Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 DOI 10.1007/s00426-012-0442-z ORIGINAL ARTICLE Conditional automaticity in subliminal morphosyntactic priming Ulrich Ansorge • Bert Reynvoet • Jessica Hendler Lennart Oettl • Stefan Evert • Received: 30 September 2011 / Accepted: 24 May 2012 / Published online: 12 June 2012 Ó Springer-Verlag 2012 Abstract We used a gender-classification task to test the principles of subliminal morphosyntactic priming. In Experiment 1, masked, subliminal feminine or masculine articles were used as primes. They preceded a visible target noun. Subliminal articles either had a morphosyntactically congruent or incongruent gender with the targets. In a gender-classification task of the target nouns, subliminal articles primed the responses: responses were faster in congruent than incongruent conditions (Experiment 1). In Experiment 2, we tested whether this congruence effect depended on gender relevance. In line with a relevancedependence, the congruence effect only occurred in a gender-classification task but was absent in another categorical discrimination of the target nouns (Experiment 2). The congruence effect also depended on correct word order. It was diminished when nouns preceded articles (Experiment 3). Finally, the congruence effect was replicated with a larger set of targets but only for masculine targets (Experiment 4). Results are discussed in light of U. Ansorge (&) Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Liebiggasse 5, 1010 Vienna, Austria e-mail: ulrich.ansorge@univie.ac.at U. Ansorge J. Hendler L. Oettl Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabrueck, Osnabrueck, Germany B. Reynvoet Department of Psychology, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium S. Evert Institute of Comparative Linguistics and Literature Studies, Technical University Darmstadt, Darmstadt, Germany theories of subliminal priming in general and of subliminal syntactic priming in particular. Introduction While hearing or reading language, syntactic and semantic processes interactively determine comprehension: preceding words determine what to expect next and how to comprehend a word. In many languages, such as German, several words are syntactically polyvalent: they can take on different syntactical functions, depending on how they are used in the context of a sentence. In the German language, for example, if presented before nouns, the definite article for a nominative singular masculine noun, ‘der’ (as in ‘der Apfel ist rot’ meaning ‘the apple is red’), is syntactically polyvalent. The reason is that ‘der’ can be alternatively used as a genitive pronoun for masculine and feminine singular and plural nouns (as in ‘abseits der Straßen’ meaning ‘off roads’). Likewise, if presented before a noun, the word ‘die’ can be used as a definite article for the nominative case of a singular feminine noun (as in ‘die Blume ist rot’ meaning ‘the flower is red’). However, alternatively, ‘die’ could be used as the definite article for the nominative case of the plural feminine and masculine nouns (as in ‘die A¨pfel sind rot’ meaning ‘the apples are red’). In addition, if presented after the nouns, the same words can again change their syntactic function. However, after the nouns, the articles ‘die’ and ‘der’ could never serve as singular nominative determiners for their preceding nouns. As a consequence of grammatical polyvalence and the function of word order for the specification of grammatical function, the exact word sequence accounts for the syntactic function of a determiner, and the determiner in turn suggests a 123 400 particular expectancy for (and interpretation of) a subsequent word. In line with the latter, after a definite feminine singular article ‘die’, the reader expects to find a feminine singular noun (or a plural noun). Violation of this expectation delays the processing of the noun (Friedrici & Jacobsen, 1999). Theoretically, such a delay might reflect syntactic or semantic processing. However, electrophysiological correlates suggested that it is a syntactic violation (Gunter, Friederici, & Schriefers, 2000; Haagort & Brown, 1999). To be precise, during gender priming with determiners, the violation is morphosyntactic because the violation is based on a non-fitting inflection of the determiner. [Otherwise these conditions are syntactically appropriate (i.e., word order is correct with the determiner preceding the noun).] Current theoretical controversy concerns the degree of automaticity of such morphosyntactic and syntactic processes, with some researchers arguing for automatic processing of syntactic (e.g., Flores d’Arcais, 1988; Forster, 1979) and morphosyntactic (e.g., Hasting & Kotz, 2008; Koester, Gunter, & Wagner, 2007) structure while others argue against this position (cf. Ayora, Janssen, Dell’Acqua, & Alario, 2009; Dent, Johnston, & Humphreys, 2008; Ferreira & Pashler, 2002; Fayol, Largie, & Lemiere, 1994). One line of evidence that suggested that syntactical processing is indeed an automatic process is its awarenessindependence. One characteristic of automatic processes is their independence of awareness (Posner & Snyder, 1975), and a number of studies suggested that this could also be true of syntactic processing both, in patients (cf. Ferreira, Bock, Wilson, & Cohen, 2008) and in healthy participants (Sereno, 1991). Consider the study by Sereno (1991): she presented three visual words in a temporal sequence. The first and the last words were clearly seen by her participants. However, she assumed that forward and backward masking rendered the second brief priming word (or prime) subliminal—that is, the priming word seemed to have been presented below the level of aware perception. In this context, masking denotes the suppression of a brief visual stimulus (here: a word) by preceding and subsequent visual stimuli at the same location (Forster, 1998). Sereno used both syntactically congruent and incongruent masked primes and compared the performance between these conditions. In the congruent condition, the masked prime word syntactically fitted the word order of the sentence. For instance, a possessive determiner as a prime word (e.g., ‘your’) completed a sequence consisting of a starting verb ‘hear’ (which also served as a forward mask for the determiner, decreasing the prime’s visibility) and a closing noun ‘world’ (working as a backward mask for the prime) to form a syntactically appropriate sentence like ‘hear your world’. In the syntactically incongruent condition, by contrast, the prime 123 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 word did not fit the typical word order because the positions of verb and noun were reverted (e.g., ‘chain your think’).1 In line with automatic syntactic processing, Sereno found a congruence effect: a lexical decision about the target’s status as a word or non-word was faster in syntactically congruent than incongruent conditions. Strong versus weak automaticity So far, however, it is unclear exactly what kind of automaticity of word processing is reflected in subliminal syntactic priming. Subliminal syntactic priming could be strongly automatic or it could be weakly automatic (Kahneman & Treisman, 1984). Strongly automatic effects are triggered by the stimulus and are neither subject to intentions nor conditional upon intentions or task sets. A strongly automatic processes runs off in an involuntary fashion, once a stimulus is presented that triggers its fitting processes. By contrast, weakly automatic effects can be at least intentionally suppressed. Also, some forms of weak automaticity might even require a fitting intention on the side of the participant in the first place. This latter conception has been denoted conditional automaticity (Bargh, 1992). To start with the strongly automatic conceptions, one strongly automatic way of how subliminal syntactic priming could be brought about is by spreading activation within the mental lexicon (Kiefer, 2002; Marcel, 1983). The mental lexicon is the memory for words and their meanings. In the mental lexicon, each word’s meaning could be represented as a node in a network of interconnected word nodes (Collins & Loftus, 1975; Neely, 1977). In this network, the spatial proximity between nodes and/or the strength of the interconnections is assumed to be proportional to the semantic relatedness of the words. In such a network, a subliminal word could activate its corresponding node as well as other nodes with semantically associated meaning via the spreading of activation along the interconnections and proportional to them (Plaut & Booth, 2000). To account for subliminal syntactic priming, we would only need to assume that reciprocal node activation is also proportional to the syntactic fit between two consecutive words. For instance, in the mental lexicon words could be represented along with a list of syntactically defined roles that they could take on in a syntactically 1 It could be argued that even this syntactically incongruent sentence was in fact syntactically congruent. However, these are original example sequences from the study of Sereno. We preferred to take this example because at least with these two sequences, the same prime word ‘your’ was used in congruent and incongruent conditions. This was not the case in many other instances of Sereno’s study in which different prime words were used in congruent than incongruent conditions. Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 valid sentence (Bock, Loebell, & Morey, 1992). If these roles also determine how closely connected two words of different word forms (e.g., a noun and the article it could take) are in the mental lexicon, it is conceivable that syntactic priming could be brought about by strongly automatic spreading activation triggered by subliminal words. Related, strongly automatic syntactic priming could also rely on the mental lexicon’s representations of asymmetric syntagmatic word–word relations (cf. Kjellmer, 1991). Some word pairs are temporally (in visual and auditory verbal communication) or ‘left–right’ (in Western visual scripture) asymmetric syntagmatic word–word relations because one of two words (word x) more frequently precedes a second word (word y) than the other way round. This is suggested by the collocation of words in text corpora (cf. Michelbacher, Evert, & Schütze, 2007). As an example, take the collocation of the two words ‘Christmas decorations’. This is an asymmetrical syntagmatic collocation: the word ‘Christmas’ is predictive of the subsequent ‘decorations’. Yet, the word ‘decorations’ is not predictive of the preceding word ‘Christmas’. If asymmetric syntagmatic word–word representations determine the length and strength of the connections between word representations in the mental lexicon one word prime that is presented to the participants and that has been frequently experienced to precede one word target because the standard syntactical order would be prime word before target word, the prime could activate the representation of the word target in the mental lexicon in a strongly automatic way, too. However, a wealth of theories acknowledges the role of intentions and top-down control in subliminal processing. Some of these theories posit that subliminal processing could be one source of cognitive control itself, thereby, essentially denying the automatic nature of subliminal processing altogether. A second theoretical position in this camp would argue that subliminal processing is weakly automatic. To start with the first of the two positions, according to an elaborate-processing view humans process subliminal words in a manner very similar to clearly visible words (cf. Dehaene et al., 1998; Naccache & Dehaene, 2001; Reynvoet, Gevers, & Caessens, 2005). In fact, defenders of an elaborate-processing view left open whether or not subliminal stimuli would be able to elicit a new intention or fitting willed behaviour in and by themselves (cf. Van den Bussche & Reynvoet, 2007). Related to this, some theories acknowledged top-down mediated influences of context on subliminal processing (cf. Kiefer & Martens, 2010; Martens, Ansorge, & Kiefer, 2011; van Opstal, Reynvoet, & Verguts, 2005). According to an opposing view, subliminal processing in general and masked priming in particular would be weakly automatic and a conditionally automatic process (Bargh, 1992). This means that masked priming cannot elicit a 401 fitting intention itself but, on the contrary, critically depends on the prior setup of a top-down or goal-directed intention before the processing of subliminal stimuli can take place. Only once such an intention, goal, or task set has been firmly established, a stimulus of which a person remains unaware would be able to elicit a stimulus processing as this would be implied by the current task settings (e.g., Ansorge & Neumann, 2005; Kunde, Reuss, & Kiesel, 2012). So far, top-down control over masked priming has been demonstrated (or claimed) for motor activation (cf. Kunde, Kiesel, & Hoffmann, 2003), for the direction of attention (cf. Ansorge, Kiss, & Eimer, 2009), and for the selection of task sets (Mattler, 2006). Mattler, for example, demonstrated that once a participant had set up a task set and knew that a visual diamond indicated that s/he had to perform an auditory task, a masked diamond was able to trigger an expectation or task set for an auditory task, although the participant was unaware of the diamond. Evidently, this sort of priming effect of the diamond could only be brought about by the top-down task settings that the participants intentionally set up in the laboratory to comply with Mattler’s task. There is no connection between diamonds and auditory tasks in the pre-experimental experience of the participants that could equally explain why a masked diamond could trigger an auditory task-set preparation. Theoretically, due to its top-down dependence, conditional automaticity provides a very plausible framework for masked syntactic priming. On a theoretical level, conditional automaticity in the form of constraints imposed by top-down intentions is akin to the constraining influence of sequentially preceding words during language comprehension that disambiguates the potential syntactical role of a syntactically polyvalent word. Thus, during natural language processing, preceding words would gate or determine the interpretation of a subsequent word. As an example, consider the syntactical role of the German article ‘der’. If in German the preceding expression ‘am ersten Sonntag nach’ (Engl. ‘on the first Sunday after’) would be presented as directly preceding the article ‘der’, this determiner ‘der’ would almost certainly be interpreted as a dative case of this determiner (as in ‘am ersten Sonntag nach der Karwoche’, Engl. ‘the first Sunday after the Holy Week’). On