International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 www.elsevier.com/locate/ijintrel Ethnic/cultural identity salience and con¯ict styles in four US ethnic groups p Stella Ting-Toomey a,*, Kimberlie K. Yee-Jung a, Robin B. Shapiro a, Wintilo Garcia a, Trina J. Wright a, John G. Oetzel b a California State University at Fullerton, USA b University of New Mexico, USA Abstract This study examined the in¯uence of ethnic background, ethnic identity, and cultural identity on con¯ict styles among African Americans, Asian Americans, European Americans, and Latino(a) Americans. Panethnic factor analysis yielded four dimensions of ethnic identity: ethnic belonging, fringe, intergroup interaction, and assimilation. A secondorder factor analysis yielded two clear identity dimensions: ethnic identity salience and cultural identity salience. In addition, panethnic factor analysis yielded seven con¯ict management styles: integrating, compromising, dominating, avoiding, neglecting, emotional expression, and third party. Some of the major ®ndings of the study are: (1) African Americans have a stronger ethnic identity and a weaker cultural identity than the other ethnic groups; (2) European Americans have a weaker ethnic identity than the other groups; (3) Latino(a) Americans and Asian Americans use avoiding and third party con¯ict styles more than African Americans, and, Asian Americans use avoiding con¯ict style more than European Americans; (4) Individuals with a strong cultural identity (i.e., identifying with the larger US culture) use integrating, compromising, and emotionally expressive con¯ict styles more than individuals with a weak cultural identity; (5) Individuals with a strong ethnic identity (i.e., identifying with their ethnic memberships) use integrating An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Annual Conference of the Speech Communication Association, New Orleans, Louisiana, November, 1994. We want to thank Bill Gudykunst and the anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments on an earlier version of this paper. * Corresponding author. Department of Speech Communication, California State University at Fullerton, Fullerton, CA 92834, USA. Fax: +1-714-278-3377. p 0147-1767/99/$ - see front matter # 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S 0 1 4 7 - 1 7 6 7 ( 9 9 ) 0 0 0 2 3 - 1 48 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 con¯ict style more than individuals with a weak ethnic identity; (6) bicultural, assimilated, and traditional-oriented groups use integrating and compromising con¯ict styles more than the marginal group, and the marginal group uses third party help more than the other three groups. # 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: ethnic identity; cultural identity; con¯ict styles Introduction Con¯ict is a pervasive human phenomenon that penetrates all forms of social relationships in all ethnic and cultural groups. From intergroup con¯ict to interpersonal friction, con¯ict is often an intrinsic part of the human relating process. Con¯ict itself, however, is not necessarily a negative or positive phenomenon. It is how we manage con¯ict that can lead to constructive or destructive outcomes. Con¯ict is de®ned in this study as an intense disagreement process between a minimum of two interdependent parties when they perceive incompatible interests, viewpoints, processes, and/or goals in an interaction episode (Ting-Toomey, 1994a, 1994b). While there are many factors that can contribute to an escalatory con¯ict episode, con¯ict style is viewed as one of the critical factors that can exacerbate an already intense con¯ict situation. Con¯ict style refers to patterned responses to con¯ict in a variety of situations. Con¯ict interaction style is learned within the primary socialization process of one's cultural or ethnic group. Individuals learn the norms and scripts of appropriate con¯ict conduct and eective con¯ict behavior in their ethnic and cultural environment. Unfortunately, while there has been a blossoming interest concerning the study of interpersonal and intercultural con¯ict communication in the past ®ve years (e.g., Cahn, 1992, 1994; Leung, Au, Fernandez-Dols, & Iwawaki, 1992; Trubisky, Ting-Toomey, & Lin, 1991), there is a paucity of research addressing the critical role of ethnic identity and its eect on con¯ict styles. Interpersonal con¯ict researchers in the US tend to focus their attention in uncovering con¯ict norms and rules of the European American group to the neglect of practices by many non-European American communities. Intercultural researchers, on the other hand, tend to focus their attention in identifying dierences of con¯ict patterns between US and non-US cultures to the neglect of examining ethnic variability of con¯ict styles within the US sample. With the changing demographic trends of the US population within the next 50 years (when one in every three US Americans will be a person of non-white heritage, Thornton, 1992), it is vital that theory-development in the area of ethnic identity and con¯ict style should be as inclusive and as ethnocentric-free as possible. As Yinger (1994) notes: ``by the middle of the twenty-®rst century, if the present trend continues, the United States will be a truly global society, with slightly over half of primarily European ancestry and nearly half of Latino, S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 49 African, Asian, and Native American background. How the United States develops as a multi-ethnic society will be of critical importance . . . for its own quality of life'' (p. 35). Understanding the relationship between ethnic identity salience and con¯ict management styles in African, Asian, European, and Latino(a) American groups will help us to understand con¯ict variations across groups. Understanding such stylistic variations can serve as a ®rst step in the constructive management of intraethnic and interethnic con¯icts. Towards this end, this project tests the eects of ethnic background, ethnic identity, and cultural identity on con¯ict styles among four US membership groups. This study is concerned with the general phenomenon of ethnic identity as a construct with composite elements that are relevant to dierent ethnic groups in the larger US culture. Identifying common ethnic/cultural identity dimensions and decoding particular con¯ict style factors will oer us a clearer, comparative picture of identity issues and con¯ict style preferences across various US groups. Understanding the complex relationships among ethnic background, ethnic identity, and cultural identity on dierent con¯ict styles will help us to deal with con¯ict modes more adaptively in a diverse range of ethnic contact situations. Ethnic identity Ethnic identity is a complex, multidimensional construct. It is the subject of continuous fascination that is debated and argued about by researchers from diverse disciplines (see, for example, Alba, 1990; De Vos & Suarez-Orozco, 1990; Espiritu, 1992; Hecht, Sedano, & Ribeau, 1993; Keefe, 1992; Mirande & Tanno, 1993; Phinney, 1990; Roosen, 1989; Waters, 1990). Ethnic identity is an elastic concept that is often shaped and molded by the researcher's lens of emphasis. There are two important issues that are central to the study of ethnic identity: ethnic identity salience and ethnic identity content. While ethnic identity salience refers to the degree of importance of ethnic identity to the individuals, ethnic identity content refers to the ethnic values that individuals subscribe to and practice. These two issues are central to understanding the role of ethnic identity on con¯ict management styles within and across the four US ethnic groups. Ethnic/cultural identity salience Drawing from social identity theory (Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel & Turner, 1979) and acculturation theory (Berry, Kim, Power, Young, & Bujaki, 1989), ethnic identity is viewed as a multidimensional construct that includes issues of group membership, self-image, ethnic aliation and larger cultural aliation, and ingroup and intergroup attitudes. More speci®cally, ethnic identity is conceptualized in this paper as a composite of attitudes, feelings, and perceptions of the degree of aliation and belonging towards one's own ethnic group and/or the larger culture. It also embodies favorable and unfavorable attitudes toward ingroup/outgroup interactions. Ethnic identity salience involves the extent to which 50 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 individuals hold their ethnicity to be of importance. Cultural identity salience, on the other hand, involves the extent to which individuals hold their larger culture to be of importance. Macro- to mico-level factors such as permeable/impermeable group boundaries, positive or negative intergroup comparisons, identity distinctiveness issues, and socialization processes shape individuals' ethnic membership preferences (Berry et al., 1989; Brewer, 1991, 1996; Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Ting-Toomey, 1993). Phinney (1991), after reviewing extensive studies on ethnic identity of multiple ethnic groups, argues that ethnic identity salience varies on a continuum from strong to weak. She contends that persons with strong ethnic identity salience selfidentify as ``group members, evaluate their group positively, prefer or are comfortable with their group membership, are interested in, knowledgeable about, and committed to the group, and are involved in ethnic practices'' (p. 194). Thus, strong ethnic identity salience can be manifested in gestalt ways. Conversely, weak ethnic identity is apparent when individuals have ``little ethnic interest, knowledge, commitment, or involvement, and negative evaluation of the group and of one's membership in the group'' (p. 194). Individuals who experience weak ethnic identity salience are less likely to embody ethnic values associated with their ethnic group memberships. Individuals may be situated at any point between the extremes of strong and weak ethnic identity salience. Likewise, individuals can identify strongly or weakly with the larger culture. Persons with strong cultural identities hold the larger culture as important to their self-images, and persons with weak cultural identities are less likely to internalize the larger culture as important to their self-images. More speci®cally, from the acculturation typological perspective (Berry, Kim, & Boski, 1987; Berry et al., 1989), ethnic identity salience can be viewed as a fourfold model that emphasizes an individual's orientation towards issues of ethnic identity maintenance and larger cultural identity maintenance. According to Berry et al. (1987), individuals who tend to favor ethnic tradition maintenance and at the same time attach low signi®cance to the values and norms of the larger culture practice the traditional-oriented (i.e., ``separation'') option. Individuals who favor ethnic tradition maintenance and at the same time express movement to become an integral part of the larger society practice the bicultural (i.e., ``integration'') option. Individuals who attach low signi®cance to their ethnic values and norms and at the same time tend to view themselves as members of the larger society practice the assimilation option. Finally, when individuals lose ethnic/psychological contact with both their ethnic group and the larger society and experience feelings of alienation, they practice the marginalization option. For example, a second-generation Asian American can commit to one of the following four ethnic identity salience categories: Asian primarily, American primarily, both, or neither. From this fourfold model, an interaction eect is posited between ethnic identity maintenance and cultural identity maintenance in a pluralistic society. Cultural identity maintenance refers to the degree of membership aliation with the larger culture. Ethnic identity maintenance refers to the degree of membership aliation with one's speci®c ethnic group. Finally, Phinney (1989, S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 51 1992) has identi®ed four common themes of ethnic identity that are relevant to dierent ethnic group members at any stage of racial/ethnic identity development: ethnic belonging (i.e., positive aliation with one's own ethnic group), ethnic identity achievement (i.e., active search for ethnic identity knowledge), ethnic practices (i.e., participation in ethnic activities), and other-group orientation (i.e., attitudes and feelings toward members of other ethnic groups). Overall, it appears that the study of ethnic identity salience has both ethnicgeneral and ethnic-speci®c elements. As a general phenomenon, ethnic identity in a pluralistic society is a composite construct that involves ethnic group belonging, ingroup/outgroup attitudes, and the larger cultural identity issues. Ethnic identity is basically concerned with the intergroup boundary maintenance theme. As a speci®c phenomenon, ethnic identity encompasses the unique history, common ancestral descent, shared fate, and shared ethnic traditions and values. Thus, in order to understand the role of ethnic identity salience