class comments and hesitant and tentative. Discouraged worker Intellectually involved but chronically depressed and personally distant. Afraid destructive impulses will lead to hurting others. Independent Self-confident, interested, involved, tend to identify with teacher and see teacher as a colleague. Have a firmer self-image than students in the above three clusters. Hero Intelligent, creative, involved, introspective, struggling to establish identity, and rebellious. Ambivalent toward teacher, erratic in performance. Attention seeker Possesses a social more than an intellectual orientation. Wants to be liked, to please others to get good grades. Both self-esteem and control depend upon periodic reinforcement from others. Silent student Speaks in class only when sure teachers will approve. Feels helpless, vulnerable, threatened in relation to teacher, fears engulfment by instructor but longs helplessly for teacher's attention. Sniper Rebellious but more defensive and less creative than the hero. Low self-esteem, afraid of introspection, attracted to authoritarian class structure. Uninvolved and indifferent toward class; stresses fact that they were required to take course. In class, tends to lash out and then quickly to withdraw. Figure 2. Interview a Student Over Lunch Develop a set of Use a combination of open- and closed-ended question questions. Be sure to probe for additional information when students respond. One of my favorite questions is, "What is the most memorable learning experience you have had and why?" Answer your questions in writing first. This is a good check on the clarity of questions and allows you to determine if any personal biases are likely to be present when you subsequently interview students. Identify personal biases in your answers and try to keep them under control when interviewing the students. For example, if your responses emphasize the lecture method, then you risk leading the interview in that direction. Take good notes on each lunchtime session. Jot down highlights during lunchtime and after returning to your office, fill in the details. Read the notes from each interview and identify important themes. Bundle themes. Collect themes across your lunchtime interviews and put similar themes together. Develop a narrative based on the themes identified. Include in your narrative student learning style themes and the instructional implications of what students are saying. Share your narrative with participants. Ask them to check it for accuracy and to add issues they think were left out and/or that should be added. Such responses should be in writing. Revise the narrative Use the information students provide to make corrections to your summary of the interviews. Develop classroom methods and processes based on your interview data. Be sure to let students know what conclusions you have reached and what you plan to do with the information you now have. Figure 3. Metaphor Evaluation of Effective and Ineffective Courses Ineffective classes Effective classes Words Repetitive, uninformative, did not meet expectations, boring, unintelligible, onesided, confusing, challenging, hard to follow Meaty, survival, innovative, aggressive, creative, experimental, exciting, demanding, challenging, interesting informative, different, integrated, complete Images Mafia, hookers, big business, death, skeletons, living in a foreign country, watching movie without sound, intimidated audience, jail Basic training, wide-eyed child, small friendly groups, dynamic interaction among people, summer island, actor on stage Feelings Bored, frustrated, lazy, confused, angry, wasteful, stupid, stressful, sad Exhausted, anxious, stressed, relaxed, happy excited, thoughtful, surprised, confident, expectant, hurried Guiding metaphors A bike without wheels; train on a circular track going nowhere; foreign movie without the subtitles and the audience can't leave the theater because the doors are locked; Adolph Hitler talking and followers afraid to ask questions Basic training survival course, point at which three streams form one one big river; survival trip into wilderness on foot; travelers taking a pleasant trip back to place where they were born; explorers in a new land Classroom procedures Lecture dwells on unimportant points; rambling style of presenting; talking over my head; lecture without ever asking questions; student told to just memorize material; lectured with back class; belittles student questions Class projects were on real problems; course drew from many previous courses, reviewed information; instructor and class worked as a team; lectures made material relevant to local issues; lots of appropriate visual aids The figure sumamarizes the words, images, feelings, and guiding metaphors students used to described courses they considered effective (i.e., enjoyed the course, got a lot out of it, considered the teaching processes used above average) and those that were ineffective (i.e., did not enjoy the course, were not satisfied with their learning, and considered the teaching processes used below average). Figure 4. Metaphor Enhancement of Effective and Ineffective Courses Suggested changes for Suggested changes for ineffective classes effective classes Additional words Enlightening, challenging, exciting, real life Supportive, more information, emphaty examples, diversity of information, participation, control, understanding, knowledgeable with students, more student participation Additional images Explorer discovering new lands; youths gathered around a wise person; relaxed audience and teacher; peace on earth Survival; demigod small groups around table at a perfect "happy hour"; students have a bigger part in moving pieces of puzzle Additional feelings Enriched, interesting, motivating, enjoyable, relaxed Success, pressured Changes in guiding metaphors A downhill racer; a wise person showing class how a puzzle comes together; traveling through a well-lit tunnel; train heading toward a specific destination Basic training survival course with an end in sight; place where three streams one large, fast-moving river Changes in classroom procedures Instructors insures that students understand; instructor shows concerns for student needs; teacher solicits questions and explains concepts clearly; instructor uses personal experiences to make points More empathy on instructor's part for students struggling to understand difficult material; more time to learn information; more discussion of cause-effect relationships; quizzes would be more difficult Summary statement