National Self-Images, Perception of Enemies, and Conflict Strategies: Psychopolitical Dimensions of International Relations Author(s): Noel Kaplowitz Source: Political Psychology, Vol. 11, No. 1 (Mar., 1990), pp. 39-82 Published by: International Society of Political Psychology Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3791515 Accessed: 24/08/2010 04:42 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ispp. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. International Society of Political Psychology is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Political Psychology. http://www.jstor.org Political Psychology, Vol. 11, No. 1, 1990 National Self-Images, Perception of Enemies, and Conflict Strategies:PsychopoliticalDimensions of InternationalRelations Noel Kaplowitz' This essay presents a psychopolitical theory of international conflict. National self-images and perceptions of enemies are explicated and analyzed as determinants of various types of conflict behavior. Self-images include how a people sees itself, that which it most likes about itself, that about which it is most disturbed, the ways in which it may want to change, perceptions of the nation's history, conceptions of national purpose and interest, and views of the nation's power and limits. Regarding perceptions of enemies, the theory emphasizes the differences with which parties perceive the same issues, events, policies, and peoples, and suggests such gaps in perception as keys to understanding conflict behavior and interactions. A typology of conflict strategies is then elaborated, rangingfrom totalism to appeasement. It takes into account both manifest aims and latent attitudes. Each strategy is explicated in terms of the means emphasized, the characteristicbeliefs about conflict entailed, and the self-images and perceptions of enemies which are its sources. A series of hypotheses is presented concerning the relationships between different types of self-images and perceptions of enemies, on the one hand, and various conflict strategies, on the other. The ultimate purpose is to understand what makes for constructive and destructive conflict outcomes. KEYWORDS:psychopolitical;self-images;perceptionsof enemies;conflict strategies;constructiveand destructiveconflict. 'PoliticalScienceDepartment,Universityof California,Davis, Davis, California95616. 39 0162-895X/90/0300-0039$06.00/1 ? 1990 International Society of Political Psychology 40 Kaplowitz INTRODUCTION This essay elaboratesthe foundationsof a psychopoliticaltheory of internationalconflict. The term "psychopolitical"refersto psychological, social-psychological,and culturalfactorsas they impingeupon politicalbehavior.The variablesexplicatedarenationalself-images;perceptionsof enemies;strategiesof conflict;and attitudes,manifestandlatent,towardissues. A series of hypotheses is presented concerning the connections between differenttypes of self-imagesand perceptionsof enemies,and conflictstrategies.Thestrategiesrangefromtotalistandlong-runtotalist,through and conciliatory.A related competitiveanddeterrent,to firm-but-cooperative work analyzesthe reciprocalinteractioneffects of partiesto conflict using these different strategiesand is concernedwith how the attitudesand behaviorsof one actoraffect those of the other(Kaplowitz,1984).The dimensions of the theoryexplicatedin this essayare indispensableto the effective and comprehensiveanalysisof such interactioneffects. The ultimateaim is to understandwhatmakesfor constructiveand destructiveoutcomesin conflict situations. Severalframeworksof analysishave dealt with a plethoraof foreign policy determinantsincluding,for example, militarycapabilities,political andinternational structures,bureaucracies, systems(Eastet al., 1979;Wilkenfeld et al., 1980).As a consequenceof relatedstudies, studentsof international relations(IR) now have a quite refined and elaboratedview of the many variableswhich can influenceforeign policy. Knowledgeaboutwhatmakesfor constructiveanddestructiveoutcomes in conflictsituations,however,is sorelylacking.Constructiveoutcomesmay be said to occurwhenall partiesemergeat least somewhatsatisfiedand feel enhanced.Destructiveoutcomesoccurwhenthe partiesare dissatisfiedand feel diminished(Deutsch,1973,p. 17). This conceptualdistinctionis particularlyuseful for studentsof internationalrelations.It assumesthat conflict is ubiquitous-as does the still-dominantschoolof PoliticalRealismin IRbut thatit is approachedin differentwaysandthatthesearecrucialin