ZDM Mathematics Education (2011) 43:637–650 DOI 10.1007/s11858-011-0368-8 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Children’s unit concepts in measurement: a teaching experiment spanning grades 2 through 5 Jeffrey E. Barrett • Craig Cullen • Julie Sarama Douglas H. Clements • David Klanderman • Amanda L. Miller • Chepina Rumsey • Accepted: 6 September 2011 / Published online: 27 September 2011 Ó FIZ Karlsruhe 2011 Abstract We examined ways of improving students’ unit concepts across spatial measurement situations. We report data from our teaching experiment during a six-semester longitudinal study from grade 2 through grade 5. Data include instructional task sequences designed to help children (a) integrate multiple representations of unit, (b) coordinate and group units into higher-order units, and (c) recognize the arbitrary nature of unit in comparison contexts and student’s responses to tasks. Our results suggest reflection on multiplicative relations among quantities prompted a more fully-developed unit concept. This research extends prior work addressing the growth of unit concepts in the contexts of length, area, and volume by demonstrating the viability of level-specific instructional actions as a means for promoting an informal theory of measurement. Keywords Measurement Length Area Volume Formative assessment Units Learning trajectory J. E. Barrett (&) C. Cullen A. L. Miller C. Rumsey Illinois State University, 4520 Math, 313 STV Hall, Normal, IL 61790-4520, USA e-mail: jbarrett@ilstu.edu J. Sarama D. H. Clements University at Buffalo, State University of New York, 505 Baldy Hall, Buffalo, NY 14260, USA D. Klanderman Trinity Christian College, 6601 West College Drive, Palos Heights, IL 60463, USA 1 Introduction We are interested in describing ways of helping elementary children establish rich conceptual knowledge of units of spatial measurement and use that knowledge as they measure in complex situations. We set out to design specific instructional environments for teaching children about spatial measurement and for promoting the development of flexible and adaptive strategies that depend on the use of mathematical structures (Verschaffel, Luwel, Torbeyns, & Van Dooren, 2009). We believe such environments for instruction will help students forge a conceptual, mathematical sense for measurement (Siegler, 2003). We share the goals of learning-sciences researchers working to describe ‘‘features of a learning environment intended to support the longer term development of learners’ engagement in disciplinary practices, including reasoning about evidence and explanation, representing and communicating information, evaluating knowledge claims and building and refining models and theories’’ (Ruthven, Laborde, Leach, & Tiberghien, 2009, p. 330). Our particular investigation describes features of a learning environment focused on unit concepts of spatial measurement including researcherdesigned interventions in the context of clinical interviews. Elementary students need a flexible, practical yet theoretical knowledge of quantity and space as a foundation for proportional reasoning, rational number knowledge, and adequate understanding of algebraic variables for later coursework in mathematics and science (Corcoran, Mosher, & Rogat, 2009). Providing opportunities to establish such knowledge requires thoughtful design of spatial measurement activities. We conjecture that an informal theory of unit measures would inform students’ concept of units of quantity across varying dimensions and tasks (Lehrer & Schauble, 2007). 123 638 Research on geometry and spatial measurement conducted by Piaget and his colleagues (1960) emphasized a sequence of logical operations that depended on a succession of restructuring operations with spatial dimensions, primarily length, then area, and also volume. More recent work in child development and classroom studies on spatial measurement have focused on conceptual foundations of measure related to unit concepts: unit attributes, unit iteration, unit structuring and tiling, proportionality among different units, additivity and the use of zero on measuring scales, constituting an informal theory of measure (Lehrer, 2003, pp. 181–182). Researchers have hypothesized that focusing on conceptual understanding of units is a productive way of anticipating and explaining changes in conceptual understanding about measurement (Clarke, Cheeseman, McDonough, & Clarke, 2003; Outhred, Mitchelmore, McPhail, & Gould, 2003). Yet these more recent studies have not addressed the relation between instruction on unit concepts and strategy development in complex task settings that engage various dimensions over long developmental timeframes, nor have they focused on comparison as a unifying operation relating measures and units. Comparative reasoning underlies every act of measurement; one must relate each object to some unit object and report its size in terms of those units. Presently, most of the curriculum for US elementary schools presupposes that spatial measurement of length, area, and volume should be taught in an isolated sequence instead of integrating these dimensions by focusing centrally on unit operations (Smith, et al., 2008). Few studies have examined the possible concurrence of unit concept development across dimensions. Such concurrent growth of unit concepts for measuring continuous spatial quantity might engender growth in students’ thinking about fractions and support their understanding of the continuous nature of rational number (Steffe & Olive, 2010) by establishing an integrated and abstract scheme for operating on units of measure in continuous spatial contexts. There is virtue to working with all three measures together, rather than treating them as separate topics. Work in the Netherlands supports the potential value of integrating instruction across length, area, weight and volume in a sequence beginning with comparing and ordering, then using a single unit to find quantity, and lastly, using an instrument to read off a scaled value (van den Heuvel-Panhuizen & Buys, 2008, p. 110). These experiences begin with the quantity length, but include the area and volume instruction in early elementary years. We hypothesize that measurement is a conceptual domain of learning that may be taught more efficiently if the ‘big ideas’ can be developed around a core set of concepts for measure units. Our purpose here is to describe tasks and ways of designing tasks that focus on conceptual aspects of 123 J. E. Barrett et al. learning measurement units, especially by comparing quantities and establishing units (Langrall, Mooney, Nisbet, & Jones, 2008). 1.1 What is already known about student’s understanding of unit concepts? Many researchers have described the importance of the unit concepts in measurement (e.g., Battista, 2006; Kamii, 2006; Mulligan, Mitchelmore, & Prescott, 2005), but rarely have they analyzed students’ use of units across several dimensions of space. Researchers concur about conceptually sophisticated ways of using units. These include the following mathematical concepts of measurement: identifying units, abstracting, iterating, coordinating nested units and using units to produce ratios (Lehrer, 2003; van den Heuvel-Panhuizen & Buys, 2008). Students’ unit concepts are rarely well formed. Although students may use unit labels to name a quantity, they often do so without being able to show the meaning of the relevant unit. Students older than 10 years often fail on broken ruler or misaligned ruler problems, indicating an inadequate unit concept (e.g., Hart, 1981). Measurement units are closely related to rational number units and proportional thinking. Yet when asked about measurement as a ratio, older elementary students often mistakenly make additive comparisons rather than making a multiplicative comparison for area units (e.g., Empson, Junk, Dominguez, & Turner, 2006), or misrepresent ratios of length measures along number lines (Moss & Case, 1999; Pettito, 1990). For example, Pettito asked students in grades 1 through 3 to locate a number along an empty number line marked with endpoints of 0 and 100. The youngest students often marked values such as 16 too far to the right, stepping right by intervals of one with over-sized steps. In contrast, the older students used midpoints to establish a ratio of and even as referent values for 50 and 25. They also counted by intervals larger than