Brands as Means of Self-Expression: A Cross-Cultural Study Sabine Kuester, Silke C. Hess, Julia Hinkel, Jennifer Young, University of Mannheim, Germany Abstract This study investigates the self-expressive function of brands in four countries that represent different cultural dimensions. Survey data collected in France, Germany, South Korea, and the US provide empirical evidence for the importance of brands as means of self-expression. Additionally, in countries where individualism is high brand identification is stronger when customers can express their actual or ideal self-concept with the brand. Whereas the motive to express one's ideal self-concept is more important in masculine countries. Furthermore, in countries where power distance is high customers choose brands to show their social status. From the empirical results we derive implications for global brand management. Introduction In marketing research customers' brand choice is explained by two basic functions that brands fulfil from a customer’s perspective (Wallin and Coote, 2007). Information economics accounts for how customers use brands as signals to assess the quality of goods and services, decrease the perceived risk, and reduce search costs (Erdem and Swait, 1998; Wernerfelt, 1988). Whereas the self-expression model in brand equity literature describes how a brand's personality offers a self-expressive benefit for the customer (Aaker, 2002). Thus, brands allow satisfying underlying needs for personal expression and social approval (Keller, 1993). For the empirical investigation of brand signalling Erdem and Swait (1998) developed a structural equation model to assess the impact of brands as signals on expected utility. The brand signalling model has already been cross-validated in seven countries and the authors observed moderating effects of the cultural constructs uncertainty avoidance and collectivism (Erdem, Swait, and Valenzuela, 2006). Therefore, there seems to be scope to investigate the self-expression function of brands across countries that show cultural differences as well. The first motivation is the ongoing trend in the globalisation of marketing activities and therewith, the importance of global brands to cope with these activities (Bauer, Exler, and Bronk, 2007; Erdem, Swait, and Valenzuela, 2006). Furthermore, Erdem, Swait, and Valenzuela (2006) did not find a significant moderating effect of the power distance dimension but point out the need for further research on the influence of this dimension by including perceived brand prestige as a new construct. Therefore, the aim of this study is to investigate how well the self-expressive function of brands explains brand preference formation in different countries. We explore differences in the way customers use brands as means of self-expression and link any such differences to customers' cultural orientation. We propose that three of Hofstede's (1980) dimensions (individualism, masculinity, and power distance) may particularly affect the way individuals use brands as means of self-expression and social approval. 1670 Theoretical Basis and Model Development Self-congruity theory indicates that customers will prefer and choose brands that are congruent with their self-concept (Sirgy, 1982, 1986). Self-concept has been defined as the sum of individuals’ thoughts and feelings that have reference to themselves as an object (Rosenberg, 1979). Rosenberg (1979) distinguishes the actual self-concept, which refers to how a person actually perceives itself, and the ideal self-concept, which refers to the image of oneself as one would like to be. Furthermore, Sirgy (1980) refers to a social self-concept, which relates to the image that individuals believe others hold of them, and an ideal social self-concept, seen as the image that one would like others to hold. There are at least two motives to strive for selfcongruity: self-consistency and self-esteem (Sirgy, 1982). The self-consistency motive relates to the need to act and behave according to one’s self-concept (Sirgy, 1982), whereas self-esteem is defined as the proximity of the actual and the ideal self concept (Rosenberg, 1979). The selfesteem motive is closely linked to the need for social approval, which in contrast evokes a motivational tendency to approach one’s ideal social self- image (Sirgy, 1986). According to the theory of self-enhancement, individuals seek for preserving and enhancing their self-concept by purchasing certain products (Braun and Wicklund, 1989; Rogers, 1951; Wicklund and Gollwitzer, 1981). Since brands serve as social symbols that