Public Opinion and Foreign Policy: Challenges to the Almond-Lippmann Consensus Mershon Series: Research Programs and Debates Author(s): Ole R. Holsti Reviewed work(s): Source: International Studies Quarterly, Vol. 36, No. 4 (Dec., 1992), pp. 439-466 Published by: Wiley-Blackwell on behalf of The International Studies Association Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2600734 . Accessed: 08/11/2012 03:12 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. . Wiley-Blackwell and The International Studies Association are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to International Studies Quarterly. http://www.jstor.org InternationalStudiesQuarterly (1992) 36, 439-466 Public Opiniionand ForeignPolicy:Challenges to theAlmond-Lippmann Consensus MershonSeries:ResearchPrograms andDebates OLE R. HOLSTI Duke University This articlesurveysand assessestheoriesand researchon public opinion and foreignpolicy.Most of the evidence is drawnfromthe literatureon the United States.Three twentieth-century wars have had a significant impact on theoryand scholarship.World War I-the firstpublic relations war-and postwar effortsto create a new internationalorder directedmuch attentionto the nature of public opinion and its impact on foreignaffairs,issues on which realistsand liberals came to quite differentconclusions.The period surroundingWorld War II coincided withthe developmentof scientificpolling. Much of the attentionduring and immediatelyafterthe warfocusedon the extentto whichthe public American role. Extensive mightsupport or oppose an internationalist researchduringthe firsttwodecades afterWorldWar II yieldeda broad agreement (the "Almond-Lippmannconsensus") on three propositions about public opinion: (1) it is volatile and thus provides inadequate foundationsfor stable and effectiveforeignpolicies, (2) it lacks coherence or structure,but (3) in the finalanalysis,it has littleif any impact on foreignpolicy.The VietnamWar and its aftermathstimulateda new on public opinion and foreignpolicy,much outburstof researchactivity of which has challenged each of these three propositions.The article concludes with suggestionsfor furtherresearch efforts,including: (1) case studiesemployingarchivalsourcesto assessmore directlythe impact of public opinion, (2) cross-nationalstudies, (3) developmentof standard questionsin orderto encourage bettercumulationof surveyresults, and (4) researchthatwillenable us to distinguishfindingsthatare timeand context-boundfromthosethattranscendthe Cold War period. Introduction Many questions about the role of public opinion in foreign policy are at the center of persisting debates between the liberal-democratic and realist approaches to foreign affairs.Is public opinion a force for enlightenment-indeed, a necessary if not sufficientcondition for sound foreign policy-as celebrated by the Wilsonians Author'snote: For helpfulcommentsand suggestionson an earlierdraftof thispaper I am gratefulto Stephen Earl Bennett,WilliamChittick,Thomas Graham,Jon Hurwitz,Benjamin Page, Mark Peffley,Philip Powlick,Bruce and twoanonymousreviewersforInternational Russett,Eugene Wittkopf, StudiesQuarterly. ?) 1992 InternationalStudiesAssociation 440 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy and otherliberals?There is a long,liberal-democratic tradition,datingback at least to Kant and Bentham,thatforeignpolicies of democraciesare more peaceful,at least in partbecause the public can play a constructive role in constrainingpolicy of to the public can restrainthewar-making makers;onlyaccountability proclivities leaders.1 Alternatively, are Hans Morgenthauand othersof the realistschool correctin and coherentdiplomacy,hindescribingpublic opinion as a barrierto thoughtful to promotenationalintereststhattranscendthe moods and passions deringefforts of the moment?The realisttraditionis intenselyskepticalof the public's contribution to effectiveforeignpolicy.At the veryminimum,most realistswould distinguish betweenforeignpolicyand other public policyissues; the public mightbe sufficiently informedto deal withlocal issues thatimpingeon theirdailylives,but foreignaffairsare too remote fromtheirexperience,and in any case theyhave littleinclinationto become more informedabout such complexand remoteissues. Finally,the effectiveconduct of diplomacyrequiressecrecy,flexibility, and other qualitiesthatwould be seriouslyjeopardized were the public to have a significant impacton foreignpolicy.Thus, to permitthe public a strongvoice in policywould be to place the democracies,ifnot the stability of the internationalsystemitself,at a distinctdisadvantage.Moreover,it would permitthe emotional to govern the rational.Hans Morgenthausummarizedthe case againstan activerole forpublic opinion in words that would gain the support of most if not all realists:"The rationalrequirementsof good foreignpolicycannot fromthe outsetcount upon the support of a public opinion whose preferencesare emotional rather than rational"(Morgenthau,1978:558). The long-standing debate betweenliberalsand realistswas intensifiedbyWorld War I, whichmightbe describedas the firstpublic relationswar.Fromitsinception both the Allied and CentralPowers tried to win over "worldopinion" in various ways,including publicationby many governmentsof highlyselectivedocument collections-the so-calledcolor books-all ofwhichwere intendedto absolvethem and to place the blame for the war on their adversaries.The propaganda war duringthe conflictwas almostas intenseas thaton thebattlefield. PresidentWilson'shopes fora new postwarworldorder depended significantly on democratizingforeign affairsand diplomacy. Elihu Root, a distinguished summarizedthe positionof Republican and formerSecretaryof State,effectively those who welcomed an increasingrole for the public in the conduct of foreign affairs. When foreignofficeswere ruled by autocraciesor oligarchiesthe danger of war was in sinisterpurpose. When foreignaffairsare ruled bydemocraciesthe danger of war will be in mistakenbeliefs.The world will be gainer by the change, for, while thereis no human wayto preventa kingfromhavinga bad heart,thereis a human way to prevent a people from having an erroneous opinion. (Root, 1922:5) By more effectiveinternationaleducation, "the people themselveswill have the means to testmisinformation and appeals to prejudiceand passionbased on error" (Root, 1922:5). But not all observersjoined Wilson and Root in applauding the prospect of popular diplomacy. During the postwar era, the journalist Walter Lippmann published two trenchantcritiquesof the central premisesof classical liberalism (Lippmann, 1922, 1925). Accordingto Lippmann,the common man is too fully engaged in the requirementsof earning a livingand otherwiseattendingto his 1 Recent research has found that the foreignpolicies of democracies are indeed different fromthose of other politiesand, further, thatdemocraciesdo n-otengage in waragainsteach other. OLE R. HOLSTI 441 most immediateneeds to have the time or inclinationto satisfythe heroic, but clearlyunrealistic,assumptionsabout the informedand engaged citizencelebrated in classicaldemocratictheory.The chasm betweentheoryand realityis especially farremovedfromthe directexperiences whichare typically wide on foreignaffairs, of the mass public. Consequently,the "picturesin the head" of the averagecitizen are unlikelyto have much correspondencewith the real world of international affairs.Moreover,journalist Lippmann became increasinglyskepticalabout the to fillin the gap betweenthe real worldand abilityof his own professioneffectively His studyof the Russianrevolution,as depictedin the averagecitizen'sstereotypes. the pages of the NewYorkTimesduringthe period 1917-20,did nothingto assuage his pessimism about the abilityof the media to serve as a source of valid informationabout the world for the public (Lippmann and Metz, 1920). The events of the 1930s and the outbreak of World War II, which seemed to raise serious questions about Wilsonian assumptions while apparently providing compelling empirical confirmationfor the realist approach to international relations,furthertipped the balance of the debate on public opinion and foreign policyin favorof the skeptics. coincided The period encompassingWorldWar II and itsimmediateaftermath withthe inceptionof scientificpublic opinion polling.Despite the wounds to the reputationof polling and pollstersinflictedby the LiteraiyDigest debacle in the Roosevelt-Landonelection, we can date the era of scientificsurveysfrom the of the Gallup poll in 1936 or of thePublic Opinion Quarterlytwoyears establishment later.It maybe worthnotingthatPresidentRooseveltwas a pioneer in the use of a public opinion consultant-Hadley Cantril,one of the foundingfathersof the new science-for guidance on policy. Policy makersand manyotherswho feltthat an irresponsibleAmericanisolationismafter1919 had contributedto the outbreakof WorldWar II worriedthat the public mood mighttrace out a patternresemblingthe experience of the followedsoon period afterWorld War I: wartimeidealism and internationalism, thereafterby cynicismand disenchantmentwith active American leadership in effortsto create a more stable internationalorder.2This concern was reflectedin the frequencywithwhichpollingorganizationsduringWorldWar II asked respondentsgeneral questionsabout the United Statestakingan activerole in, or staying out of, world affairs,and specificqueries about support for or opposition to Americanmembershipin the UnitedNations. These two features-an empirical approach that relied heavilyon systematic polling data and a normativeconcern that an isolationistpublic mightlead the United Statesto repeat the failedisolationistpolicies of the interwarperiod-may be found in two of the pioneeringworkson public opinion and foreignpolicy: Thomas Bailey's TheMan in theStreet(1948) and Gabriel Almond's TheAmerican Policy(1950). PeopleandForeign The Post-WorldWarII Consensus The availabilityafter World War II of growingsets of polling data and the of systematic studiesof votingbehavior,combinedwiththe assumption institution 2JustbeforePresidentRooseveltleftforthe YaltaConferencein 1945, Hadley Cantrilgave the presidenta memo which stated: "Although the overwhelmingmajorityof the American people now favor a strong international organizationnecessarilydominated by the big powers,it is unrealisticto assume thatAmericansare internationalThe minded. Their policyis ratherone of expediency,which,at the moment,takes the formof internationalism. restson a ratherunstablefoundation:it is recent,it is not rooted in anybroad or long-range presentinternationalism ithas littleintellectualbasis" (Cantril,1967:76). conceptionof self-interest, 442 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy of a leadership role in world affairsby the United States,served to stimulatea growthindustryin analysesof public opinion. The consensusviewthatdeveloped duringthisperiod of some fifteenor twentyyearsafterthe end of World War II and just priorto theVietnamescalationcenteredon threemajorpropositions: * Public opinion is highlyvolatileand thus it providesverydubiousfoundationsfor a soundforeignpolicy. * Public attitudeson foreignaffairsare so lackingin structureand coherencethatthey mightbestbe describedas "non-attitudes." * At the end of the day, however,public opinion has a verylimitedimpact on the conductofforeign policy. Let us examine each of these propositions,and the evidence upon which they rested,in more detail. PublicOpinionIs Volatile As noted earlier,WalterLippmann's books of the interwarperiod described the mass public as neithersufficiently interestednor informedto playthe pivotalrole assignedto it by classicaldemocratictheory.At the heightof the Cold War thirty yearslater,Lippmann had become even more alarmed,depictingthe mass public as not merelyuninterestedand uninformed,but as a powerfulforcethatwas so out of synchwithrealityas to constitutea massiveand potentiallyfatalthreatto effectivegovernmentand policies. The unhappy truthis that the prevailingpublic opinion has been destructively wrongat the criticaljunctures.The people have impresseda criticalveto upon the judgments of informed and responsible officials.They have compelled the whichusuallyknewwhatwould have been wiser,or was necessary,or government, whatwas more expedient,to be too late withtoo little,or too long withtoo much, too pacifistin peace and too bellicose in war, too neutralistor appeasing in negotiationsor too intransigent.Mass opinion has acquired mountingpower in this country.It has shown itselfto be a dangerous masterof decision when the stakesare lifeand death. (Lippmann,1955:20) Similarlypessimisticconclusions and dire warnings were emerging from disparateother quartersas well. Drawingon a growingbody of polling data and fearing that the American public might relapse into a mindless isolationism, covereda thickbedrockof because onlya thinveneerof postwarinternationalism indifferenceto the world,Gabriel Almond depicted public opinion as a volatile and mood-drivenconstraintupon foreignpolicy:"The undertowof withdrawalis stillverypowerful.Deeply ingrainedhabits do not die easy deaths. The world outside is stillveryremotefor