one, often counting by 10s to move to the right of zero. This latter strategy illustrates reasoning by multiplicative ratios, using ratios of 1/10 and 50/100 to help locate the quantities 16/100, or 44/100. Other researchers have emphasized the importance of unit operations as a basis for thinking about number and algebraic variable, thereby supporting multiplicative operations (Davydov, 1991, pp. 42–43). Davydov suggests multiplication is based on childrens’ experience of shifts between units as they structure measures of continuous quantity. He rehearses a classical problem of finding the number of spoonfuls of water in a barrel. Rather than carry out such a tedious exercise, the wise student finds a cup to fill with spoonfuls, and then a jug to fill with cupfuls, and finally fills the barrel with jugfuls. By translating from units Children’s unit concepts in measurement of spoons, to cups, to jugs to barrels, this student finds the ratio between spoonfuls and barrel. This task and solution requires the student to coordinate four different groups of volume units in a systematic way because each unit represents a different quantity of water with respect to the original spoon or barrel. Grouping units is an important way of reasoning about quantity through unit measures that yields efficient measures. Various researchers have shown patterns in children’s developing ways of noticing and using structure as they quantify space, such as the increasingly organized sets of squares used as students progress from disconnected square tiles, to uncoordinated rows of tiles, to spiral sets, and then partially coordinated sets of rows (Battista, Clements, Arnoff, Battista, & Van Auken Borrow, 1998; Outhred & Mitchelmore, 2000). Their work suggests that students benefit as they learn to recognize, employ and coordinate collections of area units in patterns and groups. Science education researchers argue that students need to gain a coherent, consistent, and ultimately theoretical way of thinking about units across a range of dimensions and attributes (Smith, Wiser, Anderson, & Krajcik, 2006). Similarly, Langrall et al. (2008) indicate the generality and importance of unit concepts in a theoretical perspective for the mathematics curriculum. Students appeared to benefit as they worked on measuring tasks emphasizing common unit and structuring concepts across spatial dimensions of length, perimeter, area and volume (Irwin, Vistro-Yu, & Ell, 2004; Mulligan, et al., 2005). Focusing on unit concepts has apparent benefits, but little is known about the kinds of decision making and task design considerations involved as a researcher and her students investigate unit concepts across a range of contexts from length, area and volume measurement. 1.2 Theoretical framework and key terms We employed hierarchic interactionalism (Clements & Sarama, 2007, pp. 463–466) as our theoretical framework, a blended perspective between nativist, socio-cultural interactionalism and empiricism that posits development as a coordination of both broad and local influences on cognitive changes in the individual; interactions among available knowledge gained from experience, established acquisition schemes, and the influence of others prompt development. Learning trajectories (LTs) serve as key theoretical tools for advancing knowledge about development within this theoretical framework. We find a complementary relation between learning trajectories and formative assessment (Heritage, Kim, & Vendlinksi, 2008). Formative assessment informs the teacher of possibilities to adapt instruction to student thinking. Learning trajectories describe students’ typical 639 ways of thinking. Both use immediate, informal student performance data and learning progressions to guide instructional dialog with students. Learning progressions are, ‘‘descriptions of successively more sophisticated ways of reasoning within a content domain based on research syntheses and conceptual analyses’’ (Smith, et al., 2006, p. 1). Yet, the conceptual aspects of student performance are difficult to discover, even with extensive observation without specifying links to productive instructional tasks for each progression level (Simon, 1995; Simon & Tzur, 2004). Thus, we prefer using learning trajectories (LTs) rather than learning progressions (LPs). LTs differ from LPs in that they are constructed by linking claims about conceptual development with specific instructional actions, tasks and support structures (Sarama & Clements, 2011). As such, LTs are closely related to what Ginsburg (2009) describes as mid-level theory, which ‘‘forms or informs instruction in a principled and effective manner’’ (p. 111). He argues that the clinical interview method provides insight into students’ underlying cognitive competence. Researchers cannot merely pose tasks, but must first situate each task, translate it for students and then interpret responses to the task. One must press for students’ ways of thinking about their own actions as they work to resolve tasks. The present report is a chronicle of the efforts of a collaborative research group to implement learning trajectories about length, area and volume to engage students in conceptual knowledge of measurement units. 1.3 Goals and research questions for teaching and learning unit concepts One goal for this study is to describe tasks and ways of designing tasks to address conceptual aspects of unit concepts. A second goal is to characterize formative assessment on measurement unit concepts. We focus on student thinking and strategies, using observations of student thinking to inform our instructional design and to improve our theoretical accounts of student learning. We pursued three questions: (1) What kinds of unit schemes and reasoning do students exhibit as they encounter measuring tasks that require the recognition of measurement units across various representations? (2) What are critical aspects of students’ unit composition schemes that constitute increasingly sophisticated ways of understanding measurement? (3) What kinds of measurement tasks, questioning routines, and scaffolding help students generalize unit concepts across spatial dimensions of length, area and volume and eventually establish their own theory of unit iteration and structuring? The study offers partial answers to these questions. 123 640 2 Methods 2.1 Using design research in a teaching experiment and hypotheses for teaching unit concepts We employed the design-research methodology of the teaching experiment (Cobb & Gravemeijer, 2008; Steffe & Thompson, 2000) to follow students’ growth, document their ways of understanding measurement, and to examine students’ ways of explaining and understanding their measuring actions. Data were from a longitudinal study of children’s measurement with students in upper elementary school. The children in this study represent a stratified range of abilities identified within a group of 45 students from two classrooms in a midwestern US city. Some data were collected during classroom sessions, but most were collected during clinical interviews with small groups or individual students. As a representative subset, we selected two low-performing students, four middle and two higher performing students, based on our initial assessment and teacher report. We began interviewing these students in clinical settings during their second grade year (students were age 7–8 years) and continued interviewing them through their grade 5 year. Seven of the students were interviewed at least 28 times each across this time frame. An eighth student left the school partway through the study, so we replaced that student with another who had participated in baseline interviews during the first semester of the study. Each interview lasted from 15 to 25 min and consisted of two to five tasks. In addition, we selected another subset of eight students using similar criteria whom we interviewed occasionally for further confirmation of student behaviors in the main group. We began each round of interviewing by choosing or designing tasks that would extend prior tasks and interview protocols. After administering the tasks for each interview, each video was reviewed to classify strategies along the learning trajectory. We report student progress along the length measurement tasks elsewhere (Barrett, et al., 2011; Sarama, Clements, Barrett, Van Dine, & McDonel, 2011). In contrast, this report focuses on results for several tasks, looking for patterns of strategy usage across measures within students, not only for length, but also area and volume. Task-level data included: researcher lesson plans with predictions related to levels of measurement from the conceptually aligned LTs for length,1 area and volume measurement described elsewhere (Sarama & Clements, 2009); video recordings of the interview sessions; analytical summary notes regarding student responses; and our subsequent framing of follow-up tasks. Unless noted 1 See Evaluation of hypothetical learning trajectory for length in the early years (Sarama et al., 2011). 