Title: Big or close-up? Evidence for a proximity and emotion, not size and emotion effect. Authors: Andrew K. Dunn*1, Nicola Taylor2 & Thom Baguley1 Email: andrew.dunn@ntu.ac.uk Email: taylor.nicola64@gmail.com Email: thomas.baguley@ntu.ac.uk Institution: 1Nottingham Trent University, Burton Street, Nottingham, NG1 4BU; 2 Independent Researcher *Corresponding author 1 Abstract Background: The size and emotion effect is the tendency for children to draw people/objects with a positive emotional charge larger than those with a negative or neutral charge. Here we explored the novel idea that drawing size might be acting as a proxy for depth (physical or perceived), in a 2D environment. Methods: Forty-two children (aged 3-11 years) chose, from 2 sets of Matryoshka dolls, a doll to represent a person with positive, negative or neutral charge, which they placed in front of themselves on a sheet of 3A paper. Results: We found that the children used proximity and not perceived depth or size, to indicate emotional charge. Conclusions: Our findings are consistent with the notion that, in drawings, children are using size as a proxy for physical closeness (proximity), as they attempt with varying success to put positive charged items closer to, or negative and neutral charge items further away from, themselves. Key words: size and emotion effect; children’s drawings; proximity; depth; attachment 2 Background Children understand emotions in drawings from an early age (≈ 4 ears) and begin representing emotions in the form of simple lines (smiles/frowns on faces) from about 5 yrs onwards - a skill that becomes increasingly more complex (Cox 2005; Ives, 1984; Morra, Caloni & Amico, 1994; Winston, Kenyon, Stewardson, & Lepine, 1995). Young children also use object size to represent emotion in their drawings, (see Cox, 2005). The tendency to use object size as an indicator of emotional charge (positive or negative feelings), towards people and other objects, is called the size and emotion effect (Cox, 2005). Typically, children (≈ 4-11 years) draw positively charged (nice/pleasant/liked) objects (e.g. people, apples, dogs) bigger than negatively charged (nasty/unpleasant /disliked) or neutral objects (Burkitt, Barrett & Davis, 2003; Thomas, Chaigne, & Fox, 1989;). Neutral charged objects are usually drawn the same size as negative ones (Thomas, Chaigne, & Fox, 1989) though negative objects are sometimes drawn as smaller than neutral ones (e.g. Burkitt, Barrett & Davis, 2003, 2004). And the effect is sensitive to methodological factors (e.g. drawing on the same/separate pages; presence/absence of drawing template) which may lead to a reversal of the effect (i.e. negative figure > positive figure; Cotterill & Thomas, 1990; Jolley, 1995), loss of the effect or else inconsistency in individual’s performance over time (Cox, 2005, Jolley, 1995; Strange, Van Papendrecht, Crawford, Candel & Hayne, 2010). Nevertheless the evidence, from our own lab (Rajput, 2006) and published evidence elsewhere (e.g. Cleeve & Bradbury, 1992; Craddick, 1963; Fox & Thomas, 1990; Ives, 1984; Sechrest & Wallace, 1964) clearly demonstrates that that young children draw positively charged objects as bigger than those that are negatively 3 charged or neutral (Burkitt, Barrett & Davis, 2003, 2004; Thomas, Chaigne, & Fox, 1989). Although it is not entirely clear why, it is generally assumed that drawing size acts as a proxy for emotional content where bigger equals more positive (Cox, 2005). Another possibility, not previously explored, is that the change in size might reflect a desire to show preference (emotional charge) by placing the objects in depth. More specifically, differences in relative object (drawing) size might be an attempt to represent associated retinal size at different distances (depth) or physical closeness (proximity), though these need not be mutually exclusive. Young children are aware of and can represent depth in 2-dimensional (2D) drawings (Cox, 2005). Thus at 3-4 years children seem to understand that object size is reduce in distant objects (Pillow & Flavell, 1986), and can use size judgements to determine object depth in pre-drawn pictures, though not necessarily in their own drawings (Perara & Cox, 2000). By 5 years children can also use object height (position on the page) to indicate distance, placing distant objects further up the page than nearer objects, which they place further down (Freeman, Eiser & Syaers, 1977). They also position multiple objects above or below each other to convey depth (Cox & Perara, 2001). However, children do not spontaneously draw objects in depth until they are around 7 years (Cox, 1986), and it is not until 8-9 years that children combine size and height (Cox & Perara, 2001), use diverging/converging lines (Cox & Littlejohn, 1995) or foreshortening (Nicholls, 1995), with any success or consistency in their drawings, without being prompted to do so. 4 In summary, notwithstanding some methodological influences, the evidence shows that children draw positive charged objects bigger than negative or natural charges objects, though it is not entirely clear why. Here we propose that the effect might be an attempt to represent physical