theoretical arguments alone, one could therefore expect syntactic and morphosyntactic processing to be weakly automatic—here, conditionally automatic. By contrast, it is difficult, if not impossible, to conceive of syntactic role-disambiguation as being brought about by the syntactically polyvalent word itself and without a scaffolding context, because if a word is syntactically ambiguous in the first place, there is simply no way for this word to be the sole origin of only its actually pertaining syntactic role. To summarize the views: according to the conditionalautomaticity view, awareness-independent automatic 123 402 morphosyntactic and syntactic processing of a word should depend on two factors, (1) general task sets that make a particular grammatical function of a word’s form relevant in the first place, and (2) contextual word-order information that constrains the participants’ expectancies about the syntactical role of an upcoming syntactically polyvalent word. Thus, the conditional automaticity declares that subliminal syntactic processing is weakly automatic and predicts strong task-set and word-order dependencies of subliminal syntactic priming. An elaborate-processing view would also be in line with these predicted task-set influences on subliminal syntactic priming. However, the elaborated-processing view is less parsimonious than the conditional-automaticity perspective. Related, the major addition of the elaborate-processing explanation as compared to the conditional-automaticity view concerns an assumed potential of the stimulus itself— here, of the word—to elicit its fitting intention or top-down task set (cf. Naccache & Dehaene, 2001; van Opstal, Reynvoet, & Verguts, 2005). However, as we have just explained above, such bottom-up priming of one particular syntactic role by which a word could impact on its own processing seems especially unsuited to disambiguate this word’s syntactical status alone. Therefore, in the following we will mostly refer to the more parsimonious conditional-automaticity view when we motivate our hypotheses although similar predictions could be derived from the more encompassing elaborate-processing view, too. Finally, on a strongly automatic view, by contrast, masked words should be able to elicit their subliminal syntactic congruence effect in a stimulus-driven way, regardless of the pertaining task sets and regardless of a favourable or less favourable word order. Overview of the current experiments The aim of the present study was to decide whether a strongly or a weakly automatic form of processing accounts for subliminal syntactic processing. In light of its theoretical plausibility, we expected that the conditional-automaticity view of subliminal syntactic priming is correct. To test our assumption, here, we investigated morphosyntactic processing with subliminal (masked) priming words presented with a short interval before visible target words. In Experiment 1, we tested whether German determiners as subliminal primes are morphosyntactically processed if the determiners singular gender deflection is task-relevant for our German-speaking participants. As explained, in the German language, determiners are gender-specific. To test whether the subliminal determiners’ gender was processed if gender was task-relevant, determiners were either gender-congruent or genderincongruent to the visible target nouns and the participants 123 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 had to discriminate between the masculine and feminine gender of the visible target nouns. In this situation, on the basis of weak and strong automaticity, we expected that the subliminal determiners’ gender-specific deflection is morphosyntactically processed and leads to a congruence effect, even if the participants are not aware of the determiners. In Experiment 2, we tested whether the masked determiners’ congruence effect indeed depended on the task relevance of the morphosyntactic gender of the determiners. This is a prediction derived from the conditional-automaticity view. We used the same words as primes and targets as in Experiment 1, but now half of the participants had to discriminate the targets regardless of their gender, and by a different type of category membership. In this situation, the gender-specific deflection of the subliminal determiners should be irrelevant for the target-discrimination performance. Therefore, on the basis of the conditional-automaticity view, the subliminal determiners should be without effect on target processing. By contrast, on the basis of a strong-automaticity view of morphosyntactic priming, the masked words should facilitate processing of a morphosyntactically congruent noun in a stimulus-driven way, so that the gender-fitting determiner should facilitate the target classification in comparison to the less fitting determiner, regardless of task (i.e., in noun-gender classification and gender-unrelated noun classification). In Experiment 3, we tested whether a disambiguating word context (here: a correct word order) indeed increased the probability of an automatic morphosyntactic congruence effect of the subliminal determiners. This was assumed under the conditional-automaticity perspective but not under the elaborated-processing view and not under a strong-automaticity view. In Experiment 3, we tested this hypothesis by studying whether the morphosyntactic congruence effect of the subliminal determiners also depended on a syntactically fitting sequence of words. In German, the definite nominative determiner that specifies the gender of a singular noun has to precede this noun. Therefore, if it is true that a syntactically correct word order (with the determiner preceding the noun) in Experiments 1 and 2 is critical for an awareness-independent morphosyntactical priming effect of the subliminal determiners, the congruence effect of the same words should be absent if the sequence of words is reversed and nouns are presented before the determiners. This hypothesis was tested in a comparison of the congruence effects between Experiment 3 and the gender-relevant congruence effects in Experiments 1 and 2. Finally, as we used only a restricted set of target nouns in Experiments 1, 2, and 3, a more representative number of target nouns was used in the final Experiment 4. In Experiment 4, we aimed to replicate the subliminal morphosyntactic congruence effect. In Experiment 4, we also Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 included a control condition to test the elaborated-processing view with respect to a semantic dimension of the words (or a category membership of the words) that was independent of the words’ gender and their morphosyntactic status. Experiment 1 We tested masked morphosyntactical priming in two-word sequences, with a masked and thus subliminal determiner as a prime word before a visible noun as a target word. Note that a pair of words consisting of one determiner and one noun is a very elliptic sentence at best. Therefore, it is unlikely that the determiner (or the determiner plus noun) was sufficient to specify the gender-specific interpretation of the syntactically polyvalent determiners in and by themselves. This is just one theoretical example, why the conditional-automaticity view of syntactic and morphosyntactic processing is very likely. However, because we used a gender-discrimination task for the visible targets, the gender-specific deflection of the prime words became task relevant. In every trial of Experiment 1, our German-speaking participants were presented with a clearly visible German singular noun as a target. The participants had to discriminate whether the noun was masculine or feminine. This gender discrimination ensured the task relevance and a particular interpretation of the morphosyntactically gender-specific status of the determiners. As masked primes, we used the German singular definite masculine (‘der’) and feminine (‘die’) articles. In congruent conditions, the prime’s morphosyntactic singular gender fitted that of the subsequent target noun (e.g., both were masculine). For example, the masculine German article ‘der’ was shown as a masked prime that preceded the visible masculine target noun ‘Teller’ (Engl. ‘plate’). In incongruent conditions, the prime-word gender did not fit that of the target. For instance, the masculine article ‘der’ preceded the feminine target noun ‘Tasse’ (Engl. ‘cup’). If the determiners are automatically processed and participants had access to the morphosyntactical status of the subliminal determiners, we expected a congruence effect: faster responses in congruent than in incongruent conditions. This expected congruence effect could be due to different mechanisms. Only, some of these mechanisms would be forms of weak automaticity. This is true of the following two mechanisms. First, the gender-specific morphosyntactical status of the subliminal determiners could bias the decision in the gender-classification task (Norris & Kinoshita, 2008). Secondly, the gender-specific morphosyntactical status of the subliminal determiners could prime the processing of a gender-related target noun but less so of an unrelated target gender noun (e.g., as by spreading activation). 403 However, a third explanation of the expected priming effect is possible and would be based on a strongly automatic stimulus-driven mechanism. Such a strongly automatic process would not be conditional on a fitting task set and hence not be conditionally automatic: participants’ preexperimental experience with the different frequencies of co-occurrences of different determiners and subsequent fitting versus non-fitting gender nouns in written language could have led to representations of associations between specific gender-deflected determiners and gender-fitting nouns in the participants’ mental lexicon. If the different word concepts are connected in the mental lexicon according to the relatedness (or an association) between concepts, encountering a masked determiner could activate representations of gender-fitting nouns in mental lexicon in a strongly automatic fashion. In a first step, the existence of the suspect co-occurrences between specific gender-deflected determiners and subsequent gender-fitting nouns was confirmed by comparison of the two frequencies of each determiner (‘der’ vs. ‘die’) for its respective preceding of (1) a singular masculine and (2) a singular feminine noun in a syntactically annotated German text corpus, the tiger treebank: this corpus contains about 750,000 words of written running text, mostly from the early 1990s Frankfurter Rundschau issues, a German newspaper (Brants et al., 2002). Our comparison confirmed that a co-occurrence existed between the feminine determiner ‘die’ and a subsequent feminine singular noun (99.97 %). This frequency compared to that of ‘die’ preceding a masculine singular noun (0.01 %). The analogue reverse relation holds true of the masculine determiner ‘der’: it frequently precedes a masculine singular noun (99.92 %) but less frequently a feminine noun (0.04 %). In a second step, the possibility that associations based on such empirically observed collocations accounted for a strongly automatic stimulus-driven priming effect will be addressed in Experiment 2. Selection of target nouns To increase the likelihood that the expected congruence effect in the current experiment depended on a prime’s morphosyntactical gender status, and not on a feature overlap between prime and target, we carefully selected the target nouns. German nouns ending with ‘-er’ (e.g., ‘Teller’, Engl. ‘plate’) are frequently masculine (masculine 77.3 %; feminine 4.82 %; neuter 17.23 %), whereas German nouns ending with ‘-e’ (e.g., ‘Tasse’, Engl. ‘cup’) are frequently feminine (feminine 52.93 %; masculine 24.97 %; neuter 18.54 %). Crucially, the word endings of the masculine and feminine determiners ‘der’ and ‘die’, 123 404 thus, are featurally overlapping with the prototypical target endings of their gender-fitting nouns. In our task, we also used nouns with such gender-prototypical endings but to prevent that our observers solely relied on the gender-diagnostic noun endings for their gender decision and the consequential feature priming by common word endings of congruent primes and targets, we used less diagnostic (or ‘not-prototypical’) as well as diagnostic (or ‘prototypical’) masculine and feminine noun endings. Thus, not all of our used nouns’ genders could be discerned on the basis of their endings alone: the feminine target nouns ‘Gabel’ (Engl. ‘fork’) and ‘Hand’ (Engl. ‘hand’) had the same respective endings as the masculine target words ‘Löffel’ (Engl. ‘spoon’) and ‘Mund’ (Engl. ‘mouth’). This choice of targets should have prevented the participants’ reliance on the noun endings and accordingly that the congruence effect was based on featural overlap alone. Also, by using a very restricted sample of only eight German nouns in total, we were able to present each of the nouns repeatedly. This has two advantages. (1) It is easier for the participants to build up a task set for this restricted sample of repeated target nouns, and we have already discussed that conditional automatic morphosyntactic processing is supported if the task sets have been completed (i.e., with words that can well be expected). (2) With many repetitions for each of the target nouns, the influence of pre-experimental experience with the target nouns (e.g., the influence of the noun frequencies or of the determiner-noun collocations in a corpus of written words) becomes relatively weak in comparison to the experimental experience. Related, with many repetitions per target noun, we do not only aim at the control for population-average prior experience with a word (as this could be derived from word norms) but also at the control for individual differences in the experience with a word. The latter can be overcome with much experience within the experiment. For example, Coane and Balota (2010) showed that repetition of lowfrequency words in an experiment decreased lexical decision times for these words. High-frequency words by contrast did not profit from repetition within an experiment, presumably because these words were already discriminated with almost perfect efficiency. It should be noted, however, that the selection of only a few target nouns has the severe drawback of testing masked syntactic priming with only a very limited set of well-practiced target words. As a consequence, it is not certain whether the outcome of Experiment 1 would also generalize to a more natural situation with a larger set of words that were less practiced in the context of the experiment (cf. Damian, 2001). Therefore, we also tested the masked syntactic priming effect with a larger set of less practiced words in a later experiment (Experiment 4). 