123 J. E. Barrett et al. otherwise, data from these tasks were obtained during interviews with the primary subset of eight students. We reviewed a database of journal notes about task design. Journal entries included plans for assessment tasks, supportive or explanatory discussion notes, along with predictions about student performance for all 28 interviews across six semesters for 8 students. We also searched our summary reflective field notes on each session for each child and our summary memos from monthly design-cycle meetings spanning all six semesters to identify interviews and tasks related to unit identity, representational issues, and coordination across different dimensions. Thus, we identified 11 different tasks that addressed three major themes: (1) the integration of multiple representations of units (see Fig. 1), (2) the coordination of related units (see Fig. 2), and (3) theoretical perspectives on unit measures, especially multiplicative relations (see Fig. 3). Our design research assumes that the development of unit and unit measurement concepts proceeds broadly as children shift from actions on quantities to making records of those actions and eventually abstract those actions (Moss & Case, 1999; Piaget, et al., 1960). This research tests that assumption. Furthermore, we purposely interspersed tasks from length, area and volume measurement spanning four academic years of the children’s development. Although this approach was a productive way of discovering student thinking about unit concepts, we do not claim that interspersing tasks from these dimensions is inherently better than the more standard sequence of teaching first length, then area and lastly volume topics. We hypothesized that children would construct meaningful and efficient ways of measuring as they forged ways of connecting images of unit and unit iteration among multiple figural and motor representations (Battista, 1999; Steffe & Cobb, 1988). We also expected children to gain efficiency in measuring and more clear knowledge about equivalence and the meaning of variable in algebraic contexts (cf. Knuth, Alibali, McNeil, Weinberg, & Stephens, 2005) as they integrated their knowledge and strategies involving multiple representations of unit (e.g., Cannon, 1992), coordinated groups of units (e.g., Stephan, Bowers, Cobb, & Gravemeijer, 2004) and implemented generalized rules for making unit assignments within various dimensions and across spatial dimensions (e.g., Carpenter & Lewis, 1976). 3 Findings: overview of design research phases We identify three design phases in the teaching experiment, which we narrate as the findings of this study. Phase One reports on students’ concept of a unit, multiple ways students might represent units, and ways of coordinating Children’s unit concepts in measurement Fig. 1 Phase One tasks 641 A Broken Ruler –Spring, Gr. 2 Ask: “Measure this strip with this ruler” and then, “What are you counting?” If tick marks, align the blue strip to a sequence of one-unit long strips and relate back to A. broken ruler or B. unnumbered ruler. Ask for explanations. B Unnumbered Ruler – Spring, Gr. 2 C Partially-covered Ruler – Spring, Gr. 2 Give an eight-foot ruler with inch markings and a ribbon obscuring the middle of the ruler. Ask, “Can you measure this ribbon?” If students count their own pointing motions, try the shorter section of ribbon. those representations to measure. Phase Two describes students’ ways of grouping or subdividing units, anticipating ways they might construct flexible and dynamic structures for quantity. Lastly, Phase Three incorporates students’ work with arbitrary units and proportionality as they built a theory of measurement. We now describe each phase in detail by describing the tasks, the student work, and our interpretation of the students’ work and strategies. 3.1 Findings and analysis, Phase One: abstracting spatial units from various representations In Phase One of our study, we focused on helping students recognize and use length, area or volume units as they compared objects by measuring them. Additionally, we expected to help students measure consistently by having them identify units represented with a variety of tools and tasks. We wanted students to count units directly for each measure. At the outset of the study, students were 7 or 8 years old (grade 2). Data from an initial clinical assessment2 indicated that seven of eight students (Arielle, Owen, Drew, Abby, Ryan, Anselm and Danny) could find length by counting linear units, and were able to iterate a single unit to represent a collection of units with few counting errors. However, they produced inexact measures in special cases where the zero point was not available on the ruler (refer to the level: unit relater and repeater in Table 1. See also: Sarama & Clements, 2009). We wanted 2 Portions of the assessment were validated in other work. students to develop a unit-based, consistent way of finding length measures even where a ruler might be misaligned or broken; we wanted them to use various images or tools and their actions with these objects to identify the units as conceptual entities. We wanted to extend this kind of unit abstraction to area and volume measuring as well. Thus, we wanted to motivate these seven students to build more efficient ways to measure length, and to make optimal use of number labels along rulers. To accomplish this, we modified the display of units on tools to encourage integration between tick marks, intervals between tick marks, and number labels. We covered up a portion of the ruler object, obscuring some of the unit intervals and tick marks along the ruler so students would need to rely on the higher number labels beyond the covered section to reason backwards and deduce the length of an object along such an obscured ruler. We expected students to identify a line segment as a unit and learn to connect number labels or tick marks with some set of unitary segments. We designed three tasks that supported recognition of unit intervals (See Fig. 1, Tasks A and B in Phase One, along with a pilot task on wraps and sides in Phase Two). These tasks were administered at two-week intervals over a six-week span. The tasks supported shifts by five of the students (Arielle, Owen, Drew, Abby, and Ryan) toward reasoning at the next level in the LT, consistent length measurer (See Table 1). These five students successfully identified units from varying representations. However, two students attributed the inconsistency of their measures with different tools to a poor fit between these tools instead 123 642 Fig. 2 Phase Two: coordinating groups of units with subordinate units or superordinate units J. E. Barrett et al. D Wraps and Sides Spring, Gr. 3 Give the pipe cleaner the length of the perimeter of one square. Ask, “How does the distance around this set of tiles compare to the length of the pipe cleaner?” If the student gives a qualitative comparison, ask “How much longer?” E Volume comparison of inch cube and box – Fall, Gr. 4 Show a pile of 30 cubes and ask the student how many would fill the large box. Arrange the 30 cubes as pictured and asked, “Compare the volume of this (inch cube) to the volume of that (box). How much larger is the box than one cube?” F Area of Blobs – Spring, Gr. 4 Provide two drawings of blobs on a single transparency and several grids. Ask, “How does the area of this small blob compare to the area of the large blob? How much bigger?” G Volume comparison of dm cube and room – Fall, Gr. 5 Ask a pair of students to compare the space inside a cubic decimeter to the space inside a classroom, given meter sticks, a decimeter cube, and a meter cube. After 5 minutes, ask, “What were you doing to solve this? How did you make progress?” of changing their own scheme in a way that would enable