Engaging children and young people in research Literature review for The National Evaluation of the Children’s Fund (NECF) October 2004 Dr. Jane Coad (School of Health Sciences, The University of Birmingham) Professor Ann Lewis (School of Education, The University of Birmingham). Acknowledgements Our thanks to Karen Peckings, Research Assistant, who carried out, with care and enthusiasm, a substantial part of the search for relevant literature. We are very grateful to Lin Walsh who provided valuable secretarial support enabling the project to progress smoothly. Felicity Shelton kindly gave us various useful documents including the Investing in Children reports, Co.Durham, cited in the review. Priscilla Alderson allowed us access to a pre-publication draft of the revised edition of Alderson, P. & Morrow, G. Ethics, Social Research and Consulting with Children and Young People. London: Barnardo's. Finally, our thanks to members of the National Evaluation of the Children Fund (NECF) team who provided a valuable sounding board for ideas and a recurrent review of the issues. Contact details Dr. Jane Coad School of Health Sciences, The University of Birmingham, B15 2TT Tel 0121 414 2272/6893; e mail j.coad@bham.ac.uk CONTENTS Acknowledgements Executive/user summary 1. 2. Introduction 1.1. Conduct of the review 1.2. Background to the review 1 Children as research participants: Policy and research context 2.1 Policy context 5 5 1 3. Children as research participants: Ethical concerns and children’s rights 3.1 Social responsibility 3.2 Access/gatekeepers 3.3 Consent/ assent 3.4 Confidentiality/ anonymity/secrecy 3.5 Recognition and feedback 3.6 Ownership 9 9 10 11 14 15 16 4. Children as research participants: Researcher-researched relationships 4.1 Building relationships 4.1.1 Entering the field 4.1.2 Balancing differential power relationships 4.1.3 Sharing control 18 18 19 20 23 5. Children as research participants: Methods and techniques used with children to explore their views 5.1 Guiding principles 5.1.1 Authenticity 5.1.2 Credibility 5.1.3 Trustworthiness 25 5.2 Overview of possible methods 5.2.1 Interviews 5.2.2 Questionnaires 5.2.3 Observation 5.2.4 Mapping 5.2.5 Drawing and posters 5.2.6 Photographs and video 5.2.7 Role play, drama and story telling 5.2.8 Journals and diaries 5.2.9 ICT-linked 6. Children as research participants: Analyses and Dissemination 6.1. Introduction 6.2. Children as data analysts? 6.3. Dissemination and impact 25 25 27 28 29 29 33 34 35 36 38 39 40 40 42 42 42 45 7. Summary and conclusions 48 1. INTRODUCTION 1.1 Conduct of the Review The purpose of this review is: 1. To provide background information in the relevant fields, about the perspectives and issues surrounding engaging children as participants in all phases of a research/ evaluation project 2. To provide a review drawn on a systematic examination of the key literature pertaining to potential methods and techniques used when exploring children’s views. The review comprised of two distinct phases. Further details of the conduct of the review are included in Appendix 1. 1.2 Background to the Review Participation of children and young people in service design, delivery and evaluation is central to the Government’s agenda for addressing social exclusion. This was reinforced in the Green Paper, Every Child Matters (DfES, 2003) and Every Child Matters; The next steps (DfES, 2004), which flagged up the bringing of children’s perspectives to bear on all aspects of government policy (see section 2.1 for further discussion of the policy context). 1 The Children’s Fund is a major government initiative targeted at children aged 5 – 13 years, who are at risk of social exclusion. A key feature of the Children’s Fund is children’s active involvement in the development of preventive services reflecting the then, Children and Young People’s Unit (CYPU) advocacy of children’s participation (CYPU, 2001; Sinclair et al, 2002). CYPU also supported the development of associated research-based guidance (Kirby et al, 2003) building on perceived good practice. Therefore, there was a clear expectation that local Children’s Fund partnerships would actively seek out the opinions of children living in the community, in order to ensure that, their views directly influence the shape, delivery and subsequent evaluation of services. The success of this initiative will be heavily dependant upon the development of strategies which both engage with, and facilitate, meaningful input from children and young people aged 5 to 13 year olds. Particularly, there is a clear expectation that the voices of marginal groups are represented. One way in which children and young people can have their say is through the use of methods which successfully elicit their views and beliefs and which enable them to influence how initiatives are developed and evaluated. 1 One can discern two important trends in research with children in this context over the past two decades. One trend, echoed in the policy changes noted above, is the development of research-based approaches to explore the views of children and young people (Kirby, 1999; Lewis and Lindsay, 2000). A second trend is the emergence of what have been termed variously ‘emancipatory research’ or ‘participatory research’ 1 Note: unless otherwise indicated the terms ‘child’/’children’ are used in the review to refer to a child or young person up to age 13. 2 approaches (Oliver, 1997; Minkler, 2003; Suarez-Balcazar and Harper, 2003). Central to both these sets of approaches is recognition of the nature of traditional power relationships between researcher-researched and attempts to re-balance these away from the researcher (discussed further in section 3 below). However, advocates of ‘emancipatory research’ or ‘participatory research’ approaches would argue that there are substantial philosophical differences between the breaking down of traditional power relationships, and research which remains controlled by researchers (but still aiming to seek the views of children). We may ask ourselves why there such contrasting views about the concepts used when engaging children and young people in research. Firstly, the legacy of multiplicity of research approaches and techniques has meant there has never been so many paradigms from which to approach a problem or given project. It is a time of discovery and rediscovery and new ways of looking, interpreting and writing are integral to this discovery. Indeed, Denzin and Lincoln (1998) note that the considerable debate about the various merits of particular epistemological positions in terms of child centred research is inevitable due to the evolving historical nature of research. It is thus part of our philosophical positions and fields from which we have evolved. What is also clear is that it is from such contrasting epistemological positions that we approach children’s participation. This will inevitably affect the basis on which the researcher-researched relationship is formed and will impact upon the chosen methodologies, methods and tools for data collection. Whilst such debate is relevant, it was decided in the literature review here outlined, to refer to these trends collectively under the umbrella term ‘child-centred approaches’. 3 The remainder of this report begins by outlining how the review was carried out, then moves on to locating, in the current policy context, the enthusiasm for exploring children’s views and involving children at all stages of research. The next section reviews power relationships and ways of redressing potential researcher-researched imbalances with children. A variety of methods and techniques are examined in the light of guiding principles for involving children as co-researchers. Finally, we give a brief discussion about two relatively unexplored aspects of involving children as co-researchers: their roles in analyses and dissemination. 4 2. CHILDREN AS RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS: POLICY AND RESEARCH CONTEXTS 2.1 Policy context The UN Convention on the Rights of the Child (UNCRC) calls for State parties to: ‘assure to the child who is capable of forming his or her own views the right to express those views freely in all matters affecting the child, the views of the child being given due weight in accordance with the age and maturity of the child’ (Article 12). These rights are aspirational and conditional. In discussions, the UN noted the important proviso that these children’s rights must respect the rights and reputations of others; rights could not be exercised in ways that would harm others. The UNCRC has been ratified by all except two nations, Somalia and the US (the US has signed, a lesser step, but not ratified the Convention). Ratification of the Convention leads to close monitoring by the UN and the semi-public nature of the subsequent reports has provided an important lever for campaigners across the world. A huge international body of policy makers and pressure groups (government and voluntary) has grown up in response to Article 12. These groups are united in a firm conviction about the importance of involving children in decision-making; this agenda overlaps with democratisation and citizenship. Web-based groups such as CRIN (Children’s Rights Information Network), CAPA (Children as Partners Alliance) and 4NCPN (4 Nations Child Policy Network) link groups and individuals world-wide in pursuit of this aim. Their agenda falls beyond the scope of this review, and whilst our 5 review provides a research-based set of guidelines for practice (see also Alderson and Morrow, in press; Porter and Lewis, in press), we have not reviewed the very extensive sets of material available through such groups. These sites and groups provide access to a wealth of resources but tend to be collection points for information, rather than providing critical overviews or critiques of individual projects and resources. Similarly, within the UK, children’s pressure/ information groups (e.g. Article 12. Children’s Rights Alliance for England (CREA); Action for Sick Children; Funky Dragon; HeadsUp; National Voice; Voices from Care etc), while relevant to our focus, take in a broader agenda than addressed here. The UK submitted progress reports to the UN in 1994 and 1999. The UN response to the first of these reports noted concerns that ‘insufficient attention has been given to the right of the child to express his/her opinion …. In this as in other decisions, including exclusion from school, the child is not systematically invited to express his/her opinion and those opinions may not be given due weight, as required under article 12 of the Convention’ (UN CRC/C/15 Add.34 15 Feb 1995; reprinted in UK, 1999 p208). These concerns were reiterated, although progress was acknowledged, when the following UK report was received: ‘The Committee is concerned that the obligations of Article 12 have not been consistently incorporated in legislation, for example; in education ... schoolchildren are not consulted in matters that affect them’ (UN 2002: Para 29). (Note the UK’s 2nd report was submitted in 1999, discussed by the UN in June 2002 and the UN’s formal response issued in October 2002.). Participation by children and the explicit attempt to include their views in matters of UK social and public policy have increased markedly in recent years (Stafford et al, 2003; 6 Willow, 2002). Contexts in which children’s views have been formally sought (in addition to the now more conventional areas of education, health, public care and child protection) include caring for parents with a mental illness (Aldridge, 2003), the Family Court Welfare Service (Buchanan, Hunt, Bretherton, and Bream, 2001), area regeneration (Crowther et al, 2003) and domestic violence (Mullender et al, 2002). Perhaps in anticipation of the UK’s quinquennial review to the UN (January 2004), there was a torrent of government initiatives, particularly from the Department of Health, involving hearing children’s views in matters that concern them. This theme is evident too in recent policy proposals (DoH, 2003; Audit Commission, 2003; DFES, 2003). Similarly, the revised Special Educational Code (SEN) Code of Practice (DFES, 2001a), associated SEN Toolkit (DFES, 2001b) and the Government’s strategy for SEN (DFES, 2004) stress the importance of building a ‘listening culture’ in organisations. The emphasis from the UK government has been on formally hearing children’s views and, for many campaigners from the children’s charities, this is a very weak response. It stops short of empowering and involving children and young people as partners in developing their services. For example, Save the Children (2000) argued that the UK response has been piecemeal, welfare- rather than rights- based, lacking support in law and failing to give all ‘vulnerable’ children the right to independent legal advice. Responses to particular documents have highlighted the nuances; for example, Young Minds response to the draft Mental Health Bill noted that decision- makers must take 'proper account' of parents' views but only 'consider' the child's views. Sociologists of childhood (Prout, 2000, 2001, 2002; James and Prout 1997; Christensen and James, 2000) provide several fascinating critiques about the rationale behind these 7 policy changes. Why, we might ask, have we recently become so concerned about hearing children’s views that doing so is an imperative for child-related policy initiatives and research endeavours? It is beyond the scope of this review to explore this whole area but two points warrant attention here. One response takes a strong generational perspective and it is argued that generational order is of equal importance to gender, ethnicity or class as a social axis (Alanen and Mayall, 2001). From this perspective children can be seen to represent human capital (the next generation) and that by ‘controlling children’ (including the ways in which we access their views) adults are trying to control the future- a land where despite adults’ best efforts the children will, in due course, rule. In support of this, some writers have noted the contrast between the abandoned private realm of children within the family and the increasing controlling, constraining and corralling which goes on around children in the public sphere. From this perspective, hearing children’s views is emphatically not about, as some rhetoric would have us believe, empowering children or devolving adult power. A different emphasis from this has been on childhood as a conceptually autonomous arena. From this perspective children are viewed, not merely as a prism through which to see adulthood and adult-led institutions but as social actors in their own right. Here, children’s multiple interactions and the ways in which children make sense of these become the focus of interest without requiring any recourse to adult perspectives. 8 3. CHILDREN AS RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS: ETHICAL CONCERNS AND CHILDREN’S RIGHTS There is, rightly, concern about the ethical aspects of involving children as researchers and in hearing their views (Beresford, 1997; Lindsay, 2000; Moore et al, 1998; Alderson and Morrow in press). Lewis and Porter (in press) provide a series of associated guiding questions to act as the basis for self-review for researchers and service providers. Concerns have revolved particularly around six areas; each of which is considered in more detail below. 3.1 Social responsibility One of the intellectual virtues embodied in the process of carrying out research is the pursuit of truth. This links with Lindsay’s (2000) discussion about the social responsibility of the researcher. The strong rights arguments around many policies concerning children and the strength with which personal value positions are held may make it difficult to sustain research endeavours that threaten to produce findings at odds with the prevailing orthodoxy. Researchers have a responsibility to acknowledge both their own value positions and whatever issues or accounts that emerge from the research process. This resonates with Pring’s (2000) reference to the ‘integrity of the research’. Complex ethical questions arising in the course of a project (such as whether a respondent has the right to alter the record of an interview) may be best answered by the researcher asking themselves what would be consistent with maintaining the integrity of the research. 9 3.2 Access/gatekeepers Unless the researcher is interviewing their own child then someone acts as a gatekeeper, providing or withholding access, to the child to be interviewed. In most cases this direct (1st level) gatekeeper will be the parent or carer. Somebody else may in turn act as an indirect (2nd level) gatekeeper to the parents and carers. In school contexts this may be the headteacher, school governors or LEA; but, depending on the focus of the research, it may, also or instead, be health, legal, and/or social service agencies. There are ethical committees and protocols designed to protect children from unwarranted intrusion by potential researchers (e.g. British Educational Research Association, 2004; McIntosh et al, 2000; Lindsay, 2000; Royal College of Nursing, 2004). These procedures and their interpretation will shape the nature of the group of children involved as researchers and hence the range of views ultimately collected. A clear illustration in the policy context occurs when a school / hospital chooses to opt out of involvement in an evaluation consequently removing a particular group from those whose views are accessed. This may also occur through tangential circumstances rather than by design, as when an organisation withdraws from the study due to, for example, staff illness or prioritising of inspection arrangements. Decisions about sample have repercussions for access (and vice versa) with consequent implications for the interpretation of the findings of the work. 10 3.3 Consent/assent The continuum from informed consent – through assent - to failure to object, highlights the distinction between consent and assent. Consent may be given by the child or by another on the child’s behalf for (a) the child to be interviewed or (b) the researcher to ask the child to be interviewed. Assent is generally used to refer to the child’s agreement to participation in the process when another has given consent. In the more conventional context of interviewing adults these two aspects are conflated, that is the adult being interviewed both consents and assents to the interview. Consent is not in itself sufficient; informed consent/assent is needed. In order to give informed consent the person needs the four aspects outlined in table 1. Table 1: Four aspects of informed consent The person needs information about the chance to participate The person needs to know about a right to withdraw from the activity, What the participant’s role will be What the outcomes are intended to be To be able to respond to all the above four aspects of informed consent the participant (or someone on their behalf) has to receive the information, understand it and respond to it (Alderson, 1995). Spelt out in this way it can be seen that obtaining informed consent may be a considerable undertaking and daunting to achieve. Some writers have argued that, while involving children and young people in research and evaluation is important, it 11 may be very difficult genuinely to obtain their informed consent (McCarthy, 1998; Clegg, 2001; Homan, 2001). There is strong agreement among commentators that allowing informed dissent is crucial. Children have a right to privacy that researchers have a moral responsibility to acknowledge (Homan, 2001). A child’s expression of informed dissent may not be easy to recognise. For example, there may be disagreement among adults about whether a particular behaviour by a child with severe or profound and multiple learning difficulties reflects dissent. Keeping an open dialogue with the network of people around the child helps to sustain checks on whether the child is continuing to assent to involvement (see Porter et al, 2001 re: validating communication). Explicit continuation of assent enables a corresponding and genuine right to withdraw at any point. In the legal context, much stress is placed on whether a person is competent to give consent: ‘A child who has the capacity to understand fully a decision affecting his or her life automatically has the capacity to make that decision unless statute law states otherwise’ (Masson, 2001: 39). This is referred to, in short, as the Gillick competence test after the Gillick (1985) case concerning under 16 year olds’ right to contraception without the permission of their parents. The court found in favour of the General Practitioner (GP). This set a precedent in that it allowed under 16 year olds to consent to medical treatment providing they could show ‘sufficient understanding’ and ‘competence to make wise choices’. Competence is defined by Masson (2001) as the level of understanding needed to make decisions. In law, there is no presumption of competence for people under 16 and those under this age must demonstrate their competence by meeting certain standards set by the courts (British Medical Association/BMA, 2003). 12 In the research context, there has been heated debate concerning at what age children are able to consider fully the implications of participation (or non-participation) (e.g. O’Donnell and Strasburger, 1998). Definitions of competence may be particularly contentious when children or young people with learning disabilities are involved. (Moore et al, 1998). Regardless of the legal debates, lack of competence does not remove the right to express a view. There is unlikely to be a blanket answer in terms of children’s ages concerning when competence as research participants can be assumed; as Masson (2001) notes that competence is directly related to the decisions to be taken and so will vary from project to project. Lengthy debate has ensued around how such competence is to be defined and demonstrated. The increasing emphasis on multi-professional working, epitomised in the work of the Children’s Fund and nascent Children’s Trusts, means that tightly defined positions (e.g. competence and legal definitions) have repercussions for all professionals working with a child. Such professional parameters can no longer be regarded as taken for granted ground rules across the whole team of co-workers. More fundamentally, notions of competence, capability and capacity have been the focus of intense debate in the philosophy literature, triggered by Amartya Sen’s seminal work. In summary, definitions of (for example) competence rest on contested assumptions about ‘normality’ (Terzi, 2003) and well-being (Saito, 2003). Thus, deciding whether a child is competent to have their views heard, or to be a research participant, prompts deeper searching which overlaps with the concerns of sociologists of childhood noted earlier (Prout, 2000; 2001). 