Laboratory Notes Psych 312: Sensation Seeking and Alcohol Consumption Research indicates that one of the major personality variables related to alcohol use is sensation seeking (e.g., Cohen & Fromme, 2002; Zuckerman, 1994) in addition to other factors such as gender, familial patterns, and family history of alcohol use (see review by Roberti, 2004). Sensation seeking is a generalized preference for high or low levels of sensory stimulation. People who are high in sensation seeking prefer a high level of stimulation and are always looking for novel and exhilarating experiences. In contrast, people who are low in sensation seeking prefer more modest levels of stimulation and tend to choose tranquility over excitement (Weiten, 2004). Sensation seeking was first described by Zuckerman (1971; 1974) as a personality trait with a strong genetic predisposition (see the history of the identification of this personality trait and the scale’s development in Zuckerman, Kolin, Price & Zoob, 1981). To determine whether one is high, moderate, or low in sensation seeking, Zuckerman (1984) created a Sensation Seeking Scale. Based on this scale, those high in sensation seeking (often termed “sensation seekers”) display the following four sets of characteristics (Zuckerman, 1979; 1994): 1) Thrill and adventure seeking. They are more willing to engage in activities that may involve a physical risk. Thus, they are more likely to go mountain climbing, skydiving, surfing, and scuba diving; 2) Experience seeking . They are more willing to volunteer for unusual experiments or activities that they may know little about. They tend to relish extensive travel, provocative art, wild parties, and unusual friends; 3) Disinhibition. They are relatively uninhibited. Hence they are prone to engaging in heavy drinking, recreational drug use, gambling, and sexual experimentation; 4) Susceptibility to boredom. They do not like monotony, have a low tolerance for routine and repetition, and they quickly and easily become bored. Interestingly, in terms of mental health, high sensation seekers are relatively tolerant of stress; they find many types of potentially stressful events to be less threatening and anxiety arousing than others (Franken, Gibson & Rowland, 1992). However, high sensation seekers are more likely than others to exhibit impulsive, last-minute decision making (Franken, 1993), poor health habits (Watanabe, 1998), reckless driving (Jonah, 1997) and high-risk sexual behavior (McCoul & Haslam, 2001). High sensation seeking is also associated with problem drinking (Parent & Newman, 1999), recreational drug use and gambling (Zuckerman & Kuhlman, 2000), delinquency (Luengo et al., 1994), and criminal behavior (Arnett, 1996). For a recent review on behaviors and other personality traits associated with high sensation seekers, see Roberti (2004). Also, see Martin Zuckerman’s interview published in Psychology Today (Zuckerman, 2000) where he discusses behaviors as well as biological characteristics associated with high sensation seeking. This lab will examine the relationship between sensation seeking and early detection of hazardous alcohol consumption among a small sample of college undergraduates. As mentioned above, research suggests that high sensation seeking is associated with hazardous drinking. We will determine whether the use of a brief sensation seeking questionnaire (Grasha & Kirschenbaum, 1986) and a brief alcohol consumption questionnaire (Saunders et al., 1993) are sufficiently sensitive to support previous research findings that indicate an association between high sensation seeking and hazardous drinking. Method: Early detection of hazardous alcohol consumption will be assessed using the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) developed by Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente, and Grant (1993). This alcohol assessment was developed to detect problem drinkers who may not yet be classified as alcoholics. It is a relatively short assessment that was derived from a cross-national data set and hence appears to be culturally unbiased. The actual questions contained in the AUDIT are presented in the Saunders et al. (1993) appendix (pp. 803-804) To obtain a rough estimate of whether one is high or moderate/low in sensation seeking, a sensation seeking scale presented in Grasha and Kirschenbaum (1986) will be used. This scale can be viewed by selecting “SS Scale” 1. Please note that this sensation seeking scale provides only a rough approximation of one’s status on this personality trait. Also, unlike the scale developed by Zuckerman (1984)2 , it does not provide separate measures along the four characteristics of sensation seeking (thrill and adventure seeking, experience seeking, disinhibition, and