FORAGING-FLOCK RECRUITMENT AT A BLACK-BILLED GULL COLONY: IMPLICATIONS FOR THE INFORMATION CENTER HYPOTHESIS ROGER M. EVANS Departmentof Zoology,Universityof Manitoba,Winnipeg,Manitoba,CanadaR3T 2N2 ABSTRACT.--The extentto which a flock leaderadvertisesits departurefrom a colonyand recruitsflockmatesis an importantissueof the InformationCenter hypothesis.At a colony of Black-billedGulls (Larusbulleri),I found that attractivecallswere given by someleaders, that leaderscalled more often than followers,and that calling leadersrecruited followers moreoften than silentleaders.Playbackexperimentsdemonstratedthat these"contact"calls were attractive.The resultsindicatethat somebenefits,mostlikely relatedto groupforaging, result from flock membershipwhen the gulls are away from the colony.Benefitsof group foragingawayfromthe colonyprovidea morelikely mechanismfor explainingfood-related selectionpressuresfavoring a colonialforaging systemthan do the presumedbenefitsderived from the morecomplex,information-transfermechanismenvisagedby the Information Center hypothesis.Received11 December 1980, accepted I July1981. ACCORDINGto the Information Center hypothesisdescribedby Ward and Zahavi (1973), certain avian assemblages,such as communal roosts and breeding colonies, evolved primarily becausethey function as information centers where birds that have been unsuccessful function as a means ies of the Red-billed Dioch [Quelea] (Quelea quelea)(Ward 1965), the Great Blue Heron (Ardea herodias)(Krebs 1974, 1978), and the Bank Swallow (Riparia riparia) (Emlen and Demong 24 Gulls nest in dense inland col- onies in New Zealand (Stead 1932, Beer 1966, Evans1970).They typically foragein flocksand utilize unpredictable but often temporarily rich food patches. Food items include fish, as well as worms and other small invertebrates that are exposed on open pasture land after rains or turned up on fields by farm implements(Dawson 1958, pers. obs.). SOME CONTEXTS FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER AT COLONIES 1975). The central concept of the Information Center hypothesisinvolves informationtransferbetween or among birds at the roost or colony. Of particular relevanceare signalswhereby a potential leader might identify itself to followers. Although information transfer of the sort envisagedby the Information Center hypothesis could occur in the absenceof specialized signals devoted to that function (Bertram 1978, Waltz in press), the extent to which a leader advertises its departure remains highly relevant to the hypothesis(Ward and Zahavi 1973). In this report, I examine some of the contexts and evolutionary implications of information transfer as it pertains to signals that could at a billed Gulls (Larus bulleri). Black-billed in locating a good food patch can benefit by following other, more successfulbirds to the latters' food. The hypothesis is consideredto apply mainly to birds that utilize food distributed in unpredictable but temporarily rich patches. Data consistentwith the Information Center hypothesishave been obtained in stud- of flock recruitment colony and describe observationsand experiments designed to assessthe occurrenceand probable function of these signals in Black- Parasitic relationship.--A parasitic relationship would arise if followers that did not know where food was ("ignorant" birds) were able to victimize leaders that did know where food was ("knowledgeable" birds) by following them to their food patches,even though such information transfer were of no advantage to the leader (cf. Krebs 1978, Waltz in press).This situation could arise if a colony evolved and were maintained for reasons unrelated to in- formation transfer, for example, if the colony provided protection against predators (Lack 1968). Possiblecoststo the leader might include those associatedwith leading potential competitors to the leader's food source. Even The Auk 99: 24-30. January1982 January1982] FlockRecruitment in Gulls if suchcoststo the leaderswere negligible, it is expectedthat knowledgeableleaderswould not be selectedto expend time and/or energy in advertisingtheir departures.The occurrence of calls or other conspicuousrecruitment displays by leaderswould not be predicted in a parasiticsystem.Attempts by leadersto slink away quietly and inconspicuouslywould be more likely (Bertram1978). 