123 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 Subliminality of the primes We also tested whether the masked primes were truly subliminal. In a second session of Experiment 1, the same participants had to discriminate between different masked primes. Otherwise the conditions were similar to the noundiscrimination task. We expected little if any visibility of the masked primes. Method Participants Fourteen paid volunteers (9 female; mean age 24.1 years) participated. One had to be excluded because he did not turn up for the second session. All had normal or correctedto-normal vision by self-report. Apparatus Stimuli were presented on a 15-inch colour VGA monitor (refresh rate = 59.1 Hz). Viewing distance was 57 cm (ensured by a chin rest). Responses were registered via the numeric keypad of a serial computer keyboard. To start a trial, participants pressed the key #5, with the right index finger. They had to release this home key immediately before their target response. Target responses were given by the keys #7 and #9. Stimuli and procedure See also Fig. 1. The masked primes were the articles ‘die’ (singular nominative feminine) and ‘der’ (singular nominative masculine). The targets were four feminine nouns, two from the category china/cutlery, ‘Gabel’ (Engl. ‘fork’), ‘Tasse’ (Engl. ‘cup’), and two from the category body parts, ‘Nase’ (Engl. ‘nose’) and ‘Hand’ (Engl. ‘hand’), and four masculine nouns, ‘Löffel’ (Engl. ‘spoon’), ‘Teller’ (Engl. ‘plate’), and ‘Mund’ (‘mouth’) and ‘Arm’ (‘arm’).2 Stimuli were black (\1 cd/m2) on white (48 cd/m2). In each trial, after the fixation cross (centred on the screen) for 750 ms, a forward mask consisting of 10 random uppercase letters (each 0.25° long 9 0.39° high) was shown for 200 ms. This was followed by the presentation of the prime 2 The gender-orthogonal category membership of every target word (as belonging either to the body parts or to the cutlery/china category) was used for target discrimination in the subsequent Experiment 2. This was done to create conditions in which gender-specific deflections of the determiners were task-irrelevant and, due to the syntactic polyvalence of the determiners, a gender-specific conditionally automatic priming effect should be absent with the same determiners as primes. Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 405 Mask; 200ms Masked Prime; 30 ms Mask; 30 ms BAEAFGILHT die AFCFDZGLZM Target; until response Gabel Time Fig. 1 Depicted is an example of a congruent trial. The arrow depicts the direction of time. Stimuli are not drawn to scale for 34 ms in lowercase letters, and a backward mask consisting of 10 independently drawn random capital letters for 34 ms. Finally, the target was shown until the response or 5 s had elapsed. All stimuli were centred on the screen and inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs) were 0 ms. Feedback concerned errors and RTs exceeding 1,250 ms. We administered a target-gender classification task and a subsequent prime-visibility task in separate sessions. Up to 2 weeks passed between the sessions. Each task took 30–45 min. During target classification, participants pressed one key for feminine and another for masculine nouns and the masked primes were not mentioned. Before the prime-visibility task, a slow motion was shown because the participants failed to see the prime. This was done to expose the primes for a longer duration and to explain the task to the participants. In the primevisibility task, seven of the participants had to classify the prime’s gender. In this task, the sequence of events within trials and the stimuli (including the identities of primes and targets) were exactly the same as in the target-classification task. Also, prime-gender discrimination is a valid and straightforward task because it requires the classification of the dimension of the word prime which we thought would be responsible for the priming effect. However, arguably gender discrimination is not the most sensitive visibility test because the gender status of the clearly visible targets interferes with the classification of prime gender. Therefore, the remaining seven participants had to discriminate between prime words and letter strings. These participants pressed one key for a string of eight identical letters (e.g., ‘AAAAAAAA’), and another for the prime word (again with primes as in the target-classification task). This task required inclusion of 50 % letter–string trials. Identities of these letter strings were randomly determined from trial to trial. Participants were informed about the probability of the different primes or letter strings and knew that prime and target identities were uncorrelated so that prime identity could not be inferred from the targets. The rationale of the word versus letter string discrimination was its simplicity and an assumed dependence of the prime’s gender-specific effect on the prime’s recognition as a word. The word–letter–string classification should be simple because it can be based on the recognition of redundant visual letter information alone. We also think that this task provided a fair measure of the prime’s awareness-dependent influence because the recognition of a word as a word is a logical prerequisite of the use of the word as a word. Participants were encouraged to guess if they failed to see the prime. No feedback was given during prime discrimination. Both tasks consisted of 3 blocks of 128 trials. This corresponds to altogether 24 repetitions of the 8 targets 9 2 primes in both of the consecutive tasks, the gender-classification task of the targets (block 1) and the prime-gender classification task (i.e., the visibility test in block 2). If the prime-visibility test required discriminating between a prime word and a string of letters, each combination was only repeated 12 times per combination because the other half of the trials contained letter strings. Trial sequence was random. Stimulus–response mappings were balanced across participants. Prior to data acquisition, participants practiced the task for 68 trials. Results Target classification For the results, see Fig. 2. As can be seen, there was a congruence effect. This impression was confirmed by formal analyses. Out of all responses, 4.6 % were excluded because individual correct RT exceeded the individual mean correct RT by more than 2 standard deviations (SDs). An ANOVA of correct mean RTs, with the variables congruence (congruent vs. incongruent), target gender (masculine vs. feminine), and target category (china/cutlery vs. body parts), revealed a significant main effect of congruence, F(1, 12) = 12.73, p \ .01, and a tendency towards a main effect of target gender, F(1, 12) = 4.06, p = .07. Responses were faster under congruent (RT = 763 ms) than incongruent conditions (RT = 775 ms), and with feminine targets (RT = 761 ms) than with masculine targets (RT = 778 ms). We observed no significant main effect of category, F \ 1.00, and no significant interactions, all Fs \ 1.30, all ps [ .27. A corresponding ANOVA of arc-sine transformed error rates revealed a tendency towards a significant main effect of congruence, F(1, 12) = 3.87, p = .07, reflecting a lower error rate (ER) under congruent (ER = 2.2 %) than incongruent conditions (ER = 2.9 %). Both remaining main effects, both Fs \ 3.10, both ps [ .10, and interactions, all Fs \ 2.00, all ps [ .19, were nonsignificant. 123 406 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 zero visibility (Greenwald et al., 1996). This was found: a = 12, p \ .01. 10 820 female; china/cutlery female; body parts 800 8 780 6 760 4 740 2 Errors [%] RT [ms] male; china/cutlery male; body parts 0 720 congruent incongruent congruent incongruent Fig. 2 Mean reaction times (RTs, in ms) on the left and mean error rates (in %) on the right as a function of congruence (congruent vs. incongruent, on the x axis), target gender (feminine circles; masculine squares), and target category (china/cutlery filled symbols; body parts empty symbols) in Experiment 1. Vertical lines depict standard errors Prime visibility Prime visibility was assessed by d0 (cf. Green & Swets, 1966). d0 is zero for chance performance and can become infinitely large with an ever increasing accuracy. d0 is a recommended measure of prime visibility because of its power to unveil residual visibility and to its relatively strong independence of judgment tendencies (cf. Reingold & Merikle, 1988, 1990). Hits were ‘word responses’ to word primes (or ‘feminine responses’ to feminine primes) and false alarms (FAs) were ‘word responses’ to letter strings (or ‘feminine responses’ to masculine primes). To obtain individual d0 values, we subtracted z-transformed FA rates from z-transformed hit rates. Mean d0 across the participants was low in the prime-gender classification task, d0 = -0.01, t(6) \ 1.00. A larger effect was found in the word–letter discrimination task, d0 = 0.10, t(5) = 2.07, p = .09. Averaged across tasks d0 was 0.04, t(12) \ 1.00. Individual d0 scores varied between -0.52 and 0.20. Prime visibility and congruence effect We also calculated a regression, with d0 as a predictor of the RT congruence effect (incongruent RT minus congruent RT) as a dependent variable. If prime visibility accounted for the RT congruence effect, we expected a significant correlation between the visibility measure of the primes and their RT congruence effect: the better the prime’s visibility, the larger the congruence effect should be (cf. Greenwald, Draine, & Abrams, 1996; Naccache & Dehaene, 2001). The regression analysis showed that there was no significant correlation between RT congruence effect and d0 from the visibility test, b = .08, p = .80. Finally, if the congruence effect is independent of awareness, we expected a positive intercept of the regression—that is, a significant interpolated congruence effect at 123 Discussion We found a significant congruence effect. Masked masculine and feminine articles affected the speed with which the participants classified the gender of visible target nouns: classification was faster under congruent than incongruent conditions. This congruence effect stood in marked contrast to the low visibility of the masked primes. Of course, it is always difficult to tell whether the visibility was absolutely nil (cf. Reingold & Merikle, 1988). However, in comparison to studies that do not even measure visibility (cf. Sereno, 1991), the present study yields a number of indicators that the participants saw very little of the primes (if any): the participants’ performance in the prime visibility test was not significantly better than chance. Moreover a regression of the congruence effect on visibility revealed that there was a significant intercept— that is, an interpolated congruence effect at zero visibility. Thus, the participants’ awareness of the masked articles was seemingly not crucial for morphosyntactically processing the articles. Together the results thus supported the notion that morphosyntactic processing occurs in an awareness-independent fashion. Interestingly, our results suggested a difference between the two visibility tasks. Performance was better in a letter– word discrimination task than in a prime-gender discrimination task. However, this does not cast too severe doubts on our interpretation that the congruence effect was independent of the visibility of the prime’s crucial features: only the prime-gender discrimination task exploited the same prime information that was also reflected in the congruence effect. This task indicated zero prime visibility. By contrast, the word–letter–string discrimination could have equally drawn on visual low-level features. This was as intended because we also wanted to use a very sensitive measure of residual visibility. [By the way, this finding of a higher sensitivity during the word–letter–string discrimination also underlines the validity of our rationale that word detection (reflected in a word–letter–string discrimination) is probably a prerequisite of word-gender congruence effects (reflected in the prime-gender classification).] In target RTs, we also found an unexpected general advantage for feminine targets. This main effect of gender on RTs might have reflected that both ‘die’ and ‘der’ more frequently precede feminine than masculine gender nouns according to our frequency counts: if we relax the constraint of our collocation counts and do consider plural masculine and feminine nouns as well as genitives in the annotated corpus, a frequency tableau shows that both ‘die’ Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 and ‘der’ likely precede feminine nouns (73.35 and 59.77 %, respectively) in comparison to masculine nouns (16.32 and 34.69 %, respectively) and neuter nouns (6.02 and 3.33 %, respectively). The reason for these collocations is that the syntactically polyvalent German words ‘die’ and ‘der’, even if preceding a target noun, can have an alternative morphosyntactical status. The word for a masculine singular definite article (‘der’) can be used as a morphosyntactically appropriate genitive determiner preceding a feminine singular noun, while the word for a feminine singular article (‘die’) can also be used as a syntactically appropriate definite article preceding the plural nominative form of a masculine noun, too.3 In terms of the collocation-based associations, the present prime words were thus generally favourable for expecting and processing of a subsequent feminine target noun in addition to the determiners’ gender-specific morphosyntactical priming effect for different singular noun genders. Experiment 2 In Experiment 2, we now wanted to test whether the subliminal congruence effect reflected a form of weak or of strong automaticity of morphosyntactic processing. Under the perspective of the conditional-automaticity view, for example, the awareness-independent morphosyntactic congruence effect should crucially depend on the task relevance of the gender status of the subliminal-priming words. This criterion was fulfilled in Experiment 1. Therefore, conditionally automatic processing might have indeed accounted for the congruence effect. However, in Experiment 1, forms of strongly automatic priming were also possible. It could be that the congruence effect reflected asymmetric syntagmatic prime-target relations rather than morphosyntactic processing (cf. Kjellmer, 1991). This was suggested by the collocation of primes and targets in text corpora by our distribution analysis preceding Experiment 1. Thus, a strongly automatic priming effect based on represented asymmetric syntagmatic relations between primes and targets, respectively, might have also accounted for the congruence effect. Such an effect could be of a strongly automatic type without the necessity 407 to refer to any type of top-down