13 3.4 Confidentiality/ anonymity/secrecy Formal guidance on research methods usually stresses the importance of confidentiality. This seems right, proper and uncontroversial. However, it may be more difficult to sustain in practice, particularly if small or atypical groups are involved, than exhortations to sustain confidentiality suggest. Confidentiality may also not be sustained for different reasons – that is, if the child reveals information that the interviewer feels should be passed on in the child’s best interests such as a child protection issue. Consequently, Oakley (2000) strongly recommends that it might be felt that it is preferable to exclude a particular type of data collection if its collection might place the researcher in an invidious ethical position (and hence jeopardise the relationship with the child). A researcher may attempt to guarantee anonymity in any written documentation (that is comments or views are not attributed in a way that could be traced back to a specific individual). This may mean that some views have to be excluded from the report (for example, if only one child with cerebral palsy is included in mainstream organisations in the sample then any comment reflecting that particular perspective could be traced back to an individual). Another issue about confidentiality arises from procedures concerning conducting interviews; privacy has to be balanced with child protection procedures. Whether parents should be present at interviews with their children has been much debated and it has been argued that parents may want, but not need, to know what happens. Relevant bodies produce ethical guidelines for researchers (e.g. see BPS, 1991; BSA, 1987; NCB, 1993; BERA, 2004; NHS, 2004; RCN, 2004) although the detail of these varies widely (see 14 Lindsay, 2000). Clegg (2001) argued that when interviews are conducted in a spirit of openness, then privacy/confidentiality is not an issue and the very notion of gatekeepers (see above) betrays a lack of trust between those involved with the children. There is a distinction between confidentiality, given to people participating in the research, and secrecy. Secrecy applies to procedures and in most cases such secrecy would probably be deemed inappropriate in educational, social and health care research, to name but three examples. However, there might be contexts in which it was felt legitimate to keep procedures secret (e.g. observation to monitor suspected bullying). 3.5 Recognition and feedback Often when children are involved in research this is presented as part of routine activities with no specific ‘reward’ for participating. However small token gifts such as holographic stickers given to all the children, whether or not participating, seem to be popular and provide a modest ‘thank you’. Alternatively, a group ‘treat’ such as a party may be organised. In more substantial projects researchers may give children gift vouchers or token payment in exchange for their involvement (with parental agreement). The basis of this exchange is respect for the children’s time and efforts. It is now widely recognised that participants should have the opportunity to receive feedback from researchers about the outcomes of the study. However, some sample groups move around geographically and make this sustained link difficult or impossible over a longer term project. With children, feedback may be done through adults known to 15 them. Little seems to have been written on this topic in published accounts of children’s views and it is potentially a sensitive area. 3.6 Ownership Ownership of data is generally presumed to belong to the researcher (although data protection measures apply, giving participants rights to access electronic data under certain conditions). Kellett and Nind (2001) propose the researcher as a banker, retaining data/information (e.g. video material or interview narrative) but giving others access to it. In the policy context, it might be argued that organisations should have access to such information and the right to use it in certain contexts. One might make a distinction here between data and information. Information refers to what is collected (e.g. a piece of video film) while the process of conversion or extraction from information generates data - the units or material analysed. Thus, the data are a subset of the information. There may be unintended outcomes of using protocols intended to safeguard the interests of children interviewed. For example, notions of ownership whereby materials are returned to children interviewed may be interpreted as a rejection or failure. Jean Ware (personal communication) has noted that destroying confidential materials at the close of a project may be read as discounting of the material by some children, particularly perhaps those with difficulties in learning. (‘Valuable’ material would have been retained or even displayed). Professional groups may take contrasting views, sometimes arising from particular legislative constraints, about what constitutes an authentic way to obtain children’s views. 16 A particular issue here is the use of facilitators. Ideally, facilitators should be chosen by the child. Facilitators act as intermediaries conveying, or translating, the views of those interviewed. This enables views to be collected from people who might otherwise be excluded from those whose views are accessed. However the filter of the facilitator/ intermediary may unwittingly distort the views held. If they are used then any report needs to acknowledge how views were collected so that the reader/listener can make a judgement about whether the conduit for views may have distorted the evidence. 17 4. CHILDREN AS RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS: RESEARCHER- RESEARCHED RELATIONSHIPS 4.1 Building relationships Despite the epistemological differences noted in section 1.2, researchers interested in child-centred approaches share concerns about basing the involvement of children and young people on an ethic of respect for children. Thus, they would regard preparation for children’s involvement, and the building of relationships, in whatever ways and forms, to be of paramount importance. However, the popularity of involving children raises questions about whose needs are being served. Researchers may feel obliged in the current climate to involve children at all stages of the research as part of building and sustaining relationships, but in so doing, there is a risk of exploiting those children. A genuine respect for the child’s position and interest in them, as individuals, may help to guard against this situation. Building up the relationship with the child, as a counter to exploitation, does however hold other possible difficulties. The child may later feel let down when the relationship ceases at the end of the research project. Booth (1998), writing of this issue in relation to research with lonely people, exposes the very real problems for researchers trying to operate ethically in this context. One solution, described by Crozier and Tracey (2000), is the sustaining of the relationship in the long term but this will not be realistic or feasible in all research contexts. It is thus pertinent to explore some of the issues relating to building relationships further. 18 4.1.1 Entering the field One issue concerning entering the field relates to control and negotiation of access. Commonly, this occurs through adult gatekeepers, predominantly parents, head-teachers, programme managers, key workers such as health, play and social care workers (Barker and Smith, 2001). Pragmatic advice on negotiating access and preparation before entering the field is discussed by a number of researchers (Christensen and James 2000; James and Prout 1997; Johnson et al 1998; Lewis and Lindsay 2000). Establishing rapport with the child needs serious consideration and several authors provide useful guidance about this. For example, Punch (2002) suggests that it is a misconception to assume that all adults will be able to build rapport with children; some adults will feel that children are very different from adults and that the researcher should not try too hard to establish a, perhaps phony, camaraderie. Coyne (1998) and Morrow (1999) share examples of establishing rapport prior to the data collection including using drawings to relax the child and clear information leaflets about the project. Their message is clear: researchers should follow what the children want in that particular research situation. This is reiterated by Morgan et al (2002) who suggest strategies for setting the scene; these include reference to an informal atmosphere, using first names, including ‘warm up’ activities, having adequate room space, and setting-up arrangements. However, such strategies assume that the adult researcher leads and controls the relationship with the child from the outset rather than a process of negotiation. Hence, this is a fundamentally different from a situation in which children are active researchers. 19 There is also a further consideration here. If establishing relationships with children means that children are invited to participate in research planning meetings at an early stage, there may then be repercussions for the use of the children’s time (e.g. can this justify taking children away from or school or from leisure activities?). 4.1.2 Balancing differential power relationships Balancing power between the adult-researcher and child has also received considerable attention. Holmes (1998) and Hood et al (1996) note that power relationships will exist in terms of age but may be exacerbated by differences in gender, ethnicity, culture and social background. Morrow and Richards (1996: 98) contend that `the biggest ethical challenge for researchers working with children is the disparities in power and status between adults and children'. Ring (2000, 2003) illustrates some issues concerning power relationships. Ring used conversational interviews, drawings (2D) and photographs (3D) with a longitudinal framework, with young children at home and at school. She wished to explore sociocultural aspects important in a child’s life at home and school and felt that seeing the world from the child’s perspective was the first step in building of the relationship. Others dispute that this can be possible when, from an adult’s perspective, the world is necessarily viewed differently from that of the child (Kirby, 1999; West, 1995;1996a; 1996b and 1997). Part of the task is to redress the power imbalance between child participant and adult researcher, in order to enable children to participate on their own terms. There are several 20 suggestions for equalising the power relations between adult researcher and child, such as the need for reflexivity, responsiveness, fun and allowing the children greater participation and control (Mayall et al, 1996; 2000; Mauthner, 1997; Clark and Moss, 2001). Butler and Williamson (1994) provide a helpful framework for balancing the gap between adult researcher and child participants (see table 2). Table 2: Balancing the imbalance of power between adult researcher and child participant Adopt a role of ‘naïve curiosity’ in which the researcher is open, honest and Understanding but not patronising. Avoid being judgmental but, rather, accepting of the child’s viewpoint as being different from that of adults. Allow the child to present their views. Be creative and flexible in approaches so as to reduce boredom and free children to talk about other issues. Adapted from Butler and Williamson 1994 The power imbalance between children and important adults in their lives may also prevent children's full participation in the research (Morgan et al, 2002). This is likely to be particularly pertinent in relation to young children and children with learning difficulties. Interestingly, Ireland and Holloway (1996) found (in a project seeking to explore children’s experiences of Asthma) that some parents tried to gently coerce their children into taking part in the study. As qualitative approaches seek to empower informants, Ireland and Holloway (1996) consequently felt concerned about this issue. There would seem no clear answers for this, but rather, a flexible attitude and approach is indicated (Faux et al, 1988). Ward (1996) recommends that the researcher talk to the children away from the parents whilst Koocher and Keith-Spiegel (1994) suggest parents 21 are a source of comfort in what potentially might be a daunting experience. In relation to child or young person with learning difficulties, the issues are ‘writ large’ with proxies or facilitators being used as conduits for, or to provide support. Another solution is to find ways to access children’s views directly, which involves thinking creatively about methods used (see section 4), for example, using ICT-supported communication. Significant other features that impact on the balance of power in the relationship are contextual characteristics such as the environment in which the research takes place (McTaggart, 1996; Scott, 2000). It is argued that, any environment will affect the researcher-child relationship (Lewis and Lindsay, 2000; Clark and Moss, 2001). Several studies highlighted environmental influences, which are presented in table 3. Table 3:Summary of environmental influences on the research context Schools (Pridmore and Bendelow, 1995; Johnson et al, 1998; Warren from Lewis and Lindsay, 2000; Horner, 2000; Hek, 2002; Ring, 2003) Early childhood centres/play/nursery schools (Clark and Moss, 2001) Hospitals and child health clinics (Ireland and Holloway, 1996; Morgan et al, 2002) Children and young people ‘clubs’ and community rooms/centres such as libraries (Fine and Sandstrom, 1988; Sheffield Children’s Fund, 2003) Children’s own home and family settings (Miller, 2000) Festivals and planned events (Willow, 2002) Work based settings as noted in adolescent research (Dashiff, 2001) Each context will bring its own challenges. In adult-led environments (for example, schools) the child may feel pressured whereas children’s own spaces can enable them to 22 feel more comfortable (McTaggart, 1996; Scott, 2000; Punch, 2002). Fine and Sandstrom (1988) refer to neutral settings such as community rooms which may help to redress the power imbalance between adult researcher and child participant. However, Horner (2000) and Ring (2000) both found that schools can also be perceived as neutral ground and the familiar sense of community can be subsequently important for the collection of rich data. Indeed, much of the educational research reviewed took place in schools and in terms of practicalities would seem a convenient location for research with children. The problem with environments, such as schools, is that they are perceived as being adult-led, are boundary-organised and so may influence responses. For example, the child participant may feel obliged to respond in a way that reflects the adult’s perceived position; an aspect known as ‘ideal speech’ (Christensen and James, 2000). Nesbitt (2000) illustrates this point with reference to her interviews with children of different faiths and denominations. She notes that some Baptist children used the term ‘vicar’ to describe their church leader when ‘minister’ would have been the appropriate term. 4.1.3 Sharing control Strategies to facilitate genuine empowerment within the research setting are reflected in Alderson (1993) and Alderson and Morrow (in press) who recommend informality as one approach to redressing the power imbalance. This must be set against the danger of an adult researcher trying too hard to part of the group, reflected in the use of child-derived colloquial language or acting as a friend in the ‘gang’, which may result in the child becoming cautious and suspicious (James et al, 1998). 23 Other authors highlight a strategy of giving some control to children from the outset so that the project leads, who are adults, hand the research gradually over to the children (Alderson, 1993; Kirby, 1999; West, 1995; 1996a; 1996b; 1997; Khan et al, 1997). In this model, the adult researchers will oversee the work and direct specific stages such as the writing of the reports. Morrow (1999) notes that in the process of giving control to children, it is still the adults who will act as ‘gatekeepers’ to the children. Others advocate approaches whereby children are the project leads and so control all stages of the work including the report writing. ‘Investing in Children’ and ‘Triumph and Success’ illustrate this approach. The ‘Investing in Children’ project is based in County Durham, UK and has involved groups of young people in completing their own research including ‘Sour Grapes’ – How young people’s views are taken into account (1998); Young People’s Mental Health (2000); ‘Fares Fair’ - Investigating local transport (2000) and Young People and the Police (2001). The ‘Triumph and Success’ two-year project involved eight young people between the ages of 15 and 21 years, from different social and economic backgrounds, who explored youth transitions in Sheffield, UK (France 2000). Each of the project teams reported that the children were given a voice in the design and implementation of the research and consequently had an impact on the shaping and development of local services. Both projects built in considerable support and training for the young researchers from experienced adults working in the respective fields, but were fully controlled and managed projects undertaken by young people. Whilst both of these projects are clearly innovative and encompass the philosophical beliefs of empowerment, challenges of research training and ongoing support cannot be overlooked. 24 5. CHILDREN AS RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS: METHODS AND TECHNIQUES USED WITH CHILDREN TO EXPLORE THEIR VIEWS 5.1 Guiding principles No research is value-free and the potential for bias in child-centred research has been well documented (Lewis, 1992; Lewis and Lindsay, 2000; Christensen and James, 2000). Inevitably, the researcher’s values will have an impact on the relationship with child participants. Lewis (1992) warns that the researcher needs to consider carefully whether the children’s inter-relationships, as well as his/her relationship with the children, will distort responses. We have thus suggested elsewhere (Lewis, 2003; 2004) that authenticity, credibility and trustworthiness represent three guiding principles when involving children in research (see Pring, 2000; Robson, 2002 for related background reading). 5.1.1 Authenticity Authenticity refers to the extent to which something genuinely comes from the child (analogous to the authenticity of a painting as having demonstrably been carried out by the attributed painter). It is doubtful whether using parents, siblings, social workers, teachers, nurses or others, to pass on the ‘child’s views’ rather than asking the child directly can be justified. On occasion, intermediaries may be needed to convey the child’s views. There is a distinction between intermediaries (sometimes termed facilitators) whose role is to convey the child’s views and proxies who speak for the child. In practice, the roles elide. The use of proxies, particularly in relation to children with learning 25 disabilities, has generated considerable controversy. For example, doubts have been raised about the extent to which proxies report fairly the views of those they purport to represent. Clegg (2003) concludes that the ‘ideal’ proxy needs to maintain the difficult balance between ‘imaginative fusion and reflective separation’ (2003: 4). Her phrase captures the need for the proxy to not only stand where the child is (metaphorically) but also to interpret that position to a wider world. Ware (in press), in her discussion of this issue, argues that inferences based on observation of the child’s behaviour may be more useful than directly elicited responses. Although Ware has in mind children who show profound and multiple learning difficulties, her point may apply equally well to young, non-disabled children. Observation of young children is a well-established approach in the developmental literature, but features in a minority of papers reviewed concerning accessing children’s views (e.g. Clark and Moss, 2001). Consequently we have not addressed this explicitly within our review of methods (section 5.2). An integral principle of child-centred research is that a child’s language is different from an adult’s language. Baumann (1997) suggests that the process of understanding children should be considered a task of translation or interpretation, in which the requirement is to be faithful to the original meaning. However, DeVellis (1991); Waksler (1991) and Alderson and Goodey (1996) all note that the process is more than translation, suggesting reflexivity is also essential in listening to the narrative and subsequently interpreting children’s voices. Thus, the distance between the translation and the original requires a compromise which is worked out in the ‘to and fro’ of dialogue. 26 Consequently, there is a need to ensure mutual comprehension between the adult researcher and the child’s language. Beresford (1997) suggests adopting the child’s language whereas other authors stress the child may view this approach with suspicion (Christensen and James, 2000). Simplicity would seem fundamental. The questions need to be concise and clear (McGurk and Glachan, 1988; Mahon et al, 1996) and the language used appropriate for the cognitive capacity of that group (Faux, 1988; Alderson and Goodey, 1996). Whilst Moston (1987) purported that children’s first responses should be accepted, Alderson and Goodey (1996) noted that researchers need to explore the meanings which children give to specific words. De Vellis (1991) also reminds researchers that any language used by participants must be considered in the context of cultural diversity. Furthermore, an operational understanding of the child’s linguistic identity such as dialect and associated ‘group’ language (such as the language used by adolescents) is required. Ignoring such issues is likely to impede the ability to understand identity and distinct sub-groups. 5.1.2 Credibility A related idea, credibility, refers to the extent to which it is believable that a response has come from the child. So a child may appear to put forward genuinely a particular viewpoint and yet that response may lack credibility i.e. it is felt that the child is, for example, echoing what she has been told by an adult. The concept of credibility is very close to notions of face validity and tends to be used by researchers working with interpretative research designs and methods. 