Measuring Classroom Discipline

advertisement

Teachers’ classroom discipline and Student Misbehavior in Australia, China and Israel

Authors

Ramon Lewis.

Associate Professor, Institute for Education, La Trobe University, Melbourne,

Victoria, Australia. 613 95258482.

Shlomo Romi.

Deputy Director, School of Education, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel. 972

35318447

Xing, Qui

Professor, Department of Psychology, Sichuan College of Education

Sichuan Province, Chengdu. 610041, PRC.

Yakov J. Katz.

Professor. Dean, School of Education, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel.

972 3 5418709

Acknowledgment

The research project reported in this paper was in part sponsored by the Institute for

Community Education and Research, School of Educatiion, Bar-Ilan University,

Israel.

Teachers’ classroom discipline and Student Misbehavior in Australia, China and Israel

Abstract

This paper reports students’ perceptions of the classroom discipline strategies utilized in Australia, China and Israel. It examines data from 748 teachers and 5521 students to ide ntify how teachers’ use of various disciplinary strategies, and the extent to which these relate to student misbehaviour differ in three national settings.

In general, Chinese teachers appear less punitive and aggressive than do those in

Israel or Austral ia and more inclusive and supportive of students’ voices. Australian classrooms are perceived as having least discussion and recognition and most punishment. In all settings greater student misbehaviour relates only to increased use of aggressive strategies. Implications are discussed.

Introduction

The issue of how best to discipline students in classrooms is of continuing interest and concern to the community. For example, in 2002, as in nearly all preceding years, students’ lack of discipline ranked within the first two most serious problems confronting the public schools in the annual Phi Delta Kappa Polls of the public’s attitudes towards the public schools (Lowell and Gallup, 2002). In 2002, 43 percent of respondents rated students’ lack of discipline as a very serious concern, with an

1

additional 33 percent suggesting it was somewhat serious. Of all the school-related factors capable of influencing student responsibility in classrooms, teachers’ discipline strategies, the focus of this paper, are among the most potent (Ingersoll,

1996; Lewis, 1997a).

Ensuring that students behave responsibly in classrooms is important for two independent reasons. First, it serves as a means of preparing students to take their place in society as responsible citizens, an aim of primary importance to schooling

(Rothstein, 2000). Secondly, without satisfactory levels of student responsibility, the best planned and potentially most engaging lessons may fail to have the desired impact. Often it may only require a small proportion of students to misbehave and they become sufficiently distracting to students and frustrating to teachers that the most carefully planned lesson fails to promote effective learning among the students

(Barton, Coley and Wenglinsky, 1998). This paper examines the relationship between discipline processes and student misbehaviour in three different national settings, namely Australia, China and Israel.

Interest in classroom discipline relates not only to the good it can do but also to the damage inappropriate discipline can cause. For example, two recent publications emphasize the potential negative impact of particular discipline strategies. The first conjectures that

“Unnecessarily harsh and punitive disciplinary practices against students create a climate that contributes to school violence. This issue is little recognized and scarcely researched.” (Hyman and Snook, 2000: 489)

The second publication reports the perceptions of over 3500 school students in

Australia (Lewis, 2001). This study demonstrates empirically that, in the view of these students, their teachers are characterized by 2 distinct discipline styles. The first of these was called “Coercive” discipline and comprised punishment and aggression (yelling in anger, sarcasm group punishments, etc). The second style, comprising Discussion, Hints, Recognition, Involvement and Punishment, was called

“Relationship based discipline”. After presenting a thorough data analysis the report concludes:

“Students who receive more Relationship based discipline are less disrupted when teachers deal with misbehavior and generally act more responsibly in that teacher’s class. In contrast, the impact of Coercive discipline appears to be more student distraction from work and less responsibility.” (p. 315)

These findings appear consistent with those of Miller et al (2000) who, after examining students’ perceptions of what factors cause classroom misbehavior, highlight the potential for teacher behavior such as shouting all the time, unfairly blaming students, picking on kids, and being rude, to stimulate student resistance and subsequent misbehavior.

“The present study has shown pupils to attribute to teachers a significantly greater responsibility for pupil misbehavior than that they attribute to parents.

(p. 93)

2

It is of interest to note that in an earlier Israeli study which also examined factors seen to be causing student misbehavior, it was reported that although students placed their teachers’ attitude the second most important cause of student misbehavior, teachers ranked it as 20 out of 26 (Guttmann, 1982).

The impetus for the research discussed in this paper came after the publication of a study of the relationship between classroom discipline and student responsibility and misbehavior in Australia (Lewis,2001). As a result of that publication, academics in a number of countries contacted the author with requests to replicate the study in their respective national settings. Two additional settings were ultimately selected. These were China and Israel. Australia is a typically western country, China is a typically oriental country and Israel is approximately half western and half oriental.

Consequently the use of these three cultural settings provided the opportunity for some robust comparisons. Further, since the academics from these settings who expressed interest in undertaking the research were senior, very experienced, and involved in teacher training for many years, the issue of classroom discipline, and the research design of the previous research appeared to have valency in both countries

In summary, there were two main foci for the research. First it permitted a comparison of the extent of usage of various discipline strategies in three significantly different national settings. Secondly, the relationship between student misbehavior and classroom discipline could be examined in each setting. In conducting this research it was acknowledged that there are likely to be cultural differences associated with styles of discipline. For example, differences of opinion between Chinese and other Americans (Mitchel, 2001) appear to focus on the relative virtues of submission to authority versus the child’s right to be assertive and individualistic.