in a pluralistic society, both ethnic identity maintenance and the larger cultural identity maintenance should be taken into consideration. In order to gain a more complete understanding of this in¯uence of ethnic/cultural identity, the following section describes the content of ethnic identity. Content of ethnic/cultural identity To understand dierences and similarities in con¯ict styles across dierent ethnic or cultural groups, it is necessary ®rst to have a perspective to explain why and how ethnic/cultural groups are dierent or similar. An ethnic variability perspective refers to how ethnic groups vary on a continuum of variations in accordance to some basic dimensions or core value characteristics. While there are many dimensions in which ethnic or cultural groups dier, one dimension that has received consistent attention from intercultural researchers around the world is individualism-collectivism. Countless intercultural and ethnic studies (see, for example, Asante & Asante, 1990; Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, & Tipton, 1985; Bond, 1991; Casas & Pytluk, 1995; Davis, 1990; Gudykunst & TingToomey, 1988; Hecht, Collier, & Ribeau, 1993; Hofstede, 1991; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Sodowsky, Kwan, & Pannu, 1995; Triandis, 1995) have provided theoretical and empirical evidence that the value dimensions of individualism and collectivism are pervasive in a wide range of ethnic and cultural communities. Value characteristics serve as the content of ethnicity and culture that guide individuals' behaviors and practices. The value dimensions of individualism and collectivism, as existing on a continuum of value tendency dierences, can be used as a beginning point to understand some of the basic ethnic/cultural communication dierences. Essentially, individualism refers to the broad value tendencies of a group in emphasizing the importance of individual identity over group identity, individual rights over group rights, and individual interests over relational or group interests. In comparison, collectivism refers to the broad value tendencies of a group in emphasizing the importance of the ``we'' identity over the ``I'' identity, group 52 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 obligations over individual rights, and ingroup-oriented needs over individual wants and desires (Ting-Toomey, 1988; Triandis, 1995). More speci®cally, examining the individualistic and collectivistic tendencies of various ethnic groups provides one means for examining the value content domains of ethnicity. Triandis (1995) states that an understanding of individualistic and collectivistic values can lead us to a better understanding of the social behavior of diverse people within and across ethnic/cultural groups. Schwartz and Bilsky (1990) describe values as re¯ecting the goals, needs, and motivations of an individual or cultural group. Individualistic values include personal achievement, self-direction, and the open expressions of individual goal-directed interests. Comparatively, collectivistic values include relational-oriented values, ingroup harmony, conformity, and the tactful expressions of mutual-oriented interests. Hofstede (1991) reports the results of the individualistic tendencies of ®fty countries worldwide. Each nation is assigned an individualism score, with the United States (with the sample consisting primarily of European American respondents) having the highest score, and Guatemala the lowest (and hence, the highest in collectivism). Hofstede's (1991) study indicates that many African countries were found to be highly collectivistic (e.g., East Africa, West Africa). Further, Jamaica, a country that is largely populated by descendants of Africans, also was found to be collectivistic. In addition, Asian countries were found to be highly collectivistic (e.g., Indonesia, Singapore, South Korea, Taiwan, and Thailand). With a few exceptions (e.g., Greece), European countries were generally found to be individualistic (e.g., Belgium, Denmark, Sweden, France, Great Britain). Finally, Latin countries were found by Hofstede's (1991) study to be collectivistic (e.g., Colombia, Ecuador, Guatemala, Mexico, Portugal, Spain, and Venezuela). With knowledge of the individualism-collectivism scores of the originating countries, it can therefore be inferred that African Americans, Asian Americans, and Latino(a) Americans who strongly identify with their traditional ethnic values and norms would tend to be collectivistic, while European Americans who strongly identify with their European values and norms would tend to be individualistic. As indicated in the prior discussion of ethnic identity salience, it is essential to consider the eect of the larger culture in shaping the cognition, emotions, and behaviors of dierent ethnic groups within the US. The high individualism score of the United States (Hofstede, 1991; Triandis, 1995) can be of in¯uence on those (European Americans as well as African Americans, Asian Americans, and Latino(a) Americans) who identify themselves primarily as ``Americans''. We can infer that individuals who have strong cultural identity salience would tend to identify with the values and norms of the larger culture. Individuals who have weak cultural identity salience would tend to identify less with the values and norms of the larger culture. Individuals who subscribe to the norms of the larger culture would tend to engage in more individualistic-oriented practices. Comparatively, individuals who subscribe weakly to the values of the larger US culture would tend to engage in less individualistic, and perhaps more collectivistic-oriented practices. S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 53 Since we do not know the precise relationship among ethnic background (i.e., the four broad ethnic categories of African, Asian, European, and Latino(a) Americans), cultural identity (i.e., identifying with the larger US culture), and ethnic identity (i.e., identifying with one's speci®c ethnic group), the ®rst research question in this study asks: RQ1 What is the eect of ethnic background on cultural identity and ethnic identity? Ethnic/cultural identity salience and con¯ict styles Con¯ict styles: general issues Ting-Toomey (1994a) de®nes con¯ict as ``the perceived and/or actual incompatibility of values, expectations, processes, or outcomes between two or more parties . . . over substantive and/or relational issues'' (p. 360). Con¯ict style is simply individuals' characteristic mode of managing con¯ict in a variety of interaction episodes (Ting-Toomey et al., 1991). Sternberg and Soriano (1984) conclude that ``individuals do have more- and less-preferred styles of con¯ict resolution, and that these styles are both generalizable across content domains and related to other measures of personal dispositions'' (p. 126). According to past research (Sternberg & Dobson, 1987; Sternberg & Soriano, 1984), preferred and manifested con¯ict communication styles are fairly stable across time. The present study focuses on patterned con¯ict styles with acquaintances from the same ethnic group. It can be argued that con¯ict behaviors can be privately negotiated in close relationships (e.g., intimate friends). The relational partners can employ their relational knowledge to adapt their con¯ict styles toward each other. Moreover, relational commitment may outweigh con¯ict style preference issue. However, in acquaintance con¯icts (e.g., neighborhood con¯icts), con¯ict parties would be more likely to rely on habituated modes of con¯ict to deal with the problematic issue. In acquaintance con¯icts, when personal knowledge is super®cial and relational stake is low, individuals would tend to engage in their habituated ways of responding to con¯icts more so than in intimate relationship con¯icts. In terms of understanding speci®c con¯ict styles, Rahim's (1983, 1992) conceptualization of con¯ict styles are employed for this study because of its compatibility with the ethnic variability dimension of individualism-collectivism. Furthermore, his con¯ict model is also compatible with Ting-Toomey's (1985, 1988) self-face and other-face dimensions as proposed in her con¯ict facenegotiation theory. For example, Rahim (1992) bases his classi®cation of con¯ict styles on the two conceptual dimensions of concern for self and concern for others. The ®rst dimension illustrates the degree (high or low) to which a person 54 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 seeks to satisfy her/his own interest or own face need. The second dimension represents the degree (high or low) to which a person desires to incorporate the other's con¯ict interest. The two dimensions combine for ®ve styles of handling interpersonal con¯ict: integrating, compromising, dominating, obliging, and avoiding. Brie¯y, the integrating style re¯ects a need for solution closure in con¯ict and involves high concern for self and high concern for other in con¯ict negotiation. Integrating style attempts to merge both self-interest goal and otherinterest goal via open channels of con¯ict negotiation. The compromising style involves a give-and-take concession approach in order to reach a mid-point agreement concerning the con¯ict issue. It is in the intermediate position of high concern for self and other. The dominating style, in turn, is characterized by an individual's need to control or dominate the con¯ict situation. It emphasizes con¯ict tactics that push for one's own position or goal at the expense of the other person's con¯ict interest. The obliging style is characterized by a high concern for the other person's con¯ict interest above and beyond one's own con¯ict interest. It emphasizes accommodating the need of the other person at the expense of self's interest. Finally, the avoiding style involves evading the con¯ict topic, the con¯ict party, or the con¯ict situation altogether. In linking Ting-Toomey's (1988) con¯ict face-negotiation theory with Rahim's (1992) styles of con¯ict management (i.e., integrating, compromising, dominating, obliging, and avoiding), certain observations can be made. Her theory proposes that members who subscribe to individualistic values tend to use direct modes of con¯ict management, such as integrating, compromising, and dominating/ controlling styles. Comparatively, members who subscribe to collectivistic, groupbased values tend to use indirect modes of con¯ict management, such as obliging/ accommodating style and avoidance style. On a broad level, Rahim's (1983, 1992) ®ve con¯ict styles serve as a useful start for this study and are compatible with Ting-Toomey's (1988) face-negotiation theory. However, on a speci®c level of application, Rahim's con¯ict approach is re¯ective of an individualistic, Western interpretation of what constitutes appropriate and eective con¯ict communication. In particular, obliging and avoiding styles often take on a Western slant of being negatively disengaged (i.e., ``placating'' or ``¯ight'' from the con¯ict scene). However, obliging and avoiding con¯ict styles are not necessarily perceived as negative by many Asian and Latin ethnic groups. These two styles are typically employed by collectivists to maintain mutual-face interests (TingToomey, 1988, 1999). Con¯ict styles: speci®c issues More speci®cally, for example, for individuals who subscribe to traditional Asian values, the moral philosophy of Confucianism guides their interpersonal behaviors and practices. The philosophy of Confucianism emphasizes harmonious interpersonal relationships and the concept of proper facework negotiation in interpersonal relationships. Obliging the needs of the other person or avoiding the con¯ict situation altogether is one way to ``give face'' and also ``save face'' for S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 55 both con¯ict parties. Thus, from the traditional-oriented Asian perspective, obliging and avoiding styles do not necessarily convey the negative connotations of being passive or elusive. The empirical work of Ting-Toomey et al. (1991) and Trubisky et al. (1991) provided some evidence that Asian samples (i.e., Chinese and Taiwanese groups) tend to use higher degrees of obliging and avoiding con¯ict styles than European Americans in dealing with acquaintance con¯icts. In addition, European Americans tend to use a higher degree of dominating con¯ict style than Asian samples. Leung et al.'s (1992) work also provided some evidence that Asians tend to use avoidance and third-party to deal with con¯ict issues, while European Americans tend to use upfront, solution-oriented style (i.e., integrating and compromising) in dealing with con¯ict problems. These researchers, however, had only examined cross-national dierences of con¯ict styles, and did not deal with ethnic variation issues in con¯ict management behaviors. In the context of traditional Latino(a) Americans' con¯ict practices, tactfulness and consideration of others' feelings are considered to be important norms in interpersonal confrontation situations (Casas & Pytluk, 1995; Garcia, 1996, Padilla, 1981). Tactfulness and consideration of others' feelings are conveyed through the use of obliging and avoiding con¯ict styles. In commenting on the cultural values and norms of Mexican Americans, for example, Locke (1992) observes that: ``Whereas members of the dominant culture of the United States are taught to value openness, frankness, and directness, the traditional MexicanAmerican approach requires the use of much diplomacy and tact when communicating with another individual. Concern and respect for the feelings of others dictate that a screen be provided behind which an individual may preserve dignity'' (p. 140). While Kagan, Knight, and Martinez-Romero's (1982) work provided empirical support for the use of avoiding con¯ict mode in Mexican Americans, Collier's (1991) interpretive work pointed to the importance of preserving relational harmony in con¯icts (in close friendship con¯icts) among Mexican Americans. The researchers, however, did not make distinctions between levels of ethnic/cultural identi®cation in the Mexican American respondents, and thereby, overlook the role of ethnic identity salience within groups. In terms of African Americans' con¯ict styles, Kochman (1981) notes that in a con¯ict situation, African Americans and European Americans are divided not only over content but also over the con¯ict engagement process. According to Kochman (1981), the ``Black mode'' of con¯ict is ``high-keyed: animated, interpersonal, and confrontational'', comparatively, the ``White mode'' of con¯ict is relatively ``low-keyed: dispassionate, impersonal and non-challenging'' (p. 18). While African Americans tend to prefer emotionally expressive and involving modes of con¯ict management, European Americans tend to engage in emotionally-restrained, factual con¯ict discussions. The explanation for the African American con¯ict modes can be explained by many factors. Two such factors can include the importance of oral artistry of traditional African practices and the ethnic socialization experiences of African Americans in the larger US society. 