The Language of Death: The Effects of Mortality Salience on Language Style Matching Sandra Alvarez, Renate Beck, Mike Kersten, and Cathy R. Cox Department of Psychology, Texas Christian University Introduction Research on behavioral synchrony suggests that people express greater feelings of attraction when they share their visual gazes, body postures, hand gestures (see e.g., Wheatley, Kang, Parkinson, & Looser, 2012, for a review), and more recently, their language style in writing or speech (e.g., Ireland & Pennebaker, 2010). One question, however, is the extent to which people become more synchronous when under threat. From the perspective of terror management theory (TMT; Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1986), close relationships are important because they serve as a source of comfort and security when mortality concerns are salient (see e.g., Mikulincer, Florian, & Hirschberger, 2003 for a review). However, of the studies that have been conducted, no research has examined the effects of death on people’s synchronous behaviors with one another. The present research examined the extent to which people match their language styles (e.g., pronoun use) following thoughts of mortality. Specifically, It was hypothesized that individuals would engage in higher levels of language style matching following reminders of death. • Next, all participants engaged in an instant messaging conversation with a confederate and all conversations were coded on the extent to which individuals matched their language styles. That is, the degree of stylistic similarity, or attempted language style matching within the written text between individuals was calculated by the program Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC; Pennebaker, Booth, & Francis, 2007). • The results revealed that following reminders of death, participants engaged in greater levels of language style matching. The current research provides additional support for the protective capacity of behavioral synchrony by showing that people engage in greater stylistic similarity through written text when mortality is salient. Further, participants engaged in more language style matching but did not self-report greater liking for the confederate. This points to the importance of how people can unconsciously initiate communicable synchronization in response to existential threat. • Despite its limitations (e.g., college-aged samples, scripted conversation), the results of the present research contribute to the growing body of literature suggesting that relationships serve important functions for dealing with mortality-related concerns (see Mikulincer et al., 2003 for a review). Further, whereas previous research has focused extensively on people’s relationships with romantic partners, the current findings suggest that interactions with strangers can also serve as a source of existential comfort when dealing with reminders of death. References Results • Results revealed that, in comparison to the control condition, participants who were reminded of death engaged in higher levels of language style matching, t(31) = 2.13, p = .04, d = .64. Death 0.85 0.8 0.75 0.7 0.65 0.6 Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror management theory. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private self (pp. 189-212). NewYork: Springer-Verlag. Ireland, M. E., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). Language style matching in writing: Synchrony in essays, correspondence, and poetry. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 99(3), 549-571. 0.9 Language Style Matching • Following a battery of personality questionnaires, participants were randomly assigned to answer 15 true/false questions regarding their death (e.g., “Do you worry about dying?”) or public speaking (e.g., “I am very much afraid of speaking in public”). Next, participants completed a word search puzzle to provide a delay following the mortality salience manipulation. The word search puzzle consisted of 8 neutral words and took approximately 5 min to complete. Transcript Example 2 Transcript Example 1 Method • Thirty-three students (26 female; Mage = 18.81) from Texas Christian University participated in exchange for course credit. The cover story stated that the study was examining “how personality traits influence the development of first impressions.” Discussion Method (Cont.) Public Speaking Ireland, M. E., Slatcher, R. B., Eastwick, P. W., Scissors, L. E., Finkel, E. J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). Language style matching predicts relationship initiation and stability. Psychological Science, 22(1), 39-44. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Hirschberger, G. (2003). The existential function of close relationships: Introducing death into the science of love. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 20-40. Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. E., & Francis, M. E. (2007). Linguistic Inquiry and word count: LIWC2007: Operator’s manual. Austin, TX:LIWC.net Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835– 846. Wheatley, T., Kang, O., Parkinson, C., & Looser, C. E. (2012). From mind perception to mental connection: Synchrony as a mechanism for social understanding. Social And Personality Psychology Compass, 6(8), 589-606 Correspondence for this poster should be addressed to Mike Kersten, Department of Psychology, Texas Christian University, 2800 S. University Dr., Fort Worth, TX, 76129. E-mail address: mike.kersten@tcu.edu