The significance of classroom discipline rests not on ly on its impact on students’ behavior and learning, as outlined above. The ability of teachers to effectively discipline students is, according to McCormick and Shi (1999) integrally related to teachers’ sense of professional adequacy. This finding appears consistent with the work of Goddard (2000), who reports the results of a study of 233 teachers’ views of their role. He notes that ‘disciplinarian’ was the third most commonly cited metaphor provided by teachers for their work, ranking only behind ‘leader’ and ‘knowledge dispenser’. It is not surprising therefore that any failure on teachers’ part to satisfactorily manage students’ classroom misbehavior can result in stress, and in the extreme case, burnout. Overall, classroom discipline is a well-documented source of teacher stress (Kyriacou, 1987; Borg et al, 1991; Blase,1986; DeRobbio and Iwanicki, 1996; Friedman, 1995; Keiper and Busselle, 1996). Some results however may be tenuous as teachers experiencing stress as a result of other factors

(for example excessive workload) may perceive student behavior more negatively

(Whiteman, Young and Fisher, 1985) and therefore inflate its significance as a stressor. Nevertheless, discipline issues rate consistently among the strongest of teacher stressors.

Chan (1998), reporting on the stressors of over 400 teachers in Hong Kong, notes that student behavior management rates as the second most significant factor

3

stressing teachers. More significantly, perhaps, Ingersoll (2001) studied approximately 6700 teachers in the US and states that approximately 30 percent of the 400 or so who chose to leave the profession identify student discipline as one of the reasons that causes them to give up teaching. It needs to be noted that for some teachers, the stress associated with classroom discipline relates as much to how they are treated by other teachers and members of the school administration as it does to their treatment at the hands of students (Martin,1994 ).

Related research shows that it is not only the stress arising from ineffective discipline that is a cause for concern. The way teachers attempt to cope with their concerns may add to their stress levels rather than lower them. For example, a recent study demonstrates that teachers who are more worried about discipline and student misbehavior report greater use of 6 maladaptive coping strategies, namely, wishful thinking, don’t tell anyone, self-blame, worry, disruption to sleeping or eating patterns and getting sick (Lewis, 1999). These strategies are among those recently shown to be predictive of a range of non-productive outcomes ranging from low self concept to depression (Frydenberg and Lewis 2000). Clearly, the issue of classroom discipline is of as much significance to teachers as it is to students.

Classroom misbehavior and discipline is a topic that has been studied in Australia

(for example, Lewis and Lovegrove, 1987 : Lewis, 2001;Oswald, 1995), China (for example, Goa, 1998; Peng, 1993; Goa and Watkins, 2001, Jin and Cortazzi, 1998) and Israel (for example, Romi and Freund, 1999; Kaplan, Gheen, and Midgley,

2002; Friedman, 1994) However, this is the first attempt to systematically examine students’ perceptions of their teachers’ disciplinary strategies and their misbehavior in these three national settings.

Measuring Classroom Discipline

To measure classroom discipline in 3 national settings was problematic.

Conceptualisation of classroom discipline strategies in one setting could not necessarily be assumed equivalent to those in the other two. It would have been possible to utilize exploratory factor analysis on data sets from respective nations to obtain assessments of discipline most appropriate to each setting. It would even have been possible to utilize confirmatory factor analyses and compare goodness of fit measures to examine the extent to which one particular measure applied equally to all three national settings. However, had students’ views of classroom discipline provided different measures, comparisons would have been prohibited.

Consequently, since as stated above it was the aim of this research to undertake a replication, care was taken to ensure that the same measures were utilized in each national setting.

To examine the relationship between discipline strategies and student misbehavior two steps were taken. First, the 35 items on the questionnaire used in the earlier

Australian study were studied by the colleagues in China and Israel to assess their relevance to their respective students. As a result, 11 items were omitted and 24 were retained. These 24 items were agreed by the researchers in each country to assess the 6 discipline strategies reported in the previous Australian study and to be of cultural relevance, albeit to differing degrees, in their respective national settings.

The strategies measured were Punishing, Rewarding, Involvement in decision-

4

making, Hinting, Discussion and Aggression. Examination of a number of discipline texts ( Charles, 2004 ; Tauber, 1999; Wolfgang, 1995; Lewis, 1997b ) indicated that one or more of these strategies were seen as underlying most of the available approaches to classroom discipline. For example, Reward and Punishment related to Interventionist approaches, Involvement to Interactional approaches and Hinting and Discussion to Non-Interventional Approaches. Although Aggression was not recommended in any text, it was seen as potentially possible in classrooms in all settings and necessary to include given its significance in previous research (Lewis,

2001).

The following 4 items were designed to assess Punishment.

Gives out consequences to students who misbehave (e.g. move their seats, detention)

Increases the level of consequence if students will not do as they are told (e.g. move seats, detention).

Increases the level of consequence if a misbehaving student argues.

Increases the level of consequence if a misbehaving student stops when told, but then does it again.

The four items developed to assess Recognition and rewards were as follows.

Rewards individual students who behave properly.

Praises the class for good behavior.

Praises individual students for good behavior.

Rewards the class when students behave well.

The following two sets of items were included to determine the extent to which teachers attempted to incl ude students’ voice in the decision-making related to discipline.

The first four relate to an emphasis on the class as the determiner of the discipline process.

Organizes the class to work out the rules for good behavior.

Decides with the class what should happen to students who misbehave.

Makes students leave the room until they decide to behave properly.

Lets students know that the way they are behaving is not how the class expects them to.

The next four items provide for the voice of individual students.

 Discusses students’ behavior with them to allow them to figure out a better way to behave in future.