56 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 According to Boadu (1990), the oral artistry in the traditional African society serves three functions: ritual, entertainment, and education. The pedagogical role of the oral artist in the traditional African society ``is illustrated by the art of storytelling. An elderly person tells stories to the young ones around the courtyard in the moonlight. The storyteller performs not for any ®nancial gain but for other reasons Ð artistic commitment, for teaching the moral values of their society, and to exposing them to the sociocultural background of their society'' (p. 85). Oral artistry, in traditional African society, has an ``imaginative quality and relies on a play of the mind and spontaneity of composition'' (Boadu, 1990, p. 84). Spontaneous aective expressions and rich storytelling are highly valued communication practices in traditional African society. Additionally, the ethnic socialization experiences of African Americans within the larger US society may also contribute to their emotionally expressive con¯ict style. As Locke (1992) comments that ``future successes for their sons [and daughters] hinges on an ability to be alternatively assertive and acquiescent . . . the environment of African-American children is an ambiguous and marginal one in which they live simultaneously in two worlds Ð the African-American world and the world of the dominant culture'' (p. 21). Phinney and Chavira (1995), in investigating ethnic socialization by parents of ethnic minority adolescents, found that African American parents reported more frequent discussion of prejudice and discrimination issues with their daughter/son than Japanese American and Mexican American parents. Ting-Toomey (1986), in testing Kochman's (1981, 1986) thesis on ethnic con¯ict style, found that African American females tend to use more emotionally expressive con¯ict style (i.e., in close relationship con¯icts) than African American males or European American males and females. Overall, it appears that while African Americans prefer emotionally engaged con¯ict styles in dealing with dierences, European Americans prefer factual inductive modes in dealing with con¯ict problems. In sum, while past studies have examined con¯ict styles on either the crosscultural national level or racial dierence level, the objective of this study intends to understand the intricate relationships among ethnic background, ethnic identity salience, and cultural identity salience on various con¯ict styles in four US ethnic groups. The remaining four research questions guide the analysis of the results of this study: RQ2 RQ3 RQ4 RQ5 What is the eect of ethnic backgound on the con¯ict styles of the four US ethnic groups? What is the eect of ethnic identity salience on the con¯ict styles of the four US ethnic groups? What is the eect of cultural identity salience on the con¯ict styles of the four US ethnic groups? What is the interaction eect of ethnic background, ethnic identity salience, and cultural identity salience on the con¯ict styles of the four US ethnic groups? S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 57 Method Respondents The overall sample size for the four ethnic groups contained 662 respondents. There were 238 males (36.1%) and 422 females (63.9%), and two missing values. There were 194 European American respondents, 135 African American respondents, 181 Asian American respondents, and 152 Latino(a) American respondents. The majority of the respondents were recruited from several mediumsized universities in the southern region of California. Other respondents included professionals from the southern part of California. The average age of the participants was 23.83 (SD=7.81). The average educational level of the respondents was at the level of a college sophomore. Overall, 41.9% of the respondents reported their con¯ict with classmates, 42.2% with coworkers, 4.7% with neighbors, and 11.1% marked ``other''. In the European American sample, there were 77 (39.9%) males and 116 (60.1%) females. The European American self-report ethnic background labels included 67.2% ``Europeans'', 6.8% ``Italian Americans'', 3.1% ``German Americans'', and others. In terms of generation, 7.7% of European Americans identi®ed themselves as ®rst generation immigrants, 11.9% as second generation, 19.6 as third generation, and 60.8% marked ``other''. In the African American sample, there were 41 (30.4%) males and 94 (69.6%) females. The African Americans self-report ethnic background or labels included 59% ``African Americans'', 23.9% ``Blacks'', 6% ``Afro-Americans'', and others. In terms of generation, 0.7% of African Americans identi®ed themselves as ®rst generation immigrants, 1.5% as second generation, 17.0% as third generation, 78.5% marked ``other''. In the Asian American sample, there were 64 (35.6%) males and 116 (64.4%) females. The Asian Americans self-report ethnic background or labels included 32.7% ``Chinese Americans'', 19.9% ``Vietnamese Americans'', 12.2% ``Asian Americans'', 8.3% ``Filipino Americans'', 8.3% ``Japanese Americans'', 6.4% ``Asian Indians'', 5.8% ``South East Asian Americans'', 5.1% ``Korean Americans'', and others. In terms of generation, 75.7% of Asian Americans identi®ed themselves as ®rst generation immigrants, 14.4% as second generation, 3.9% as third generation, and 1.1% marked ``other''. In the Latino(a) American sample, there were 56 (36.8%) males and 96 (63.2%) females. The Latino(a) Americans self-report ethnic background or labels included 35.8% ``Hispanics'', 20.4% ``Mexicans'', 16.8% ``Mexican Americans'', 12.4% ``Latino(a)s'', 7.3% ``Chicano(a)s'', and others. In terms of generation, 34.9% of Latino(a) Americans identi®ed themselves as ®rst generation immigrants, 19.1% as second generation, 24.3% as third generation, and 15.1% marked ``other''. Procedures The objective of the study was to examine the in¯uence of ethnic background, 58 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 ethnic identity salience, and cultural identity salience on con¯ict styles in four US ethnic groups. A questionnaire was developed to assess ethnic/cultural identity salience and con¯ict styles across dierent ethnic groups in the US. Independent (ethnic background, cultural identity, ethnic identity salience) and dependent variables (con¯ict styles) were measured through self-report survey data. The questionnaire was constructed by using items from various existing scales concerning ethnic/cultural identity and con¯ict styles. Additionally, some items were written speci®cally for this study to complement and tap dimensions not addressed by the instruments used. Many of the con¯ict questions were reworded to address the context of con¯ict style patterns among acquaintances. Each questionnaire took approximately 30±45 min to complete. In section I, participants were instructed that when ®lling out the con¯ict section, think of how they typically handle face-to-face con¯icts with acquaintances (e.g., classmates, neighbors, co-workers) who are from the same sex and same ethnic group as the respondents. Con¯ict was de®ned for them as any ``intense disagreement between two parties which involves incompatible goals, needs, or viewpoints''. After completing the ®rst section of the survey, the respondents were asked what kinds of acquaintances they have in mind in ®lling out the con¯ict responses. Four options were provided: classmates, co-workers, neighbors, and others. Those who indicated that they answered with anyone other than an acquaintance (e.g., family members, best friends) were excluded from this analysis. In section II, prior to completing the ethnic/cultural identity section of the questionnaire, respondents were instructed to think of the most important ethnic group which aects their everyday experience or behavior. A blank was provided for them to self-report their ethnic background label. Section II contained two parts: ethnic/cultural identity salience items and intergroup attitude items. After ®lling out the ethnic/cultural identity items, respondents ®lled out answers in the demographic section. Measurements of ethnic background, ethnic identity salience, cultural identity salience, and con¯ict styles Ethnic background was operationalized via self-report labels used by the respondents in section II of the survey. Ethnic and cultural identity salience items appeared in the second section of the questionnaire. The items were drawn from the various scales used in past research, and new items were generated for this study. Aspects of ethnic/cultural identity salience from the acculturation perspective (Berry et al., 1987, 1989), and Phinney's (1992) general ethnic identity salience were examined. More speci®cally, items were included and adapted from Phinney's (1992) Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM), Helms and Parham's (1993) Black Racial Identity Attitude Scale (RIAS-B), and Luhtanen and Crocker's (1992) Collective Self-Esteem scale. Additional items were also written for the purpose of this study. There were a total of 84 ethnic/cultural identity items included in the survey. The items were randomly distributed in ÿ.60 .15 .06 ÿ.16 ÿ.15 .67 ÿ.18 ÿ.18 .64 .36 .09 .05 (continued on next page) ÿ.06 .06 .01 .59 .09 ÿ.02 ÿ.13 ÿ.10 .15 ÿ.03 ÿ.07 ÿ.02 ÿ.08 .02 .06 ÿ.08 ÿ.04 ÿ.13 ÿ.05 ÿ.01 ÿ.02 ÿ.07 .03 ÿ.08 .04 ÿ.10 .03 .56 .26 .04 ÿ.06 ÿ.06 ÿ.52 .01 .20 ÿ.27 .32 .02 ÿ.02 .19 .28 .22 .01 ÿ.63 .32 .11 ÿ.25 .12 .11 ÿ.10 ÿ.72 ÿ.05 .55 .08 .65 .18 ÿ.24 .02 .63 .03 .55 .11 .09 .63 .71 .68 .65 ÿ.06 .65 .57 .07 .50 .03 .62 ÿ.04 ÿ.14 1. I have spent time trying to ®nd out more about my own ethnic group, such as history, traditions and customs. 4. I like meeting and getting to know people from ethnic groups other than my own. 5. I think a lot about how my life will be aected by my ethnic group membership. 7. I sometimes feel it would be better if dierent ethnic groups didn't try to mix together. 8. I am not very clear about the role of my ethnicity in my life. 9. I often spend time with people from ethnic groups other than my own. 13. In order to learn more about my ethnic background, I have often talked to other people about my ethnic group. 15. I do not try to become friends with people from other ethnic group(s). 16. I participate in cultural practices of my own ethnic group, such as special food, music, or customs. 17. I am involved in activities with people from other ethnic groups. 19. I enjoy being around people from ethnic groups other than my own. 23. I am increasing my involvement in activities with my ethnic group. 24. I feel an overwhelming attachment to being a member of my ethnic group. 25. I involve myself in causes that will help members of my ethnic group. 26. I am determined to ®nd my ethnic identity. 27. I often regret that I belong to the ethnic group I do. 28. I feel excitement in my own ethnic environment. 29. I ®nd myself thinking more about my ethnic group membership than when I was younger. 31. I generally do not feel comfortable being around members of other ethnic groups. 32. I am active in social clubs which include mostly members of my own ethnic group. 33. It is easy for me to get along with members of dierent ethnic groups. 