25 be selectedto render itself conspicuousor in- conspicuousto otherswhen it leavesthe colony on a foraging trip. Whenever a bird flies away from a colony,its actionsnecessarilyprovide information in the form of visual cues to any observersthat might be watching.Vocalizations, in contrast,need not be given and, when present,canbe taken as evidencefor the emission of ritualized signals subjectto positive selectionpressures(Smith 1977)of the sort couldoccurat a colonyin a contextof reciprocal expectedfrom a bird selectedto make itself altruism if a bird A leads another bird B to food conspicuous,but not from one that is selected on one trip, and bird B reciprocatesand leads to be inconspicuous. Gulls, including the bird A to a food patch on somelater trip (Krebs Black-billedGull, are highly vocal,and prelim1978). As pointed out by severalinvestigators inary observationsindicatedthat loud callsare with foragingflocks.The (Trivers 1971,Maynard Smith 1978,Davies and sometimesassociated Krebs 1978, Vehrencamp1979), however, sys- occurrence of calls and their function when tems based on reciprocalaltruism are usually emitted were used to test the above predicopen to invasion by "cheaters."A cheaterin tions. Someforagerscall when leavingthe colony.the present situation can be defined as a bird Reciprocal altruism.--Information that follows others transfer to their food but does not lead others to any rich patches it might find itself. As in the parasitic situation, leaders in a system based on reciprocal altruism could Black-billedGullsdepartingfrombreedingcolonies and roostscommonlyemitted loud calls asthey left. Similarcallswere heardfrom flocks en route to or from a feeding site and again facecostsassociatedwith leadingcompetitors upon arrival at a roost, colony, or foraging site. to the food supply and would not be predicted Callingwas alsocommonin flocksflying over to expend time or energy in advertising their departureswith calls or other recruitment displays. Cooperationbetweenselfishleadersand followers.--A system in which knowledgeable leadersand ignorant followersall benefit is a third context in which information could occur. This situation colonies transfer at could arise whenever leaders would benefit directly from the companyof flock membersaway from the colonysite, for example,if flockingen route to the foraginggroundsor while foragingwere beneficial as an antipredator adaptation (Bertram 1978),or if groupforagingwere beneficial (Rand 1954, Fisher 1958). When these away- the river prior to colony establishment (see Beer 1966 for similar observations). In all of thesecontexts,the loud, single-notecallsappeared to function as "contact" calls (Smith 1977),broadcastingthe currentlocationof the caller to others in the vicinity. These observations suffice to demonstrate that at least someforagerscall as they leave a colony.The mere existenceof loud calls,however, doesnot necessarilyprove that they function to attract and recruit others into a depart- ing foragingflock.Three setsof data relevant to the possiblefunctional role of these loud contactcallsin recruitingflock mateswere obtained at a largebreedingcolonyon the Ashley from-colonybenefitsof flockingoccur,leaders River, as outlined below. Leaderscall morethanfollowers.--Black-billed and followersshouldall benefit by leaving in a flock. If leaving as a member of a flock is Gulls leaving a colonyto obtain food normally beneficial, leaders should be selected to recruit departin long, stragglingflocksthat commonly followers.This predictionis oppositeto tha'• coalesceinto more dense units as they move derived from the previoustwo situations(par- away. Intervalsbetweenflocksrangefrom several secondsto severalminutes and are usually sufficient to permit a clear distinction between successiveflocks. In these studies, I used only flocksthat could be distinguishedas separate asitism and reciprocal altruism). TESTS OF PREDICTIONS The main predictions derived from the units. By sitting in a blind at the edge of the colonydirectlyunder the currentlyprevailing extent to which a potential flock leader should flight path takenby departingforagers,I could above theoretical considerations deal with the 26 record the number ROGER M. EVANS of instances in which flock [Auk, Vol. 99 lowers) into the flock. This result is entirely consistent with the interpretation that flock leadersderive some averagebenefit from having otherswith them when they leavethe colony, becauseit presumablywould be very easy for them to forego calling if recruiting others were not advantageous.The resultsprovide no support for the alternative possibilities that followers were parasitizing leaders that were As indicated above, not all leaders called. I at the colony for some other adaptive reason made use of this fact to test the hypothesisthat or were participating in a system maintained those leaders that did call gained followers solely by reciprocalaltruism. more often than those that did not. Of the 65 It remains possible that a mixture of paraleaders that called, 51 (78%) gained recruits. sitism, reciprocal altruism, and selfish coopOnly 40 (41%) of the 98 that did not callgained eration occurred at the colonies. For example, coloniesmay confersomeantipredatoror other recruits (X2 = 20.96, P < 0.001). Contact calls are attractive.