Genetic Foundations of Mind & Behavior Honors Psychology Two Main Questions • • The Classic Nature-Nurture Question: Are there biological reasons for why people are so different from one another? (Behavioral Genetics) The New Nature-Nurture Question: How is the nervous system built? (Developmental Neuroscience) • • Two Main Questions • The Classic Nature-Nurture Question: Are there biological reasons for why people are so different from one another? (Behavioral Genetics) NOT “What matters more--the genes or the environment?” • • What makes a rectangle big? height or length? 6” 9” What makes a rectangle big? Area = height X length! 6” 9” What makes us X? nature or nurture? genes or environment? Genetic and Environmental Forces • Hereditary and environmental interactions are best illustrated by this model: Genetic and Environmental Forces • Hereditary and environmental interactions are best illustrated by this model: • • • Children get all of their genes from their parents. A gene is a segment of DNA that is the code for the production of one particular protein Genes affect development by specifying a protein template by regulating other genes • • • • Children get all of their genes from their parents. Each parent contributes 50% of the child’s genes • • Because chromosomes come in pairs (one from each parent), so do genes Some pairs of genes are identical (homozygous) and some are not (heterozygous) • The difference between homozygous and heterozygous gene pairs (alleles) has implications for how the child’s genotype affects his/her phenotype 2 GENOTYPE/Child → PHENOTYPE/Child 2 GENOTYPE/Child → PHENOTYPE/Child • Two ways children’s genes affect their behavior One gene can control behavior (Mendelian inheritance) Many genes can control behavior (Polygenic inheritance) • • 2 GENOTYPE/Child → PHENOTYPE/Child • A third of genes have two or more different forms, known as alleles. Some physical traits, such as straight hair, require matching “recessive” alleles, one from each parent, for expression. Others, such as curly hair, require only the inheritance of one “dominant” allele, which will override a “recessive” allele from the other parent. When traits are controlled by a single allele, a Mendelian distribution will be observed • • • Mendelian distribution: Mendel’s Peas Mendelian distribution: Hair Color Mendelian distribution: Wizarding Mendelian Psychological Traits • • Like the skin of peas, hair color, and wizarding, some psychological traits also appear to be controlled by a single gene Scott & Fuller (1965) found that all bansenjis were afraid of a novel person, whereas the cocker spaniels were seldom afraid. Was there a genetic link? • • Mendelian Distribution: Fear Responses • • • First, they crossbred pure cockers and pure basenjis. Some hybrids were raised by cockers; some by basenjis. All hybrids were fearful, like purebred basenjis They hypothesized that fearfulness was controlled by a singe dominant gene Mendelian Distribution: Fear Responses • • To test their hypothesis, they crossbred the hybrids. They found the same pattern Mendel found with the peas. FF Ff Mendelian Distribution: Fear Responses • Finally, they backcrossed the original hybrids with purebred cockers. Half were afraid (like the basenji), and half were not (like the cockers), just as one would predict if a single fear-controlling gene were dominant. • Mendelian Distribution: PKU & SLI • • PKU—Phenylketonuria: total language loss and mental retardation (prevented by monitoring the child’s diet) SLI—Specific Language Impairment has been linked to the FOXp2 gene Polygenic Distribution • Most traits and behaviors of psychological interest involve contributions by several genes, such as infant temperament shyness aggression risk-taking behavior empathy TV viewing • • • • • • Polygenic Distribution • When many individuals are tested for a polygenic characteristics, the results follow a normal distribution. What is this kind of figure called? Population Genetics • Population genetics attempts to find a role for genes by looking at differences between people and linking it to (genetic) family history ask how much variation in the behavior of a group is a function of genetic differences (”heritability”, h2) versus environmental differences If h2 = 1, all of the differences stem from genetic differences If h2 = 0, none of the differences stem from genetic differences If h2 = .5, half of the differences stem from genetic • • • • • Variation in Area World A World B Variation due to heights Variation due to lengths How much of the variation in area is a function of height versus width? Variation in Behavior World A World B Variation due to heights Variation due to lengths How much of the variation in behavior is a function of genetic differences versus environmental differences? Heritability • Familiality versus Heritability Traits are familial if members of the same family share them, for whatever reason—whether shared genotypes or shared environments Traits are heritable only if the similarity arises