Lets students talk about their side of things so that it can be clearly understood.

Gets students to understand why their behavior is a problem for others by discussing it with them.

Gets students to change the way they behave by helping them understand how their behavior affects others.

The next set of four items relates to a strategy that usually precedes more formal intervention by the teacher, and provides students with awareness that in the teacher’s eyes, all is not as it should be. It probably also communicates some level

5

of trust that students will self-regulate their behavior. The following four items were designed to assess this process which was called Hinting.

Describes what students are doing wrong, and expect them to stop.

Asks students questions like "What are you doing?" to get them to think about how to behave better.

Reminds misbehaving students about the class rules.

Describes how students are misbehaving to make them decide whether to stop or not.

The final four items were written to permit measurement of teacher Aggression. This strategy was defined as the use of strategies which, while legal, may in some settings negate t he student’s sense of well-being and possession of natural rights.

Yells angrily at students who misbehave.

Deliberately embarrasses students who misbehave

Keeps the class in because some students misbehave

Makes sarcastic comments to students who misbehave

In summary therefore, the six classroom discipline strategies examined in the previous study (Lewis, 2001) were assessed by a total of 24 questionnaire items.

To enable the collection of data without identifying any individual teacher by name, questionnaires specified one of six subject areas taught (for example Science,

Mathematics, English (Chinese, Hebrew), Social Studies ). Students were then requested to concentrate on that one class and the teacher who teaches it when completing the questionnaire.

To measure the extent of student misbehavior students were asked to indicate “How often do you misbehave in this teacher’s class?”. To respond they chose from the alternatives, Almost never, Only a little, Sometimes and Often, which were coded 1 to 4 respectively.

The questionnaire was translated into both Chinese and Hebrew and in each case back-translated into English to ensure accuracy. To respond to the discipline items, students indicated on a 6 point scale how frequently the teacher acted as described in the statement “when trying to deal with misbehavior”. The response alternatives provided, namely, Nearly always, Most of the time, A lot of the time, Some of the time, Hardly ever and Never were coded 6 to 1 respectively. Students at grade levels

7 to 12 completed these items documenting their perception of their teacher’s use of each classroom discipline strategy. The introduction to the questionnaire was brief and indicated that the questions to follow focused on “classroom discipline and how you feel about it”. There was no indication as to the research questions being addressed. Written explanation of response formats were provided as required.

Sample.

The three purposive samples utilized in this study, and described below, were restricted to students attending years 7 to 12 at coeducational schools. Although representative sampling was not attempted, care was taken when selecting participating schools to ensure that the sample included both larger and smaller schools, situated in a range of socioeconomic and geographic areas. In addition, schools which appeared ‘atypical’ were not included, for example extremely large,

6

small or isolated schools, or schools which were selective in intake. In Australia, all secondary schools (years 7 to 12) in the North Eastern region of Victoria and a small number in the Melbourne metropolitan region were invited to participate in the study.

The response rate of 70 percent reflects the importance attributed to the topic of classroom discipline in Secondary schools.

In Israel, a sample of 4 high schools (years 10 to12) and 8 Junior high Schools

(years 7 to 9) in the geographic center of Israel were invited to participate in the study. All accepted.

In China, the sample of teachers and students was drawn from 8 schools in

Chengdu region, Sichuan Province. Two of these were lower Secondary schools

(years 7 to 9) and the remainder comprised years 7 to 12. In each Chinese and

Israeli school a random sample of classes at all year levels were selected. As a research assistant administered questionnaires to these classes their teachers completed their questionnaires.

Table 1 below records the number of teachers, and students at year levels 7-8, 9-10 and 11-12 for Australia, Israel and China respectively.

INSERT TABLE 1 HERE

Of the total of 5521 students, 48% are males, although the percentage of males varies from 38 to 60 percent depending on year level within country. The gender distribution of the 748 teachers is less evenly divided. There were 11, 42 and 46 percent of males respectively in Israel, Australia and china. It is of interest to note the small proportion of men in the Israeli sample. This is not atypical however, as noted in previous research (Romi and Katz, 2002), and reflects the predominance of women in the teaching force in non-religious schools in Israel.

Findings

Prior to comparing the extent to which the various discipline strategies are utilised in different national settings and examining the relationship between various types of discipline perceived by students and classroom misbehaviour, it was considered helpful to document how significant an issue classroom discipline is for the teachers of the students responding to the survey. To gain such information, a 10 percent sample of the teachers at the selected schools answered 2 questions as part of the survey. The first required them to state “How many students misbehave in the first class you would normally be teaching next Monday?” Five alternative responses were provided, namely Nearly all, Most, Some, Hardly any and None. These responses were coded 5 to 1 respectively. Since what constituted “misbehavior” was not defined or exemplified, it was acknowledged that the data could only provide a general indication of the extent of the issue.

The second question asked “ To what extent is the issue of classroom discipline and student misbehavior an issue of concern to you?” To answer, teachers selected either Of major concern, Of moderate concern, Of minor concern, Of almost no

7

concern or Of no concern. These alternatives were coded 5 to 1 respectively. Tables

2 and 3 report the relevant data for teachers from the three national settings.

TABLE 2 HERE

TABLE 3 HERE

Inspection of the Chi-Square figures for table 2 (Chi Square (16)=20.191, p= .212) shows there are no statistically significant differences in the perceived levels of classroom misbehavior in Australia, Israel and China.

