J. Field Ornithol., 58(2):190-197 ORGANIZATION MEREDITH OF SINGING IN HOUSE E. PLATT • AND MILLICENT WRENS S. FICKEN Departmentof BiologicalSciences Universityof Wisconsin-Milwaukee Milwaukee, Wisconsin 53207 USA Abstract.--House Wren songsare complexand the repertoiresof individualsare large. Most of the songsin this study were composed of nine syllablesand they were most frequentlymade up of only two different syllabletypes.The numberof repetitionsof a particularsyllabletype varied.Syllablearrangementwas alsovariable;particularsyllables usuallywere not foundin only one specificpart of the song.There was frequentsyllable sharingamongbirdsof this population,but little songtype sharingoccurred.Often, part of the songwas sharedbut the presence of uniquesyllablesproducedmany differentsong types and, consequently,distinctsongrepertoiresamong birds. A numberof theorieshavebeenproposed to explainthe evolutionof complexsongsand large repertoiresin birds.In HouseWrens large repertoiresmay be involvedin enhancing territorial defensealthoughone hypothesisrelated to territorial defense,the Beau Geste hypothesis,was rejected.Also, large repertoiresmay have evolvedthroughfemale choice. ORGANIZACI(•NDEL CANTO DEL REYEZUELO(TROGLODYTES AEDON) Sinopsis.--E1cantodc Troglodytes aedoncscomplcjoy conun ampliorcpcrtorio.La mayorcadc las cancioncs tichen9 silabas,aunqucs61odosdc cstassondifcrcntcs.La rcpctici6n dc una silabaparticularvaria entreindividuos.E1 arrcglodc las silabastambigncsvariable. No sc cncontr6silabasparticulatesquc cstuvicrancolocadas cn partescspcc[ficas dc la canci6n.Aunqucpartcdc la canci6ncscompartida, la prcscncia dc s•labasparticulates da origen a muchascancioncsdifcrcntcsy pot cndc a un gran rcpcrtorio.Sc hah propucsto variastcoriaspara cxplicarla cvoluciondc complcjos dc canclones y rcpcrtorioscn aves. En cl ave cstudiadacs posiblcquc cl gran rcpcrtoriopucda scr dc utilidad cn la dcfcnza dcl tcrritorio, aunqucla hipotcsisBeau Gcstccsrcchazada.Un gran rcpcrtoriopucdchabcr cvolucionado comorcspucstaa la sclccci6ndc parcja pot partc dc hcmbras. Compared to most other North American wren species,the House Wren (Troglodytes aedon)has fairly complexsongsand individualshave large songrepertoires(Kroodsma1977). The objectives of this studyare to (1) determinesongorganizationand repertoire size of individualsin a local population,(2) determinethe degreeof syllableand songtype sharingin a population,and (3) test the predictionof the Beau Geste hypothesis (Krebs1977) that songtypechanges occurconcomitantly with perch switches. METHODS Songsof four male HouseWrens were recordedat the Universityof Wisconsin-MilwaukeeField Station,Saukville(OzaukeeCounty) Wisconsinbetween7 June and 23 July, 1982. Songswere recordedat various timesof the day (0530-1600, C.D.T.) for any particular individual. Two of the birds had territoriesin an upland apple orchard,one was located • Presentaddress.' Invertebrate ZoologySection,MilwaukeePublicMuseum,800 W. WellsSt., Milwaukee, Wisconsin53233 USA. 190 Vol.58,No.2 Singing inHouse Wrens [ 19 1 in a low brushy area adjacentto an open field, and one occupiedan upland area of primarily aspen (Populustrernuloides). The territoriesof Birds I, III, and IV were contiguous.Bird II was locatedapproximately 100 m from the other three. All were probablywithin hearingrange of one another and these were the only House Wrens in the immediate area. Althoughthe birds were not banded,individualswere easilyidentified by territorial behavior;they tended to sing at or near their own nestsitesand only rarely enteredanotherbird's territory. A Uher 4200 Reporttape recorderand an Electrovoice644 directional microphonewere usedto recordthe songs.Informationdescribingsinging locationswas recordedand distancesbetweensongpercheswere measured, enabling comparisonsto be made betweensongtypes given and distancesmovedbetweensongs.Intervals betweensuccessive songstarts were also recorded.Sonagramswere made with a Kay 6061 B SonaGraph at an intermediateband width setting(150 Hz). Identificationof syllableswasprimarily by shape,usingfrequencyand temporal characteristics.Letter symbolswere assignedto syllablesto facilitateanalysis.Sequences of identicalsyllabletypes,regardlessof the numberof repetitionsof eachtype, characterizedunique songtypes.For example, AAAAABBBBBKKKK and AAAAABBBKKKKKKK were consideredto be the same song type but they were not the same as AAAAAJJJJKKKKKKK. RESULTS Songdescription and organization.