them to identify the unit concepts across tools. Although we saw progress in seven of the eight students’ ability to integrate units among representations, three of the seven students still did not take advantage of number labels; instead, they relied on counting visually perceptible units. To determine if these students did not effectively use the number labels because they did not possess the computational skills or if they preferred to identify and count available units, we constrained their access to perceptual displays of units along tools. For example, given a 96-in. ruler, students were asked to measure a 26-inch ribbon stretched next to the ruler between the 8th and 34th tick marks. There was a second ribbon obscuring part of the ruler between the 15th and 29th tick marks (Task C, Fig. 1). The interviewer asked 123 students to measure without uncovering that part of the ruler (cf. Steffe & Cobb, 1988). In response, two students adopted a ‘‘counting on’’ or ‘‘adding on’’ strategy to produce a value for the ribbon length. For example, Arielle explained that she could get from the initial value of 8 to the final value of 34 by adding two 10s, one 2, and one 4. Thus, her answer of 26 was composed of subunits of size 10, 2, and 4. Working on a related task, Anselm explained that he would find the length of an object that spanned from 3 to 27 on the ruler because he could ‘‘back it up 3’’ and the end would be at 24. These strategies illustrate the use of numbers to reason about units of length, exactly as we intended. All seven students began to coordinate tick marks, intervals and number labels successfully. However, incomplete knowledge of arithmetic operations on numbers greater than 20 Children’s unit concepts in measurement 643 Fig. 3 Phase Three: building an informal theory of units and arbitrary unit assignments A 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 impeded three students’ success at the computational aspects of the task. This task fits into a sequence of tasks that incorporates numerical approaches and strategies with spatial strategies for measuring by demanding systematic integration of tick marks, interval spaces, and number labels beside the tick marks on a ruler. It helped shift students’ attention to the comparison of quantities, rather than an analysis of isolated objects and their lengths. Students were prompted to notice and coordinate units across varied representations. The ribbon tasks were further revised to compare pairs of ribbons stretched along different portions of the same long ruler, highlighting the comparative nature of measurement activity. We asked them to explain how much longer one ribbon is than the other, rather than simply identifying the longer ribbon. 3.2 Findings and analysis, Phase Two: efficient grouping of superordinate and subordinate units During the second semester of the study we began Phase Two, looking for ways to help students coordinate sub- and superordinate units with units. Our review of our notes and our summary analysis of student work from the prior semester indicated that students struggled to relate different-sized units in a systematic way. We expected to help students find ways of coordinating units within a measuring system. For example, we know mm2, cm2 are smaller denominations of units that can be grouped to fit exactly into the larger denomination of a square meter. Similarly, we wanted to find whether students might invent or adopt ways of coordinating small units in composite units, for the sake of efficiency. 123 644 J. E. Barrett et al. Table 1 Learning trajectory for length measurement Trajectory level Student thinking and actions Instructional tasks End-to-end length measurer Lays units endto-end and counts to report length. May not recognize the need for equal-length units. Needs a complete set of units to span along an object Student expects that length is quantifiable as a composition of shorter lengths. Compares an end-to-end train of countable objects to the linear extent of an object. The scheme is enhanced by the growing conception of length measuring as sweeping through large units coordinated with composing a length with unit sticks Provide student with fewer objects than needed to span along an object and ask for length Length unit relater and repeater Measures by repeated use of a unit, even if imprecisely. Relates size and number of units explicitly. Can add lengths to obtain the length of a whole Student can iterate a mental unit along an object. Cardinal values are connected to space units for small quantities but weaker beyond these. Student recognizes that fewer larger units will be required, if units are visible Instructor provides a tick mark tool with no number labels and a band of connected unit strips. Student is asked to coordinate these tools to measure various lengths Consistent length measurer Finds length on a bent path as the sum of its parts. Measures consistently, knowing the need for identical units and a zero point. May coordinate units and subunits Can compose and partition length units. Can think of the length of a bent path as the sum of its parts. Mentally iterates a unit and subunits (internalized ruler) Student is asked to predict length with mental iterations before checking with a tool Conceptual ruler measurer Finds length by imagining an end-to-end collection of units Operates mentally with units and composite units. Can mentally project a known length along an object to measure or partition an unknown length Instructor asks students to find multiple shapes given the constraint of a constant perimeter We began with an exploratory task to coordinate the side lengths and the entire perimeter of square tiles as two different, but related linear units of measure in several comparison situations (see Fig. 2, Task D). Researchers administered the task to all 45 students in both classrooms. Although all of the students related the longer units called a wrap (the perimeter of one square, and the length of the pipe cleaner) to just one part of the wrap called a side (a side length of one square), they rarely integrated wraps and sides together with a single unit. They reported them separately; students reported the mixed fraction number 2 wraps and 3 sides, but did not report it as 2 and wraps, nor did they describe it as 11 sides. By asking students to switch from one unit to the other and characterize the same unit, students were challenged to relate sides and wraps in two different ways. We wanted to demonstrate measuring situations that would naturally involve the coordination of small, large and larger units. Thus, we designed tasks like the Blobs task to motivate translations within a system of units (See Task F in Fig. 2). This type of task represented a substantive design shift. Previously, we asked students to compare quantities that were nearly the same. Here we called for comparisons of quantities spanning a broad shift of magnitude (e.g., compare the sizes of a small puddle and a vast lake); we wanted students to engage in multiplicative relations and gain efficiency as they measure area (Davydov, 1991). We used comparison problems, 123 Instructor makes exaggerated mistakes by gapping and overlapping sets of units with the expectation that the student will identify and correct these errors Provide broken ruler for student to measure objects Instructor provides a physical unit and a ruler to measure objects that require the student to both iterate and subdivide the unit. Objects should require quarter units and eighth units emphasizing the coordination of various units to promote a clear translation from small units to a bundled set of units seen as a larger unit. We reviewed eight students’ responses to area measure tasks based on our learning trajectory on area (Sarama & Clements, 2009, p. 302). We summarize the upper four levels of the area trajectory to situate our subsequent analysis. At the Primitive Coverer level, children represent a covering of a region by drawing, but their alignment of shapes is intuitive; therefore, the rows and columns are not accurate. Not until the Partial Row Structurer level do they structure the rectangle as a set of rows, understanding the collinearity of rows and the constraint that each row must have the same number of units. At the following level, Row and Column Structurer, children have a mental construct of a row as consisting of a composite of aligned, congruent unit