In contrast, initial inspection of the data in Table 3 would seem to show that teachers in Israel report significantly more concern about classroom misbehavior than do teachers from either Australia or China. However, the substantial differences were due to another cause. Although great care was taken to ensure that translation of all terms on the questionnaire were accurate, an error occurred for this question. In the

Hebrew version of the questionnaire the word “concern” was translated as

“Importance”. This discrepancy was not noted after back-translation to English.

Therefore, rather than reporting to what extent the issues of classroom discipline and student misbehavior was an issue of concern to them, the Israeli teachers indicated the extent it was an issue of importance. The comparison of the Australian and equivalent Chinese data showed no significant differences (Chi square (4)= 2.774, p=.596).

It is of interest to note that in a recent study 294 Australian teachers were asked to indicate their levels of concern related to any inability to discipline classrooms as they would prefer. In reporting the se data Lewis (1999: 162) states “it can be argued that the gap between best and current discipline practice was, on average, of only moderate concern to these teachers.” It would appear therefore that the 2 sets of data, collected over 3 years apart, from independent samples of teachers, are very consistent.

As stated above, the data in Tables 2 and 3, which provide a general framework for the subsequent discussion on classroom discipline strategies used in classrooms, show that a substantial group of teachers in 2 independent national settings report at least moderate levels of concern over student misbehavior in class. In addition, the

Israeli teachers sampled clearly highlight the significance of the topic. It is likely that part of the teachers’ interest in classroom discipline is based on their perception that, on average, a little fewer than “some” but more than “hardly any” students in the classes they expect to teach are likely to misbehave.

To document the reliability of the measures of teach ers’ discipline strategies, the students’ responses to the sets of 4 items were considered for internal consistency in each national setting, Cronbach alpha coefficients were computed for each set. If the deletion of any item from a set increased the magnitude of the respective alpha, that item was to be removed. The reliance on Alpha did not reflect a belief that high values for internal consistency of responses within a set of items was essential to justify the value of a scale. It was assumed, given the diversity of ways in which some strategies may be operationalized in different national settings, that increased likelihood of one behavior in a set of 4 may only be a weak predictor of the utilization

8

of those comprising the remainder of the set. Nevertheless, items would be treated as a scale only if each item added shared explanatory variance to the measure.

The only scale for which any items were excluded was Involvement. In both Australia and Israel, the removal of the item “Makes students leave the room until they decide to behave properly” improved the magnitude of the respective alpha coefficient. On this basis it was removed from the scale. Consequently there were only 3 items remaining in the Involvement scale.

Table 4 reports the scale means (M), average item means for the irems comprising respective scales (AIM), standard deviations (SD) and Cronbach alpha coefficients for each of the six scales measuring classroom discipline strategies.

TABLE 4 HERE

Inspection of the alpha coefficients in Table 4 shows that most alpha’s are moderate.

Some are clearly low and reflect a loose (although statistically significant) association between the behaviors related to one strategy. For example, the

Involvement scale’s alpha of approximately .5 reflects correlations of .17, .26 and .40 between the three respective pairs of items. The likelihood of teachers letting students know that “the way they are behaving is not how the class expects them to” is not highly associated with the likelihood of them organizing the class to work out the rules for good behavior and what should happen to students who misbehave, even though the latter two behaviors are more likely to co-occur. Another explanation for the low alpha coefficients is the difficulty inherent in transferring strategies conceptualized in one culture to another. Nevertheless, despite some concern about the low internal consistency of responses to items in the Involvement and Hinting scales, it was determined to use all six scales for purposes of replication, acknowledging the tenuousness of finding relating to the less reliable measures.

Classroom discipline strategies in Australia, Israel and China

Before examining the relationship between classroom discipline and misbehavior, consideration was given to the comparison of the frequency of usage of different strategies in differing national settings and the corelatons between strategies. .

Inspection of the average item means, also reported in Table 4, indicates that the pattern of usage of the various classroom discipline strategies appears relatively similar in Australia and Israel. In both countries, teachers commonly react to misbehavior by letting students know that there is a problem in the hope that they will improve their behavior. In addition they are more than sometimes likely to Punish misbehaving students and discuss with them the impact their misbehavior has on others in a bid to have them determine a better way to behave. They recognize appropriate behavior more than sometimes, to increase the likelihood of its reoccurrence, The two strategies utilized less frequently than sometimes are aggression and involvement of the class in setting rules and consequences. The pattern in China is a little different in that students report greater use of all strategies except Aggression and Punishment. Although the relative usage of strategies also varies by Country the only strategy to vary rank within a country by more than 2 ranks is Punishment, which ranks as the most common strategy in Australia, and the fourth and fifth most commonly used strategy in Israel and China respectively.

9

In interpreting these data, it is of value to note that there are a number of substantial correlations between strategies. In each national setting, when correlations greater than .4 are considered, four strategies intercorrelate. These are discussion, reward, hint and involve. All appear to demonstrate a positive attitude towards students. In addition, in each setting, aggression correlates with punishment, forming a potentially more negative response to misbehavior, although in Australia punishment also correlates with hint. These patterns of relationships are very similar in different national settings, suggesting that teachers may be seen by students in terms of discipli ne “styles” comprising a number of strategies.

In order to more systematically compare national differences in the extent of usage of different discipline strategies, a 5 way MANOVA was performed where Country, level of student misbehavior in class, gender of the student, the gender of the teacher and year level (year 7/8, 9/10 or 11/12) were utilized as independent variables and the 6 discipline strategies were the dependent variables. This enabled the consideration of the students’ age and sex and the sex of the teacher to be considered in the analysis in addition to national setting and level of misbehavior.