--The results deas yet unidentified benefits,which couldfavor scribedabove suggestthat contactcallsemitted a degree of parasitic following of knowledgeby leadersfunctionto recruit followers.To test able leaders. This could still be compatible this hypothesis further, I recorded a series of with the emissionof loud callsby leadersif the contactcallsby birds departing from a colony benefits of leaving in flocks were sufficient to and then played them back at a different col- more than offsetany coststo leadersthat might ony. A Uher 4000 Report tape recorder, aug- otherwise derive from being parasitically folmented for playbackwith a 12-V amplifierand lowed. Benefits from flocking could also prean external, remotely placed 8-ohm speaker, sumably maintain a degree of reciprocation was used. For the playback experiments, I between leaders and followers, because the placed the loudspeaker approximately 50 m leaders on any one day could be followers from the colony,in a directionaway from that another day. The resultsare thus entirely combeing used by most departing foragersat that patible with a mixture of these three evolutime. I then noted the number of birds that tionary strategies, but only if the benefits of flew over toward the loudspeaker during a se- flockingaway from the colonyare sufficientto ries of 10 2-min playback intervals and com- more than offset potential costsof calling and pared this with the number that flew in the leading, thereby maintaining the evolutionary same direction in the immediately preceding stability of the system. Although the resultssuggestthat flock lead2-min periods. An average of 3.3 birds per 2 min flew .toward the speaker during the play- ers obtained some benefit from flocking away back periods, compared with 0.8 birds per 2 from the colony, they do not permit a distincmin during the immediately preceding, silent, tion between different types of potential bencontrol periods (T = 2.5, P < 0.01, Wilcoxon efits, for example those derived from protecmatched pairs test). These results do not pro- tion from predators while foraging or those vide evidence that the contact calls tested are derived from more effective group foraging necessarilymore or less effective than other apart from predator effects. Consideration of callsor displaysof Black-billedGulls, but they potential predator pressures and the foraging do provide evidence that the calls given by methods of Black-billed Gulls are required to departing foragersare attractiveto others and, assessthe relative merits of these two potential hence, could act to recruit flock mates. benefits of flocking. Defense from predators could be a relevant advantageof flocking in Black-billedGulls, but DISCUSSION such an interpretation is weakened appreciaThe main finding of this study was that bly by the historicallack of large mammalian when flocks of Black-billed Gulls leave a color other quadruped predatorsin New Zealand ony, the first bird out (defined as the leader) (reviewed in Beer 1966). Large avian predators often advertisesits departure by emitting loud are also rare (Falla et al. 1970). The predator calls that attract and recruit other foragers (fol- defensehypothesisis alsoweakenedfor Blackleaders did or did not emit loud calls as they passedby within hearingrange.The samedata were, then obtained for subsequentmembers of the departing flock (followers).Of 163 flock leaders, 65 (40%) emitted calls as they departed, while only 75 (17%) of a total of 443 followers called (X2 = 33.5, P < 0.0001). Leadersthat call gain followersmore often.- January1982] bills because FlockRecruitment in Gulls of their reduced levels 27 of mob- did not, a powerful mechanismwould exist for bing or other antipredator behavior compared identifying the most successfulbirds. This with their northern hemisphererelatives(Beer asymmetryfits well with what one would ex1966, pers. obs.). Benefit in the form of protec- pect from the leaders, because they are astion from predators cannot be ruled out but sumed to benefit by calling to recruit others remains highly unconvincing for Black-billed into their flocksfor purposesof group foraging Gulls. Benefits from group foraging are a more plausibleform of away-from-colonybenefitfor Black-billed Gulls. Elsewhere(Evans