27 5.1.3 Trustworthiness Trustworthiness, or reliability, encompasses the idea that the child’s input/ response is representative, or a fair reflection, of what the child believes. The idea that children can be trusted to give (and, presumably, even more strongly- to obtain) reliable evidence has often been challenged. For example, less than 20 years ago Heydon, an English lawyer, voiced considerable scepticism: First, a child’s powers of observation are less reliable than an adult’s. Second, children are prone to live in a make believe world … Thirdly, they are also very egocentric…Fourthly because of their immaturity they are very suggestible... A fifth danger is that children often have little notion of the duty to speak the truth. Finally children sometimes behave in a way evil beyond their years (1984: 84). In contrast to this perspective, recent work (e.g. Johnson, 2002) takes children’s evidence sufficiently seriously to warrant micro-analysis of particular elements (e.g. Soutterances) leading her to conclude that institutional context has a more powerful effect than interviewer style on the properties of talk. Many contextual factors are likely to affect the trustworthiness of children’s input/responses (e.g. the relationship between interviewer and interviewee, the setting for the interview; see Christensen and James, 2000 for review and section 4 above). Further, children’s cognitive capabilities interact with their memory and emotions (Dockrell, 2000) thus while in one situation a child may play down their views, in another situation they may exaggerate them. 28 5.2 Overview of possible methods There exists an abundance of publications and grey literature highlighting the many diverse methods for use in child-centred research. Some of the techniques are complex, use a multiplicity and are highly technical whilst others are uncomplicated and reflexive. A range are reviewed here. 5.2.1 Interviews – Individual and focus groups By far the most favoured technique both historically and current is the use of interviews. Indeed, many writers have offered guidance on a range of interviews, including one to one, family and group interviews (see Alderson, 1993; Steward et al, 1993; Mahon and Glendinning, 1996; Faux et al, 1988; Coyne, 1998; Thomas and O’Kane, 1998; Lewis and Lindsay, 2000; Sartain et al, 2000; Miller, 2000; Hibbert 2002; Kortesluoma et al, 2003; Callery et al, 2003). General contextual issues are firstly explored followed by specific notes about one to one and group interviews. Although interviews require well-developed communication skills, they are potentially very effective in giving children a voice. Hill et al (1996) considered interviews to be the most economical form of data collection in child-centred research. A basic requirement is that the researcher is genuinely interested in the response. Therefore, interview schedules should be well-planned to ensure that open-ended questions are used as much as possible (Moston, 1990; Dockrell, 2000). Clearly, questions may need re-phrasing if words are unfamiliar, and linguistic and/ or cognitive skills limited. Careful piloting is likely to help 29 the interviewer to anticipate appropriate re-phrasings (Faux et al, 1988; Saywitz and Snyder, 1993). This applies to both adult and child interviewers. Our earlier review about the full information of participants (section 4) is also relevant here. In order to encourage rich responses from children, Hill et al (1996) recommend that they are given full information about the focus and purpose of the interview beforehand. Children, especially those of school age, may take part to please the adult researcher or alternatively may do so purely through enjoyment of the attention and/or novelty of the occasion (Alderson, 1993; Faux et al, 1988; Gunther, 1991). Thomas and O’Kane (1998), who used a combination of individual and small group interviews, suggest offering the child choice about how the interview is to be set up. For example, children may prefer to participate with others in the same situation and so prefer a focus group interview. Alternatively they may not wish to disclose information regarded as personal to them and therefore prefer an individual, one-to-one interview. During the interview process, Lewis (2004) has summarised points for good practice in questioning technique when exploring children’s views during the interview process, particularly with the chronologically or developmentally young. These points, drawing on research-based evidence from the developmental and cognitive psychology literature about forensic interviewing of young children include: Encouraging ‘don’t know’ responses Encouraging requests for clarification Using statements rather than questions Avoiding repeat questions Avoiding yes/no alternatives Aiming for an uninterrupted narrative 30 The implications of these suggestions, for children as researchers (e.g. as interviewers), is an unexplored area, which warrants attention as children increasingly become participants in all stages of the research process. Interestingly, Johnson et al (1998) suggest that one of the biggest problems faced by researchers is keeping the momentum going and not losing the child’s attention. Therefore, a further consideration for researchers is the use of distraction strategies in order to help facilitate the data collection. Doherty and Sandelowski (1999) suggest the strategy of offering props to stimulate recall but also to provide a distraction. Alternatively, Milne and Bull (2001) note that one way to counter the dangers of recurrent and over-specific prompting, as well as possibly inadvertent misinformation, is to endeavour to set up the interview context in a way which prompts an uninterrupted narrative. Individual interviews Many writers appear to use one-to-one interviews successfully, but frequently we found that the research accounts focused on findings rather than methods; a point also noted by Lewis and Lindsay (2000). There are conflicting views about the suitability of structured interviews with young children. Mahon and Glendinning (1996) suggest that one to one interviews are most effective with older children, particularly when exploring the biography and autobiography. However, Amato & Orhiltree (1987) gained rich and informative responses from 8-9 year olds as well as from 15-16 year olds. Similarly, 31 Miller (2000) used individual interviews effectively with children aged 7-12 years of age to elicit their experience of Diabetes Mellitus. It is worth noting that some authors suggest that it is useful to cross-check individual interviews using other methods such as group interviews or use of art techniques (Thomas and O'Kane, 1998; Coad 2002). Small group interviews The group interview is described as a discussion with a purpose that can take many forms. Clear guidance about setting these up with children is given in Lewis (1992); Horner (2000) and Morgan et al (2002). Advice includes reference to setting up the room, group support, peer influence, strategies to encourage participation, payments to children, balancing the power relationships and sustaining the interview. Number and composition of the group is important. Hill et al (1996) elicited views using groups of up to six children and Lewis and Lindsay (2000) suggest four children in a group, while Mauthner (1997) felt that three, was the optimum group size to avoid distractions. Gender is yet another important consideration. Single sex groups can be more successful than mixed groups in which boys may dominate at certain ages (Morgan, 1986; Mauthner, 1997; Warren, 2000). Horner (2000) also notes that differences in ages of group members can create an imbalance with the older or younger children dominating or, worse still, being excluded 32 In summary, advantages of group interviews with children include the potential for eliciting a greater number and broader range of responses, a less intimidating context than in individual interviews; and the value of debate between participants in clarifying understanding and generating new ideas. On the other hand, if not carefully planned and experienced interviewers used, group interviews may be disastrous. Children may feel too inhibited to speak (especially if dominant individuals are allowed to take over) or be exposed to ridicule (Balen et al, 2000; Ring, 2000; 2003). 5.2.2 Questionnaires Questionnaires and survey designs are commonly used in research, but involves skills such as reading and writing. Consequently, Barker and Weller (2003: 48) contend that children have largely been ‘rendered invisible in most large scale quantitative research’, that is, they have been excluded from participating in such approaches However, the review found several positive examples of questionnaires with children, including questionnaires designed by children (see Solberg, 1996; Miller, 1999; Oakley, 2000; Scott, 2000; Dockerell et al, 2000; Walker et al, 2002; Barker and Weller, 2003; Carney et al, 2003; Sheffield Children’s Fund, 2003). Morrow and Richards (1996) suggest using questionnaires with other data collection techniques and that, through data triangulation, validity of the project findings may be improved. However, different methods are likely to be eliciting different information and so are complementary rather than providing a validity check on other methods (see above for a similar point re interviews). 33 Lewis and Lindsay (2000: 194) note that ‘the use of questionnaires with children seems to have received little attention in methodology texts’. This review supports that conclusion as, 4 years on, we found very limited critical reviews about the methodology of the use of research questionnaires by, and with, children (see Scott (2000) for an exception). Balen et al (2000) note that questionnaires may be most effective with older children and adolescents. Carney et al (2003) evaluated several different types of questionnaire response formats with children. They concluded that a verbal structured questionnaire (compared with verbal unstructured, visual structured and visual unstructured) was the most effective tool in engaging children. 5.2.3 Observation Observational techniques have been used widely in child psychology and several articles drew on research methods derived from that field. In some work they have been used as a single technique with a group of children (Takai, 2004) whilst in other work they have been included amongst multiple methods (Davies, 1989; Warren, 2000). Observational methods can be classified along several dimensions (degree of observer’s participation, overt or covert nature of the observation and degree of structure imposed on the setting). In the present context overt, participant, unstructured observation is the usual approach taken. Corsaro and Molinari (2000), in a study about children’s transition from pre-school to elementary school in Modena, Italy, provide a rich account of how the field is entered and how this approach to observation was carried out. They note that one of the crucial elements of participant observation is the field entry; in this case, dealing with the 34 ‘gatekeepers’ who were the teachers. Corsaro and Molinari (2000) report that initially the teachers ‘teased’ the researcher about his limited use of Italian (the researcher was American and had limited command of English). Consequently, in order to achieve acceptance the researcher not only had to become part of the children’s group (undertaking and being drawn into all the children’s activities including meal breaks) but also had to overcome other challenges like the language. Consequently, Corsaro and Molinari (2000) suggest that these experiences had an impact on the observations and serve to highlight the challenging and time consuming nature of participant observation. A further potential problem, also noted by Warren (2000), is that participant observation may require a taxing dual role of the researcher. They may be both an ‘insider’ where closeness and depth of recording observations is required, but at the same time take a position of ‘outsider’ when distance and impartiality are needed. 5.2.4 Mapping Many of the methods commonly used in research, which tries to engage children in research are visual techniques. Children, including those who are not literate can use such techniques. They can, therefore, provide child-centred structure to enable children to describe their environments (Mauthner, 1997). Save the Children (2000) suggest a mapping exercise can be a small-scale model or full size simulation and commonly used for the participants’ interpretations and explanations. Clark and Moss (2001) found mapping useful with young children (under 5 years) who first took photographs, reviewed them and then made maps to illustrate how they viewed their environment. Findings highlighted their fascination with rooms they were allowed 35 in, rooms they were not allowed in, rooms where favourite activities occurred and rooms where favourite people worked. The work was supported with audiotapes of the map making sessions and conveyed valuable insights into their worlds. Similar techniques have been used with children and young people with learning difficulties and have wide applicability. Other forms of mapping are concept maps, first used as a tool to assist learners in building their own understanding (Novak and Gowin, 1984). Mavers (2001) used concept mapping in 60 schools, in an innovative project commissioned by the DFES/Becta to gain an insight into primary and secondary school pupils’ thinking about ‘Computers in My World’. Following standardised instructions about the task (aiming to reveal understanding about networked technologies on educational attainment), pupils had just 20 minutes to draw concept maps on paper. Over 3,000 concept maps were submitted in total (June 2000 and June 2001) which were analysed quantitatively and qualitatively. A different type of mapping is visual grids. Thomas and O’Kane (1999) used three stages each with its own participatory technique. At the first stage they invited children to set up their own decision-making chart with two axes on a large sheet of paper: ‘what sort of decisions’ was the top axis and ‘what people’ was the side axis. The grid was useful as it not only facilitated the children’s decision-making but it enabled the researchers to explore what the child saw as important decisions. This method draws attention to how children are able to clearly voice their issues given the opportunity to do so. A development of this approach utilises a diamond pattern in which children can place cards e.g. representing people or events, significant for them, in an array ranging in importance on both horizontal and vertical axes. These types of approach have strong intuitive appeal 36 and considerable potential (particularly with children who lack speech or language skills) but they lack systematic evaluation. 5.2.5 Drawing and posters Drawing techniques have also been used with variable success (Pridmore and Bendelow, 1995; Action for Sick Children, 1998; Punch, 2002; Ring, 2000; 2003; Barker and Weller, 2003; Sheffield Children’s Trust, 2003; Coates 2004). What was interesting to note is that we found that drawings and poster making were rarely used in isolation. Instead, they were frequently used to support other data collection techniques. Punch (2002) used drawings in an exploratory study to discover what children considered to be important aspects of their lives. The researchers concluded that drawings were a valuable technique in encouraging children to participate in research. Hill et al (1996) suggest that young children and pre-adolescents may find it difficult to convey feelings verbally so drawing is an opportunity to express fears, feelings, sensitive issues and fun through drawing and painting. Other authors have used drawings for warm up exercises (see section 3), fill-in activities during the methods and at the end whilst waiting for others to finish (Boyden and Ennew 1997). Several writers refer to children producing what is expected, such as drawing a stylised apple to represent healthy eating). So caution is needed in order to avoid over-interpreting data derived from drawings. The dangers of this are demonstrated well in experimental work by Thomas and Silk (1990). Hart (1992), in a useful overview of visual techniques, suggests that researchers may discuss the apparent ‘meaning’ although the drawing may actually mean very little to the child. There are several problems with this approach. 37 Firstly, this implies the adult researcher is collecting data from the child (seeking information as opposed to participatory techniques) and secondly the adult researcher is not a child and so may find it difficult (even impossible) to find meaning in the drawing. To overcome such problems, Pridmore and Bendelow (1995) and Bendelow et al (1996) identified ways of understanding how supplementary techniques, such as ‘draw and write’ techniques that might be effectively used in conjunction with drawings. In this technique, once the drawing is complete the researcher can spend time with the child or children discussing the drawing and adding written labels or cards to highlight meanings. Kirby (1999) refers to a range of visual techniques including the asking children to design an idea (such as a youth centre, as in Curd and YARD 1998) to be submitted on a poster (to be later analysed by the research team). Others have used the creation of posters as an integral part of an event, which aimed to develop and child-friendly techniques, that could be used to ascertain children and young people’s views in measuring a quality service (Coad et al, 2004 in press). 5.2.6 Photographs and/video This type of method allows children, equipped with cameras, to make a film of their lives or their worlds as they see them. Faulkner (1998) allowed the children to evolve into ‘reporters’ and through the experience were empowered to produce, direct, film and act and finally edit their own films. Cameras can be used in a similar way by children with learning difficulties (Germain, in press) also used to support a Talking Mats approach (Brewster, in press). 38 Photographs have also been used to highlight issues that were important to the child. Johnson (2003) used photos with a group of Australian school-aged children to help them to communicate what they liked about their schools (the colour, entrance door, playground) and what they did not like (the alleyways, rubbish). Miller (1996) asked children as young as 3 to 8 years old to take photographs of things important to them in their community for local councillors to review. As a consequence of their report, complete with their spelling mistakes, a new playground was planned. Additionally, in an unusual study, Miller (1998) used the designing of videos to support young people in saying what life was like for them in their village. The video entitled ‘No Fun in Bilsthorpe’ highlighted community issues including vandalism and crime, thus producing an important record of these young people’s worlds (Miller, 1998). 5.2.7 Role play, drama and story telling Save the Children (2000) suggest that children may find it easier to communicate through drama and oral techniques such as role play, story telling, drama, puppets and music making rather than answering direct questions (verbally or in writing). Role-play includes individual or group mimes as well as child-centred plays (preferably written by the children). Alternatively, story telling has a long history of ‘entertaining’ children but more recently used as a research technique to effectively develop rapport and identify relevant thematic findings. (Clark and Moss, 2001; Christensen and James, 2003; Coad et al, 2004 in press). However, some children may find this type of research challenging if they do not want to perform or have the required cognitive listening abilities. Whilst there 39 are limited critical reviews of such approaches, what would seem indicated is that the facilitator must have well-developed skills in order for quality evidence to be collated. 5.2.8 Journals and diaries Many participatory techniques will employ supplementary techniques (Christensen and James, 2000). Where children are literate and able this is useful. However, Save the Children (2000) note that children do not have to be expert writers as they can list, fill in forms or complete a questionnaire with adult help. Writing letters and devising poetry has been used with a degree of success although again much depends on the child’s reading and writing abilities (Save the Children, 2000). 5.2.9 Information and Communications Technology (ICT) -linked Information Communications Technology (ICT) is providing new vehicles for engaging children in research. For example; Gettings and Gladstone (2001) reported the use of PowerPoint as a stimulus to child researchers interviewing other children. They also used computer-based questionnaires to attempt to gain the perceptions of a group of students, post 16 years, with severe learning difficulties about the role and value of collaborating within the Young Enterprise Scheme. They reflected that eliciting the views of a heterogeneous group of students with severe learning difficulties was complex and fraught with difficulties; procedural, methodological and ethical. However, ICT was used in order to attempt to meaningfully gain the views of those students for whom paperbased questionnaires were not accessible. The student group were involved in the design of the questionnaires and photo, pictorial, symbol and sound cues were used to support access to the questionnaire as well as a variety of switch access modes. Whilst Gettings 40 and Gladstone (2001) note that obtaining valid and reliable views is problematic and multi-methods were also used, they conclude that this technique enabled the children to engage and views were collated effectively. As Gettings and Gladstone (2001) illustrate, the internet provides a possible forum for web-based questionnaires and on line interviews. Both are relatively new and as yet largely untested approaches as far as engaging children is concerned. These approaches have the potential to make a useful supplement to the more conventional techniques discussed above. As children tend to be avid ICT users these may have strong motivational aspects in comparison with conventional methods. We also might expect to see a growing methodological literature in relation to children’s involvement as coresearchers in web-based approaches. This would parallel texts concerning web-based data collection involving adult participants (e.g. Hewson, 2003). In due course, the web context may generate unique situations for children as co-researchers; for example, their status and identity as children could be masked and fluid, enabling ‘age-free’ identities to be explored. 41 6. CHILDREN AS RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS: ANALYSES AND DISSEMINATION 6.1. Introduction Whereas, as noted earlier, there is a strong and growing body of work about child-centred approaches to research, we found very little material that related explicitly to how traditional power relationships are broken down and how children’s roles may develop as researchers, with respect to the analysis and dissemination of evidence (see section 1.2). For example, Greig and Taylor‘s (1999) text on research with children appeared to suggest that all data analyses and dissemination would be conducted by an adult researcher. One explanation for that omission may be the date of the book as ideas in this field have changed very rapidly in recent years. These topics raise a number of pressing questions. 