susceptibility to boredom). Instead, these separate measures are combined. Results of this scale will be summarized as either 1) High sensation seeking or 2) moderate/low sensation seeking in contrast to the typical way that the scale is scored since we are only interested in identifying those who are high sensation seekers and those who are not. The scoring methods used for each scale are summarized at the bottom of each scale Results: A correlation will be used to analyze whether an increase in sensation seeking is positively correlated with an increase in sensation seeking. Note: Although the cutoff scores for the Audit and Sensation Seeking Scale are typically used for these scales (Audit = harmful drinking: score of 8 or higher vs. non-harmful drinking: score of 7 or lower; Sensation Seeking Scale = high sensation seeking: score of 11 or higher vs. moderate to low sensation seeking: score of 10 or below), you will be conducting a correlation based on the actual scores obtained from these two scales for each participant. The cutoff scores will not be used, but you can use them to get an idea if you engage in harmful vs. non-harmful drinking and whether you are high, moderate, or low in sensation seeking. Footnotes: 1 For an example the Grasha and Kirschenbaum (1986) Sensation Seeking Questionnaire used in this study see “SS Scale”; If you would like to determine whether you are high, moderate, or low based on this scale, go to: http://hksrch.com.hk/quiz/sen2.htm 2 For an example of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality (Sensation Seeking) Questionnaire typically used to assess sensation seeking go to: http://www.rta.nsw.gov.au/licensing/tests/driverqualificationtest/sensationseekingscale/ References: Cohen, E. S., & Fromme, K. (2002). Differential determinants of young adult substance use and high-risk sexual behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(6), 1124-1150. Franken, R. E. (1993). Sensation seeking and keeping your options open. Personality and Individual Differences, 14(1), 247-249. Franken, R. E., Gibson, K. J., & Rowland, G. L. (1992). Sensation seeking and the tendency to view the world as threatening. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(1), 31-38. Grasha, A. F., & Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1986). Adjustment and competence: Concepts and application. Saint-Paul, MN: West. Jonah, B. A. (1997). Sensation seeking and risky driving: A review and synthesis of the literature. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 29 (5), 651-665. Luengo, M. A., Otero, J. M., Carrillo-de- la-Pena, M. T., & Miron, L. (1994). Dimensions of antisocial behavior in juvenile delinquency: A study of personality variables. Psychology, Crime & Law, 1(1), 27-37. McCoul, M. D., & Haslam, N. (2001). Predicting high risk sexual behavior in heterosexual and homosexual men: The roles of impulsivity and sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 31(8), 1303-1310. Parent, E. C., & Newman, D. L. (1999). The role of sensation-seeking in alcohol use and risk-taking behavior among college women. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 44(2), 12-28. Roberti, J. W. (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(3), 256-279. Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption: II. Addiction, 88(6), 791-804. Saunders, J. B., Baily, S., Phillips, M., & Allsop, S. (1993). Women with alcohol problems: Do they relapse for reasons different to their male counterparts? Addiction, 88(10), 1413-1422. Watanabe, M. (1998). The relationship between sensation seeking and health risk behavior: A survey on traffic-related risk behavior, cigarette smoking and alcoho l drinking among university students. Japanese Journal of Health Psychology, 11(1), 28-38. Weiten, W. (2004). Psychology: Themes and variations. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Zuckerman, M. (1971). Dimensions of sensation seeking. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 36(1), 45-52. Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Zuckerman, M. (1984). Sensation seeking: A comparative approach to a human trait. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(3), 413-471. Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Zuckerman, M. (2000). Are you a risk taker? Psychology Today, 33, 52-57. Zuckerman, M., Kolin, E. A., Price, L., & Zoob, I. (1964). Development of a sensation-seeking scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 28, 477-482. Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: Common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68(6), 999-1029. Zuckerman, M., Murtaugh, T., & Siegel, J. (1974). Sensation seeking and cortical augmentingreducing. Psychophysiology, 11(5), 535-542.