of learning needs and styles In sum, students want clear structure and goals; material that is presented in an exciting manner that helps them to piece together the divergent and sometimes contradictory data in a field; students are personally challenged through questions and other activities In sum, pace of instruction is increased and information is made more challenging; instructor takes time to insure that students are keeping up; small group processes used for discussion of information The figure sumamarizes the words, images, feelings, and guiding metaphors students used to described courses they considered effective (i.e., enjoyed the course, got a lot out of it, considered the teaching processes used above average) and those that were ineffective (i.e., did not enjoy the course, were not satisfied with their learning, and considered the teaching processes used below average). The metaphors they use for what they want in their courses have important implications for the students' learning preferences and for their learning styles. Table 1.--Naturalistic Styles Checklist: Summary of Data for Learning Projects Number of times checked Information about learning Category of learning New skills/enhanced existing skills 188 Gained new knowledge/insight 133 Attitude change 83 Emotional change 58 Hours spent More than 100 hours 59 Between 51 and 99 hours 34 Between 8 and 25 hours 29 Between 26 and 50 hours 27 Learning processes employed Learning by doing 150 Observing a model 104 Asking a friend 95 Practice of physical skills 94 Learning related to Leisure/social life 137 Personal growth 130 Work 59 Motivated by Personal growth/status 200 Curiosity/interest/novelty 194 Desire to be successful 96 Problem to solve 76 Planned by Self 131 Instructor/resource person 71 Friend/family member 68 Written instructions 33 Rewarded with Self-satisfaction 169 Recognition by others 90 Grade/degree credit 52 Promotion/status 30 Risk required Physical 84 Emotional 79 Psychological 76 Financial 42 Cognitive processes used Thinking logically/rationally 107 Analyzing information 99 Using rules to guide thinking 89 Forming principles 80 Ways of relating to others Participating/cooperating 186 Acting independent 99 Being friendly 94 Competing 72 Note: Responses from a sample of fifty University of Cincinnati freshmen who were enrolled in a 180-student section of introductory psychology. The students who participated were from the Colleges of Arts and Sciences, Business Administration, Engineering, and the College Conservatory of Music. Each completed a ninety-item learning project checklist divided into ten categories for each of three learning projects they had completed over the past three years. The categories and the top four items checked within each category are presented. The numbers represent the total number of times that item was checked across the three learning projects. A copy of the checklist is available from the author. Sample of self-descriptions: Individuals completing the checklist were asked to write a brief narrative to describe themselves. The statement below is how one of the participants completing the checklist described himself. Participants were asked to review their responses, to look for areas where they showed preferences (i.e., checking an item for at least two of the three learning projects they listed). Learner 1: I like to acquire new skills and improve existing ones. I readily ask friends for help, I read books for needed information, and actively practice the things I am trying to learn. I am motivated by curiosity and interest. My activities are planned by myself, and the major reward is self-satisfaction. I am most at risk in the emotional sphere. REFERENCES Belenky, M. F., B. M. Clinchy, N. R. Goldberger, and J. M. Tarule. 1986. Women's ways of knowing. New York: Basic Books. Claxton, C. S. and P. H. Murrell. 1987. Learning styles: Implications for improving educational practices. ASHE-ERIC Education Report, College Station, Texas. Fuhrmann, B.S. and A. F. Grasha. 1983. A practical handbook for college teachers. Boston: Little-Brown. Grasha, A. F. 1984. The journey from Greenwich Observatory (1796) to the college classroom (1984). Improving College and University Teaching 32(1):46-53. Grasha, A. F. 1987a. The WIF metaphor generation process in curriculum evaluation. Research Report, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, Institute for Consultation and Training. Grasha, A. F. 1987b. Practical applications of psychology. Boston: LittleBrown. Grasha, A. F. 1989. Learning styles: Implications for improving educational practices. Teaching Sociology. In press. Gregory, M. 1987. If education is a feast, why do we restrict the menu? College Teaching 35 (Summer):101-05. Keefe, J. 1982. Student learning styles and brain behavior. Reston, Virginia: NASSP. King, P. 1978. William Perry's theory of intellectual and ethical development. New Directions for Student Services, 35-41. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Kloss, R. J. 1987. Coaching and playing right field: Trying on metaphors for teaching. College Teaching 35 (Fall):134-39. Lakoff, G. and M. Johnson. 1980. Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Mann, R. D., B. E. Ringwald, S. Arnold, J. Binder, S. Cytrynbaum, and J. W. Rosenwein. 1970. Conflict and style in the college classroom. New York: Wiley. Nelson, C. E. 1989. Skewered on the unicorn's horn: The illusion of a tragic tradeoff between content and critical thinking in the teaching of science. In Enhancing critical thinking in the sciences, edited by L. Crow. Washington, D.C.: Society of College Science Teachers. Perry, W. G., Jr. 1970. Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Pollio, H. 1986. Practical poetry: Metaphoric thinking in science, art, literature and nearly everywhere else. Teaching/ Learning Issues (Fall) Knoxville, Tenn.: University of Tennessee Learning Research Center. Toppins, A. D. 1987. Teaching students to teach themselves. College Teaching 35 (Summer):95-99. Tough, A. 1979. The adult's learning projects. Austin, Texas: Learning Concepts. ~~~~~~~~ By Tony Grasha Tony Grasha, an executive editor of College Teaching, is a professor of psychology at the University of Cincinnati. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Copyright of College Teaching is the property of Heldref Publications and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: College Teaching, Summer90, Vol. 38 Issue 3, p106, 8p, 5 charts. Item Number: 9709144349 Result 123 of 127 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Result 7 of 7 [Go To Full Text] [Tips] Title: Learning styles of the multiculturally diverse. Subject(s): LEARNING -- Cross-cultural studies Source: Emergency Librarian, Mar/Apr93, Vol. 20 Issue 4, p24, 9p, 7 charts, 6 graphs, 2bw Author(s): Dunn, Rita Abstract: Identifies specific learning styles common among various cultures using the Learning Styles Inventory. Academic learning styles of students from different cultures; Synthesis of the findings of multicultural research with the learning style inventory; Learning styles by performance groups and sex; International study of the learning, processing and leisure-time styles. AN: 9706113892 ISSN: 0315-8888 Database: Academic Search Premier Print: Click here to mark for print. [Go To Citation] LEARNING STYLES OF THE MULTICULTURALLY DIVERSE Many researchers have analyzed how culturally different students begin to concentrate, process and retain new and difficult academic information -- their "learning style." They have reported that, in all groups, gifted, average and high achievers' styles tend to differ significantly[*]. In addition, although each of the ethnic and racial groups involved in the studies included individuals with widely diversified styles, certain populations included statistically more individuals with clusters of specific learning style characteristics than others (Dunn & Griggs, 1990). Regardless of those revealed differences, within most families the learning styles of spouses, their offspring and siblings tend to differ dramatically. Furthermore, when individuals are taught with either instructional resources or strategies that complement their learning styles, their scores on standardized achievement and attitude tests increase significantly and are accompanied by a decreased number of discipline referrals. Indeed, the California Achievement Test (CAT) reading and math scores, of essentially low socioeconomic, elementary school students in the Brightwood Elementary School, Greensboro, North Carolina, rose from the 30th percentile in 1986 to the 83rd percentile in 1988; they leveled off at the 89th percentile in 1989. Indeed, Brightwood's students have consistently shown 15% to 20% improvement above their own previous test scores every year since the school began working with learning styles. By 1989, its black students were 21% above the system and the North Carolina state average! The only change made for those children during that three to five year period was the introduction of learning styles-based instruction (Andrews, 1990, 1991). Instruments Used to Identify Learning Style Dunn, Dunn, & Price's Learning Style Inventory (LSI) and Productivity Environmental Preference Survey (PEPS) were used to identify students' learning styles in research conducted at more than 70 colleges and universities in the United States and abroad. The Ohio State University's National Center for Research in Vocational Education published the results of its two-year study of instruments and reported that the LSI had "impressive reliability and face and construct validity" (Kirby, 1979). Since 1979, the LSI evidenced consistently high predictive validity (Dunn, 1990a). In a comparative analysis of the conceptualizations of learning style and the psychometric standards of nine different instruments that purportedly measure learning style preference, only the LSI was rated as having good or very good reliability and validity. Of the 18 instruments reviewed, including an additional nine concerned with information processing, the LSI was one of only three with good or better reliability (Curry, 1987). Perhaps because of that, Keefe (1982) found that it "is the most widely used instrument in elementary and secondary schools" (p. 52). The LSI and PEPS define learning style in terms of individual student reactions to 22 elements: (a) the immediate instructional environment (Sound. Light, Temperature, Seating Design); (b) each person's emotionality (Motivation, Persistence, Responsibility [conformity/nonconformity], Structure [internal/external]); (c) social preferences (Learning Alone, in a Pair, with Peers, in a small Team, with an Adult): (d) physiological uniqueness (Perceptual preferences--auditory, visual, tactual, kinesthetic); Intake (eating, drinking, chewing, biting); Time-of-day energy highs and lows; Mobility versus Passivity needs (see Figure 1). Processing inclinations are suggested by correlations with Sound, Light, Design, Persistence and Intake (Dunn, Bruno, Sklar, Zenhausern, & Beaudry, 1990; Dunn, Cavanaugh, Eberte, & Zenhausern, 1982). Synthesis of the Findings of Multicultural Research with the Learning style inventory The instruments used to identify learning styles in at least 15 independent studies were either the LSI for students in grades 3-12 or the PEPS for post high school adults. Subjects ranged from children to adults in rural, urban and suburban areas of the United States and foreign countries and were of low, middle, or high socioeconomic status. The cultural groups represented within the United States were gifted, average and underachieving black[**], white, Chinese, Greek, Mexican and the general population of students as a whole. The groups outside the United States were Chinese from Singapore, Bahamians, Brazilians, Canadians, Cree Indians from Manitoba, Israelis, Jamaicans, Latinos and Mayans from Guatemala, Koreans, and the Philippines (see Table 1). Many populations included statistically more or fewer individuals with specific environmental, physiological, or social preferences than others (Dunn & Griggs, 1990). In addition, certain ethnic or racial groups revealed statistically different characteristics that warrant further analysis. For example, Jalali (1989) reported on a population of 300 students from each of four backgrounds -- Afro-[**], Chinese-, and Greek-Americans from New York and Mexican-Americans from a predominantly rural public school in La Joya, Texas. All were fourth, fifth and sixth graders, and all but the Afro-Americans spoke an other-than-English primary language at home. A series of six graphs and associated tables of means compared each ethnic group. The score on a particular element represents the mean deviation of a group mean from the mean of all groups on that element. The LSI original scores also were analyzed statistically by means of Completely Randomized Analyses of Variance across the four cultural groups on each of the LSI 22 elements of learning style. We cannot be certain of how to interpret these findings, but