--HouseWren songconsistsof two parts.The introductorypart is madeup of very low amplitude"chattery" notes,but the terminal sectioncontainsclearer,louder, "bubbly" notes. As the introductorypart of the songin our recordingswas too faint to be analyzedaccurately,this report deals only with the louder, terminal portion.Severalsongcharacteristics (e.g.,total syllablenumberper song, numberof differentsyllablesper song,syllableorder)variedconsiderably amongbirds. Figure 1 illustratesthe syllablesin the study population and Figure 2 showsseveralrepresentativesongsof four different individuals,demonstratingthe ways in which syllablesare combined. Of the 1993 songsanalyzed,the total number of syllablesper song ranged from 3 to 22, with a mean of 11.49 (SD = 12.06). The mode was 9. The number of different syllabletypesper songrangedfrom 1 to 6, with a mean of 2.92 (SD = 3.10) and a mode of 2. Order and numberof repetitionsof eachsyllabletype oftenvaried in different songs,even in the same individual. Most syllableswere not specificto a particularpart of the song.For example,if a certainsyllable occurredin the beginningof a particular songand was also presentin anothersong,it did not necessarilyoccurin the beginningof that other song. Syllablesranged in frequencyfrom 1.5 to 8.0 KHz. Song duration rangedfrom 0.33 to 2.48 s (N = 220) with a mean of 1.25 (SD = 0.35). 192] A B D M. E. PlattandM. S.Ficken E F G J K O P T j. Field Ornithol. Spring 1987 U AB CDE F H I J KL i T U W•Z A B C E H I J K O Q X Y FIGURE 1. Syllablesin the songs(terminal, "bubbly" sectionsonly) of House Wrens at the Universityof Wisconsin-MilwaukeeField Station.(a) Bird I (b) Bird II (c) Bird III (d) Bird IV. Generally,malessangmostrapidly when a femalewas closeto or in the nest. Amountof syllablesharing.--Examination of sonagramsrevealedan extensiveamount of syllablesharing.Sevenof the 26 syllablesusedby the four birds were commonto all. Six other syllableswere sharedby three out of the four birds. Twelve syllablesmay have been unique to individuals.Bird I shared l0 of its 12 syllableswith at least one other individual, Bird II shared13 of its 16 syllables,Bird III shared14 of its 19 syllables,and Bird IV shared 11 of its 13 syllables.Coincidence indices(Dice 1945) were calculatedto show degreeof similarity of syllable typesbetweenpairs of birds. In the equation Coincidenceindex = 2h(a + b) h = number of sharedsyllables,a --- number of syllablesof Bird A, and b -- numberof syllablesof Bird B. Values of the coincidence index range from 0, indicatingcompletelack of sharingof songsyllablesof the two individuals,to 1.0, indicating completesharing. In this study, values FIGURE2. Examplesof House Wren songsshowingthe songtype variability that may occurboth amongand within individualbirds. (a) Bird I (b) Bird II (c) Bird III (d) Bird IV. Vol.58,No.2 Singing inHouse Wrens [ 193 8 ' 0 ,," DE D T U - J K ; Time Time (sec) (sec) 8 , •. T U L B 1 Time T•me (sec) (sec) 8 .•. • K ' • m I I • I I B . Time (sec) B Time (8ec) s T K ø•' 1½I'i,!.•, •2 I K B J T•me K (sec) Time 0 Time (sec) B DE Time (sec) 194] m. E. PlattandM. S.Ficken T^•Lw Bird I (12) a IX (16) III (19) IV (13) 1. J.Field Ornithol. Spring 1987 Coincidenceindicesfor syllablesharing. II 0.71 III 0.65 0.74 IV 0.56 0.69 0.69 Total number of different syllables. rangedfrom 0.56 to 0.74 (Table 1), with a mean value of 0.67 (SD = 0.063), indicatinga relativelyhigh degreeof similarity betweenindividuals with regard to syllabletypes. Unique syllableswere generallylocatedat either of the two endsof the songs.Both of Bird I's two unique syllableswere locatedat one end or the other of the 15 songsin which they occurred.Bird II had three unique syllables.Of the 71 songsin which they occurred,they were at the beginningor end in 47 of them. In the other 24 songs,they were near the end and each time they were followedby a syllabletype (B) which terminatedthe song.Bird III had five unique syllables,one of which initiated the songin 39 of the 62 songsin which they occurred. In the other 23, they were near the beginningof the song,beingpreceded by only one syllable (Q). Bird IV had two unique syllables.Of the 10 songsin which they occurred,they were within the bodyof the songonly twice, and in both casesa unique syllablealsobeganthe song. Amountof songtype sharing.