in prep), I describe instances where Black-billed Gulls locatecurrentfood sitesby cueing in on other foragers ("local enhancement," Thorpe 1963). away from the colony.This interpretationfails, however, to accountadequatelyfor the behavior of the silent leaders that are presumed not to know where food is located. These presumed ignorant leadersare the very birds that are expected to gain the most from group searchfor a new food patch or from other ben- Local enhancement as a mechanism for food efits of group foraging;hence, they shouldbe finding is well documentedand of widespread even more strongly selectedthan the knowloccurrencein gulls and other large, conspicu- edgeable birds to emit calls or other conspicous birds (Rand 1954; Sealy 1973; Scott 1973; uous recruitment signals when they initiate a Krebs 1974, 1978; Kushlan 1977; Porter 1981). departure from the colony. The interpretation Instancesin which foraging flockslanded se- that knowledgeable leaders call while ignorant lectivelyamongbirds alreadyat a foragingsite leadersdo not is thus not a viable explanation were observedrepeatedly on the study area. for a mixed-callingstrategyby leaders. Local enhancementalso occurredamong flock An alternative explanation for a mixed-callmembers already at terrestrial sites and in ing strategy by leaders is the reverse of the flocks foraging over water (see Stead 1932, for above scenerio. Because birds that do not know further documentation of local enhancement in where a current food patch is located should Black-billed Gulls). Group foraging could also benefit the most by recruiting others into a be a benefit when it results in a more efficient departing flock, ignorant leaders might be search of a localized area (Cody 1974). Other more likely to call. Knowledgeable leaders, possible on-site benefits of group foraging who already know where food is, might leave have been identified in birds (Rand 1954), but quietly. This interpretation,like the preceding I was unable to detect any evidence of their one, provides a mechanism whereby knowloccurrence in Black-billed Gulls. edgeableand ignorant leaderscould be readily Mixed vocal strategies.--Althoughthe inter- distinguished. In this case,selectivefollowing pretation that selfish leaders and followers all of silent flock leaderswould be favored by rebenefit from group foraging away from the col- cruits requiring information about current ony site appears to provide the best explana- food locations. This interpretation fails, howtion for the presenceof loud callsgiven by flock ever, becauseselectivefollowing of silent leadleaders at Black-billed Gull colonies, it is not ers is directly contradictedby the data, which immediatelyevident from the predictionsand show that vocal leaders were followed selecobservations considered thus far why flock tively. leaders should have exhibited a mixed vocal strategy, in which some called and others did What emerges from the above two scenarios is the conclusionthat the only evolutionarily not. Because fewer than half of the flock leaders stable strategy(ESS)is for ignorant flock leadactually called, evidently some complicating ers to do exactly what knowledgeable flock factorwas acting. A simple, unmixed strategy leadersdo, i.e. they shouldcallwith equal frewhereby all leaders benefit from recruiting quencywhen they leavethe colony.If ignorant others into the flock predicts the occurrenceof leaders call less than knowledgeable leaders, callsin virtually all cases. they will suffer a potential cost as a result of One possible explanation for the mixed-call- having fewer recruitswith them for group foring strategy of leaders that has relevance for aging. If ignorant leaderscall more frequently the InformationCenter hypothesisis that call- than knowledgeableleaders,they immediately ing was dependent upon foraging success.If set up a situation in which the following of leaders that were successful on their most resilent birds is favored, and, again, the ignorant cent foraging trip called and unsuccessfulbirds flock leaders will obtain fewer recruits than 28 RocER M. EVANS they would if they called at the same rate as knowledgeableleaders.Thus, differentialrates of callingby knowledgeableand ignorantflock leaders is not an ESS and is not a viable explanation for the observedmixed-calling strategy. A definitive explanation for the mixed-calling strategy of flock leaders may not be possible from existing data. The following explanation is offered as one possible hypothesis. My interpretation is that the rate of calling upon leaving a colonyor other assemblagewill depend on the amount of calling immediately