from shared genotypes. In experimental organisms, such as fruit flies, it is possible to control the environment and thereby distinguish effects of genetic similarity vs. effects of environmental similarity. • • • Experiment to Establish Heritability Tryon’s Study of Maze Learning • Tryon (1942) This chart depicts the progress Tryon made in selectively breeding rats for their ability to get through mazes after only a few errors. The critical step Tryon made was to cross-foster the rats (bright rats raised by dull rats, dull rats raised by bright ones). Regardless, offspring scores resembled those of their parents. • • Estimating H2 • • In humans, less control over environmental similarity is possible. Two common strategies: To estimate environmentality—the proportion of variance due to environmental variation, compare identical twins raised apart (some environmental similarity) to identical twins raised together (more environmental similarity) To estimate heritability—the proportion of variance due to genetic variation, compare identical twins raised together (some environmental similarity) to fraternal twins raised together (same amount of environmental similarity) • • Family Studies of IQ Relationship Correlation MZ, reared together 0.86 DZ, reared together 0.60 MZ, reared apart 0.72 unrelated, reared together 0.00 h2= (MZ - DZ) x 2 = (.86-.60) x 2 = .52 e2= (MZrt - MZra) = .86-.72 = .14 3 ENVIRONMENT/Child → PHENOTYPE/Child 3 ENVIRONMENT/Child → PHENOTYPE/Child • Genotypes are expressed differently in different environments. • The “norm of reaction” is the range of all possible phenotypes in relation to all possible environments. Family Studies of IQ Relationship Correlation ! T 0.86 N A T S N O DZ, reared together A C 0.60 T O N S 2 I MZ, reared apart 0.72 H MZ, reared together unrelated, reared together h2= (MZ - DZ) x 2 = (.86-.60) x 2 = .52 0.00 e2= (MZrt - MZra) = .86-.72 = .14 Norm of reaction: Example #1 • For the sake of argument, assume that depression/ happiness is entirely the result of genetics Who is most likely to be depressed? A Mr. A’s identical twin has a history of depression B Mr. B’s identical twin has no history of depression C Mr. C’s fraternal twin has a history of depression D Mr. D’s fraternal twin has no history of depression Norm of reaction: Example #1 A C D B Kendler et al. (1995) Am J Psychiatry, 152, 833-842 tivity (25) that investigated two tasks with right hemisphere dominance demonstrated top-down effects that were specific for the right hemisphere, i.e., from the right middle frontal gyrus (area 46) on right extrastriate areas. It is thus likely that our findings generalize to other lateralized tasks. Although we cannot exclude lateralization contingent on stimulus type in some situations, our results are consistent with previous findings from split-brain patient studies (7) and positron emission tomography (11) showing hemispheric specialization based on task demands. Research on hemispheric specialization should move beyond analyses of asymmetric regional activations and focus more strongly on functional interactions within and between hemispheres. 19. A. W. MacDonald, J. D. Cohen, V. A. Stenger, C. S. Carter, Science 288, 1835 (2000). 20. B. J. Casey et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 8728 (2000). 21. C. S. Carter et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 1944 (2000). National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institute on Drug Abuse, and the National Cancer Institute. We thank our volunteers, K. Amunts for helpful anatomical discussions, and the radiographers at the Research Center Jülich for technical assistance. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/301/5631/384/DC1 Materials and Methods References Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on January 12, 2009 ed) during visuospatial decisions. Note the specificity of this result: Even when the threshold was reduced to P # 0.05, uncorrected, no other significant clusters were found throughout the brain. (B) This schema summarizes the negative findings for right ACC: As indicated by the gray dashed lines, right ACC shows no context-dependent contributions to any left-hemispheric area during visuospatial decisions and none to any left- or right-hemispheric area at all during letter decisions. Norm of reaction: Example #1 Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene REPORTS References and Notes 1. J. L. Bradshaw, N. C. Nettleton, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 51 (1981). 2. J. B. Hellige, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41, 55 (1990). 3. J. C. Marshall, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 72 (1981). 4. K. Hugdahl, R. J. Davidson, The Asymmetrical Brain (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2003). 5. J. Sergent, Psychol. Bull. 93, 481 (1983). 6. C. Chiarello, J. Senehi, M. Soulier, Neuropsychologia 4, 521 (1986). 7. P. M. Corballis, M. G. Funnell, M. S. Gazzaniga, NeuroReport 10, 2183 (1999). 