36. I think a lot about how my life will be aected by my ethnic group membership. 39. I often feel lost about who I am as an ethnic being. 40. I believe that the best way for members of dierent ethnic groups to get along is to assimilate to the overall US culture. 41. I usually do not feel comfortable around members of my own ethnic group. 42. It is important for me to identify closely with the overall US culture. 43. I generally identify strongly with the overall US culture. 47. The ethnic group I belong to is an important re¯ection of who I am. .00 ÿ.57 .02 .50 ÿ.02 ÿ.67 ÿ.01 .54 ÿ.05 ÿ.61 ÿ.64 .08 .17 .13 .10 .20 .02 ÿ.08 .60 .29 ÿ.62 .12 ÿ.03 .02 Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Item Table 1 Ethnic identity factors S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 59 48. 49. 52. 55. 59. 61. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 73. 74. 76. 77. 79. 81. 82. 83. Items used to de®ne factors. I feel I do not have much to oer to the ethnic group I belong to. I often feel confused about which ethnic group I should identify myself with. The ethnic group I belong to is important to my sense of what kind of person I am. In general, belonging to my ethnic group is an important part of my self-image. I feel good about the ethnic group I belong to. I usually go by the values of the overall US culture. I feel very ``confused'' about my sense of ethnic membership. It is important for me to internalize the overall US cultural values. I feel comfortable identifying with both my ethnic heritage and the overall US culture. I often feel ``left out'' when others around me talk about ethnic identity issues. The overall US culture is an important re¯ection of who I am. I often feel ``suspended'' and ``lost'' as far as ethnic group membership is concerned. It is important for me to be accepted both by my ethnic group and the overall US culture. I feel like I live on the ``fringe'' in terms of my sense of ethnic group belongingness. The values of my own ethnic group are very compatible with that of the overall US culture. I do not spend much time with members of the other ethnic group(s). I feel unable to involve myself in activities with members of the other ethnic group(s). I frequently involve myself in activities with members of the other ethnic group(s). Sometimes I feel it would be better that my ethnic group did not mix with members of the other ethnic group(s). I often ®nd myself referring to members of the other ethnic group(s) in a negative way. I have many friends from the other ethnic group(s). I feel unable to involve myself comfortably in activities with members of the other ethnic group(s). I generally do not trust members of the other ethnic group(s). Item Table 1 (continued ) ÿ.16 ÿ.08 .62 .67 .41 ÿ.09 ÿ.09 .12 .17 ÿ.15 .06 ÿ.14 .31 .05 ÿ.16 .07 .04 ÿ.01 .18 .24 .05 .05 .19 ÿ.53 ÿ.67 .30 .33 .62 ÿ.17 ÿ.73 ÿ.24 .24 ÿ.62 ÿ.15 ÿ.72 ÿ.03 ÿ.65 .05 .06 ÿ.05 ÿ.13 ÿ.10 ÿ.11 ÿ.05 ÿ.12 ÿ.07 .10 .08 .13 .10 ÿ.10 ÿ.09 ÿ.02 ÿ.02 ÿ.24 .10 ÿ.03 .02 ÿ.06 .03 ÿ.00 .70 .66 ÿ.63 .65 .53 ÿ.63 .66 .54 .23 .10 .06 .06 .11 .67 .08 .60 .55 .11 .68 .10 .54 .15 .55 ÿ.01 .04 .05 ÿ.10 ÿ.10 .12 ÿ.00 ÿ.16 Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 60 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 61 section II and participants were asked to respond on a ®ve-point Likert-type scale, from 1=strongly disagree to 5=strongly agree. In measuring con¯ict styles, items from several scales and items written speci®cally for this study were generated. Rahim's (1983) ROCI±II (Rahim Organizational Con¯ict Inventory±II) scale was modi®ed and used to measure the following styles of con¯ict: integrating, compromising, dominating, avoiding, and obliging. Other con¯ict style items were also included for the purpose of tapping into a diverse range of ethnic con¯ict styles. In particular, items from the Disputing Process Instrument (DPI) (Morrill & Thomas, 1992), the Dissatisfaction in Friendship Instrument (DFI) (Healey & Bell, 1990), and the Aective Orientation Scale (AOS) (Booth-Butter®eld & Booth-Butter®eld, 1990) were adapted to capture the multifaceted factors of panethnic con¯ict characteristics. New con¯ict items (e.g., third party, neglect, and emotional expression) were also written to tap a wider range of ethnic con¯ict styles. Respondents were asked to respond on a scale 5-point Likert-type scale, from 1=very little extent, to 5=very great extent. Results The data were analyzed using principal components of factor analysis, multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), t-tests, and Tukey post hoc comparisons. The results are presented in two sections: factor analysis and MANOVA ®ndings. Factor analysis In exploring the dimensions of ethnic identity salience, factor analysis with varimax rotation was used to uncover the cross-ethnic dimensions of ethnic/ cultural identity. Several criteria were used to determine and interpret the factors. First, a .50 (with the exception of one con¯ict item which loaded at .49) and .20 criterion was used to decide if an item loaded on a factor (i.e., the item had to have a primary loading of at least .50 and its primary factor had to be .20 greater than all secondary factors). Second, for a factor to be considered, a minimum of ®ve items had to load on that factor. Ethnic/cultural identity The initial factor analysis performed for ethnic identity was limited to four factors. Based on Berry et al.'s (1989) acculturation typology, four identity types should emerge. Indeed, four clear factors emerged from the factor analysis. All four factors met the criteria for interpretation (henceforth, the Ethnic/Cultural Identity Dimensions Ð EID Scale; see Table 1). The ®rst factor was comprised of 15 items. The items tapped at feelings of ethnic belonging. As a result, this factor was labeled ``belonging''. These items together yielded an alpha of .91. for the panethnic sample. The alpha coecients 62 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .90, Asian Americans .89, European Americans .88, and Latino(a) Americans .90. The second factor consisted of 11 items. All items that loaded for this factor re¯ected a sense of marginalism or the feelings of being on the fringe of ethnic identity. Thus, this factor was labeled ``fringe''. The combination of these items yielded an alpha of .89. The alpha scores for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .90, Asian Americans .89, European Americans .84, and Latino(a) Americans .89. The third factor was made up of 14 items. These items re¯ected the desire or the lack of desire to interact with members of other ethnic groups. Consequently, this factor was labeled ``interaction''. The alpha for this factor was .89. The alpha scores for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .88, Asian Americans .87, European Americans .92, and Latino(a) Americans .90. The fourth factor contained nine items. Generally, all these items re¯ected primarily assimilation attitudes. As a result, the factor was labeled ``assimilation''. The resulting alpha for assimilation was .83 for the panethnic sample. The alpha scores for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .88, Asian Americans .76; European Americans .82, and Latino(a) Americans .76. In examining the four ethnic identity dimensions, it was determined that they could be dimensions of two overarching factors that measure strong and weak ethnic identity and strong and weak cultural identity. Items with positive and negative factor loadings were recoded to be consistent within each factor. A second order factor analysis was performed on the four ethnic identity factors (see Table 2). These four factors loaded on two distinct factors, with belong (.78) and fringe (.89) loading on factor one (which will henceforth be labeled as salience of ``ethnic identity''), and interaction (.87) and assimilation (.59) loading on factor two (which will be referred to as salience of ``cultural identity''). Salience of ethnic identity was dichotomized to form strong and weak ethnic identity categories using a median split. Salience of cultural identity was also dichotomized to form strong and weak cultural identity using a median split. The median for ethnic identity salience was 3.60. The median for cultural identity salience was 3.51. The overall results of the ®rst-order factor analysis yielded an initial four ethnic identity dimensions, namely, belonging, fringe, intergroup interaction, and assimilation. The second-order factor analysis produced two distinctive dimensions: ethnic identity salience and cultural identity salience. Table 2 Second-order factor analysis for ethnic identity Rotated Factor Matrix Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor Factor Factor Factor .78 .89 .15 ÿ.25 ÿ.24 .12 .87 .59 1: 2: 3: 4: Belong Fringe Interact Assimilate Items used to de®ne factors. 1. I would attempt to avoid being ``put on the spot'' and try to keep my con¯ict with the other person to myself. 3. I would try to stay away from disagreement with the other person. 6. I would give some to get some in order to reach a compromise. 8. I would use my in¯uence to get my ideas accepted. 9. I would try to ®nd a middle course to resolve an impasse. 10. I would use my authority to make a decision in my favor. 12. I would win some and lose some so that a compromise could be reached. 13. I would avoid an encounter with the other person. 14. I would argue my case with the other person to show the merits of my position. 17. I would try to keep my disagreement with the other person to myself in order to avoid hard feelings. 18. I would use my expertise to make a decision in my favor. 20. I would usually propose a middle ground for breaking deadlocks. 21. I would try to avoid unpleasant exchanges with the other person. 22. I am generally ®rm in pursuing my side of the issue. 25. I would generally avoid an argument with the other person. 26. I would sometimes use my power to win a competitive situation. 28. I would use a ``give and take'' so that a compromise could be made. 29. I would sit down with the other person to negotiate a resolution to his/ her objectionable behavior. 30. I would generally ask a third person to intervene in our dispute and settle it for us. 33. I would be emotionally expressive in the con¯ict situation. 34. I would ask a third party to make a decision about how to settle the dispute between myself and the other person. 36. I would rely on a third person to negotiate a resolution to the con¯ict. Item Table 3 Panethnic con¯ict factors ÿ.09 ÿ.09 .20 ÿ.01 .10 ÿ.09 .28 ÿ.18 .22 ÿ.03 .10 .22 .11 .20 ÿ.04 .02 .34 .53 ÿ.00 .11 .01 ÿ.02 .52 .03 ÿ.07 .06 ÿ.05 .14 .55 ÿ.14 .59 .01 .13 .53 ÿ.21 .61 .01 .07 ÿ.04 .10 ÿ.08 .16 .09 Factor 2 .50 Factor 1 .21 .09 .18 .79 .09 .79 .10 .79 .09 ÿ.05 ÿ.23 .05 ÿ.21 .20 ÿ.08 .06 ÿ.08 ÿ.06 .10 ÿ.20 ÿ.07 .03 ÿ.05 .23 ÿ.08 ÿ.24 Factor 4 ÿ.03 ÿ.01 ÿ.06 ÿ.12 ÿ.01 .07 .05 .11 .02 .11 ÿ.14 .03 .01 .08 ÿ.01 .12 ÿ.02 ÿ.01 Factor 3 .00 .00 .00 .01 .01 .59 .25 ÿ.02 .27 ÿ.02 .60 .25 .11 .07 .04 .14 .54 .09 .63 ÿ.03 .60 .17 .01 ÿ.03 .52 .05 ÿ.07 ÿ.01 .01 .00 .08 .08 .10 ÿ.03 ÿ.03 .68 .01 .09 .51 .05 .70 ÿ.05 .10 .06 .04 .11 .03 .11 .15 .06 .05 .08 .04 ÿ.00 Factor 7 .04 .02 .50 .07 .02 .64 .10 .65 ÿ.03 .06 Factor 6 .00 ÿ.05 (continued on next page) Factor 5 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 63 37. I would generally ``grin and bear it'' when the other person did something I did not like. 38. I would typically leave the other person alone. 40. I would ask a third person for advice in settling the dispute. 41. I would meet with the other person to see if we could work out a resolution to our con¯ict. 42. I would use my feelings to guide my con¯ict behaviors. 43. I would prefer the other person to be emotionally expressive with me in the con¯ict situation. 44. I would generally endure actions by the other person that I did not like. 46. I would integrate my viewpoints with the other person to achieve a joint decision about the con¯ict. 47. I would typically go through a third party to settle our con¯ict. 48. I would meet with the other person to bargain for a resolution to our con¯ict. 49. I would appeal to a person at a higher level to settle my con¯ict with the other person. 50. I would use my feelings to determine what I should do in the con¯ict situation. 51. I would work with the other person to reach a joint resolution to our con¯ict. 52. I would ask another person to help negotiate a disagreement with the other person about his/her behavior. 55. I would try to tolerate our disagreement and not make waves. 56. I would be patient and hope the other person would change his/her behavior. 58. I would use my feelings to determine whether to trust the other person. 59. I would usually bear my resentment in silence. 60. I would attempt to solve our problems by talking things over in a calm and polite manner. 