Since level of student misbehavior was one of the independent variables, the analyse also permitted the relationship between discipline and misbehavior to be examined in the different settings.

The analysis indicated a total of 7 statistically significant effects. Since the analysis investigated 31 predictors, a conservative probability level of .01 was utilized for statistical significance. Table 5 reports the Mutivariate F values (Pillai’s trace) for the significant effects.

TABLE 5 HERE

Inspection of the data in Table 5 shows that 3 main effects and 4 interaction effects were statistically significant. Consideration of the relevant univariate tests and Sheffe tests for post hoc comparisons (p<.05) for year level indicate that there is significantly more Discussion (F= 7.04, .001), Recognition (F=5.96, p=.003) and

Involvement (F=6.72, p=.001) in year 7 and 8 than in years 9 and 10, and 11 and 12

(which did not differ significantly). In addition, there is more Hinting perceived in years 7 and 8, and 9 and 19, than in years 11 and 12. These data may reflect a greater need for intervention with younger students who are new to Secondary schooling and in the process of adaption. However, if that were the case it is surprising that there in not significantly more Punishment at this level also. In summary however, it may be argued that teachers are willing to use more positive strategiiess, ones which imply trust, with younger students.

The main effect for Country applied to all 6 classroom discipline techniques and showed that students in China, compared to those in Australia or Israel, report less usage of Punishment (F=15.71, p=.000) and Aggression (F=11.92, p-.000), and greater use of Recognition (F=56.65, p=.000), Discussion (F=71.92, p=.000), Hinting

(F=14.66, p=.000) and Involvement (F=100.15, p=.000). Whereas students in

Australia and Israel don’t differ in reported levels of Involvement, Aggression and

10

Hinting, Australian students report less use of Discussion, Recognition and more

Punishment than do those in Israel.

The final main effect indicated that students who reported greater levels of misbehavior were more likely to perceive Aggression by teachers (F=7.87, p=.000), although no other strategy produced statistically significant univariate results.

Some elaboration of these main effects comes about when interaction effects are considered. The only interaction effect which failed to produce a statistically significant univariate effect (p<.01) was Country by Year by Misbehavior. The other 4 therefore required explanation.

Firstly, the Country by Teacher Sex interaction showed that even though, as reported above, there is less Aggression reported in China, it is primarily due to it being seen as used less by men (F=7.69, p=.000). Secondly, the Country by year level interaction indicated that there were 3 strategies with statistically significant differences, namely Punishment (F=6.38, p=.000), Aggression (F=5.73, p=.000) and

Discussion (F=6.60, p=.000). The first two of these show that whereas year 11 and

12 students in Australia and Israel are least likely to report the use of Punishment and Aggression, those in China report such strategies more frequently than do younger students. The last Country by Year level interaction effect, for Discussion, shows that in Israel the variation in usage by year level is four times that found in either China or Australia.

The final significant interaction (Country by Teacher sex by Year level) sheds a little more light on some of the earlier findings. Four univariate effects were noted as significant, namely for Punishment (F= 5.54, p=.000), Aggression (F=3.68, p=.005),

Involvement (F=7.05, p=.000) and Hinting (F=3.55, p=.007). The first two results indicate that the main reason older students in China report more Punishment and

Aggression (see above) is because it is seen as coming primarily from women teachers (as is Punishment in Israel). In Australia male and female teachers display similar, low levels of Aggression and Punishment with older students.

The level of involvement of students in classroom management decision-making and the amount of hinting at the inappropriateness of student behavior by women teachers in both Australia and China decreases with increasing student age. For women teachers in Israel, most student involvement occurs with students at years

11 and 12, although the differences between year levels are very small. For men the picture is similar in that most Hinting and Involvement occurs for students in years 7 and 8. The least is offered to years 9 and 10 in both Israel and China, and 11 and 12 in Australia. In China the differences are very small for Hinting.

In Israel, there is more Hinting at, and Involving of, students in years 7 and 8 by men teachers than by women. At years 9 and 10 the opposite is the case. In contrast, in

Australia, women teachers are more likely than men to Involve and Hint at students in all year levels, although at years 9 and 10 the difference is very small. In China, men teachers are more likely to hint at, and involve, students in years 11 and 12. At years 7 and 8, women are more likely than men to Hint at their students.

11

Discussion

The broad pattern of results indicates that teachers sampled from China appear more inclusive and supportive of students voices when it comes to classroom discipline, and are less authoritarian (punitive and aggressive) than those in Israel or

Australia. In contrast, the Australian classrooms are perceived as having least discussion and recognition and most punishment. Israeli teachers are situated between these two positions, even though they exhibit the most aggression.

(Inspection of relevant means shows that their aggression is primarily in the form of yelling in anger at their students).

Cultural factors may be posited as part of the reason for these patterns. In China, teachers are held in very high esteem (Li et al, 1998) and as argued by Jin and

Cortazzi (1998), C hinese students would “follow what teachers say out of respect”.

In a recent study for example, which compared Chinese and American students’ perceptions of the aims of schooling, Lau et al note that the former were more likely to report that learning to respect authority was a significant outcome of education.

Consequently, even though Chinese teachers report as much “misbehavior” in class as do Australian and Israeli teachers, the nature of the misbehavior in Chinese classrooms may be less extreme. These assumptions are supported by the work of

Aldridge, Fraser and Huang (1999) in Taiwan. After visiting classrooms in both countries they report

Classroom observations and interviews suggest that there could be differences in the ways in which students regard their teachers; students in Taiwan had more respect for teachers than did students in Australia.