6.2. Children as data analysts? The first consideration in children being data analysts, is to what extent, would like to be involved in the process of analysis. For example children may decide to fully opt in, opt in for a small involvement or totally opt out of the analysis. Another suggestion offered is that adults undertake the analysis and allow children to verify the findings (Kirby, 1999). Deatrick and Faux (1991) support this, suggesting that if the project is thorough during the planning and data collection processes, then it should follow that the researcher will be a competent interpreter of the child’s world. In this way, verification strategies are 42 built in after analysis has taken place by the adult researcher. However, Mayall (1996; 2000) found that, when undertaking such a procedure, a time lapse of one year had taken place from the time of data collection to verification of the analysis. A concern then was that children’s views may have altered over that time span. Such issues are clearly contentious, and not easily resolved by the literature. Punch (2002) provides a useful, but alternative view, in which she suggests that, in relation to analysis, children should be seen as positioned along a continuum which moves back and forth according to individual needs and desires. Although her discussion paper is largely descriptive it would be useful to test out the approach during the data analysis stage of a project. It is clear from the literature that a decision needs to be made at the research planning stage about children’s level of involvement in analysis (Kirby, 1999; Christensen and James, 2000). Second, who should undertake the process of interpreting data i.e. should this be the child or the adult researcher? In many of the projects reviewed, the adult researcher had interpreted the findings (Wigglesworth, 1997; Sartain and Clarke, 2000; Miller, 2000). This was due in part to the approach taken in which the adult researcher took the lead role, sometimes as a result of the (perceived) limitations in the skills and cognitive abilities of the children. This appears to be a feasible and realistic approach but one where the position overlooks the growing recognition that adults and children inhabit different cultural worlds (McGurk and Glachan, 1988; Fine and Sandstrom, 1988; Alderson, 2000 and Kirby, 1999). Thus, a small number of researchers (e.g. West, 1996a; 1996b; Shelton 2004) argue that children do have the cognitive understanding and skills to undertake 43 analysis of data, and indeed, if research is to represent their views should be given the training and support. Third, are there certain approaches that lend themselves to analyses by child coresearchers? Wigglesworth (1997) notes that the ability to organise a narrative appears to be age linked. However, Kirby (1999) does not enter in to such debate, illustrating how children may code and categorise data. Kirby (1999) notes that in the case of quantitative data it may be coded by children on to coding sheets which are clear and designed simply for data input whilst for qualitative data it can be coded by children using a line by line reading of transcripts. A series of studies by one author outlined a ‘fully’ participatory model of children undertaking their own data analysis (West 1996a; 1996b). West (1996a; 1996b) invited children to be involved in the process of analysis and subsequent write up of the report. Building on this work, Shelton (2004) used similar processes whereby children were solely responsible for the analysis and write up of their findings. West (1996a; 1996b) notes that whilst support and training are required, the benefits of children researching and speaking for themselves outweighs the challenges that this may bring. Other authors, such as Ward (1997), sought a compromise whereby the young people who were research participants commented on reports at an early stage. The question for researchers remains: how much should adult researchers be involved in the analysis of data if children’s voices are to be heard? 44 6.3. Dissemination and impact Dissemination is crucial in achieving any impact from research. The growing involvement of children in the policy-making arena (see section 2.1) is providing various forums in which children are developing skills of being active disseminators. Indeed, we found that related web sites (see 2.1) provided illustrations of materials produced by children to disseminate collective or individual views of research. It is worth noting that some of the points made in the previous section apply also in relation to children, as coresearchers, in the dissemination process. However, there are essential, general considerations surrounding this issue, some of which include: Who is the audience? What is your message? What is the most effective way to get your message across? When is it most likely to have impact and for what purpose? Specific issues will be elaborated upon. It is noted that research undertaken by children may have a ‘novelty’ value given the current user-involvement climate (Kirby 1999). In the potential distribution lists, publications, press releases, presentations and/or media interviews such innovation may be subsequently viewed as a strength. Children have been involved in dissemination both to peers and to adults. There are interesting examples (West, 1996a; 1996b; 1997; Kirby, 1999) of children’s involvement in dissemination; techniques include presentations, web sites, newsletters and interviews in local media. These activities can include children with learning difficulties (Beresford, 1997; Morris, 1998; 2003). Importantly, some concerns have been voiced about the possible exploitation of children in these contexts. Allard (1996) notes that children may feel that 45 placing them in situations like conferences and/or meetings may be ‘embarrassing or indeed counter-productive’. Therefore, careful dissemination processes require solid, detailed planning and discussion. Furthermore, as Stafford et al (2003) note that following consultation with children, any information to be disseminated should also be agreed using a similar child-centred approach. There is also a quality issue related to dissemination. Many authors purport that children have a voice and can produce good quality research for wide dissemination given the support required (Save the Children, 2003). One example is Shelton (2004), who developed training sessions and new models of working in order to ensure children disseminated their own work, in their own words. Thus, models of good practice can only emerge through experience. Some professionals, however, question the validity of the research undertaken and note that subsequent impact may be lost (Lewis and Lindsay, 2000). In addition, involving children in the dissemination of research can have impact on professionals, both those involved in the project and the readers, making them more aware of children’s lives (Morrow, 2001a; 2001b). Involving local and national professionals throughout the project may help the commitment to the agenda. This is particularly relevant to those who will have to act upon the findings and recommendations of the project, so in turn fuelling the change agenda (Stafford et al, 2003). The alternative of the dissemination by children of findings, is researchers’ dissemination of results to child participants. This is now recognised good practice in research involving 46 children as participants and reflects the ethic of involvement. Increasingly, such feedback is adapted for a level suitable to young children or to children with special needs and/or learning difficulties. Such approaches reflect a logical progression from the careful introduction of the research questions to child participants even when they may have severe learning difficulties or severe autistic disorders (Tozer and Beresford, 2002). Dissemination to children with special needs may require appropriate costings (e.g. for braille translations) to be written into proposals and such costs recognised by funding bodies. 47 7. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS In conclusion, this review used a systematic approach to highlight some issues found in the growing literature on engaging children and young people in research. We thus hope that the review is not only a useful background resource for the NECF team and colleagues in the relevant fields, but will also help to inform subsequent participatory research with children and young people. Throughout the review, we have aimed through the numerous examples to draw attention to children and young people being participants in a variety of ways in research projects. We have stressed that children are a primary source about their own views and experiences and must be therefore listened to. However, as outlined in the review, engaging children in research raises particular challenges and concerns. Thus, there is a need to carefully consider such issues if research with children and young people is to be taken seriously. 48 Appendix 1 The review process The processes embodied in meta analyses and systematic reviews have been widely critiqued in recent years as government agencies, in particular, have been drawn to such approaches as guides to policy-making (e.g. Altman and Chalmers, 2001). The use of such reviews as bases for policy decisions reflects wider issues about the relationships between the academy and government, sadly beyond the scope of this review. Criticisms of the medically-oriented Cochrane reviews, and many of the similarly narrow although socially-oriented Campbell collaboration reviews, have centred on the nature of evidence excluded. Those approaches privilege a particular type of evidence, obscure the processes of combining/balancing contrasting sources and offer false promises about simplistic evidence-practice links (Hammersley 2001). In response to such critiques, other systematic reviews in social policy have tended to adopt a less narrow and less rigid approach. We took this latter orientation and thus included a broad spectrum of papers encompassing quasi experimental designs, ethnographic research, case study, surveys, practitioner accounts, critiques and opinion pieces. This inevitably generated a vast array of material from our first search (see Appendix 2). This work identified approximately 4000 publications and grey literature pertinent to exploring the views of children and young people spanning social policy, education, health and psychology. The review process is now described here in full. It comprised two phases, the first of these phases consisted of three distinct elements. 49 1.2.1. Phase 1 The first part of Phase 1 involved successive searching and refining of relevant material. This procedure paralleled established approaches taken in major systematic reviews (e.g. CRD 2001, ScHARR 1996) and EPPI (2003). (See http://www.campbellcollaboration.org and http://www.cochrane.org for a range of such reviews as well as links to methodological papers; for example, comparing hand with electronic searches as the basis for systematic reviews; and the use of grey literature.) Relevant research reports and articles were located through systematic searches of electronic databases and libraries. Systematic searches focused on 1990 onwards; the results of these searches were supplemented with a small number of seminal papers known through our prior knowledge of the field. Search terms included terms from the fields of health, education, social policy and psychology. These processes generated source material in English, which was predominantly from the United Kingdom (UK). The following electronic databases were searched recurrently (two to three occasions per search engine between early September and November, 2003): British Education Index Cochrane/York databases Campbell collaboration databases Cumulative Index Nursing and Health Literature (CINAHL) Embase ERIC (Educational Resources Information Center). Medline PsychLit 50 Given the potential breadth of the review, alongside time and wordage constraints, existing reviews were used in relation to four sets of work: (i) cognitive and socio-emotional development of children across early and middle childhood (ii) the psychology of questioning techniques with children (iii) data collection and analysis frameworks used with children (iv) epistemologies of research Key texts for the respective fields are shown in Table A. Table A: Secondary reviews used Cognitive and socio-emotional Wood, 1998 development of children across early and Berk, 2003 middle childhood The psychology of techniques with children questioning Ceci and Bruck, 1995 Walker, 1999 Zaragoza, 1995 Data collection and analysis frameworks Robson, 2002 used with children Sapsford and Jupp, 1996 Silverman, 1993 Epistemologies of research Oakley, 2000 Pring, 2000 Denzin and Lincoln, 1998 The second element of Phase 1 focused on searching key journals and publications to identify relevant sources. Related, important publications were located such as the Commission for Health Improvement (CHI). This publication appraises 59 reports provided to CHI by both statutory and voluntary organisations in which children’s and 51 young people’s views have been sought (Boylan 2004). In addition, relevant literature known to us through our previous work in the field but not otherwise located was included in the set of material reviewed (see appendix 2). These approaches enabled a stronger set of non-UK literature to be included. The third element of Phase 1 involved contacting key workers in the field of child-centred research for their own, and other, key publications (including material in press, see Appendix 2). Contacts included: a range of academics (from the Universities of Sheffield, Stirling and Warwick), personnel from voluntary bodies known for their work in this field (Action for Sick Children, Barnardo's, National Children’s Bureau, Save the Children, NCH) multi-professional contacts specialising in this topic and identified by reputation and NECF team members. Process of collating Phase 1 material and some observations about the process Endnote was used to collate, summarise, categorise, store and retrieve the output from the searches described above. The list of material generated by the searches was converted to a Word document (see Appendix 2). The multi-professional and cross-disciplinary nature of the field generated a vast number of relevant sources and we amassed over 300 references including published and ‘grey’ literature. This enabled us to identify key theorists and researchers in the field. The extensive pool of relevant material masks considerable similarities across sub-disciplines 52 and professional groups as well as, however, points of significant difference (particularly concerning ethical guidelines). Interestingly, our impression was of researchers and writers still working predominantly within their own spheres in terms of documents cited, leading to extensive but parallel material. Consequently material, unusually, referenced across these boundaries (e.g. Alderson and Morrow, 1995; Christensen and James, 2000; Lewis and Lindsay, 2000) is likely to have been important in spanning professional or academic groups and hence we tend to make strong use of these ‘boundary-crossing’ references. 1.2.2 Phase 2 A very small minority of apparently peripheral references collated through phase 1 were discarded at this stage (resulting in the list given in Appendix 2). The remaining references (Appendix 2) were reviewed to produce a more focused sub-set of source material, to be used in depth in the review (Appendix 3). Judgements were made on the basis of relevance of each article to the review’s focus (see 1.1) and was initially carried out from information such as the title, abstract and keywords and our knowledge of the field. More specifically (following ScHARR 1996) papers included in the in-depth sub-set reflected: prominence within the field relevance to the review’s focus strengths and clarity of methodology strengths and clarity of methods The end result of this process meant that brief details of a range of papers could be accessed quickly. 53 The main constraints of the review process were time (the whole process to be completed between September 2003 and January 2004, extended to March 2004) and wordage. Missing data was also a common problem in the review process (ScHARR 1996). Therefore, if the information required was not available from one source we attempted to find alternative sources. We found that many articles were summaries of much larger reports of studies and these often contained the information that we required. 54 Appendix 2 Bibliography Action for Sick Children (1998) Pictures of Healthcare. A child’s eye view. Action for Sick Children, London Alanen, L. and Mayall, B. (2001). Conceptualising child-adult relationships. London: RoutledgeFalmer. Alderson, P. (1993). Children's Consent to Surgery. Buckingham: Open University. Alderson, P., Goodey C. (1996).Research with disabled children: how useful are childcentred ethics. Children and Society 10, 106 -116. Alderson, P. (1995). Listening to Children: Children, Ethics and Social Research. Barkingside: Barnardos. Alderson, P. (1999). Disturbed young people: research for what, research for whom? In S. Hood and B. Mayall and S. Oliver (Eds.), Critical Issues in Social Research. (pp. 54-67). Chichester: Wiley. Alderson, P. (2000) ‘Children as Researchers – The effects of Participation Rights on Research Methodology’, in P.Christensen and A. James, Research with Children: Perspectives and Practices, London: Falmer Press, 241-257. Alderson, P. (2000). Save the Children Young Children’s Rights, London: Jessica Kingsley Alderson, P. and Morrow, G. (in press). Ethics, social research and consulting with children and young people. London: Barnardo's. Aldridge, M. and Wood, J. (1998). Interviewing Children: A guide for child care and forensic practitioners. Chichester: Wiley. Aldridge, J. (2003) Children caring for parents with mental illness : perspectives of young carers. Bristol: Policy Press Alldred, P. (1998). ‘Dilemmas in Representing the Voices of Children: Ethnography and Discourse Analysis’ in J.Ribbens and R.Edwards (eds.) Feminist Dilemmas in Qualitative Research: Public Knowledge, Private Lives, Sage Publications, 147170 Allmark, P. (?date) The ethics of research with children Nurse Researcher Vol 10, no 2 Altman, D.G. and Chalmers, I. (2001) Systematic reviews in health care: meta-analysis in context 2nd ed. London : BMJ Books Amato, P.R. and Orhiltree, G. (1987) Interviewing children about their families: a note on quality. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 49, p669-675. 55 Anderson, N. L. R., Koniak-Griffin, D., Keenan, C. K., Uman, G., Duggal, B. R. and Casey, C.(1999). Evaluating the outcomes of parent-child family life education... including commentary by Hayman LL. Scholarly Inquiry for Nursing Practice, 13(3), 211-238. Anzul, M., Evans, J. F., King, R., and Tellier-Robinson, D. (2001). Moving Beyond a Deficit Perspective with Qualitative Research Methods. Exceptional Children, 67(2), 235-249. Armstrong, C., Hill, M., and Secker, J. (1998). Listening to Children. London: Mental Health Foundation. Audit Commission. (2003). Services for Disabled Children. London: Audit Commission. Balen, R., Holroyd, C., Mountain, G. and Wood, B. (2000). Giving children a voice: methodological and practical implications of research involving children. Paediatric Nursing, 12(10), 24-29. Barker, J. and Smith, F. (2001). ‘Power, Positionality and Practicalities: Carrying Out Fieldwork with Children’ Ethics, Place and Environment, 4, 142-147. Barker, J. and Weller, S. (2003). ‘Never Work with Children? Methodological Issues in Children’s Geographies’ Qualitative Research. Barker, J. and Weller, S. (2003). ‘Is it Fun?’ Developing Children Centred Methods’ International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy. Bauchner, H. and Sharfstein, J. (2001). Failure to report ethical approval in child health research: review of published papers. British Medical Journal, 323(7308), 318319. Baumann, S.L. (1997). Qualitative research with children. Nursing Science Quarterly. 10, 2, 68-69. Begley, A. (2000). The educational self-perceptions of children with Down Syndrome. In A. A. Lewis and G. Lindsay (Eds.), Researching Children's Perspectives (pp. 98111). Buckingham: Open University. Bemak, F. (1996). Street Researchers: A New Paradigm Redefining Future Research with Street Children. Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 3(2), 147-156. Ben-Arieh, A. and Ofir, A. (2002). Time for (More) Time-Use Studies: Studying the Daily Activities of Children. Opinion, Dialogue, Review. Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 9(2), 225-248. Bendelow, G., Willams, S.J. and Aclu, A. (1996). It makes you bald: Children’s knowledge and beliefs about health and cancer prevention. Health Education. 3: 12 –19. 56 Bennett, F. and Roche, C. (2000) Developing indicators: the scope for participatory approaches. New Economy. 7(1): 24-28. Bennett, F. and Roberts, M. (2003) Participatory Research on Poverty. Joseph Rowntree Foundation. BERA (2004). Revised Ethical Guidelines for Educational Research. Edinburgh: BERA/SCRE. Beresford, B. (1997). Personal accounts: Involving disabled children in research. Social Policy Research Unit, University of York: Stationery Office. Berk, L. E. (2003) Child Development 6th ed. London: Allyn and Bacon. Berger, P. and Berger, B. (1991). Becoming a member of society . In Waksler F (Ed). 1991. Studying the Social Worlds of Children. London, Falmer Press. Berman, H. (2003). Getting critical with children: empowering approaches with a disempowered group. ANS - Advances in Nursing Science, 26(2), 102-113. Berrick, J. D., Frasch, K. and Fox, A. (2000). Note on research methodology. Assessing children's experiences of out-of-home care: methodological challenges and opportunities. Social Work Research, 24(2), 119-127. Blinn-Pike, L. M. and Others, A.. (1993). Assessing What High Risk Young Children Know about Drugs: Verbal versus Pictorial Methods. Journal of Drug Education, 23(3), 151-169. Bliss L.S. McCabe A. Miranda A.E. (1998). Narrative Assessment Profile: discourse analysis for school-age children. Journal of Communication Disorders. 31(4):34763, 365-8, Jul-Aug. BMA. (2003). Consent Tool Kit. London: BMA. Booth, T. and Booth, W. (1996). Sounds of silence: narrative research with inarticulate subjects. Disability and Society, 11(1), 55-57-. Booth, W. (1998). Doing research with lonely people. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 132-136. Bourg, W., Broderick, R., Flagor, R., Meeks Kelly D., Lang Ervin, D. and Butler, J. (1999). A Child Interviewer's Handbook. Thousand Oaks, Calif: Sage. Boyden, J. and J. Ennew (1997). Children in Focus – a manual for participatory research with children, Rädda Barnen, Stockholm Boylan, P. (2004) Children’s Voices project: Feedback from children and young people about their experiences and expectations of healthcare. Commission for Health Improvement. Department of Health. www.chi.nhs.uk/childrens-voices/Report.pdf 57 Brannen, J. and O’Brien, M. (?date) (eds) Children and Families: Research and Policy. Open University. Brewster, S. (in press). Putting words into their mouths? Interviewing people with learning disabilities and little/ no speech. British Journal of Learning Disabilities. Bricher, G. (1999). Children and qualitative research methods: a review of the literature related to interview and interpretive processes Nurse Researcher, Vol 6 No 4, Summer. British-Psychological-Society. (1991). Code of Conduct, Ethical Principles and Guidelines. Leicester: BPS. Broome, M.E. and Richards, D.J. (2003). The influence of relationships on children’s and adolescents participation in research. Nurse Researcher, May –June 52 (3). 191-7 Brotherson, M. J. (1999). Challenged To Expand Our Repertoire of Skills in Qualitative Research. Journal of Early Intervention, 22(4), 289-290. Brown, L. M. and Gilligan, C. (1991). Listening for Voice in Narratives of Relationship. New Directions for Child Development, 54, 43-62. Buchanan, A., Hunt, J., Bretherton, H, and Bream, V. (2001). Families in conflict: Perspectives of children and parents on the Family Court Welfare Service. Bristol: Policy Press. Bull, R. (1995). Innovative techniques for the questioning of child witnesses, especially those who are young and those with learning disability. In M. S. Zaragoza (Ed.), Memory and Testimony in the Child Witness (pp. 179-194). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Bull, R. (1998). Obtaining information from child witnesses. In M. e. al (Ed.), Psychology and Law: Truthfulness, Accuracy and Credibility. (pp. 188-209). London: McGraw Hill. Burman, E. (1994). Deconstructing Developmental Psychology. London, Routledge. Butler, I. and Williamson, H. (1994) Children Speak: Children, Trauma and Social Work. London, Longman. Byrne, B. M. and Watkins, D. (2003). The issue of measurement invariance revisited. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34(2), 155-175. Campbell, R. and Berry, H. (2001). Action Research Toolkit, Edinburgh Youth Social Inclusion Partnership Canino, G., Shrout, P. E., Alegria, M., Rubio-Stipec, M., Chavez, L. M., Ribera, J. C., Bravo, M., Bauermeister, J. J., Fabregas, L. M., Horwitz, S. H. and MartinezTaboas, A. (2002). Methodological challenges in assessing children's mental health services utilization. Mental Health Services Research, 4(2), 97-107. 58 Carlisle, J. (2000). Professional issues... commentary on McPherson G, Thorne S (2000). Children's voices: can we hear them? Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 15(1), 22-29. Carlisle, J. (2000). Professional issues... commentary on McPherson G, Thorne S (2000). Children's voices: can we hear them? Journal of Child and Family Nursing, 3(5), 366-367. Callery, P., Milnes, L., Verduyn, C. (2003) Qualitative study of young people’s beliefs about childhood asthma. British Journal of General Practice. 53, pp 85 Carney, T., Murphy, S., McClure, J., Bishop, E., Kerr, C., Parker, J., Scott, F., Shields, C. and Wilson, L. (2003). Children's views of hospitalization: an exploratory study of data collection. Journal of Child Health Care, 7(1), 27-40. Ceci, S. J. and Bruck, M. (1993). The suggestibility of the child witness: An historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 349-403. Ceci, S. J. and Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the Courtroom. Washington: APA. Chappell, A. L. (2000). Emergence of participatory methodology in learning difficulty research: understanding the context . British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28(1), p38-43. Christakis, D. A., Johnston, B. D. and Connell, F. A. (2001). Methodologic??? issues in pediatric outcomes research. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 59-62. Christensen, P. and James, A. (Eds.). (2000). Research with Children: Perspectives and Practices. London: Falmer. 120-135 Clark, A. and Moss, P. (2001). Listening to Young Children. The Mosaic approach. London: Joseph Rowntree Foundation/ National Children's Bureau. Clarke, J., Yates, E. and Shears, S. (2002). Can we do research without writing it down? Paper presented at the SCUTREA, 32nd Annual Conference, University of Stirling. Clegg, J. (2001). Healthcare ethics from a hermeneutic perspective. Paper presented at the seminar series: Methodological issues in interviewing children and young people with learning difficulties. Funded by ESRC 2001-3, University of Birmingham, School of Education. Clegg, J. (2003). The ideal proxy informant. Ethics and Intellectual Disability, 7(2), 1 and 4-5. Cloke, C. and Davies, M. (1995). Participation and Empowerment in Child Protection. Chichester: Wiley/ NSPCC. Clough, P. (1998). Differently articulate? Some indices of disturbed/ disturbing voices. In P. Clough and L. Barton (Eds.), Articulating with Difficulty (pp. 128-145). London: Paul Chapman. 59 Clough, P. and Barton, L. (Eds.). (1998). Articulating with Difficulty: Research Voices in Inclusive Education. London: Paul Chapman. Coad, J. (2002). Research and commentary: conducting focus groups with children. Paediatric Nursing, 14(10), 12. Coad, J & Morgan, L (2004) Chapter 8. ‘Getting Research into Children’s Nursing’ Clifford C. and Clark J. ‘Getting Research into Practice’ Harcourt Brace. Coad, J, Horsley, C and McCabe, A (2004 in press) Participatory Workshops for Indicators. Report. NECF. Birmingham. Coates, E. (2004) ‘I forgot the sky!’ Children’s stories contained within their drawings. In Lewis et al (eds.) The Reality of Research with Children and Young People. The Open University. Sage. London Cole-Hamilton., Harrop., Street, C. (2002). Making the case for play. Children’s Play Council. National Children’s Bureau. London. Conte, J. R. and Savage, S. B. (2003). Concluding observations. Journal of Interpersonal Violence., 18(4), 452-468. Cook, B. G. and Rumrill, P. d. J. (2001). Speaking of research. New directions in special education research. Work, 17(2), 143-150. Corsaro and Molinari (2000) Chapter 9. Christensen, P. and James, A. (Eds.). (2000). Research with Children: Perspectives and Practices. London: Falmer. Corey, G., Callanan, M. S. and Corey, P. (2002). Issues and Ethics in the Helping Professions (6TH 03 Edition): Brooks/Cole. Cox, A. (1999). Children's role in participatory action research: Australian Association for Research in Education (AARE). Conference (1999: Melbourne); New Zealand Association for Research in Education (NZARE). Conference (1999 : Melbourne); Australian Association for Research in Education (AARE) and the New Zealand Association for Research in Education (NZARE) Joint Conference (1999 : Melbourne). Coyne, I. T. (1998). Researching children: some methodological and ethical considerations. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 7(5), 409-416. CRD (2001) Undertaking Systematic Reviews of Research on Effectiveness CRD Report Number 4 (2nd Edition) York. Crowther, D., Cummings, C., Dyson, A. and Millward, A. (2003). Schools and area regeneration. London: Policy Press/Joseph Rowntree. Crozier, J. and Tracey (2000). Falling out of school; A young woman's reflections on her chequered experience of schooling. In A. Lewis and G. Lindsay (Eds.), 60 Researching Children's Perspectives (pp. 173-186). Buckingham: Open University. Cunningham, H. (1995). Children and Childhood in Western Society since 1500. London: Longman. Curtin, C. (2001). Eliciting children's voices in qualitative research. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 55(3), 295-302. CYPU. (2001). Learning to Listen: Core principles on the involvement and engagement of children and young people. CYPU. (2002). UK implementation of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child (CRC): An update to the UK's second report to the UN Committee on the Rights of the Child in 1999. da Cunha Rego, L. (2003). Beyond conventions: a psycho-educational perspective on children's right to participation. PhD thesis, University of Warwick, Coventry. Daiute, C. (1998). Points of View in Children's Writing. . Language Arts, 75(2), 138-149. D'Arcangelo, M. (2003). On the Mind of a Child: A Conversation with Sally Shaywitz. Educational Leadership; v60 n7 p6-10 Apr Dashiff, C. (2001). Data Collection with Adolescents. Journal of Advanced Nursing. Vol 33 (3); p343-349. Feb. Davis, J. M. (1998). Understanding the Meanings of Children: A Reflexive Process. Children and Society, 12(5), 325-335. Davis, J. and Watson, N. (2001). Where are the children's experiences? Analysing social and cultural exclusion in 'special' and mainstream schools. Disability and Society, 16(5), 671-687. Davis, J., N., W. and S., C.-B. (2000). Learning the Lives of Disabled Children: Developing a reflexive Approach. In P. Christensen and A. James (Eds.), Research with Children. Perspectives and Practices (pp. 201-224). London: Farmer. Deatrick, J.A. and Faux, S.A. (1991). Conducting qualitative studies with children and adolescents.In Morse J (Ed). Qualitative Nursing Research: A Contemporary Dialogue. Newbury Park CA, Sage. de Koning, K. and Martin, M. (eds) (1996) Participatory research in health : issues and experiences London : Zed. Dent, H. R. (1986). An experimental study of the effectiveness of different techniques of questioning mentally handicapped child witnesses. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 25, 13-17. 61 Denzin, N.K. and Lincoln, S.K. (1998) The Handbook of Qualitative Research Vols 1, 2 and 3. Sage. London. Desjean-Perrotta, B. (1998). Through Children's Eyes: Using the Shadow Study Technique for Program Evaluation. Early Childhood Education Journal, 25(4), 259-263. Detheridge, T. (2000). Research involving children with severe learning difficulties. In A. Lewis and G. Lindsay (Eds.), Researching Children's Perspectives (pp. 112-121). Buckingham: Open University Press. DeVellis, R.F. (1991) Scale development: Theory and application. Sage. Newbury Park, CA. Devine, D. (2002). Children's Citizenship and the Structuring of Adult-Child Relations in the Primary School. Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 9(3), 303320. DoH. (2003). Department of Health. National Service Frameworks. London. DoH DFES. (2001a). Code of Practice on the Identification and Assessment of Pupils with Special Educational Needs. London: DFES. DFES. (2001b). Toolkit for Special Educational Needs. London: DFES. DFES. (2003). Every child matters. Green paper. London: DFES. DFES (2004). Removing Barriers to Achievement. London: DFES. DFES. (2004). Every child matters; The next steps. March. London: DFES. Di Gallo, A. (2001). Drawing as a means of communication at the initial interview with children with cancer Journal of Child Psychotherapy, Vol.27,no.2: Aug p197-210 Docherty S, Sandelowski M. (1999). Focus on Qualitative Methods. Interviewing children. Research in Nursing and Health. 22, (2): 177-185. Dockett, S. and Perry, B. (1999). Starting School: What Do the Children Say? Early Child Development and Care, 159, 107-119. Dockrell, J., Lewis, A. and Lindsay, G. (2000). Researching Children's Perspectives -.a psychological perspective. In A. Lewis and G. Lindsay (Eds.), Researching Children's Perspectives (pp. 46-58). Buckingham: Open University. Dorn, L. D., Susman, E. J. and Fletcher, J. C. (1995). Informed consent in children and adolescents: age, maturation and psychological state. Journal of Adolescent Health, 16(3), 185-190. Druin, A. (2002). The role of children in the design of new technology. Behaviour and Information Technology, 21(1), 1-25. 62 Dunlop, A.W. (1998). Using Research Methodology To Explore Quality of Social Interaction. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 6(1), 87104. Earls, F. and Carlson, M. (1999). Children at the Margins of Society: Research and Practice. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development(85), 71-82. Educable. (2000). No choice: no chance. The educational experiences of young people with disabilities Belfast: Save the Children. EPPI. (2003). Supporting pupils with emotional and behavioural difficulties (EBD) in mainstream primary schools: a systematic review of recent research on strategy effectiveness (1999-2000). London: EPPI Centre, University of London Institute of Education. Evans, M. E., Mejia-Maya, L. J., Zayas, L. H., Boothroyd, R. A. and Rodriguez, O. (2001). Conducting research in culturally diverse inner-city neighborhoods: some lessons learned. Journal of Transcultural Nursing, 12(1), 6-14. Evans, P. and Fuller, M. (1998) Perceptions of children International Journal of Early Years Education 6 1 60-74 Everson-Bates, S. (1988). Research involving children: ethical concerns and dilemmas. Journal of Pediatric Health Care, 2(5), 234-239. Faulkner, D (1998). Cited from Christensen, P. and James, A. (Eds.). (2000). Research with Children: Perspectives and Practices. London: Falmer. Faux, S.A. et al. (1988).Intensive interviewing with children and adolescents. Western Journal of Nursing Research. 10,2, 180-194. Fine, G.A, Sandstrom, K.L. (1988). Knowing Children: Participant Observation with Minors. Newbury Park CA, Sage. Fivush, R. (1991) The social construction of personal narratives, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 59-82. Ford, M. and Fasoli, L. (2001). Indigenous early childhood educators' narratives : some methodological considerations. Australian Journal of Early Childhood, 26(3), 1217. France, A. (2000). Youth researching youth: The triumph and success peer research project. National Youth Agency/Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Fraser, S., Lewis,V., Ding, S. et al. (2003). Doing research with children and young people. Sage. London. Freeman, C., Henderson, P., Kettle, J. (1999). Planning with children for better communities: the challenge to professionals, Bristol: Policy Press. 63 Friedland D. and Penn C. (2003). Conversation analysis as a technique for exploring the dynamics of a mediated interview. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 38(1):95-111, 2003 Jan-Mar. Fritzley, V. H. and Lee, K. (2003). Do young children always say yes to yes-no questions? A meta-developmental study of the affirmation bias. Child Development . 74, 1297-1313. Garth, (2003). ‘I value what you have to say with a disability’ – Seeking perspectives of children Disability and Society 18 (5) P561-576. Garth, B. and Aroni, R. (2003). 'I value what you have to say'. Seeking the perspective of children with a disability, not just their parents. Disability and Society, 18(5), 561576. Germain, R. (in press). An exploratory study using cameras and Talking Mats to access the views of young people with learning disabilities on their out of school activities. British Journal of Learning Disabilities. Gersch, I. (1996) Listening to children in educational contexts, in R.Davie, G.Upton, and V. Varma (eds) The Voice of the child; A handbook for professionals London: Falmer Gettings, D and Gladstone, C (2001) Using ICT based questionnaires in attempting to gain the views of students with severe learning difficulties, ERSC series; University of Birmingham Gillen, J. and Young, S. (2000). Participatory Research with Young Children: Engagements in Dialogue by Instrumental Music-Making and Telephone Talk. Paper presented at British Educational Association Conference. University of Sussex. September 2-5. Ginsburg, H.P. (1997). Entering the child’s mind. University Press. Cambridge. Gordon, A. (2001). Exclusions in England: Children's Voices and Adult Solutions? Educational Studies, 27(1), 69-85. Grady, S. (1999). Asking the Audience: Talking to Children about Representation in Children's Theatre. Youth Theatre Journal, 13, 82-92. Grainger, T., Goouch, K. and Lambirth, A. (2002). The Voice of the Child: "We're Writers" Project. Reading: Literacy and Language, 36(3), 135-139. Greig, A. and Taylor, J. (1999). Doing Research with Children. London: Sage. Gunther, H. (1991) Interviewing street children in a Brazilian city. Journal of Social Psychology. 132, p359-367. 64 Hammersley, M. (2001) Some questions about evidence-based practice in education. Paper in Symposium: Evidence-based practice. BERA conference, September 2001 Hamill, P. and Boyd, B. (2002). Equality, Fairness and Rights – The Young Person’s Voice. British Journal of Special Education, 29(3), 111-117. Harr, J. (2001). Issues in researching the perspectives of deaf children. Journal of Research in SEN, 1.3. Harrell, J. S., Bradley, C., Dennis, J., Frauman, A. C. and Criswell, E. S. (2000). Schoolbased research: problems of access and consent. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 15(1), 14-21. Hart, R. (1992). ‘Children’s Participation: from tokenism to citizenship’, Innocenti Essay No.4, UNICEF, New York. Hart, R. A. (1997) Children's participation: the theory and practice of involving young citizens London: UNICEF and Earthscan Hart, S. N. (2002). Making Sure The Child's Voice Is Heard. International Review of Education, 48 (3-4), 251-258. Hazel, N. (1995). Seen and heard: An examination of methods for collecting data from young. Unpublished MSc. University of Stirling. Hazel, N. (2003). Elicitation techniques with young people. Social Research, 12. University of Surrey and Unpublished thesis. University of Stirling. DoH (1992). Memorandum of Good Practice on Video-recorded Interviews with Child Witnesses for Criminal Proceedings. London: HMSO. Hek, R. (2002). ‘Integration not segregation’ – experiences of young refugees in British Schools. FMR, p14-15. Hek, R and Sales, R (2001) Supporting refugee and asylum seeking children: An examination of experiences and support structures that facilitate settlement in school. Unpublished report. Middlesex University. Hengst, H. (1987) The liquidation of childhood, in J. Qvortup (ed) The sociology of childhood: International Journal of Sociology 17 2 58-80 Henry, L. A. and Gudjonsson, G. H. (1999). Eyewitness memory and suggestibility in children with mental retardation. AJMR, 104(6), 491-508. Hewson, C. (2003) Conducting research on the internet. The Psychologist, 16(6), 290293. 65 Hibbert, P. (2002) Voices and choices: young people participating in inspections. Barnados. www.barnardos.org.uk/resources Hickmann, M. (2003). Children's Discourse: Person, Space and Time across Languages. Cambridge Studies in Linguistics. Paper. Hill, M., Laybourn, A. and Borland, M. (1996). Engaging with primary-aged children about their emotions and well-being: methodological considerations. Children and Society, 10, 129-144. Holmes, R. (1998). Fieldwork with Children, Sage Publications. Homan, R. (2001). The principle of assumed consent: the ethics of gatekeeping. Journal of Philosophy of Education, 35(3), 329-343. Hood, S. et al. (1996). Children as research subjects: a risky enterprise. Children and Society. Vol 10. June. 117-128. Hood, S., Mayall, B. and Oliver, S. (Eds.). (1999). Critical Issues in Social Research. Buckingham: Open University Press. Horner, S. D. (2000). Focus on research methods. Using focus group methods with middle school children. Research in Nursing and Health, 23(6), 510-517. Hoskins, M. L., Pence, A. and Chambers, E. (1999). Quality and Day Care: What Do Children Have To Say? Early Child Development and Care, 157, 51-66. Hurley, J.C. and Underwood, M.K. (2002). Children’s understanding of their research rights before and after debriefing: Informed assent, confidentiality and stopping participation. Child Development, 73, 132-143. Huston, A. C. (2002). From Research to Policy: Choosing Questions and Interpreting the Answers. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development(98), 29-41. Innes, M., Moss, T. and Smigiel, H. What do the children say? The importance of student voice. Research in Drama Education, 6(2), 207-221. Inspire Training and Development. (2002). ‘I Am We Are’ Activities Pack: creative activities to help children explore their identities, Smethwick: Inspire. Investing in Children Projects (1998 – 2001) ‘Sour Grapes’ – How young people’s views are taken into account (1998) Young People’s Mental Health (2000), Fares Fair’ Investigating local transport (2000) and Young People and the Police (2001), Courtesy of Felicity Shelton (see acknowledgements); Co. Durham, UK Ireland L., Holloway I. (1996). Qualitative health research with children. Children and Society. 10, 155-164. James, A. (1996) Learning to be friends: methodological lessons from participant observation among English school children Childhood 3 313-330 66 James, A. et al. (1998). Theorizing Childhood. Cambridge: Polity Press. James, A. and Prout A. (Eds). (1997). Constructing and Reconstructing Childhood: Contemporary issues in the Sociological Study of Childhood. Falmer Press, London. Johnson, A. J. (2002). So..? Pragmatic implications of So-Prefaced questions in formal police interviews. In J. Cotterill (Ed.), Language in the Legal Process (pp. 91110). Houndmills, Basingstoke: Palgrave MacMillan. Johnson, B.H. (1990). Children’s Drawings as a projective technique. Pediatric Nursing. Vol 16 (no 1) 11- 17. Johnson, K (2003) Use of photographs with a group of Australian school-aged children. Unpublished. By kind permission. Johnson, V., E. Ivan-Smith, G.Gordon, P.Pridmore, P.Scott (eds.) (1998). Stepping Forward. Children and young people’s participation in the development process. Intermediate Technology Publications. Filey, N.Yorkshire. Johnston, J. (2001). Evaluating National Initiatives: The Case of "On Track." Children and Society, 15(1), 33-36. Jordan, K. M., Kolak, A. M. and Jason, L. A. (1997). Research with children and adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: methodologies, designs, and special considerations. Journal of the Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, 3(2), 3-13. Joseph, R. M. (1998). Intention and Knowledge in Preschoolers' Conception of Pretend. Child Development, 69(4), 966-980. Joseph-Rowntree-Foundation. (2001). Findings: consulting with disabled children and young people. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Karppinen, S. (2000). Repertory Grid Technique in Early Childhood as a Tool for Reflective Conversations in Arts Education. Kassam-Adams N. Newman E. (2002).The reactions to research participation questionnaires for children and for parents. General Hospital Psychiatry. Vol 24(5) (pp 336-342). Kefyalew, F. (1996). The Reality of Child Participation in Research: Experience from a Capacity-Building Programme. Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 3(2), 203-213. Kelly, N. and Norwich, B. (2002). Perspectives of pupils with MLD on their mainstream and special school provision and themselves. Exeter: University of Exeter/Nuffield Foundation. 