61. I would say nothing and wait for things to get better. Item Table 3 (continued ) ÿ.19 .11 .04 ÿ.01 .74 .08 .11 .07 .59 ÿ.04 .55 .12 .08 .61 .02 .09 .20 .17 ÿ.00 .57 ÿ.21 .51 .00 .16 ÿ.08 .16 .08 .03 .16 .62 .62 .13 .64 .08 .65 .10 .60 .14 ÿ.11 .06 ÿ.13 .11 .06 .18 ÿ.01 .77 .15 .26 ÿ.02 .03 ÿ.19 ÿ.06 ÿ.04 .08 .16 .01 .03 .15 .13 ÿ.05 .01 .01 .10 ÿ.01 ÿ.21 .04 ÿ.04 .02 .67 .04 ÿ.07 .14 .56 .10 Factor 4 .53 .14 ÿ.13 .07 Factor 3 ÿ.05 Factor 2 .58 Factor 1 ÿ.05 .05 ÿ.02 .08 ÿ.04 ÿ.14 ÿ.06 .21 .00 ÿ.08 ÿ.01 .20 ÿ.00 .25 .04 .01 .16 .07 .16 .09 Factor 5 ÿ.06 .08 ÿ.01 .05 ÿ.03 ÿ.06 .05 ÿ.08 .08 .13 ÿ.01 .03 ÿ.08 ÿ.09 .14 ÿ.05 ÿ.06 ÿ.04 .07 ÿ.05 Factor 6 ÿ.04 .61 ÿ.11 ÿ.04 ÿ.05 ÿ.00 .07 .10 .74 ÿ.03 .01 .05 .11 .01 .75 .59 .08 .03 .15 ÿ.04 Factor 7 64 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 Items used to de®ne factors. 63. I would generally keep quiet and wait for things to improve. 64. When we discuss the problem I would refuse to cooperate. 65. I would listen to what my ``gut'' or ``heart'' says in the con¯ict situation. 66. I would try to get us to work together to settle our dierences. 67. Out of anger, I would say things to damage the other person's reputation. 68. I would say nothing and deal with the situation by adopting a strategy of forgive and forget. 70. I would make sure the other person realized that resolving our dierences was important. 71. I would hope that the situation would solve itself. 72. I would say nasty things about the other person to other people. 73. I would let the other person know that I did not want him/her to ever talk to me again. 74. I would usually let my anger be known in a con¯ict situation. 75. I would try to negotiate upfront a solution to our con¯ict. 76. I would allow things to cool o rather than taking any action. 78. I would tell the other person that there were problems and suggest that we work them out. 79. I would say and do things out of anger to make the other person feel bad. 80. While in the presence of the other person, I would act as though he/she did not exist. 81. I would tell the other person what was bothering me and ask for his/her opinions on the matter. 83. I would talk openly and honestly about our dierences. Item Table 3 (continued ) ÿ.05 .15 .09 .01 .07 .06 ÿ.05 .63 ÿ.17 .10 .61 ÿ.07 ÿ.23 ÿ.08 .04 .56 .02 .61 ÿ.20 ÿ.19 .56 .57 .02 ÿ.01 .52 ÿ.09 .53 .04 .05 ÿ.20 ÿ.14 .54 ÿ.16 ÿ.03 .17 ÿ.21 ÿ.15 .16 ÿ.08 .01 .01 ÿ.03 .08 .21 .09 .09 .24 ÿ.03 Factor 3 ÿ.18 ÿ.26 .11 Factor 2 .63 .06 .11 Factor 1 ÿ.07 ÿ.09 .65 .63 ÿ.13 ÿ.03 .03 .02 .13 .15 .08 .55 ÿ.03 ÿ.02 ÿ.23 ÿ.03 ÿ.02 ÿ.00 ÿ.12 .07 ÿ.09 .18 ÿ.06 .02 ÿ.18 .09 Factor 5 .23 .63 .65 ÿ.29 .18 ÿ.24 .63 .21 .49 .05 Factor 4 .05 ÿ.02 .11 .17 .13 .17 ÿ.08 .10 ÿ.04 .09 .11 .06 ÿ.18 ÿ.06 .15 ÿ.08 .10 .14 Factor 6 .10 .12 .05 .16 .29 .02 .01 .06 .02 .11 .03 .17 ÿ.05 .15 .13 ÿ.07 ÿ.09 .56 Factor 7 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 65 66 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 Con¯ict styles The analysis performed for con¯ict styles was limited to eight factors. An eight-factor solution was used because of Rahim's (1983) ®ve con¯ict style dimensions, plus the additional con¯ict factors (from various scales and newly written items) of neglect, third party help, and emotional expression. Seven out of the eight met both criteria for interpretation. These seven-factor con¯ict styles are, henceforth, labeled as the Con¯ict Style Dimensions (CSD) Scale (see Table 3). Reliability coecients (Cronbach's alpha) also were calculated for all measurement scales. The ®rst factor contained 17 items. All items that loaded on this factor re¯ected an emphasis on the avoiding con¯ict style. Thus, the ®rst factor was labeled ``avoidance''. A combination of the 17 items yielded an alpha of .88 for the panethnic sample. The alphas for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .86, Asian Americans .85, European Americans .92, and Latino(a) Americans .87. The second factor was composed of 12 items. All items that loaded for this factor re¯ected an emphasis on integrating. As a result, this second factor was labeled ``integrating''. The alpha result of these 12 items was .87. The alphas for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .86, Asian Americans .88, European Americans .89, and Latino(a) Americans .84. The third factor was comprised of seven items. All of these items re¯ected the preference of third party help in dealing with the con¯ict. Consequently, this dimension was termed ``third party''. The resulting alpha was .88. The alphas for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .85, Asian Americans .89, European Americans .90, and Latino(a) Americans .88. The fourth factor contained seven items. All items re¯ected the dimension of neglect (i.e., passive aggressive anger responses) con¯ict style. This factor was labeled ``neglect''. These items yielded an alpha of .83. The alphas for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .83, Asian Americans .79, European Americans .85, and Latino(a) Americans .82. The ®fth con¯ict style factor consisted of ®ve items. All items re¯ect the compromising style of con¯ict management. The items on the ``compromising'' factor yielded an alpha of .75. The alphas for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .80, Asian Americans .69, European Americans .80, and Latino(a) Americans .60. The sixth factor consisted of six items. All of these items re¯ected the dominating style of con¯ict, thus this factor was labelled ``dominating''. The resulting alpha for this factor was .73. The alphas for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .74., Asian Americans .79, European Americans .76, and Latino(a) Americans .73. The seventh factor was comprised of six items. Items re¯ected emotional expression (plus relying on emotions to guide con¯ict responses) during con¯ict situations. This factor was labelled ``emotion''. The alpha for this factor was .75 for the panethnic sample. The alphas for the speci®c ethnic groups were: African Americans .75, Asian Americans .73, European Americans .80, and Latino(a) Americans .71. Thus, according to the results of factor analysis, seven con¯ict style factors emerged across the four ethnic groups. S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 67 MANOVA Research question one The ®rst research question focused on the relationship between ethnic background and ethnic/cultural identity salience. The data were analyzed using multivariate analysis of variance. The independent variable was ethnic background (i.e., the broad ethnic categories) and the dependent variables were ethnic and cultural identity salience. Bartlett's test of sphericity (.47, 1 df, P = ns) indicated that multivariate analysis of variance was not warranted. Thus univariate tests were utilized. The univariate tests for both ethnic identity salience (F[3,587]=36.32, P < .001) and cultural identity salience (F[3,587]=22.17, P < .001) were signi®cant. Post hoc comparisons of the means using Tukey tests (P = .05) revealed that African Americans had a higher ethnic identity than the other ethnic groups and that European Americans had a lower ethnic identity than the other ethnic groups. Further, post hoc comparisons revealed that African Americans had a lower cultural identity than the other ethnic groups (see Table 4 for means and standard deviations). Research questions 2±5 The other four research questions focused on the in¯uence of ethnic background, ethnic/cultural identity salience on con¯ict styles. The data were analyzed using multivariate analysis of variance. The independent variables were ethnic background, ethnic identity salience, and cultural identity salience. The dependent variables were integrating, compromising, dominating, avoiding, neglecting, emotional expression, and third party con¯ict styles. Table 5 reports the means and standard deviations for the main eects. Bartlett's test of sphericity (496.31, 21 df, P < .001) indicated that multivariate analysis of variance was warranted. The multivariate main eect for ethnic background was signi®cant (Wilks' lambda=.93, F[21,1634]=1.87, P = .01). Two of the univariate tests were signi®cant: avoiding (F[3,575]=4.95, P < .01) and third party (F[3,575]=4.41, P < .01). Integrating (F[3,575]=1.11, P = ns), compromising, (F[3,575]=1.52, P = ns), neglecting (F[3,575]=2.40, P = ns), dominating (F[3,575]=.67, P = ns), and emotional expression (F[3,575]=.42, P = ns) were not signi®cant. Post hoc Table 4 Means and standard deviations for ethnic identity and cultural identity among the ethnic groups Ethnic group African American Asian American European American Latino(a) American Ethnic identity Cultural identity M SD M SD 3.99 3.67 3.29 3.66 .63 .59 .49 .61 3.16 3.57 3.56 3.50 .59 .42 .43 .50 68 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 Table 5 Means and standard deviations for con¯ict styles Main eect Ethnic background African American Asian American European American Latino(a) American Ethnic identity Strong Weak Cultural identity Strong Weak Ethnic background African American Asian American European American Latino(a) American Ethnic identity Strong Weak Cultural identity Strong Weak Integrating Compromising Dominating Avoiding M SD M SD M SD M SD 3.52 3.45 3.38 3.43 .68 .63 .67 .63 3.46 3.50 3.52 3.43 .74 .58 .61 .56 3.19 3.20 3.24 3.28 .73 .76 .69 .71 2.74 3.13 2.92 2.94 .62 .57 .70 .64 3.56 3.33 .67 .61 3.51 3.43 .61 .64 3.26 3.18 .75 .69 2.89 2.98 .69 .61 3.56 3.31 .66 .31 3.60 3.34 .63 .59 3.25 3.18 .74 .70 2.96 2.91 .69 .62 Neglecting Emotional expression Third party M SD M SD M SD 1.96 2.13 2.08 2.27 .77 .69 .75 .84 3.16 3.20 3.19 3.28 .75 .69 .70 .71 2.24 2.59 2.36 2.56 .83 .86 .83 .87 1.99 2.24 .76 .77 3.26 3.11 .73 .68 2.31 2.58 .90 .79 1.95 2.28 .73 .79 3.28 3.11 .71 .70 2.36 2.53 .85 .85 comparisons of the means with Tukey tests (P = .05) revealed that Latino(a) Americans and Asian Americans use the avoiding and third party styles more than African Americans. Also, Asian Americans use avoiding more than European Americans. The multivariate main eect for ethnic identity salience was also signi®cant (Wilks' lambda=.95, F[7,569]=4.32, P < .001). Three of the univariate eects were signi®cant: integrating (F[1,575]=9.92, P < .01), neglecting (F[1,575]=6.82, P < .01), and third party (F[1,575]=14.17, P < .001). Compromising (F[1,575]=.75, P = ns), avoiding (F[1,575]=.94, P = ns), dominating (F[1,575]=1.28, P = ns), and emotional expression (F[1,575]=2.19, P = ns) were not signi®cant. An examination of the means in Table 5 indicates that individuals with a strong ethnic identity use integrating style more and neglecting and third party styles less than individuals with a weak ethnic identity. The multivariate main eect for cultural identity salience was also signi®cant S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 69 (Wilks' lambda=.90, F[7,569]=8.78, P < .001). Four of the univariate tests were signi®cant: integrating (F[1,575]=26.79, P < .001), compromising (F[1,575]=22.18, P < .001), neglecting (F[1,575]=33.46, P < .001), and emotional expression (F[1,575]=4.72, P < .05). Avoiding, (F[1,575]=.54, P = ns), dominating (F[1,575]=.40, P = ns), and third party (F[1,575]=3.52, P = ns) were not signi®cant. An examination of the means in Table 5 indicates that individuals with a strong cultural identity use integrating, compromising, and emotionally expressive styles more and neglecting style less than individuals with a weak cultural identity. The multivariate interaction eect for ethnic background, ethnic identity salience, and cultural identity salience (Wilks' lambda=.95, F[21,1634]=1.42, P = ns) was not signi®cant. Likewise, the interaction eect for ethnic background and ethnic identity salience (Wilks' lambda=.95, F[21,1634]=1.28, P = ns) was not signi®cant. The multivariate interaction eect for ethnic identity salience and cultural identity salience was signi®cant (Wilks' lambda=.96 F[7,569]=3.70, P < .01). Three of the univariate eects were signi®cant: integrating (F[1,575]=5.15, P < .05), compromising (F[1,575]=12.35, P < .001), and third party (F[1,575]=8.58, P < .01). Avoiding (F[1,575]=1.84, P = ns), neglecting (F[1,575]=3.23, P = ns) dominating, (F[1,575]=.04, P = ns), and emotional expression (F[1,575]=.51, P = ns) were not signi®cant. Table 6 displays the means and standard deviations for the signi®cant univariate interaction eects of cultural and ethnic identity salience. T-tests and an examination of the means in Table 6 indicate that bicultural (t=ÿ6.76, df=362, P < .001), assimilated (t=ÿ5.56, df=329, P < .001), and traditional-oriented groups (t=ÿ4.97, df=329, P < .001) use the integrating con¯ict style more than the marginal group. Assimilated (t=ÿ5.67, df=329, P < .001), bicultural (t=ÿ4.91, df=362, P < .001), and traditional-oriented groups (t=ÿ2.57, df=329, P < .05) also use compromising con¯ict style more than the marginal group. Additionally, t-tests and examination of the means in Table 6 further indicate that the marginal group uses third party more than assimilated (t = 4.43, df=329, P < .001), bicultural (t = 4.53, df=362, P < .001), and traditional-oriented groups (t = 5.45, df=329, P < .001). Finally, t-tests and examination of the means in Table 6 indicate that assimilated (t=ÿ3.02, df=296, P < .01) and bicultural individuals (t=ÿ2.11, Table 6 Means and standard deviations for integrating, compromising, and third party in the cells for the cultural identity and ethnic identity interaction eect Acculturation type Bicultural Traditional-oriented