distance between the child and the object in relation to its (the object’s) emotional charge (big = positive charge = close; small = negative charge = further away). We further suggest that inconsistencies or reversals (i.e. negative charge smaller than positive and neutral objects or observed) in the effect reflect the difficulties children experience with producing depth/proximity cues in 2D space. Here we tested this idea in 3-dimensional (3D) space by asking young children to select 3 dolls (positive charge, negative charge or neutral charge) from 2 sets (7 piece) of highly similar Matryoshka (Russian) dolls, and to then place them on a sheet of A3 paper in front of them. We hypothesized that if the children are using actual size to indicate emotional charge then they should consistently chose bigger (taller and/or bigger base circumference) dolls to represent positive charge, relative to dolls chosen to represent negative or neutral charge. However, if they are using perceived depth as an index of charge, then relative position of the doll should vary according to the actual size of the doll selected (i.e. a small doll chosen for positive charge should be placed closer to the child than a larger doll chosen to reflect a negative charge). If so, the positive doll would subtend a greater visual angle than the negative charge doll. But if the child is using physical proximity then positively charged dolls should be placed closer to the child than negative or neutral charged dolls, irrespective of doll size (height or base circumference). 5 Methods Subjects: Forty-two children (median age=5yrs 6months; range 3-11years) from a Derbyshire Montessori nursery class and a Church of England primary group consented with parental permission, to take part in this experiment. Note the data was collected latter half of the school year. Following clearance by the Social Science ethics board (Nottingham Trent University), formal permission to take part in the study was obtained from the nursery/school and parents (child opt-out) before data collection began. No child was made to take part if, at testing, they did not want to. Apparatus and procedure: The children were tested individually in a quiet area away from any other children. Each child was seated at a table such that the table was about level with the middle of their stomach and that they could see and reach freely across the space around them. Having been seated, it was explained to each the child that in this task they were going to be asked to choose and pick up 3 wooden dolls (one at a time) from a range of wooden Russian (Matryoshka) dolls, and that one should represent someone they liked (positive charge), one should someone they did not like (negative charge) and one should just be a person someone they neither liked nor disliked (neutral charge). The 2 sets of (highly similar) Matryoshka dolls were then placed one at a time in height order (height order direction randomised for each child) in front of them, along the back edge of a sheet of A3 paper (see Table 1 for details of the dolls). Next the children were told each time they chose a doll, they should place the doll on the 6 piece of paper in front of them. And that they could choose any doll and place it anywhere on the paper. Table 1: here Before beginning the task, the experimenter first made sure that the children understood what they were being asked to do and that they still wanted to take part. All children agreed to take part in the task. At test, the order of doll section was randomised for each child, and the child was given a scripted set of selection instructions. These were as follows: Positive charge doll: “Please can you think of a person you like, once you have thought of somebody can you choose one of those dolls in front of you that could be the person that you like. Once you have chosen the doll could you please place it anywhere you like on the piece of paper in front of you?” Negative charge doll: “Please can you think of a person you do not like, once you have thought of somebody can you choose one of those dolls in front of you that could be that person that you do not like. Once you have chosen the doll could you please place it anywhere you like on the piece of paper in front of you?’ Neutral charge doll: “Please can you think of a neutral person, just a person, it can be anyone at all. Once you have thought of somebody can you choose one 7 of those dolls in front of you that that person. Once you have chosen the doll could you please place it anywhere you like on the piece of paper in front of you?” We used standardised instructions so as to maintain consistency in what was being asked, however, we were aware that some children might not fully understand what was meant by like, dislike, or neutral. For this reason we took great pains both before and during test that the children were as clear as they could be about the terms being used. For example, where children did not understand the instructions, the child was reassured, the instructions were repeated and the child encouraged to make their selection to match the condition. In practice all of the children even the very young ones, appeared to understand what was