67 Kennedy, C., Charlesworth, A. and Chen, J. (2003). Interactive data collection: benefits of integrating new media into pediatric research. CIN: Computers, Informatics, Nursing, 21(3), 120-127. Kerr, A., Makuluni, A. H. and Nieves, M. (2000). The Research Process: Parents, Kids, and Teachers as Ethnographers. Primary Voices K-6, 8(3), 14-23. Khan, S. (1997) A Street Children’s Research. Save the Children. October. Kinard, E. M. (1998). Methodological Issues in Assessing Resilience in Maltreated Children. Child Abuse and Neglect. The International Journal, 22(7), 669-680. Kirby, P. (1999). Involving Young Researchers: How to enable young people to design and conduct research, York: JRF / Youth Work Press. Kirby, P., Lanyon, C., Kronin, K. and Sinclair, R. (2003). Building a culture of participation. London: CYPU. Kirkbride, L. (1999). I'll go first: The planning and review toolkit for use with children with disabilities. London: Children's Society. Klein, R (2003) We want our say: children as active participants in their education. Stoke on Trent: Trentham. Koocher, G.P, Keith-Spiegel, P. (1994). Scientific issues in psychosocial and educational research with children. In Godwin, Glanz LH (Eds). Children as Research Subjects. New York NY, Oxford. University Press. Kortesluoma, R.-L., Hentinen, M. and Nikkonen, M. (2003). Conducting a qualitative child interview: methodological considerations. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 42(5), 434. Kurtz, Z. and Thornes, R. (2000). The health needs of school aged children – the views of children, parents and teachers linked to local and national information. Department of Education and Employment. London. Kyngas, H., Barlow J. (1995). Diabetes: an adolescent’s perspective. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 22, 941-947. Lansdown, G. (2001) Promoting chidlren’s participation in democratic decision-making, Florence: UNICEF Lansdown, G. (1994). Children’s Rights. In Mayall, B (Editor); Children’s Childhoods Observed and Experienced, London: Falmer Press. Lansdown, G. and Lancaster, Y. P. (2001) Promoting chidlren’s welfare by respecting tehri rights in G. Pugh (ed) Contemporary Issues in the Early Years London: Paul Chapman/ Sage 68 Lansdown, G., Waterston, T. and Baum, D. (1996). Implementing the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child. British Medical Journal, 313, 21-28. Lewis, A. (2001). Reflections on Interviewing Children and Young People as a Method of Inquiry in Exploring their Perspectives on Integration/Inclusion. Journal for Research in Special Educational Needs, 1(3), www.nasen.uk.com/ejournal. Lewis, A. (2001) Research involving young children, in T. David (ed) Promoting Evidence-based Practice in Early Childhood Education: Research and its implications. Advances in Applied Early Childhood Education. Volume 1. pp 251-269 London; JAI/ Elsevier Science. Lewis, A. (2002). Accessing, through research interviews, the views of children with difficulties in learning. Support for Learning, 17(3), 110-116. Lewis A. (2004) ‘And When Did You Last See Your Father?’ Exploring the views of children with learning difficulties/ disabilities British Journal of Special Education 31(1), 4-10. Lewis, A. (in press) Researching a marginalized population: methodological issues. In P. Garner et al (eds) International handbook of Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties London: Sage Lewis, A. (in press) Exploring the views of children with learning disabilities, in C. Toren and G. Evans (eds) Methods for making sense of children’s experiences Uxbridge: Brunel University, Centre for Child-Focused Anthropological Research Lewis, A. and Lindsay, G. E. (Eds.). (2000) Researching Children's Perspectives. Buckingham: Open University Press. Lewis, A. and Porter, J. (in press). Interviewing children and young people with learning disabilities: Guidelines for researchers and multi professional practice. British Journal of Learning Disabilities Lewis, V., Kellet. M. et al (2003). The Reality of research with children and young people. Sage/ Open University: London. Lightfoot, J. and Sloper, P. (2002). Having a say in health: involving young people with a chronic illness or physical disability in local health services development. Children and Society. Lindsay, G. (2000). Researching children's perspectives: ethical issues. In A. Lewis and G. Lindsay (Eds.), Researching Children's Perspectives (pp. 1-20). Buckingham: Open University. Luxmoore, N. (2000) Listening to young people in school, youth work and counselling London : Jessica Kingsley Lyle, S. (2002). Talking to learn: the voices of children, aged 9-11, engaged in role-play. Language and Education. Vol 16, No 4. 303-317. 69 Masson, J. (2000). Researching children's perspectives: legal issues. In A. Lewis and G. Lindsay (Eds.), Researching Children's Perspectives (pp. 34-45). Buckingham: Open University Press. Mahon, A., Glendinning, C., Clarke, K. and Craig, M. (1996). Researching children: methods and ethics. Children and Society, 10, 145-154. Marchant, R. and Cross, M. (2002). How it is. London: NSPCC. Mason, J., and Urquhart, R. Developing a model for participation by children in research on decision making. Children Australia, 26(4), 16-21. Masson, J. (2001). Ethical issues from a legal perspective. Paper presented at the seminar series: Methodological issues in interviewing children and young people with learning difficulties. Funded by ESRC 2001-3, School of Education, University of Birmingham. Mauthner, M. (1997). Methodological aspects of collecting data from children; lessons from three projects. Children and Society. 11 (1) 16-28. Mavers, D (2001) Concept Maps: Representing Thinking. ERSC series. University of Birmingham. Mayall, B. et al. (1996).Children’s Health in Primary Schools. London, Falcon Press. Mayall, B. (1998). Towards a sociology of child health. Sociology of Health and Illness. 20, 3, 269-288. Mayall, B. (2000). ‘Conversations with Children – Working with Generational Issues’ in P. Moss, J. (2000) Researching children’s perspectives: a book review textualised by Liam’s sorting drawing. Australian Educational Researcher. V. 27 (3) 185191. McCarthy, M. (1998). Interviewing people with learning disabilities about sensitive topics: a discussion of ethical issues. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 140-145. McCrum S, Bemal P. (1994). Interviewing Children: A Training Pack for Journalists. Buckfastleigh, Save the Children Fund. McGurk, H. and Glachan, M. (1988). Children’s conversations with adults. Children and Society. Vol 2, 20-34. McIntosh, N., Normand, C., Alderson, P., Brykczynska, G., Cooke, R., Dunstan, G., Lloyd, D. J., Montgomery, J., Nicholson, R., Pembrey, M., Bates, P., Goldman, A., Harvey, D., Larcher, V., McCrae, D., McKinnon, A., Patton, M., Saunders, J., Shelley, P. and Hull, D. (2000). Guidelines for the ethical conduct of medical research involving children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 82(2), 177-182. 70 McKenzie, B. C. (1997). Developing First Nations Child Welfare Standards: Using Evaluation Research within a Participatory Framework. Journal of Program Evaluation/La Revue canadienne d'evaluation de programme, 12(1), 133-148. McNeish, D. (1999).Promoting participation for children and young peopl: Some key questions for health and scoial welfare organisaitons. Journal of Social Work Practice. Vol 13 McNeish, D., Newman, T., Roberts, H. (2002). (eds) What Works for Children? Buckingham: OUP McTaggart, R (1996) Issues for Participatory Action Researchers. In Ortrun ZuberSkerrit (ed) New Directions in Action Research. London, Falmer Press. Medd, W. (2001). Making (Dis)Connections: Complexity and the Policy Process? Social Issues, 1(2), http://www.whb.co.uk/socialissues/undex.htm. Merton, B. (2002). Build It In. Evaluating innovation in work with young people. NYA. Miller, J (1996) Never too young – How young children can take responsibility and make decisions. A handbook for early years workers. The National Early Years Network/Save The Children. London. Miller, J. (1998) ‘No fun in Bilsthorpe’ Social Action Today. January. No 7, pp9-11. Miller, S. (1999) Hearing from children who have diabetes. Journal of Child Health. 2 (1); pp5-12 Miller, S. (2000). Analysis of phenomenological data generated with children as research participants. Nurse Researcher, Vol 10, No 4 74, 1297-1313. Mills, J. with Mills, R. W. (2000) Childhood studies : a reader in perspectives of childhood London : Routledge Milne, R. and Bull, R. (2001). Interviewing witnesses with learning disabilities for legal purposes: A review. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 93-97. Minkler, M. (2003) Community based participatory research for health Chichester: John Wiley Montandon, C. and Osiek, F. (1998). Children's Perspectives on Their Education. Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 5(3), 247-263. Moore, M., Beazeley, S. and Maelzer, J. (1998). Researching Disability Issues. Buckingham: Open University Press. Morgan, M., Gibbs, S., Maxwell, K. and Britten, N. (2002). Hearing children's voices: methodological issues in conducting focus groups with children aged 7-11 years. Qualitative Research, 2(1), 5-20. 71 Morris, J. (1998). Don't leave us out: Involving disabled children and young people with communication impairments. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Morris, J. (2003). Including all children: finding out about the experiences of children with communication and/or cognitive impairments. Children and Society. Morrow, V. (1998) Understanding families: children's perspectives London: National Children's Bureau/ Joseph Rowntree Morrow, V. (1999). It's cool ... 'cos you can't give us detentions and things, can you? In P. Milner and B. Carolin (Eds.), Time to listen to children (pp. 203-215). London: Routledge. Morrow, V. (2001a). Using qualitative methods to elicit young people's perspectives on their environments: Some ideas for community health initiatives. Health Education Research, 16(3), 255-268. Morrow, V. (2001b). Young people's explanations and experiences of social exclusion: retrieving Bourdieu's concept of social capital. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy 21(4/5/6) 37-63 Morrow, V. and Richards, M. (1996). The ethics of social research with children: an overview. Children and Society, 10, 90-105. Moss, J. (2000). Review essay: Researching Children's Perspectives : a book review textualised by Liam's sorting drawing. Australian Educational Researcher, 27(3), 185-191. Moston, S. (1987). The suggestibility of children in interview studies. First Language, 7, 67-78. Mulderij K. (1996). Research into the lifeworld of physically disabled children. Child: Care, Health and Development. 22, 5, 311-22. Mullender, A. et al. (2002) Children's perspectives on domestic violence London: Sage. Murphy, J. (2002). Let your mats do the talking. Speech and Language Therapy in Practice, Spring, 18-20. Narring, F. (2001). Qualitative research methodology: A critical review of its application to adolescents. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health. Vol 13(1) (pp 25-29), 2001 National Children’s Bureau. (1993). Guidelines for research. London: NCB. NCH. (2001). Participating in good practice: A resource pack to support user participation in NCH projects. London: NCH. 72 NEF. (1998). Participation Works! 21 Techniques of community participation for children and young people, Save the Children, London. Nesbitt, E. (2000) Researching 8 to 13 year olds’ perspectives on their experience of religion In A. Lewis and G. Lindsay (Eds.), Researching Children's Perspectives (pp. 135-149) Buckingham: Open University. Nichols, S. (2002). Parents' construction of their children as gendered, literate subjects: a critical discourse analysis Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, Vol.2,no.2: Aug 2002 123-144. NHS (2004) National Health Service. Standard Operating Procedures for Research Ethics Committees in the United Kingdom. Central Office for Research Ethics (COREC). Version 1.0. February. Novak, J. D. and Gowin, D. B. (1984) Learning How to Learn. New York: Cambridge University Press. NSPCC (2000) Listening to children: a guide for parents and carers London; Wordworks Oakley, M. (2000). Children and young people and care proceedings. In A. Lewis and G. Lindsay (Eds.), Researching Children's Perspectives (pp. 73-85). Buckingham: Open University. O'Donnell, L. N. and Strasburger, V. (1998). Parental permission in adolescent health research (multiple letters). Journal of Adolescent Health, 23(5), 252-253. O'Donnell, L. N., Duran, R. H., San Doval, A., Breslin, M. J., Juhn, G. M. and Stueve, A. (1997). Obtaining written parent permission for school-based health surveys of urban young adolescents... including commentary by Santelli J. Journal of Adolescent Health, 21(6), 376-387. O’Quigley, A. (2000) Listening to children’s views: The findings and recommendations of recent research York: Joseph Rowntree Oliver, M. (1997) Emancipatory research: Realsitic goal or impossible dream? In C. Barnes and G. Mercer Doing Disability Research Disability Press Ovadia, R., Hemphill, L., Winner, K., Bellinger, D. (2000) Just Pretend: Participation in Symbolic Talk by Children with Histories of Early Corrective Heart Surgery. Applied Psycholinguistics; v21 n3 p321-40 Sep Parkinson, D. D. (2001). Securing Trustworthy Data from an Interview Situation with Young Children: Six Integrated Interview Strategies. Child Study Journal, 31(3), 137-156. Partington, L. (undated). We want to be heard. London: Children's Society. 73 Ponder, K. W. (2001). Early Childhood Research Quarterly. Practitioner Perspective: Assessing Child-Care Quality with a Telephone Interview, 16(2), 241-243. Ponder, Karen W. (2001). Practitioner Perspective: Assessing Child-Care Quality with a Telephone Interview. Early Childhood Research Quarterly; v16 n2 p241-43 2001. Porter, J. and Lewis, A. (2001). Methodological issues in interviewing children and young people with learning difficulties. Paper presented at the ESRC-funded seminar series: Methodological issues in interviewing children and young people with learning difficulties. School of Education: University of Birmingham. Porter, J., Ouvry, C., Morgan, M. and Downs, C. (2001). Interpreting the Communication of people with profound and Multiple Learning Difficulties. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29(1), 12-16. Pridmore, P. and Bendelow, G. (1995). Images of health: Exploring beliefs of children using ‘draw and write’ technique. Health Education Journal. 54. 473-488. Pring, R. (2000). Philosophy of Educational Research. London: Continuum. Prior, M. e. (2002). Investing in our children: developing a research agenda.: Academy of the Social Sciences in Australia. Prout, A. (2000). Children's Participation: control and self-realisation in British Late Modernity. Children and Society, 14, 304-315. Prout, A. (2001). Representing Children: Reflections on the Children 5-16 Programme. Children and Society, 15(3), 193-201. Prout, A. (2002). Researching Children as Social Actors: An Introduction to the Children 5-16 Programme. Children and Society, 16(2), 67-76. Punch, S. (2002). Interviewing strategies with young people: the secret box, a stimulus material and task-based activities. Children and Society. Vol 16, Issue 1. January. 45-56. Punch, S. G. (2002). Research with Children: The Same or Different from Research with Adults? Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 9(3), 321-341. Radford, J. and Tarplee, C. (2000). The management of conversational topic by a ten year old child with pragmatic difficulties. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. 14(5):387-403, Jul-Aug Raschick, M. and Critchley, R. (1998). Guidelines for conducting site-based evaluations of intensive family preservation programs. Child Welfare, 77(6), 643-660. Rechner, M. (1990). Adolescents with cancer: getting on with life. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing. 7, 4, 139-144. Rich, J. (1968). Interviewing children and adolescents. Macmillan press. London. 74 Ridge, T. (2002) Childhood poverty and social exclusion : from a child's perspective Bristol : Policy Press, 2002 Ring, K. (2000). Young children talking about their drawings: methodological dilemmas. Paper presented at the British Educational Research Association Annual Conference, Cardiff University. Ring, K. (2003). Young children drawing: the significance of the context. Paper presented at the Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the British Educational Research Association, 13-15 September 2001, University of Leeds, England. Robertson, C. (2001). Autonomy and identity: the need for new dialogues in education and welfare. Support for Learning, 16(3), 122-127. Robson, C. (2002). Real World Research (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell. Rollins, P.R. McCabe, A. Bliss, L. (2000). Culturally sensitive assessment of narrative skills in children. Seminars in Speech and Language. 21(3):223-34, 275-6. Ronen, G. M., Rosenbaum, P., Law, M. and Streiner, D. L. (2001). Health-related quality of life in childhood disorders: A modified focus group technique to involve children. Quality of Life Research, 10(1), 71-79. Rosenblatt, A. and Woodbridge, M. W. (2003). Deconstructing research on systems of care for youth with EBD: frameworks for policy research. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 11(1), 27-37. Rosenquest, B. B. (2002). The 3 Rs of Research in Child Care: Roles, Rights, and Responsibilities. Child Care Information Exchange, 144, 54-59. Rotherham Participation Project, (2001). Young People’s Charter of Participation, The Children’s Society. Royal College of Nursing (2004) Research Ethics The RCN Research Society Ethics Guidance Group. RCN, London Saito, M. (2003) Amartya Sen’s Capability Approach to Education: A Critical Exploration Journal of Philosophy of Education 37 1 17-33 Sapsford, R. and Jupp, V. (1996) Data Collection and Analysis London: Sage Sartain, S, Clarke, C.L. (2000). Hearing the voices of children with chronic illness. Journal of Advanced Nursing Vol 32 (4); p913-921.Oct. Save the Children (1997) A Matter of Opinion. Save the Children, London. Save-the-Children. (2000). Children and Participation: Research, monitoring and evaluation with children and young people. London: Save the Children. 75 Save the Children (2003) All Together Now. London: Save the Children. Saywitz, K. J. and Snyder, L. (1993) Improving children's testimony with preparation. In G. S. Goodman and B. Bottom (Eds.), Child victims, child witnesses: understanding and improving testimony. New York: Guilford Press. Saywitz, K. J. (1995). Improving Children's Testimony: The question, the answer and the environment. In M. S. Zaragoza (Ed.), Memory and Testimony in the Child Witness (pp. 113-140). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Scharer, K. (1996). Researching sensitive issues in child psychiatric nursing: ethical concerns. Journal of Child and Adolescent; Psychiatric Nursing. 9, 3, 17-27. Schiller, W. (1999). Adult/Child Interaction: How Patterns and Perceptions Can Influence Planning. Early Child Development and Care, 159, 75-92. Schofield, G. and Thoburn, J. (1996). Child Protection: The Voice of the Child in Decision Making London: IPPR Scott, D. (2000). Realism and Educational Research. London, Routledge/Falmer. Scotland. (2002). VITAL VOICES - Helping Vulnerable Witnesses Give Evidence - Policy Statement - Scotland. Sengstock, M. C. and Hwalek, M. (1999). Issues to Be Considered in Evaluating Programs for Children and Youth. Research and Practice: Completing the Circle. New Designs for Youth Development, 15(2), 8-12. Shaw, I. (1996). Unbroken voices: Children, young people and qualitative methods. P1936. In Butler, L. and Shaw, I (eds) A case of neglect? Children’s experiences and the sociology of childhood. Aldershot, Avebury. Sheffield Children’s Fund (2003). Humberstone, C and Corbett, A. Let’s Get Involved! A practical guide for involving and consulting children and young people. Sheffield. August. Shepard, M. P., Orsi, A. J., Mahon, M. M. and Carroll, R. M. (2002). Mixed-methods research with vulnerable families. Journal of Family Nursing, 8(4), 334-352. Shin, H. S. (2001). A review of school-based drug prevention program evaluations in the 1990s. Journal of Health Education, 32(3), 139-149. Silverman, D. (1993) Interpreting qualitative data: Methods for analyzing talk, text and interaction. Sage, London. Sinclair, R., Cronin, K., Lanyon, C., Stone, V. and Hulusi, A. (2002). Aim High Stay Real - Outcomes for Children and Young People: The Views of Children, Parents and Practitioners. London: CYPU. 76 Skinner, J. (1998). Research as a Counselling Activity? A Discussion of Some Uses of Counselling within the Context of Research on Sensitive Issue. British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 26(4), 533-540. Solberg, A. (1996). The challenge in child research: from ‘Being’ to ‘Doing’. In Brannen, J and O’Brien, M (Eds). Children in Families. Falmer, London Spencer, J. R. and Flin, R. (1990). The evidence of children. London: Blackstone Press. Stafford, A., Laybourn, A., Hill, M. and Walker, M. (2003). Having a say: children and young people talk about consultation. Children and Society. Stanley, B., Sieber, J. (1992). The ethics of participatory research on children and adolescents; ethical issues. Sage, London. Steward, M et al. (1993). Implications of developmental research for interviewing children. Child Abuse and Neglect 17, 25-37. Stiegler, L.N. and Hoffman, P.R. (2001). Discourse-based intervention for word finding in children. Journal of Communication Disorders. 34(4):277-303, 367-70, 2001 Jul-Aug. Suarez-Balcazar, Y and Harper, G.W. (eds) (2003) Empowerment and participatory evaluation of community interventions : Multiple Benefits New York: Haworth Press Swain, J., Keyman, B. and Gilman, M. (1998). Public research, private concerns: Ethical issues in the use of open-ended interviews with people who have learning difficulties. Disability and Society, 13(1), 21-36. Takai W (2004) How do deaf infants attain first signs? In Lewis V et al. (eds). The Reality of Research with Children and Young People. The Open University. Sage. London. Tammivaara, J., Enright D. (1986). On eliciting information: dialogues with child informants.Anthropology and Education Quarterly. 17, 218-238. Terzi, L. (2003) A capability perspective on impairment, disability and special, needs: Towards social justice in education; paper prepared for 3rd conference on the Capability approach: From sustainable development to sustainable freedom; Pavia, Italy, Sept 03 Tetley, J and Hanson, E. (?date). Participatory research; Nurse Researcher Vol 8, No 1 Thomas, J. A. and Montgomery, P. (1998). On Becoming a Good Teacher: Reflective Practice with Regard to Children's Voices. Journal of Teacher Education, 49(5), 372-380. Thomas, N. and O'Kane, C. (1998). The Ethics of Participatory Research with Children. Children and Society; v12 n5 p336-48 Nov. 77 Thomas, G. V. and Silk, A. M. (1990). An Introduction to the Psychology of Children's Drawings. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Treseder, P. (1997). Empowering Children and Young People, Children’s Rights Office and Save the Children. London. van't Riet, A., Berg, M., Hiddema, F. and Sol, K. (2001). Meeting patients' needs with patient information systems: Potential benefits of qualitative research methods. International Journal of Medical Informatics, 64(1), 1-14. Varga, D. (2000). Hyperbole and Humor in Children's Language Play. Journal of Research in Childhood Education; v14 n2 p142-51, 14(2), 142-151. Varga, D. (2000). Hyperbole and Humor in Children's Language Play. Journal of Research in Childhood Education; v14 n2 p142-51 Spr-Sum. Waksler, F. (1991). Studying children: phenomenological insights. In Waksler F (Ed). Studying the Social Worlds of Children. London, Falmer Press. 