malleability, such as conditional automaticity, at all. Besides asymmetrical syntagmatic priming, bottom-up feature overlap between prime and targets with a genderprototypical ending is a second task-invariant property of prime-target relations that would be a conceivable contributor to Experiment 1’s congruence effect. Both of these influences would not be affected by the relevance of the gender discrimination. In contrast to these task-invariant mechanisms, we assumed that the morphosyntactic gender congruence effect of the masked primes crucially depends on the task of classifying the noun genders. Remember that we used the gender-classification task in Experiment 1 because this task’s relevance was assumed to be critical for syntactic processing and, thus, crucial for the subliminal morphosyntactic priming effect. We hypothesized that only the gender task constrained the interpretation of the syntactically polyvalent prime determiners. The task was therefore critical for disambiguation of the determiners’ morphosyntactic status because in German, the words ‘die’ and ‘der’, even if preceding the target noun, can take on alternative morphosyntactical roles. Therefore, if no gender-classification task prompts the determiners’ genderspecific deflections’ task relevance and constrains their particular morphosyntactical interpretation, these words as primes should not be uniformly congruent or incongruent to the subsequent target nouns. In Experiment 2, we tested the prediction that task relevance created a critical constraint for the morphosyntactic gender-based congruence effect. In a control condition to the gender task of Experiment 2, our participants discriminated between target nouns by categories that were orthogonal to target gender. These participants decided whether the target noun was from the china/cutlery category (e.g., ‘plate’) or from the body parts category (e.g., ‘nose’). In that task, we used exactly the same words as primes and targets as during gender classification. We expected no congruence effect if the congruence effect reflected conditionally automatic morphosyntactic assessment of the prime word gender status. By contrast, with bottom-up letter priming or asymmetric syntagmatic association priming, we expected the same congruence effect in the control condition as in the experimental condition of Experiment 2, a gender-classification task, and as in Experiment 1. 3 In the present study, this was only true for the two masculine china/ cutlery target nouns that had the same singular as plural word form (‘Teller’, and ‘Löffel’). For the two masculine body-part target nouns with different singular (‘Arm’ and ‘Mund’) than plural word forms (‘Arme’ and ‘Münder’, respectively) the German word ‘die’ was not a syntactically fitting plural article. The masculine body-part target conditions allowed us therefore to assess task-independent syntactic prime-target congruence effects in Experiment 2’s gender-independent category task. No such congruence effect was found. Therefore, the task relevance was crucial for the primes’ syntactic use. Method Participants Twenty-eight paid volunteers (14 female, mean age 23.7 years) participated. All had normal or corrected-tonormal vision by self-report. 123 408 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure 10 780 760 6 720 4 700 2 680 Target classification In Fig. 3, one can see that the gender-discrimination task again led to a significant congruence effect. As can be seen from Fig. 4, however, the congruence effect was absent in the control conditions in which the morphosyntactic status of the primes was task-irrelevant. This impression was corroborated by ANOVAs. Out of all responses, 4.2 % were excluded because correct RT exceeded the mean by more than 2 SDs. An ANOVA of correct mean RTs, with the within-participant variables congruence (congruent vs. incongruent), target gender (masculine vs. feminine), and target category (china/cutlery vs. body parts), and the between-participants variable task (gender classification vs. control task) revealed a significant main effect of congruence, F(1, 23) = 15.34, p \ .01. Responses were faster under congruent (RT = 718 ms) than incongruent conditions (RT = 726 ms). A significant congruence 9 task interaction, F(1, 26) = 11.44, p \ .01, meant that the congruence effect was restricted to the gender-classification task 10 780 female; china/cutlery female; body parts 760 8 740 6 720 4 700 2 680 Errors [%] RT [ms] male; china/cutlery male; body parts 0 congruent incongruent congruent incongruent Fig. 3 Mean reaction times (RTs, in ms) on the left and mean error rates (in %) on the right as a function of congruence (congruent vs. incongruent, on the x axis), target gender (feminine circles; masculine squares), and target category (china/cutlery filled symbols; body parts empty symbols) in the target gender-classification task of Experiment 2. Vertical lines depict standard errors 123 Errors [%] 740 0 congruent Results 8 male; china/cutlery male, body parts RT [ms] Half of the participants classified targets as belonging to the china/cutlery category or to the body parts category. This was the control condition. Half of the participants classified target gender. This was the experimental condition. Otherwise, the procedure was identical to the targetclassification task of Experiment 1. (The prime-visibility measure was skipped because essentially the same conditions as in Experiment 1 were used.) female; china/cutlery female; body parts incongruent congruent incongruent Fig. 4 Mean reaction times (RTs, in ms) on the left and mean error rates (in %) on the right as a function of congruence (congruent vs. incongruent, on the x axis), target gender (feminine circles; masculine squares), and target category (china/cutlery filled symbols; body parts empty symbols) in the control condition of Experiment 2. Vertical lines depict standard errors [congruent RT = 723 ms; incongruent RT = 739 ms, t(13) = 5.38, p \ .01, see Fig. 3]. No congruence effect was found in the control task (congruent RT = 723 ms; incongruent RT = 714 ms, see Fig. 4). A significant gender 9 task interaction, F(1, 26) = 10.51, p \ .01, reflected that in the gender-classification task responses were (nonsignificantly) slower for masculine targets (RT = 735 ms) than feminine targets (RT = 727 ms), t(13) = 1.24, p = .24, whereas in the control task, this pattern reversed [masculine targets: RT = 703 ms; feminine targets: RT = 722 ms, t(13) = 3.61, p \ .01]. In addition, we found tendencies towards significant gender 9 congruence, F(1, 26) = 3.14, p = .09, and gender 9 congruence 9 task interactions, F(1, 26) = 3.27, p = .08. As can be seen by comparing Figs. 3 and 4, a stronger congruence effect for masculine than feminine targets was restricted to the gender-classification task. (In the control task, the congruence effect was equally small under all target conditions.) The other main effects, all Fs \ 1.80, all ps [ .19, and interactions, all Fs \ 2.80, all ps [ .10, were nonsignificant. A corresponding ANOVA of arc-sine transformed error rates revealed a significant main effect of gender, F(1, 26) = 5.62, p \ .01. It reflected a lower error rate (ER) for masculine (ER = 1.0 %) than feminine targets (ER = 1.8 %). In addition, there was a significant congruence 9 target category interaction, F(1, 26) = 5.62, p \ .01, with a significant congruence effect in the china/ cutlery category (congruent ER = 1.0 %; incongruent ER = 2.0 %), t(27) = 2.49, p \ .05, but not in the bodyparts category (congruent ER = 1.3 %; incongruent ER = 1.3 %) category, t \ 1.00. Main effects, all Fs \ 2.30, all ps [ .14, and the remaining interactions, all Fs \ 1.50, all ps [ .23, were nonsignificant. Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 Discussion In line with the notion that weak automaticity in general and conditional automaticity in particular were responsible for the morphosyntactic priming effect, the RT results showed a task dependence of the congruence effect. Only, the gender-classification task prompted the relevance of the gender status of the masked determiners and constrained interpretation of the (morphosyntactical) role of the masked articles in such a way that a significant congruence effect resulted. In the gender-classification task, the morphosyntactic status of the prime could have either facilitated processing of a congruent target gender but less so of an incongruent target gender, or the prime’s morphosyntactic status could have directly biased the decision (cf. Norris & Kinoshita, 2008). The results are also in line with an elaborated-processing view of subliminal priming. According to this view, participants would have applied their task sets to the subliminal primes, too. From an elaborated-processing view, however, it is not so clear why the classification of target categories should have prevented the congruence effect. Thus, it seems that the greater flexibility of the elaborated-processing view allowing to explain task set influences on masked priming comes at the expense of less clear predictions. Unexpectedly, the gender-congruence effect in the experimental group was slightly stronger with masculine nouns than with feminine nouns. It is possible that this was a mere chance finding because no such interaction was found in Experiment 1. However, it is also possible that subtle differences between ‘die’ and ‘der’ accounted for this difference. For example, if more closure of the end letter ‘e’ of the feminine article allowed a quicker activation of the representation of the feminine article in the mental lexicon than less closure of the final letter ‘r’ of the masculine article, this might explain that on average the activation of the masculine prime word was slower than that of the feminine article. This in turn could have diminished interference of the incongruent masculine primes with the feminine nouns (compare to Fig. 3), thereby, mitigating the net congruence effect with the feminine targets. In any case, in the control conditions, target gender was irrelevant for the task and this changed task failed to support an interpretation of the masked primes as singular gender-specific articles. As a consequence, we observed little evidence for a gender-based article-noun RT congruence effect. Moreover, the RT advantage for the feminine target nouns that we found in Experiment 1 was also no longer observed in the control conditions. We do not know how the reverse RT advantage for masculine targets (as compared to feminine targets) in the control condition of Experiment 2 came about. 409 Maybe the greater visual resemblance of all feminine targets that all shared the vowel ‘a’ at the second letter position (‘Tasse’; ‘Gabel’; ‘Hand’; ‘Nase’) made it taxing to classify these targets as belonging to different categories. In comparison, not all of the masculine articles did share a letter at one position (‘Teller’; ‘Mund’; ‘Arm’, ‘Nase’) and this overall visual distinctiveness might have facilitated their classification into different categories. Whatever the exact origin of the male targets’ RT advantage in the control condition, importantly, without a task constraining prime interpretation to a particular morphosyntactical role (here: a gender-specific inflexion), the masked prime seemingly frequently failed to figure as a genitive determiner or a plural article, too (but see below). However, with the error rates the picture in the control condition was different. Here, the congruence effects were fairly similar across tasks. The error rates in the china/ cutlery category reflected a residual prime/article-target/ noun gender congruence effect in the control condition. This is interesting. Masculine target nouns in this category (‘Teller’, and ‘Löffel’) have the same singular and plural word forms. In this condition, the ‘incongruent prime word’ ‘die’ would therefore have syntactically fitted if interpreted as a definite plural article. Maybe the six remaining noun targets prevented interpretation of the masculine china/cutlery targets as plurals. The six remaining noun targets were singular (and have a different singular than plural word form). Therefore, the majority of singular target nouns might have suggested interpretation of all target nouns as singular. It is of course also possible that this residual genderbased article-noun congruence effect reflected strongly automatic priming. Whatever the reason for the residual congruence effect: as predicted, the RT congruence effect was significantly diminished in the control condition. Therefore, priming on the basis of one of two strongly automatic principles—that is, strongly automatic priming via bottom-up stimulus-driven feature overlap of target noun endings by congruent prime article endings or priming via asymmetric syntagmatic relations, is not the major origin of the congruence effect. Experiment 3 According to a weak-automaticity view in general and the conditional-automaticity perspective in particular, we have argued for the supportive if not necessary role of the wordorder context for subliminal morphosyntactic processing. In Experiment 3, we tested this hypothesis. Again, task relevance was ensured by a gender-discrimination task. However, in contrast to Experiments 1 and 2, we changed the word order which would be necessary for automatic 123 410 morphosyntactic processing of the primes as gender-specific deflections for a subsequent singular-case noun. This means that if the congruence effect in Experiments 1 and 2 was truly of a morphosyntactic origin, a fitting word order disambiguating the determiner’s grammatical role should be also crucial for a morphosyntactical congruence effect. In general, word order and word form (or morphosyntactical deflection) interact during syntactical processing to figure both as expectancy refinements (Friederici & Weissenborn, 2007). In the case of a gender-specifying singular determiner, the correct German word order requires that the determiner precedes the noun to specify the noun’s gender. Therefore, if in the gender-discrimination tasks of Experiments 1 and 2, the conditionally automatic morphosyntactical processing of the prime accounted for the congruence effect, the congruence effect should be diminished if the word order of article and noun is reversed. In Experiment 3, using nouns as primes that precede determiners as targets, the subsequent determiner no longer specifies the preceding singular noun’s gender. One particular source of the word-congruence effect that has the potential to survive the reversal of word order in the current experiment is automatic retrieval of determiners as closed words from mental lexicon: it is possible that a particular determiner with a fitting gender is automatically retrieved as part of the representation of a noun in the mental lexicon (cf. Garrett, 1988). Therefore, it is possible that the priming noun’s gender in the present experiment activated the congruent target determiners but not the incongruent determiners by way of a joint representation of nouns with their gender-specific fitting determiners in the mental lexicon. As a consequence, the joint representation of nouns and determiners in the mental lexicon could lead to a congruence effect in the present experiment. To more exhaustively test this prediction of a strongly automatic origin of the congruence effects of the determiners, we also included indefinite articles as targets in the present experiment. In German, the indefinite masculine (‘ein’) and feminine (‘eine’) articles are not used as plural determiners. These determiners have therefore an even higher probability of being stored together with the nouns in a gender-specific way in mental lexicon. Also according to a strongly automatic spreading-activation explanation, we might also expect a prime-target gender-congruence effect under the conditions of the present experiment. If the gender-congruence effect in the preceding experiments reflected an activation of shared genders of prime and target but regardless of whether or not the sequence of words was syntactically appropriate, we should also find a prime-target congruence effect with the reversed order of nouns as primes and determiners as targets. 