preceding the time of departure of the bird in question.Consideredfunctionally,sucha negative-feedbacksystemwould mean that a bird departing the colony soon after other calling birds had departed would be more likely to encounter flock mates en route to or at a for- aging site than would a bird that left sometime after others had gone. The latter bird is less likely to encounter previously departed birds and would, on average,gain relatively more by recruitinga flockof its own. It would therefore be more stronglyselectedto call. The same interpretationwould predict that recruitscall less frequently, on average,than do flock leaders. Recruitsinto a flock are most certain of having others with them to forage and are also most likely to have been exposed very recently to calls by the leader. The fact that recruits did call less than flock leaders thus provides some supportfor this interpretation.Nothing in this hypothesis prohibits variations in absolute calling frequency. For example, if food were very difficult to find, the absolutelevel of calling might well go up, but a differentialin calling rates between different individuals, dependenton the amountof callingto which they were exposedin someprecedingtime interval, could still occur. Circumstantial evidence that absolutecalling rates may vary was provided by observationsat one colony that was abandoned soon after nest initiation. Calling was virtually incessantas birds wheeled about over the colony prior to abandonment,and an inspection of the region for severalkilometers adjacentto the colonyfailed to reveal any evidence of foraging gulls. This interpretation is thus consistentwith existing data for Blackbilled Gulls but requires further testing. Selectionpressures for colonialnesting.--Several ecologicalconditions that could selectfor nesting in colonieshave been advanced (Mock 1980). Protection from predators at the colony [Auk, VoL 99 site may be one important condition for some species(Lack 1968). In Black-billedGulls, the predator-protection hypothesis is not convincing due to a paucity of potential predators(see above, Beer 1966). As discussed above, the oc- currence of calls by flock leaders is also contrary to expectationif Black-billedGull colonies exist primarily for protectionfrom predatorsat the colony site. The Information Center hypothesis itself constitutes another potential explanation for the evolution or maintenance of roosts or col- onies (Ward and Zahavi 1973, Krebs 1978). Accordingto this hypothesis,benefitsderived from information transfer at the colony are adequate to accountfor the occurrenceof suchan assemblage.For this interpretation to be considered valid, other primary benefits at or away from the colony must be excluded as major evolutionary pressuresinvolved in the evolution or maintenance of colonies. Some form of reciprocal altruism or a colony based on assistanceto kin (e.g. Waltz in press)seems essentialif information transfer at the colony is to be the primary evolutionary explanation for colonies. As discussed above, a system based on reciprocalaltruism seemsentirely inadequate, becauseit is open to cheatersand hence is not an ESS (see Waltz in press, for similar conclusions). That kin selection is a viable mechanism whereby information transfer of the sort envisagedby the Information Center hypothesis could select for coloniesis a definite possibilitybut must remain in doubt until evidence for selective following of kin is obtained. Coloniesas assemblypointsfor group foraging.--The resultsof this study suggestthat a much simpler mechanism for the transfer of information about current food supplies could be involved in the evolution or maintenance of colonies than that required by the Information Center hypothesis.The finding that the pattern of calling by flock leaders at Blackbilled Gull colonies is best interpreted as indicating that all flock members, leaders and followers alike, benefit from leaving in a flock suggeststhe simple alternativethat coloniesof this speciesfunctionas assemblypointswhere dispersedforagerscan reunite for purposesof subsequent group feeding activities. Because the very nature of efficient searchfor patchy and unpredictable food clumps necessitatesa degree of dispersalover the habitat, foragers January1982] FlockRecruitment in Gulls run the risk of becoming more and more 29 evolutionary approach (J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies, Eds.). Sunderland, Sinauer Assoc. widely dispersedastheymoveaboutsearching for goodfood patches.To ensurethat excessive Cot)Y, M. L. 1974. Optimization in ecology.Science 183: 1156-1164. dispersal does not occur (i.e. to maintain a population sufficiently large to ensure maxi- DAVIES, N. B., & J. R. KREBS. 