1991. Walker, A. Graffam (1999) Handbook on questioning children : a linguistic perspective 2nd ed. - Washington, D.C.: ABA Center on Children and the Law. Walker, Z; Townsend, J; Oakley, L; Donovan, C; Smith, H. (2002) Health Promotion for adolescents in primary care. British Medical Journal. 325; 524. Walters, A. E. (2001). Making art: the child's perspective.: Queensland University of Technology. Ward, L. and Flynn, M. (1994). What matters most: disability, research and empowerment. In Rioux, M.H and Bach, M. (?date) (eds) Disability is not measles. New Research Paradigms in Disability. North York, Ontario, Roeher Institute. Ward, L. (1996). Seen and Heard: Involving disabled children and young people in research and development projects. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Ward, L. (1998). Practising partnership: Involving people with learning difficulties in research. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 128-132. Ware, J. (in press). Interviewing people with Profound and Multiple Learning Difficulties - contextual issues. British Journal of Learning Disabilities. Warren, S (2000) Chapter 10. Lewis, A. and Lindsay, G. E. (Eds.). (2000) Researching Children's Perspectives. Buckingham: Open University Press. Weisner, T. S. (1999). Bringing Together Variable-Based and Person-Based Methods. Journal of Early Intervention, 22(4), 291-293. 78 Wellman, H., Phillips, A,. Rodriguez, T. (2000). Young Children's Understanding of Perception, Desire, and Emotion. Child Development; v71 n4 p895-912 Jul-Aug. West, A. (1995) You’re on your own. Young People’s research on leaving care. Save the Children, London. West, A. (1996a) Young People, participatory research and experiences of leaving care. PLA notes. February. West, A. (1996b) Crying out for more. Community Care. 26th September-2nd October edition; 8. West, A. (1997) Participatory Research BY Street Children, Dhaka. Interim Report. Save The Children. London. West, P. (1990). The status and validity of accounts obtained at interview: a contrast between two studies of families with a disabled child. Social Science and Medicine. 30, 11, 1229-39. Whitmore, K. F. and Norton-Meier, L. A. (2000). Reflections. Primary Voices, K-6(3), 43-47. Whitney, J. D. (2000). Down the rabbit hole or in search of evidence? Journal of Wocn???????????, 27(1), 17-19. Whittles, S. (1998). Can you hear us?: including the views of disabled children and young people. London: Save the Children. Wigglesworth, G. (1997). Children's individual approaches to the organization of narrative. Journal of Child Language. 24(2):279-309, Jun. Wilkinson, J. E. and Others, A. (1995). A Participatory Methodology for the Evaluation of Innovation in the Context of the Integrated Pre-school Services. Early Child Development and Care, 108, 35-49. Williams, P. (2001). Children's Ways of Experiencing Peer Interaction. Early Child Development and Care, 168, 17-38. Williams, V. (1999). Researching together. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 48-51. Willow, C. (2002). Participation in Practice: Children and Young People as Partners in Change. London: Children's Society. Wilson, C. and Powell, M. (2001). A Guide to Interviewing Children. London: Routledge. Wood, D. (1998) How Children think and Learn. 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell Woodhead, M. (1999). Combatting Child Labour: Listen to What the Children Say. Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 6(1), 27-49. 79 Yang, L. and Fox, K. (1999). Ethnography in health research and practice, including commentary by Roberts H. Ambulatory Child Health, 5(4), 339-349. Young, L. and Barrett, H. (2001). 'Ethics and Participation: Reflections on research with street children' Ethics, Place and Environment 4, 130-134. Zaragoza, M. S. (Ed.). (1995). Memory and Testimony in the Child Witness. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Zimmerman, D.P. (2002). Parallel dimensions in child, adolescent and adult analytic work. Therapeutic Communities: International Journal for Therapeutic and Supportive Organizations. Vol 23(3) (pp 204-218). 80 Appendix 3 Systematic mapping for the review Details of studies (n=56) to demonstrate the systematic map Key – numbers refer column number as per table 1. Item details To include author (s), title and details 2. Focus/Theme Basics Ethics Children’s Rights Legal aspects Research context Access Methodological issues (Methods) Building child-researcher relationships Balancing differential power relationships. Levels of child participation in research Research parameters [Authenticity/ credibility/ trustworthiness] Techniques used with children to elicit their views Conceptual issues Children’s competencies and perceptions Discourse Analysis Research issues Challenges /Gains Policy Wider impact International context Professional Base Health, Social work, Education, Psychology, Legal 3. Sample and Method Includes size re: numbers (n=) and age of participants Brief detail of method used Interviews, Art/ drawing Drama, / role play Debate Mapping Questionnaire Survey Observation Type of material: such as discursive material critique /opinion piece literature/systematic review policy document Type of analysis such as Discourse Analysis Conversation analysis 5. Field impact / Key points Final comments or perceptions 81 82 Version 2: Details of studies in the systematic map Item details (Author/title) Author: Alderson, P.; Morrow, G. Year: in press Title: Ethics, social research and consulting with children and young people. City: London Institution: Barnardo's Alderson P, Goodey C. (1996). Research with disabled children: how useful are childcentred ethics. Children and Society 10, 106 -116. Focus/Theme Allmark, P The ethics of research with children Nurse Researcher Vol 10, no 2 Ethics, Children Rights Health Critique Overview voluntary sector psychology ethics social policy Methodology/ approach Respect for chn’s rights as key theme; increasing interest in research ethics Comprehensive discussion of ethics, rights, methods, policy context Sample +method Field impact / Key points N/A updated version of seminal 95 text. incl ref to changing policy agenda since 95; imp of multiple methods of accessing chn's views; impact of moving away from the testing of chn; revised context of newer approaches to child development e.g. Burman Ethics Article –Question = 22 schools using Social Science ‘what kind of school is interviews with 45 Research study best for me’ children aged 7 – 17, & opinion piece their parents, teachers and classroom assistants, governors, councillors and LEA staff. Article - Guidelines on N/A ethical conduct of research with chn focus primarily quantitative research (e.g. RCPCH 2000). 1 Discusses access issues in urban working-class area (named as East City and West County). Uses personal experience (written in 1st person) so informative and easy to read. Ethics and gaining access outlined including dilemmas. Child-centred ethics critically evaluated as having many weaknesses and suppositions. Some assumptions and terminology, before briefly describing the history of the development of health ethical regulation. Specific ethical issues are scientific validity, welfare, and rights and dignity. Very useful overview on this field. Barker J, and Weller S. (2003). ‘Is it Fun?’ Developing Children Centred Methods’ International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy Research Study Baumann S.L. (1997). Qualitative research with children. Nursing Science Quarterly. 10, 2, 68-69. Author: Beresford, B. Year: 1997 Title: Personal accounts: Involving disabled children in research City: Social Policy Research Unit, University of York Publisher: Stationery Office Opinion piece Rights Article – Two research projects adopting child centred methodologies. Children consulted in design and use of pseudonyms. Used photographs, drawings, diaries, in-depth interviews, questionnaires. N/A Early text in the field but has stood the test of Research time well. Argues that 'it techniques is [however] from the field of research with models of disabled adults that the participation most useful models of participation [re chn] can be gleaned' [p 64] Study 1 = 4 to 11 year old Study 2 = 13 to 16 year olds. Highlights wealth of information about methods and techniques. Adult and child power-relationships are covered. Refers to work more children centred and less adult centred. Very clear article to read. N/A Two page brief overview of how qualitative research with children as participants. Useful scene set. N/a incl background section on chn's rights, adults as proxies. Research techniques [esp qual] summarised incl indiv +group interviews, observn, + specific tools. How research with chn differs from that with adults > ethical issues, practicalities, language/ communication, cognitive level, context. 2 Bricher, G. (1999). Children and qualitative research methods. Nurse Researcher, Vol 6 No 4, Summer. Methods & research context Health Socio. Literature review Article – literature Lit review centred review around chn. aged Discussion relates to 6 – 12 yrs primary school age children, between the ages of six and 12 years. 3 Useful Australian review - interviewing chn and interpretive processes. Review discusses challenges to researchers, partly because there is limited guidance within the nursing literature related to interviewing children. Key articles reviewed which crosses disciplinary boundaries, predominantly into sociological discourse. Discusses the lack of literature addressing the ways in which researchers can interpret the perceptions, feelings and experiences of children. Purports that challenges occur across the disciplines that undertake research with child participants. Issues pertaining to privacy and confidentiality and the ethical and legal complexities of undertaking research with children, are recognised as significant and interconnected to the ways in which research is carried out but have not been explored in this paper. Author: Bull, R Year: 1998 Title: Obtaining information from child witnesses Editor: Memon, A.; Vrij, A.; Bull, R. Book Title: Psychology and Law: Truthfulness, Accuracy and Credibility. City: London Publisher: McGraw Hill Pages: 188-209 Law Methods Reviews various contextual matters e.g. closed circuit TV. suggestibility- v broad review. cites Ceci. Latest wave of research has lked at conditions under which chn are/ not suggestible [rather than asking -are chn suggestible]. Stereotypes- Ceci 'Sam Stone' exp [cf earlier work on naughty Harry]. N/a 'Most of the research on the investigative interviewing of child witnesses/ victims has been published in the last ten years' (p 189). Interesting re raising issues about adults’ reliability as witnesses. Ambiguity re whether rapport is nec a gd thing. script theory cf Saywitz. If chn undeveloped re expectations about scripts then less likely than adult to be misled 'an adult with a well developed script may be more likely to be tricked by a scriptconsistent (yet incorrect) suggestion' (p 200). 4 Author: Bull, R. Year: 1995 Title: Innovative techniques for the questioning of child witnesses, especially those who are young and those with learning disability Editor: Zaragoza, M.S. Book Title: Memory and Testimony in the Child Witness City: Thousand Oaks Publisher: Sage Pages: 179-194 Methods Psychology Law Review re HO docs. Outline of interview protocol re chn. following HO memoranda of 1992 [?since revised]: 4 phases: rapport, free narrative, questioning, closing interview. Specifics re chn with LD. -notes that HO memo says little re this. N/a Key point: debate about acquiescencedraws heavily on Sigelman et al 81. Notes paucity of research -based evidence about interviewing LD. General vs specific questions cf Dent's research. 5 Carney, T., Murphy, s., McClure, J. Bishop, E., Kerr, C., Parker, J., Scott, F., Shields, C., & Wilson, L. (2003). Children's views of hospitalization an exploratory study of data collection. Journal of Child Health Care, 7(1), 27-40. Research study Author: Ceci, S. J.; Bruck, M. Year: 1995 Title: Jeopardy in the Courtroom City: Washington Publisher: APA Law Methods Overall aim of the study was to investigate the personal views of a children regarding their stay in hospital, to aid continuing development of a child-centred service. Used 1 in 4 questionnaires to cover both structured /unstructured & verbal /visual questionnaire types. The verbal unstructured questionnaire asked children to write a story or poem about their stay in hospital. The visual questionnaire consisted of five hospitalisation drawings and each child was invited to write their feelings about the picture. Focus is on the credibility of chn as witnesses; mainly US research/ context. Much referenced to suspected child abuse + interviewing of chn in that context.. All school aged children (but age in yrs not cited) over a three month period, (n= 213), from the paediatric wards of two district hospitals. 9 categories identified, each important to children - Organisation of events, physical environment, procedures that had occurred (pain), interaction with staff, emotions, perception of hospital, information given, home life. Describing pictures were found to be limited as the predetermined drawings did not fit in with what the children were actually verbally describing. Key debate = The verbal structured questionnaire was found to be the most effective tool with this age group. Findings highlighted that hospitalisation experience relates to age/development of the child and that preparation/explanations are required to help children settle. N/a Important text in the field, much quoted and summarizing seminal research by Ceci and co-workers. V small section on ethics [surprisingly understated] -take on this is a negative one about what is unacceptable. Wider context e.g. rights issues/ notions of childhood not explored 6 Author: Cloke, C; Davies, M. Year: 1995 Title: Participation and Empowerment in Child Protection City: Chichester Publisher: Wiley/ NSPCC Eclectic range 14 chapters, rights and n/a of papers around social policy emphases. theme of chn’s participation Coyne, I. T. (1998). Researching children: some methodological and ethical considerations Journal of Clinical Nursing, 7(5), 409-416. Method issues Conduct Child & researcher Ethics Informed consent Health setting Reflective account on research study Book Key note authors Christensen, P., & James, A. (Eds.). (2000). Research with Children: Perspectives and Practices. London: Falmer. Incl Lansdown on critique of chn’s rights to participation –useful review of field by key figure in the area. Incl individual chapters on marginalised groups eg black chn, disabled children, looked after chn, abused chn. See esp Cloke on policy links Article – methodological & ethical issues of research with experiences of hospitalized children aged 7 – 15. Interviews Clearly written outline of Rapport established methodological issues. Useful on with establishing rapport & conduct during 1 drawings interview process. Some case examples 2 Leaflets included. Some issues relating to 3 Tape rec. decision-making e.g single or group interviews; privacy and confidentiality Sample – 14 N/A N/A Excellent wide resource book written by a wide range of authors in the field. Draws on experience covering issues pertaining to perspectives of childhood 7 how to facilitate participatory research with children including listening and relationship. 7 Davis, J. M. (1998). Understanding the Meanings of Children: A Reflexive Process. Children & Society, 12(5), 325-335. Ethics Reflexive process listening children N/A Davis, J., Watson. N , & CunninghamBurley, S. (2000). Learning the Lives of Disabled Children: Developing a reflexive Approach. In P. Christensen & A. James (Eds.), Research with Children. Perspectives and Practices (pp. 201224). London: Farmer. Participation Reflexivity Docherty S, Sandelowski M. (1999). Focus on Qualitative Methods. Interviewing children. Research in Nursing and Health. 22, (2): 177-185. Opinion piece/ N/A Literature Paper – identifies process of understanding children’s voices. Includes ethics, roles and tools for listening to children. N/A of to Includes illustrative Entry into the field a Involved chn from a special school material from small key focus [PMLD] in Scotland. Stresses the group ‘interviews’ importance of researchers into children's lives exploring their own preconceptions about children's worlds. Ref to Bourdieu -social identity as defined and asserted through difference and associated group process (cross ref with Prout and co-workers). Particular focus on entry into the field and ways in which interpretations are negotiated between participants. Key message – in health shift from seeking views about children to seeking views from them. Discusses practical issues about content of an interview, timing and number, structure prompts and props. Largely conduct of interviews; analysis not covered. N/A 8 Author: Educable. Year: 2000 Title: No choice: no chance. The educational experiences of young people with disabilities City: Belfast Publisher: Save the Children Methods Participation interesting study as Survey reports research project conducted by [with support from various vol bodies] 9 young people with various disabilities who came together to carry out research. Friedland D. & Penn C. (2003). Conversation analysis as a technique for exploring the dynamics of a mediated interview. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 38(1):95111, 2003 Jan-Mar. Research study Conversation (CA) stresses heterogeneity of researchers and researched. Cites Beresford (97) that disabled are ‘doubly disadvantaged ‘ re research [i.e. as children and as disabled] Analysis Parents of head- Brief introduction about method used. injured child in a Principles of CA are outlined. Patterns speech-language that emerged between interviewer, practice. mediator/ translator and parents are identified e.g. establishment of roles, distribution of roles, interview techniques, code switching (where parties understand each other and codes they use switch), familiarity issues, inhibitors, interruptions, errors and trends. Key message = CA helps provide a systematic process of reviewing and reflecting on interview data such as dynamics, shifting roles and power. Also useful in cross-cultural and crosslingual work. 9 Author: Fritzley, V.H. ; Lee, K. Year: 2003 Title: Do young children always say yes to yes-no questions? A metadevelopmental study of the affirmation bias. Journal: Child Development Volume: 74, Pages: 1297-1313 Psychology Methods Acquiescence Author: Hart, S. N. Year: 2002 Title: Making Sure The Child's Voice Is Heard Journal: International Review of Education Volume: 48 Issue: 3-4 Pages: 251-258 ethics rights 395 chn aged 2-5 yrs asked a range of yesno questions to do with familiar and unfamiliar objects. Opinion piece, informed by background as (US) academic working in SEN field. Two-year-olds showed a consistent tendency to answer "Yes", irrespective of whether they were familiar with the object, or whether they understood the question. By contrast, the only bias shown by 4-5-yr-olds was to say "No" when they were asked questions they didn't understand. Results from the 3-yrolds were a mixed bag, suggesting they might be at a developmental transition phase between the other age groups. It was very rare for any of the children to answer "I don't know" even when they were positively encouraged to do so. The authors argue that their findings "have important implications for designing developmental studies as well as for conducting forensic interviews with preschool children". UNCRC articles 12 critiqued. Notes breadth/ depth of 'child's voice' and range of spheres for its influence. Section re evolving needs, capacities and 'best interests'. Asks -how much weight should be given to child's views/ how this is to be determined. [NOTEchallenging position] Reviews mechanisms through which to hear chn's views [does not address chn as researchers explicitly] 10 Hazel, N. (1995). Seen and heard: An examination of methods for collecting data from young. Unpublished MSc. University of Stirling. Hazel, N. (2003). Elicitation techniques with young people. Social Research, 12. University of Surrey and Unpublished thesis. University of Stirling (Grey lit) Doctoral and MSc Thesis Research study (two reflections) Qualitative project in Children aged 11 to 14 Scottish schools setting years. using multi methods to elicit views Range of fieldwork issues discussed. Include – vignettes, pictures, photographs, quotations e.g . well known catch phrases or titles from popular culture. Popular culture used such as TV programmes and media events. Presented problems and rules for discussion. Key message = variety of techniques used with supplement interviews helpful in gaining confidence and eliciting information (includes sensitive information) Hek, R. (2001). ‘Integration not segregation’ – experiences of young refugees in British Schools. FMR, p14-15. Unpublished report by kind permission Research Study Social Policy Education Pilot project to examine structures for refugee and asylum seeking children at school. 2 school – North London area. In-depth interviews. School A = 10 students (6 girls + 4 boys) and 2 teachers. School B = 5 boys + head. Wide diversity of young refugees. Records and procedures also analysed. Key messages = aspects that aid settlement were specialist teachers, friends, attitude of school & teachers, bullying, links to home & emotional support. Hence importance of treating child as an individual and listening was important. Technique. 11 Author: Henry, L.A.; Gudjonsson, G.H. Year: 1999 Title: Eyewitness memory and suggestibility in children with mental retardation Journal: AJMR Volume: 104 Issue: 6 Pages: 491-508 methods suggestibility Live scene enacted in mornings [cf Ceci] .Chn with MR tested on recall NB MR=mental of staged event 1 day retardation later cf CA and MA matched peers Hill, M., Laybourn, A., & Borland, M. (1996). Engaging with primary-aged children about their emotions and wellbeing: methodological considerations Children and Society, 10, 129-144. Methods Social policy Research study MR grp=28 chn age 11-12. 