123 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 In Experiment 3, the nouns that we used as primes differed between blocks. In one block, we used the words ‘Nase’ and ‘Arm’ as primes. In a second block, we used the words ‘Hand’ and ‘Mund’ as primes. In a third block, the prime nouns were ‘Tasse’ and ‘Teller’, and in a fourth block, the primes were the nouns ‘Gabel’ and ‘Löffel’. In this way, we aimed to keep the number of different primes per each of the blocks low and akin to the number of different article primes in the preceding Experiments 1 and 2. Also, the inclusion of the variable prime type (block) in our analysis should shed light on the role of the word endings for the congruence effect in the present experiment. For example, if the gender-prototypicality of the noun-prime endings promoted their gender-based congruence effect, we should find a significant effect when ‘Tasse’ and ‘Teller’ are used as primes, but not when ‘Hand’ and ‘Mund’ are used as primes. Method Participants Seventeen paid volunteers (11 female, mean age 26.1 years) participated. All had normal or corrected-tonormal vision by self-report. Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure German masculine and feminine definite articles (used as primes in the preceding experiments) and indefinite articles (feminine: ‘eine’, masculine: ‘ein’) were used as visible targets. The nouns that we used in the preceding experiments were the masked primes. Each noun prime was orthogonally combined with each target, leading to 32 combinations (8 nouns 9 4 articles). Each combination was repeated 16 times. In each of four blocks of the target-gender classification task, only two nouns were used as primes. Blockwise separated, one feminine and one masculine noun was used, either (1) ‘Tasse’ and ‘Teller’, (2) ‘Gabel’ and ‘Löffel’, (3) ‘Hand’ and ‘Mund’, or (4) ‘Nase’ and ‘Arm’. The different block orders were balanced across participants. By using only two nouns per block, the primes varied less. This made the procedures in Experiments 1, 2, and 3 more similar to one another. After the target-classification task, a prime-gender classification was used as a prime-visibility test. This was done to ascertain that the visibility of the masked nouns was also low. (Note that this cannot be derived from the visibility of the articles in the preceding experiments.) Otherwise, procedures were as in Experiment 1. Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 411 Results Target classification For the results see Table 1. Out of all responses, 4.5 % were excluded because correct RTs deviated by more than 2 SDs from the mean. We ran an ANOVA of the correct mean RTs with the within-participant variables target type (definite article; indefinite article), congruence (congruent; incongruent), target gender (masculine; feminine), and prime type/block (‘Hand’–‘Mund’; ‘Tasse’–‘Teller’; ‘Nase’–‘Arm’; ‘Gabel’–‘Löffel’). Of major interest in the present context, this ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of congruence, F(1, 16) = 4.69, p \ .05, and a significant three-way interaction of congruence 9 gender 9 prime type/block, F(3, 48) = 3.63, p \ .05. The main effect of congruence reflected the typical finding of faster RTs in congruent (RT = 673 ms) than in incongruent conditions (RT = 678 ms). More interestingly, the three-way interaction showed that this congruence effect reflected significant congruence effects in only three out of eight prime-type cases. Two out of the significant three prime-type conditions were masked noun primes with a gender-prototypical ending, the masculine noun ‘Teller’ (congruence effect = 17 ms, t[17] = 2.89, p \ .05) and the feminine noun ‘Nase’ (congruence effect = 10 ms, t[17] = 2.12, p \ .05). In addition, the masculine noun ‘Arm’ that was presented in the same blocks as ‘Nase’ led to a significant congruence effect (13 ms, t[17] = 2.63, p \ .05), too. Also noteworthy, the congruence effect failed in the blocks in which the noun primes had a similar ending (i.e., in blocks in which the noun primes ‘Hand’ and ‘Mund’ were used, and in blocks in which the noun primes ‘Gabel’ and ‘Löffel’ were used), as well as with the noun prime ‘Tasse’ (which was used in the same blocks as the noun prime ‘Teller’), all non-significant congruence effects, ts \ 1.20, all ps [ .17. To summarize these findings, if the noun endings discriminated between genders, this was a favourable (if not necessary) condition for the congruence effect. By contrast, the beneficial impact of the gender-prototypicality of the noun prime’s ending on the congruence effect was less certain. In addition, in this ANOVA we found a main effect of target type, F(1, 16) = 11.17, p \ .01, and a target type 9 gender interaction, F(1, 16) = 43.86, p \ .01. RTs were faster with definite (RT = 665 ms) than with indefinite (RT = 686 ms) articles as targets. This, however, was qualified by the interaction which showed that the masculine targets accounted for the advantage of the definite determiners (RT = 655 ms) as compared to the indefinite determiners (RT = 700 ms), t(16) = 5.76, p \ .01, whereas with the feminine targets there was no strong difference between indefinite (RT = 775 ms) and definite determiners (RT = 772 ms), t \ 1.00. All other effects and interactions were not significant, all Fs \ 1.80, all ps [ .20. Table 1 Mean reaction times (RTs) in milliseconds and error rates (ERs) in percent (in parentheses) for the different conditions of Experiment 3 Prime Target Congruence Mean RT (ER) Standard errors Net congruence effects Mund der Congruent 650 (0.7) 16 (0.5) 15 (2.4) Hand der Incongruent 665 (3.1) 17 (1.6) Hand die Congruent 688 (4.8) 15 (1.5) Mund die Incongruent 681 (6.1) 16 (1.6) Arm der Congruent 650 (1.9) 19 (1.2) Nase der Incongruent 652 (2.3) 17 (0.9) Nase die Congruent 665 (1.2) 17 (0.7) Arm Löffel die der Incongruent Congruent 670 (1.6) 664 (2.6) 20 (0.7) 16 (1.1) Gabel der Incongruent 657 (1.6) 16 (1.0) Gabel die Congruent 678 (2.2) 16 (0.8) Löffel die Incongruent 682 (2.4) 17 (0.8) Teller der Congruent 654 (1.5) 12 (0.9) Tasse der Incongruent 651 (0.8) 16 (0.5) Tasse die Congruent 663 (3.5) 14 (1.4) Teller die Incongruent 673 (3.7) 14 (1.4) -7 (1.3) 2 (0.4) Prime Target Congruence Mean RT (ER) Standard errors Mund ein Congruent 699 (7.4) 15 (2.4) Hand ein Incongruent 699 (3.8) 16 (1.3) Hand eine Congruent 683 (3.6) 17 (1.2) Mund eine Incongruent 680 (1.9) 16 (1.2) Arm ein Congruent 682 (5.0) 17 (1.5) Nase ein Incongruent 700 (5.8) 18 (1.5) Net congruence effects 1 (-3.6) -3 (-1.7) 18 (0.8) 5 (0.4) Nase eine Congruent 657 (2.7) 19 (1.3) 21 (-0.8) -7 (-1.0) Arm Löffel eine ein Incongruent Congruent 678 (1.9) 705 (3.6) 19 (0.7) 15 (2.0) 3 (2.9) Gabel ein Incongruent 708 (6.5) 16 (2.9) Gabel eine Congruent 671 (3.1) 15 (1.1) Löffel eine Incongruent 674 (1.1) 17 (0.9) Teller ein Congruent 710 (7.2) 17 (1.8) Tasse ein Incongruent 696 (8.6) 13 (2.0) Tasse eine Congruent 653 (4.1) 15 (1.4) Teller eine Incongruent 678 (3.8) 16 (1.1) 4 (0.2) -3 (-0.7) 10 (0.2) 3 (-2.0) -14 (1.4) 15 (-0.3) Mean RTs and ERs are compared in the congruent and incongruent conditions and the net congruence effect is calculated as the mean performance in the incongruent condition minus the mean performance in the congruent condition. Standard errors from the mean values are also provided (for error rates in parentheses) 123 412 A corresponding ANOVA of the arc-sine transformed ERs led to a significant main effect of target type, F(1, 16) = 4.72, p \ .05, and again to a significant target type 9 gender interaction, F(1, 16) = 20.82, p \ .01. As was the case with the RTs, the main effect of target type reflected better performance with definite articles (ER = 2.5 %) than with indefinite articles (ER = 4.4 %) as targets, and the interaction revealed that this advantage owes to the masculine targets for which latter the definite articles (ER = 1.8 %) outperformed the indefinite articles (ER = 6.0 %), t(16) = 3.61, p \ .01. By contrast, the differences between feminine definite (ER = 3.2 %) and indefinite articles (ER = 2.8 %) were again negligible, t \ 1.00. There was also a significant prime type/block 9 target type interaction, F(3, 48) = 3.99, p \ .05 (e = 0.86). Interestingly, this effect reflected that the advantages of definite as compared to indefinite articles as targets were found within the same blocks in which significant congruence effects were found. The target-type effect (indefinite articles’ ER minus definite articles’ ER) was found in the blocks with ‘Nase’ and ‘Arm’ noun primes [2.0 %; t(16) = 3.01, p \ .01] and in blocks with ‘Tasse’ and ‘Teller’ noun primes [3.1 %; t(16) = 2.96, p \ .01]. By contrast the effect of target type was absent in the remaining blocks, both nonsignificant ts \ 1.00. In addition, we observed an almost significant main effect of prime type/block, F(3, 48) = 2.87, p = .07 (e = 0.70), and an almost significant three-way interaction of target type 9 prime type/block 9 gender, F(3, 48) = 2.69, p = .06. The effect of prime type/block reflected somewhat elevated ERs in blocks with ‘Hand’ and ‘Mund’ primes (ER = 3.9 %) or with ‘Tasse’ and ‘Teller’ primes (ER = 4.1 %) as compared to blocks with ‘Nase’ and ‘Arm’ primes (2.8 %) or with ‘Gabel’ and ‘Löffel’ primes (2.9 %). The three-way interaction was again suggestive of a role of word endings. A striking example is the different influence of the ‘Hand’ and ‘Mund’ primes on masculine definite (ER = 1.9 %) and indefinite (ER = 5.6 %) articles versus on feminine definite (ER = 5.5 %) and indefinite (ER = 2.8 %) articles that showed a definite-article advantage with masculine targets and an indefinite-article advantage with feminine targets. (Other differences existed but were not as strong as the example above. The performance in the other conditions can be inferred from Table 2.) The results were not quite significant and unpredicted but again point to the word endings as one powerful factor for how well the targets could be processed. Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 from Experiment 2’s control condition with an alternative classification task were not considered.) We restricted Experiment 3’s data to the definite article conditions because only these were used as primes in Experiments 1 and 2 and can, thus, be strictly compared. This ANOVA included the additional between-participants variable sequential prime-target order [article primes noun (Experiments 1 and 2); noun primes article (Experiment 3)]. It revealed a significant main effect of congruence, F(1, 42) = 16.26, p \ .01, and, most importantly, a significant congruence 9 prime-target order interaction, F(1, 42) = 8.33, p \ .01. Responses were faster in congruent (RT = 703 ms) than incongruent conditions (RT = 711 ms), but the interaction reflected that this was only true for definite articles that primed nouns (congruent RT = 742 ms; incongruent RT = 756 ms), t(26) = 6.32, p \ .01, but not for the reversed order in which nouns primed definite articles (congruent RT = 664 ms; incongruent RT = 666 ms), t(16) \ 1.00. (The absence of the congruence effect means that the residual overall congruence effect in Experiment 3 stems mostly from the trials with indefinite articles as targets.) In addition, the ANOVA confirmed a significant main effect of experiments, F(1, 28) = 14.03, p \ .01, with faster responses in Experiment 3 (RT = 655 ms) than Experiments 1 and 2 (RT = 750 ms), and a significant target gender 9 prime-target order interaction. Responses to feminine targets (RT = 743 ms) were faster than to masculine targets (RT = 755 ms), t(26) = 2.36, p \ .05, in Experiments 1 and 2 but they were faster to masculine (RT = 655 ms) than to feminine targets (RT = 675 ms), t(16) = 3.03, p \ .01, in Experiment 3. Prime visibility Masking was reasonable as indicated by a small and nonsignificant d0 = 0.05, t(16) = 1.87, p = .08. Individual d0 values varied between -0.10 and 0.19. Prime visibility and congruence effect As in Experiment 1, we ran a regression analysis to test whether a significant correlation between the RT congruence effect and d0 from the visibility test obtained. This was not the case, b = .00, p = .99. However, in line with an awarenessindependent congruence effect, we found a positive intercept of the regression, a = 4.51, p \ .05 (one-sided). Congruence effects as a function of sequential prime-target order Discussion A further ANOVA concerned the correct RTs of Experiments 1, 2 and 3’s target-gender classification data. (Data With respect to our question whether the masked primes led to a strongly automatic or to a conditionally automatic 123 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 congruence effect, the results of Experiment 3 were mixed. On the one hand, we found a stronger congruence effect of the definite articles in Experiment 1 where the sequential order fostered the morphosyntactic priming effect than in the current experiment where the sequential order was reversed. This finding would be in line with the assumed conditional dependence of a morphosyntactic priming effect on a fitting syntactical context. However, on the other hand, several caveats needed to be considered. First of all, it is possible that it took more time to process the gender of the longer noun prime words in the present experiment than the shorter article prime words in Experiment 1. As a consequence, the participants might have derived a noun prime’s gender