--Sharingof songtypes (unique syllable combinations)occurredinfrequently. Of the 130 songtypes that were usedby the four birds, 120 were unique. Only one songtype was used by all of them and only two were sharedby three out of the four individuals.More specifically,Bird I shared7 of its 33 songtypes,Bird II shared9 of its 37 songtypes,Bird III shared5 of its 52 songtypes,and Bird IV shared3 of its 22 songtypes.Coincidenceindicesof the degree of sharingof songtypesbetweenpairs of birdsrangedfrom 0.03 to 0.17 (Table 2), with a mean value of 0.09 (SD = 0.047). These indices, however,do not take into accountsongsthat were similar but not identical. Many songsdiffered by the interjectionor deletionof only one syllable-typebut were still classifiedas different songtypes. Specificrepertoirecomparisons.--Repertoires of four birds were compared. Sevenof the twelve more frequentlyusedsongtypeswere specific to particular individuals. For example, AJKB was given 91 times, but only by Bird III, and CODE was given 105 times, 103 times by Bird III. The most commonsong,AB, was given by all of the birds but the percent of the entire repertoire devotedto this song varied from one individual'with 6% to others with 12, 24, and 36%. Possible influences on songtype.--Changein perch site,changein song type, and interval betweensuccessive songstartswere testedfor interactionusinga log-likelihoodratio test(Sokaland Rohlf 1981) on a three- TABLE Bird I II III IV [195 Singingin HouseWrens Vol. 58, No. 2 (33) a (37) (52) (22) 2. Coincidenceindicesfor songtype sharing. II III IV 0.17 0.07 0.09 0.07 0.10 0.03 aTotal number of different songtypes. way contingency table (Table 3). No significantthree-way interaction occurred(G[PST] = 0.056, P > 0.50). All combinations of two of the factors were tested for interaction with the effectsof the third factor removed.There was no significantrelationship between whether perch site changedand whether song type changedbetweentwo successive songswhen the effect of time between songswas removed (G[PS(T}] = 0.220, P > 0.50), where T = time intervalbetweensongs,P -- change/nochangein perchsite,$ = change/ no changein songtype. Also, neither changesin perch sitesnor song type changesshoweda significantdependence on lengthof time between songswhen the effectof the otherfactorwasremoved(G[PT(S}] = 0.570, P > 0.50 and G[ST(P•] = 0.056, P > 0.50 respectively). DISCUSSION Most North American wren speciesorganize their songboutsinto a pattern that Hartshorne (1973) termed "eventualvariety" (AAA ... BBB... where A and B are differentsongtypes),singingthe samesong type severaltimesbeforeswitching.A few species, includingthe Marsh Wren (Cistothoruspalustris) and Sedge Wren (Cistothorusplatensis) (Kroodsma1977), haveboutpatternsthat Hartshornecalled"immediate variety"(ABCD ...), in whichthereis little or no immediatesongtype repetition.Kroodsmaclassifiesthe House Wren as an AAA... BBB... songster.Sinceeachunique arrangementwas considered to be a distinct songtype,theseHouseWrens had severaldissimilarsongtypesin a row, resultingin songbouts that were made up of sequences of immediate variety interspersedwithin the eventualvariety pattern. House Wrens in this study sharedmany syllableswith at least one otherbird, but eachof the birds alsohad severalunique syllables.Song typeswere sharedinfrequently.In many avianspecies,suchasthe Tufted Titmouse(Parusbicolor),individualssharemostor all of their songtypes with their neighbors(Schroederand Wiley 1983). Verner (1975) found that male Marsh Wrens in