1978. Introduction: ecology,natural selectionand socialbehaviour. mum net benefits of group foraging), I suggest Pp. 1-18 in Behaviouralecology,an evolutionary that the periodic return of dispersedforagers approach (J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies, Eds.). to a centralassemblypoint can be favored and, Sunderland, Sinauer Assoc. hence, can provide a selectionpressurefavor- DAWSON,E. W. 1958. Food of young Black-billed ing the evolution or maintenance of roosts or Gulls (Larusbulleri)in a breedingcolony,North colonies. In Black-billed Gulls, locating food Canterbury. Notornis 8: 32-38. patchesby cueingin on others(localenhance- EtaLEN,S. T., & N.J. DEMONC. 1975. Adaptive significanceof synchronizedbreedingin a colonial ment) seemsto be a particularlylikely benefit bird: a new hypothesis. Science188: 1029-1031. derived from flock foraging (see above). Significantly, local enhancementhas also been EVANS,R. M. 1970. Parental recognition and the "mew call" in Black-billed Gulls (Larus bulleri). noted in the same speciesfor which the conAuk 87: 503-513. cept of Information Centers has been invoked (e.g. Ward 1965; Ward and Zahavi 1973; Krebs FALLA, R. A., R. B. SIB$ON, & E.G. TURBOTT. 1970. A field guide to the birds of New Zealand. Lon- 1974, 1978). don, Collins. It shouldbe emphasizedthat the hypothesis FISHER,J. 1958. Evolution and bird sociality. Pp. that colonies could have evolved or are main- 71-83 in Evolution as a process(J. Huxley, A. C. Hardy, and E. B. Ford, Eds.). London, Allen and tained as assembly points for subsequent Unwin. group foraging does not preclude the occurKREBS, J. R. 1974. Colonial nesting and socialfeedrenceof informationtransferat the colony.The ing as strategiesfor exploitingfood resourcesin presentinterpretationoffersan alternativeway in which food-related the Great Blue Heron (Ardea herodias).Behav- benefits could select for colonies. Once formed, colonies could be ex- ploited for information transfer in a manner consistent with the Information Center hypothesis.This interpretationmeansthat information transfer at colonies, if it occurs (and iour --. 51: 99-134. 1978. Colonialnestingin birds, with special reference to the Ciconiiformes. Pp. 299-314 in Wading birds (A. Sprunt, IV, J. C. Ogden, and S. Winckler, Eds.). New York, National Audubon Soc. Res. Rept. No. 7. that remains to be proven), is at most a sec- KUSHLAN, J. A. 1977. The significanceof plumage colourin the formationof feedingaggregations ondary influence on the evolution of colonyof Ciconiiforms. Ibis 119: 361-364. based foraging systemssuch as that exhibited LACK, D. 1968. Ecological adaptationsfor breeding by Black-billedGulls (cf. Wittenberger1981). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS in birds. London, Methuen and Co. MAYNARD SMITH, J. 1978. The evolution of behav- ior. Sci. Amer., September: 176-192. MOCK,D. W. 1980. "Information centers"as a prileavefrom the University of Manitoba in 1979-1980. mary function of heron coloniality. Colonial I thank John Warham and the Department of ZoolWaterbird Group, 4th Annual Meeting, Ottawa. ogy, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Pp. 10-11 (abstract). Zealand for the use of facilities. Financial support PORTER,J. M. 1981. The dynamics of seabird mulwas provided by a travel grant and operatinggrant tispecies flocks in Barkley Sound, British Cofrontthe NaturalSciences and EngineeringResearch lumbia. Unpublished M.Sc. thesis. Winnipeg, Council of Canada, Ottawa, Canada. I thank J. Brian Manitoba, Univ. Manitoba. O'Malley, DouglasMock, and EdwardWaltz for their RAND,A. L. 1954. Socialfeeding behaviorof birds. commentson various aspectsof the study. Fieldiana: Zoology 36: 1-71. Scott, J. M. 1973. Resourceallocation in four synLITERATURE CITED topic speciesof marine diving birds. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. Corvallis, Oregon, BEER,C. G. 1966. Adaptationsto nestinghabitat in the reproductive behaviour of the Black-billed Oregon StateUniv. Gull Larus buIleri. Ibis 108: 394-410. SEALY,S. G. 1973. Interspecificfeedingassemblages of marine birds off British Columbia. Auk 90: BERtRAm4, B.C. R. 1978. Living in groups:predators 796-802. and prey. Pp. 64-96 in Behaviouralecology,an This study was conductedwhile I was on research 30 ROGERM. EVAN.S cation (P. Marler and J. G. Vandenbergh, Eds.). SMtTH,W. J. 1977. The behavior of communicating. Cambridge,Massachusetts, Harvard Univ. Press. STEAV, E. F. 1932. The life histories of New Zealand birds. London, Search Publ. Co. Ltd. THORPE,W. H. 