Lecture 6 Aiming the spotlight: Selective attention, absorption, and the construction of reality. Readings for this week: Baars, Theater, Chapter 4. (We are going back to Chapter 4 to pick up some important ideas. The spotlight of selective attention versus the conscious contents of the bright spot. p. 1 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. Selective attention corresponds to the selective role of the spotlight, scanning the world and ourselves from moment to moment. It’s a metaphor, of course --- but useful, as long as we are careful with it. Notice the difference in the theater metaphor between conscious contents --- Mickey Mouse performing in the bright spot on stage --- and selective attention, which controls where the spotlight is aimed. The spotlight makes selections, based on goal priorities and salient inputs --- alarms and opportunities. But Mickey Mouse doing his act represent the contents of consciousness --- the things that are selected by the spotlight. It’s a fairly subtle but important distinction. If you have a pet, you can easily see them responding to alarms and opportunities. And you know pretty well what their ongoing goals are --- they include food, comfort, companionship, the opportunity to play, sex, and protection of their territories. So it’s easy to see them orienting their eyes, ears and nose to opportunities, jumping at alarming events and then orienting to them, and pursuing their ongoing goals. We can’t see directly what they are conscious of, but we can identify with what the food looks and smells like in our own consciousness. (Just pretend you have a really good sense of smell, and no color vision). In certain states of mind, we have pretty good voluntary control over our selective attention ---- so we can guide pretty well where our consciousness is going. That’s presumbly the case for you now! In other states, like drowsy states, or when we feel tired, we have less voluntary control over selective attention, and our p. 2 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. attention wanders hither and thither. Both of these kinds of states are very interesting. In the “directed” state, we can do effortful thinking, step-by-step reasoning, and deal with difficult topics. The ability to directe effortful attention bears a close relationship to the biological component of intelligence (the so-called “g” factor). In the “undirected states” all kinds of nonvoluntary and quasi-conscious and unconscious influences move our consciousness in unexpected directions. That tells us a lot about those less-than-completely-conscious states and what they allow to emerge when we are relatively unguarded, and more willing express who we are… We are less alert to manage our social appearances. William James. In perhaps the most famous quotation from William James’ great Principles of Psychology of 1890, both the “directed” and “undirected” attentional states are discussed. First, the more “directed state” where we select conscious contents deliberately: “Every one knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought. Focalization, concentration, of consciousness are of its essence. It implies withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively with others …” Second, the more “undirected state,” where conscious p. 3 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. contents seem to happen to us: “ (The directed state) has a real opposite in the confused, dazed, scatterbrained state which in French is called distraction…” “We all know this latter state … “Most people probably fall several times a day into a fit of something like this: The eyes are fixed on vacancy, the sounds of the world melt into confused unity, the attention is dispersed so that the whole body is felt, as it were, at once, and the foreground of consciousness is filled, if by anything, by a sort of solemn sense of surrender to the empty passing of time. In the dim background of our mind we know meanwhile what we ought to be doing: getting up, dressing ourselves, answering the person who has spoken to us, trying to make the next step in our reasoning. But somehow we cannot start; the [thoughts in back of the mind] fail to pierce the shell of lethargy that wraps our state about. Every moment we expect the spell to break, for we know no reason why it should continue. But it does continue, pulse after pulse, and we float with it, until - also without reason that we can discover - an energy is given, something - we know not what - enables us to gather ourselves together, we wink our eyes, we shake our heads, the background-ideas become effective, and the wheels of life go round again.” “This curious state … can for a few moments be produced at will by fixing the eyes on vacancy. Some persons can voluntarily empty their minds and 'think of nothing. … Fatigue, monotonous mechanical occupations that end by being automatically carried on, tend to reproduce it in men. It is not sleep; and yet when aroused from such a state, a person will often hardly be able to say what he has been thinking about. Subjects of the hypnotic trance seem to lapse into it when left to themselves; asked what they are thinking p. 4 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. of, they reply, 'of nothing particular'.” This James quote captures the basic points of this Lecture, including the fact that hypnotic states --- more suggestible states of mind --- may have this quality of being somewhat undirected, or effort-free. That doesn’t mean there aren’t a lot of guiding influences in dreams, daydreams, and effort-free states. It just means we don’t have a sense of effort, of “pushing” our attention toward one particular goal. Effort-free states. In *absorbed states,* sometimes called *the experience of Flow* we feel alert, but our conscious contents are deeply involved in making music, dancing, running, or playing. Creative activities often take place in such a "Flow" state. In Flow states we usually do not feel conflicts about our goals, or whether to pay attention to this or that. We do not worry about things that are not relevant to our current experiences. We are capable of more "imaginative involvement," in the words of Jerome Singer, one of the pioneers in this area. Absorption has a small but reliable correlation with suggestibility, which can be considered to be a state in which we don't bother to judge ourselves --- so we are not constantly evaluating our own conscious