determiningthat most importantof factors-the outcome. In a nuclearworld, understandingthe determinantsof constructiveand destructiveoutcomes should be a centralconcernof both scholarsand practitioners. Deterrencetheory,the majorbody of workin the IR literatureon how to deal with adversaries,as Jervis(1979,pp. 292-93)pointsout in his review and critique,does not deal with what is assumedhereto be that most basic and importantof issues-"transforminghostilerelationsinto peacefulones." Deterrencetheory focuses on how adversariesmay be precludedfrom encroachingon one'sown vitalinterests.But it does not addressthe questions, Psychopolitical Dimensions of InternationalRelations 41 "When can others goals be changed. . . ?" and "When can others be persuaded to adopt a 'problem solving' approach. .. ?" Indeed, Jervis adds, astoundinglybut correctly,"Thereis littlein the internationalrelationsliterature to provideeven a start to answeringthese questions." A varietyof conflictstrategieshavebeenexploredon the interpersonal levelandthroughlaboratoryexperiments,the Prisoner'sDilemma,andcomputerizedgames(Axelrod, 1980;Deutschet al., 1967,pp. 345-60;Lengand level,howWheeler,1979,pp. 658-65).Systematicstudieson the international and ever,arelacking.Thisessayanalyzespsychological,social-psychological, culturalvariableswhichaffect the possibilitiesfor cooperationandconstructive outcomesin internationalconflicts.2 COGNITIVE, DEPTH-PSYCHOLOGICAL, AND BEHAVIORAL ANALYSIS This enterprisewill ultimatelyrequirea synthesisof cognitive,depthpsychological,and behavioralapproachesin psychology,as they bearupon the analysisof internationalrelations.Put schematically,cognitivepsychology focuses on perceptionsand beliefs, and the ways in whichthey are orof behavior.Behaviorismemphasizes ganizedand processed,as determinants the environmentas it operatesthroughreinforcementand resultsin learning. Depth-psychologyfocuses on needs, emotions, and resultantdefenses. Until now, each of theseapproacheshas most often beenappliedseparately in psychologicalstudies of IR. Early psychoanalyticwork emphasizedthe interpretationof war as a resultof the projectionand displacementof aggressiveimpulses(Durbinand Bowlby, 1939;Glover, 1933).Thisapproachdid not directlyfurtherempirical research,however, or help in the formulationof effective policies for constructiveconflictresolution.It did not accountfor the substantialvariation in the behaviorof nations, the many periodsof peace in international relationsat the of cooperative anyof the determinants 2Verylittleworkhas beendoneconcerning level.Jervis(1978),in one of the few pertinentarticles,has theorizedaboutseveral international structural and situationalvariableswhichaffectthe possibilitiesfor cooperativebehavior.These and technological includethe advantageof defenseoveroffense,as influencedby geographical factors;andthe natureof the payoffin conflictsituations,as determined by factorswhichaffect the tangiblecostsand gainsof aggressionand cooperation.In a laterwork(1982b)he suggests additionalstructuralfactorswhichmakesecurityregimesdifficultto achieve.Theseincludethe in manysecurityissues-whatonesidewinstheotherloses(asin disputes inherentcompetitiveness the highstakesinvolved;the difficultiesin measuringsecurity;and the costliness overterritory); of mistakes. 42 Kaplowitz history, or the many pairs and groups of nations which enjoy cooperative relations. It did not help to answer the crucial question-what accounts for the difference? At the same time, this earlierwork did generate several highly illuminating ideas. The most important of these for political psychology in general and psychology and IR in particular is that political attitudes and behavior can be generated and affected by needs, emotions, and images of which actors may be only dimly aware, if at all; and that such attitudes and behaviors can serve the defensive function of protecting against underlying feelings and dispositions which could be threatening, anxiety-producing, or damaging to self-esteem if they became manifest. More recent developments in psychoanalytic theory have elaborated the idea of an "extended self' which comes to include the nation and its symbols. Related work illuminates connections between injury to the "group self' or nation and damage to individual self-esteem, with consequent aggressive, hate, and even rage reactions. This rich and suggestivebody of theory is potentially useful for further explicating the concept of national self-imagery developed in this essay and