Psychological Bulletin 2010, Vol. 136, No. 3, 341–343 © 2010 American Psychological Association 0033-2909/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019116 Are There Shared Environmental Influences on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder? Reply to Wood, Buitelaar, Rijsdijk, Asherson, and Kunsti (2010) S. Alexandra Burt Michigan State University A recent large-scale meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies indicated that shared environmental influences make important contributions to most forms of child and adolescent psychopathology (Burt, 2009b). The sole exception to this robust pattern of results was observed for attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which appeared to be largely genetic (and particularly nonadditive genetic) in origin, with no observable influence of the shared environment. The central thesis of Wood, Buitelaar, Rijsdijk, Asherson, and Kunsti (2010) is that, contrary to these findings, shared environmental influences are important for ADHD. As evidence for this thesis, Wood et al. presented a summary of prior twin studies, followed by a discussion of 4 methodological issues that may account for my findings in Burt (2009b). I argue that, although the methodological concerns raised by Wood et al. are very important, they do not undermine my earlier results (Burt, 2009b). I close with a discussion of 2 issues that may allow for some shared environmental influences on ADHD. Keywords: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, shared environment, meta-analysis Wood et al. (2010) summarized 22 child and adolescent twin studies of ADHD published during the past 10 years. They averaged available genetic and shared environmental parameter estimates across the 100 subpopulations examined within these 22 studies (averages were 61.8% and 22.4%, respectively), and they used these averages as the basis for their discussion. Although this straightforward approach to summarizing twin study results is in some ways intuitive, there are methodological limitations that reduce confidence in such results. First, and as noted by Wood et al., 84% of the subpopulations showed no evidence of (significant) shared environmental influences. Moreover, these investigations frequently reported results from best fitting reduced models that omitted C because of lack of significance. This absence of any estimate of C from such a large proportion of studies makes it difficult to interpret averages of the available parameter estimates. Second, analyses were unweighted by sample size (weighting places more value on larger samples, which should have more reliable estimates) and treated nonindependent data (e.g., mother and teacher reports on the same twins) as though they were independent, both of which are rather strongly advised against (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). I sought to circumvent these difficulties in Burt (2009b) by analyzing intraclass correlations, which are necessarily weighted by sample size in the analyses (Neale, Boker, Xie, & Maes, 2003) and which avoid the issue of missing parameter estimates from best fitting models. I also omitted nonindependent samples and used weighted averages to adjust for any nonindependence that remained in the data after omitting these samples. I was thus able to include all available (and nonindependent) data in the analysis. When taking these more rigorous analytical steps, results were simply not consistent with the presence of shared environmental influences on ADHD (see Burt, 2009b). One often-cited conclusion from behavioral genetics research has been that the crucial environmental influences result in differences between siblings (i.e., referred to as nonshared effects; E), whereas environmental influences that create similarities between siblings (i.e., shared effects; C) are indistinguishable from zero (Plomin & Daniels, 1987; Turkheimer, 2000). Although this finding does appear to hold in adulthood, a recent meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies (n ! 490) indicated that C makes important contributions to most forms of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence (Burt, 2009b). Analyses revealed that shared environmental influences generally accounted for 10%– 30% of the variance within conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, anxiety, depression, and broad internalizing and externalizing spectrum disorders. Furthermore, these estimates did not vary across twin and adoption studies, suggesting that shared environmental influences reflect actual environmental factors common to siblings. The only exception to this robust pattern of results was for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which instead appeared to be largely genetic (and particularly nonadditive genetic) in origin (i.e., 26% additive genetic and 44% dominant genetic), with no significant, observable influence of the shared environment. The central thesis of Wood, Buitelaar, Rijsdijk, Asherson, and Kunsti (2010) is that, contrary to these findings, shared environmental influences are important for ADHD. As evidence for this thesis, Wood et al. first presented a summary of prior twin studies of ADHD, followed by a discussion of four methodological issues that may account for the findings of Burt (2009b). I begin with a discussion of the summary. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to S. Alexandra Burt, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, 107D, Psychology Building, East Lansing, MI 48823. E-mail: burts@msu.edu 341 342 BURT Wood et al. (2010) then highlighted four key issues that may hamper the ability to detect shared environmental influences in the classical twin study design and thus in Burt (2009b). I review each of these in turn. Issue 1 Wood et al. (2010) noted that the power to detect C is low in traditional twin study designs. This is quite true (see Martin, Eaves, Kearsey, & Davies, 1978) and, indeed, is a point I made myself in Burt (2009b) when arguing the merits of the shared environment. However, the large-scale meta-analyses performed in Burt (2009b) were not limited by these sorts of power constraints. Even after adjusting for nonindependence, my analysis of ADHD was based on 25,712 sibling pairs, an extremely large sample by any standard and one with enough power to detect even very small estimates of C. Indeed, power analyses suggest that I had more than 80% power to detect C estimates as low as 5%, given a 60% heritability estimate. Issue 2 Wood et al. (2010) correctly stated that shared environmental influences are confounded with dominant genetic influences in the classical twin design (Keller & Coventry, 2005; Keller & Medland, 2008). As discussed in Burt (2009b), recent simulation work by Keller and Medland (2008) revealed that the simultaneous presence of shared environmental and nonadditive genetic influences (a biologically plausible scenario) led to notable underestimates of shared environmental effects within the classical twin design (i.e., the true estimate of C was 15%, whereas the observed estimate was 2%). It is therefore possible that the shared environment does influence ADHD but that these effects are obscured by the simultaneous presence of nonadditive genetic effects. Fortunately, the examination of adoptive sibling pairs circumvents this otherwise rather significant potential confound in classical twin studies. In particular, adoptive siblings do not share any segregating genetic material; thus, any similarity between adoptive siblings functions as a direct estimate of C. In other words, should shared environmental influences in adoption studies be equivalent to those in twin studies, it would argue against this sort of underestimate of C (as well as other limitations of twin studies; see Burt, 2009b). There was one adoption study (van den Oord, Boomsma, & Verhulst, 1994) included in the Burt (2009b) ADHD analyses (of two available but nonindependent adoption studies). Because the ADE (i.e., additive genetic, dominant genetic, and nonshared environmental) model emerged as the better fitting model, I did not originally compare estimates of the shared environment across twin and adoption studies. However, because the point raised by Wood et al. (2010) is extremely important, I now show results of this analysis. Using the analytic procedures outlined in Burt (2009b), I compared shared environmental influences on ADHD across twin and adoption studies. Results are presented in Table 1. Genetic effects emerged as moderate to strong and as significant predictors of ADHD regardless of study type, as did nonshared environmental influences. By contrast, shared environmental influences were near zero (or zero) and nonsignificant in both twin and adoption studies. Moreover, the shared environmental parameter could be con- Table 1 ACE Parameter Estimates for ADHD Across Study Type and Informant Study type and informant A C E Adoption study Twin study .49! [.04, .90] .04 [.00, .18] .47! [.14, .86] .70! [.68, .72] .00 [.00, .002] .31! [.30, .33] Teacher informant report Child self-report Peer informant report .72! [.67, .75] .00 [.00, .04] .34! [.21, .40] .01 [.00, .12] .71! [.47, .84] .00 [.00, .21] .28! [.27, .29] .64! [.60, .69] .29! [.24, .36] Note. 95% confidence intervals for each estimate are reported in brackets. A ! genetic effects; C ! shared environmental effects; E ! nonshared environmental influences; ADHD ! attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. ! p $ .05, estimate is significantly greater than zero. strained to be equal across twin and adoption studies without a significant decrement in fit, "#2(1) ! 0.395, p ! .53. In short, there was virtually no evidence of shared environmental influence on ADHD in either twin or adoption studies, arguing against the notion that methodological limitations of classical twin studies hampered my ability to identify this effect (Burt, 2009b). As with any single study, however, it is possible that van den Oord et al.’s (1994) study yielded peculiar estimates. To evaluate this possibility, I computed a weighted average of the 44 male– male, 48 female–female, and 129 male–female adoptive sibling correlations for each of the six psychopathological conditions evaluated in van den Oord et al.’s study. For externalizing, internalizing, and anxiety problems, the average adoptive sibling correlations were .18, .34, and .24, respectively, results that are consistent with moderate shared environmental influences (i.e., 18%–34%) on those phenotypes. Such results are notably consistent with the overall results of Burt (2009b). For conduct problems, average correlations were computed separately for the aggressive and rule-breaking behavioral subtypes. Consistent with findings from a separate meta-analysis of conduct problems data (Burt, 2009a), shared environmental influences appeared to be more prominent for rule-breaking than aggressive conduct problems (average adoptive sibling correlations were .19 and .08, respectively). By contrast, the average adoptive sibling correlation for attention problems was quite small (weighted r ! .04). Such findings are consistent with the relative absence of shared environmental influences on ADHD (and aggressive conduct problems) but not on other psychological disorders in this particular adoption sample. Issue 3 Wood et al. (2010) next noted that the role of contrast effects (i.e., when parents overemphasize differences in hyperactivity in their dizygotic twins) must be considered, since rater contrast can mimic nonadditive genetic effects by accentuating the difference in similarity between monozygotic and dizygotic twins (and, in this way, inhibit researchers’ ability to detect shared environmental influences). Although theoretically possible as an explanation for the lack of C observed for parent reports, it is unclear why this phenomenon would apply only to ADHD and not to other child behavioral problems, particularly given the high levels of comor- SHARED ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ADHD: REPLY bidity between ADHD and other child externalizing disorders. Regardless, because rater contrast effects are generally not thought to influence other informant reports (as discussed in Rietveld, Hudziak, Bartels, van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2003), I sought to evaluate the presence of shared environmental effects in the absence of possible rater contrast by computing estimates of C for teacher, child, and peer informants. I thus re-examined the data in Burt (2009b) within an ACE (i.e., additive genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental) model. As seen in Table 1, shared environmental influences were uniformly nonsignificant and were estimated to be zero or near zero. In short, rater contrast effects also do not appear to explain the absence of shared environmental influences that I observed (Burt, 2009b). Issue 4 Finally, Wood et al. (2010) argued that the distributional properties of the measures must also be considered, because the presence of significant skew (as well as the use of transformations to normalize this skew) may give rise to biased parameter estimates. This issue of distributional properties is an interesting one that deserves additional research. Indeed, as reported in Burt (2009b), nonshared environmental estimates were uniformly higher (and were so at the expense of genetic and/or shared environmental estimates) when disorders were assessed through a diagnostic interview as compared with a questionnaire (the former of which is far more skewed than the latter). In short, there is evidence to suggest that assessment strategy exerts a profound effect on genetic and environmental parameter estimates. It is thus possible that, as argued by Wood et al. (2010), shared environmental influences on ADHD have been artifactually suppressed by these distributional issues. It is worth noting, however, that shared environmental influences were estimated to be zero for questionnaire measures of ADHD as well as for diagnostic interviews. Moreover, it is unclear why shared environmental influences would be suppressed on ADHD questionnaires but not on questionnaires for other disorders, given that the level of skew is similar across constructs. Could Shared Environmental Effects Influence ADHD? Although I would argue that the concerns I have discussed here do not undermine the results of Burt (2009b), there may yet be some room for shared environmental influences on ADHD. First, although it would have been statistically preferable to examine variance– covariance matrices, the broad scope of my analyses (Burt, 2009b) meant that I was logistically limited to examining only published data (i.e., intraclass correlations). This difference is not a trivial one. In their meta-analysis of antisocial behavior, Rhee and Waldman (2002) found that the exclusion of weight matrices from the analyses caused them to systematically underestimate shared environmental influences in particular (i.e., from 28% to 17%). Although this limitation augments the importance of the shared environmental influences identified in my research (Burt, 2009b), it may also mean that (small) shared environmental influ- 343 ences on ADHD have been obscured. Future research should seek to use variance– covariance matrices to meta-analyze twin and adoption studies on ADHD. Second, Wood et al. (2010) noted that questionnaires designed to assess the presence of both psychopathological and positive behaviors were more likely to yield significant estimates of the shared environment, as were mechanical measures (e.g., actigraphs). Although the former were included in my meta-analysis (Burt, 2009b), the latter were not (because they measure only activity level and thus are not clear measures of psychopathology). However, should these instruments ultimately prove to be more valid measures of ADHD than are other (more highly skewed) instruments (e.g., diagnostic interviews, questionnaires), conclusions regarding the absence of shared environmental influences on ADHD may need to be reconsidered. References Burt, S. A. (2009a). Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 163–178. Burt, S. A. (2009b). Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: A meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 608 – 637. Keller, M. C., & Coventry, W. L. (2005). Quantifying and addressing parameter indeterminacy in the classical twin design. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 8, 201–213. Keller, M. C., & Medland, S. E. (2008, June). Evaluation of the cascade model—A new extended twin family model. Paper presented at the conference of the Behavior Genetics Association, Louisville, Kentucky. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Kearsey, M. J., & Davies, P. (1978). The power of the classical twin study. Heredity, 40, 97–116. Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2003). Mx: Statistical Modeling (6th ed.) [Computer software]. Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University. Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 1– 60. Rhee, S., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490 –529. Rietveld, M. J. H., Hudziak, J. J., Bartels, M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Heritability of attention problems in children: I. Cross-sectional results from a study of twins, age 3–12 years. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 117, 102–113. Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 160 –164. van den Oord, E. J. C. G., Boomsma, D. I., & Verhulst, F. C. (1994). A study of problem behaviors in 10- to 15-year-old biologically related and unrelated international adoptees. Behavior Genetics, 24, 193–205. Wood, A. C., Buitelaar, J., Rijsdijk, F., Asherson, P., & Kunsti, J. (2010). Rethinking shared environment as a source of variance underlying attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: Comment on Burt (2009). Psychological Bulletin, 136, 331–340. Received January 26, 2010 Revision received January 26, 2010 Accepted January 28, 2010 !