being asked of them and what the terms being used meant before test; they required very little prompting or additional explanation/support during test. Following the placement of dolls, four measures were obtained: (1) Doll height – base to top (cm), (2) Doll base circumference (cm), (3) Physical distance from doll to child (cm) - a circle was drawn around each doll and the distance from the centre point to the middle of the child’s seated position was measured. (4) Visual angle (degrees) calculated from the height of the doll and the physical distance where 1°= 1cm at 57cms. Results: The data set indicated that there is tendency for positively charged dolls to be slightly bigger (taller and broader) and placed closer to the child than neutral or 8 negatively charged dolls (see Table 2). To explore this formally we ran 4 repeated measures ANOVAs (height; circumference; distance; visual angle), each with 3 levels (positive charge, negative charge, neutral charge). Only the analysis for distance was significant (F(2,82)=5.26; MSE=15.535; P=0.007; (F(2,82)=0.68; MSE=23.647; P=0.510; MSE=50.025; P=0.335; P=0.994; ; =0.0.855). The analyses for height =0.0433), circumference (F(2,82)=1.11; =0.0654) and visual angle (F(2,82)=0.06; MSE=2.383; =0.000) were not significant. The patterns of results are presented in Figure 2 with Cousineau-Morey style difference-adjusted confidence intervals (Baguley, 2012a). These are adjusted so that means with non-overlapping error bars are different with approximately 95% confidence. As can be seen in Figure 1b) there is a significant difference between the positive charged dolls and both the negative and neutral charged dolls (P<0.05) but not between the negative and neutral charged dolls (P>0.05). Table 2 about here Figure 1 about here Note we have not include age as a covariate here because age is, by definition, accounted for in the between subjects error term (Baguley, 2012b) accordingly including age or other individual difference variables as covariates has no influence on the pattern of results. 9 Discussion The results indicated that whilst the children are not consistently selecting taller or wider based dolls, or positioning the dolls in relation to perceived size, they are placing the positive charged dolls physically closer to them than the negative or neutral charged dolls. This finding is consistent with our proximity proposal and the notion that size might be acting as a proxy for physical closeness in 2D figure drawings. Although there is evidence for the size and emotion effect, there have been inconsistencies in the literature, with methodological factors playing an important role. Indeed, Jolley (1995; Thomas & Jolley, 1998) has argued that the evidence is not very strong, and is tricky to elicit, for either increased sized for positively charged items or reduced size for negatively charged items. Furthermore, no effect has been found where very strong negative or positive feelings might be expected to be expressed in drawings e.g. in patients with depression (Joiner, Schmidt & Barnett, 1996) or in children that have experience military conflict (Jolley & Vulic-Prtoric, 2001). However, it is should be noted that whilst children’s drawings have proven to be a useful tool for exploring typical cognitive development, there is a very clear history of inappropriate interpretation and use of children's drawings as a diagnostic tool (Cox, 2005). Indeed it is not clear that drawings are the best way to explore extreme emotional stress (Joiner & Schmidt, 1997). In fact the evidence for their use as a clinical diagnostic tool does not appear to exist (Flanagan & Motta, 2007). Thus the absence of a size emotion effect for extreme emotion is not absence of evidence, for children trying to express emotional charge. Moreover, the real issue surrounding 10 the presence/absence of the size-emotion effect seems to be about methodological factors such as sample size and procedure. For example, Jolley’s (1995) work used a repeated measures design but may have been underpowered, having a fairly small sample size (see Burkitt et al., 2003). Further, since children are not being asked to directly manipulate size in the drawing tasks then it is likely that the observed effects will never be very large (they are in effect a shadow cast by the task). On balance, where attention is paid to methodology, there is reason to believe that children do tend modulate object size with emotional charge (see Cox, 2005; Burkitt et al., 2003). The work here was in part motivated to explore the size –emotion effect because of methodological issues and the idea that size might be acting as a proxy for depth or closeness. That is, some of the inconsistencies (e.g. reverse findings – negative charged items being positioned further away than positive or neutral charged items) because the children are struggling to represent depth/closeness in a 2D drawing. To operationalize this idea, the emotional charge task was transferred from 2 dimensions to 3 dimensions using dolls instead of drawings. Certainly young children are aware of depth cues and are able to use them in their drawings (see above; Cox, 2005) but they are limited. Similarly, young