the mean scores on the LSI elements for Afro-American and Chinese-American children are presented in Table 2 (Dunn, Gemake, Jalali, Zenhausern, Quinn, & Spiridakis, 1990). The two groups differed significantly on 15 of the 22 LSI scales. The pattern of these differences can be seen in Figure 2 where the profiles of the two groups are almost perfect mirror images in terms of both direction and extent! Note that, of this population of black and oriental youngsters, almost three-fourths of one group's style is different from the other's. Had these children been classmates, they would have learned in diametrically opposite ways. What might have "worked" with many in one group, would not have been effective with many in the other. For example, as a group, the Chinese-Americans were more alert in the morning-- which was the worst time of day for the Afro-Americans, who experienced their highest energy levels in the afternoon. Afro-American children were more nonconforming than the Chinese and, thus, required: (a) an explanation of why what they were required to learn was important to their teacher, (b) being spoken to collegially rather than authoritatively; and (c) instructional options. The Chinese-American youngsters required quiet and a formal design while learning whereas many Afro-American students preferred sound (music) and informal seating while learning. Temperature differences between the two groups were extreme, with AfroAmerican children requiring more warmth for comfort than the Chinese (see Figure 2). The Chinese-Americans were more able to undertake assignments independently; Afro-Americans worked more effectively with peers than by themselves and strongly preferred routines and patterns to a variety of instructional approaches (which the orientals preferred)! One group learned best by listening (auditorially) and the other by experiencing (kinesthetically). Both groups could be taught exactly the same thing, but the media (perceptual resource), length and type of task, and environment needed to be different for each. A word of caution: all blacks and all Chinese do not have the same learning style. What this graph indicates is that, of this sample of children, many in one group learned differently from many in the other group. However, individuals in both groups learned in the other group's majority style. Consider the differences between the Afro-American and Chinese-American youngsters and the similar--but not quite so extreme -differences between the mean scores on the LSI elements for the AfroAmerican and Mexican-American children, which are presented in Table 3, and the comparison of their profiles in Figure 3. For 12 of the 22 elements, the differences between the two groups reached significance. The graphic representation of the learning style profiles showed the same mirror-image pattern noted for the Chinese-American and Afro-American children, although the effect was not as strong. Comparisons of Afro-Americans and Greek-Americans in Table 4 and Figure 4 show that these two groups differed significantly on only nine of the 22 subscales, and the profiles show more similarities than differences. However, the statistical and graphic comparisons of Chinese-American and Greek-American children in Table 5 and Figure 5 reveal 13 significant differences between those two groups on the 22 LSI elements. The comparison of these two profiles indicated another mirror-image pattern quite similar to the one between Chinese-American and Afro-American youngsters. Given the differences between the groups depicted in Tables 2 through 4, it is interesting to conjecture about the statistical comparisons between the Chinese-Americans and the Mexican-Americans. Table 6 indicates that only nine of the 22 LSI elements were significantly different between the two groups. Indeed, the comparison of profiles shown in Figure 6 reveals more of a parallel than a mirror-image pattern. On the other hand, examination of Table 7 shows that the Greek-American and Mexican-American children had the fewest number of significant differences -- only six of the possible 22. Graphic representations of the LSI profiles shown in Figure 7 reveal a clear mirrorimage pattern. It is easy to identify how children from different ethnic groups differ, but what does it mean? Many of these same differences do, but many others do not, persist in each area of the United States. Apparently differences are also influenced by urban, rural and suburban living (Ramirez, 1982: Tappenden, 1983), processing style (Cody, 1983; Dunn, Cavanaugh, Eberle, & Zenhausern, 1982; Dunn, Bruno, Sklar, Zenhausern, & Beaudry., 1990: Dunn & Price, inpress) and sex (Dunn & Price, in press; Lam-Phoon, 1986; Mariash, 1983). On the other hand, dear similarities exist among these students across the board -- similarities that question conventional school practices. For example, with the sole exception of Chinese and ChineseAmerican students, late morning and afternoon is a better learning time than early morning for more than 70% of elementary school students and 47% of secondary students, but it is an even more effective concentration period for Mexican-Americans than for students in the general population of the United States. (13% of secondary United States students are "night owls"!) Females of all groups tend to stay with a task to completion (persistent) more and more often than males. Males are more in need of an informal seating arrangement than females and are far more likely to appear "hyperactive" because of their high tactual, kinesthetic and mobility needs. Those data are consistent with Branton's (1966) findings that when a person is seated on a wooden, steel, or plastic chair, approximately 75% of the total body weight is supported on only four square inches of bone. The resulting stress on the tissues of the buttocks often causes fatigue, discomfort and the need for frequent postural change. Boys of every cultural group are less able to sit still than girls; they also are less well padded exactly where they need to be to permit comfortable sitting at conventional desks, corroborating Restak's (1979) and Thies' (1979) conclusions concerning the biological basis of learning style. Children of all cultural groups tend to be more motivated than their teachers might suspect; they merely cannot achieve when they are taught through strategies disparate with how they learn. Indeed, Mexican-Americans appeared more motivated to learn than the general population in the United States. Of all groups, these boys were the most