mostAmericans;and the tragiclessons of the last decades have not been fullydigested"(Almond,1950:85). Consequently,"Perhaps is thatof the instability of the gravestgeneral problem confrontingpolicy-makers mass moods, the cyclicalfluctuationswhich stand in the way of policy stability" (Almond,1950:239).3Six yearslater,Almondrestatedhis thesisin Lippmannesque of public moods, but otherdeficienciesas language,citingnot onlythe instability well. He told an audience at the National War College, "For persons responsible forthe makingof securitypolicythese mood impactsof the public have a highly 3 Almond's use of the term "mood" differsfromthat of Frank Klingberg.Almond refersto sudden shiftsof interestand preferences,whereas Klingberghas used the term to explain American foreignpolicy in termsof generation-longsocietal swingsbetween introversionand extraversion.For the latterusage, see Klingberg(1952, 1979, 1983) and Holmes (1985). OLE R. HOLSTI 443 irrationaleffect.Often the public is apatheticwhen it should be concerned,and panickywhenit should be calm" (Almond,1956:372,376). Otherswhose writingsprovidedsupportforthe main outlinesof the pessimistic Almond-Lippmannthesis included a distinguishedlist: the dean of American diplomatic historians(Thomas A. Bailey); the foremostproponent of a realist approach to internationalaffairs(Hans J. Morgenthau); and the diplomat-historianwho has oftenbeen depicted as the intellectualfatherof the Americanpolicy of containment,George F. Kennan. For the latter,the metaphorof a dinosaur vividly depictedtheproblemsof democraticforeignpolicy. But I sometimeswonderwhetherin thisrespecta democracyis not uncomfortably similarto one of thoseprehistoricmonsterswitha bodyas long as thisroom and a brain the size of a pin: he lies there in his comfortableprimevalmud and pays littleattentionto his environment;he is slow to wrath-in fact,you practically have to whackhis tailoffto make him aware thathis interestsare being disturbed; but,once he graspsthis,he laysabout him withsuch blind determinationthathe not only destroyshis adversarybut largelywreckshis native habitat. (Kennan, 1951:59) Furthersupportforthe criticsand skepticsemerged fromthe growingbody of polling data whichyieldedample evidence of the public's limitedstoreof factual knowledgeabout foreignaffairs.Innumerablesurveysrevealedsuch stunninggaps in informationas: X percentof the Americanpublic are unaware that there is a communistgovernmentin China, Y percent believe that the Soviet Union is a memberof NATO, or Z percentcannot identify a singlenationborderingon the PacificOcean. Such data reinforcedthe case of the criticsand led some of themto propose measuresto reduce the influenceof the public. Thus, Lippmann (1955) called forstrongerexecutiveprerogativesin foreignaffairs,and Bailey (1948:13) wonderedwhetherthe requirementsof an effective foreignpolicymightmake it necessaryfortheexecutivedeliberatelyto misleadthe public. PublicOpinionLacksStructure and Coherence A growingvolume of data on public opinion and voting behavior, as well as increasinglysophisticatedmethodologies,enabled analystsnot only to describe aggregateresultsand trends,but also to delve into the structureof politicalbeliefs. Owing to immediatepolicyconcernsabout the U.S. role in the postwarera, many of the earlystudieswerelargelydescriptive, focusingon such issuesas participation in internationalorganizationsand alliances, the deploymentof troops abroad, securitycommitments,foreignaid, trade and protectionism,and the like. The dimensionwould underlyingpremisewas thata single internationalist-isolationist serveto structureforeignpolicybeliefs,much in the waythata liberal-conservative dimensionwas assumedto providecoherence to preferenceson domesticissues. In a classic studybased on data fromthe late 1950s and early 1960s, Philip Converse(1964) concluded thatthe politicalbeliefsof the mass public lack a real structureor coherence. Comparingresponsesacross severaldomesticand foreign or underlyingideological structure policyissues,he foundlittleifany "constraint" thatmightprovidesome coherence to politicalthinking.In contrast,his analyses of elites-congressional candidates-revealed substantiallyhigher correlations among responsesto variousissues.Moreover,Conversefound thatboth mass and elite attitudeson a given issue had a short half-life.Responses in 1956 only modestlypredictedresponsestwoyearslater,much less in 1960. These findingsled him to conclude that mass politicalbeliefsare best described as "non-attitudes." AlthoughConverse'sfindingswere laterto become the centerof an activedebate, it should be emphasized thathis was not a lone voice in the wilderness.His data of Michigan,and were drawnfromthe National Election Studiesat the University his findingswere onlythe mostwidelyquoted of a seriesof studiesfromthe NES 444 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy that came to essentiallythe same conclusion about the absence of structure, coherence,or persistencein the politicalbeliefsof the mass public-especially on foreignaffairs(Campbell,Converse,Miller,and Stokes,1964). ImpactonForeign Policy PublicOpinionHas Limited A significant reason forinterestin public opinion on foreignaffairsarisesfromthe assumptionthatin some waysand at least some of the time it has an impact,for betteror worse,on the conduct of the nation's externalpolicy.Certainlyit is easy to find public expressionsby policy makers avowingthe importanceof public opinion. Duringhis debateswithStephenDouglas,AbrahamLincoln assertedthat, "withpublic sentimenton its side, everythingsucceeds; with public sentiment against it, nothingsucceeds"; and in 1936 Secretaryof State Cordell Hull stated that,"sincethe timewhen ThomasJefferson insistedupon a 'decent respectto the opinions of mankind,'public opinion has controlledforeignpolicyin all democracies"(NewYorkTimes, Dec. 6, 1936). Althoughsuch hyperbolicstatementsare unlikelyto withstandseriousempirical scrutiny,the drivingforce behind much of the post-WorldWar II attentionto public opinion on foreignpolicy issues was the fear that an ill-informedand emotional mass public would serve as a powerfulconstrainton the conduct of American diplomacy, establishing unwise limits on policy makers, creating otherwisedoing unrealisticexpectationsabout whatwas feasiblein foreignaffairs, seriousmischiefto Americandiplomacyand, giventheAmericanrole in theworld, As BernardCohen (1973) demonstratedin perhaps even to internationalstability. a criticalsurveyof the literature,however,the constrainingrole of public opinion was oftenassertedbut rarelydemonstrated-or even put to a systematic test. By the middle of the 1960s a consensus in factseemed to emerge on a third point: Public opinion has littleif any real impact on policy. Or, as the point was made most pithilyby one State Department official:"To hell with public opinion.... We should lead, and not follow" (quoted in Cohen, 1973:62). The weightof research evidence cast doubt on the potencyof public opinion as a constraintupon, foreignpolicy-making. drivingforcebehind,or even a significant For example, a classic studyof the public-legislatorrelationshiprevealed that constituents'attitudeson foreignpolicyhad less impacton membersof the House of Representativesthan did theirviews on domestic issues (Miller and Stokes, 1963). Cohen's research on the foreignpolicy bureaucracyindicated that State Departmentofficialshad a rathermodest interestin public opinion, and to the extent that they even thought about the public, it was as an entityto be "educated" rather than a lodestar by which to be guided (Cohen, 1973). The propositionthatthe presidenthas "almosta freehand" in the conduct of foreign affairsreceived support from other analysts,including Lipset (1966), LaFeber (1977), Levering(1978), Paterson(1979), and Graebner (1983). This period also witnesseda proliferationof case studiesof keyforeignpolicy decisions; with rare exceptions, however, they make no reference to public opinion. But it is not alwaysclear whetherthatis because: (1) public opinion was irrelevantas an explanation to the decisions under consideration,(2) decisionmakersquietlyanticipatepublic opinion withoutconsciouslydoing so, (3) it was excluded from the research design and thus no effortwas made to assess its impact,or (4) disproportionateresearchattentionto internationalcrises-events thatare usuallycharacterizedby shortdecision time-tended to exclude episodes in whichdecisionsare the culminationof a long politicalprocess;all otherthings being equal, the more protractedthe decision process,the more likelyare policy makersto be subjected to the impactof public opinion throughthe activitiesof Congress,pressuregroups,the media,and opinion leaders. OLE R. HOLSTI 445 Although these studies did not answer all questions about the impact of the public-for example, the realitiesof researchaccess required Cohen to focus on preciselythose personswho are most shelteredfromthe effectsof elections-the weightof the evidence would at least have assuaged those who shared fearsthat mass public opinion "has shownitselfto be a dangerousmasterof decision when the stakesare lifeand death" (Lippmann, 1955:20). The Renaissanceof Interestin Public Opinion and ForeignPolicy Justas WorldWar II and fearsof postwarisolationismamong the mass public gave rise to concern about public opinion and its impacton foreignpolicy,the war in Vietnamwas the impetusfora renewedinterestin the subject.It was a majorcatalyst in stimulatinga reexaminationof the consensusthathad emergedduringthe two decades afterWorldWar II. The VietnamWar had at least twodirecteffects.Most broadly, manyof thosewho had believedthata strongerexecutivehand on the tiller of public policy,relatively freefromthe whimsand vagariesof public moods, best servesboth nationalinterestsand global stability, came to reexaminetheirviewsin the lightof the VietnamWar. Indeed, the widelyread columnistWalterLippmann, who only a littlemore than a decade earlier had despaired of the tyranny of a fecklesspublic and had called for a strongerexecutiveto counteractthe mass public, became a leading criticof the Johnson administration's Vietnam policy; eventuallyhe came to regardthepublic,whichhad become increasingly skepticalof thewareffort, as more enlightenedthanthe administration. At a narrower level, some criticsof the war became increasinglypersuaded that the Gallup, Harris,and other commercialpolls distortedpublic attitudestoward the war by posing excessivelyrestrictiveand simplisticquestions. For example, among the most widelyasked questions was whetherrespondentssupportedor opposed currentAmerican policy in Vietnam; deeper probes that might have offeredrespondents an opportunityto express their views about other policy optionswere farless commonlyemployedby these pollingorganizations.Thus, in addition to secondaryanalysesof surveydata relatingto the war (e.g., Mueller, 1973), the conflictin SoutheastAsia also stimulatedindependentsurveysdesigned specificallyto assess foreignpolicyin greaterdepth and breadth than the typical surveyconductedbyGallup and the othermajorpollingorganizations. The firstof these studies, the Verba-Stanfordsurveys,focused on American policy in Vietnam (Verba, Brody,Parker,Nie, Polsby,Ekman, and Black, 1967; Verba and Brody,1970). Verba and his colleagues in factfound supportfor the administration'sVietnam policy, but they also unearthed approval for such alternativepolicies as negotiatingan end to the conflict.The period followingthe Verba-Stanford polls has witnesseda proliferationof studieswitha foreignaffairs focus,includingsurveysof both the mass public (Rielly,1975, 1979, 1983, 1987, 1991; Hurwitz and Peffley,1987, 1990; Americans Talk Security,1987-1991; Americans Talk Issues, 1991) and of opinion leaders (Barton, 1974-75, 1980; Rielly, 1975, 1979, 1983, 1987, 1991; Russett and Hanson, 1975; Holsti and Rosenau, 1984, 1988, 1990; Chittickand Billingsley,1989; Koopman, Snyder,and Jervis,1990a, 1990b, 1991). As a consequence, we are no longer totallydependent on evidence generated by the major polling organizations.Moreover, these independent surveysare oftendesigned withpolicyand/or theoreticalconcerns be probed bysecondaryanalysesof the Gallup and other, thatcan onlyimperfectly more generalpublic opinion polls. Thus, armed with growingcentral archivesof data generated by the major polling organizationsas well as evidence produced by the independent surveys, duringthe past twodecades analystshave begun to challengeimportantaspectsof the consensusdescribedabove. 