Assimilated Marginalized Integrating Compromising Third Party M SD M SD M SD 3.61 3.49 3.53 3.17 .69 .63 .61 .56 3.57 3.43 3.65 3.26 .61 .59 .66 .58 2.35 2.26 2.37 2.75 .92 .87 .77 .77 70 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 df=329, P < .05) use compromising more than traditional-oriented individuals. All remaining t-tests were not signi®cant at the .05 level. The multivariate interaction eect for cultural identity salience and ethnic background was signi®cant (Wilks' lambda=.92, F[21,1634]=2.16, P < .01). Three of the univariate eects were signi®cant: compromising (F[3,575]=4.64, P < .01), avoiding (F[3,575]=4.04, P < .01), and neglecting (F[3,575]=3.78, P < .05). Integrating (F[3,575]=1.39, P = ns), dominating (F[3,575]=1.26, P = ns), third party (F[3,575]=.78, P = ns), emotional expression (F[3,575]=.14, P = ns) were not signi®cant. (F[3,575]=4.64, P < .01). Table 7 reports the means, Table 7 Means, standard deviations, and t-tests for compromising, avoiding, and neglect in the cells for the cultural identity by ethnic background interaction eecta Cell M SD N 1 2 3 Compromising 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 3.77 3.33 3.61 3.36 3.58 3.44 3.54 3.32 .74 .70 .59 .53 .60 .61 .62 .47 39 95 89 67 100 65 67 69 ÿ3.29 ÿ1.36 ÿ3.32 ÿ1.63 ÿ2.51 ÿ1.76 ÿ3.92 2.89 .30 2.64 1.00 1.95 ÿ.15 ÿ2.68 .35 ÿ1.74 ÿ.71 ÿ3.34 Avoiding 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 3.02 2.63 3.10 3.18 2.88 2.97 2.95 2.94 .69 .56 .64 .47 .71 .68 .70 .58 39 95 89 67 100 65 67 69 ÿ3.49 .57 1.35 ÿ1.06 ÿ.42 ÿ.55 ÿ.68 5.29 6.55 3.46 2.81 3.22 3.49 Neglecting 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1.66 2.08 2.08 2.19 1.98 2.22 1.89 2.63 .66 .78 .72 .66 .76 .73 .67 .82 39 95 89 67 100 65 67 69 2.95 3.12 3.99 2.34 3.96 1.75 6.69 .03 .94 ÿ.87 1.18 ÿ1.56 4.35 4 5 6 7 2.37 .76 1.76 ÿ.52 ÿ1.44 ÿ.40 ÿ3.01 .94 ÿ1.29 ÿ2.35 .86 ÿ1.21 ÿ2.14 ÿ1.37 ÿ1.59 ÿ2.06 ÿ2.96 ÿ2.21 ÿ2.61 .74 .57 .54 ÿ.16 ÿ.25 ÿ.07 .94 ÿ.92 1.19 ÿ1.65 4.43 ÿ1.80 .29 ÿ2.55 3.44 2.02 ÿ.77 5.25 ÿ2.69 3.01 5.67 a Note: 1=African American and strong cultural identity, 2=African American and weak cultural identity, 3=Asian American and strong cultural identity, 4=Asian American and weak cultural identity, 5=European American and strong cultural identity, 6=European American and weak cultural identity, 7=Latino(a) American and strong cultural identity, and 8=Latino(a) American and weak cultural identity; P < .05, P < .01. S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 71 standard deviations, and t-tests for the signi®cant univariate interaction eects of cultural identity and ethnic background. For the sake of parsimony, the ®ndings of the t-tests will only be summarized here. First, t-tests revealed a trend that individuals with a strong cultural identity used compromising more than individuals with a weak cultural identity. There were some exceptions (especially Latino(a) Americans with a strong cultural identity), but this was a consistent trend for the various ethnic groups. Second, t-tests revealed that African Americans with a weak cultural identity used avoiding less than all other groups. Further, Asian Americans with a weak cultural identity used avoiding more than most of the other groups (except Asian and African Americans with a strong cultural identity). Finally, Latino(a) Americans with a weak cultural identity used neglecting more than all other groups. Additionally, African Americans with a strong cultural identity used neglecting less than almost every other group (except Latino(a) Americans with a strong cultural identity). Discussion The purpose of this study was to investigate the in¯uence of ethnic background, ethnic identity salience, and cultural identity salience on con¯ict styles among African Americans, Asian Americans, European Americans, and Latino(a) Americans. Panethnic factor analysis yielded four dimensions of ethnic identity: ethnic belonging, fringe, intergroup interaction, and assimilation. A second-order factor analysis yielded two clear identity dimensions: ethnic identity salience and cultural identity salience. In addition, panethnic factor analysis yielded seven con¯ict management styles: integrating, compromising, dominating, avoiding, neglecting, emotional expression, and third party. Using the discovered factors, the in¯uence of ethnic identity salience, cultural identity salience, and ethnic background on con¯ict styles was determined. The major ®ndings of the study are: (1) African Americans have a stronger ethnic identity and a weaker cultural identity than the other ethnic groups; (2) European Americans have a weaker ethnic identity than the other ethnic groups; (3) Latino(a) Americans and Asian Americans use avoiding and third party con¯ict styles more than African Americans, and Asian Americans use avoiding con¯ict style more than European Americans; (4) Individuals with a strong cultural identity (i.e., identifying with the larger US culture) use integrating, compromising, and emotionally expressive con¯ict styles more and neglecting con¯ict styles less than individuals with a weak cultural identity; (5) Individuals with a strong ethnic identity (i.e., identifying with their ethnic memberships) use integrating con¯ict style more and neglecting and third party con¯ict styles less than individuals with a weak ethnic identity; (6) bicultural, assimilated, and traditional-oriented groups use integrating con¯ict style more than the marginal group and the marginal group uses third party help more than the other three groups; (7) assimilated, bicultural, and traditional-oriented individuals prefer compromising con¯ict style more than the marginal group, and, the assimilated 72 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 and bicultural groups prefer compromising style more than the traditionaloriented group; (8) Asian Americans with a weak cultural identity use avoiding con¯ict style more than other ethnic groups regardless of cultural identity, while African Americans with a strong cultural identity use avoiding con¯ict style less than other ethnic groups regardless of cultural identity; and (9) Latino(a) Americans with a weak cultural identity use neglecting con¯ict style more than other ethnic groups regardless of cultural identity, while African Americans with a strong cultural identity use neglecting con¯ict style less than other ethnic groups regardless of cultural identity. In this section, the ®ndings will be discussed in accordance to the ®ve research questions. Ethnic/cultural identity salience The ®rst research question focused on the relationship between ethnic background and ethnic/cultural identity salience. A factor analysis was conducted to determine the relevant factors for ethnic/cultural identity. The four ethnic/ cultural identity dimensions that were found in the initial factor analysis include: belonging, fringe, interaction, and assimilation. These dimensions consist of items from the various scales and also items written speci®cally for this study. All four dimensions yielded fairly high reliability coecients across groups. The dimension of ethnic belonging re¯ects the degree to which members feel attached and comfortable with their own ethnic group. This dimension is similar to Berry et al.'s (1989) traditional-oriented type. The dimension of fringe refers to the degree of clarity or confusion individuals have concerning their own ethnicity. This dimension re¯ects, to some extent, Berry et al.'s (1989) marginal-oriented type. Many newly-written items loaded on this factor. The third dimension emerged in the factor analysis was degree of perceived positive/negative intergroup interaction. This dimension captures Phinney's (1992) ``other-group orientation'' concept. Many items were also written by the authors of this study to tap dominant/ minority group contact attitudes. Finally, the dimension of assimilation reveals the degree to which individuals identify with the overall US culture. Many of the items that loaded on this dimension were written for the present study and adhere closely to Berry et al.'s (1989) assimilation type. In the second-order factor analysis, these four dimensions loaded on two distinct factors: salience of ethnic identity and salience of cultural identity. Belonging and fringe loaded on the ®rst factor and captured the essence of the salience of ethnic identity (i.e., strong ethnic membership belonging with ethnic identity clarity). Intergroup interaction and assimilation loaded on the second factor and re¯ected the essence of the salience of cultural identity (i.e., strong cultural membership belonging with positive dominant/minority group contact attitudes). Aspects of ethnic identity salience, according to this study include, but are not limited to, the extent to which people feel a sense of belongingness, involvement in ethnic activities, favorable ingroup attitudes, feel that the ethnic group is an important re¯ection of the self, and that there is a sense of ethnic identity clarity. Aspects of cultural identity salience were found to include a S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 73 following of the larger US cultural values and practices, a sense of assimilation to the larger US culture, positive/negative attitudes concerning intergroup contact, and a feeling that the larger US culture is a re¯ection of the self. Thus, the stronger the respondents identify with the ethnic identity factor, the more likely they exhibit ethnic-oriented values and favorable ingroup attitudes. The stronger the respondents identify with the cultural identity factor, the more strongly they hold assimilationist values and positive intergroup contact attitudes. These ethnic and cultural identity salience dimensions are consistent with past research (e.g., Chung & Ting-Toomey, 1994) in that the important aspects of ethnic identity that emerge deal with individuals' attitudes and feelings toward their own ingroups and also toward relations with outgroups. Overall, the results of factor analysis concerning the ethnic and cultural identity salience dimensions support contentions made in the past (e.g., Berry et al., 1989) regarding the importance of ethnic identity as well as that of cultural identity. It is clear that in a pluralistic society such as the United States, the in¯uence of ethnic group membership, as well as that of the overarching culture must be taken into consideration in the study of ethnic/cultural identity salience. The results of the ethnic identity factor analysis further corroborate the claim that was made earlier, that ethnic identity is a multidimensional phenomenon. In this study, four common ethnic identity factors (namely, ethnic belonging, fringe, intergroup interaction, and assimilation) among African Americans, Asian Americans, European Americans, and Latino(a) Americans were uncovered. In addition, these four speci®c factors collapsed into two core identity salience dimensions: ethnic identity salience and cultural identity salience. Once the factors were identi®ed, MANOVA was utilized to determine that African Americans strongly identi®ed with their own ethnic group, Latino(a) Americans and Asian Americans identi®ed with both their ethnic groups and the US culture, and European Americans identi®ed with primarily the larger US culture. These ®ndings support previous research on identity. For example, past studies (e.g., Alba, 1990; Waters, 1990) have consistently found that middle-class European Americans often practice ``symbolic ethnicity'' more so than any other indigenous or immigrant groups in the US. As Alba (1990) explains that ``symbolic ethnicity'' is an ethnicity of last resort: ``Because of widespread social mobility and intermarriage, ethnicity has become increasingly peripheral to the lives of many [European] Americans . . . But they do not relinquish ethnic identity entirely; rather, they adapt it to their current circumstances, selecting from an ethnic heritage a few symbolic elements that do not interfere with the need to intermix socially, turning ethnicity thereby into an occasionally practiced avocation'' (p. 29). For White ethnics, ``symbolic ethnicity'' satis®es both their personal and group membership desires. For ethnic minority members, however, prescribed ethnicity has both symbolic and substantive implications. As Waters (1990) concludes: ``The reality is that White ethnics have a lot more choice and room for maneuver than they themselves think they do. The situation is very dierent for members of racial minorities, whose lives are strongly in¯uenced by their race or national origin regardless of how much they may choose not to 74 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 identify themselves in ethnic or racial terms'' (pp. 157±158). Indeed, for many European Americans, ethnic and cultural identity involves voluntary choices rather than constraints. However, for many minority group members, ethnic and cultural identity maintenance issues involve the continuum of voluntary± involuntary dimension and the continuum of perceived intergroup acceptancerejection dimension. One reason for this is phenotype identity (Cox, 1994) and the resulting political and social issues