children can understand, and do convey, emotion in pictures but with they are limited. Thus very young children (up to 5/6 years) tend to be more literal in their understanding and generation of emotional content (e.g. Cox, 2005, Jolley, Cox & Barlow, 2003; Winston et al., 1995). It is also worth noting that even adults (who have received some training at school) find representing depth and emotion in 2D scenes difficult (Cox, 2005). Thus by transforming the task it was possible to allow the children to modulate height, width 11 and depth (perceived and physical) whilst avoiding some of the methodological difficulties usually associated with the drawing task (see Cox, 2005). Of course, the approach taken here was not without issue. For example, although great care was taken, some children (especially the younger ones) may not have fully understood the task instructions, particularly for the neutral figure. Certainly, some of the children in this task were very young (3-4 years) and other studies (e.g. Burkitt, et al., 2003) have used more user-friendly terminology. However, any differences in age would have accounted for in the between subjects error term of the ANOVA. At testing there did not appear to be any difficulty with the instructions however, even if some children did not fully understand what was meant by, for example ‘…a neutral person’, the results show that positively charged dolls were position physically closer than negatively charged dolls: differentiating between the positive and negative charges is consistent with the pertinent literature even if the variable being manipulated (c.f. proximity rather than size) is not. Without further research, it remains unclear as to why children would want to convey emotional charge by manipulating size or proximity. However, it is conceivable that the distancing effects observed here, might reflect aspects of attachment, or the processes of attachment, namely safe base/proximity behaviour (e.g. Ainsworth, 1973; Bowlby 1969). If this is true then the distancing effects might vary with attachment type, attachment stage and possibly even temperament. Similarly, there might also be cross-cultural differences in the size or presence of the effect as a function of the culture in which the child is being raised. For example, 12 there should be differences in the magnitude (or presence) of the effect when comparing between cultures that favour close proximity, increased emotion or physical contact, in social situations verses those that do not. Quite what those differences might be is at present unclear and must remain speculative. Conclusions: It has been shown here that when asked to select dolls taken from two sets of highly similar Matryoshka dolls, to represent a person with positive, negative or neutral charge, children aged between 3-11 years, place the positively charged dolls physically closer to them than the negatively or neutrally charged dolls. This finding is consistent with the hypothesised proximity proposal and the notion that size might be acting as a proxy for physical closeness in 2D figure drawings. Author contribution: AKD conceived and designed the experiment carried out the analysis and interpretation of data and wrote most of the manuscript for publication. NT was involved in the initial design, collected all of the data and write an early draft of the methods section, TB carried out some additional data analysis, contributed to the interpretation of data and later drafts of the manuscript. 13 References Complete book Calfee, RC, & Valencia, RR (1991). APA guide to preparing manuscripts for journal publication. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Book chapter, or an article within a book O'Neil, JM, & Egan, J (1992). Men's and women's gender role journeys: Metaphor for healing, transition, and transformation. In BR Wainrib (Ed.), Gender issues across the life cycle (pp. 107-123). New York: Springer. Ainsworth, MDS (1973). The development of infant-mother attachment. In BM Caldwell & HN Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of Child Development Research.Vol.3. (pp. 1-94). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Baguley, T. (2012a). Calculating and graphing within-subject confidence intervals for ANOVA. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 158-175. Baguley, T. (2012b). Serious stats: A guide to advanced statistics for the behavioral sciences. Basingstoke: Palgrave. Burkitt, E, Barrett, M, Davis, A. (2003). The effect of affective characterizations on the size of children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 565-584. Burkitt, E, Barrett, M, Davis, A. (2004). The effect of affective characterizations on the use of size and colour in drawings produced by children in the absence of a model. Educational Psychology, 24. 315-343. Bowlby, J. (1969). Separation and Loss. Volume 1: Attachment. New York, Basic Books. 14 Cleeve, H, Bradbury, RJ. (1992). Children’s productions and judgments of drawings of people of different importance. Poster presented at the 5th European Conference of Developmental Psychology, Seville. Cotterill, A, Thomas, GV. (1990). Children’s production and perception of drawings of emotionally significant topics. Poster presented at the forth European Conference on Developmental Psychology, University of Stirling. Cox, M. (2005). The pictorial world of the child. New York, NY US: Cambridge University Press. Cox, MV. (1986). Cubes are different things to draw. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 341-345 Cox MV, Littlejohn, K. (1995). Children’s use of converging obliques in their perspective drawings. Educational Psychology, 15, 127-139. Cox, MV, Perara, J. (2001). Children’s use of height and size cues to depict a projective depth relationship in their pictures. Psychologia, 41, 171182. Craddick, RA. (1963). Size of Hallowe’en witch drawings prior to, on and after Hallowe’en. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 16, 235-238. Flanagan, F, Motta, RW. (2007). Figure drawings: A popular method. Psychology in the Schools, 44, 257-270. Fox, T, Thomas, GV. (1990). Children’s drawings of an anxiety-eliciting topic: Effect on size of the drawing. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 29, 71-81. 15 Freeman, NH, Eiser, C, Sayers, J. (1977). Children’s strategies in producing 3-D relationships on a 2-D surface. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 305-314. Ives, W. (1984). The development of expressivity in drawing’, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 152-159. Joiner, TE, Schmidt, KL. (1997). Drawing conclusions or not from drawings. Journal of Personality Assessment, 69, 476-481. Joiner, TE, Schmidt, KL, Barnett, J. (1996). Size, detail, and line heaviness in children’s drawings as correlates of emotional distress: (more) negative evidence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67, 127-141. Jolley, RP. (1995). Children’s production and perception of visual metaphors for mood and emotion in line drawings and in art. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Birmingham, Birmingham. Jolley, RP, Vulic-Prtoric, A. (2001). Croatian children’ s experience of war is not reflected in the size and placement of emotive topics in their drawings. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 107–110. Jolley, RP, Cox, MV, Barlow, CM. (2003). What develops and why in British children’s expressive drawings. Paper presented at the British Psychological Society (Education section), Manchester. Morra, S, Caloni, B, d’Amigo, MR. (1994). Working memory and the international depiction of emotions. Archives de Psychologie, 62, 7187. 16 Nicholls, AL. (1995). Influence of visual projection on young children’s depictions of object proportions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 304-326. Perara, J, Cox, MV. (2000). The effect of background context on children’s understanding of the spatial depth arrangement of objects in a drawing, Psychologia, 43, 144-153. Pillow, BH, Flavell, JH. (1986). Intellectual realism: the role of children’s interpretations of pictures and perceptual verbs, Child Development, 56, 664-670. Rajput, M. (2006). Internal representation of affective preferences for liked, disliked and neutral human figures drawings: representation as size or distance as a function of emotional preference. Unpublished undergraduate research project, Nottingham Trent University. Sechrest, L, Wallace, J. (1964). Figure drawings and naturally occurring events: Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, 42–44. Strange, D. Van Papendrecht, H, Crawford, E, Candel, I, Hayne, H. (2010). Size doesn't matter: Emotional content does not determine the size of objects in children's drawings. Psychology, Crime & Law, 16, 459-476. Thomas, G, Chaigne, E, Fox, T. (1989). Children's drawings of topics differing in significance: Effects on size of drawing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 321-331. Thomas, GV, & Jolley, RP. (1998). Drawing conclusions: an examination of empirical and conceptual bases for psychological evaluation of children from their drawings. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 127-139. 17 Winston, AS, Kenyon, B, Stewardson, J, Lepine, T. (1995). Children's sensitivity to expression of emotion in drawings. Visual Arts Research, 1-14. 18 !"#$%&'(&!"##$%&'()'*$+%&(,-.$'/(00'#1$,"%1#12+,'3,1+,'4'$2/'567' SET 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 ' ' ! Figure 1 Doll 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Height 2.9cm 5cm 6.6cm 8.8cm 10.5cm 12.8cm 15.7cm 3cm 4.8cm 6.7cm 8.5cm 10.9cm 13cm 16.8cm Widest Circumference 5.1cm 7.7cm 10.9cm 14cm 18.1cm 21.2cm 26.7cm 5cm 7.8cm 10.7cm 14cm 17.7cm 21.5cm 26.2cm Circumference Base 4.08cm 5.96cm 7.85cm 10.36cm 12.88cm 16.65cm 23.56cm 4.71cm 5.6cm 7.53cm 10.68cm 12.56cm 16.02cm 24.81cm Diameter 1.3cm 1.9cm 2.5cm 3.3cm 4.1cm 5.3cm 7.5cm 1.5cm 1.8cm 2.4cm 3.4cm 4cm 5.1cm 7.9cm !"#$%&'(&!"#$%#$&%'(#$&#)&%&"*+#(+,$'%-,)%"#./%,-%(/"%-,0)%1"#'0)"'%2/"+3/(4% .+).01-")"$."4%&+'(#$."%#$&%*+'0#5%#$35" 7% 8/#)3"% >,'+(+*"% I"3#(+*"% I"0()#5%% % ! Figure 2 % !% 2?: % !% 2?: % !% 2?: % 9"+3/(% 2.1 % ∃#7∃)%% 2(7∃∋ % ,7#+%% 2(7#∃ % ,7∃%% 2&7(& % 8+).01-")"$."% 2.1 % ∃∋7)(%% 2∗7(∋ % ∃%7+#%% 2∗7(∃ % ∃%7((% 2∋7+& % :+'(#$."% 2.1 % ∃%7,&%% 2(7∋# % ∃(7∋∋%% 2∋7)& % ∃(7%(%% 2∋7∋) % ;+'0#5%<$35"%% 2= % %7∋(%% 2∃7)∋ % %7∋+%% 2∃7∗# % %7∋∋%% 2∃7%∃ % Figure 3