16 from spec sch, mean mental age =7.0. n=19 in CA comparable grp from local sec sch. n=21 in MA comparable grp. from 2 local prim schs. MR comparable with CA re free recall, gen q, open q, correctly leading q, More suggestible re closed leading q than were chn in CA comparable grp. yield vs shift. re suggestibility. Major difference between MR vs CA comparable = re closed leading questions where MR=more suggestible. poss bec weaker memory trace? BUT may have been under-using strategies they did have. hence imp for ecol validity NOTE- MR sim to MA comparable grp re suggestibility. Stresses sim re recall for MR compared with CA ie comparable FOR NONLEADING QUESTIONS. Increase in free recall and decrease in suggestibility 7-11 yrs. Flags poss source monitoring diffs. Article –links to study 12 groups of 6 Overview on methodological chn. Qualitative focus children (n=96) considerations – Focus Group interviews group & individual Aged 5 –12 with aim used with brainstorming & visual interviews used to describe emotional prompts of cards etc. 28 individual and mental well being. interviews. Benefits of combining focus groups ints with individual ints are outlined. Practical advise on field work with chn. Techniques are shared that researchers found useful. Some findings implicit to article but not data analysis. 12 Home Office. (2002). Achieving best evidence in criminal proceedings: guidance for vulnerable or intimidated witnesses, including children. London: Home Office Communication Directorate. Ethics Rights Law Methods Hood et al (1996) Children as research subjects: a risky business. Children and Society. 10, 117128. Access Competence Social Science Research study Replaces 1992 Includes sub-sections Memorandum of good on planning practice interviews, questioning techniques,; addressed separately for (1) children and (2) vulnerable and intimidated witnesses. Specific sections on the legal context, notably witness support and preparation, and witnesses at court. Article – Draws on Interviews at home qualitative research with children and study to assess ‘Risk’ families in 1 area. 46 chn/59 parents Includes brief reference to specialist techniques such as cognitive interview. Important reference document for legal contexts. Particularly relevant in e.g. interviewing children in cases of suspected child abuse; cross reference with material on forensic interviewing e.g. Bull (see above and bibliography) Age related article – uses sociological underpinning to work. Sound comments related to context & environment about researching chn at home, school, gatekeepers e.g. head Also – 6 pairs of 9 yr teacher, G.P. old chn at school & 3 Interview process discussed – topics of groups of 12 yr old risk included choices, decisions, things chn at a youth club. that worry chn, understanding of risk. Some narrative accounts discussed (parental influence on child). No discussion about analysis 13 Author: Hurley, J.C. ; Underwood, M.K. Year: 2002 Title: Children's understanding of their research rights before and after debriefing: Informed assent, confidentiality and stopping participation Journal: Child Development Volume: 73 Pages: 132-143 Rights Ethics Experimental designstudy to test claims routinely given about ethical procedures re chn- questions how far chn really understand these. N=178, age 8-12. Qaire to complete pre/post involvement in research. Researcher adhered to usual ethical protocols. Ireland L, Holloway I. (1996). Qualitative health research with children. Children and Society. 10, 155-164 Research study Health research Qualitative study to Purposive sample of Practical and ethical issues outlined. explore experience of 10 children aged 9 – Research relationship, negotiating childhood asthma. 12 yrs. access, ethics, issue of informed consent, children’s language, researcher conduct. 14 Post debriefing qaire suggested that older chn understood the debriefing but younger chn less clear eg younger chn did not understand implics re confidentiality. Many chn still had diffs describing research goals accurately. Author: Johnson, A.J. Year: 2002 Title: So..? Pragmatic implications of SoPrefaced questions in formal police interviews Editor: Cotterill, J. Book Title: Language in the Legal Process City: Houndmills, Basingstoke Publisher: Palgrave MacMillan Law Kirby, P. (1999). Involving Young Researchers: How to enable young people to design and conduct research, York: JRF / Youth Work Press. Participation Young researchers Linguistic analysis focus on so- utterances in legal contexts > micro-level analysis 5 child interviews [child witnesses in suspected abuse cases] and 5 adult interviews [defendants in range of cases] scrutinized.. Typology of so- usage [4 classes]. 1. Contrasting pattern of usage for interviewers [adverbial/ questionprefacing] cf child +adult interviewees [conjunction]. 2. more use in adult interviews by interviewee bec more narrative 3. absence of so- turns in very young children (prefer 'and') [implies developmental cause] . So- used as a topic boundary marker. Concludes that 'institutional context is more influential on the formal and pragmatic properties of talk than [is] interviewer style' [p 108] Book – range of projects discussed NA An excellent practical, resource book used by both authors. Highlights how researchers can involve children at all stages of research. Small exemplars help read. 15 Author: Kirby, P.; Lanyon, Clair; Kronin, K.; Sinclair, R. Year: 2003 Title: Building a culture of participation City: London Publisher: CYPU Participation Extensive review + handbooks for increasing children’s participation 29 case studies Lewis, A. 2004'And When Did You Last See Your Father?' Exploring the views of children with learning difficulties/disabiliti es. British Journal of Special Education, 31(1), 4-10. Methods Policy Review article drawing UN context discussed, together policy and review of evidence methods aspects from psychological work and forensic interviewing concerning interviewing children, especially those with learning difficulties Concludes with 4 challenges: dangers in over-formalising the process of exploring children’s views, defining competence/ capability, developing a collective voce, and taking appropriate follow up action once children’s views have been heard. Lewis, A and Lindsay G. (eds) 2000 Researching Children’s Perspectives Buckingham Open University Press Ethics Methods Book – ethical, legal, psychological and sociological perspectives critiqued and range of individual projects discussed Integrates a range of methodological, professional and disciplinary perspectives ; strong inclusion of material related to ‘special needs’ contexts. Editors suggest that choice of method will reflect researchers’ epistemological position, practicality, personal preferences and ethical considerations. Reconciling methods and purpose is discussed; and concerns about validity and reliability examined. 5 ‘perspectives’ examined; 9 individual projects discussed – spanning health, social, psychological and educational contexts; overview by editors summarises the emerging issues. Some sample schedules etc incl in appendices. 16 Review of range of participation, Info + practical advice Promoting effective participation Mahon, A., Glendinning, C., Clarke, K., & Craig, M. (1996). Researching children: methods and ethics. Children and Society, 10, 145154. Ethics Methods Social Science & policy Paper Reflection on methods & ethics with chn. Article – Reflection from researcher experience from 2 studies (Study 1 & 2) Miller, S. (2000a). Nurse Researcher, Vol 10, No 4 Analysis of phenomenological data generated with children as research participants. 74,, 1297-1313. Phenomenology Research study Phenomenological study with children who have diabetes mellitus. Indepth conversations with parents and children. Health related Interviews – Authors draw on own experiences of Study 1. 25 families interviewing chn. A number of trends chn. age 9-20 yrs are identified – social (changing family), political, legal which had impact on Study 2. 12 chn. Aged theoretical problems and validity of their 10-17 yrs. studies. Methodological issues outlined about researcher conduct and ethical issues that gave them concern in their studies. Some practical issues are included at the end of the article which are very helpful in considerations. Six children aged 7-12 Both studies build up a profile of this years interesting study about chdn with diabetes mellitus and their experiences. Key message = process of data analysis is clearly outlined (one few articles that spells out how this was undertaken). Also common issues of challenge e.g. ethics, seeking consent, access, communicating, location and privacy. Miller, S (2000b) Researching children: issues arising from a phenomenological study with children who have diabetes mellitus. Journal of Advanced Nursing 31 (5), p 1228-1234 17 Morgan, M., Gibbs, S., Maxwell, K., & Britten, N. (2002). Hearing children's voices: methodological issues in conducting focus groups with children aged 7-11 years. Qualitative Research, 2(1), 5-20. Focus group interviews were used which complemented an earlier phase that used personal interviews and drawings. Performed with an ‘experienced’ facilitator and cofacilitator conducting the groups and a third person recording the observations and undertaking field notes. Broad topics were used for the discussion. 42 children aged 7-11 years, drawn from a socio-economically and ethnically mixed urban area. The sample was identified by local general practitioner’s (G.P.) in the appropriate age group who were prescribed a type of prevention medication for Asthma. 18 Aim of the study was to elicit children’s own views and experiences of living with Asthma. Sound process. The findings indicated that sizes of groups were problematic if they were too large (resulting in non-interactive sessions) and too small (2 – 3 children was found to be tiring for all concerned). Key debate = Issues of participation were devised such as strategies for setting the scene (including informal atmosphere, using first names and warm up activities), room space, setting-up arrangements and permitting ‘fiddling’ all appeared to help towards collecting data. Assessing the children’s meanings was facilitated with the use of probing, clarification checks and introducing supplementary data collection tools such as art and role playing. The use of observation was also important in relation to the assessing the group dynamics and most specifically provided important insights about tensions and ‘sensitive’ issues such as disclosures of bullying. Author: Morris, J. Year: 1998 Title: Still missing? Vol.1 : Experience of disabled children and young people living away from their families City: London Publisher: Who Cares? Trust Rights Survey /discussion Morrow, V., & Richards, M. (1996). The ethics of social research with children: an overview. Children and Society, 10, 90105. child interviews N/A ethics Social science Paper as an overview – Opinion piece incl views of 30 severely disabled; 17/30 had been at boarding school incl these young people in steering grp Notes: incl egs of facilitators not being helpful. Focus is on disabled chn who lived away from home, as 'research subjects' [p 46 -this term dates the bk] and not as co-researchers but ahead of its time in taking seriously attempts to access views of this grp. Relatively little on methods. N/A Overview of ethical issues re social research + practical & methodological suggestions. Sound intro for current debate in this field. Key point =way in which chn are conceptualised [vulnerable/ incompetent/ powerless] impacts on research. Over-protectionism as possibly leading to aspects of chn's lives about which adults know little. Links with sociology of childhood eg Prout, James, Qvortrop. Key debate = whether general ethical principles/ codes dev with adults in mind apply equally to research with chn. Issue of how childhood/chn are defined + how this varies across rof/disciplinary contexts. Consent cf assent. Stresses heterogeinty of 'child' group. Imp of multiple methods. Chn as researchers. 19 Morrow, V. (1999). It's cool ... 'cos you can't give us detentions and things, can you? In P. Milner & B. Carolin (Eds.), Time to listen to children (pp. 203-215). London: Routledge. Author: Morrow,V. Year: 1998 Title: Understanding Families: Children's Perspectives City: London Publisher: National Children's Bureau / Joseph Rowntree Morrow, V. (2001). Using qualitative methods to elicit young people's perspectives on their environments: Some ideas for community health initiatives. Health Education Research, 16(3), 255-268. Chapter Qualitative study to 2 schools in Well written and easy to read about Researcher explore chdn views of Cambridge UK, authors experience of school based account from school. Range of sample of 96 chdn study. Key messages – theoretical own study techniques used aged 8 – 14 yrs. underpinning, field entry, techniques including drawing and writing, sentences completion and writing and focus group discussions. Issues related to ethical permission are also helpful. Excellent key debate about power relationships hence title of article. Chn’s views of Multi-methods approach bData colln 96-7, 8 class groups in 4 schs the family incl to access views of range in east Anglia: 2 sec + 2 primary ( 1 ‘unconventional of chn incl Pakistani sub village + 1 town for each ) schs, Chn age ’ families. group. c 8-13 Methods = Draw and write Text focuses on discussion of findings Sentence completion rather than methods Short qaire Grp discussion V little comment evaluating method; as we note elsewhere v little on process of analysis Research study Qualitative project in 102 children aged 12 – Paper discusses research setting, Health Education 15 yrs in 2 parts of methods used, including visual methods, Authority in order to deprived town in S. consent and ethical issues that arose. explore young peoples Eastern England Discusses how interconnected data idea of well-being and emerged to bring together issues of health quality of life. 20 Moston, S. (1987). The suggestibility of children in interview studies. First Language, 7, 67-78. Research study One to one interviews 72 chdn from 3 age following witnessing a groups (6, 8 and 10 Reliability of previous ‘staged event’. years) chdn testimony 16 questions; 8 were real about the event and 8 were false. Don’t know response was an option in responses. Nichols, S. (2002). Parents' construction of their children as gendered, literate subjects: a critical discourse analysis Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, Vol.2,no.2: Aug 2002 123-144. Northway, R. (2002). University of Glamorgan, UK. Commentary Nurse Researcher Research study Discourse analysis and interpretive frameworks Qualitative study Semi-structured interviews with 56 middle class Australian parents (31 mothers and 25 fathers). Opinion piece Commentary N/A N/A Really interesting paper on issues relating to chdn language and answering questions. Quantitative responses were measured. Repeated questioning = decrease in number of correct answers. Key messages = Issues about chdn use of ‘don’t know’. Recommend don’t know should be accepted in good faith not indicative of incomprehension. First answers should be accepted as when repeated question chdn changed their mind. Outline of discourse theory. Parents discussed how they viewed child as an individual (using gendered labelled descriptors), child as an active learner, and perceptions of child as normal, advanced or delayed. Key message = discourse analysis model identified gender web (e.g. boys vs. girls reading). Key message = Vulnerable groups encourage researchers to develop new ways of facilitating data collection, ethics and analysis. Message is about flexibility. 21 Pridmore, P. & Research study Bendelow, G. (1995). Images of health: Exploring beliefs of children using ‘draw and write’ technique. Health Education Journal. 54. 473-488. Two case studies using ‘Draw and write’ method and interviews Case study 1 – 100 Review of literature and draw and write chdn aged 9 or 10 method outlined. Key message = years in S.E. England. sophisticated data from draw and write method resulting in valuable insight into Case study 2 – 11 chdn’s health perceptions. Simple Bushmen children and technique but concern over 100 Botswana chdn interpretation is noted. Critical appraisal aged 9-10 years included of method and need for debriefing identified. Author: Prout, A Year: 2000 Title: Children's Participation: control and self-realization in British Late Modernity Journal: Children & Society Volume: 14 Pages: 304-15 Nature of childhd future-oriented. N/a ethics sociology Policy changes oriented to those chn's adult lives not lives as chn. Chn's participation in public life cf tension between control and self realisation found in late modernity. Public policy/practice marked by intensification of control around chn. Stress on 'early intervention' but as this recurrently fails chn subject to more control. Chn as human capital > a means of controlling the future. Contrasted with private sphere in which chn increasingly 'sequestered' by the family. The space of chn as becoming more specialised and more localised. Cites O’Neill 94 re advocacy of a civic childhd that guarantees all chn minimum levels.. Argues for 'more responsive institutions that engage in a more creative dialogue with their users ' p. 312 22 Punch, S. G. (2002). Research with Children: The Same or Different from Research with Adults? Childhood: A Global Journal of Child Research, 9(3), 321-341. Paper Literature/ Opinion piece N/A N/A Article explores several methodological issues detail – ways of seeing children affect ways of listening; differences with children from adults; reflections of task based methods; drawings; photographs; PRA (Participatory Rural Techniques); Diaries, worksheets, combining methods, 23 Ring, K. (2000). Young children talking about their drawings: methodological dilemmas. Paper presented at the British Educational Research Association Annual Conference, Cardiff University. AND Ring, K. (2003). Young children drawing: the significance of the context. Paper presented at the Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the British Educational Research Association, 13-15 September 2001., University of Leeds, England, Conference papers Research study Qualitative longitudinal research study (3 yrs time frame) to explore socio-cultural aspects important in child’s life at home and school that help children make meaning. Uses conversational interviews , 2D drawings and photographs (3D) of children’s found objects. 7 young children at home and at school aged rising 5’s to 7 yrs. Parent / adult interviews. 24 Preliminary findings but these very interesting papers (written in ‘I’) very helpful in decision-making process. Excellent key messages on preparation and when & how best to convey information to child in interview. Dynamic process is clearly exemplified in extracts of transcripts. Significance of drawing, photos and distraction is also underpinned. Rollins, P.R. McCabe, A. Bliss, L. (2000). Culturally sensitive assessment of narrative skills in children. Seminars in Speech & Language. 21(3):223-34, 275-6. Sartain, S, Clarke, C.L. (2000). Hearing the voices of children with chronic illness. Journal of Advanced Nursing Vol 32 (4); p913921.Oct. Narrative skills N/A Framework – discussion paper N/A Qualitative research study Chronic illness Grounded Theory using exploratory interviews in family home with art /drawing techniques. Data analysed as open, axial and selective coding. Convenient sample of 7 families, their carers and children who were aged 8 –14 yrs. Inclusion due to child being able to articulate their perceptions. Stiegler, L.N.& Hoffman, P.R. (2001). Discoursebased intervention for word finding in children.. J. of Communication Disorders. 34(4):277-303, 36770, 2001 Jul-Aug. Evaluation method of Discourse Based Intervention using multi-methods of Research study pictures, stories, conversation starters in Language school. Included five phased approach of 15 mins per session per subject. Three 9 year old boys with diagnosed language-learning disabilities (LLD) 25 Chldn personal narratives as formulated by Labov’s work. Work focuses on narratives from differing cultural groups and considers developmental sequence. Very clearly written in a step by step process of narrative skills including diagrammatic representation (Eliciting, coding and scoring the narrative are outlined). Appendix worked example also included. Children were described as competent interpreters of their world (Deatrick and Faux 1991) . Understood services so must be listened to. Key messages = competence and rights. Quantitative analysis performed on data. Identified and categorised chdn word finding as they occur in discourse. Potentially has useful considerations when undertaking discourse analysis. Steward, M et al. (1993). Implications of developmental research for interviewing children. Child Abuse and Neglect 17, 25-37. Research literature review N/A N/A Covers literature (1993 published) pertaining to children’s development issues and their cognition, memory and language. Provides pointers for interviewing children and pointers for children’s reporting about abuse. Thomas, N. & O'Kane, C. (1998). The Ethics of Participatory Research with Children. Children & Society; V12 n5 p336-48 Nov. UNICEF (2004) State of the World’s Children Research study N/A – reflections Ethical problems N/A Paper discusses ethical problems in participatory research. Shares issues pertaining to participation and choice and how this can improve validity. Interesting reflections on payments and power balance Child interpretations on data analysis are noted. World wide Review incl review incl ref statistical data to children ’s participation key N/a 26 Author: Ward, L. Year: 1996 Title: Seen and Heard: Involving disabled children and young people in research and development projects City: York Publisher: Joseph Rowntree Foundation Ethics West, P. (1990). The status and validity of accounts obtained at interview: a contrast between two studies of families with a disabled child. Social Science and Medicine. 30, 11, 1229-39 Research study Two studies explored Not included about Chn. from mid 1970’s. Each accounts similar topics and approach in relation to disabled child. Medical Sociolgy Procedures discusses inv chn and N/a young people as partners to projects at planning, consultancy, advisory group, data collection, analysis and dissemination stages short sections on rights/ ethics.. Also acknowledges equal opps issues re age, gender, race, disability. Includes a 10 point checklist for action; involvement, ethics, support, methodology, practicalities, inclusion, dissemination, budget, proposal and partnership. 27 Study 1 – a ‘glowing’ account of the way parents make sense of having a disabled child and Study 2 – a ‘gloomy’ account of struggle encountered. Contrasts between the two are made. Key message – status and validity of accounts in an interview. Suggests process of triangulation. Author: Willow, C. Year: 2002 Title: Participation in Practice: Children and Young People as Partners in Change City: London Publisher: Children's Society rights social policy review of UNCRC/ N/a article 12 and ensuing policy. links with rights agenda, repercussions of implementing article 12 in the UK. Detailed grid of policy changes/ guidelines associated with this. Warning note that pursuit of ‘practical tips’ may ‘conceal fundamental questions about the purpose of promoting participation’ [p 30] 28