Although the teachers in both countries complained about discipline problems with students, we noted that there was more evidence of disruptive behavior in science classes in Australia (described in the first story as answering back and chatting between friends) than in Taiwan. (p.58)

An assumption of less provocative forms of misbehavior in Chinese classrooms could explain the lesser likelihood of the more stringent forms of disciplinary interventions such as Punishment and Aggression. The Chinese teachers’ greater usage of hinting, discussion and inclusion may reflect confidence on their part that the children will listen to them and to their peers, as argued by Jin and Cortazzi

(1998). Chinese teachers can also rely on parents to support them in their attempts to make students self-disciplined (Peng, 1993; Goa, 1998). With regard to the reported greater use of Aggression and Punishment by women teachers in China (in higher year levels), and greater involvement of older students by male teachers, it may be that more traditional respect is accorded to males within the Chinese culture, particularly by older students . Consequently male teachers are less likely than females to need to resort to coercive strategies to reorient misbehaving students.

This explanation could also account for why women teachers in China are also less likely to try and involve older students in determining rules, and consequences for misbehavior. An alternative explanation may be related to the reported greater stress levels experienced by female teachers in China (Dong, 2001: Zhou et al, 1998).

Such stress is likely to be more pronounced in higher year levels because of the college entrance examination, and the low entrance rate of students to universities.

12

It may be argued that the Australian and Israeli teachers’ relative unwillingness to empower students in the decision-making surrounding classroom discipline is related to the lower levels of unconditional respect they are likely to receive from students, and the reduced levels of support parents provide. The fact that the Australian teachers report less use of discussion and more punishment than do those in Israel may signify that th e former have relatively less “Legitimate” power, and need to rely on more “Coercive” power (Tauber, 1999) to manage their classrooms. The greater likelihood for women teachers of years 8 and 9 in Australia and Israel to be inclusive could signify a lesser concern about their legitimate power, although as reported, in

Israel men teachers were more inclusive for years 7 and 8.

This explanation based on power may also account for the greater inclusion of younger students in all three countries, (although the effect is most pronounced in

Israel). That is, students in year 7 and 8 are more likely to accord teachers unconditional respect than are those in later years.

The results for teachers’ usage of recognition and reward for good behavior is difficult to interpret in a way consistent with the above analysis. One might have assumed that teachers who have less legitimate power may try to use more

“Reward” power (Tauber, 1999) to compensate. The data however show greatest use of recognition and reward by the Chinese teachers, followed by those in Israel, with the Australian sample of teachers using least. These finding suggest an additional explanation for some of the findings that is consistent with the reported associations between students’ self-reported misbehavior and teacher aggression.

As stated earlier, in all three settings, students more prone to misbehavior report greater levels of aggressive teacher disciplinary behavior. It may be assumed that the more provocative students stimulate more anger in teachers and consequently more aggression. Angry or upset teachers, as argued by Glasser (1997), may not be interested in being reasonable towards unreasonable and disrespectful students.

They therefore will find it unpalatable to recognize very difficult students when they act appropriately. Rewarding ‘Neanderthals’ for being normal may not come naturally. Similarly they may find it unpleasant and unproductive to spend time letting such students tell their side of events, trying to get them to acknowledge that their behavior is unfair and needs to change.

Having argued why it is understandable for teachers to react to provocative and possibly confronting student behavior by becoming more aggressive, this does not excuse such a response. It is recognised that

“children who have significant emotional and behavioral problems respond less positively to others and this elicits fewer positive responses and more negative responses from others” (Pace et al, 1999: 151)

However teachers are professionals who need to respond in the best interests of their clients, the students. As argued by Roeser et al (2000) “Teachers need to protect adolescents from situations they perceive as threatening to their self …or threatening to their social image” (p.454). If not, then “adolescents will feel less motivated to learn and more unhappy and will be more likely to manifest academic or social problems”(p.454). In summary, teachers can not allow themselves and difficult

13

students to be locked into “a vicious cycle of reciprocal causation” (Pace et al,

1999:151).

In making a recommendation that teachers need to work harder to foster quality relationships with difficult students, we are aware how difficult that may be. However a clear starting point would be to minimize the usage of aggressive disciplinary strategies while increasing the frequency with which they recognize responsible behavior, however rare it may be. Secondly they need to make the opportunity let students talk about their side of things so that it can be clearly understood, to get them to understand why their behavior is a problem for others and to obtain a plan for a better future. In many schools in Australia, Israel and China, this conversation is conducted by someone other than the classroom teacher, as a resu lt of a ‘referral’.

It is the year level coordinator (class teacher), school counselor or a senior teacher who gets the advantage of the relationship power that arises from such conversations (Tauber, 1999), whereas it is the classroom teacher who needs it.

Encouraging teachers to build rather than destroy good-will with students who are more provocative is a challenging request. It will not be easy. In the experience of one of the authors who is working in schools to achieve this aim it can take many years of persistent effort accompanied by considerable support (Lewis, In press). If teachers are to act in the best interests of students in an area as emotive as classroom discipline, then

"creating professional work environments where teachers feel supported by other professionals and school leaders in relation to their own needs for competence, autonomy, and quality relationships is essential”(Roeser et al,

2000:466)

References

14

Aldridge, J. M., Fraser, B. J., & Huang, Tai-Chu. I. (1999). Investigating Classroom

Environments in Taiwan and Australia with Multiple research Methods. The Journal of Educational Research, 93 (1), 48-61.

.

Barton, P. E., Coley, R. J., & Wenglinsky, H . (1998). Order in the classroom:

Violence, discipline and student achievement.