8. C. J. Price, R. J. S. Wise, R. S. J. Frackowiak, Cereb. Cortex 6, 62 (1996). 9. G. R. Fink et al., Nature 382, 626 (1996). 10. L. E. Nystrom et al., Neuroimage 11, 424 (2000). 11. S. M. Courtney, L. G. Ungerleider, K. Keil, J. V. Haxby, Cereb. Cortex 6, 39 (1996). 12. J. Levy, C. Trevarthen, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2, 299 (1976). 13. M. S. Gazzaniga, Brain 123, 1293 (2000). 14. M. I. Posner, S. E. Petersen, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25 (1990). 15. T. Shallice, From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1988). 16. R. Desimone, J. Duncan, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193 (1995). 17. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 18. Using a 2 ! 2 ! 2 factorial design, with letter vs. visuospatial decisions, left vs. right visual fields of presentation, and left vs. right response hand as the three experimental factors, the eight conditions occurred equally often and were varied systematically as blocked conditions in a pseudorandom fashion. 386 23 April 2003; accepted 6 June 2003 Avshalom Caspi,1,2 Karen Sugden,1 Terrie E. Moffitt,1,2* Alan Taylor,1 Ian W. Craig,1 HonaLee Harrington,2 Joseph McClay,1 Jonathan Mill,1 Judy Martin,3 Antony Braithwaite,4 Richie Poulton3 In a prospective-longitudinal study of a representative birth cohort, we tested why stressful experiences lead to depression in some people but not in others. A functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter (5-HT T) gene was found to moderate the influence of stressful life events on depression. Individuals with one or two copies of the short allele of the 5-HT T promoter polymorphism exhibited more depressive symptoms, diagnosable depression, and suicidality in relation to stressful life events than individuals homozygous for the long allele. This epidemiological study thus provides evidence of a gene-by-environment interaction, in which an individual’s response to environmental insults is moderated by his or her genetic makeup. Depression is among the top five leading causes of disability and disease burden throughout the world (1). Across the life span, stressful life events that involve threat, loss, humiliation, or defeat influence the onset and course of depresMedical Research Council Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, PO80 De Crespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK. 2Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA. 3Dunedin School of Medicine, 4Department of Pathology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. 1 *To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: t.moffitt@iop.kcl.ac.uk sion (2–5). However, not all people who encounter a stressful life experience succumb to its depressogenic effect. Diathesis-stress theories of depression predict that individuals’ sensitivity to stressful events depends on their genetic makeup (6, 7). Behavioral genetics research supports this prediction, documenting that the risk of depression after a stressful event is elevated among people who are at high genetic risk and diminished among those at low genetic risk (8). However, whether specific genes exacerbate or buffer the effect of stressful life events on depression is unknown. In this study, a functional polymorphism in the pro- 18 JULY 2003 VOL 301 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org s = allele for “short” serotonin transporter l = allele for “long” serotonin transporter tivity (25) that investigated two tasks with right hemisphere dominance demonstrated top-down effects that were specific for the right hemisphere, i.e., from the right middle frontal gyrus (area 46) on right extrastriate areas. It is thus likely that our findings generalize to other lateralized tasks. Although we cannot exclude lateralization contingent on stimulus type in some situations, our results are consistent with previous findings from split-brain patient studies (7) and positron emission tomography (11) showing hemispheric specialization based on task demands. Research on hemispheric specialization should move beyond analyses of asymmetric regional activations and focus more strongly on functional interactions within and between hemispheres. 19. A. W. MacDonald, J. D. Cohen, V. A. Stenger, C. S. Carter, Science 288, 1835 (2000). 20. B. J. Casey et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 8728 (2000). 21. C. S. Carter et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 1944 (2000). National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institute on Drug Abuse, and the National Cancer Institute. We thank our volunteers, K. Amunts for helpful anatomical discussions, and the radiographers at the Research Center Jülich for technical assistance. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/301/5631/384/DC1 Materials and Methods References Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on January 12, 2009 ed) during visuospatial decisions. Note the specificity of this result: Even when the threshold was reduced to P # 0.05, uncorrected, no other significant clusters were found throughout the brain. (B) This schema summarizes the negative findings for right ACC: As indicated by the gray dashed lines, right ACC shows no context-dependent contributions to any left-hemispheric area during visuospatial decisions and none to any left- or right-hemispheric area at all during letter decisions. Norm of reaction: Example #1 Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene Avshalom Caspi,1,2 Karen Sugden,1 Terrie E. Moffitt,1,2* Alan Taylor,1 Ian W. Craig,1 HonaLee Harrington,2 Joseph McClay,1 Jonathan Mill,1 Judy Martin,3 Antony Braithwaite,4 Richie Poulton3 References and Notes 1. J. L. Bradshaw, N. C. Nettleton, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 51 (1981). 2. J. B. Hellige, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41, 55 (1990). 3. J. C. Marshall, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 72 (1981). 4. K. Hugdahl, R. J. Davidson, The Asymmetrical Brain (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2003). 5. J. Sergent, Psychol. Bull. 93, 481 (1983). 6. C. Chiarello, J. Senehi, M. Soulier, Neuropsychologia 4, 521 (1986). 7. P. M. Corballis, M. G. Funnell, M. S. Gazzaniga, NeuroReport 10, 2183 (1999). 8. C. J. Price, R. J. S. Wise, R. S. J. Frackowiak, Cereb. Cortex 6, 62 (1996). 9. G. R. Fink et al., Nature 382, 626 (1996). 10. L. E. Nystrom et al., Neuroimage 11, 424 (2000). 11. S. M. Courtney, L. G. Ungerleider, K. Keil, J. V. Haxby, Cereb. Cortex 6, 39 (1996). 12. J. Levy, C. Trevarthen, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2, 299 (1976). 13. M. S. Gazzaniga, Brain 123, 1293 (2000). 14. M. I. Posner, S. E. Petersen, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25 (1990). 15. T. Shallice, From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1988). 16. R. Desimone, J. Duncan, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193 (1995). 17. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 18. Using a 2 ! 2 ! 2 factorial design, with letter vs. visuospatial decisions, left vs. right visual fields of presentation, and left vs. right response hand as the three experimental factors, the eight conditions occurred equally often and were varied systematically as blocked conditions in a pseudorandom fashion. 386 23 April 2003; accepted 6 June 2003 In a prospective-longitudinal study of a representative birth cohort, we tested why stressful experiences lead to depression in some people but not in others. A functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter (5-HT T) gene was found to moderate the influence of stressful life events on depression. Individuals with one or two copies of the short allele of the 5-HT T promoter polymorphism exhibited more depressive symptoms, diagnosable depression, and suicidality in relation to stressful life events than individuals homozygous for the long allele. This epidemiological study thus provides evidence of a gene-by-environment interaction, in which an individual’s response to environmental insults is moderated by his or her genetic makeup. Depression is among the top five leading causes of disability and disease burden throughout the world (1). Across the life span, stressful life events that involve threat, loss, humiliation, or defeat influence the onset and course of depresMedical Research Council Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, PO80 De Crespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK. 2Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA. 3Dunedin School of Medicine, 4Department of Pathology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. 1 *To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: t.moffitt@iop.kcl.ac.uk sion (2–5). However, not all people who encounter a stressful life experience succumb to its depressogenic effect. Diathesis-stress theories of depression predict that individuals’ sensitivity to stressful events depends on their genetic makeup (6, 7). Behavioral genetics research supports this prediction, documenting that the risk of depression after a stressful event is elevated among people who are at high genetic risk and diminished among those at low genetic risk (8). However, whether specific genes exacerbate or buffer the effect of stressful life events on depression is unknown. In this study, a functional polymorphism in the pro- 18 JULY 2003 VOL 301 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org s = allele for “short” serotonin transporter l = allele for “long” serotonin transporter tivity (25) that investigated two tasks with right hemisphere dominance demonstrated top-down effects that were specific for the right hemisphere, i.e., from the right middle frontal gyrus (area 46) on right extrastriate areas. It is thus likely that our findings generalize to other lateralized tasks. Although we cannot exclude lateralization contingent on stimulus type in some situations, our results are consistent with previous findings from split-brain patient studies (7) and positron emission tomography (11) showing hemispheric specialization based on task demands. Research on hemispheric specialization should move beyond analyses of asymmetric regional activations and focus more strongly on functional interactions within and between hemispheres. 