446 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy #1:Is PublicOpinionReallySo Volatile? Challenge challenge to the Almond thesisthat WilliamCasparypresentedthe firstsystematic public opinion on foreignaffairsis best characterizedbyvolatilemoods.4He took issuewithAlmond'sheavyrelianceon a singlequestionin whichrespondentswere asked to identify"the mostimportantissue today."Caspary'sanalysisof a broader set of questionsled him to conclude that "Americanpublic opinion is characterized by a strongand stable permissivemood" towardactiveinternationalinvolvement (Caspary,1970:546). Mueller's (1973) studyof public opinion towardthe Korean and Vietnamwars posed anotherchallenge to the thesisof mindlesschanges in public attitudes.To be sure,public supportforthe U.S. wareffortin both conflictseventuallychanged, but in waysthatseemed explicableand rational,ratherthanrandomand mindless. More specifically, he foundthatincreasingpublic oppositionto the conflictstraced out a patternthatfita curveof risingbattledeaths,suggestingthatthe public used an understandable,ifsimple,heuristicto assessAmericanpolicy.5 The most comprehensive challenge to the Almond-Lippmannthesis has emergedfromstudiesconductedbyBenjaminPage and RobertShapiro.Their evidence includes all questionsthathave been posed by major polling organizations since the inception of systematicsurveysin the 1930s. Of the more than 6000 questions,almost 20 percenthave been asked at least twice,providingPage and and change in mass Shapiro witha large data set to assess the degree of stability public attitudes.Employinga cutoffpoint of a differenceof 6 percentfromone surveyto another to distinguishbetween continuityand change, theyfound that mass opinion in the aggregateis in factcharacterizedby a good deal of stability, and thatthisis no less true of foreignpolicythan on domesticissues (Page and Shapiro, 1988). More important,when attitudeshiftstake place, theyseem to be neitherrandom nor 180 degrees removed fromthe true state of world affairs. Rather, changes appear to be "reasonable, event driven"reactions to the real upon whichtheyare based is marginallyadequate at world,even ifthe information best.Theyconcluded that, all the rapid shifts[in public opinion] we foundwere related to political virtually eventswhichsensiblecitizenswould and economic circumstancesor to significant take into account. In particular,mostabruptforeignpolicychanges took place in connection withwars,confrontations, or crises in which major changes in the actions of the United States or other nations quite naturallyaffectpreferences about whatpolicies to pursue.6 Because theiranalysesare based on aggregateresponsesratherthan on panel studies in which the same respondents are interviewedrepeatedly,Page and one aspect of the volatility question: Precisely Shapiro cannot address definitively whatproportionof individualshave in factchanged theirminds?For an issue on whichthe public divided 50 percent-50percentin each of two timeperiods,it is approaching theoretically possible thatall respondentsdid so. However,volatility 4 By 1960, Almond himselfwas backing away from his most pessimisticdiagnoses. See his new preface to a Policy. reprintingof TheAmerican PeopleandForeign1 5 "During the summerof 1965, as the JohnsonAdministration was movingtowardfatefuldecisions regarding Vietnam,George Ball warned:'We can't win,' he said, his deep voice dominatingthe Cabinet Room. 'The warwillbe long and protracted,withheavycasualties.The mostwe can hope foris a messyconclusion.We mustmeasure this loss thatwillresultfromwithdrawal.'Producinga chartthatcorrelatedpublic long-termprice againstthe short-term opinion withAmericancasualtiesin Korea, Ball predictedthat the Americanpublic would not supporta long and inconclusivewar" (Clifford,1991:412). 6 Page and Shapiro (1982:34); see also Page and Shapiro (1983, 1984, 1988, 1992); Page, Shapiro,anidDempsey (1987); Shapiro and Page (1988). OLE R. HOLSTI 447 this magnitudeseems highlyunlikelybecause, as Page and Shapiro have shown, opinion changestend to be in directionsthat"makesense"in termsof events. ratherthan The volatilitythesiscan be tested most directlyby individual-level aggregate analysisof opinion data. Using differentmethods for correctingfor thatat the individual measurementerror,severalstudieshave shownconvincingly level mass foreignpolicy attitudesare everybit as stable as domestic attitudes (Achen, 1975; Peffleyand Hurwitz,1992a). These studiesrevealed an impressive level of stabilityduring times of constancyin the internationalenvironment.A in panel studyby Peffleyand Hurwitz(1992a) also foundverysubstantialstability policyattitudesand internationalimages even duringa period-the late 1980sthatwitnessedrapid and dramaticchanges in Soviet-Americanrelationsand other aspectsof internationalaffairs. Similar conclusions,supportingPage and Shapiro and castingdoubt on the Almond-Lippmannthesis,have also emergedfromotherstudies.Jentleson(1992) era, variationsin public supportforthe use of foundthatduringthe post-Vietnam force are best explained by differencesbetween force to coerce foreignpolicy restraintby others,and force to influenceor impose internalpolitical changes withinanotherstate;the formergoal has receivedmuch strongersupportthan the latter.7 An interestingvariantof the "rationalpublic" thesisstipulatesthat the public attemptsto moderateAmericanbehavior towardthe USSR by expressingpreferwhile supporting ences for a conciliatorystance from hawkishadministrations more assertivepoliciesfromdovishones (Nincic,1988). To the extentthatone can to generalizefromthisstudyfocusingon the Carterand Reagan administrations otherperiods or otheraspectsof foreignpolicy,it furtherchallengesthe AlmondLippmann thesis-indeed, it turnsthat propositionon its head-for it identifies the public as a source of moderationand continuity and ratherthan of instability unpredictability. It is important to emphasize that none of these challenges to the Almond-Lippmannthesisis based on some newlyfoundevidencethatthe public is in factwell informedabout foreignaffairs.Not onlydo polls repeatedlyrevealthat the mass public has a very thin veneer of factual knowledge about politics, economics,and geography;theyalso reveal that it is poorlyinformedabout the of specificsof conflicts,treaties,negotiationswith other nations, characteristics weapons systems,foreignleaders, and the like. Because the modest factualbasis upon which the mass public reacts to international affairs remains an unchallenged-and unchallengeable-fact, we are faced with a puzzle: If a generallypoorlyinformedmass public does indeed reactto internationalaffairsin rational manner,what are the means that permitit to do so? an events-driven, Recall thata not-insignificant bodyof researchevidenceindicatedthatmasspublic attitudeslack the kind of ideological structurethatwould providesome coherence acrossspecificissuesand persistencethroughtime. LackStructure and Coherence? #2:Do PublicAttitudes Challenge Philip Converse's (1964) chapteron mass beliefsystemsis one of the mostwidely cited studiesin the Americanpoliticalscience literature.In recentyearsit has also spawned a vast literaturewhich has, on the one hand, vigorouslychallenged his findingsand, on the other,supportedthe main thrustof Converse'sconclusions 7 For additional evidence about the "rationalpublic," the stabilityof policy preferences,and issue voting,see Bennett(1972:742), Free and Watts(1980:50), Wittkopf(1986, 1990), Graham (1986, 1988, 1989), Krosnick(1988a, 1988b, 1990, 1991), Russett(1990), and Peffleyand Hurwitz(1992a, 1992b). 448 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy thatmass public attitudeslack ideological structure, whereasthose of leaders are characterizedby far greater coherence. Part of the debate is methodological, centeringon the manner in which questionsare framed,the clarityof questions, the degree to whichthe unsure are prodded to statea position,and similarissues of research procedures. Did the evidentiarybase include enough questions to support the conclusions? Did the analytical methods deal adequately with problems of measurementerror? Does an analysisthat examines correlations across specificpublic policy issues exhaust the possible structuresthat mightbe used to lend coherence to politicalthinking?Studies thathave raised significant methodologicalquestionsabout the Conversefindingsinclude Achen (1975) and Sullivan,Piereson,and Marcus (1978). Anotherpartof the controversy focuseson trends,specifically on the durability of findingsthat,to a largedegree,drewfromevidencegeneratedduringthe 1950s. This was a period of American economic, political,and militarydominance in foreignaffairs-theshock of Sputnikin 1957 notwithstanding-with the 1956 and 1960 elections takingplace afterthe Korean War and before escalation of the Vietnamconflict.Domestically,the Eisenhoweryearsweremarkedbyrelatively low inflationand unemploymentand, despite the Montgomerybus boycott and Greensborosit-ins,the fullimpactof the civilrightsmovementhad yetto be felt. Accordingto the critics,this period, both celebrated and criticizedfor marking "the end of ideology,"is insufficiently representative for assessingthe degree of ideological consistencyamong the general public. In support of that view, a number of analystsfound that,beginningwith the Johnson-Goldwater election campaign of 1964, ideological consistencyamong the public did in factincrease (Nie and Anderson,1974; Nie, Verba, and Petrocik,1976). Some corroborating evidence also appeared to emerge from Hero's (1969) assessmentof public opinion polls on domesticand foreignpolicyissuesfromthe late 1930s to 1967. In general,he founda veryweak relationshipbetweenthe two,withsome indications of a strengthening duringthepost-Eisenhower years. Those who claim to have found a greaterideological consistencyamong the general public duringthe turbulentera of the 1960s and 1970s have also encountered criticism.Are the claims of greaterissue consistencyrooted in increasing ideological consciousness?Alternatively, are theymerelythe resultof parrotingof ideological rhetoric,or of some methodologicalartifact?This is not the place to providea blow-by-blow account of the manyand variedanswersto theseand other questionson the issue; forexcellentand detailed summariesof the vastliterature, see Kinder (1983), Kinderand Sears (1985), and Snidermanand Tetlock (1986). It will sufficeto say that there appears to be an emergingconsensus that public responses to issues are not adequately captured by the most familiarbipolar dimensions: liberal-to-conservative If these or internationalist-to-isolationist. dimensionsconstitutethe standard,then mass public attitudesdo indeed appear to lack structure.Given that tentativeconclusion,does the literatureon foreign policy attitudesreveal anythingelse about organizingconcepts that mightlend some coherence to masspublic attitudeson internationalaffairs? Althoughthe more recentresearchliteraturehas yetto createa consensuson all aspects of the question, there does appear to be a considerable convergenceof findingson twogeneralpointsrelatingto beliefstructures: 1. Even though the general public may be ratherpoorly informed,attitudes about foreignaffairsare in factstructuredin at least moderatelycoherent ways.Indeed, low informationand an ambiguous foreignpolicy environment are actually likely to motivate rather than preclude some type of attitudestructure. 2. A singleisolationist-to-internationalist dimensioninadequatelydescribesthe main dimensionsof public opinion on internationalaffairs. OLE R. HOLSTI 449 An early study,based on the firstof the quadrennial Chicago Council on employedfactoranalysisand othermethodsto ForeignRelations(CCFR) surveys, "conservative uncover three foreignpolicy outlooks: "liberal internationalism," internationalism,"and "non-internationalism"(Mandelbaum and Schneider, 1979). A comparable trichotomy("three-headed eagle") emerged from early analysesof the data on opinion leadersgeneratedbythe ForeignPolicyLeadership Project(FPLP) (Holsti,1979; Holstiand Rosenau, 1979, 1984). Others have questioned the divisionof foreignpolicyattitudesinto three types and theyhave offeredcompellingevidence in supportof ratherthan dimensions, theircritiques.Chittickand Billingsley(1989) have undertakenboth originaland includingone secondaryanalyseswhich indicated the need for three dimensions, that taps unilateralist-multilateralist sentiments,not three types,to describe adequatelythe foreignpolicybeliefsof both the masspublic and leaders. (See also and Travis,1990). Bardes and Oldendick,1978; Chittick,Billingsley, A major set of contributionsto the debate about how best to describe foreign policy attitudeshas come fromWittkopf'sexemplarysecondaryanalysesof the 1986, 1990). His CCFR surveysof both the general public and leaders (Wittkopf, revealedthatwitha fromthe firstfourCCFR surveys, results,developed inductively singleexception,twodimensionsare necessaryto describeforeignpolicyattitudes: (MI) and "support-opposecooperative "support-opposemilitantinternationalism" internationalism" (CI). Dichotomizingand crossingthese dimensionsyieldsfour (support MI, oppose CI), types, with the quadrants labeled as hard-liners (oppose MI, oppose CI), and internationalists (supportMI, supportCI), isolationists accommodationists (oppose MI, supportCI). Support for Wittkopf'sMI/Cl scheme also emerges froma reanalysisof the FPLP data on Americanopinion leaders (Holsti and Rosenau, 1990). That study put the MI/Cl scheme to a demandingtestbecause of threemajor differencesin the data sets: (1) The CCFR surveyswere undertakenin 1974, 1978, 1982, and 1986, whereasthe fourFPLP studiesfollowedtwoyearslaterin each case; (2) the two sets of surveyshave only a fewquestionnaireitemsin common; and (3) the