of oppression, power struggles, and discrimination (Asante & Asante, 1990). As a result, many ethnic minorities identify strongly with their ethnic group. In a review of previous studies of ethnic identity, Cox (1994) found that signi®cant numbers of Latino(a), Asian, and African Americans identify strongly with their respective ethnic groups. Ethnic background, ethnic/cultural identity salience on con¯ict styles Research Questions 2±5 focused on the in¯uence of ethnic background and ethnic/cultural identity salience on con¯ict styles. A factor analysis was conducted to determine the relevant con¯ict styles. The results of the factor analysis indicated that con¯ict styles can be conceptualized in terms of seven factors across dierent ethnic groups in the larger US culture: integrating, compromising, dominating, avoiding, neglecting, emotion, and third-party. The con¯ict style factors that emerged were from a mixture of scales and items created speci®cally for this study. All con¯ict factors yielded acceptable reliability coecients across the four ethnic groups. However, the scales from the original instruments are not fully represented in the factors that emerged in this study. To illustrate, Rahim's (1983) ROCI±II consists of ®ve con¯ict style scales: integrating, compromising, dominating, avoiding, and obliging. The present study uncovered four of these ®ve con¯ict styles: integrating, compromising, dominating, and avoiding. Obliging dropped out from the factor analysis. It can be reasoned that for acquaintance con¯icts, respondents in this study did not perceive obliging con¯ict style as an option in managing their acquaintance disagreements. They appear to approach acquaintance con¯ict either directly through the use of dominating, integrating, and compromising con¯ict styles or exit strategies such as avoidance. The second research question focused on the in¯uence of ethnic background on con¯ict styles. MANOVA revealed that African Americans use avoiding and third party less than Latino(a) and Asian Americans, while European Americans use avoiding less than Asian Americans. These ®ndings are consistent with previous research on con¯ict styles used by these groups. For example, Kochman (1981) found that African Americans, during con¯ict, prefer to confront individuals directly. Hecht et al. (1993) explain that this style is considered appropriate in African American membership group because of concerns for expressiveness and individuality. Using a third party is one way to avoid direct confrontation with another, and thus, would not be consistent with African American interaction norms. In addition, European Americans also tend not to use avoiding style of con¯ict management because of individualism and self-face concerns (TingToomey, 1988). In contrast, Latino(a) and Asian Americans tend to be more S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 75 collectivistic than African and European Americans and, as a result, they prefer avoidance style in dealing with con¯icts (Marin & Marin, 1991; Ting-Toomey, 1988). The third research question explored the in¯uence of ethnic identity salience on con¯ict styles. MANOVA indicated that individuals with strong ethnic identities use integrating more and neglecting and third party less than individuals with weak ethnic identities. In other words, individuals with strong ethnic identities tend to be more direct and confrontive during con¯ict than individuals with weak ethnic identities. Perhaps individuals with strong ethnic identities have developed a strong sense of self in comparison to individuals with weak ethnic identities (TingToomey, 1993). With a secure sense of self, individuals become more con®dent and assertive in dealing with con¯ict issues. With an insecure sense of self, individuals become more hesitant and unsure of how they should approach con¯ict. Individuals with weak ethnic identities tend to prefer a neglecting style (i.e., venting anger in a passive aggressive way) of con¯ict or seek third party help in mediating the con¯ict situation. The fourth research question investigated the in¯uence of cultural identity on con¯ict styles. MANOVA revealed that individuals with strong cultural identities use integrating, compromising, and emotional expression more and neglecting less than individuals with weak cultural identities. These results provide some support for Ting-Toomey's (1988) propositions on con¯ict face-negotiation theory. Individuals with strong cultural identities tend to subscribe to the con¯ict norms of upfront, open communication because of the predominance of individualism in the US. In comparison, individuals with weak cultural identities tend to subscribe less to the low-context way of con¯ict practices. Individuals with weak cultural identities in this study tend to prefer the neglect mode of con¯ict more so than individuals with strong cultural identities. This fact can be due to their identity vulnerability nature of approaching con¯ict situations assertively and con®dently. The ®nal research question focused on the in¯uence of the interactions among ethnic background and ethnic/cultural identity salience. MANOVA and t-tests revealed two relevant interaction eects. First, there was an interaction eect on con¯ict styles for cultural identity and ethnic identity. The interaction of these two variables creates four categories consistent with Berry et al.'s (1987) acculturation typology: traditional-oriented (strong ethnic identity and weak cultural identity), bicultural (strong ethnic identity and strong cultural identity), assimilated (weak ethnic identity and strong cultural identity), and marginalized (weak ethnic identity and weak cultural identity). Two ®ndings were discovered: (1) bicultural, assimilated, and traditional-oriented groups use integrating con¯ict style more than the marginal group and the marginal group uses third party help more than the other three groups; (2) assimilated, bicultural, and traditional-oriented individuals prefer compromising con¯ict style more than the marginal group, and, the assimilated and bicultural groups prefer compromising style more than the traditional-oriented group. It appears that individuals with strong bicultural, assimilated, and ethnicoriented identities tend to work harder in incorporating self-interest and other- 76 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 interest in managing con¯ict substantive issues. They are more comfortable in using integrative con¯ict techniques to work out their con¯ict dierences in comparison to the encapsulated marginals. Encapsulated marginals refer to individuals who are ``bueted by con¯icting cultural loyalties and unable to construct a uni®ed identity'' (Bennett, 1993, p. 113). They are trapped by their sense of alienation or marginality and they ®nd it dicult to discuss issues openly and decisively. They do not appear to feel secure enough to directly ``voice'' during con¯ict situations. They tend to use neglecting or third-party help more as alternatives than do the other types of individuals. There was also an interaction eect on con¯ict styles for ethnic background and cultural identity salience. T-tests revealed that: (1) individuals with a strong cultural identity use compromising more than individuals with a weak cultural identity, (2) Asian Americans with a weak cultural identity use avoiding more than the other groups, while African Americans with a weak cultural identity use avoiding less than the other groups, and (3) Latino(a) Americans with a weak cultural identity use neglecting more than the other groups, while African Americans with a strong cultural identity use neglecting less than the other groups. It appears that individuals identifying strongly with the larger US culture will directly negotiate con¯ict because of low-context mode of con¯ict practices (Ting-Toomey, 1988). The second ®nding is consistent with previous con¯ict research about Asian and African Americans. Avoiding re¯ects collectivistic, highcontext values. Asian Americans with weak cultural identities tend to lean more towards group-oriented values. As a result, they would tend to use avoiding style to preserve mutual ``face'' and relational harmony in acquaintance con¯ict situations (Ting-Toomey, 1988). In comparison, African Americans with weak cultural identities are likely to continue their practice of oral self-expressions and emotional engagement in con¯ict. These communication values are consistent with traditional African interaction norms. The ®nal discovery concerns the use of neglect style by Latino(a) Americans with weak cultural identities. A key ethnic value in many of the Latin American groups is respeto or the maintenance of respect and dignity in social interactions (Marin & Marin, 1991). If identity respect (i.e., a face-related issue) is not properly maintained and coordinated, Latino(a) Americans would be more likely to respond with neglect than any other ethnic groups. Conclusions The speci®c results illustrate that ethnic background, ethnic identity salience, and cultural identity salience in¯uence the various con¯ict styles in dierent ways. Ethnic identity salience and cultural identity salience interact and produce some interesting ®ndings. More speci®cally, bicultural, assimilated, and traditionaloriented groups prefer the integrating con¯ict style in acquaintanceship con¯icts more than the marginal group. The marginal group uses third party help more than the other three groups. Assimilated, bicultural, and traditional-oriented S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 77 groups also prefer the compromising con¯ict style more than the marginal group. Lastly, the assimilated and bicultural groups prefer the compromising style more than the traditional-oriented group. One interesting and surprising ®nding was that a clear relationship was not established for the dominating con¯ict style. This may be because ethnic identity and cultural identity do not directly in¯uence dominating con¯ict mode. Instead, personality may be a more important factor. Speci®cally, self-construal, or one's conception of oneself as either independent or interdependent (Markus & Kitayama, 1991) may be more important for dominating style. Oetzel (1998) found that independent self-construal is positively associated with the dominating con¯ict style. Thus, future research should investigate the role of self-construal as an important factor for con¯ict styles among dierent ethnic groups. In addition, there are several conceptual and methodological limitations in this study. First, the concept of ethnic/cultural identity salience is such a complex phenomenon that it is dicult to try and capture the essence of it by way of a few dimensions. For example, ethnic identity salience has probably both stable and situational characteristics. While this study attempts to examine ethnic/cultural identity salience as manifested via stable attitudes and practices, ethnic identity salience may also take on situational characteristics. For example, for some individuals, ethnic identity only becomes salient when they are forced to confront interpersonal issues of ``being dierent'' like stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination. While the present study focuses on situation-general tendencies of ethnic identity salience, perhaps a situation-speci®c probe would provide more insight into such issues as salience of ethnic/cultural identity. Second, in terms of con¯ict styles, seven con¯ict styles have been uncovered in this study. Like the examination of ethnic identity salience, con¯ict styles have been de®ned as situational-general tendencies of con¯ict responses. While this study obtained some meaningful results concerning the eect of ethnic/cultural identity salience on the seven con¯ict styles, there may be other con¯ict factors that are not tapped by this study. Future research needs to work on developing etic-derived con¯ict styles and delineate speci®c high-context modes (e.g., via subtle nonverbal and relational-oriented strategies) of con¯ict management. Stronger attention should also be paid to link the relationship among facework identity, con¯ict style, con¯ict topic salience, and con¯ict competence issues. Third, this study examines the in¯uence of ethnic/cultural identity salience on con¯ict styles in four ethnic groups. Within each group, however, there exists ethnic variations (such as between Chinese American and Vietnamese American) within each broad category of ``Asian Americans'', ``Latino(a) Americans'', ``African Americans'', or ``European Americans''. While panethnic factor analysis yielded common dimensions of ethnicity that enable us to maintain conceptual consistency in interpreting the comparative ®ndings, the study does not capture the ethnicspeci®c elements of ethnic/cultural identity. Although conceptual limitations are important to consider, there are also some methodological limitations to the present study. First, this study's ®ndings are based more on the responses of female respondents than male respondents. For 78 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 every male respondent in this study, we have two