squares and determine the number of squares by iterating those rows. At the Array Structurer level, children understand that the rectangle’s dimensions provide the number of squares in rows and columns and thus can meaningfully calculate the area from these dimensions without perceptual support. We expected five of the students (Arielle, Abby, Jessie, Ryan, and Drew) to partially structure regions and to count by groups of units (area LT level: Partial Row Structurer) with Task F. We believed they could also align collections of units in columns but had not yet coordinated rows and columns at once; they often miscounted the corner unit. Children’s unit concepts in measurement 645 Table 2 Observed strategies of students and LT levels while working on Blobs task Number of students Observed strategies Level within area LT 5 of 8 Jessie*, Ryan*, Arielle*, Abby*, Owen Created an upper bound to compare areas, counting individual squares systematically in either rows or columns Partial Row Structurer–if they drew; or if not, then Row and Column Structurer 1 of 8 Danny Created a lower bound to compare areas, counting individual squares unsystematically in the interior Partial Row Structurer 2 of 8 Anselm*, Drew Used a decomposition and recomposition strategy to account for the partial units, counting whole units per row or per column Area conserver/Row and Column Structurer 1 of 8 Owen* Counted around the border of the blob and then created a partial array on the interior Row and Column Structurer In contrast, we expected the other three students (Owen, Danny and Anselm) to count more systematically using groups of rows or columns (Row and Column Structurer) because they had demonstrated more advanced reasoning using arrays. We gave them drawings of two different blobs on a single sheet of paper along with three different-sized square grids on separate clear plastic sheets. These sheets were provided as measuring tools. The larger blob contained an area 290 times larger than the area of the small blob. The grids in the Blob task were constructed so that each grid could be related to the others in a simple ratio (they were commensurate area units in the ratio 1:16:64). Hypothetically, if a student treated one square from the larger grid as a 1 9 1 unit, then the same square region on the middle grid would be a 4 9 4 unit of units, and the same-sized square region on the smallest grid would be an 8 9 8 unit of units. If a student wanted to report an area measure using both the smallest and middle-sized grids, they would need to understand that four of the smallest grid squares could fit within each square of the middle-sized grid. The interviewer negotiated students’ responses to the task in the following ways: If the student offered a qualitative comparison (e.g., saying, ‘‘this blob is much larger!’’), then the interviewer asked, ‘‘How many times larger is it?’’ By asking students to measure across a vast change of magnitude, we expected them to attempt to group units of units to coordinate quantities among the three grids. We also expected them to make consistent translations from smaller to larger units by referring to ratios between groups of the two smaller units and each large unit. Based on previous research (Lehrer, Jenkins, & Osana, 1998) and earlier interviews, the research team expected some students to use an upper bound strategy by counting every square that had any part inside the blob shape or a lower bound strategy by counting only squares completely inside that blob shape. We also expected some students to use a decomposition and re-composition strategy. During the interviews, we observed the following strategies (see Table 2). We noted six students (*) performed at the level we predicted. The task prompted six of eight students to form a ratio and use it to coordinate units, but not necessarily the ratio we intended. Two students, Arielle and Abby, used a visual comparison to estimate that the small blob was the area of one of the squares in the smallest grid. Three students, Abby, Anselm, and Jessie, correctly related the 64 small square units to 1 large square. Danny attempted to relate the small and large square area units but computed the ratio as 1:67 instead of 1:64 due to counting errors. Owen related 16 medium square units to 1 large square unit. In all, six students coordinated area units to compare regions as we hoped. The grids may have prompted these students to see a smaller 2D array within a large 2D array. Five students correctly reported eight small squares across and eight small squares down within a larger grid square. These students apparently coordinated their unit of 1 square of 8 rows of 8 small tiles to form rows that constituted a superunit of 64 squares. Abby, Danny, and Jessie reported multiplicative comparisons of the small to large blobs in terms of the small squares: 112, 402, and 288, respectively (-61, 39, and -1% error) through the use of an intermediate unit. Although the percentage errors are high for two answers, the students each demonstrated ability to coordinate multiple units through a set of multiplication operations to relate the smallest unit to the largest unit. We take this strategy as more sophisticated than directly counting how many additional of the smallest squares it would take to be as large as the largest region. In contrast, Owen and Jessie (in a second response) gave additive comparisons. Owen said the larger blob was 686 more by counting medium squares and Jessie said it would be 2,870 more small squares than the large blob. Nevertheless, all eight students related different units from the available grids in coordinated patterns, suggesting an initial multiplicative scheme. Based on the outcomes with Task F, we decided to continue setting tasks that required a vast shift in magnitude to motivate and support students to coordinate commensurate units, subunits and super-ordinate units. 123 646 We shifted dimensions from area to volume to help students generalize the concept of unit coordination. We eventually used the volume comparison of the inch cube and box task (Task E in Fig. 2) and the volume comparison of dm cubes (Task G in Fig. 2) to help students continue to coordinate various units in a systematic and efficient way. Surprisingly, while students coordinated units into portions of layers, layers, and sets of layers to report the number of inch cubes (reporting a range from 900 to 1,500 cubes), students did not use that same measurement to answer: ‘‘how many times larger is the box than the one inch cube?’’ For example, a student who claimed the larger box could contain 900 cubes also said that it was 899 times larger than the small cube (899 is the difference, rather than the ratio). Apparently students do not always interpret a measurement as a ratio between the unit and the object being measured. To summarize Phase Two, we found three of eight students using multiplicative reasoning extensively and all eight coordinating units on some aspects of the Blob task. For the task on volume, students mistakenly discriminated between our question about the volume measured in unit cubes and how many times larger it would be than a unit cube. Nevertheless, they did coordinate units by grouping them and multiplying to find a measure in a comprehensive unit value (i.e., they were purposely relating different-sized units). Having addressed students’ integration of units across representations, and having prompted them to coordinate units within a system of related units, we now report on our efforts to support students as they developed a theory of measure to include the use of arbitrary units and to emphasize multiplicative relations among quantities involving units. 