simply too late to allow an effect on the processing of the target articles in the present experiment. In addition, with the extremely simple task of classifying just four target words, the current experiment might have also discouraged an in-depth processing of the target words’ meanings and this in turn could have mitigated all effects depending on an in-depth processing of the word meanings, such as a priming effect of the noun-prime gender. For instance, the participants might have neglected the target-gender information completely and could have just looked at an article’s discriminative letters for their target classifications. In line with this possibility, there was a main effect of target gender and an interaction of target gender and target type that could be interpreted along these lines. With the masculine article targets, participants were better at definite (‘der’) than indefinite (‘ein’) determiners. This could have reflected a low overlap of only one shared letter between different genders for (1) ‘der’ and ‘die’, and (2) ‘der’ and ‘eine’. This should have facilitated the recognition and gender-classification of ‘der’. This advantage was found to be especially strong in comparison to the indefinite masculine article ‘ein’, possibly because ‘ein’ shared all letters with the alternative gender target ‘eine’, and because ‘ein’ shared two of its letters with the alternative gender target ‘die’. In summary, it is plausible that a non-semantic decision, for instance, a mere letter-based discrimination of the articles’ genders, sufficed to successfully classify the targets and this could have undermined a semantic effect of the primes. Finally, it is possible that the changed context of prime words per block in the present experiment counteracted a congruence effect. In Experiment 1, all primes had a gender-prototypical ending and prime genders could be discriminated by the prime-word endings in all of the trials. By contrast, in Experiment 3, only half of the primes had a gender-prototypical word ending and the primes’ gender could only be discriminated by the prime endings in half of the blocks. For instance, it is impossible to discriminate between (the gender of) the masculine prime ‘Mund’ and 413 the feminine prime ‘Hand’ by looking at their endings (e.g., ‘-nd’). Also, in line with a supportive role of the prime-word endings for the congruence effect, the RT congruence effect was only found when the different gender primes had different endings. For example, the congruence effect was found in a block where the masculine prime ‘Arm’ and the feminine prime ‘Nase’ were used. Therefore, Experiments 1 and 3 did not only differ by their respective order of articles and targets but also by factors, such as the degree to which word meaning was helpful to solve the task and the context of prime words (i.e., whether or not the prime gender could be discriminated by the prime-word endings). Of course, on the basis of a strongly automatic spreading-activation account, we would have expected that prime and target meaning would be processed and should become effective in the form of a congruence effect in Experiment 3 anyway. However, the residual congruence effect in the present experiment is also compatible with a stimulusdriven explanation of the congruence effect that puts an emphasis on the prime-word endings rather than on the prime meaning to account for the congruence effect (e.g., priming of the target ‘der’ by the shared letters of the prime ‘Teller’; but see Experiments 2 and 4 for counter evidence). Experiment 4 Experiments 1, 2, and 3 used a very restricted target set. This is a limitation because, in general, prior research has shown that a restricted target set and many repetitions of targets are beneficial for relatively simple masked-priming effects that have little to do with deeper-going processing of the primes (cf. Damian, 2001; Kouider & Dupoux, 2004). Damian (2001), for example, found out that only the words that belonged to the set of target words created a subliminal congruence effect and that word repetitions in his experiment were very helpful for the subliminal congruence effect. Of course, the explanation of Damian (2001) that the primes acquired a particular response meaning via practice with their visible target-counterparts cannot explain our findings in Experiments 1 and 2 because as primes we used ‘novel’ words that were not contained in the target set. The use of novel words as primes is generally regarded as a favourable condition for the fool-safe demonstration of the more sophisticated forms of subliminal priming, such as an elaborate semantic processing of the masked prime (cf. Naccache & Dehaene, 2001). Yet, our suspicions regarding the participants’ use of the (low-level) target-word letters for classifying the targets in Experiment 3 illustrated the limitations of using only a restricted target set. If the participants used the target letters (e.g., in particular the target-word endings) for the target classification, 123 414 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 Table 2 German nouns used in Experiment 4, their English translations (in italics), and the word frequency counts (retrieved from http://dict.uni-leipzig.de/; on January 15, 2012) Feminine/animals Feminine/unanimate Masculine/animals Masculine/unanimate Maus (mouse) 2180 Glut (glowing fire) 291 Hund (dog) 11 Herd (hearth) 893 Laus (louse) 119 Uhr (clock) 369 Fuchs (fox) 2683 Kamm (comb) 520 Gans (goose) 322 Schnur (string) 558 Frosch (frog) 526 Knopf (button) 795 Muschel (mussel) 188 Gabel (fork) 394 Igel (hedgehog) 567 Wimpel (pennant) 158 Wachtel (quail) 131 Nudel (noodle) 63 Esel (donkey) 814 Pinsel (brush) 748 Amsel (blackbird) 164 Nadel (needle) 826 Gimpel (bullfinch) 28 Kegel (skittle) 233 Kobra (cobra) Natter (adder) 119 38 Tuba (tuba) Zither (zither) 266 11 Panda (panda) Tiger (tiger) 196 1877 Talar (gown) Kleister (paste) 118 79 Viper (viper) 57 Klapper (rattle) 15 Hamster (hamster) 223 Kleber (glue) 234 Flunder (flounder) 67 Leier (lyra) 197 Kaefer (beetle) 1063 Wecker (alarm clock) 947 Elster (mapgpie) 131 Butter (butter) 1326 Marder (marten) 154 Besen (broom) 577 Fliege (fly) 920 Saege (saw) 299 Habicht (goshawk) 71 Zirkel (dividers) 741 Katze (cat) 1623 Spritze (syringe) 603 Maulwurf (mole) 238 Wuerfel (dice) 690 58 Biene (bee) 190 Kanne (jug) 182 Iltis (polecat) 43 Duebel (dowel) Muecke (moscito) 234 Muenze (coin) 816 Kiebitz (pewit) 79 Hobel (plane) 65 Meise (titmouse) 78 Brause (shower) 109 Schakal (jackal) 38 Fueller (fountain-pen) 125 Ziege (goat) 1179 Geige (violin) 1025 Fasan (pheasant) 65 Spaten (spade) 387 Ente (duck) 697 Taste (key) 813 Falter (butterfly) 229 Rechen (rake) 7 Eule (owl) 273 Wolle (wool) 902 Bussard (buzzard) 46 Sextant (sextant) 18 Qualle (jelly-fish) 44 Kelle (scoop) 163 Bulle (bull) 365 Becher (cup) 663 Spinne (spider) 240 Robe (robe) 245 Loewe (lion) 1019 Opal (opal) 48 Wespe (wasp) Taube (pigeon) 87 579 Lampe (lamp) Salbe (ointment) 861 107 Rabe (raven) Affe (monkey) 362 359 Kessel (kettle) Loeffel (spoon) 811 515 Motte (moth) 257 Matte (mat) 568 Hase (hare) 839 Haken (hook) 1561 Kroete (toad) 266 Floete (flute) 715 Ochse (ox) 106 Ofen (stove) 1108 No. letters/word Frequency No. letters/word Frequency No. letters/word Frequency No. letters/word Frequency Ø = 5.32 Ø = 407.32 Ø = 5.20 Ø = 468.96 Ø = 5.48 Ø = 478.24 Ø = 5.68 Ø = 483.9 The average frequencies below the columns were computed relative to the 400,000 entries in the online data bank (cited above) and retrieved on that day this could at least account for the residual congruence effect of the prime words (i.e., in particular the prime-word endings) that we found in Experiment 3 and in the error rates of the control condition of Experiment 2. However, if subliminal morphosyntactic priming was truly at work, it should be possible to demonstrate the principle of masked morphosyntactic priming with a broader set of target words, too. This test was conducted in the present experiment in which a set of 100 different target nouns was used, 50 of them masculine and 50 feminine. As in Experiment 1, the singular feminine and masculine definite determiners were used as masked primes. These determiners as primes were orthogonally combined with the nouns as targets to create equal amounts of syntactically gender-congruent and syntactically gender-incongruent conditions. If the morphosyntactic status of the primes is responsible for the congruence effect we should find a congruence effect with the larger set of nouns of 123 Experiment 4: we expected a gender-congruence effect, with faster responses in syntactically gender-congruent than in syntactically gender-incongruent conditions because the participants again had to classify the target noun’s gender, so that a gender-congruent determiner should facilitate this classification (as compared to a gender-incongruent prime) even if the congruence effect critically hinges on the participants’ intentions. Also, in Experiment 4, we included masked nouns as primes in half of the trials. This was done to compare the morphosyntactic gender-priming effect with a genderindependent category-priming effect. The noun primes were either of a congruent or of an incongruent category in comparison to the category of the noun targets. Here, in category-congruent conditions, both prime noun and target noun denoted animals (e.g., ‘quail–blackbird’) or both denoted unanimated items (e.g., ‘saw–fork’). Likewise, in category-incongruent conditions, the prime noun denoted Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 an animal and the target noun an unanimated item (e.g., ‘cat–violin’) or the prime noun denoted an unanimated item and the target noun an animal (e.g., ‘coin–spider’). Prior research has demonstrated that a masked categorycongruence effect based on such a category membership can at least be found if the category difference between animals and unanimated items is task relevant (e.g., Finkbeiner & Palermo, 2009). In the present experiment, we did not necessarily expect such a straightforward category-congruence effect, with faster responses in category-congruent than categoryincongruent conditions. The reason is that the conditionalautomaticity view implies that task relevance is critical for the processing of unconscious stimuli. Thus, it could also be that the participants ignored the task-irrelevant genderindependent category membership of the noun primes in the present experiment because the category membership of the nouns was irrelevant for the task at hand and even misleading with respect to the task because half of all masculine targets denoted animals (and half inanimate items) and half of all feminine targets denoted animals (and half inanimate items). However, according to a spreading-activation explanation of masked priming, the subliminal nouns could well have triggered an in-depth semantic processing of their meaning even if this would not be required. The same would be true according to an elaborated-processing view (cf. Dehaene et al., 1998; Naccache & Dehaene, 2001). Thus, if the morphosyntactic gender-congruence effect of the masked articles reflected a kind of category-priming effect which was based on spreading activation or on an elaborate processing of all masked word prime meaning and regardless of the task relevance, we expected a category-congruence effect of the masked nouns, too. Method Participants Twenty-one volunteers (18 female; mean age 23 years) participated in exchange for course credit. Two participants had to be excluded because they conducted more than 20 % errors on average. One bilingual (native Hungarian) participant had to be excluded because after the experiment she reported difficulties with the task. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision by self-report. Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure were the same as in Experiment 1, with the following exceptions. A larger number of target nouns was used, 50 per genus, feminine and masculine, half of which were from the category of animal names and half were from the broad category of unanimated items. All nouns were high-frequency words 415 (with more than 60 counted instances in 1 million words, cf. Jescheniak & Levelt, 1994). In the present experiment, frequency estimates were based on word counts in the Wortschatz Lexikon of the University of Leipzig, http://dict.uni-leipzig.de/, and frequencies were computed relative to 400.000 entries, contained in the Wortschatz Lexikon on the date of retrieval (January 15, 2012). All nouns denoted relatively concrete objects (e.g., kitchen utensils, musical instruments, tools, animals), and the lists of the two gender-independent noun categories (animals vs. unanimated items) as well as the lists of the two different gender-type nouns (of feminine vs. masculine nouns) were approximately equated for the average noun lengths and noun frequencies (see Table 2). For each list of the different gender targets, the nouns were carefully selected in such a way that the noun endings of the same gender varied [e.g., the German feminine nouns ‘Taube’ (Engl. ‘pigeon’) vs. ‘Natter’ (Engl. ‘adder’)], and that the noun beginnings of the same gender varied [e.g., the German masculine nouns ‘Kessel’ (Engl. ‘kettle’) vs. ‘Besen’ (Engl. ‘broom’)]. Likewise, nouns were selected so that the endings of nouns of different genders were sometimes the same [e.g., the German feminine noun ‘Amsel’ (Engl. ‘blackbird’) and the masculine ‘Wimpel’ (Engl ‘pennant’)], and the same was true of the noun beginnings (e.g., the German feminine noun ‘Wespe’ (Engl. ‘wasp’) and the masculine ‘Wecker’ (Engl. ‘alarm clock’)]. As a consequence, neither the noun beginnings nor the noun endings were diagnostic of the task-relevant gender categories. Again, the use of different noun endings also allowed us to include both, nouns with a gender-prototypical ending [e.g., the feminine noun Ente (Engl. ‘duck’) ending on the prototypical feminine ending ‘-e’] as well as less prototypical endings [e.g. the feminine noun Elster (Engl. ‘magpie’) ending on the prototypical masculine ending ‘-er’]. This time we also included a formal analysis of the syntactic gender-congruence effect based on only the nouns with a non-prototypical ending. This was done to assess whether the syntactical gender-congruence effect of the masked determiners can also be replicated when the tobe-classified target nouns have a non-prototypical ending with respect to their gender. Different from the preceding experiments, in half of all trials we also used nouns as primes and targets. In these conditions, we measured a ‘category-congruence effect.’ In the following, we reserve the labels of ‘category congruence’ and ‘category incongruence’ to denote different degrees of gender-independent category membership of prime and target (i.e., a higher degree of category membership in the congruent and a lower degree in the incongruent conditions, respectively). In the category-congruent conditions, both— prime and target—were animal names or both denoted unanimated items. In the category-incongruent conditions, the 123 416 prime was an animal name and the target denoted an unanimated item (or vice versa). Note that the performance during both category-congruent and category-incongruent noun–noun pairings was assessed independently of the noun gender because all of these pairings were gender-congruent. For instance, in the category-incongruent condition, an animal name, such as the German noun ‘Fuchs’ (Engl. ‘fox’) might have preceded the label of an inanimate item, such as the German noun ‘Duebel’ (Engl. ‘dowel’) but even in these cases both would have been exemplars of the same gender (in the example both would have been masculine). In this way, we aimed for an assessment of the categorycongruence effect that was independent of the gendercongruence. (We restricted the noun–noun pairings to the gender-congruent cases to prevent combinatorial explosion of the conditions, and to overcome the resulting difficulties in the balancing of morphosyntactical priming conditions on the one hand and of category-priming conditions on the other hand.) In a total of 400 trials, each noun was used 4 times as a target and two times as a prime (only in the categorypriming conditions). Each article was equally often used as a prime (in exactly 100 trials). Half of all trials were used to assess the subliminal morphosyntactic congruence effect. In the morphosyntactic priming conditions, the noun targets were orthogonally combined with each of two article primes, leading to 25 % syntactically congruent and to 25 % syntactically incongruent conditions out of all trials. The other half of the trials was used to assess the category-congruence effect. In the category-priming conditions, each noun was equally often preceded by (a randomly chosen) noun from the same category and by a (randomly chosen) noun from a different category, leading to another 25 % category-congruent trials and to 25 % category-incongruent trials. Within each of five blocks of 80 trials, the different conditions were presented in a random order, with the following constraints: syntactic (or article) primes and category (or noun) primes were equally likely and every individual noun was at best used once as a target and was additionally at best used once as a prime. Again, repetition priming (i.e., identical prime and target nouns within a trial) was prevented. Different from the preceding experiments, prime visibility was estimated during the target-classification trials. After each target classification, the participants had to judge whether a noun had been presented as a masked prime before the target. This prime-visibility test was selected because on the basis of the conditional-automaticity view, we expected a difference between the processing of the noun primes and the article primes: gender-congruence effects with the prime articles but not necessarily category-congruence effects with the noun 123 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 primes. This instruction also allowed us not to mention the article primes. (If a participant wanted to know what other objects or words than nouns would be presented, we told her or him that it was her or his task to find out.) In this manner, again, it was ensured that the article primes did not belong to a ‘set of expected words’ (if that is what counts for whether or not a priming effect is trivial) and, thus, could be considered as ‘novel’. Also, the method of collecting target-classification responses and prime-visibility judgments in the same trials has the additional advantage of measuring the prime visibility under the very conditions in which prime visibility matters for the congruence effect: during target classification (cf. Ansorge, Fuchs, Khalid, & Kunde, 2011; Finkbeiner & Palermo, 2009). The experiment consisted of 400 trials in total. Prior to the first block, participants practiced the task for at least 20 trials. Together with short breaks between the blocks, the experiment took between 60 and 75 min. Results Target classification Out of all responses, 6.7 % were excluded because correct RTs deviated by more than 2 SDs from the mean. An ANOVA of correct mean RTs, with the within-participant variables congruence (congruent vs. incongruent), target gender (feminine vs. masculine) and prime type (noun vs. article) revealed a significant congruence 9 prime type interaction, F(1, 17) = 38.97, p \ .01. Follow-up t tests split up for prime types and target gender confirmed the existence of a significant syntactic congruence effect of the masked masculine articles, t(17) = 3.02, p \ .01, with faster RTs in gendercongruent (RT = 825 ms) than in gender-incongruent conditions (RT = 848 ms). The effect of the masked feminine articles was smaller [gender-congruent RT = 817 ms; gender-incongruent RT = 833 ms; t(17) = 2.17, p \ .05]. By contrast, a significant reverse categorycongruence effect was found with the feminine noun primes, t(17) = 5.29, p \ .01, with slower responses in category-congruent (RT = 840 ms) than category-incongruent conditions (RT = 804 ms), and a similar tendency was found with the masculine noun primes [category-congruent RT = 844 ms; category-incongruent RT = 830 ms; t(17) = 1.55, p = .14]. The main effects of congruence, prime type, both Fs \ 1.00, and of target gender, F(1, 19) = 1.12, p = .30, were not significant. The same was true of the remaining two-way interactions, Fs \ 2.50, both ps [ .14, and the three-way interaction, F(1, 17) = 1.80, p = .20. Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 In a complementary analysis, we assessed whether the syntactic congruence effect could also be observed when the target nouns all had a non-prototypical ending. To that end, we excluded all trials with target nouns of a genderprototypical ending that could be primed by their congruent article’s ending (all feminine nouns ending on ‘-e’, and all masculine nouns ending on ‘-er’), and again compared the congruent and incongruent RTs. In the corresponding t tests, a significant congruence effect with the masculine targets was successfully replicated, t(17) = 2.73, p \ .05, with faster responses in morphosyntactically congruent conditions (RT = 840 ms) than in incongruent conditions (RT = 866 ms). However, no significant syntactic congruence effect was found with the feminine targets [gender-congruent RT = 825 ms; gender-incongruent RT = 841 ms; t(17) = 1.55, p = .14]. A corresponding ANOVA of the arc-sine transformed error rates revealed only a tendency towards a significant main effect of congruence, F(1, 17) = 4.32, p = .053. ERs were slightly higher in the congruent (4.8 %) than in the incongruent condition (3.5 %). [As was the case in the RTs, this tendency towards a reverse congruence effect owes mostly to the category-priming conditions, in which the congruence effect (incongruent ER–congruent ER) amounted to -1.6 %, t(17) = 2.75, p \ .05. This compares to the (reverse) morphosyntactic congruence effect of -0.9 %, t(17) = 1.06, p = .30. However, like the main effects of target gender, F(1, 17) = 3.00, p = .10, and of prime type, F(1, 17) = 1.18, p = .29, on ERs, the gender 9 prime type interaction, F(1, 17) = 2.36, p = .14, the congruence 9 prime type interaction, the congruence 9 gender interaction, and the three-way interaction, F \ 1.00, failed to become significant in the ER analysis. Prime visibility Masking was relatively effective as indicated by a small and non-significant d0 = 0.05, t(17) = 1.73, p = .10. Individual d0 values varied between -0.08 and 0.38. Prime visibility and congruence effect Again, if prime visibility accounted for the RT congruence effect, we expected a significant correlation between the visibility measure of the primes and their RT congruence effect. However, there was neither a significant correlation between d0 and the gender-congruence effect of the masculine article primes, b = -.28, p = .26, nor of the feminine article primes, b = -.36, p = .15, nor between visibility and the reversed category-congruence effect of the feminine noun primes, b = -.17, p = .50. The intercept of the regression with the masculine article primes was significant, a = 25, p \ .01, whereas it was not significant 417 with the feminine article primes, a = 12, p = .13, and with the feminine noun primes there was also a significant negative intercept, a = -31, p \ .01. Discussion Experiment 4 replicated the morphosyntactic priming effect with a larger number of different target nouns but only with male targets. With male targets, if a masked determiner was gender-congruent to the visible target, target-gender classification was facilitated. Facilitation was found in comparison to a gender-incongruent condition, in which the masked determiner was of a different gender than the visible target. This congruence effect with the male targets could also be replicated by looking at the target nouns with non-prototypical gender endings alone and although the masked prime articles were not seen by our participants. By contrast, there was no significant morphosyntactic congruence effect with the female targets when we restricted our analysis to the non-prototypically ending nouns. Likewise, we were not able to establish a truly subliminal morphosyntactic congruence effect with the feminine determiners by an intercept criterion of the regression predicting the RT congruence effect from visibility. Also, because all target words, feminine and masculine nouns, were carefully equated for major determinants of processing speed such as length and frequency, we believe that the absence of the congruence effect is most likely due to the specifics of the priming principles of ‘die’ versus ‘der’. Maybe the feminine targets’ congruence effect was mediated by the perceptual priming of the word endings. With the feminine targets, if the prime’s ending letters and the target’s ending letters matched (as in the example with a prime ‘die’ before the target ‘Kroete’; Engl. ‘toad’) this could have facilitated RTs in comparison to the case where they did not match (e.g., with the prime ‘der’ before the target ‘Kroete’). In addition to the morphosyntactic congruence effect, a control condition with noun primes shed interesting light on the facets and limits of subliminal processing. With the feminine noun targets, we found a reverse category-congruence effect based on whether or not the noun prime and the noun target belonged to the same category (of animals or inanimate items) or to different categories. This finding is in line with the elaborate-processing hypothesis of masked priming. According to the elaborate processing explanation, unconscious stimuli are semantically analysed (Dehaene et al., 1998). This semantic analysis could also be carried out with respect to semantic features that are not defined as task-relevant (cf. Van Opstal et al., 2005). 123 418 Note, however, that even if the elaborate-processing view holds true for the gender-unrelated meaning it would still be an open question whether or not the morphosyntactic congruence effect was also due to elaborated processing. Related, conditional automaticity might also account for the reverse category-priming effect. In the present experiment, the same priming words that led to a reverse category-congruence effect were also clearly seen because these words were also used as targets where their gender had to be judged. In this situation, a conditionalautomaticity view could be defended by assuming that participants have found it helpful to suppress the nouns’ gender-independent task-irrelevant category membership because this feat was equally distributed across the relevant gender conditions and thus provided a distracting dimension. So it existed, the participants might have also inadvertently applied this active top-down set to suppress the target words’ task-irrelevant category membership to the masked prime nouns. General discussion Several lines of past evidence indicated that syntactic and morphosyntactic processing is automatic (e.g., Gunter et al., 2000; Pulvermüller, Shtyrov, Hasting, & Carlyon, 2008). For example, of particular importance in the context of the present study, prior findings showed awarenessindependent syntactic processing in patients suffering from amnesia (Ferreira et al., 2008) and during subliminalpriming studies with healthy participants (cf. Sereno, 1991). However, which kind of automatic processing, a weak or a strong form of automaticity was reflected in subliminal morphosyntactic priming was not so clear. On the basis of the necessity to disambiguate the syntactic function of syntactically polyvalent words during human language comprehension by preceding contextual information, we have argued for a conditional-automaticity view of subliminal morphosyntactic processing (cf. Ansorge & Neumann, 2005; Bargh, 1992; Kunde et al., 2003). According to this view, once task sets or preceding word-order contexts have sufficiently disambiguated the potential syntactic role of an upcoming word, morphosyntactic processing of this word could run off in a quick and awareness-independent fashion. This kind of conditional automaticity would be a weak form of automatic processing because it would not be purely stimulus-driven. However, we have also explained that stimulus-driven forms of intention-independent processing, that is, strong automaticity, could account for subliminal morphosyntactic priming. For example, a word might elicit an activation that spreads to other words in the mental lexicon in a 123 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 stimulus-driven way. This spreading of activation might lead to a subliminal-priming effect based on the tighter connections between particular syntagmatically related words or based on a closer connection between words that are in line with a subliminal-priming word’s syntactical roles. In both these cases, particular instances of subliminal priming of syntactically fitting target words by preceding subliminal prime words could be explained without the necessity to refer to intentions and task sets. To decide between these explanations, we derived several predictions. On the basis of the conditional-automaticity hypothesis of morphosyntactic processing, we expected that morphosyntactic priming with subliminal words can be found but that these awareness-independent priming effects are dependent on the task relevance of the syntactic status of the priming words and on the supportive context in which the priming words are presented. To test our hypotheses, we used German determiners as masked primes. These determiners have an ambiguous