19. A. W. MacDonald, J. D. Cohen, V. A. Stenger, C. S. Carter, Science 288, 1835 (2000). 20. B. J. Casey et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 8728 (2000). 21. C. S. Carter et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 1944 (2000). National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institute on Drug Abuse, and the National Cancer Institute. We thank our volunteers, K. Amunts for helpful anatomical discussions, and the radiographers at the Research Center Jülich for technical assistance. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/301/5631/384/DC1 Materials and Methods References Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on January 12, 2009 ed) during visuospatial decisions. Note the specificity of this result: Even when the threshold was reduced to P # 0.05, uncorrected, no other significant clusters were found throughout the brain. (B) This schema summarizes the negative findings for right ACC: As indicated by the gray dashed lines, right ACC shows no context-dependent contributions to any left-hemispheric area during visuospatial decisions and none to any left- or right-hemispheric area at all during letter decisions. Norm of reaction: Example #1 Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene Avshalom Caspi,1,2 Karen Sugden,1 Terrie E. Moffitt,1,2* Alan Taylor,1 Ian W. Craig,1 HonaLee Harrington,2 Joseph McClay,1 Jonathan Mill,1 Judy Martin,3 Antony Braithwaite,4 Richie Poulton3 References and Notes 1. J. L. Bradshaw, N. C. Nettleton, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 51 (1981). 2. J. B. Hellige, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41, 55 (1990). 3. J. C. Marshall, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 72 (1981). 4. K. Hugdahl, R. J. Davidson, The Asymmetrical Brain (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2003). 5. J. Sergent, Psychol. Bull. 93, 481 (1983). 6. C. Chiarello, J. Senehi, M. Soulier, Neuropsychologia 4, 521 (1986). 7. P. M. Corballis, M. G. Funnell, M. S. Gazzaniga, NeuroReport 10, 2183 (1999). 8. C. J. Price, R. J. S. Wise, R. S. J. Frackowiak, Cereb. Cortex 6, 62 (1996). 9. G. R. Fink et al., Nature 382, 626 (1996). 10. L. E. Nystrom et al., Neuroimage 11, 424 (2000). 11. S. M. Courtney, L. G. Ungerleider, K. Keil, J. V. Haxby, Cereb. Cortex 6, 39 (1996). 12. J. Levy, C. Trevarthen, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2, 299 (1976). 13. M. S. Gazzaniga, Brain 123, 1293 (2000). 14. M. I. Posner, S. E. Petersen, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25 (1990). 15. T. Shallice, From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1988). 16. R. Desimone, J. Duncan, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193 (1995). 17. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 18. Using a 2 ! 2 ! 2 factorial design, with letter vs. visuospatial decisions, left vs. right visual fields of presentation, and left vs. right response hand as the three experimental factors, the eight conditions occurred equally often and were varied systematically as blocked conditions in a pseudorandom fashion. 386 23 April 2003; accepted 6 June 2003 In a prospective-longitudinal study of a representative birth cohort, we tested why stressful experiences lead to depression in some people but not in others. A functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter (5-HT T) gene was found to moderate the influence of stressful life events on depression. Individuals with one or two copies of the short allele of the 5-HT T promoter polymorphism exhibited more depressive symptoms, diagnosable depression, and suicidality in relation to stressful life events than individuals homozygous for the long allele. This epidemiological study thus provides evidence of a gene-by-environment interaction, in which an individual’s response to environmental insults is moderated by his or her genetic makeup. Depression is among the top five leading causes of disability and disease burden throughout the world (1). Across the life span, stressful life events that involve threat, loss, humiliation, or defeat influence the onset and course of depresMedical Research Council Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, PO80 De Crespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK. 2Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA. 3Dunedin School of Medicine, 4Department of Pathology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. 