Princeton, NJ: Policy Information

Center. Educational testing service.

Blase, J. J. (1986). A qualitative analysis of sources of teacher stress:

Consequences for performance . American Educational Research Journal , 23 , 23-

40.

Borg, M. G., Riding, R. J., & Falzon, J. M. (1991). Stress in Teaching: A study of

Occupational stress and its determinants, job satisfaction and career commitment among primary schoolteachers. Educational Psychology, 11 (1), 59-75.

Canter, L., & Canter, M. (1992). Assertive discipline: A take charge approach for today's educator.

California. Canter and Associates.

Chan, D. W. (1998). Stress, Coping strategies, and Psychological distress among

Secondary Teachers in Hong Kong. American Educational Research Journal,

35 (1),145-163.

Charles, C. (1992). Building Classroom Discipline: From models to practise (

Ed ) New York. Longman.

Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). Motivation and

Education: The Self-Determination Perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26 (3

& 4), 325-346.

DeRobbio, R. A., & Iwanicki, E. F. (1996).

Factors accounting for burnout among secondary school teachers .

Paper presented at the Annual conference of the

American Educational Research Association. New York.

.Dong, H (2001). A Psychological Investigation of teachers' over

Strees and Anxiety. Journal of Urumqi Adult Education College, 2, 55-58.

Freiberg, H. J. (1996). From Tourists to Citizens in the Classroom. Educational

Leadership, 54 (1), 32-36.

Friedman, I. A. (1994). Conceptualising and measuring teacher-perceived student behaviors: Disrespect, sociability and attentiveness . Educational and

Psychological Measurement , 54 (4), 949-958.

Friedman I. A. (1995). Student behavior patterns contributing to teacher burnout

The Journal of Educational Research, 88 (5), 281-289.

Frydenberg, E., & Lewis, R.. (2000). The Coping Scale for Adults: Construct validity and what the instrument tells us. ERIC TM031533 ED443 888. :12

Glasser, W. (1969). School without failure . New York. Harper and Row.

15

Glasser, W. (1997). A New Look at School Failure and School Success. Phi Delta

Kappan , April, 597-602.

Gao. L. (1998). Cultural context of school science teaching and learning in the

People’s Republic of China.

Science Education, 82, 1-13 .

Goddard, J. T. (2000). Teaching in Turbulent times: Teachers’ perceptions of the effects of external factors on their professional lives. Alberta Journal of

Educational Research, 46(4), 293-310.

Gordon, T. (1974). Teacher effectiveness training.

New York. P.H. Wyden.

Guttmann, J. (1982). Pupils’, teachers’ and parents’ causal attributions for problem behavior at school. Journal of Educational Research, 76 (1), 14-21.

Hart P. M., Wearing, A. J., & Conn, M. (1995). Conventional wisdom is a poor predictor of the relationship between discipline policy, student misbehavior and teacher stress. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65 (1), 27-48.

Hyman, I. A., & Snook, P. A. (2000).

Phi Delta Kappan. 81 (7),489-501 .

Ingersoll, R. M. (1996). Teachers' decision-making power and school conflict

Sociology of Education, 68 (2), April, 159-176.

Ingersoll, R. M. (2001).Teacher turnover and teacher shortages: An organizational analysis. American Educational Research Journal, 38 (3), 499-534.

Jin, L., & Cortazzi, M. (1998). Dimensions of dialogue: Large classes in China.

International Journal of Educational Research, 29 , 739-761 .

Kaplan, A., Gheen, M., & Midgley, C. (2002) Classroom goal structure and student disruptive behavior. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72 (2), 191-212.

Kohn, A. (1996) Beyond Discipline: From Compliance to Community .

Alexandria, VA. Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Kyriacou, C. (1980). Stress, health, and school-teachers:A comparison with other professions . Cambridge Journal of Education, 10 , 154-59.

Lau, S., Nicholls, J. G., Thorkilsen, T. A., & Patashnick, M. (2000). Chinese and

American adolescents’ perceptions of the purposes of education and beliefs about the world of work . Social Behavior and personality, 28 (1), 73-89.

Lewis, R., and Lovegrove, M.N. (1987). What students think of teacher's classroom control techniques: Results from four studies. In Hastings, J., Schwieso, J., eds ,

16

New Directions in Educational Psychology, Vol. 2: Behaviour and Motivation.

The Falmer Press.pp.93-113.

Lewis, R. (1997a). Discipline in Schools. In L. J. Saha (Ed.) International

Encyclopedia of the Sociology in Education, 404-411. Oxford, UK.

Permagon.

Lewis R. (1997b). The discipline dilemma , (2nd ed). Melbourne. The Australian

Council for Educational Research.

Lewis, R. (1999). Teachers coping with the stress of classroom discipline . Social

Psychology of Education, 3 , 1-17.

Lewis, R. (2001) Classroom Discipline and Student Res ponsibility: The Students’ view. Teaching and Teacher Education, 17 (3), 307-319.

Li, X. Y., Xie, J. S., & Wang, Z. H.(1998 ). Reform and Development of Chinese

Higher Teacher Education . Jiangxi, China: Jiangxi Higher Education Publishing

House.

Lowell, C., & Gallup, A. M. (2002). The 34 th Annual Phi Delta Kappa/Gallup poll of the Public’s attitude towards the Public Schools. Phi Delta Kappan, 84 (1),41-56.

Martin, S. C. (1994). A preliminary evaluation of the adoption and implementation of assertive discipline at Robinton High School. School Organisation 14 (3), 321-330.