experiences. Meditation-related states can have this quality of being absorbing. So do dreams and daydreams. “Twilight states” between waking and sleeping are a natural place to explore a spontaneous flow of experiences. (See our P-Lab for this week). Some researchers believe that this unguided, spontaneous flow of consciousness goes along with low activity in parts of the frontal lobes (frontal hypometabolism). That happens in drowsy states, but also in alert states where we just are not trying hard to p. 5 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. make something happen, or to avoid something. When we are focused in an effortful way, there is good evidence that parts of the frontal lobes are very active. (Particularly the "anterior cingulate cortex," which we'll show in a brain figure.) When we are not trying to guide our attention and consciousness in one way or another, there is some evidence that the frontal lobes are less busy. In REM dreams (with rapid eye movements) we usually follow an inner movie --- a narrative flow of visual and meaningful but surreal events. In other sleep states there are now believed to be other kinds of mentation, including a kind of very literal flow of inner speech. During “lucid dreaming” we experience both an inner movie and also maintain the knowledge that we are dreaming. So that is a kind of “metacognition during dreaming,” implying more frontal lobe activity. Phenomenology Lab. The P-Lab for this week explores “twilight states” --- the state between sleep and waking. There are some interesting things we would like you to explore about your own twilight experiences. All of us experience some twilight states at least twice a day, when we are waking up and going to sleep. Sometimes we may have them more often, when we feel sleepy during the day, or when we are near waking at night. It is a very normal and frequent state, but we rarely pay much attention to it. Different people have different attentional styles. One interesting aspect is the question of absorption, and the interestingly related one of suggestibility. Two psychometric scales have been used extensively to p. 6 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. study Absorption and Suggestibility (a better word than hypnosis). They are both available on the web. You might want to have a good look at the specific items on those scales, which were chosen because work statistically to predict some behavior. 1. Absorption: Absorption is being fully conscious of one stream of events, like a movie or a book you are reading. It is easy to notice when you have a sense of “time loss.” This is a sense of surprise that it is later than you thought! The psychometric scale that is used most often is the Tellegen Absorption Scale: http://istsocrates.berkeley.edu/~kihlstrm/TAS.htm Tellegen & Atkinson (1974) define Absorption as … "full commitment of available perceptual, motoric, imaginative, and ideational resources to a unified representation of the attentional object" (p. 274). Meaning, whatever we are conscious of! There is a consistent finding of about 30 percent correlation between the tendency toward Absorption and the tendency to be Suggesible. Not huge, but consistent. So… 2. Suggestibility: While there is still ongoing controversy over hypnosis, there has been some solid progress in understanding. One clear finding is that about one-quarter of the normal population is highly suggestible, as measured by standard scales. So Suggestibility is an “individual differences” variable, more than it is the result of any specific hypnotic induction. A lot of this work was done at Stanford University by Ernest R. Hilgard and coworkers. You can see the Stanford Hypnotizability Scale at stsocrates.berkeley.edu/~kihlstrm/ PDFfiles/Hypnotizability/SHSSC%20Script.pdf p. 7 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. Again, we recommend that you take a look at the items on the scale. Suggestibility is assessed by doing a traditional series of suggestions about relaxation, looking at a point on the ceiling, closing the eyes, and so on. Then there is a series of tests --suggestions to do things that are increasingly improbable. Most people will allow their arms to float upward upon suggestion, for example, but only a quarter of the population might be suggested into hearing a buzzing fly. A tiny percentage of people can be suggested into a “divided consciousness” state, an apparent dissociative state. More about this later on. 3. One selective attention system in the brain. Professor Tutis Villis: A brain circuit involved in the control of eye movement. http://www.med.uwo.ca/physiology/courses/sensesweb/ Click on Prof. Villis’ Lecture 11, Eye movements, for an excellent graphics show of some basic ways in which your brain moves your eyes. There are many attentional systems in the brain, if we define “attention” as the selection of conscious contents. Eye movement control can be considered as one basic example. 4. There are some interesting current hypotheses about brain differences between highly suggestible and low-suggestible people. Helen J. Crawford’s laboratory site at the University of Vermont shows articles claiming that the front part of the corpus callosum --- the great bridge of neurons that runs between the two p. 8 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. hemispheres --- is thicker in people who show high suggestibility. That may mean that there is a denser population of neurons in the front of the brain, the prefrontal cortex, involved in voluntary control of attention. See http://www.psyc.vt.edu/?p=faculty&f=hjc 5. Finally, there is good description of classical studies of the remarkable tenacity of our belief systems --- the basic conceptual world in which we live and act. Leon Festinger’s book When Prophecies Fail studied some spectacular examples among flying saucer devotees and “end of the world” cults. In both kinds of groups, falsification of their predictions did not change most of their beliefs. See When Prophecies Fail: http://www.freeminds.org/psych/propfail.htm p. 9 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. Other aspects of selective attention: Aiming the spotlight. As we have noted, it is useful to define "attention" as the SELECTION of conscious contents. In the theater metaphor it is whatever it is that guides the spotlight that casts the bright spot of consciousness on stage. You can think of it this way, if you imagine