1 *To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: t.moffitt@iop.kcl.ac.uk sion (2–5). However, not all people who encounter a stressful life experience succumb to its depressogenic effect. Diathesis-stress theories of depression predict that individuals’ sensitivity to stressful events depends on their genetic makeup (6, 7). Behavioral genetics research supports this prediction, documenting that the risk of depression after a stressful event is elevated among people who are at high genetic risk and diminished among those at low genetic risk (8). However, whether specific genes exacerbate or buffer the effect of stressful life events on depression is unknown. In this study, a functional polymorphism in the pro- s = allele for “short” serotonin transporter l = allele for “long” serotonin transporter 18 JULY 2003 VOL 301 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org Fig. 1. Results of multiple regression analyses estimating the associati tivity (25) that investigated two tasks with right hemisphere dominance demonstrated top-down effects that were specific for the right hemisphere, i.e., from the right middle frontal gyrus (area 46) on right extrastriate areas. It is thus likely that our findings generalize to other lateralized tasks. Although we cannot exclude lateralization contingent on stimulus type in some situations, our results are consistent with previous findings from split-brain patient studies (7) and positron emission tomography (11) showing hemispheric specialization based on task demands. Research on hemispheric specialization should move beyond analyses of asymmetric regional activations and focus more strongly on functional interactions within and between hemispheres. 19. A. W. MacDonald, J. D. Cohen, V. A. Stenger, C. S. Carter, Science 288, 1835 (2000). 20. B. J. Casey et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 8728 (2000). 21. C. S. Carter et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 1944 (2000). National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institute on Drug Abuse, and the National Cancer Institute. We thank our volunteers, K. Amunts for helpful anatomical discussions, and the radiographers at the Research Center Jülich for technical assistance. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/301/5631/384/DC1 Materials and Methods References Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on January 12, 2009 ed) during visuospatial decisions. Note the specificity of this result: Even when the threshold was reduced to P # 0.05, uncorrected, no other significant clusters were found throughout the brain. (B) This schema summarizes the negative findings for right ACC: As indicated by the gray dashed lines, right ACC shows no context-dependent contributions to any left-hemispheric area during visuospatial decisions and none to any left- or right-hemispheric area at all during letter decisions. Norm of reaction: Example #1 Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene Avshalom Caspi,1,2 Karen Sugden,1 Terrie E. Moffitt,1,2* Alan Taylor,1 Ian W. Craig,1 HonaLee Harrington,2 Joseph McClay,1 Jonathan Mill,1 Judy Martin,3 Antony Braithwaite,4 Richie Poulton3 References and Notes 1. J. L. Bradshaw, N. C. Nettleton, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 51 (1981). 2. J. B. Hellige, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41, 55 (1990). 3. J. C. Marshall, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 72 (1981). 4. K. Hugdahl, R. J. Davidson, The Asymmetrical Brain (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2003). 5. J. Sergent, Psychol. Bull. 93, 481 (1983). 6. C. Chiarello, J. Senehi, M. Soulier, Neuropsychologia 4, 521 (1986). 7. P. M. Corballis, M. G. Funnell, M. S. Gazzaniga, NeuroReport 10, 2183 (1999). 8. C. J. Price, R. J. S. Wise, R. S. J. Frackowiak, Cereb. Cortex 6, 62 (1996). 9. G. R. Fink et al., Nature 382, 626 (1996). 10. L. E. Nystrom et al., Neuroimage 11, 424 (2000). 11. S. M. Courtney, L. G. Ungerleider, K. Keil, J. V. Haxby, Cereb. Cortex 6, 39 (1996). 12. J. Levy, C. Trevarthen, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2, 299 (1976). 13. M. S. Gazzaniga, Brain 123, 1293 (2000). 14. M. I. Posner, S. E. Petersen, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25 (1990). 15. T. Shallice, From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1988). 16. R. Desimone, J. Duncan, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193 (1995). 17. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 18. Using a 2 ! 2 ! 2 factorial design, with letter vs. visuospatial decisions, left vs. right visual fields of presentation, and left vs. right response hand as the three experimental factors, the eight conditions occurred equally often and were varied systematically as blocked conditions in a pseudorandom fashion. 