easternWashingtonsharedmostof their song typeswith eachother. Many hypotheses havebeensuggested for large repertoires(reviewed by Krebs and Kroodsma1980). In additionto the possibilityof song matchingwith territorial neighbors,large songrepertoiresmay enhance territorial defensethroughthe proposedBeau Gestephenomenon(Krebs 196] m. E. PlattandM. S.Fieken J.Field Ornithol. Spring1987 TABLE3. Three-way contingencytable of the relationshipsamonginterval, changeof perch,and changeof songtype. Time interval Changeof perch Change in songtype yes Short (0.0-6.0 s) Long (•6.0 s) Totals no yes 17 12 no 43 37 29 80 60 49 109 yes 18 14 32 no 39 32 71 57 46 103 117 95 212 Totals 1977) involving deceit of incoming males by the diversity of songsby territory holders.A predictionof this hypothesisis that a bird will switch songtypesas it movesfrom one songperchto another.A studyof Chaffinches(Fringillacoelebs) by Dawsonand Jenkins(1983) did not support the Beau Geste hypothesis.Yasukawa (1981) demonstratedthat RedwingedBlackbirds(Agelaius phoeniceus) do switchsongsat differentperch sitesbut he and Searcy(1985) showedthat the number of songtypesis not usedto assess male densities.Our studyof wrens fails to supportthe prediction.Krebs (1977) developedthe Beau Gestehypothesisto account for repertoire evolution in specieswith repertoire sizes of 2-20 song types,so it was not surprisingthat the songswitchingpredictiondid not hold true for House Wrens which have very large repertoiresizes. Thus, althoughthis studydoesnot supportone of the key predictions of the Beau Gestehypothesis,that songtypeschangewith perchswitches, large songrepertoiresmay still be involvedin territorial defensethrough advantagesof songmatchingby territorial neighborsor through reduction of habituation. Large repertoiresin House Wrens may also have evolvedthroughtheir effecton females.For example,the mostreasonable explanationof the very low amplitudesyllablesat the beginningof songs is that theseare directedat the mate who is probablyin closeproximity, rather than at a territorial neighborthat is probablymuch more distant. In addition,there is the possibilitythat large repertoiresare attractiveto femalesand stimulatethem more physiologicallythan would repetitions of a singlesongtype (e.g., Kroodsma1976). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank J. D. Buntin, L. S. Johnson,J. A. Reinartz, C. V. Sommer,and C. M. Weise for helpful commentson the manuscript.This is publicationnumber80 of the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Field Station. LITERATURE CITED DAWSON, S. M., ANDP. F. JENKINS. 1983. Chaffinchsongrepertoiresand the Beau Geste hypothesis.Behaviour87:256-269. Vol.58,No.2 Singing inHouse Wrens [ 197 DICE, L. R. 1945. Measures of the amount of ecologicalassociationbetween species. Ecology 26:297-302. HARTSHORNE, C. 1973. Born to sing.Indiana University Press,Bloomington,Indiana. KREBS, J.R. 1977. The significance of songrepertoires:the Beau Gestehypothesis.Anim. Behav. 25:475-478. KREBS,J. R., AND D. E. KROODSMA.1980. Repertoiresand geographicalvariation in bird song.Pp. 143-177, in J. Rosenblattet al., eds.Advancesin the studyof behavior. Academic Press, New York. KROODSMA, D. E. 1976. Reproductivedevelopmentin a female songbird:differential stimulationby quality of male song.Science192:574-575. 1977. Correlatesof songorganizationamong North American wrens. Am. Nat. 11• :995-1008. SCHROEDER, D. J., AND R. H. WILEY. 1983. Communicationwith sharedsongthemes in Tufted Titmice. Auk 100:414-424. SOKAL,R. R., ANDF. J. ROHLF. 1981. Biometry.W. H. Freemanand Company,San Francisco, California. VERNER,J. 1975. Complex songrepertoireof male Long-billed Marsh Wrens in eastern Washington. Living Bird 14:263-300. Y^SUK^W^, K. 1981. Songrepertoiresin the Red-wingedBlackbird(Agelaius phoeniceus): a testof the Beau Gestehypothesis. Anim. Behav.29:114-125. --, ^NDW. A. SE^RC¾.1985. Songrepertoiresanddensityassessment in Red-winged Blackbirds:further testsof the Beau Gestehypothesis.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.16:171175. Received23 June 1986; accepted2 Dec. 1986.