MI/Cl scheme was developed largelyfromdata on the mass public, whereasthe FPLP surveysfocusedsolelyon opinion leaders. It may be worthnoting that although the originsof the MI/Cl scheme are and cooperativeinternationalism strictly inductive,the militantinternationalism dimensionscorrespondcloselyto the mostvenerableapproaches to international relations: realism and liberalism.Realism views conflictbetween nations as a natural state of affairsrather than an aberrationthat is subject to permanent amelioration.Such realistconcepts as securitydilemma,relativecapabilities,and "zero sum"viewof conflictare also basic to the MI dimension.There are similarly dimension. intimatelinksbetweenliberalismand the cooperativeinternationalism Liberalism denies that conflictis an immutableelement of relations between nations.It definessecurityin termsthatare broader than the geopolitical-military dimensions, and it emphasizes the cooperative aspects of relations between nations.Institutionbuilding,improvedinternationaleducation and communication,and tradeare but a fewof thewaysin whichnationsmayjointlygain and thus mitigate,if not eliminate,the harshestfeaturesof internationalrelationsin an anarchic system.In short,the CL dimension shares importantelementswiththe liberal school of internationalrelationstheory.These MI and CL dimensionsalso seem clearlyrelated to other conceptualizationsof Americanthoughton foreign affairs.For example,Hughes's (1980:50) distinctionbetweenthe "security culture" and "equityculture"in Americanforeignpolicy,or Billington's(1987:632) cateand "idealist-liberals" appear to parallel, if not gories of "realist-conservatives" matchexactly,the MI and CL dimensions. Even if one accepts the necessityof tapping attitudeson both militantand however,there is also some evidence that theyare cooperativeinternationalism, 450 PublicOpinionand ForeignPolicy foreignpolicyattitudes.A not sufficient to describeall contoursof contemporary has been suggestedbya further distinctionbetweenunilateralismand multilateralism numberof studies(Wittkopf, 1986; Hinckley,1988, 1992; Chittickand Billingsley, 1989; Russett,1990). It is not reasonable to demand that any belief structure and thereis indeed rather should encompassall possibleaspectsof foreignaffairs, persuasiveevidence thatattitudestowardsome ratherimportantissues cut across an issue that is the main dimensionsidentifiedabove. Trade and protectionism, likelyto become more ratherthan less contentiousduringthe 1990s,is one such example; questions revolvingaround Israel and American policy toward that nationappear to formanotherclusterof attitudesthatdoes not fitneatlyinto the MI/Cl scheme. A somewhatdifferent emergesfromseveral approach towardattitudestructures importantstudiesof the mass public conducted byJonHurwitzand Mark Peffley (1987, 1990; Peffleyand Hurwitz,1985, 1992a, 1992b). In contrastto Converse's search for "horizontal"coherence that relies on correlationsamong attitudes towardvarious issues, Hurwitzand Peffleyproposed and tested a hierarchically organized foreignpolicybelief structurein which specificpolicypreferencesare anticommunism, and isolationism)that,in turn, derivedfrompostures(militarism, are assumed to be constrainedby a set of corevalues (moralityof warfare,ethnocentrism)about the internationalcommunity.They found thatsuch structures did in factexist among respondentsto theirsurveys.Thus, a fewrathergeneral or a generalpreferencetowarda beliefs-forexample,attitudestowardmilitarism, "tough-minded"approach toward foreignaffairs-appear to serve as heuristics whichenable one to respondin a reasonablycoherentmannerto a broad range of issues, including defense spending, nuclear arms policy, militaryinvolvement 1987). abroad,Sovietpolicy,and internationaltrade (Hurwitzand Peffley, It is importantto state once again that none of these studies challenges the overwhelmingevidence that the American public is poorly informed about internationalaffairs;indeed, even the Persian Gulf War, the firstconflictto be telecastin real time,increased the normallylow level of informationamong the generalpublicbyonlya verymodestamount (Bennett,1992). Rather,thesestudies appear to suggestthat,even in the absence of much factualknowledge,members in order of the mass public use some simple-perhaps even simplistic-heuristics to make some sense of an increasingly complexworld;a fewsalientcriteriarather than completeinformationmayserveas the bases ofjudgment.Stated differently, although lacking a deep reservoirof factualinformation,membersof the mass public mayoperate as "cognitivemisers,"employinga fewsuperordinatebeliefsto guide theirthinkingon a broad range of issues.For furtherevidence thatpeople organize theirpoliticalworldsin richerand more diversewaysthan indicatedby Converseand his colleagues,see Conoverand Feldman (1984). Clearlythe recentresearchhas yetto produce a consensuson manyimportant issues relatingto the structureand foreignpolicybeliefsamong the mass public. Nevertheless,it is evidentthat the earlierconsensusdepictingpublic attitudesas lackinganyreal coherence has been challengedfromvariousquarters.As a result of substantialempiricalresearch,there is now a good deal of credible evidence suggestingthatthe public does use variousheuristics-althoughnot necessarilythe or internationalist-isolationist traditional liberal-to-conservative blueprints-for organizingpoliticalthinking. #3:Is PublicOpinionReallyImpotent? Challenge Amongthe mostimportantquestionsabout public opinion are: To whatextent,on what kinds of issues, under what circumstances,and in what typesof political OLE R. HOLSTI 451 ifany,does it have an impacton public policy?If it has an influence,what systems, are the means bywhichpublic attitudesmake theirimpactfeltbydecision-makers? These are also the most difficultquestions,for our abilityto answerthem is not materiallyenhanced by the manytechnicalimprovements thathave characterized public opinion research during the past half century:better sampling designs, models greaterattentionto constructionof questions,more sophisticatedstatistical to analyzethe data, and the like. Not surprisingly, then,we have a good deal more systematic evidence describingthe stateof, or trendsin, public opinion than on how it has affectedthe actual conductof foreignaffairs. As noted earlier,most of the evidence throughthe 1960s pointed towardthe conclusionof public impotencein the foreignpolicy-makingprocess.Even when thereappeared to be some correspondencebetweenpublic sentimentsand foreign policy,not all analystswere prepared to accept the inferencethatthe formerhad any independent impact on the latter.Accordingto several of them,including revisionisthistorians,any evidence of a correlationbetween public opinion and of effortsby policy foreignpolicy merelyservesto underscore the effectiveness makers,aided and abettedbypliantprintand electronicmedia, to manipulatethe masspublic intoacceptanceof therulingelites'class-basedinterests. There is no shortageof evidence thatmostpost-WorldWar II presidentshave followedTheodore Rooseveltin thinkingthatthe WhiteHouse is a "bullypulpit," whetherit is used to "scarethe hell out of them"in orderto gain supportforaid to Greece and Turkey,to warn against the dangers of "unwarrantedinfluence, whethersoughtor unsought,by the military-industrial complex,"or to drum up supportforassistanceto the contrasin Nicaragua.It is also evidentthatsuch efforts have not been equally successful.At least one noted public opinion analysthas asserted that the relationshipbetween leaders and the public has changed"farewellto 'the Presidentknowsbest,"'as he put it-but it remainsto be demonstratedthatthe equation has been permanently changed (Yankelovich,1979). The more difficult questionconcernsthe influencein the otherdirection.How much did public impatiencelead the Carteradministration to embarkon the illfated effortto free Americanhostages in Iran, or the Reagan administrationto withdrawU.S. Marines fromLebanon? Did PresidentKennedygenuinelybelieve thathe would be impeached should he failto forceremovalof Sovietmissilesfrom Cuba, as he told his brother,or was he merelyseekingto bolsterdecisionsarrived at forreasons thathad nothingto do withpublic opinion? Perhaps a more telling example from the Caribbean confrontationemerges from recentlypublished transcripts of a crucial White House meetingon October 27, the day before the crisiswas resolvedpeacefully.It appears that Kennedywas prepared to accept a compromise solution-withdrawalof Soviet missiles in Cuba in exchange for removalof Americanmissilesin Turkey-ratherthan initiatea furtherescalation of the confrontation, and thathe would have done so in large part becauseit would had been havebeenhardtoexplaintothepublicwhysucha seemingly equitable arrangement rejected(Bundyand Blight,1987). Some other anecdotal evidence may also be suggestive,but it hardlyoffers irrefutable answersto thisquestion.FranklinD. Rooseveltwas the firstpresidentto make extensiveuse of public opinion data, but during recent decades every few administration has employedpublic opinion professionals.We have relatively detailedaccountsfromtheseanalystsabout how theirexpertiseand data wereused in the policyprocess,but those thatexist (e.g., Cantril,1967; Beal and Hinckley, 1984; Hinckley,1992) suggestthat the mass public is not viewed merelyas an essentiallyshapeless,malleable lump that can readilybe molded throughpublic relations activitiesand compliant media to meet the immediate needs of the administration.Hadley Cantril, who undertook public opinion analyses for 452 PublicOpinionand ForeignPolicy PresidentsRoosevelt,Eisenhower,and Kennedy,summarizedhis experiencein this manner:"I wantto emphasizethatno claimis made here thatthe [publicopinion] data and suggestionsLambert and I provided the President [Roosevelt] were crucial to his decisions. But actions taken were certainlyveryoften completely consistentwithour recommendations"(Cantril,1967:42). Althoughit bears repeatingthatthe evidence describingpublic opinion stillfar outstrips,both in quantityand quality,that on the causal links between public opinion and foreignpolicy,researchin recentyearshas begun to castdoubt on the earlierconsensusabout public impotence.In addition to anecdotal evidence,two classes of studies have contributedto challengingthe thesis that foreignpolicy processes are imperviousto impact from the public: quantitative/correlational analysesand case studies. Severalrecentquantitativestudieshave challengedsome importantfoundations of the theorythat,at least on foreignand defense issues, the public is virtually freeagents impotent.One elementof thatthesisis thatpolicymakersare relatively on foreignpolicy questions because these issues pose few dangers of electoral retributionby voters; elections are said to be decided by domestic questions, especiallythosesometimesdescribedas "pocketbook"or "breadand butter"issues. However,a systematicstudyof presidentialcampaigns between 1952 and 1984 revealedthatin fiveof the nine electionsduringthe period, foreignpolicyissues had "largeeffects."Or, as the authorsput it,when presidentialcandidatesdevote campaign time and other resourcesto foreignpolicyissues,theyare not merely "waltzingbeforea blind audience" (Aldrich,Sullivan,and Borgida,1989). Recent research on votingbehavior has also emphasized the importanceof retrospectiveevaluations on performanceon voter choice among candidates, especiallywhen one of themis an incumbent(Fiorina,1981; Abramson,Aldrich, and Rhode, 1990). Because votersare perceived as punishingincumbentcandidates or partiesforforeignpolicyfailures(forexample,the Iran hostageepisode) or rewardingthemforsuccesses (forexample, the invasionof Panama to capture General Noriega), decisionsbyforeignpolicyleaders maybe made in anticipation of public reactionsand the probabilitiesof successor failure. kind of testin a study The electoralretributionhypothesisreceiveda different ofAmericanpolicytowardChina duringthe threedecades followingestablishment of Mao Tse-tung'sgovernmentin 1949. Kusnitzfound that,withfewexceptions, the correspondencebetween public preferencesand U.S. policywas remarkably high.At timespolicyled opinion and at othertimesopinion led policy,but on the whole the two remained in harmony.These findingsare explained by issue visibility, partisan differences,and the nonrandom changes of opinion, which combined to create the belief among leaders that the possibilityof electoral retributionrequired them to pay close attentionto public opinion on the China issue (Kusnitz,1984:173,176). Anotherrecentstudyseems to cast some doubt on the universalvalidityof the classicMiller-Stokes(1963) findingthat,compared to domesticissues,public attitudes on foreignpolicyquestionshave farless impacton membersof Congress.A careful analysisof voting on Pentagon appropriationsat the beginning of the Reagan administration's defense buildup revealed that ". . . public opinion was a powerfulforceforpolicychange in the realm of defensespendingin the firstyear of the Reagan administration.Moreover, the impact of constituencyopinion broad-based,influencingall sortsof representaappears to have been remarkably tivesacrossa