authority-and parent-oriented and required frequent encouragement from their teachers. Direct parent involvement would be an important component of the education of Mexican-American males (Dunn & Price, in press). And, just as in the Dunn, Gemake, Jalali, Zenhausern, Quinn, & Spiridakis (1989) study, many Mexican-American students were far more peer-oriented than the general population. Those peer-oriented youngsters would be more likely to achieve well through small-group techniques like Team Learning, Circle of Knowledge and Cooperative Learning than with independent studies or Contract Activity Packages. Learning Styles of Low and High Performance Groups Underachieving children of all cultural groups have certain learning style characteristics in common: they enjoy a variety of alternative instructional strategies rather than routines and patterns, much prefer learning with a hands-on, experiential approach than by listening to lectures or reading, have a short attention span, appear to be hyperactive and in need of mobility, and often are either teacher or peer motivated. They are rarely self- or authorityoriented; instead they prefer collegial adults. High achievers of all cultural groups often are self- and authority-motivated, although Korean (Suh & Price, in press) and Filipino (Ingham & Price, in press) adolescents were highly self- and parent-motivated. Among children from diverse cultural groups, high achievers remain on-task, do what they are told, sit in their seats without much hyperactivity and feel secure once routines have been established. On the other hand, average students learn more easily by reading or seeing rather than by listening, experience energy highs later in the day rather than in the early morning, and want to achieve despite the difficulties they experience with traditional schooling. Contrast those characteristics with underachievers' traits. Many in the latter group feel warm when classmates are comfortable, cannot sit passively for any length of time and, thus, are thought to be -- and are labeled --"hyperactive," rarely do what they are told because they are nonconforming and resist authoritative directives, and rarely complete tasks without supervision because they prefer working on several things simultaneously, need options within structure and appreciate breaks. Findings concerning learning style differences among the various achievement levels appear to be consistent across the board for students regardless of their cultural background. Learning Styles of Males and Females On the other hand, there appear to be more differences between how boys and girls learn than between cultural groups. Males require more intake and mobility while learning than females, who are considerably more persistent and conforming. It can be argued that mobility and intake may be socially imposed attributes, but experts proclaim them to be biologically based (Restak, 1979; Thies, 1979) and persistence frequently correlates with an analytic, rather than a global, processing style. In the same vein, females have more of a preference for auditory instruction and males much prefer tactual and kinesthetic learning. In this regard, an groups differ significantly from Asians, who prefer significantly more learningby-listening than learning-by-doing -- which may, in some way, contribute to the academic success of many Asian children in American schools. Also, regardless of culture, many more boys than girls can tolerate, and often prefer, sound in their instructional environment; girls often require quiet while learning. This phenomenon may be an outgrowth of girls' auditory strengths. They hear better and thus are more distracted by noise (Pizzo, 1981; Pizzo, Dunn & Dunn, 1990). International Study of the Learning, Processing, and Leisure-Time Styles of Gifted and Talented Versus Non-Gifted Adolescents As reported by Price (Dunn, Milgram, & Price, in press), a total of seven gifted and non-gifted ethnic groups were compared on the LSI. A systematic pattern of similarities was revealed across the 22 learning style areas for the seven groups studied in Brazil, Canada, Guatemala, Israel, Korea, the Philippines and the United States. The greatest similarity among the learning styles of the gifted were evidenced in the areas of Self-, Parent- and Teacher-Motivated, Persistence, Responsibility (conformity) and Learning Alone -- elements that Thies (1979) described as being developmental, emerging from one's experiences in life. There were greater variations among the gifted in the areas of Sound, Light, Temperature, Design, Perceptual Strengths and Mobility -variables that Garger (1990), Griggs (1991), Restak (1979) and Thies (1979) indicated are biologically imposed. Although the gifted revealed essentially similar learning style characteristics in certain areas, they differed among themselves in degree. For example, the gifted in the Philippines were significantly more Parent- and Self-Motivated than all other ethnic groups in that international study (Ingham & Price, in press). In addition, significant differences were evidenced between the learning styles of gifted and non-gifted students in those seven nations. In particular, although the gifted also preferred to learn tactually and kinesthetically, they were perceptually strong in three or four modalities -- including the auditory and visual, whereas the nongifted's perceptual strengths were only tactual or kinesthetic, or tactual or kinesthetic first with a weak second auditory preference. Relatively few students, either gifted or non-gifted, preferred learning by listening. Most preferred learning either through active participation (kinesthetically), with hands-on instructional resources (tactually), or by reading (visual/analytic) or seeing charts and illustrations (visual/global) (Dunn, Bruno, Sklar, Zenhausern, & Beaudry, 1990; Dunn, Cavanaugh, Eberle, & Zenhausern, 1982). The gifted also were more self-motivated and, in this population of several thousand, more nonconforming than the non-gifted population. A different researcher, Lan Yong (1989), found that low achieving, gifted, United States secondary students liked to eat when studying, and that motivation, needing a variety of resources to maintain interest, and learning tactually contributed to the predictability of academic performance of gifted, secondary students. Of special interest were the similarities evidenced across the multicultural groups for each unique gifted population in science and math, language, music, art, dance, leadership