for extending our understanding of the ways in which self-imagery is related to political attitudes and behavior.3 At the outset of the "behavioralrevolution" in political science, a group of scholars basing themselves, in part, on a preceding generation of cultureand-personality research-itself heavily influenced by depth-psychological theory-elaborated the concept of political culture as those aspects of the more general culture, both manifest and underlying, which are specifically related to political behavior (Almond and Verba, 1963; Pye, 1962, 1965; Verba, 1965). There has been little effort, however, to systematically relate political culture to international behavior. The subsequent focus upon attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs is a necessary and useful step in the direction of specificity. Interestingly enough, there have been few behaviorist studies of international relations - despite the enormous impact of behaviorism in psychology (Brewer, 1975). At the same time, a host of implicit assumptions and sometimes explicit theories about the effects of threat and punishment(though not promises and rewards) suffuses the strategic literature. A psychopoliti- 3A review of this substantialbody of literatureis beyond the scope of the presentessay. Heinz Kohut, the major theorist of self-psychology, reflects upon its applicationsto the humanitiesand social sciences in a useful volume edited by Strozier (1985). A capsule statement of the core of the theory, as it may be applied to internationalconflict, is presentedby the Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry(1978, pp. 408-422). Integrationof recent psychoanalyticself psychology with the psychopolitical theory developed in this essay remains a separate and important task. Psychopolitical Dimensions of InternationalRelations 43 cal theoryof internationalconflictmusttakeinto accountthe effectsof both negativesanctionsandpositiveinducementson the tangiblecostsandbenefits for an adversary.In this respect, it will integratethe assumptionsof behaviorismregardingthe environment,includingother actors, as conditioners of behavior.4 In recentyearstherehas been a trendin psychologyand IR studiesof thinkingin purelycognitiveterms, so that "psychological"and "cognitive" have been close to becoming synonymous. Jervis (1976) pioneered in thoroughlyexaminingcognitivepsychologyand bringingit to bear, in illuminatingways,uponthe understandingof foreignpolicydecision-making. His instructivestudy openeda whole domainof inquiryin the field of psychologyand IR. At the sametime, "cold"cognitivefactorswereemphasized (1976,p. 3), to the neglectof affectivesourcesand componentsof attitudes. Jervis'work touches briefly upon desiresand fears, as they influenceperceptions(1976, pp. 355-81).It does not deal with other kindsof affect, and relatedneedsand motiveswhichcan influenceperceptionsand policiessuch as humiliation,hate, envy, contempt,rage, revenge,powerlessness,shame, admiration,and esteem.Nor does it treatrelatedculturalvariables.Larson theimportanceof cognitiveprocessesin a workwhich (1985)furtherunderlines to bear the brings upon studyof AmericanCold Warpolicy, the most recent developmentsin this field, in particularschematheory. Cognitivefactors are cruciallyimportantdeterminantsof decision-makingand must be taken into accountin a psychopolitical conflict.At the same theoryof international a more is if the time, integratedapproach required insights and riches of the broaddomainof psychologyare to be most fully and fruitfullyused to deepenthe understandingand make more effectivethe practiceof international relations.5 4Neo-Realisttheory in IR (Waltz, 1979) relatedlyemphasizesthe structureof the internationalsystem and prevailingpower configurationsas primarydeterminantsof national behavior. Since the internationalsystem can be thought of as the environmentwithin which national behavior occurs, neo-Realismis, in this sense, congruentwith behaviorism.I am indebetedto an anonymousreviewer for calling attentionto this importantsimilarity.At the same time, there have been very few studies of foreign policy which explicitlyfocus on reinforcementand learning-key processesin behavioral psychology. The power relationsof nations are factored into that part of our theory which deals with interaction effects and outcomes (Kaplowitz, 1984, pp. 368-400). 