wide spectrumofspecificdefensespendingissues"(Bartels,1991:467). Finally,two major studieshave measured the congruencebetween changes in public preferencesand a broad range of policies overextended periods.The first, a studyof public opinion and policy outcomes spanning the years 1960-1974, revealed thatin almost two thirdsof 222 cases, policyoutcomes correspondedto OLE R. HOLSTI 453 public preferences.The consistencywas especiallyhigh (92%) on foreignpolicy issues. Monroe (1979:11) offersthree possible explanations for his findings: Foreignpolicyissues permitmore decision-makingby the executive,are likelyto be the object of relatively less interestand influenceby organizedinterestgroups, and are especiallysusceptibleto elite manipulation.The second studycoveredan even longerspan-1935 to 1979-which included 357 significant changesof public preferences(Page and Shapiro, 1983). Of the 231 instancesof subsequentpolicy changes,153 (66%) werecongruentwithchangesin public preferences.There was littledifferencein the level of congruencefordomestic(70%) and foreignpolicy (62%) issues. Although anecdotal evidence and correlational analyses can make useful contributionstowardunderstandingthe opinion-policyrelationship,theyare not an entirelysatisfactory substitutefor intensivecase studiesthat could shed more directlighton how,ifat all, public opinion influencesforeignpolicy-making.It is to describethe stateof or trendsin public opinion on an issue not whollysufficient immediatelypreceding or during foreignpolicy decisions.A findingthat major decisionsseemed to be correlatedwithpublic preferencesdoes not,byitself,establisha causal link;forexample,policy-makers mightbe respondingto pressuresand constraintsfrom the internationalsystem-as realist theoristsinsist that they should-without anysignificant attentionto public sentimentson the issue,even if those attitudes are highly congruent with those of the decision-makers. the actual directionof causalitymightrun frompolicy-makers to the Alternatively, public ratherthanvice versa,as depicted by criticswho describethe public as the malleable targetsof public relationseffortsby Americanelites (Ginsburg,1988; Margolisand Mauser, 1989). When opinion change precedes policychange, this loses potency.However,we could not rule out stillanother possiinterpretation bility:the administration manipulatesevents;the events,now part of the information available to the public,resultin opinion change, followedby policychanges thatare congruentwithopinions.A somewhatrelatedvariantof thissequence is the "rally'round the president"hypothesis,accordingto whichthe executivemay and responding manipulatethe public indirectly byundertakingexternalinitiatives to eventsabroad in a mannercalculated to increase his popularitywithdomestic constituents(Brodyand Shapiro,1989; Marra,Ostrom,and Simon,1990). Among the more difficultcases are those dealing with public opinion as a possible constrainton action. During the 1980s, the Reagan administration undertook a massivepublic relationscampaign of dubious legalityto generate public support for assistance to the "contra" rebels in Nicaragua (Parry and Kornbluth,1988), but a carefulanalysisof surveyson the issue revealed that a majorityof the public opposed Americanmilitary involvementin CentralAmerica (Sobel, 1989; see also Hinckley,1992). Would the Reagan administrationhave intervenedmore directlyor massively in Nicaraguaor El Salvadorin the absence of such attitudes?Solid evidenceabout contemporary non-eventsis, to understatethe case, ratherhard to come by.Case studiesseem to be the onlywayto addresssuch questions,although even this approach is not whollyfree of potentialproblems. Does an absence of documentaryreferencesto public opinion indicate a lack of interestby decision-makers?Alternatively, was attentionto public attitudesso deeplyingrainedin theirworkinghabitsthatit was unnecessaryto make constant referencesto it? Are frequentreferencesto public opinion an indication of a significantimpact on decisions-or of a desire on the part of officialsto be "on record"as havingpaid attentionto public sentiments? These examples do not implythat we are limited to simple one-directional models of the links between the public and policy makers; examples of more complex alternativesmay be found in Rosenau (1961), Graber (1968), Russett (1990), Hughes (1978), and Hinckley (1992). Moreover,a full analysisof the 454 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy opinion-policylinks would often require explorationsinto many aspects of the domesticpoliticalprocess,includingthe role of partiesand candidatesin raising issues, the impact of interestgroups, the role of the media, the level of elite competitionon specificissues,and the like.The literatureon each of thesetopics, even if strictly limitedto foreignpolicyissues in the United States,is enormous, and space limitationspreclude their inclusion in this essay; Bauer, Pool, and Dexter (1963) and Hughes (1978) are usefulin thisrespect. In orderto selectbetweencompetinghypothesesabout opinion-policylinkages, alternativesto carefullycraftedcase studies employing there are no satisfactory interviewsand/or archivalresearch designed to uncover how, if at all, decisionmakersperceive public opinion; feel themselvesmotivatedor constrainedby it; factorit into theiranalysesof policyoptions; and otherwisetake it into account when selectinga course of action,includinga decisionnot to takeexternalaction. Althoughthe literatureaddressingthese questionsis not large,especiallywhen compared to the number of studies that describe the state of public opinion, several examples illustratethis type of research. The availabilityof substantial collectionsof documents relatingto the 1914 European crisisenabled Richard Fagen (1960) to studythe uses and assessmentsof public opinion duringtheweeks leading up to World War I. Doris Graber undertookan intensivestudyof four decisionsduringthe earlyperiod of Americanhistory-Adams'sdecision to renew negotiationswith France in 1800, the Louisiana Purchase, Madison's policies leading up to the War of 1812, and enunciationof the Monroe Doctrine.Despite personality,ideological, and other differencesamong the four presidents,she found that in each case public opinion was "an importantfactorin decision making,but byno means the mostimportantsinglefactor"(Graber,1968:318). A studyofforeignpolicy-makingabout a centurylatercame to a ratherdifferent conclusion.RobertHilderbrandwas unable to discoverthatpublic opinion had a significantimpact on foreign policy-making during the quarter century (1897-1921) encompassingthe McKinley,Roosevelt,Taft,and Wilson administrations;to the extentthatpublic opinion entered into executivediscussions,it was onlyafterpolicydecisionshad been made (Hilderbrand,1981:202). Stilldifferent findingsemerged from a studyof public opinion and foreignpolicy from the period leading up to World War II through PresidentTruman's 1947 speech requestingaid to Greece and Turkey.Michael Leigh (1976) testedtwoapproaches to the foreignpolicy process: the traditionalmodel that the public constrains Americanpolicymakers,versusthe radical model thatmanipulationof the public in favorof predeterminedpolicychoices not only takes place but also invariably succeeds.His findingsvalidatedneithermodel. impacton policyemerges Strikingevidence thatpublic opinion has a significant froma studyof fourcases of Americanarmscontrolpolicy-internationalcontrol of atomicenergy,the LimitedTest Ban Treaty,the SALT I/ABM Treaty,and SALT II-spanning every administrationbetween Presidents Truman and Reagan. Graham (1989) used a researchdesign thatincluded an analysisof over500 public opinion surveysand an examinationof primarysource materialsto determineif correlationsbetweenpublic opinion and policydecisionswere causal or spurious. The evidence revealed thatpublic opinion had an importantimpacton decisions at all stagesof the policyprocess,fromgettingon the agenda throughnegotiation, ratification,and implementation.Moreover, its impact varied, depending on whether public support for a policy reached the level of majority(50-59%), consensus(60-69%), preponderant(70-79%) or virtuallyunanimous (80%+). Studies of the opinion-policylinksare not limitedto cases in which sufficient time has passed to permit full examination of the relevant archives. Philip Powlick'sanalysisof the role of public opinion in U.S. decisions on the Lebanon OLE R. HOLSTI 455 interventionduring the firstReagan administrationrelied almost wholly on interviews withpolicymakers.Whereas public opinion influencedmanymid-level officialsand a fewhigher ones-for example, Caspar Weinberger8-it had little impact on others, including Ronald Reagan, Robert McFarlane, and George Shultz. Foreign policy officialstended to regard congressional moods as the relevantmanifestation of public opinion.9Powlickconcluded thatpublic opinion formedthe basis of severalrecommendationsto pull the Marinesout of Lebanon; it helped ensure thatthe decision to withdrawwould be warmlyreceivedby most officialsand members of Congress. However, President Reagan's decision to withdrawwas apparentlyless influencedby public opinion than by the kinds of externalsources that realistswould advocate following.Public opinion was thus onlyone of severalfactorsthatcame togetherto bringabout the evacuationfrom Beirutin February,1984 (Powlick,forthcoming). Taken as a whole, these case studieswould seem to suggestthatthe impactof public opinion has increasedduringrecentdecades. This tentativeconclusionalso receives some support from interviewsof foreignpolicy officials.Although the bureaucratsinterviewed by Powlick(1991) were not notablymore sanguineabout the public than were those takingpart in similarresearchby Cohen (1973) two decades earlier,theyweremore inclinedto accept the legitimacy of a public contribution to the policyprocess.Consequently,these officialstended to avoid policies thatwereseen as likelyto engenderpublic opposition.In contrast,Graham (1989) foundlittlechange over timebecause the public opinion has playedan important role in arms controldecisions since the Truman years.This, then, is one of the areas in whichthe need foradditionalresearchis mostapparent. OtherRecentResearchDevelopments OpinionLeaders Until recently,one of the glaringgaps in public opinion researchhas been the neglectof opinion leaders.Virtuallyall approaches to government-fromtheories that view the United States as a pluralistdemocracyto those that depict it as eliteswho seek to use foreignpolicy pseudo-democracyruled by self-perpetuating as an instrumentof narrow,class-basedinterests-recognizethe disproportionate influence of some citizens (Devine, 1970). Moreover,at least since Almond's seminalstudyof TheAmerican PeopleandForeign Policy(1950), it has been customary to distinguishbetweenvariousstrataof the public. Typicallya distinctionhas been drawnbetweenopinion leaders,the informedpublic,and the mass or uninformed public,althoughthe precise termsand the shape of the distribution among strata mayvaryfromstudyto study(compare Kreisberg,1949; Almond, 1950; Rosenau, 1961; Genco, 1984; and Neuman, 1986). But onlyrarelyhave systematic studiesof respondents in the top strata been undertaken. On occasion the Gallup Organizationhas surveyedsamples of personslistedin Who's Who in America-for example, a July1953 poll on tariffs-butnot frequently enough to be of value in 8Among six conditionsthatshould be met beforethe United Statescommitscombatforcesabroad, accordingto Weinberger,"theremustbe some reasonable assurancewe willhave the supportof the Americanpeople" (New York Times,November 26, 1984, A5:1). There is also evidence that General Schwarzkopfwas reluctantto fightin the PersianGulfwithoutappropriatepublic support(Hinckley,1992:20). 9 If congressionalmoods are more generallyviewed by the executiveas expressionsof public opinion, then it opens up another large body of evidence on interveningvariablesbetweenpublic opinion and foreignpolicy.For recentstudiesthatexplore theselinkageson the StrategicDefense Initiative, weapons procurement,and sanctionson South Africa,see Lindsay(1990, 1991) and Hill (1992). 