and how they spent their leisure time. Although wide variations occurred among the gifted in different talent areas, within each talent area the learning styles of the gifted revealed strong patterns of similarity. Based on this international study (Milgram, Dunn. & Price, in press), it may be possible to: (a) identify potentially gifted students early by diagnosing their learning style characteristics; and (b) determine with reasonable accuracy the areas of talent in which they will excel. There were other significant differences identified in Price's (in press) concluding chapter but, overall, he indicated that the construct of learning style based on the LSI, as translated into various languages, was able to diagnose individual differences among the different multicultural groups. Next Steps Studies by Guzzo (1987), L, (1983), Roberts (1984) and Vazquez (1985) examined the learning styles of students in Brazil, the Philippines, Bahamas and Jamaica, and Puerto Rico respectively. Replications were undertaken in Brazil (Wechsler, in press) and in the Philippines (Ingham & Price, in press), and new investigations were designed for other nations (Brodhead & Price, in press; Milgram & Price, in press; Sinatra, deMendez, & Price, in press; Suh & Price, in press). We currently are awaiting the findings of studies initiated in Egypt and Greece last year. Continuing Questions Regardless the diversity among cultures, the learning styles of: (a) spouses; (b) parents and their offspring; and (c) siblings tend to differ. Why that occurs is not clear, particularly in light of neurobiologist Richard Restak's (1979) and psychologist Arming Tries' (1979) independent assertions concerning the biological nature of much of learning style. Tries epicifically reported that the environmental, physiological and psychological characteristics the Dunns described in their model were biological in nature (1979). Garger (1990) suggested possible links between some of the Dunns' elements and neurophysiology. Physicians Richard Crews, president of Columbia Pacific University (1990), and Melvin D. Levine, Professor of Pediatrics, School of Medicine (1990), each have similar beliefs concerning the relationships between neurodevelopmental phenomena and their implications for school learning. These are advocates with convincing credentials. However. (1) If learning style is biological, why do siblings often have styles diametrically different from each other? Why don't the styles of offspring necessarily reflect those of their parents? Why do people with different learning styles tend to marry? (2) If almost three-fifths of learning style is biological (the environmental, physiological and psychological elements) (Thies, 1979), to what extent are the developmental (emotional and sociological) elements influenced by those thought to be biological? Do individuals possess more or less sensitivity to either people or things because of their biological makeup? (3) If individuals have significantly different learning styles -- as they appear to have -- is it not unprofessional, irresponsible and immoral to teach all students the same lesson in the same way without identifying their unique strengths and then providing responsive instruction? Twenty years ago, researchers at St. John's University asked the question, "Will teaching to underachievers' learning styles impact on their academic achievement?" That question has been answered affirmatively many times (Andrews, 1990, 1991; Brunner & Majewski, 1990; Dunn & Griggs, 1988: Dunn, 1990b; Harp & Orsak, 1990; Lemmon, 1985; Middle School Reading Program, 1991; Orsak, 1990; Perrin, 1990; Sinatra, 1990). Now that we have begun learning about the learning styles of multicultural students, certain things are clear: (a) individuals do learn differently from groups; (b) groups do learn differently from each other, (c) responding to how students learn significantly increases their achievement and attitude test scores; (d) no learning style characteristic is better or worse than any other learning style characteristic; and (e) apparently all children can learn -- but they need to be taught to their individual learning style strengths if they are to master new and difficult academic material. * In this report, the term "significantly" is used to indicate statistical differences. ** The nomenclature used to describe each ethnic group in this manuscript was used by the researcher in the investigation being reported. TABLE 1 Students of the Learning Styles of Students with Different Cultural Backgrounds Legend for Chart: A B C D - RESEARCHER, YEAR GEOGRAPHIC REGION ACADEMIC LEVEL CULTURAL GROUP A B C D Brodhead & Price (in-press) Ottawa, Canada Secondary Urban, artistically talented Dunn, Griggs, & Price (in-press) United States Secondary General Population Dun & Price (in-press) Texas Elementary Mexican-Americans Ingham & Price (in-press) The Philippines Secondary High SES Secondary Gifted/Nongifted Jacobs (1987) Southern U.S. High, Middle, Low Achievers Middle School Afro-American Euro-American Jalali (1989) New York (Urban and Suburban) Texas (Rural) Elementary Airo-American Chinese American Mexican American Lam-Phoon (1986) Lansing, Michigan (Middle SES) Singapore (Middle SES) College Asian-American Caucasian Asian Mariash (1983) Northeast Manitoba (Rural, ESL) Secondary Elementary Cree Indian Milgram & Price (in-press) Israel (General population) Secondary Giften/Nongifted High, Average, Middle SES Roberts Bahama, Jamaica Secondary African Descent Sims (1988) California (Low SES, Oregon (Rural Migrant; Low SES) Elementary Black American Mecian American White Sinatra, deMendez, & Price (in-press) Guatemala (General population) Secondary Mayan, Guatemalan Suh & Price (in-press) Korea Secondary Korean (High SES) Vazquez (1985) Puerto Rico College Puerto Ricans Wechsler & Price (in-press) Brazil Secondary Low, Middle SES Brazillians NOTE: ESL = English as a second language. Table 2: Statistical Comparison of the LSI Elements for Afro-American and Chinese-American Children ELEMENT AFRO AMERICAN CHINESE AMERICAN 15 sig Sound 8.16 Light 8.40 Temp 12.20 Design 8.04 Motivation 20.80 Persistence 11.48 Responsibility 7.88 Structure 9.96 Alone 11.72 Authority 8.96 Variety 7.60 Auditory 8.08 Visual 6.36 Tactile 11.40 Kinesthetic 15.88 Intake 10.52 Morning 10.40 Late Morning 6.60 Afternoon 11.12 Mobility 8.68 Parent 11.28 Teacher 13.68 Table 3: Statistical Comparison of the LSI Elements for Afro-American and Mexican-American Children 9.88[*] 8.32 9.96[*] 8.80[*] 19.64[*] 11.16 