3.3 Findings and analysis, Phase Three: theoretical analysis of measure units We began Phase Three of our design research work by synthesizing our design work from the prior two phases; we still wanted students to connect representations and coordinate units in groups. Further, we wanted them to understand units as arbitrary assignments of value; the value of a measure depends on the selected unit (e.g., see Task I, Fig. 3, carried out with the secondary set of eight students). As early as the third semester of the project (grade 3 spring semester), we designed tasks to investigate students’ understanding of units of volume and their ability to cope with inverse relations among unit size and quantity. We illustrate the design of this group of tasks by describing our design of Task H (see Fig. 3), the volume comparison of prisms. We used Task H to prompt students to reflect on the process of selecting and identifying a unit of measure and 123 J. E. Barrett et al. to emphasize the inverse relation of unit and measures. Based on the students’ performance on an assessment of volume measurement knowledge during the previous spring, the research team conjectured that all but one of the students seemed to have at least a partial understanding of cubes as filling a space in a countable and structured arrangement (at least primitive 3D array counter: in Sarama & Clements, 2009, pp. 306–308). Five students (Arielle, Abby, Ryan, Drew, and Anselm) counted cubes one at a time in a carefully structured context but were unable to use more systematic and accurate counting strategies (partial 3D structurer). Two students (Danny and Owen) demonstrated ability to count more systematically than the other six, accounting for internal and external cubes and operating on composite columns of cubes (3D Row and Column Structurer). Thus, each of the seven students who exhibited a structuring strategy for identifying a set of unit cubes was given this task (See Fig. 3, Task H). Each student was shown two rectangular prisms whose volumes were in a ratio of 1 to 2 and then was asked to compare these two prisms by volume. Next, they were given further pairs of prisms to compare. We expected the students to correctly identify the volume of the larger prism for any pairing using the smaller prism as unit. We thought they would struggle to identify the volume of a smaller prism if the unit was the larger prism; this would require fractional quantities (halves or quarters). In order to relate observed strategies (see Table 3) with the final solutions (see Table 4), the names of students producing correct volume comparisons have been bolded; the names of students producing incorrect volume comparisons have been italicized. Among these eight students, we expected Danny and Owen to make correct comparisons and they did. Three others, Arielle, Ryan, and Anselm (with a minor error) also produced correct values despite our classification of their strategy at a lower level that would predict difficulties on such a task. Two of the three students who made inaccurate Table 3 Students exhibited strategies for the multiple prism task Number of students Observed strategies 3 of 8 Owen, Drew, Sara Visually compared prisms without setting them side-by-side, keeping them separate 1 of 8 Danny Directly compared prisms by setting them side-by-side or by moving the smaller prism step-wise as if to traverse the space occupied by the larger prism Alternated between directly comparing prisms side-by-side, and indirectly comparing them while separate 3 of 8 Arielle, Abby, Anselm 1 of 8 Ryan Used transitive reasoning to compare the three prisms at once without moving them Children’s unit concepts in measurement Table 4 Students’ responses to the multiple prism task Number of students Correctness 4 of 8 Owen, Arielle, Danny, Ryan Correctly identified the volume of each prism when comparing all three prisms without any errors, regardless of which one was chosen as the unit 1 of 8 Anselm Correctly identified the volume of each prism when comparing all three prisms, regardless of which one was chosen as the unit, yet with one minor error Made multiple inaccurate quantitative comparisons 3 of 8 Abby, Drew, Sara comparisons, as predicted, used only visual comparisons without moving or iterating a prism to arrange it next to another. The third, Abby, used motions with the related prisms, but appeared to guess at quantities without carefully iterating the smaller along the larger prism. However, Ryan successfully compared all pairings without moving any prisms. The four students who correctly identified the volume, given all combinations of unit and target prisms, were posed a follow-up task. They were asked to compare the volume of a composite solid made of one small and one medium prism to the volume of the large prism. Ryan analyzed the task successfully, identifying a quantity of 4/3 as the measure of the larger prism, taking the composite solid as a unit. Ryan, Arielle, and Owen correctly identified a quantity of 3/4 as the measure of the composite prism, treating the larger prism as the unit, but did not find the quantity if they were given composite prisms as a unit. The following excerpt of an interview exemplifies the structured process of questioning, including ways of extending questions to probe an incorrect response: I (interviewer): Let’s say again this (smallest prism, S, 1 9 1 9 2 units) is our unit, how big is that (pointing to largest prism, L, 2 9 2 9 2)? Abby: Three of those (medium prism, M, 1 9 29 2) and 9 of those (pointed to S). [Actually, M measures 2S and L measures 4S] I: Ok, so, this thing (L) is 3 of these (pointed to M)? Abby: Yeah, and 9 of those (pointed to S, but pausing). No, wait it’s not 9 is it. I: You can touch them. Would it help? (Abby picked up S and iterated it four times on the top face of the L. Then she picked up M and iterated it twice above L.) Abby: This one (L) is two of these (M). [correct] Later in this interview, Abby identified the relations among the prisms when the medium prism was assigned 647 the value of 1 unit, correctly changing the measures of the other two. In contrast, the other seven students were unable to restate values and consistently find related quantities based on the renaming of a unit object. We often allowed students multiple trials if they were able to revise their strategies like this because we wanted to know what they could do given feedback and opportunities to verify claims (Lesh & Hjalmarson, 2008). We also developed a task (See Task J, Fig. 3) using those prisms to find whether students would be able to set a correspondence to a number line and map volume quantity to a number line. We intended to prompt students to coordinate volume units and length units as they interpreted the prism locations along the line. We wanted to find whether students would recognize that the ratio between measures of the prisms would depend on the unit location. Here we present anecdotal evidence of responses from one student in grade 4. The student correctly identified the ratio between the unit and object being measured whenever the objects’ volumes were in a ratio of 1:2 or 1:4 if he was allowed to put the smaller prism inside the other (they were hollow). In each case the student needed to handle both prisms simultaneously and physically iterate the smaller inside the larger. However, the student did not solve a task where the middle prism was located at the position of one on the number line, requiring a placement at one half for the smaller prism. The student was only able to solve cases where the smallest prism was placed at or beyond the value of one. A key idea in guiding students along the trajectories for length, area and volume is to shift toward multiplicative comparisons instead of additive comparisons (Sarama & Clements, 2009, pp. 304, 308). The highest level of our trajectories for area and volume specify age 9 (grade 3 or 4) based on multiplicative operations. Understanding that all measures are in fact ratios between a unit quantity and other quantities constitutes sophisticated and conceptual knowledge for measurement. During the third semester, our students were at least this age, yet they tended to compare by additive operations. So we designed tasks to prompt multiplicative relations. We wanted students to integrate both additive and multiplicative schemes for measuring. Based on our work with the prism tasks in Phase Three and the Blobs task in Phase Two, we tested and carried out a task on length (Fig. 3, Task K) that required students to integrate arithmetic and multiplicative comparisons as they searched for invariance. We asked for a multiplicative comparison by holding the ratio of 1:3 constant across three pairs of line segments and asking students what was the same in all three pairings. Two of eight students given this task were able to make both multiplicative and additive comparisons. Two more students noticed multiplicative comparisons. The other four students only noticed additive comparisons. 123 648 We also attempted to direct students’ attention to multiplicative comparisons by modifying our questions. We prompted students to compare prisms by volume and followed up by asking, ‘‘How many times larger is the box compared to one unit cube?’’ (Task E in Fig. 2). Only two of the eight students noticed that the measure of the box and the comparison between the box and the unit cube as a ratio should be the same. We plan to address this difficulty by giving students more opportunities to describe measures in words, including asking them to state a ratio between a unit and the object measured with that unit. 4 Discussion and implications Students in the study developed increasingly integrated representations for units of length during Phase One. At first, the students often relied on figural images alone to measure length. Thus, we developed tasks to emphasize arithmetic operations with number labels along rulers. As the interviewer prompted repeated measures with different tools and highlighted inconsistent cases, the students came to integrate number labels along rulers, tick marks along ruler objects, and the intervals between marks (Barrett, Clements, Klanderman, Pennisi, & Polaki, 2006, p. 218). Students learned to represent units in a variety of ways including iterative motions, and the collection of segments or ‘‘sticks’’; we found support for claims that students benefit by encountering related measures carried out by moving their finger along a path in a syncopated motion and by pointing to successive midpoints of intervals (Cullen & Barrett, 2010; Lakoff & Nunez, 2000). It appeared to be critical to devise tasks like the Covered Ruler task to integrate length units among representations. Otherwise, students were counting directly, an inefficient but reliable approach. Further development of similar tasks would likely benefit students by prompting students to integrate schemes for visual and motor counting operations as they measure. We also noticed that the students struggled to coordinate sub- and superordinate units. This prompted Phase Two of our work, focusing on the coordination of units. Although the challenges students face in learning to coordinate units are clear from previous research about structuring space (Battista, et al., 1998; Mulligan, et al., 2005; Outhred & Mitchelmore, 2004; Piaget, 1970), it is less clear how to design tasks and administer tasks in formative assessment so students develop more structured unit concepts. We found that devising effective tasks required purposeful shifts in phrasing from qualitative comparison to quantitative comparison (often additive in nature) and asking specifically about multiplicative comparisons (Cullen, et al., 2010). We also found that tasks requiring students to 123 J. E. Barrett et al. compare objects with extremely different areas or volumes motivated students to coordinate a wide range of units; this led to the growth of a coordination scheme for relating counting at various scales within a single scale. Students struggled in this study to transition from making additive comparisons of measures to making multiplicative comparisons, even when given several opportunities, supporting prior research (Empson, et al., 2006; Moss & Case, 1999). Although we were surprised that measures were not often understood as ratios, we take it as a challenge to prompt multiplicative relations with more focused questions; this requires further research. We conjecture that students have not had enough experience reporting measures in verbal ways, and believe they should be asked to explain ratios that are implicit with any measurement. That is, saying that a board is 1.3 m long is the same thing as saying that the board is 1.3 times as long as a meter, but the latter phrasing better supports unit concept growth. During the second phase, we expected students to understand the multiplicative relations that constitute a sophisticated level of theorizing about measures. This prompted Phase Three of design in this study that included both the ideas of multiple representations of unit and the coordination of units, but also the arbitrary nature of unit assignments, and the proportionality of related units within a theoretical account of measurement. Our findings about Task G illustrated one way of helping students shift units as they compared; repeated shifts among units and measures within one context may prompt students to recognize the arbitrary nature of assignments of unit value (Seymour & Lehrer, 2006). The three phases described in this paper constitute a pattern of increasingly sophisticated practices for establishing and using quantity to conduct scientific inquiry or perhaps to test models in technological or engineering settings (Ruthven, et al., 2009). Using the concept of a unit as our analytic theme, we began by attending to students’ ways of coordinating representations. Next, we studied student’s ways of grouping or subdividing units, allowing flexible and dynamic structures for quantity. We also included students’ work with arbitrary units and proportionality to establish a theory of measurement that built upon the prior phases. Taken together, these phases allowed a wide-angled view of student cognitive development rather than focusing narrowly on a single trajectory. The phases served to integrate a broad collection of related concepts. Other researchers may benefit by using a similar pattern for coordinating across learning trajectories as we have coordinated length, area and volume. One might analyze multiple content strands at once by taking such a lens on increasingly sophisticated mathematical practices as we exemplified in these three phases. This account of Children’s unit concepts in measurement our design phases contributes to the broad understanding of what constitutes a child’s theory of measurement (Lehrer & Schauble, 2007, p. 156). Unit representations, unit coordination tasks, and arbitrary unit assignments that prompt reflection on multiplicative relations are critical elements for such a theory of measurement. Acknowledgments The research reported here was supported by the National Science Foundation through Grant No. DRL-0732217, ‘‘A Longitudinal Account of Children’s Knowledge of Measurement.’’ The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the NSF. References Barrett, J. E., Clements, D. H., Klanderman, D., Pennisi, S. J., & Polaki, M. V. (2006). Students’ coordination of geometric reasoning and measuring strategies on a fixed perimeter task: Developing mathematical understanding of linear measurement. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37(3), 187–221. Barrett, J. E., Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., Cullen, C., McCool, J., Witkowski Rumsey, C., et al. (2011). Children’s abstraction of iterative units to measure linear space: A trajectory. Mathematical Thinking and Learning (in press). Battista, M. T. (1999). Fifth graders’ enumeration of cubes in 3D arrays: Conceptual progress in an inquiry-based classroom. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30(4), 417–448. Battista, M. T. (2006). Applying cognition-based assessment to elementary students’ development of understanding of area, and volume measurement. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 6(2), 185–204. Battista, M. T., Clements, D. H., Arnoff, J., Battista, K., & Van Auken Borrow, C. (1998). Students’ spatial structuring of 2D arrays of squares. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29(5), 503–532. Cannon, L. (1992). Middle grade students’ representations of linear units. In W. Geeslin & K. Graham (Eds.), Proceedings of the Sixteenth PME Conference (Vol. I, pp. 105–112). Durham: Program Committee of the 16th PME Conference. Carpenter, T. P., & Lewis, R. (1976). The development of the concept of a standard unit of measure in young children. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 7, 53–58. Clarke, D., Cheeseman, J., McDonough, A., & Clarke, B. (2003). Assessing and developing measurement with young children. In D. H. Clements & G. Bright (Eds.), Learning and teaching measurement: 2003 yearbook (pp. 68–80). Reston: NCTM. Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007). Early childhood mathematics learning. In F. K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 461–555). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Cobb, P., & Gravemeijer, K. (2008). Experimenting to support and understand learning processes. In A. E. Kelly, R. A. Lesh, & J. Y. Baek (Eds.), Handbook of design research methods in education: Innovations in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics learning and teaching (pp. 68–95). New York: Routledge. Corcoran, T., Mosher, F. A., & Rogat, A. (2009). Learning progressions in science: An evidence-based approach to reform. New York: Teachers College-Columbia University. Cullen, C., & Barrett, J. E. (2010, July 18–23). Strategy use indicative of an understanding of units of length. Paper presented at the 649 34th Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics in Education, Belo Horizonte, Brazil. Cullen, C., Witkowski, C., Miller, A., Barrett, J. E., Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2010). The key components for measurement tasks. Paper presented at the 32nd Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Columbus, OH. Davydov, V. V. (1991). Psychological abilities of primary school children in learning mathematics. Soviet Studies in Mathematics Education, 6. Empson, S. B., Junk, D., Dominguez, H., & Turner, E. (2006). Fractions as the coordination of multiplicatively related quantities: A cross-sectional study of children’s thinking. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 63(1), 1–28. Ginsburg, H. P. (2009). The challenge of formative assessment in mathematics education: Children’s minds, teachers’ minds. Human Development, 52, 109–128. Hart, K. M. (1981). Children’s Understanding of Mathematics: 11–16. London: John Murray. Heritage, M., Kim, J., & Vendlinksi, T. (2008). From evidence to action: A seamless process in formative assessment? Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Annual Meeting. Irwin, K. C., Vistro-Yu, C. P., & Ell, F. R. (2004). Understanding linear measurement: A comparison of Filipino and New Zealand children. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 16(2), 3–24. Kamii, C. (2006). Measurement of length: How can we teach it better? Teaching children mathematics, 13(3), 5. Knuth, E. J., Alibali, M. W., McNeil, N. M., Weinberg, A., & Stephens, A. C. (2005). Middle school students’ understanding of core algebraic concepts: Equivalence & variable. ZDM: the International Journal for Mathematics Education, 37(1), 1–9. Lakoff, G., & Nunez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books. Langrall, C., Mooney, E., Nisbet, S., & Jones, G. A. (2008). Elementary Children’s access to powerful mathematical ideas. In L. D. English (Ed.), Handbook of international research in mathematics education (pp. 109–135). New York: Routledge. Lehrer, R. (2003). Developing understanding of measurement. In J. Kilpatrick, G. Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), Research companion to principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Lehrer, R., Jenkins, M., & Osana, H. (1998). Longitudinal study of children’s reasoning about space and geometry. In R. Lehrer & D. Chazan (Eds.), Designing learning environments for developing understanding of geometry and space (Vol. 1, pp. 137–167). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2007). Contrasting emerging conceptions of distribution in contexts of error and natural variation. In M. C. Lovett & P. Shah (Eds.), Thinking with data (pp. 149–176). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lesh, R. A., & Hjalmarson, M. A. (2008). Design research: Engineering, systems, products and processes for innovation. In Handbook of International Research in Mathematics Education (pp. 520–534). New York: Routledge. Moss, J., & Case, R. (1999). Developing children’s understanding of the rational numbers: A new model and an experimental curriculum. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 122–147. Mulligan, J., Mitchelmore, M., & Prescott, A. (2005). Case studies of children’s development of structure in early mathematics: A twoyear longitudinal study. Paper presented at the 29th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Melbourne, Australia. 123 650 Outhred, L. N., & Mitchelmore, M. C. (2000). Young children’s intuitive understanding of rectangular area measurement. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 31(2), 144–167. Outhred, L., & Mitchelmore, M. (2004). Students’ structuring of rectangular arrays. International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, 28th, Bergen, Norway, July 14–18, 2004. Outhred, L., Mitchelmore, M., McPhail, D., & Gould, P. (2003). Count me into measurement: A program for the early elementary school. In D. H. Clements (Ed.), Learning and teaching measurement (Vol. 1, pp. 81–99). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Pettito, A. L. (1990). Development of numberline and measurement concepts. Cognition and Instruction, 7, 55–78. Piaget, J. (1970). Structuralism (C. Maschler, Trans.). New York: Harper & Row Publishers. Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. (1960). The child’s conception of geometry (E. A. Lunzer, Trans. 1981 Basic Books, Inc. ed.). New York: W. W. Norton & Company. Ruthven, K., Laborde, C., Leach, J., & Tiberghien, A. (2009). Design tools in didactical research: Instrumenting the epistemological and cognitive aspects of the design of teaching sequences. Educational Researcher, 38(5), 329–342. Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2009). Early childhood mathematics education research: Learning trajectories for young children. New York: Routledge. Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2011). Learning Trajectories: Theoretical Approaches Supporting the Development of Curriculum, Standards and Assessment Research in Mathematics Education. In J. Confrey, A. Maloney & K. Nguyen (Eds.), Learning Trajectories: Tools for Research in Mathematics Education (tentative title) (ZDM, issue 5). Raleigh, North Carolina: Information Age Publishing (in press). Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., Barrett, J. E., Van Dine, D. W., & McDonel, J. S. (2011). Evaluation of a learning trajectory for length in the early years. ZDM: the International Journal for Mathematics Education, issue 5 (this volume). Seymour, J. R., & Lehrer, R. (2006). Tracing the evolution of pedagogical content knowledge as the development of interanimated discourses. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(4), 549–582. Siegler, R. S. (2003). Implications of cognitive science research for mathematics education. In J. Kilpatrick, W. B. Martin, & 123 J. E. Barrett et al. D. E. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Simon, M. A. (1995). Reconstructing Mathematics Pedagogy from a Constructivist Perspective. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26(2), 114–145. Simon, M. A., & Tzur, R. (2004). Explicating the role of mathematical tasks in conceptual learning: An elaboration of the hypothetical learning trajectory [research]. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 6(2), 91–104. Smith, J., Sisman, G., Figueras, H., Lee, K., Dietiker, L., & Lehrer, R. (2008). Assessing Curricular Contributions to Poor Measurement Learning. Unpublished Powerpoint at Research Presession Symposium. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Smith, C. L., Wiser, M., Anderson, C. W., & Krajcik, J. (2006). Implications of research on children’s learning for standards and assessment: A proposed learning progression for matter and atomic molecular theory. Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives, 4(1 and 2), 1–98. Steffe, L. P., & Cobb, P. (1988). Construction of Arithmetical Meanings and Strategies. New York: Springer. Steffe, L. P., & Olive, J. (2010). Children’s Fractional Knowledge. New York: Springer. Steffe, L. P., & Thompson, P. W. (2000). Teaching experiment methodology: Underlying principles and essential elements. In R. Lesh & E. Kelley (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 267–306). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Stephan, M., Bowers, J., Cobb, P., & Gravemeijer, K. (2004). Supporting students’ development of measuring conceptions: Analyzing students’ learning in social context (Journal for Research in Mathematics Education Monograph #12). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M., & Buys, K. (Eds.). (2008). Young children learn measurement and geometry (Vol. 3). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers. Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., & Van Dooren, W. (2009). Conceptualizing, investigating and enhancing adaptive expertise in elementary mathematics education. European Journal of Psychology of Education, XXIV(3), 335–359.