5In a later work which deals with the omissions of deterrence theory, Jervis (1979: 296-297, 305-306) recognizes the influence of culture upon foreign policy. One of the fundamental ommissions of such theory is that it has not taken into account how the political culture and psychology of an opponent may lead him to respond to deterrence attempts in ways which one has not anticipated. Rather, this work has tended to project onto opponents assumptions about conflict behavior that grow out of one's own political culture. In a retrospective look at his study on misperceptions, Jervis (1982a; pp. 6-10) suggests that motivated errors and related needs, values, and affect play a larger role than he had allowed for in his earlier cognitive work. In still more recent essays (1985a, 1985b), he further stresses these ideas. 44 Kaplowitz PREVIOUS FRAMEWORKS OF ANALYSIS IN PSYCHOLOGY AND IR Virtually all frameworks which have attempted to bring psychology to bear upon international relations have focused on perceptions of enemies and beliefs about conflict on the individual level, as determinants of foreign policy decision-making within single nations, on one side of a conflict situation. Brecher et al. (1969), in a framework which has informed considerable research, elaborated an "attitudinal prism" and a set of "elite images" that influence decision-making. They differed from more orthodox IR analysts in their emphasis upon perceptions of the traditionally considered variables of power capabilities, domestic politics, and the international system. In a subsequent work, Brecher (1980, pp. 30, 102) briefly alludes to what we here term "national self-images." The focus, however, is on perceptions of the enemy and conflict beliefs, and their connections to foreign policy which are analyzed in illuminating ways. The "Operational Code" framework explicated by George (1969) also suggests attention to particularenemy perceptions and conflict beliefs. Much subsequent research has indicated a relationship between these variables and decision-making(George, 1979; Holsti, 1976). Thus, the "OperationalCode," which derives from Leites' (1953) classic study of Bolshevik perceptions and beliefs and which, as richly explicated by George, has become an important construct in the evolving field of psychology and IR, can help to illuminate the sources of foreign policy. But it cannot, by itself, explain the interaction dynamics between say, the Soviet Union and the United States, or the outcomes in conflicts between them. While useful for understanding decisionmaking, this framework does not focus upon conflict relations and does not address the questions: how can states effectively produce change in adversaries; how can they move them from hostile to more cooperative approaches; and how can they increase the probability of constructive outcomes in conflict situations? At the same time, Leites, George, and Holsti-the formulators, explicators, and operationalizers of the code, and pioneers in the psychopolitical study of international relations-have provided a rich and indispensable basis for the present study. Several scholars have examined the impact of cognitive structure (in contrast to the content of perceptions) upon decision-making. They have found that cognitive complexity is related to the range of behaviors suggested by actors regarding international problems (Shapiro and Bonham, 1973) and that integrative complexity diminishes preceding the outbreak of war (Suedfeld et al., 1977). But they have not suggested that complexity is related to policies likely to result in constructive outcomes, a key concern here. However, complexity has also been related to favorable responses regarding Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 45 other nations(Hermann,1980)and such responsesare, indeed, likelyto be associatedwith constructiveoutcomes. Extrapolatingfrom this work, one can hypothesizethat highcomplexityis likelyto be associatedwiththe more intermediatestrategiesexplicatedlaterin this essayandwithconstructiveoutcomes, and low complexitywith the more extremestrategiesand with destructiveoutcomes. The studyof personalityand foreignpolicyhas a substantialhistoryto which severalscholarshave recentlyadded. Hermann(1980)found aggressivebehaviorassociatedwithhighsuspiciousness, andwiththe needfor power and control;and conciliatorybehaviorwithlow suspiciousness,and withthe need for affiliationand friendlyrelations.Etheredge(1978)found support of threatand force, and exclusiveas opposedto inclusivepoliciesregarding the SovietUnionrelatedto hostility,competitiveness,ambition,distrust,low self-esteem,and the need for