female respondents. Thus, the results may not represent ethnic/cultural identity salience and con¯ict style issues in the larger population. Second, no speci®c con¯ict scenarios were provided for the respondents. The recall method of con¯ict styles may not tap the actual strategies of con¯ict in real-life situations. Finally, participants were asked to think of acquaintances when completing the con¯ict portion of the questionnaire. However, it is dicult to assess who exactly the participants had in mind when completing the survey. Despite these limitations, this study makes several contributions. First, this study has identi®ed two clear dimensions of ethnicity in a pluralistic society, namely, ethnic identity salience and cultural identity salience, across four ethnic groups. In addition, con¯ict styles such as neglect, third party, and emotional expression Ð in conjunction with integrating, compromising, dominating, and avoiding Ð have been uncovered as important con¯ict style factors across four ethnic groups. Third, ethnic/cultural identity variations of con¯ict management styles have been identi®ed. Finally, this study lends additional support to TingToomey's (1988) theory of con¯ict face-negotiation theory in which individuals who subscribe to the larger US culture (i.e., with emphasis on individualistic values) are posited to use upfront, confrontive modes of con¯ict. Individuals who subscribe to collectivistic ethnic memberships tend to prefer the use of indirect, high-context modes (e.g., avoidance and third party) of con¯ict. This study serves as an exploratory step in uncovering ethnic/cultural identity dimensions and con¯ict management style factors across four ethnic groups in the US. The results of this study point to some consistent patterns of con¯ict styles in relation to strong/weak ethnic identity and strong/weak cultural identity. Methodologically, this study contributes two new scales that maybe useful to measure ethnic and cultural identity issues, and con¯ict management styles across four ethnic groups within the US. Two panethnic scales, the Ethnic/Cultural Identity Dimensions (EID) Scale and the Con¯ict Style Dimensions (CSD) Scale appeared to have reasonably high reliability coecients. They can be further tested and re®ned by researchers interested in other identity-related or con¯ictrelated phenomenon. This study paves the groundwork for future research in the area of interethnic con¯ict negotiation competence. References Alba, R. (1990). Ethnic identity: transformation of White America. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Asante, M., & Asante, K. (1990). African culture: the rhythms of unity. Trenton, NJ: African World Press. Bellah, R., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W., Swidler, A., & Tipton, S. (1985). Habits of the heart: individualism and commitment in American life. Berkeley: University of California Press. Bennett, J. (1993). Cultural marginality: identity issues in intercultural training. In R. M. Paige (Ed.), Education for the intercultural experience. Yarmouth, Maine: Intercultural Press. S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 79 Berry, J. W., Kim, U., & Boski, P. (1987). Psychological acculturation of immigrants. In Y. Y. Kim & W. Gudykunst (Eds.), Cross-cultural adaptation: current approaches. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Berry, J., Kim, U., Power, S., Young, M., & Bujaki, M. (1989). Acculturation attitudes in plural societies. Applied Psychology, 38, 185±206. Boadu, S. O. (1990). African oral artistry and the new social order. In M. Asante & K. Asante (Eds.), African culture: the rhythms of unity. Trenton, NJ: African World Press. Bond, M. (1991). Beyond the Chinese face. Hong Kong: Oxford University Press. Booth-Butter®eld, M., & Booth-Butter®eld, S. (1990). Conceptualization aect as information in communication production. Human Communication Research, 16, 451±476. Brewer, M. (1991). The social self: on being same and dierent at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 475±482. Brewer, M. (1996). When contact is not enough: social identity and intergroup cooperation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20, 291±303. Cahn, D. (1992). Con¯ict in intimate relationships. New York: The Guilford Press. Cahn, D. (Ed.) (1994). Intimate con¯ict in personal relationships. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Casas, J. M., & Pytluk, S. (1995). Hispanic identity development: Implications for research and practice. In J. Ponterotto, J. Casas, L. Suzuki & C. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural counseling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Chung, L. C., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1994). Ethnic identity and relational expectations among Asian Americans. Paper presented at the International Communication Association Convention, Sydney, Australia. Collier, M. J. (1991). Con¯ict competence within African, Mexican, and Anglo American friendships. In S. Ting-Toomey & F. Korzenny (Eds.), Cross-cultural interpersonal communication. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Cox, T. (1994). Cultural diversity in organizations: theory, research, and practice. San Francisco: BerrettKoehler. Davis, M. (1990). Mexican voices/American dream. New York: Henry Holt & Company. De Vos, G., & Suarez-Orozco, M. (1990). Status inequality: the self in culture. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Espiritu, Y. (1992). Asian American panethnicity. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Garcia, W. R. (1996). Respeto: a Mexican base for interpersonal relationships. In W. Gudykunst, S. Ting-Toomey & T. Nishida (Eds.), Communication in personal relationships across cultures. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Gudykunst, W., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1988). Culture and interpersonal communication. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Healey, J., & Bell, R. (1990). Assessing alternative responses to con¯icts in friendship. In D. Cahn (Ed.), Intimates in con¯ict: a communication perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hecht, M., Collier, M. J., & Ribeau, S. (1993). African American communication: ethnic identity and cultural interpretation. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hecht, M., Sedano, M., & Ribeau, S. (1993). Understanding culture, communication, and research: applications to Chicanos and Mexican Americans. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 17, 157±165. Helms, J., & Parham, T. A. (1993). Black racial identity attitude scale (Form RIAS-B). In J. Helms (Ed.), Black and white racial identity: theory, research, and practice. New York: Greenwood Press. Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and organizations: software of the mind. London: McGraw-Hill. Kagan, S., Knight, G. P., & Martinez-Romero, S. (1982). Culture and the development of con¯ict resolution style. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 13, 43±58. Keefe, S. E. (1992). Ethnic identity: the domain of perceptions and attachment to ethnic groups and cultures. Human Organization, 51, 35±41. Kochman, T. (1981). Black and white styles in con¯ict. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kochman, T. (1986). Black verbal dueling strategies in interethnic communication. In Y. Y. Kim (Ed.), Interethnic communication: current research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Leung, K., Au, Y-F., Fernandez-Dols, J. M., & Iwawaki, S. (1992). Preference for methods of con¯ict processing in two collectivistic cultures. International Journal of Psychology, 27, 195±209. 80 S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 Locke, D. (1992). Increasing multicultural understanding: a comprehensive model. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: self-evaluation of one's social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 302±318. Marin, G., & Marin, B. (1991). Research with Hispanic populations. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 2, 224±253. MirandeÂ, A., & Tanno, D. V. (1993). Labels, researcher perspective, and contextual validation: a commentary. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 17, 149±155. Morrill, C., & Thomas, C. K. (1992). Organizational con¯ict management as disputing process: the problem of social escalation. Human Communication Research, 18, 400±428. Oetzel, J. G. (1998). The eects of self-construals and ethnicity on self-reported con¯ict styles. Communication Reports, 11, 133±144. Padilla, A. (1981). Pluralistic counseling and psychotherapy for Hispanic Americans. In A. Marsella & P. Pedersen (Eds.), Cross-cultural counseling and psychotherapy. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Phinney, J. (1989). Stages of ethnic identity development in minority group adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 9, 34±49. Phinney, J. (1990). Ethnic identity in adolescence and adulthood: a review. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 499±514. Phinney, J. (1991). Ethnic identity and self-esteem: a review and integration. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 13, 193±208. Phinney, J. S. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity measure: a new scale for use with diverse groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 156±176. Phinney, J., & Chavira, V. (1995). Parental ethnic socialization and adolescent coping with problems related to ethnicity. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 5, 31±54. Rahim, M. A. (1983). A measure of styles of handling interpersonal con¯ict. Academy of Management Journal, 26, 368±376. Rahim, M. A. (1992). Managing con¯ict in organizations (2nd ed.). Westport, CT: Praeger. Roosen, J. (1989). Creating ethnicity. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Schwartz, S., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the universal content and structure of values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 878±891. Sodowsky, G., Kwan, K-L., & Pannu, R. (1995). Ethnic identity of Asians in the United States. In J. Ponterotto, J. Casas, L. Suzuki & C. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural counseling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Sternberg, R. J., & Soriano, L. J. (1984). Styles of con¯ict resolution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 115±126. Sternberg, R. J., & Dobson, D. M. (1987). Resolving interpersonal con¯icts: an analysis of stylistic consistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 794±812. Tajfel, H. (Ed.) (1978). Dierentiation between social groups. London: Academic Press. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup con¯ict. In W. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. Thornton, M. (1992). The quiet immigration: foreign spouses of US citizens, 1945±1985. In M. Root (Ed.), Racially-mixed people in America. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ting-Toomey, S. (1985). Toward a theory of con¯ict and culture. In W. Gudykunst, L. Stewart & S. Ting-Toomey (Eds.), Communication, culture, and organizational processes. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Ting-Toomey, S. (1986). Con¯ict communication styles in Black and White subjective cultures. In Y. Y. Kim (Ed.), Interethnic communication: current research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ting-Toomey, S. (1988). Intercultural con¯ict styles: a face-negotiation theory. In Y. Kim & W. Gudykunst (Eds.), Theories in intercultural communication. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ting-Toomey, S. (1993). Communicative resourcefulness: an identity negotiation perspective. In R. Wiseman & J. Koester (Eds.), Intercultural communication competence. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ting-Toomey, S. (1994a). Managing intercultural con¯icts eectively. In L. Samovar & R. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: a reader (7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. S. Ting-Toomey et al. / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 (2000) 47±81 81 Ting-Toomey, S. (1994b). Managing con¯ict in intimate intercultural relationships. In D. Cahn (Ed.), Intimate con¯ict in personal relationships. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Ting-Toomey, S. (1999). Communicating across cultures. New York: The Guilford Press. Ting-Toomey, S., Gao, G., Trubisky, P., Yang, Z., Kim, H. S., Lin, S-L., & Nishida, T. (1991). Culture, face maintenance, and styles of handling interpersonal con¯ict: a study in ®ve cultures. The International Journal of Con¯ict Management, 2, 275±296. Triandis, H. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Trubisky, P., Ting-Toomey, S., & Lin, S-L. (1991). The in¯uence of individualism-collectivism and selfmonitoring on con¯ict styles. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 15, 65±84. Waters, M. (1990). Ethnic options: choosing identities in America. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Yinger, J. M. (1994). Ethnicity: source of strength? Source of con¯ict?. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.