McCormick, J., & Shi, G. (1999). Teachers’ attributions for responsibility for their occupational stress in the People’s Republic of China and Australia. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 393-407.

Miller, A., Ferguson, E., & Byrne, I. (2000). Pupils’ causal attributions for difficult classroom behavior. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 85-96.

Mitchel, K. (2001). Education for Democratic Citizenship: Transnationalism,

Multiculturalism, and the limits of Liberalism. Harvard Educational Review, 71 (1),

51-78.

Oswald, U. (1995). difficult to manage students: a survey of children who fail to respond to student discipline strategies in government schools . Educational

Studies, 21 , 265-276.

Pace, T. M., Mullins, L. L., Beesely, D., Hill, J. S. & Carson, K. (1999). The relationship between children’s emotional and behavioral problems and the social responses of elementary school teachers. Contemporary Educational

Psychology, 24, 140-155.

Pearl A., & Knight. A. (1998) Democratic Schooling: Theory to Guide

Educational Practice . N.J.

Hampton Press.

17

Peng, S. S. (1993). Fostering student discipline and effort: Approaches used in

Chinese schools. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American

Educational Research Association (April 12-16, 1993).

Roeser , R. W., Eccles, J. S. & Sameroff, A. J.(2000). School as a Context of early

Adolescents’ aademic and Social-Emotional Development: A Summary of the

Research Findings . The Elementary School Teacher, 11 (5), 443-471.

Romi, S., & Freund, M. (1999). Teachers’, Students’ and Parents’ Attitudes towards

Disruptive Behaviour Problems in High School; A case study. Educational

Psychology, 19 (1), 53-70.

Romi, S., & Katz, Y. (2003). Affective Education: The Nature and Characteristics of Teachers’ and Students’ Attitudes Toward School in Israel

. Unpublished paper, School of Education, Bar Ilan University. Israel.

Rothstein, R. (20000. Towards a composite index of school performance. The

Elementary School Teacher, 100 (5), 409-441.

Schneider, E. (1996). Giving students a voice in the classroom. Educational

Leadership, 54 (1), 22-26.

Swinson, J. , & Melling, R. (1995). Assertive discipline: Four wheels on this wagon: A reply to Robinson and Maines.

Educational Psychology in Practice, 11 (3), 3-8.

Tauber, R. T. (1999). Classroom Management: Sound Theory and Effective practice.

Penn. Bergin & Garvey.

Wolfgang, C. H. (1995).

Solving Discipline Problems: Strategies for classroom teachers.

(3 rd Ed).). Massachusetts:,Allyn and Bacon.

Whiteman, J. L., Young, J. C., & Fisher, Lynette. (1985). Teacher burnout and the perception of student behavior.

Education, 105 , 299-305.

Zhou, Z., & Zhao, D.(1998). A Study on the Symptoms of Anxiety and Related

Factors of Primary and Secondary School Teachers . Journal of China Civilian

Medicine, 10( 5), 305-306.

18

Teachers’ classroom discipline and Student Misbehavior in Australia, China and Israel

Table 1. Study sample

Country

Teachers

Australia 491

Respondents year 7-8

1713

Students year 9-10

1624 year 11-12 Total

846 4183

Israel

China

98

159

261

159

334

147

241

196

836

502

Teachers’ classroom discipline and Student Misbehavior in Australia, China and Israel

Table 2. Level of Perceived classroom misbehavior by setting

Level of perceived misbehavior Country

Australia

Nearly all

1

Most Some Hardly any None

2 33 54 10

Israel 1 2 54 41 2

19

China 0 2 42 50 6

Teachers’ classroom discipline and Student Misbehavior in Australia, China and Israel

Table 3. Level of concern by Setting

Country Level of Concern

Australia

Major Moderate Minor Almost None None

12

90

27

10

25

0

24

0

12

0 Israel

China 11 26 30 25 8

Teachers’ classroom discipline and Student Misbehavior in Australia, China and Israel

20

Table 4. Discipline strategy usage by country

Scale China

Country

Israel Australia

X(Ave X) SD Alpha X(Ave X) SD Alpha X(Ave X) SD Alpha

Punishment 11.98 (3.00) 3.94 .62 13.02 (3.26) 4.28 .75 14.54 (3.64) 4.75 .79

(n=4)

Discussion 17.45 (4.35) 4.30 .72 13.45 (3.36) 4.77 .77 13.10 (3.28) 4.46 .73

(n=4)

Recognition 16.32 (4.08) 4.36 .66 13.16 (3.29) 4.92 .79 12.51 (3.13) 5.32 .83

(n=4)

Aggression 9.22 (2.30) 4.03 .69 11.65 (2.92) 4.22 .62 11.02 (2.76) 4.88 .73

(n=4)

Involvement 11.80 (3.93) 3.37 .49 8.49 (2..83) 3.11 .48 8.49 (2.83) 3.14 .53

(n=3)

Hinting 16.04 (4.01) 3.91 .58 14.62 (3.65) 4.03 .62 14.48 (3.62) 4.04 .66

(n=4)

Teachers’ classroom discipline and Student Misbehavior in Australia, China and Israel

Table 5. Significant predictors of Classroom Discipline strategies.

Independent variable

Year level

Country

Misbehavior

F value Hyp df Error df Significance level

2.28

30.10

2.92

12 9022

12 9022

18 13536

24 18052

.007

.000

.000

.000 Country X Year level

Country X Teacher Sex

3.25

2.23

Country X Year X Misb’r 1.49

12 9022

66 27090

.000

.006

Country X Tchr sex X Year 3.65 24 18052 .000

21

Download