386 23 April 2003; accepted 6 June 2003 In a prospective-longitudinal study of a representative birth cohort, we tested why stressful experiences lead to depression in some people but not in others. A functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter (5-HT T) gene was found to moderate the influence of stressful life events on depression. Individuals with one or two copies of the short allele of the 5-HT T promoter polymorphism exhibited more depressive symptoms, diagnosable depression, and suicidality in relation to stressful life events than individuals homozygous for the long allele. This epidemiological study thus provides evidence of a gene-by-environment interaction, in which an individual’s response to environmental insults is moderated by his or her genetic makeup. Depression is among the top five leading causes of disability and disease burden throughout the world (1). Across the life span, stressful life events that involve threat, loss, humiliation, or defeat influence the onset and course of depresMedical Research Council Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, PO80 De Crespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK. 2Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA. 3Dunedin School of Medicine, 4Department of Pathology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. 1 *To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: t.moffitt@iop.kcl.ac.uk sion (2–5). However, not all people who encounter a stressful life experience succumb to its depressogenic effect. Diathesis-stress theories of depression predict that individuals’ sensitivity to stressful events depends on their genetic makeup (6, 7). Behavioral genetics research supports this prediction, documenting that the risk of depression after a stressful event is elevated among people who are at high genetic risk and diminished among those at low genetic risk (8). However, whether specific genes exacerbate or buffer the effect of stressful life events on depression is unknown. In this study, a functional polymorphism in the pro- 18 JULY 2003 VOL 301 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org ssion analyses estimating the association between number of Fig. 2. Results of regres s = association allele for betwe the “short” serotonin ment (between the age transporter adult depression (ages of 5-HT T genotype. A l = allele for mozygotes, 92 (63%), “long”members serotonin study were i probable maltreatmen transporter ment groups, respectiv heterozygotes, 286 (66 (8%) were in the no maltreatment groups. mozygotes, 172 (65%) tivity (25) that investigated two tasks with right hemisphere dominance demonstrated top-down effects that were specific for the right hemisphere, i.e., from the right middle frontal gyrus (area 46) on right extrastriate areas. It is thus likely that our findings generalize to other lateralized tasks. Although we cannot exclude lateralization contingent on stimulus type in some situations, our results are consistent with previous findings from split-brain patient studies (7) and positron emission tomography (11) showing hemispheric specialization based on task demands. Research on hemispheric specialization should move beyond analyses of asymmetric regional activations and focus more strongly on functional interactions within and between hemispheres. 19. A. W. MacDonald, J. D. Cohen, V. A. Stenger, C. S. Carter, Science 288, 1835 (2000). 20. B. J. Casey et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 8728 (2000). 21. C. S. Carter et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 1944 (2000). National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institute on Drug Abuse, and the National Cancer Institute. We thank our volunteers, K. Amunts for helpful anatomical discussions, and the radiographers at the Research Center Jülich for technical assistance. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/301/5631/384/DC1 Materials and Methods References Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on January 12, 2009 ed) during visuospatial decisions. Note the specificity of this result: Even when the threshold was reduced to P # 0.05, uncorrected, no other significant clusters were found throughout the brain. (B) This schema summarizes the negative findings for right ACC: As indicated by the gray dashed lines, right ACC shows no context-dependent contributions to any left-hemispheric area during visuospatial decisions and none to any left- or right-hemispheric area at all during letter decisions. Norm of reaction: Example #1 Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene Avshalom Caspi,1,2 Karen Sugden,1 Terrie E. Moffitt,1,2* Alan Taylor,1 Ian W. Craig,1 HonaLee Harrington,2 Joseph McClay,1 Jonathan Mill,1 Judy Martin,3 Antony Braithwaite,4 Richie Poulton3 References and Notes 1. J. L. Bradshaw, N. C. Nettleton, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 51 (1981). 2. J. B. Hellige, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 41, 55 (1990). 3. J. C. Marshall, Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 72 (1981). 4. K. Hugdahl, R. J. Davidson, The Asymmetrical Brain (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2003). 5. J. Sergent, Psychol. Bull. 93, 481 (1983). 6. C. Chiarello, J. Senehi, M. Soulier, Neuropsychologia 4, 521 (1986). 7. P. M. Corballis, M. G. Funnell, M. S. Gazzaniga, NeuroReport 10, 2183 (1999). 8. C. J. Price, R. J. S. Wise, R. S. J. Frackowiak, Cereb. Cortex 6, 62 (1996). 9. G. R. Fink et al., Nature 382, 626 (1996). 10. L. E. Nystrom et al., Neuroimage 11, 424 (2000). 