comprise a shared meaning, individual consumption behaviours through the use of brands contribute substantially to the preservation and enhancement of customers’ self-concepts (Grubb and Grathwohl, 1967). Thus, symbolic benefits represent the desire for brands that fulfil needs such as self-enhancement (Park, Jaworski, and MacInnis, 1986). Thereby, in this symbolic communication process brands can represent intrinsic values (e.g., self-expression) or extrinsic values (e.g., prestige), depending on whether the individual is communicating with itself or with its social surrounding (Grubb and Grathwohl, 1967). Thus, we propose the following hypotheses: H1 : The greater the congruity between brand personality and actual self-concept (actual congruity), the greater is the self-expression value of the brand. H2 : The greater the congruity between brand personality and ideal self-concept (ideal congruity), the greater is the self-expression value of the brand. H3 : The greater the congruity between brand personality and ideal social self-concept (ideal social congruity), the greater is the prestige value of the brand. The concept of identification is closely related to the self-expression value, but goes a step further in providing a close emotional relationship to the particular object (Foote, 1951). Identification manifests as an individual's feelings of psychological attachment to a social entity that includes companies and brands (Bhattacharya and Sen, 2003). A brand that is congruent with one’s self-concept and that allows customers to express themselves, will also lead them to be more committed to the brand (Kim and Sherman, 2007). Similarly, the prestige value of a brand allows individuals to position themselves in their social context, and to express their prestige, wealth, and status (Deeter-Schmelz, Moore, and Goeble, 2000). Hence, also extrinsic values can cause a relationship between individuals and brands (Tan Tsu Wee and Chua Han Ming, 2003). Finally, a considerable amount of studies in consumer research has stated that identification directly results into consumption (e.g., Belk, 1988). Thus, we propose the following hypotheses: H4 : The higher the self-expression value, the higher is the identification with the brand. H5 : The higher the prestige value, the higher is the identification with the brand. 1671 H6 : The higher the identification with the brand, the higher is the behavioural intention. Our conceptual development is depicted in the structural equation model in figure 1: Actual Self Congruity (AS) + Self-expression Value (Self-Exp) + + Brand Identification (Ident) Ideal Self Congruity (IS) + Behavioural Intention (BI) + + Prestige Value (Prest) Ideal Social Self Congruity (ISS) Figure 1: Hypothesised Structural Equation Model Similar to Erdem, Swait, and Valenzuela (2006) we expect a moderating effect of culture. To formulate hypotheses for cross-cultural studies, Hofstede’s (1980) framework turned out to be appropriate (Soares, Farhangmehr, and Shoham, 2007). Hence, in this particular study we consider individualism (IND), masculinity (MAS), and power distance (PD) as relevant constructs. In individualistic countries individuals tend to care only for themselves (Hofstede and Hofstede, 2005). Thus, in such countries people are considered to be independent, selfcontained, and autonomous individuals that need to express their internal attributes (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). In masculine societies people are oriented towards earnings and achievement (Hofstede, 2003). Consequently, they feel a strong need to enhance their self-concept (De Mooij, 2004). In countries where PD is high people are vastly motivated by status (Roth, 1995), as interpersonal relationships are organised hierarchically (Erdem, Swait, and Valenzuela, 2006). Thus, we propose the following hypotheses: H7 : The effect of actual congruity on self-expression value is stronger when IND is high. H8 : The effect of ideal congruity on self-expression value is stronger when MAS is high. H9 : The effect of ideal social congruity on prestige value is stronger when PD is high. H10 : The effect of self-expression value on brand identification is stronger when IND is high. H11 : The effect of prestige value on brand identification is stronger when PD is high. Data Analysis To test and validate our model across countries, we collected comparable samples of respondents in France (n = 126), Germany (n = 104), South Korea (n = 115), and the United States (n = 110) as these countries differ with regard to IND, MAS, and PD (see Table 1). We