456 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy assessing trends. Until the firstof the Chicago Council on Foreign Relations surveysin 1974 (Rielly,1975) -followed bysimilarstudiesin 1978, 1982, 1986,and 1990 (Rielly,1979, 1983, 1987, 1991)-and the ForeignPolicyLeadership surveys institutedtwoyearslater (1976, 1980, 1984, 1988, and 1992), therewas relatively littlesystematic informationabout leadershipviewson foreignaffairs.In addition to the CCFR and FPLP surveys, duringrecentyearstherehave also been a number of one-time studies of opinion leaders (Barton, 1974-75, 1980; Russett and Hanson, 1975; Sussman,1976; Chittickand Billingsley,1989). A continuingseries of surveys,focused on national securityexperts,has also been initiatedrecently (Koopman, Snyder,andJervis,1990a, 1990b, 1991). The prior question of identifying"opinion leaders" is among the possible barriersand sources of controversy in studiesof thistype.There is, of course, an extensivedebate on the precisedefinitionof opinion leaders in the United States, and if consensuson thatquestion were a prerequisiteto elite surveys,theywould bypassed never be undertaken.Those designingleadershipstudieshave typically the broader question and selected subjectsin one of twoways:(1) identifying key groups roles and then surveying a sample of those fillingthem,or (2) identifying thoughtto be logical sources of opinion leaders and then drawingsamples from directoriesor rostersof such persons-for example, biographiesin Who'sWhoin America, Who's Who in American Politics, and similar sources; subscribers to the or studentsat the NationalWar College. journal International Security; A somewhatnarrowerarea of disagreementrevolvesaround the abilityto use data from national probabilitysamples for studyingelites. To oversimplify somewhat,the contendingviewscan be reduced to twopositionson the adequacy of respondents'education levels as a surrogatemeasure of leadershipstatus.The affirmative viewwas presentedin a prognosisof how public opinion researchis likelyto develop duringthe nexthalfcentury. The mathematicsand economics of surveysmake them most cost-effective for assessinglarge undifferentiated populations,i.e. mass publics. And untilyou get to a handful of individualsat the verytop, you don't learn verymuch from studying"elites"-theyseem to be just like bettereducated people in the general population. These two generalizationssuggestno major shiftin our attention. The same is truein totalitariancountries(and, I suspect,poor countries). (Davis, 1987:S178-S179) Although the question is far fromsettled,at this point the proponentsof the opposite viewpointwould appear to have at least a plausible case. A number of studieshave found that,by itself,the level of educationalattainmentis an inadeforidentifying quate yardstick opinionleaders.Extensiveanalysesof the 1968, 1980, and 1984 National ElectionStudiesand the firstthreeChicago Council surveysby indicatorof Krosnickand Carnot (1988) indicatethateducation is an insufficient the attentiveforeignpolicypublic,much less of foreignpolicyopinion leadership. Their findingsreveal little support for the hypothesisthat the "foreignpolicy attentivepublic"is composed simplyof highlyeducated personswho are concerned of withall aspectsof publicpolicy.An earlierstudyalso castdoubton the sufficiency educationas a measureofleadership(Rogers,Stuhler,and Koenig,1967). Several studies have compared the viewsof opinion leaders and the general public (Luttbeg, 1968; Rielly,1975, 1979, 1983, 1987, 1991; Holsti, 1987, 1991; Chitticket al., 1990; Wittkopf,1990; Schneider, 1992). Much of the evidence indicatesgreatersimilaritiesthan differencesin the waysthatrespondentsin the two groups structure their political beliefs (Luttbeg, 1968; Wittkopf,1990), but Chittickand his colleagues (1990) found some differenceswhen foreignpolicy beliefstructuresare tracedover time.On the otherhand, thereexistsome rather of theirpolicy opinions. A few examples substantialdifferencesin the substance fromthe mostrecent (1990) Chicago Council surveywillbe sufficient to illustrate OLE R. HOLSTI 457 some of the more persistentgaps betweenopinion leaders and the general public (Rielly,1991). Leaders overwhelmingly (97%) supported an active U.S. role in worldaffairs, whereasthe general public was more tepid in itsenthusiasm(59%). Both groupshave shownlittlevariationin thisrespectover a period of nearlytwo decades. Almostthreefourthsof the leaders agreed thatthe Cold War has ended, but only about a third of the general public expressed that view. Whereas 90 percentof the leaders favoredforeigneconomic aid, especiallyto EasternEurope, onlyhalfthatproportionof the generalpublic did so; moreover,among the latter, fightingdrugsin Latin Americawas a favoritetargetof assistanceprograms.When asked to identifythe mostimportantforeignpolicyproblems,the top threeitems forleaders included issueswithan overwhelmingly internationalcharacter:spread of nuclear weapons,armscontrol,and improvementof the internationalenvironment.The comparablelistforthe generalpublic included issueswitha significant domestic dimension: protectionof Americanjobs, protectionof U.S. business interestsabroad, and adequate energysupplies. The momentouschanges in the formerSovietUnion and elsewherethathave characterizedthe period since 1988 suggestthe possibility-butcertainlynot the inevitability-ofa "great debate" on the appropriate values, goals, roles, and strategiesthat the United Statesshould pursue in the post-Cold War era. If one assumes that top officialsand opinion leaders will have overwhelminginfluence, whereasthe generalpublic willhave littleor none, then the evidencesuggeststhat the United Stateswillcontinueto pursuean internationalist foreignpolicy,broadly defined. In that case, the debates are likelyto center on how the United States participates(hard-linersvs. accommodationists).If, on the other hand, public preferencesplay a significantrole in shaping at least the broad contours of the Americanforeignpolicy,then the policydebates are likelyto focuson whether United Statesshould playan activeinternationalrole or focusmore on issuesthat vs.isolationists). have a directdomesticimpact(internationalists TheSourcesofForeign PolicyOpinions During the early post-WorldWar II years,bipartisancooperation between the White House and Congress made possible such initiativesas aid to Greece and Turkey,the Marshall Plan, and NATO. Each of these strikingdeparturesfrom traditionalforeignpolicieshad rathersolid public support;forfurtherevidenceon these and other undertakings,see Levering (1978), Foster (1983), and Wittkopf (1990:166-193). Agreementamong prominentleaders of the twomajor partiesno doubt contributedto the fact that,among the general public, Democrats and Republicansdifferedlittlewithrespectto these and other internationalist foreign A 1946 Gallup Poll revealedthat72 percentof respondentsin policyundertakings. both politicalpartiesfavoredan "active"internationalrole for the United States, and the 1947 programof aid to Greece and Turkeyreceived approval from56 percent of both Democrats and Republicans. Nor were there notable partisan in oppositionto sendingU.S. troopsto Indo-Chinain 1953 and 1954. differences reinforcedby even deeper Since the VietnamWar, sharp partisandifferences, ideological cleavages,have characterizedthe foreignpolicybeliefsof both opinion leaders and the general public (Wittkopf, 1990; Holsti and Rosenau, 1984, 1990). These differenceshave persisted into the 1990s, although President Reagan's second-termturnaroundon such issues as arms control and conciliationof the SovietUnion did tend to reduce Republican and conservativeoppositionto such policies,therebyclosingsomewhatthe partisanand ideologicalgaps (Holsti,1991). The ideological-partisan cleavageson domesticissuestend to reinforcethoseon not the case duringthe period betweenthe end foreignpolicy.This was distinctly of World War II and escalation of the conflictin Vietnam.In theirstudyof the 458 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy 1948 election, Berelson and his colleagues (1954) found a limited correlation between domesticeconomic issues and either civilrightsor internationalissues. 'To know,forexample,thatsomeone supportedthe New Deal on economic issues provided no indication of his internationalor civil rightsposition" (Berelson, Lazarsfeld,and McPhee, 1954:197-198). For furtherevidence of the lack of correlationbetweendomesticand foreignpolicyissues duringthe decade and a halfafterWorldWarII, see Campbellet al. (1964:113) and Key (1961:158). Duringthe past twodecades the linesof cleavageon domesticand foreignpolicy issues have increasinglycome to overlap (Russettand Hanson, 1975; Wittkopf, 1990). The evidence is especiallystrongat the level of opinion leadership (Holsti and Rosenau, 1988). Consequently,the putative,moderatinginfluenceof cross-cuttingcleavageshas oftenbeen absentin the stridentpoliticsof the 1970sand 1980s. Two other "gaps" have received considerableattentionfromopinion analysts. The VietnamWar era spawned interestin generationaltheoriesof politics.The fromKarl Mannheimto Sam Huntington, writingof distinguishedsocial scientists, seemed to offercogent hypothesesto explain the divisionsof Americansociety duringthe Vietnamtrauma,pitting,accordingto some, the "Munichgeneration," which had witnessedthe bitterconsequences of effortsto appease expansionist dictators,againstthe 'Vietnamgeneration,"whichargued thatitwas experiencing effortto apply the "lessonsof Munich" to the poisoned legacyof an ill-informed the jungles of SoutheastAsia (Allison,1970-71; Roskin,1974). This explanation has encountered mixed success when confrontedwith hard evidence. Wittkopf (1990) found evidence of generationaldifferencesamong the general public. However,at the level of opinion leaders the primarylines of cleavageappear to lie withinratherthan between generations(Holsti and Rosenau, 1980, 1990). The generationalhypothesisappears to farebest when applied to specificgroups such as politicalactivists(Converse,1987) or protesters(Jennings,1987). The term "gender gap" has also become a prominentpart of our political vocabulary.There is some evidence of a systematicgender gap. For example, Converse (1987:61) found substantialgender differencesand that "'Rambo' from males." According to Baxter and themes come very disproportionately Lansing (1983), war/peaceconcernsare an exceptionto the generalrule thatmen and women agree on most issues. Wittkopf(1990) also found gender-based and cooperativeinternationdifferencesalong both the militantinternationalist alist dimensions, and another study identified gender as among the most importantdemographicpredictorsof foreignpolicy attitudes(Fite, Genest,and Wilcox, 1990). A broad examination of surveydata led Shapiro and Mahajan (1986) to the conclusion that systematicgender differencesalso emerge from "compassion" issues and those involvingregulation and protection.However, Conover (1988) argues that "feminism,"an ideological predispositiontowarda certainstance on women's issues,overshadowsgender in explainingbeliefsand values.A numberof recentAmericansTalk Securitysurveysalso cast some doubt Women consistently on stereotypical viewsof gender differences. expressedmore skepticalopinions about the USSR than did men. Althoughwomen were more inclined to describethemselvesas doves (ATS-9:119), men offeredmore support on all fivearmscontrolitemsby an averagemarginof 6 percent;expressedmore trustin the USSR on six of seven items (6 percent); assessed Gorbachev more on eightitems(9 percent); had more benignviewsof Sovietmotiveson favorably six of nine items (3 percent); assessed relationsbetween the superpowersmore favorablyon ten of thirteenitems (5 percent); and expressed a more optimistic viewon the likelihoodof nuclear war (10 percent). In the onlyexception to this pattern,womenweremore criticalon threeitemsrelatingto the impactof defense spending(6 percent). OLE R. HOLSTI 459 At the level of opinion leaders, evidence of systematicgender differencesis harderto come by,excepton such issuesas tradeand the environment; womenare more protectionistand pro-environment. Four FPLP surveysspanning a dozen years indicate that women in leadership positions hold attitudes,including on war/peace issues,thatare generallyquite similarto those of men in comparable roles. Moreover,in theirpoliticalattitudes,liberal women are likelyto resemble liberalmen,Republicanwomentend to resembleRepublicanmen,and so on. Surveyresearchershave also produced evidence on other backgroundfactors, includingoccupation,education,military service,travelabroad, region,and so on, but the mutuallyreinforcingpartisanand ideological cleavageshave clearlybeen dominant in recent years. The public opinion data generally support the conclusion of three perceptive observers: "For two decades, the making of Americanforeignpolicyhas been growingfarmore political-or more precisely, farmore partisanand ideological" (Destler,Gelb, and Lake, 1984:13). Whetherthe end of the Cold War will contributeto softeningand blurringthe partisanideologicalchasmremainsto be demonstrated. Conclusion The consensus of the mid-1960son the nature,structure,and impact of public opinion has clearly come under vigorous challenge during the past quarter century.The VietnamWar,whilenot the sole causal factorin the reexaminationof the conventionalwisdom,was certainlya catalyst.If a new consensus has yet to emerge on all of the issues discussedabove, at least it seems safe to statethatthe fieldis markedbyinnovativeresearchand activedebateson the implicationsof the results.Nevertheless,there are at least fourareas thatseem to call foradditional effort:case studiesemployingarchivalsources thatwill providethe most directly relevant evidence on the impact of public opinion, cross-nationalresearch, developmentof standardquestions in order to encourage cumulationof survey results,and researchthatwill enable us to distinguishresultsthat are time-and context-bound fromthosethattranscendthe Cold War period. As indicatedearlier,byfarthe leastwell developed of the areas discussedin this essayhas been the opinion-policylink.We have