1963. Learning and instinct in animals. Cambridge,Massachusetts, Harvard Univ. Press. New York, Plenum. WALTZ,E. C. In press. Resourcecharacteristicsand the evolution of information-centers. Amer. Natur. WARD, P. 1965. Feeding ecologyof the Black-faced Dioch Quelea queleain Nigeria. Ibis 107: 173214. TRIVERS,R. L. 1971. The evolution of reciprocalaItruism. Quart. Rev. Biol. 46: 35-57. VEHREN.CAMP,S. L. 1979. The roles of individual, [Auk, Vol. 99 --, & A. ZAHAVL 1973. The importance of certain assemblagesof birds as "information-centers" for food-finding. Ibis 115: 517-534. kin, and group selectionin the evolution of soJ. F. 1981. Animal social behavior. ciality. Pp. 351-394 in Handbook of behavioral WITTEN'BERGER, Boston, Duxbury Press. neurobiology3: social behavior and communi- The Frank M. Chapman Memorial Fund of the American Museum of Natural History is administered by a committeethat meetstwiceannuallyto reviewapplications for grantsand fellowships.While thereis no restrictionon who may apply,the Committeeparticularlywelcomesapplicationsfromgraduatestudents; managementprojectsand projectsby seniorinvestigators are seldomfunded. Applicationsshouldbe submitted not later than 15 Februaryand 15 September.Applicationformsmay be obtainedfrom the Frank M. ChapmanMemorial Fund Committee,The AmericanMuseum of Natural History, Central Park West at 79th St., New York, New York 10024. Chapmangrantsduring 1981,totalling$44,173with a mean of $485, were awarded to: Riley J. Atkins, Roost microclimate, huddling behavior, and hypothermiaon the nocturnalenergybudgetof the Common Bushtit; Graeme Charles Backhurst, The Ngulia bird ringing project;KennethN. Baker,Distribution of bird species abundances and mor- phologiesalong elevationalgradientsin a southern Appalachianmountain range; Gregory Ball, Sexual partitioningof parentalbehavior in Barn Swallows; G. ThomasBancroft,Energeticsof postnatalgrowthin Boat-tailedGracklesrelative to sex;Wylie C. Barrow, Jr., Comparativeecologyof two sympatricEmpidonax flycatcherscoexistingin an easterndeciduousforest; StevenR. Beissinger,Mate desertionin the Everglade Kite (Rostrhamus sociabilis); L. David BeIetsky,Variation and information content in the vocalizations of femaleRed-wingedBlackbirds(Agelaiusphoeniceus); Robert E. Bleiweiss,Taxonomyand systematicsof Andean hummingbirds; David Edward Blockstein, Reproductivebehavior and parental investment in the Mourning Dove (Zenaida macroura);Jerram L. Brown, Cooperationand competitionin the Mexican Jay;Robert N. Buchsbaum,Effectsof phenolicacids andnutrientinputson the feedingecologyof geesein saltmarshes;TerenceA. Burke,Evolutionarygenetics of the House Sparrow, Passerdomesticus, in northern India; Gregory S. Butcher,Sexualdimorphism in the colorand behavior of orioles(genusIcterus);William Carmen, Juvenile dispersal, flocking behavior, and habitat use in the California Scrub Jay (Aphelocoma coerulescens californica);JohnH. Carothers,Foraging Michael D. Carter, Socialorganizationand parasitic habits of breeding Bronzed Cowbirds (Molothrus aeneus);Martha Leah Chaiken, Starling colony on groundsof Stroud'swater researchcenterin Avondale, Pa.; Robert L. Curry, Evolution and ecologyof communal breeding in Galapagos Mockingbirds; Veronique Delesalle,Mate choiceand reproductive biology of the House Finch, Carpodacus mexicanus, with emphasison the role of male plumage variabil- ity; TodRobertDeLong,Nocturnalbehaviorof nesting Long-earedOwls (Asiootus);David F. DeSante, Stability and dynamicsof a subalpinebreedingbird community; Jennifer Shopland Dillon, Effect of mixed-speciesflockson the foragingsuccessand interspecificinteractions of two speciesof territorial warblers (Parulidae);StefanDontchev, Geographical variation and subspecificstatusof Passeriformesfrom Bulgaria; Peter J. Dunne, Owl migration through Cape May Point, New Jersey;Bonita Eliason,Test of mating systemtheory in the BlackpollWarbler, Dendroicastriata;Richard Donald Elliot, Nesting dispersion and egg predation in the Lapwing Vanellusvanellus;David N. Ewert, Songvariationof the Lincoln's Sparrow;JonFjeldsg,Adaptationsfor ecologicalisolation in grebes;RobertC. Fleischer,Host specificity and egg mimicry in the Brown-headedCowbird; LeonardA. Freed, Evolution of clutchsize in a tropical passerine;DouglasC. Gayou, Socialbehavior of the Green Jay in Texas; Steven Michael Goodman, Systematics and distribution of Prinia gracilisin Egypt; BradleyM. Gottfried,Effectof eggsequence,clutch size, and breeding seasonon egg characteristicsof Gary R. Graves, and aggression in nectarivorous Maul Drepanidids; House Sparrows(Passerdomesticus); (continuedon p. 57)