recruited matched samples of undergraduate students in the four countries with demographics 1672 representative for the respective countries. The selection of matched samples on the basis of some set of characteristics of interest has been identified as one way to achieve sample comparability (Sekaran, 1983). Furthermore, as the differences in sociodemographics were small it seemed justifiable to attribute the observed differences among countries to their cultural differences. Table 1: Country Rating by Cultural Dimensions (Hofstede, 2003) Country France Germany South Korea United States Individualism 71 67 18 91 Masculinity 43 66 39 62 Power Distance 68 35 60 40 The questionnaire was distributed in the respective language of the country and presented the following scenario: The students were invited to imagine that their parents ask them to choose a wristwatch brand in the value of up to US$ 200 as a present for their university graduation. The students could indicate any brand they desired as a present from their parents. We then presented closed questions referring to this particular brand. The latent constructs were measured using adapted existing multi- item measures and five point Likert-scales. The perceived congruence between the brand personality and the self-concepts were measured directly as Sirgy et al.’s (1997) findings demonstrate a higher predictive validity of this measurement method. The statistical procedures applied to validate the measures included assessments of item and scale reliability as well as convergent validity (Anderson and Gerbing, 1988). All paths from the individual items to the latent constructs were statistically significant (p <.05). Measurement validity for the pooled data (n = 455) was assessed by confirmatory factor analyses using LISREL 8.7 (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1993). To test our hypotheses we used multiple-group analysis with LISREL 8.7 (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1993). We first estimated our proposed SEM on country-level by specifying the various countries as different groups of the data set. Table 2 presents the standardized SEM coefficients for each country. The overall fit measures (goodness of fit index [GFI] = .96, normed fit index [NFI] = 1.0, and comparative fit index [CFI] = 1.0) display values that exceed the threshold value of .9, which is usually recommended (Bagozzi and Yi, 1988; Baumgartner and Homburg, 1996). In addition, the RMSEA (.091) and the ratio of chi-square and degrees of freedom (1.94) suggest that the model provides a satisfying fit for the data. Of the 24 hypothesized structural relationships 22 are supported. The g parameter estimates are significant on the .05 or .01 level (see Table 2). Thus, we observed that brand preference is explained by the brand’s self-expression function. Two relationships are not supported. The effect of ideal-self congruity on self-expression value in France and the effect of prestige value on brand identification in Germany are insignificant on a .05 level. 1673 Table 2: SEM Estimation Results by Country Path AS› S e l f -Exp IS› S e l f -Exp ISS› P r e s t Self-Exp› I d e n t Prest› Ident Ident› B I France .96** .26 .83** .93** .10* .69** Standardized Coefficients (g) Germany South Korea .59** .72** ** .41 .16* .66** .76** .99** .93** .04 .13** .42** .46** United States .48** .47** .78** .90** .31** .64** * g is significant at p < .05; ** g is significant at p < .01 We then analysed the differences between the four countries and considered culture as a moderator. Therefore, we stepwise compared two countries at a time with respect to one of the model’s path coefficients. We restricted this parameter to be equal across both countries and compared the chi-square with the chi-square of the unrestricted model that allows this parameter to vary across the two groups. As the unrestricted model has one degree of freedom (d.f.) less then the restricted model, we assessed significance of the difference of the respective path coefficient on the basis of the chi-square distribution with 1 d.f. The results of the multiple group causal analysis partly support the hypothesized cross-cultural effects. The effect of ideal congruity on self-expression value is significantly stronger in the US than in France ( c² =21.81, p <.001) or South Korea ( c² =3.58, p <.05) as the US score high on the MAS dimension. Similarly, this effect is also significantly stronger in Germany than in South Korea ( c² =16.61, p <.001). Although the path coefficient for the German sub sample (γ = .41) is clearly