the spotlight pointing in different directions, to cast light on different objects: Attention as “selection of conscious contents” fits our common sense --- we ask kids in school to "pay attention" to the teacher, on the assumption that they will therefore become conscious of whatever the teacher says. In fact, the distinction between selective attention and the resulting conscious experiences pervades our language. But if that's our working definition, there must be MANY different selective attention mechanisms. For example, eye p. 10 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. movements result in conscious visual experiences. But most of our eye movements are not consciously controlled! If you carefully watch another person reading a book, you will notice that their eyes don't move smoothly --- they jump from place to place. We have no experience of that subjectively --- my reading eye movements appears to be very smooth --- but all of us do it anyway. It’s easy to see. So eye movements are one set of selective mechanisms that result in conscious visual experiences, but which we do not control or track consciously most of the time. Well, that implies that the "spotlight controller" might be sitting in the dark unconscious --- a lot of selection of our conscious contents goes on unconsciously. And so we arrive at an old Jamesian distinction between "voluntary" and "nonvoluntary" control of selective attention. When we ask somebody to pay attention, that is obviously voluntary (i.e. he person is given a conscious goal to achieve). In humans that requires prefrontal cortex. When a loud noise startles you, that is one kind of nonvoluntary attention, controlled by the early stages of the auditory system, located in the brainstem. These are completely different brain regions, but they both result in something becoming conscious! In the real world, our attentional selection is always a mix of voluntary and nonvoluntary elements. James talked about attentional habits, such as the habit of reading that you are indulging in right now! You may not remember it in any detail, but at one time in your early life you had to pay voluntary attention to the differences between letters like “b,” “p,” “d” and “q”. Those letters are now automatically recognizable, they are habitually selected to come to consciousness. One result of these automatic (unconscious) attentional habits is the famous Stroop Effect. This is a color naming task: As p. 11 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. quickly as you can, call out the colors you see, not the words you are reading: Was it hard? It is for us. We always tend to make the mistake of calling out the words, rather than the colors of the words. It is easy to show that this is due to our “attentional habits” of selecting the sounds and meanings of words, rather than their colors. To show this, all we need to do is turn the same set of words upside down so that you can’t read them easily, and try it again. Just call out the colors as quickly as you can: Now do you find it easier to name the colors? We do. That is because our automatic “attentional habits” of reading words are not p. 12 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. getting in the way. In fact, the Stroop Effect involves a conflict between voluntary and nonvoluntary selective attention. If you ever experience a conflict between eating that delicious chocolate ice cream cone and staying on your diet, you’ll know pretty much what we mean. Interestingly, conflictual tasks seem to activate the same part of the brain, regardless of the kind of conflict involved. (The anterior cingulate cortex). As James says beautifully (see Week 1), what we experience consciously over the hours and years of normal life is what we SELECT. We are not what we eat, but what we read, listen to, watch on TV and movies, and otherwise allow to enter our consciousness. “…what is called our 'experience' is almost entirely determined by our habits of attention. A thing may be present to a man a hundred times, but if he persistently fails to notice it, it cannot be said to enter into his experience. We are all seeing flies, moths, and beetles by the thousand, but to whom, save an entomologist, do they say anything distinct? On the other hand, a thing met only once in a lifetime may leave an indelible experience in the memory. Let four men make a tour in Europe. One will bring home only picturesque impressions -- costumes and colors, parks and views and works of architecture, pictures and statues. To another all this will be non-existent; and distances and prices, populations and drainage-arrangements, door- and windowfastenings, and other useful statistics will take their place. A third will give a rich account of the theatres, restaurants, and public halls, and naught besides; whilst the fourth will perhaps have been so wrapped in his own subjective broodings as to be able to tell little more than a few names of places through which he passed. Each has selected, out of the same mass of presented objects, those which suited his private interest and has made his experience thereby. …” p. 13 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005. (Needless to say, by “men” James meant “people.”) This is not to say that we can arbitrarily select what we are going to be conscious of. For example, try crossing a busy highway without paying attention to oncoming traffic. We’ll bet it’s impossible. (But don’t try it at home.) Selective attention is almost always functional, it helps us to survive, seek pleasure and avoid pain, solve problems and generally find our way through life. Dreams, fantasies, and imaginative involvement. REPEATED THEMES: From Domhoff *** The major unexpected finding from studying several different lengthy dream series using the Hall and Van de Castle (1966) coding system is how consistent dream content is over years and decades for most people, whatever their motives may be for writing down their dreams (Domhoff, 1996, 2003). Future courses. We are pleased with the progress of this WebCourse, and with the active debates going on in the Discussion threads. Now we are planning for a more advanced course, Consciousness II: The WebCourse. It will cover “Emotions, higher states and the brain.” If you would like to share your thoughts about future courses, please send an email to Dr. Baars. p. 14 – Lecture 6. Consciousness: The WebCourse. B.J. Baars & K.A. McGovern, 2005.