11. S. M. Courtney, L. G. Ungerleider, K. Keil, J. V. Haxby, Cereb. Cortex 6, 39 (1996). 12. J. Levy, C. Trevarthen, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2, 299 (1976). 13. M. S. Gazzaniga, Brain 123, 1293 (2000). 14. M. I. Posner, S. E. Petersen, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25 (1990). 15. T. Shallice, From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1988). 16. R. Desimone, J. Duncan, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193 (1995). 17. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 18. Using a 2 ! 2 ! 2 factorial design, with letter vs. visuospatial decisions, left vs. right visual fields of presentation, and left vs. right response hand as the three experimental factors, the eight conditions occurred equally often and were varied systematically as blocked conditions in a pseudorandom fashion. 386 23 April 2003; accepted 6 June 2003 In a prospective-longitudinal study of a representative birth cohort, we tested why stressful experiences lead to depression in some people but not in others. A functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter (5-HT T) gene was found to moderate the influence of stressful life events on depression. Individuals with one or two copies of the short allele of the 5-HT T promoter polymorphism exhibited more depressive symptoms, diagnosable depression, and suicidality in relation to stressful life events than individuals homozygous for the long allele. This epidemiological study thus provides evidence of a gene-by-environment interaction, in which an individual’s response to environmental insults is moderated by his or her genetic makeup. Depression is among the top five leading causes of disability and disease burden throughout the world (1). Across the life span, stressful life events that involve threat, loss, humiliation, or defeat influence the onset and course of depresMedical Research Council Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, PO80 De Crespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK. 2Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA. 3Dunedin School of Medicine, 4Department of Pathology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand. 1 *To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: t.moffitt@iop.kcl.ac.uk sion (2–5). However, not all people who encounter a stressful life experience succumb to its depressogenic effect. Diathesis-stress theories of depression predict that individuals’ sensitivity to stressful events depends on their genetic makeup (6, 7). Behavioral genetics research supports this prediction, documenting that the risk of depression after a stressful event is elevated among people who are at high genetic risk and diminished among those at low genetic risk (8). However, whether specific genes exacerbate or buffer the effect of stressful life events on depression is unknown. In this study, a functional polymorphism in the pro- 18 JULY 2003 VOL 301 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org Fig. 2. Results of regression analysis estimating s = allele for “short” serotonin transporter l = allele for “long” serotonin transporter Norm of Reaction • Even when there is a substantial genetic contribution to a psychological trait, the amount of variation in the trait that is explained by genetic similarity (i.e., heritability) depends on the subjects’ environment PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Norm of reaction: Example #2 • • Turkheimer et al. (2003) calculated IQ heritability for twins who differed in SES Sample median family income $22,000 (in 1997 dollars); 1997 US median $53,000 Fig. 3. Proportion of total Full-Scale IQ variance accounted for by A, C, and E plotted as a function of obser Norm of Reaction • • Does the norm of reaction imply that nature’s role is somehow arbitrary, uncertain, or unreal? Consider PKU disorder In 100% of cases, the defective allele creates a defective enzyme that is from 0-50% effective as normal in breaking down phenylalanine In 100% of cases the build up of phenylalanine is poisonous to the brain and results in severe mental retardation Norm of reaction still applies: In an environment with NO phenylalanine, the defective gene won’t make any difference in behavior In an environment WITH phenylalanine, the gene will make a huge difference in behavior • • • • • 4. Child’s Phenotype—Child’s Environment 4. Child’s Phenotype—Child’s Environment • Children are active sources of their own development in two ways: They actively evoke certain responses from others (e.g., calm, beautiful babies) They actively select surroundings and experiences conducive to their innate interests, talents, and personality characteristics. h2 + e2 ≠ 1 • • • Developmental Neuroscience • • So far we’ve been talking about genes that vary from person to person, but genes shared by (virtually) all people do a lot of work recipe for bodies recipe for brains Developmental neuroscience is interested in the recipe (and cooking) of the nervous system • •