higher than for the French (γ = .26), this difference is not significant. Furthermore, the effect of ideal social congruity on prestige value is significantly stronger for France that scores high on PD than for the US ( c² =46.69, p <.001), but not than Germany although the standardized path coefficient is clearly higher. However, the effect is significantly stronger for South Korea than for Germany ( c² =30.9, p <.001). Additionally, the effect of self-expression value on identification is significantly stronger for France and Germany than for South Korea where IND is low ( c² =10.93, p <.001; c² =26.52, p <.001). On the other hand, the effect of prestige value on identification is significantly stronger for France than for Germany that scores low on PD ( c² =5.96, p <.05). Although this effect is also higher for the Korean sub-sample (γ = .13) than for the German (γ =.04), this difference is not significant. Conclusions, Implications, and Further Research We found empirical support for the function of brands as means of self-expression in all four countries. Brands are used to express one’s actual self, to enhance the self-concept by choosing brands that are congruent to one’s ideal self-concept, and for social approval in the respective social surrounding. However, the importance of these different motives varies between these countries as they differ in IND, MAS, and PD. In three countries (France, Germany, and South Korea) the differences of the path coefficients were observed as expected with one exception: In the collectivistic country South Korea the effect of self congruity on a brand’s self-expression value is as important as in countries where IND is high. Contrary to our hypotheses, we observed a strong effect of ideal social congruity on prestige value and a moderate effect of prestige value on brand identification for the US, although PD is low here. These findings indicate that in the US brands are also intensively used for social approval, which has to be 1674 explained by other variables than IND and PD. Brands are consumption symbols and therefore, also carriers of culture (Aaker, Benet-Martinez, and Garolera, 2001). The US brand personality scale encompasses the dimensions sincerity, excitement, competence, sophistication, and ruggedness (Aaker, 1997), whereas the first three dimensions are also present in human personality scales “sophistication and ruggedness capture more aspirational images associated with wealth and status” (Aaker, Benet-Martinez, and Garolera, 2001, p. 494). These findings have important implications for global brand management. Global brands are besides their worldwide distribution characterised as highly standardised regarding positioning and marketing mix (Aaker and Joachismthaler, 1999; Levitt, 1983; Quelch, 1999). Therefore, global brand management has to take into account that brands as means of self-expression are used in different ways. This has to be considered in formulating country-specific communication and selling propositions. Whereas in masculine countries brands that show high ideal congruity are used for self-enhancement and means for achievement, in countries where PD is high customers choose brands to show their social status. Certainly this study points out avenues for further research. As one limitation of our study is the non-representativeness of the student samples, one could seek out samples comprising different customer groups. This would help to enhance the generalizability of our results. 1675 References Aaker, D.A., 2002. Building strong brands, Simon & Schuster, London, UK. Aaker, D.A., Joachismthaler, E., 1999. The lure of global branding. Harvard Business Review 77 (6), 137-144. Aaker, J.L., 1997. Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research 34 (3), 342352. Aaker, J.L., Benet-Martinez, V., Garolera, J., 2001. Consumption symbols as carriers of culture: A study of Japanese and Spanish brand personality constructs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 (3), 492-508. Anderson, J.C., Gerbing, D.W., 1988. Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin 103 (4), 411-423. Bagozzi, R.P., Yi, Y., 1988. On the evaluation of structural equation models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 16 (1), 74-97. Baumgartner, H., Homburg, C., 1996. Applications of structural equation modeling in marketing and consumer research: A review. International Journal of Research in Marketing 13 (2), 139-161. Bauer, H.H., Exler, S., Bronk, L., 2007. Brand perception: Is global always better? Proceedings of the American Marketing Association Winter Educators’ Conference. San Diego, CA, USA, 307-308. Belk, R., 1988. Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research 15 (2), 139168. Bhattacharya, C.B., Sen, S., 2003. Consumer-company identification: A framework for understanding consumers' relationships with companies. Journal of Marketing 67 (2), 76-88. Braun, O.L., Wicklund, R.A., 1989. Psychological antecedents of conspicuous consumption. Journal of Economic Psychology 10 (2), 161-187. Deeter-Schmelz, D.R., Moore, J.N., Goeble, D.J., 2000. Prestige clothing shopping by consumers: A confirmatory assessment and refinement of the Precon scale with managerial implications. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice 8 (4), 43-58. De Mooij, M.K., 2004. Consumer behaviour and culture: Consequences for global marketing and advertising, Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA, USA. Erdem, T., Swait, J., 1998. Brand equity as a signaling phenomenon. Journal of Consumer Psychology 7 (2), 131-157. Erdem, T., Swait, J., Valenzuela, A., 2006. Brands as signals: A cross-country validation study. Journal of Marketing 70 (1), 34-49. Foote, N.N., 1951. Identification as the basis for a theory of motivation. American Sociological Review 16 (1), 14-21. 1676 Grubb, E., Grathwohl, H., 1967. Consumer self-concept, symbolism and market behavior. A theoretical approach, Journal of Marketing 31 (4), 22-27. Hofstede, G., 1980. Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values, Sage Publications, Beverly Hill, CA, USA. Hofstede, G., 2003. Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations, 2nd ed., Sage Publications, Thousands Oaks, CA, USA. Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G.J., 2005. Cultures and organizations. Software of the mind, 2nd ed., McGraw-Hill, New York et al., USA. Jöreskog, K.G., Sörbom, D., 1993. LISREL 8: A guide to the program and applications, SPSS Inc., Chicago, USA. Keller, K.L., 1993. Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand equity, Journal of Marketing 57 (1), 1-22. Kim, H.S., Sherman, D.K., 2007. "Express Yourself": Culture and the effect of self-expression on choice. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 92 (1), 1-11. Levitt, Th., 1983. The globalization of markets. Harvard Business Review 61 (3), 2-11. Markus, H., Kitayama, S., 1991. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review 98 (2), 224-253. Park, C.W., Jaworski, B.J., MacInnis, D.J., 1986. Strategic brand concept-image management. Journal of Marketing 50 (4), 135-145. Quelch, J., 1999. Global brands: Taking stock. Business Strategy Review 10 (1), 1-14. Rogers, C.R., 1951. Client-centered therapy, Houghton Mifflin, Boston, MA, USA. Rosenberg, M., 1979. Conceiving the self, Basic Books, New York, USA. Roth, M.S., 1995. The effects of culture and socioeconomics on the performance of global image strategies. Journal of Marketing Research 32 (2), 163-176. Sekaran, U., 1983. Methodological and theoretical issues and advancements in cross-cultural research. Journal of International Business Studies 14 (2), 61-73. Sirgy, J.M., 1980. Self-concept in relation to product preference and purchase intention. In: Bellur V.V. (Ed.), Developments in Marketing Science. Academy of Marketing Science, Marquette, MI, USA, pp. 350-354. Sirgy, J.M., 1982. Self-concept in consumer behavior: A critical review. Journal of Consumer Research 9 (3), 287-300. Sirgy, J.M., 1986. Self-congruity: Toward a theory of personality and cybernetics, Praeger Publishers, New York, USA. 1677 Sirgy, J.M., Grewal, D., Mangleburg, T., Park, J., Chon, K.-S., Claiborne, C.B., Johar, J.S., Berkman, H. 1997. Assessing the predictive validity of two methods of measuring self- image congruence. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 25 (3), 229-241. Soares, A.M., Farhangmehr, M., Shoham, A., 2007. Hofstede's dimensions of culture in international marketing studies. Journal of Business Research 60 (3), 277-284. Tan Tsu Wee, Th., Chua Han Ming, M., 2003. Leveraging on symbolic values and meanings in branding. Journal of Brand Management 10 (3), 208-218. Wallin, A.C., Coote, L.V., 2007. What do brands signal? Proceedings of the American Marketing Association Winter Educators’ Conference. San Diego, CA, USA, 90. Wernerfelt, B., 1988. Umbrella branding as a signal of new product quality: An example of signaling by posting a bond. Rand Journal of Economics 19 (3), 458-466. Wicklund, R., Gollwitzer, P., 1981. Symbolic self-completion, attempted influence, and self deprecation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 2 (2), 89-114. 1678