impressivecorrelationalevidence that policy changes are in fact predominantlyin the directionfavoredby the public,but our confidencewould be enhanced by more substantialevidence of a causal nature.The typeof researchdesign employedin the previouslycited study by Thomas Graham (1989), combining analyses of surveydata with archival researchin orderto assessthe causal impactof public opinion on decisionmaking, is a good model forfuturestudies.More researchof thistypeshould go a long way towardansweringsome of the mostimportantquestionsabout the opinion-policy relationship.It should also providefurtherinsightinto otherimportantquestions: What are the relevantindicatorsof public opinion? How much do polls matter? What about expressionsof congressionalpreferences?What about the impact of the media or interest groups? How and why do policy makers (and administrations)differ in their sensitivityto differentindicators of public sentiments? It willno doubt have occurredto readersby now thatthisessayis almostwholly confined to evidence of American public opinion. But questions about public opinion and foreignpolicyare obviouslyof much broaderconcern,especiallyin an era of expandingdemocracy,and a good deal of futureeffortshould be directed towardcomparativeanalysis.Some examples of innovativecomparativeresearch include Eichenberg (1989) and Risse-Kappen (1991). The latter found that 460 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy althoughpublic opinion was importantin each of fourcountries-France,Japan, affectedby West Germany,and the United States-its impact was significantly domesticstructuresand coalition-building processesamong elites. Distinctionsof thiskindclearlytakeus a long waytowarda fullerunderstandingof opinion-policy linkages.Dramaticchangesin EasternEurope and the formerSovietUnion should open up possibilitiesfora range of comparativestudiesthatwould have been quite impossiblejust a fewyearsago. Mueller (1973:265) has appropriatelypointed to an importantlimitationon opinion surveysarisingfromthe factthat "the poll interviewis a ratherprimitive social situationin whichpoorlythoughtout answersare casually stimulus-response fittedto questionsthatare overlyingenuous."At minimum,thatwarningreminds us to be cautiousabout makinginferencesfroma singledatum on public opinion. It is permissibleto be moreventuresomein drawingtrendsfromlongitudinaldata, but doing so assumes thatthe questionsposed have remained constant.Whereas commercialpolling organizationsask certainstandardquestions at quite regular intervals-for example, on rating presidentialperformance-there is nothing comparable with a strictlyforeignpolicy focus. During and immediatelyafter WorldWar II, respondentswere regularlyasked about the desirability of an active U.S. role in the world,but interestin that question appears to have died in the mid-1950s;it was only revivedin the wake of the war in Vietnam when global activismcame under increasingcriticism.In othercases, longitudinalanalysesmay be rendered suspect,as in 1956 when Gallup made a "minor"alterationto its standard question on foreignaid by adding the phrase, "to preventtheir [the recipientsof aid] going communistic"at the end of the question. Among the usefulfeaturesof the Chicago Council quadrennial studies (Rielly, 1975-1991) has been a carryover of certainquestionsfromsurveyto survey.Their question askingrespondentsto rate the importanceof a series of foreignpolicy goals has been especiallyusefulforthosewithan interestin trendanalyses. With a few exceptions, however,the independent surveysthat have been undertakenin recentyearsappear to have taken littlenote of questionsin other studiesthatmightprovide the basis for comparativeanalysis.In one sense thisis understandable;the whole rationale for an independent surveyis to undertake probes that have been overlookedby others.But it is also regrettablethat there appears to be ratherlimitedcommunicationat the planningstage betweenthose The developmentof even a handfulof standardforeign who are designingsurveys. policyquestions thatwould be included in all such surveyswould go a long way recordof cumulativefindings. towardimprovinga less-than-outstanding Finally,so much of the evidence cited above has emerged froma four-decadelong period in whichforeignaffairsweredominatedbythe Cold War thatwe need to address questions about whetherand how the end of that confrontationmay affector even render obsolete what we have learned about public opinion and foreignpolicy.At the most obvious level, there has been a sea change in public all of the issuesthatdominatedthe Cold War era. Indeed, attitudestowardvirtually one could make a plausible case thatin manyrespectschangingpublic attitudes precededratherthan followedthose at the pinnacles of governmenton such issues as the appropriatelevel of defense spending, the primarythreatsto American national security,assessmentsof Mikhail Gorbachev'sgoals, and the motivations underlyingSoviet foreignpolicy (ATS surveys;Holsti, 1991). For example, well before the demolitionof the Berlin Wall or the final collapse of the USSR, the public ranked the danger to Americannationalsecurityfromthe SovietUnion in seventhplace,just behind thegreenhouseeffect(ATS-9:51-54). The end of the Cold War also raises some questions about the structuring of foreignpolicy attitudes.Substantialevidence indicates that assessmentsof the SovietUnion have playeda keyrole in foreignpolicybeliefstructures; forexample, OLE R. HOLSTI 461 it is a centralelementin the hierarchicalmodel developed byHurwitzand Peffley (1990) as well as in Wittkopf's(1986, 1990) militantinternationalism dimension. researchquestionsemergefromthe collapse of the SovietUnion. Some interesting Will it resultformanyin a loss of structureand consequent disorientationabout In a replacementof the Sovietsbyanotheradversarysuch as Japan? foreignaffairs? Are there persons who, if deprived of one enemy, must search for another? Alternatively, are the key concepts that structurebeliefs about foreign affairs sufficiently genericthattheywillsurvivethe dramaticinternationalchanges of the past fewyears?It is my guess that such dimensionsas militantinternationalism, and unilateralism-multilateralism cooperative internationalism, will continue to structureforeignpolicyattitudes,but the changeswe have witnessedsince the late 1980s are of such unprecedented magnitude that this must be treated as a hypothesisthatrequiressystematic testing. At the broadest level, if we are indeed enteringinto a period of fewermajor and greaterattentionto such nonmilitaryissues as trade, power confrontations, immigration, the environment, and the like-there is ample surveydata thatmuch of the Americanpublic believesthisto be the case-it mayalso be an era in which public opinion playsa more autonomousrole. Even thosewho do not fullyaccept the "manipulatedpublic" thesiswould acknowledgethatcrisesand confrontations abroad providea settingin whichopportunitiesand temptationsformanipulating the public are far greater than on nonstrategicissues. Not only are the latter resolvedover a longer time period-providing greateropportunitiesfor typically the public, pressuregroups,the media, and Congressto play a significantrolebut they also tend to be more resistantto claims that the needs for secrecy, flexibility, and speed of action make it both necessaryand legitimatefor the executiveto have a relatively freehand. In short,we may be movinginto a period in which the relationshipbetween public opinion and foreign policy takes on added rather than diminished but we should also be waryof assumptionsthatthe theories,evidence, significance, and linkages emergingfromresearch during the Cold War era will necessarily travelintactintoan era of strikingly different circumstances. References P., J. H. ALDRICH, AND J. RHODE (1990) Change and Continuity in the 1988 Election. Washington,DC: CongressionalQuarterly. AcHEN, C. H. (1975) Mass PoliticalAttitudesand the SurveyResponse. American PoliticalScienceReview 69:1218-1231. ALDRICH,J. H., J. L. SULLIVAN, AND E. BORGIDA (1989) ForeignAffairs and Issue Voting:Do Presidential PoliticalScienceReview83:123-141. Candidates "Waltzbeforea Blind Audience?"American ALLISON, G. T. (1970-71) Cool It: The ForeignPolicyofYoungAmericans.Foreign Policy1:144-160. ALMOND, G. (1950) 7he American Peopleand Foreign Policy.New York:Praeger.Reprintedin 1960 witha new introduction. ALMOND, G. (1956) Public Opinion and NationalSecurity.PublicOpinionQuarterly 20:371-378. AMERICANS TALK ISSUES (1991) Serial National Surveys of Americans on Public Policy Issues. Washington,DC: AmericansTalk Issues Foundation. AMERICANS TALK SECURITY (1987-91) A Series of FifteenSurveysConcerningNational SecurityIssues. Winchester,MA: AmericansTalk Security. BAILEY, T. A. (1948) 7'heMan in theStreet: 7he ImpactofAmerican PublicOpinionon ForeignPolicy.New York:Macmillan. BARDES, B. A., AND R. OLDENDICK (1978) Beyond Internationalism: The Case forMultipleDimensions in ForeignPolicyAttitudes.SocialScienceQuarterly 59:496-508. BARTELS, L. M. (1991) ConstituencyOpinion and CongressionalPolicyMaking:The Reagan Defense Buildup. American PoliticalScienceReviezv 85:457-474. ABRAMSON, 462 PublicOpinionand ForeignPolicy BARTON,A. H. (1974-75) Conflictand Consensus among AmericanLeaders. PublicOpinionQuarterly 38:507-530. BARTON,A. H. (1980) Fault Lines in AmericanElite Consensus.Daedalus 109:1-24. BAUER, R. A., I. DES. POOL, AND L. A. DEXTER (1963) American Businessand PublicPolicy.New York: AthertonPress. BAXTER,S., AND M. LANSING (1983) Womenand PoliticsThe VisibleMajority. Ann Arbor: Universityof MichiganPress. BEAL,R. S., AND R. H. HINCKLEY (1984) PresidentialDecision Making and Opinion Polls. Annals 472:72-84. BENNETr, S. E. (1972) Attitude Structuresand Foreign Policy Opinions. Social ScienceQuarterly 55:732-742. BENNETT,S. E. (1992) The Persian Gulf War's Impact on Americans' Political Information. Washington,DC: NES Conferenceon "The PoliticalConsequences ofWar." BERELSON,B., P. F. LAZARSFELD, ANDW. N. MCPHEE (1954) Voting. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. BILLINGTON,J.(1987) Realismand Visionin ForeignPolicy.Foreign Affairs 65:630-652. BRODY,R., AND C. SHAPIRO(1989) "A Reconsiderationof the RallyPhenomenon in Public Opinion." In PoliticalBehavior Annual,edited byS. Long. Boulder,CO: WestviewPress. AND J. G. BLIGHT [EDITOR] (1987) "October 27, 1962: Transcriptof the BUNDY, M. [TRANSCRIBER], Meetings of the ExComm." InternationalSecurity12:30-92. A., P. E. CONVERSE, W. E. MILLER, AND D. E. STOKES (1964) TheAmericanVoter. New York: Wiley. CANTRIL,H. (1967) The Human Dimension:Experiences in PolicyResearch.New Brunswick,NJ: Rutgers CAMPBELL, University Press. CASPARY,W. R. (1970) The Mood Theory:A Studyof Public Opinion and Foreign Policy.Ame7ican PoliticalScienceReview64:536-547. CHITTICK, W., AND K. R. BILLINGSLEY(1989) The Structureof Elite Foreign Policy Beliefs. Western PoliticalQuarterly 42:201-224. W., K R. BILLINGSLEY, AND R. TRAvIS (1990) Persistenceand Change in Elite and Mass AttitudestowardU.S. ForeignPolicy.PoliticalPsychology 11:385-402. CLIFFORD,C., WITHR. HOLBROOK (1991) CounseltothePresident: A Memoir. New York:Random House. COHEN, B. C. (1973) ThePublic'sImpactonForeign Policy.Boston:Little,Brown. CONOVER,P.J. (1988) Feministsand the Gender Gap. JournalofPolitics 50:985-1010. CONOVER,P. J.,AND S. FELDMAN(1984) How People Organize the PoliticalWorld:A SchematicModel. JournalofPoliticalScience28:95-126. Amenican edited CONVERSE,P. E. (1964) "The Natureof BeliefSystemsin Mass Publics."In Ideology and Discontent, byD. E. Apter.New York:Free Press. CONVERSE,P. E. (1987) "The EnduringImpact of the VietnamWar on AmericanPublic Opinion." In theStorm: American a Decadeafter theVietnam War.Taipei, R.O.C.: Academic Sinica. Society After DAvIS,J. A. (1987) The Future Study of Public Opinion: A Symposium.Public Opinion Quarterly 51:S178-S179. DESTLER, I. M., L. H. GELB, ANDA. LAKE (1984) Our OwnWorst New York:Simon and Schuster. Enemy. DEVINE,D. J. (1970) TheAttentive Public.Chicago: Rand McNally. in Western EICHENBERG,R. (1989) Public Opinionand National Security Europe.Ithaca, NY: Cornell Press. University FAGEN,R. (1960) Some Assessmentsand Uses of Public Opinion in Diplomacy.PublicOpinionQuarterly 24:448-457. in American Press. NationalElections. New Haven,CT: Yale University FIORINA,M. (1981) Retrospective Voting FITE, D., M. GENEST,AND C. WILCOX (1990) Gender Differencesin Foreign PolicyAttitudes.American PoliticsQuarterly 18:492-513. FOSTER,H. S. (1983) Activism ReplacesInternationalism. Washington,DC: Foxhall Press. Comes ofAge ... Again. PublicOpinion3 (April-May): FREE, L., ANDW. WATTS (1980) Internationalism 46-50. GENCO, S. J. (1984) The AttentivePublic and American Foreign Policy.Ph.D. dissertation,Stanford CHITTICK, University,Stanford, CA. StatePower.New York:Basic Books. GINSBURG,B. (1988) TheCaptivePublic:How Mass OpinionPromotes and ForeignPolicy:Four Case StudiesfromtheFormative GRABER,D. A. (1968) PublicOpinion,thePresident, Years.New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. GRAEBNER,N. A. (1983) "PublicOpinion and Foreign Policy:A PragmaticView."In Interaction: Foreign Policyand PublicPolicy,edited by D. C. Piper and R. J. Terchek. Washington,DC: American Enterprise Institute. OLE R. HOLSTI 463 GRAHAM, T. W. (1986) PublicAttitudes towardActiveDefense: ABM and StarWars,1945-1985. Cambridge, MA: CenterforInternationalStudies,M.I.T. GRAHAM, T. W. (1988) The Patternand Importanceof Public Knowledgein the Nuclear Age.Journalof Conflict Resolution 32:319-334. GRAHAM, T. W. (1989) The Politicsof Failure: StrategicNuclear Arms Control,Public Opinion, and DomesticPoliticsin the United States-1945-1980. Ph.D. dissertation, M.I.T., Cambridge,MA. HERO,A. O., JR. (1969) Liberalism-Conservatism Revisited:Foreign vs. Domestic Federal Policies, 1937-1967. PublicOpinionQuarterly 33:399-408. R. C. (1981) Powerand thePeople:Executive HILDERBRAND, Management ofPublicOpinionin Foreign Affairs, 1897-1921. Chapel Hill: University of NorthCarolina Press. HILL, K. A. (1992) The Domestic Sources of Foreign Policymaking:American Mass Attitudesand CongressionalVotingon South AfricanSanctions.Paper presentedat the annual meetingof the InternationalStudiesAssociation,Atlanta,GA. HINCKLEY, R. H. (1988) Public AttitudestowardKey Foreign PolicyEvents.JournalofConflict Resolution 32:295-318. HINCKLEY, R. H. (1992) People,Polls,and Policy-Makers. New York:LexingtonBooks. HOLMES, J. E. (1985) TheMood/Interest Theory ofAmerican ForeignPolicy.Lexington:University Press of Kentucky. HOLSTI,0. R. (1979) The Three-Headed Eagle: The United States and SystemChange. International StudiesQuarterly 23:339-359. HOLSTI,0. R. (1987) "PublicOpinion and Containment."In Containing theSovietUnion,edited byT. L. Deibel andJ. L. Gaddis. Washington,DC: Pergamon-Brassey's. HOLSTI,0. R. (1991) "AmericanReactions to the USSR: Public Opinion." In Soviet-American Relations after theColdWar,edited byR.Jervisand S. Bialer.Durham,NC: Duke University Press. HOLSTI,0. R., AND J. N. ROSENAU (1979) Vietnam,Consensus, and the Belief Systemsof American Leaders. WorldPolitics32:1-56. HOLSTI,0. R., AND J.N. ROSENAU (1980) Does Where You Stand Depend on When You Were Born? The Impact of Generation on Post-VietnamForeign Policy Beliefs. Public Opinion Quarterly 44:1-22. HOLSTI, 0. R., AND J. N. ROSENAU (1984) American in WorldAffairs: Leadership Vietnam and theBreakdown ofConsensus. London: Allen and Unwin. HOLSTI, 0. R., AND J. N. ROSENAU (1988) The Domestic and Foreign Policy Beliefs of American Leaders.JournalofConflict Resolution 32:248-294. HOLSTI, 0. R., AND J. N. ROSENAU (1990) The Structureof Foreign PolicyAttitudesamong American Leaders.JournalofPolitics 52:94-125. HUGHES, B. B. (1978) TheDomestic Context ofAmerican Foreign Policy.San Francisco:Freeman. HUGHES, T. L. (1980) The Crack-Up.Foreign Policy40:33-60. HULL, C. (1936) Speech at Pan AmericanConference,Buenos Aires,December 5, 1936. Text in New YorkTimes[Dec. 6, 1936], p. 47. HURWITZ, J., AND M. PEFFLEY (1987) How Are Foreign Policy AttitudesStructured?A Hierarchical Model. American PoliticalScienceReview81:1099-1120. HURwITz,J., AND M. PEFFLEY (1990) Public Images of the SovietUnion.Journal ofPolitics52:3-28. JENNINGS, M. K. (1987) Residue of a Movement:The Aging of the American ProtestGeneration. American PoliticalScienceReview81:367-382. JENTLESON, B. W. (1992) The Pretty PrudentPublic: Post-PostVietnamAmericanOpinion on the Use of MilitaryForce. International StudiesQuarterly 36:49-73. KENNAN, G. F. (1951) American of Chicago Press. Diplomacy, 1900-1950. Chicago: University KEY,V. O., JR.(1961) PublicOpinionand American New York:Knopf. Democracy. KINDER, D. R. (1983) "Diversity and Complexityin AmericanPublic Opinion." In PoliticalScience:The State of theDiscipline,edited by A. W. Finifter.Washington,DC: American Political Science Association. KINDER, D. R., AND D. 0. SEARs (1985) "Public Opinion and PoliticalAction." In HandbookofSocial Psychology, edited byE. Aronsonand G. Lindzey.New York:Random House. KLINGBERG, F. L. (1952) The HistoricalAlternationof Moods in AmericanForeignPolicy.World Politics 4:239-273. KLINGBERG, F. L. (1979) "Cyclical Trends in American Foreign Policy Moods and Their Policy Implications."In Challengesto America:UnitedStatesForeignPolicyin the1980s, edited by C. W. Kegley,Jr.,and P.J. McGowan.BeverlyHills,CA: Sage. KLINGBERG, F. L. (1983) CyclicalTrendsin American ForeignPolicyMoods:The Unfolding ofAmerica'sWorld Role.Lanham, MD: University PressofAmerica. 464 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy KOOPMAN, C.,J. SNYDER, AND R.JERvIs(1990a) AmericanEliteViewsof Relationswiththe SovietUnion. Journal ofSocialIssues45:119-138. KOOPMAN, C., WITH J. SNYDER AND R. JERVIS (1990b) Theory-Drivenvs. Data-DrivenAssessmentsin a Crisis. Journal ofConflict Resolution 34:694-722. KOOPMAN, C., J. SNYDER, AND R.JERVIS (1991) U.S. Elite Viewsabout Change and Changes in Viewson Soviet-U.S. Relations. Paper prepared for the annual meeting of the InternationalSocietyof PoliticalPsychology, Helsinki. KREISBERG, M. (1949) "DarkAreasof Ignorance." In PublicOpinionand Foreign editedby Policy, L. Markel.New York:Harper. KROSNICK, J. A. (1988a) AttitudeImportanceand AttitudeChange.JournalofExperimental Social Psychology 24:240-255. KROSNICK, J. A. (1988b) The Role of AttitudeImportance in Social Evaluation: A Study of Policy Preferences,PresidentialCandidate Evaluations,and Voting Behavior.JournalofPersonality and SocialPsychology 55:196-210. J. A. (1990) Americans' Perceptionsof PresidentialCandidates: A Test of the Projection Hypothesis.JournalofSocialIssues46:159-182. KROSNICK, J. A. (1991) The Stabilityof Political Preferences: Comparisons of Symbolic and NonsymbolicAttitudes.American JournalofPoliticalScience35:547-576. the ForeignAffairsAttentivePublic in the U.S.: A KROSNICK, J. A., AND C. CARNOT (1988) Identifying Comparisonof CompetingTheories. Mimeographed.Ohio StateUniversity, Columbus,OH. KROSNICK, andForeign ChinaPolicy. CT: Greenwood KUSNITZ,L. A. (1984) Public Opinion Policy: America's Westport, Press. W. (1977) "AmericanPolicy-Makers, Public Opinion and the Outbreak of the Cold War, 1945-1950." In The Originsof theCold Warin Asia, edited by Y. Nagai and A. Iriye.New York: Columbia University Press. Consent: PublicOpinionand American CT: LEIGH, M. (1976) Mobilizing Foreign Policy.Westport, GreenwoodPress. LAFEBER, andAmerican NewYork:Morrow. LEVERING,R. B. (1978) ThePublic Foreign Policy, 1918-1978. J. (1990) Parochialism,Policy, and ConstituencyConstraints:Congressional Voting on JournalofPoliticalScience34:936-960. StrategicWeapons Systems.American LINDSAY, J. (1991) Testing the Parochial Hypothesis:Congress and the StrategicDefense Initiative. LINDSAY, 53:860-876. Journal ofPolitics New York:Macmillan. LIPPMANN, W. (1922) Public Opinion. W. (1925) ThePhantom Public. LIPPMANN, New York:Harcourt,Brace. inthePublic LIPPMANN, W. (1955)Essays Philosophy. Boston:Little,Brown. LIPPMANN, W.,AND C. METZ(1920) A Test of the News.Special Supplement.NewRepublic 23:1-42. LIPSET,S. M. (1966) The President,the Polls,and Vietnam.Transaction (September/October):19-24. LUTTBEG, N. R. (1968) The Structureof Beliefsamong Leaders and the Public. PublicOpinionQuarterly 32:398-409. In EagleEntangled: U.S. MANDELBAUM, M., AND W. SCHNEIDER (1979) "The New Internationalisms." edited byK. A. Oye etal. New York:Longman. Foreign Policyin a Complex World, PublicOpinion.PacificGrove,CA: MARGOLIS, M., AND G. A. MAUSER, EDS. (1989) Manipulating Brooks/Cole. MARRA, R. A., C. W. OSTROM, AND D. M. SIMON (1990) Foreign Policy and PresidentialPopularity. Resolution 34:588-623. Journal ofConflict in Congress. Political Science Influence American W. E., AND D. E. STOKES (1963) Constituency MILLER, Review57:45-46. A. D. (1979) ConsistencybetweenPublic Preferencesand National PolicyDecision. American MONROE, Politics 7:3-19. Quarterly 5thed.,rev.NewYork:Knopf. H.J.(1978)Politics among Nations, Presidents andPublicOpinion. NewYork:Wiley. MUELLER,J.E. (1973) War, and Opinionin theAmerican Electorate. NEUMAN,W. R. (1986) TheParadoxofMassPolitics: Knowledge MORGENTHAU, NIE, NIE, Press. Cambridge,MA: HarvardUniversity N., AND K. ANDERSON (1974) Mass Belief SystemsRevisited:Political Change and Attitude Structure. 36:540-591. Journal ofPolitics MA:Harvard Voter. American N., S. VERBA, AND J. R. PETROCIK (1976) TheChanging Cambridge, Press. University OLE R. HOLSTI 465 NINCIC,M. (1988) The United States,the Soviet Union, and the Politicsof Opposites. WorldPolitics 40:452-475. PAGE,B. I., AND R. Y. SHAPIRO (1982) Changes in Americans' Policy Preferences,1935-1979. Public OpinionQuarterly 46:24-42. PAGE,B. I., AND R. Y. S-APIRO (1983) Effectsof Public Opinion on Policy. AmericanPoliticalScience Review77:175-190. PAGE,B. I., AND R. Y. SHAPIRO (1984) Presidentsas Opinion Leaders: Some New Evidence. PolicyStudies Journal12:649-661. PAGE,B. I., AND R. Y SHAPIRO(1988) Foreign Policy and the Rational Public. Journalof Conflict Resolution 32:211-247. Policy PAGE, B. I., AND R. Y. SHAPIRO (1992) The RationalPublic:FiftyYearsof Trendsin Americans' of Chicago Press. Preferences. Chicago: University ANDG. R. DEMPSEY (1987) What Moves Public Opinion? AmericanPolitical PAGE, B. I., R. Y. SHAPIRO, ScienceReview81:23-43. Policy72:3-30. PARRY, R., AND P. KORNBLUTH (1988) Iran-Contra'sUntold Story.Foreign T. G. (1979) PresidentialForeignPolicy,Public Opinion, and Congress:The TrumanYears. PATERSON, Diplomatic History 3:1-18. of PEFFLEY, M. A., ANDJ.HURWITZ (1985) A HierarchicalModel of AttitudeConstraint.AmericanJournal PoliticalScience29:871-890. PEFFLEY, M., ANDJ. HURWITZ (1922a) InternationalEventsand ForeignPolicyBeliefs:Public Responses JournalofPoliticalScience36:431-461. to ChangingSoviet-AmericanRelations.American PoliticalBehavior. PEFFLEY, M., ANDJ. HURWITZ (1922b) Models ofAttitudeConstraintin ForeignAffairs. POWLICK, P. J. (1991) The AttitudinalBases for Responsivenessto Public Opinion among Foreign Resolution 35:611-641. PolicyOfficials. JournalofConflict P. J. (forthcoming)"Public Opinion and the Lebanon Intervention."In Mass Communication, POWLICK, Westport,CT: Greenwood. Democratization, and thePoliticalProcess. PublicOpinionand U.S. ForeignPolicy1975. Chicago: Chicago Council RIELLY, J. E., ED. (1975) American on ForeignRelations. PublicOpinionand U.S. ForeignPolicy1979. Chicago: Chicago Council RIELLY,J. E., ED. (1979) American on ForeignRelations. PublicOpinionand U.S. ForeignPolicy1983. Chicago: Chicago Council RIELLY,J. E., ED. (1983) American on ForeignRelations. PublicOpinionand U.S. ForeignPolicy1987. Chicago: Chicago Council RIELLY, J. E., ED. (1987) American on ForeignRelations. RIELLY, J. E., ED. (1991) American PublicOpinionand U.S. ForeignPolicy1991. Chicago: Chicago Council on ForeignRelations. RiSSE-KAPPEN,T. (1991) Public Opinion, Domestic Structure, and Foreign Policy in Liberal Democracies. World Politics43:479-512. AND D. KOENIG(1967) A Comparison of Informedand General Public W. C., B. STUHLER, ROGERS, 31:242-252. Opinion on U.S. ForeignPolicy.PublicOpinionQuarterly 1:3-10. Affairs RoOT,E. (1922) A Requisiteforthe Success of Popular Diplomacy.Foreign N. (1961) PublicOpinionandForeign Policy.New York:Random House. RoSENAU,J. ROSKIN,M. (1974) From Pearl Harbor to Vietnam: ShiftingGenerational Paradigms and Foreign 89:563-588. Policy.PoliticalScienceQuarterly Press. B. (1990) Controlling theSword.Cambridge,MA: HarvardUniversity RUSSETT, and Ideology:The ForeignPolicyBeliefsof American RUSSETT, B. M., AND E. C. HANSON (1975) Interest Businessmen. San Francisco:Freeman. edited by SCHNEIDER, W. (1992) "The Old Politicsand the New World Order." In Eagle in a NewWorld, K. A. Oye, R.J. Lieber,and D. Rothchild.New York:HarperCollins. R. Y., AND H. MAHAJAN SHAPIRO, (1986) Gender Differencesin Policy Preferences:A Summaryof Trends fromthe 1960s to the 1980s. PublicOpinionQuarterly 50:42-61. SHAPIRO, R. Y, AND B. I. PAGE (1988) Foreign Policy and the Rational Public. Journalof Conflict Resolution 32:211-247. of Political Ideology and Public SNIDERMAN, P. M., AND P. E. TETLOCK (1986) "Inter-relationship edited byM. G. Hermann. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass. Opinion." In PoliticalPsychology, in El Salvadorand Nicaragua. Public SOBEL, R. (1989) Public Opinion about United StatesIntervention 53:114-128. OpinionQuarterly 466 Public Opinionand ForeignPolicy J. L., J. E. PIERESON, AND G. E. MARcus(1978) Ideological Constraintin the Mass Public: A MethodologicalCritiqueand Some New Findings.American JournalofPoliticalScience22:234-249. B. (1976) Elitesin America. SUSSMAN, Post. Washington,DC: Washington VERBA, S., AND R. A. BRODY (1970) Participation,PolicyPreferences,and the War in Vietnam.Public OpinionQuarterly 34:325-332. VERBA, S., R. A. BRODY, E. B. PARKER, N. H. NIE, N. W. POLSBY, P. EKMAN, AND G. BLACK (1967) Public PoliticalScienceReview61:317-333. Opinion and theWar in Vietnam.American WITTKOPF, E. R. (1986) On the Foreign PolicyBeliefsof the AmericanPeople: A Critique and Some Evidence. International StudiesQuarterly 30:425-445. WITTKOPF, E. R. (1990) Faces ofInternationalism: PublicOpinionand ForeignPolicy.Durham, NC: Duke University Press. D. (1979) Farewellto "PresidentKnowsBest."Foreign YANKELOVICH, 57:670-693. Affairs SULLIVAN,