morphosyntactical status, and we used tasks and sequential word orders (of determiner and noun) to disambiguate the morphosyntactical status of the primes in varying degrees. Due to efficient masking, the prime words were barely seen. In this sense, the priming effects that we observed were independent of the participants’ awareness. In Experiments 1 and 2, both the task of classifying the gender of a clearly visible target word as well as the sequential word order (with the determiner as a prime preceding the noun as a target), fostered a morphosyntactical role of the subliminal determiners as masculine or feminine definite articles of the nouns. In line with our expectations, we observed a significant congruence effect. Articles with a gender that fitted to the visible nouns facilitated gender classification of the nouns. This was in comparison to trials in which articles had an opposite gender as compared with the targets. Next, we verified that the task relevance of the gender status of the primes was indeed crucial for the congruence effect. To start with, in German, the determiners that we used are morphosyntactically polyvalent or ambiguous. The feminine determiner ‘die’, for example, can be used as a definite singular article for only feminine nouns but it is used as a grammatically valid plural article for feminine and masculine nouns. Likewise, in German, the masculine article ‘der’ of singular masculine nouns can also be interpreted as a genitive determiner for a feminine singular noun. Therefore, we think that syntactic processing is inherently conditional on task sets or on constraining contextual information and that the gender classification was a necessary precondition for the gender-specific interpretation of the articles and the resultant congruence effect. This prediction of the conditional-automaticity view of morphosyntactic priming was tested and confirmed in Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 Experiment 2’s control task. If in a control task the participants had to classify the same noun targets as belonging to different semantic categories (china/cutlery versus body parts) orthogonal to the target’s gender status, noun gender can be disregarded. In this condition, the task no longer suggested that the same masked prime determiners were interpreted as singular definite articles of a particular gender. Accordingly, the gender-based congruence effect was absent in the control task of Experiment 2. This is clear support for weak automaticity. By contrast, on the basis of a merely stimulus-driven view of subliminal morphosyntactic priming, we would have expected equal congruence effects in both tasks. For example, if the functional role of the prime representation would have automatically primed all fitting gender targets in a stimulus-driven way, this effect should have occurred in both tasks. This prediction of a strong-automaticity view was clearly falsified in Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, we confirmed that the masked congruence effect of the definite determiners was also sensitive to the sequential order of the words. For its gender-based morphosyntactical congruence effect, a German definite article has to precede a noun. Therefore, if the congruence effect of the definite articles indeed reflected conditionally automatic morphosyntactical processing of the article, we expected the diminution of the congruence effect if the word order of nouns and articles was reversed. This hypothesis was tested in Experiment 3 in which nouns were used as masked primes preceding visible determiners as targets. The results supported the conditional-automaticity view of morphosyntactic priming because the congruence effect in Experiment 3’s reversed word-order conditions with definite articles was significantly weaker than in Experiment 1 and 2’s ‘correct-syntactic order’ conditions. This was found although a gender-discrimination task was used in all of the conditions of Experiment 3. In Experiment 3, we also found a significant residual gender-based congruence effect that evidently did not depend on the syntactically correct word order of determiners before nouns. This congruence effect could have reflected a joint representation of the articles and the nouns in mental lexicon (cf. Garrett, 1988). However, on closer inspection, this congruence effect was found to be also fostered by particular masked noun primes—that is, noun primes of a different gender that had different word endings. Some but not all of these noun primes had also gender-prototypical endings. Thus, stimulus factors such as gender-discriminating letters at the end of the subliminal primes or even an overlap between subliminal prime and target letter endings might have contributed to the subliminal congruence effect. This was corroborated in the final Experiment 4. In Experiment 4, we found clear evidence for an article-noun congruence effect when a larger 419 number of targets had to be classified. However, in this experiment, we also observed that the overlap of the article and noun endings supported the congruence effect at least with the feminine targets. Experiment 4 also yielded some evidence for an intention-independent elaborated processing of the subliminal word primes. With noun–noun prime-target pairings, a significant negative congruence effect in a task-unrelated dimension (an animate–unanimate category-membership distinction of the words) was found. This reverse congruence effect meant that even aspects of the subliminal prime-word meanings which were task-irrelevant were processed. The elaborated-processing view would account for this result. The elaborated-processing view would also be in line with the observed top-down dependence of the subliminal articles’ congruence effects in Experiments 2 and 3. In this respect, the elaborated-processing view is as powerful a principle as the conditional-automaticity view. However, it is not certain whether the elaborated-processing view truly accounts for the subliminal morphosyntactic priming effect. In particular, the explanation of an elaborated processing of the subliminal gender status and its active suppression appears less parsimonious than the conditional-automaticity view according to which the masked articles’ gender-specific syntactical role would have simply only be processed with a fitting task set and would have not been processed without such a fitting task set. In conclusion, with respect to human subliminal morphosyntactic processing, our results nicely confirmed some predictions derived from a conditional-automaticity view in particular and a weak-automaticity view in general. They also confirmed previous claims by Sereno (1991) who first concluded the existence of a masked syntactic congruence effect, albeit with a few caveats that we wanted to overcome (such as uncertain degrees of unawareness of her participants). Acknowledgments This work was supported by GRC Grants AN 393/2-1 and AN 393/5-1 (Germany). References Ansorge, U., Fuchs, I., Khalid, S., & Kunde, W. (2011). No conflict control in the absence of awareness. Psychological Research, 75, 351–365. Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2009). Goal-driven attentional capture by invisible colours: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 648–653. Ansorge, U., & Neumann, O. (2005). Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: Evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cueing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 762–777. Ayora, P., Janssen, N., Dell’Acqua, R., & Alario, F.-X. (2009). Attentional requirements for the selection of words from 123 420 different grammatical categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1344–1351. Bargh, J. A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. American Journal of Psychology, 105, 181–199. Bock, K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). Frome conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150–171. Brants, S., Dipper, S., Hansen, S., Lezius, W., & Smith, G. (2002). The TIGER treebank. Proceedings of the Workshop on Treebanks and Linguistic Theories. Sozopol, Bulgaria. Coane, J. H., & Balota, D. A. (2010). Repetition priming across distinct contexts: Effects of lexical status, word frequency, and retrieval test. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2367–2398. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428. Damian, M. F. (2001). Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: Automaticity rather than semantic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 154–164. Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600. Dent, K., Johnston, R. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Age of acquisition and word frequency effects in picture naming: A dual-task investigations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 282–301. Fayol, M., Largy, P., & Lemaire, P. (1994). Cognitive overload and orthographic errors: When cognitive overload enhances subjectverb agreement errors. A study in French written language. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 437–464. Ferreira, V. F., Bock, K., Wilson, M. P., & Cohen, N. J. (2008). Memory for syntax despite amnesia. Psychological Science, 19, 940–946. Ferreira, V. S., & Pashler, H. (2002). Central bottleneck influences on the processing stages of word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 1187–1199. Finkbeiner, M., & Palermo, R. (2009). The role of spatial attention in nonconscious processing: A comparison of face and non-face stimuli. Psychological Science, 20, 42–51. Flores d’Arcais, G. B. (1988). Automatic processes in language comprehension. In B. Denes, C. Semenza, & P. Bisiach (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive neuropsychology (pp. 93–114). Mawah: Leah. Forster, K. I. (1979). Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 27–85). New Jersey: Erlbaum. Forster, K. I. (1998). The pros and cons of masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680–698. Friedrici, A. D., & Jacobsen, T. (1999). Processing grammatical gender during language comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 467–484. Friederici, A. D., & Weissenborn, J. (2007). Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax-semantic interface. Brain Research, 1146, 50–58. Garrett, M. F. (1988). Processes in language production. In F. N. Newmeyer (Ed.), Language: Psychological and biological aspects (pp. 69–96). New York: Cambridge University Press. Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley. Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273, 1699–1702. 123 Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 Gunter, T. C., Friedrici, A. D., & Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 556–568. Haagort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Gender electrified: ERP evidence on the syntactic nature of gender processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 715–728. Hasting, A. S., & Kotz, S. A. (2008). Speeding up syntax: On the relative timing and automaticity of local phrase structure and morphosyntactic processing as reflected in event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1207–1219. Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, J. M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 824–843. Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman, R. Davies, & J. Beatty (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 29–61). New York: Academic Press. Kiefer, M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: Further evidence for a spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 27–39. Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 464–489. Kjellmer, G. (1991). A mint of phrases. In K. Ajmer & B. Altenberg (Eds.), English corpus linguistics. London: Longman. Koester, D., Gunter, T. C., & Wagner, S. (2007). The morphosyntactic decomposition and semantic composition of German compound words investigated by ERP. Brain and Language, 102, 64–79. Kouider, S., & Dupoux, E. (2004). Partial awareness creates the ‘‘illusion’’ of subliminal semantic priming. Psychological Science, 15, 75–81. Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88, 223–242. Kunde, W., Reuss, H., & Kiesel, A. (2012). Consciousness and cognitive control. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 9–18. Marcel, A. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197–237. Martens, U., Ansorge, U., & Kiefer, M. (2011). Controlling the unconscious: Attentional task sets modulate subliminal semantic and visuo-motor processes differentially. Psychological Science, 22, 282–291. Mattler, U. (2006). On the locus of priming and inverse priming effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 975–991. Michelbacher, L., Evert, S., & Schütze, H. (2007). Asymmetric association measures. Proceedings of the international conference on recent advances in natural language processing. Borovets, Bulgaria. Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition, 80, 215–229. Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading of activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254. Norris, D., & Kinoshita, S. (2008). Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference: Insights from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 433–455. Plaut, D. C., & Booth, J. R. (2000). Individual and developmental differences in semantic priming: Empirical and computational Psychological Research (2013) 77:399–421 support for a single-mechanism account of lexical processing. Psychological Review, 107, 786–823. Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y., Hasting, A. S., & Carlyon, R. P. (2008). Syntax as a reflex. Neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing. Brain and Language, 104, 244–253. Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. (1988). Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 563–575. Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. (1990). On the inter-relatedness of theory and measurement in the study of unconscious processes. Mind and Language, 5, 9–28. 421 Reynvoet, B., Gevers, W., & Caessens, B. (2005). Unconscious primes activate motor codes through semantics. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 991–1000. Sereno, J. A. (1991). Graphemic, associative, and syntactic priming effects at a brief stimulus onset asynchrony in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 459–477. Van den Bussche, E., & Reynvoet, B. (2007). Masked priming effects in semantic categorization are independent of category size. Experimental Psychology, 54, 225–235. van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B., & Verguts, T. (2005). Unconscious semantic categorization and mask interactions: An elaborate response to Kunde et al. (2005). Cognition, 97, 107–113. 123