9.32[*] 10.12 17.44[*] 9.32 10.24[*] 8.84[*] 7.04[*] 12.00 16.72 8.64[*] 13.24[*] 8.52[*] 10.52 7.96[*] 10.52[*] 12.72[*] ELEMENT AFRO AMERICAN MEXICAN AMERICAN 12 sig 8.16 8.40 12.20 8.04 20.80 11.48 7.88 9.96 11.72 8.96 7.60 8.08 6.36 11.40 15.88 10.52 10.40 6.60 11.12 8.64 7.76 9.40[*] 8.12 19.96 10.80 8.60 10.36[*] 15.40[*] 9.12 9.04[*] 8.68[*] 7.76[*] 11.24[*] 16.40[*] 9.72 12.92[*] 7.24[*] 10.68 Sound Light Temp Design Motivation Persistence Responsibility Structure Alone Authority Variety Auditory Visual Tactile Kinesthetic Intake Morning Late Morning Afternoon Mobility 8.68 Parent 11.28 Teacher 13.68 Table 4: Statistical Comparison of the LSI Elements for Afro-American and Greek-American Children ELEMENT AFRO AMERICAN 8.00[*] 10.68 13.24 GREEK AMERICAN 9 sig Sound 8.16 Light 8.40 Temp 12.20 Design 8.04 Motivation 20.80 Persistence 11.48 Responsibility 7.88 Structure 9.96 Alone 11.72 Authority 8.96 Variety 7.60 Auditory 8.08 Visual 6.36 Tactile 11.40 Kinesthetic 15.88 Intake 10.52 Morning 10.40 Late Morning 6.60 Afternoon 11.12 Mobility 8.68 Parent 11.28 Teacher 13.68 Table 5: Statistical Comparison of the LSI Elements for Chinese-American Children and Greek-American 8.44 7.40[*] 10.48[*] 7.60 19.66[*] 11.92 9.23[*] 7.72[*] 14.20[*] 8.84 8.76[*] 9.60[*] 6.92 10.88 15.60 9.00[*] 10.76 7.12 10.96 8.76 10.96 12.88[*] ELEMENT CHINESE AMERICAN GREEK AMERICAN 13 sig 9.88 8.32 9.96 8.80 19.64 11.16 9.32 10.12 17.44 9.32 10.24 8.84 7.04 12.00 16.72 8.64 13.24 8.52 10.52 7.96 10.52 12.72 8.44[*] 7.40[*] 10.48 7.60[*] 19.66 11.92[*] 9.23 7.72[*] 14.20[*] 8.84 8.76[*] 9.60[*] 6.92 10.88[*] 15.60[*] 9.00 10.76[*] 7.12[*] 10.96 8.76[*] 10.96 12.88 Sound Light Temp Design Motivation Persistence Responsibility Structure Alone Authority Variety Auditory Visual Tactile Kinesthetic Intake Morning Late Morning Afternoon Mobility Parent Teacher Table 6: Statistical Comparison of the LSI Elements for Mexican-American and Chinese-American Children ELEMENT MEXICAN AMERICAN CHINESE AMERICAN 8 sig Sound 8.64 Light 7.76 Temp 9.40 Design 8.12 Motivation 19.96 Persistence 10.80 Responsibility 8.60 Structure 10.36 Alone 15.40 Authority 9.12 Variety 9.04 Auditory 8.68 Visual 7.76 Tactile 11.24 Kinesthetic 16.40 Intake 9.72 Morning 10.40 Late Morning 12.92 Afternoon 7.24 Mobility 10.68 Parent 8.00 Teacher 13.24 Table 7: Statistical Comparison of the LSI Elements for Greek-American and Mexican-American Children 9.88[*] 8.32 9.96 8.80[*] 19.64 11.16[*] 9.32 10.12[*] 17.44 9.32 10.24[*] 8.84 7.04[*] 12.00[*] 16.72 8.64[*] 13.24 8.52[*] 10.52 7.96 10.52 12.72 ELEMENT GREEK AMERICAN MEXICAN AMERICAN 8 sig 8.44 7.40 10.48 7.60 19.66 11.92 9.23 7.72 14.20 8.84 8.76 9.60 6.92 10.88 15.60 9.00 10.76 7.12 10.96 8.76 10.96 12.88 8.64 7.76 9.40[*] 8.12 19.96 10.80[*] 8.60 10.36[*] 15.40 9.12 9.04 8.68[*] 7.76[*] 11.24 16.40 9.72 12.92[*] 7.24 10.68 8.00 10.68 13.24 Sound Light Temp Design Motivation Persistence Responsibility Structure Alone Authority Variety Auditory Visual Tactile Kinesthetic Intake Morning Late Morning Afternoon Mobility Parent Teacher DIAGRAM: Figure 1. Simultaneous or Successive Processing GRAPH: Figure 2: LSI Profiles, Chinese and Afro Students. GRAPH: Figure 3: LSI Profiles. Afro and Mexican Students. GRAPH: Figure 4: LSI Profiles, Afro and Greek Students GRAPH: Figure 5: LSI Profiles, Chinese and Greek Students. GRAPH: Figure 6: LSI Profiles, Chinese and Mexican Students. GRAPH: Figure 7: LSI Profiles, Greek and Mexican Students. PHOTO (BLACK & WHITE): Rita Dunn References Andrews, R. (1990). The development of a learning styles program in a low socioeconomic, underachieving North Carolina elementary school. Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities: International, 6(3), 307-314. Andrews, R. (1991). Insights into education: An elementary principal's perspective. Hands-on approaches to training style: A practical guide to successful schooling. New Wilmington, PA: The Association for the Advancement of International Education, Special Edition. 50-52. Branton, O. (1966). The comfort of easy chairs (FIRA Technical Report No. 11). Hertfordshire, England: Furniture Industry Research Association. Brunner, C., & Majewski. W. (1990). Mildly handicapped students can succeed with learning styles. Educational Leadership, 48(2), 21-23. Brodhead, M. (in press). The learning styles of artistically talented adolescents in Canada. In R. Milgram, R. Dunn, & G. Price, (Eds.), Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescent through their learning styles: An international perspective. New York: Praeger. Crews, R. (1990). Telephone conversation with author concerning the physiological nature of many learning style attributes subsequent to publication of Educational Leadership's October 1990 issue. Dr. Crews is a physician and president of Columbia Pacific University, San Rafael, CA. Curry, L. (1987). Integrating concepts of cognitive learning style: A review with attention to psychometric standards. Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: Canadian College of Health Science Executives. Dunn, R. (1990a). Understanding the Dunn and Dunn learning styles model and the need for individual diagnosis and prescription. Journal of Reading Writing, and Learning Disabilities: International, 6(3), 223-247. Dunn, R. (1990b). Rita Dunn answers questions people ask about learning styles. Educational Leadership, 48(2), 15-19. Dunn, R., Beaudry, J., & Klavas, A. (1989). Survey of research on learning styles. Educational Leadership, 48(6), 50-58. Dunn, R., Bruno, J., Sklar, R., Zenhausern, R., & Beaudry, J. (1990). Effects of matching and mismatching minority developmental college students' hemispheric preferences on mathematics scores. Journal of Educational Research, 83(5), 283-288. Dunn, R., Cavanaugh, D., Eberle, B., & Zenhausern, R. (1982). Hemispheric preference: The newest element of learning style. The American Biology Teacher, 44(5), 291-294. Dunn, R., Dunn, K., & Price, G. (1975, 1979, 1981, 1985, 1987, 1989). Learning style inventory. Lawrence, KS: Price Systems. Dunn, R., Dunn, K., & Price, G. (1982, 1990). Productivity environmental preference survey. Lawrence, KS: Price Systems. Dunn, R., Gemake, J, Jalali, F., Zenhausern, R., Quinn, P., and Spiridakis, J. (1990). Cross-cultural differences in learning styles of elementary-age students from four ethnic backgrounds. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 18(2), 68-93. Dunn, R., & Griggs, S. (1990). Research on the learning style characteristics of selected racial and ethnic groups. Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities: International, 6(3), 261-280. Dunn, R., & Price, G. (in press). Comparison of the learning styles of fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade male and female Mexican American students in southern Texas and same grade students in the general population of the United States. Journal of Multicultural Development and Counseling. Garger, S. (1990). Is there a link between learning style and neurophysiology? Educational Leadership, 48(2), 63-65. Guzzo, R. (1987). Dificuldades de apprenddizagem: Modalidade de attencao e analise de tarefas em materials didaticos. Doctoral dissertation, University of Saint Paulo, Institute of Psychology, Brazil, 1987. Harp, T., & Orsak, L. (1990). One administrator's challenge: Implementing a learning style program at the secondary level. Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities: International, 6(3), 335342. Ingham, J., & Price, G. (in press). The learning styles of adolescents in the Philippines. In R. Milgram, R. Dunn, & G. Price (Eds.), Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescents through their learning styles. New York: Praeger. Jacobs, R. (1987). An investigation of the learning style differences among Afro-American and Euro-American, high, average, and low achievers. (Doctoral dissertation, Peabody University, 1987). Jalali, F. (1989). A cross-cultural comparative analysis of the learning styles and field dependence/independence characteristics of selected fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade students of Afro, Chinese, Greek, and Mexican heritage. Doctoral dissertation, St. John's University, 1989. Keefe, J. (1982). Assessing student learning styles. In Keefe, J. W. (Ed.). Student learning styles and brain behavior (pp. 1-18). Reston, VA: National Association Secondary School Principals. Kirby, P. (1979). Cognitive styles, learning styles, and transfer skill acquisition. (Information Series No. 195, National Center for Research in Vocational Education). Columbus, OH: Ohio State University. Lam-Phoon, S. (1986). A comparative study of the learning styles of Southeast Asian and American Caucasian college students of two Seventh-Day Adventist campuses. Doctoral dissertation, Andrews University, Berrien Springs, MI, 1986. Lan Yong, F. (1989). Ethnic, gender, and grade differences in the learning style preferences of gifted minority students. (Doctoral dissertation, Southern Illinois University at Carbondale, 1989). Lemmon, P. (1985). A school where learning styles make a difference. Principal, 64(4), 26-29. Levine, M. D. (1990). Neurodevelopmental phenomena in school: Their implications for learning and teaching. NC: University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill. L. T. C. (1983). The learning styles of the Filipino graduate students of the evangelical seminaries in metro Manila. Doctoral dissertation, Asia Graduate School of Theology, Philippines, 1989. Mariash, L. (1983). Identification of characteristics of learning styles existent among students attending school in selected northeastern Manitoba communities. Unpublished master's dissertation, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Canada. Milgram, R., & Dunn, R. (Eds.). (in press). Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescents through their learning styles: An international perspective. New York: Praeger. Milgram, R., & Price, G. (in press). The learning styles of adolescents in Israel. In R. Milgram, R. Dunn, & G. Price (Eds.) Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescents through their learning styles. New York: Praeger. Orsak, L. (1990). Learning styles and love: A winning combination. Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities: International, 6(3), 343-346. Perrin, J. (1990). The learning styles project for potential dropouts. Educational Leadership, 48(2), 23-24. Price, G. (in press). Comparative analysis of the learning styles of gifted and talented adolescents in eight nations: An international perspective. In R. Milgram, R. Dunn, & G. Price (Eds.) Teaching gifted and talented adolescents through their learning styles. New York: Praeger. Restak, R. (1979). The brain: The last frontier. New York: Doubleday. Roberts, O. (1984). Investigation of the relationship between learning style and temperament of senior high school students in the Bahamas and Jamaica. Unpublished master's thesis, Andrews University, Berrien Springs, MI. Sims, J. (1988). Learning styles: A comparative analysis of the learning styles of black-American, Mexican-American, and white-American third-and fourth grade students in traditional public schools. Doctoral dissertation, University of Santa Barbara, CA. Sims, J. (1989). Learning style: Should it be considered? The Oregon Elementary Principal, 5-0(2), 28. Sinatra, C. (1990). Five diverse secondary schools where learning style instruction works. Journal of Reading. Writing and Learning Disabilities: International, 6(3), 323-334. Sinatra, R. Demendez, E., & Price, G. (in press). The learning styles of adolescents in Guatemala. In R. Milgram, R. Dunn. & G. Price (Eds.), Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescents through their learning styles. New York: Praeger. Suh, B., & Price, G. (in press). The learning styles of adolescents in Korea. In R. Milgram, R. Dunn. & G. Price (Eds.), Teaching and counseling gifted and talented adolescents through their learning styles. New York: Praeger. Thies, A. (1979). A brain-behavior analysis of learning style. In J. Keefe (Ed.), Students learning styles: Diagnosing and prescribing programs (pp. 55-61). Reston, VA: National Association of Secondary School Principals. Vazquez, A. (1985). Description of learning styles of high-risk adult students taking courses in urban community colleges in Puerto Rico. Dissertation Abstracts International, 47 (04), SECA, 1157. Wechsler, S. (in press). The learning styles of adolescents in Brazil. In R. Milgram, R. Dunn, & G. Price. Teaching gifted and talented adolescents through their learning styles. New York: Praeger. ~~~~~~~~ By Rita Dunn Dr. Rita Dunn is professor in the Division of Administrative and Instructional Leadership and Director of the Center for the Study of Learning and Teaching Styles, St. John's University, New York. Copyright of Emergency Librarian is the property of Dyad Services and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Emergency Librarian, Mar/Apr93, Vol. 20 Issue 4, p24, 9p, 7 charts, 6 graphs, 2bw. Item Number: 9706113892 Result 7 of 7 [Go To Full Text] [Tips]