dominance.Their dichotomousconceptions of aggressiveand conciliatorybehavioroverlap,albeitin a veryroughway, with the conflict strategiesexplicatedbelow. The personalitytraitsto which they point may thereforebe associatedwith particularstrategiesand with differentoutcomes, suggestinga potentiallink betweensuch work and the theory developedhere. Focusingon the personalitiesof individualscan generateimportantand usefuldata for decision-makersas they confrontparticularadversaries.The predictivepower of this level of analysisconcerningstate behaviorand its potential for theory-buildingregardinginternationalrelations is limited, however,by the particularandoften idiosyncraticpersonalityconfigurations of leaderswho happento be in power. Psychologicallyinformedtheories of internationalrelationsthus need to establishthe ways in which conflict strategiesand outcomesaredeterminedby perceptions,beliefs,and attitudes held by parties and groups within nations, and which characterizebroad schoolsof thoughtregardingforeignpolicythattranscendindividualleaders. THE PRESENTTHEORY The theorywe explicate,thoughit buildsupon previousefforts, differs from and adds to them in severalways. First, it introduces,explicates,and emphasizesnationalself-imagesas a crucialvariabledeterminingconflictbehavior. Second, with regardto perceptionsof enemies,it focuses on differencesin perceptionsbetweenpartiesto conflict regardingthe samepeoples, issues, and events, and on the implicationsof these differencesfor conflict dynamics.Previouswork has been concernedwith correctversusincorrect 46 Kaplowitz perceptions. Third, it explicates the relationship between various kinds of self-images and perceptions of enemies on the one hand, and different conflict strategies on the other, and presents a series of related hypotheses. Fourth, it suggests the exploration of modal images and attitudes which transcend individual leaders and are likely to change only gradually. In this way, the theory and related policy implications are not dependent upon and circumscribed by turnovers in individual leadership and by the idiosyncracies of particular personalities. The theoretical dimensions explicated in this essay are requirementsfor analysis of the reciprocal interaction effects of parties to conflict using different strategies, as these strategies produce constructive and destructive outcomes (Kaplowitz, 1984). Such work necessitates the researcher'sengagement in the analytical tasks delineated here regarding the perceptual and motivational underpinningsof conflict strategies. If scholars and policy-makersthen come to understand the images, perceptions, and beliefs which determine such strategies, and the outcomes which result from interactions of parties using different strategies, we will be in a better position to construct theory, make predictions, and design policies for bringing about constructive outcomes.6 NATIONAL SELF-IMAGES How a People Sees Itself Robert Lane (1969, p. 1), in his work on the needs and motives which energize political thought and behavior, remarks that if you ask a man why he believes what he does, he is likely to tell you about the world and not about himself: ". . . he sees things that way because they are that way." In the same way, a foreign policy decision-maker, in explaining why his nation behaves the way it does, is likely to tell you about others -their inten6A detaileddiscussionof the differentlevelsof analysisin IR studiesis beyondthe scopeof the andstatelevelsinfluence presentessay.We notelaterthata varietyof factorsat the international conflicts.At the sametime, it is clearthat not all stateseven foreignpoliciesand international withthesamepoliticalandeconomiccharacteristics behavein the samewaysin similarsituations. Not all democraticor authoritarian, capitalistor socialist,largeor smallstatesbehavesimilarly. Nor is the relativelyanarchicinternational systema sufficientconditionfor waror evencompetitivebehaviorsin all placesat all times.Hence,analysisof the psychological andculturalvariables of foreignpolicyand the amelioration of internaexplicatedhereis crucialto an understanding tionalconflicts. Dimensionsof International Relations Psychopolitical 47 tions, their power, their character."We behave the way we do," he may understandablydeclare,"becausewe operatein an anarchicalworld where securityand, ultimately,survivalareat stake,wherepoweris thereforewhat counts, and because we are faced with dangerous, deceptive, and unscrupulousenemies." Thedispositionto look outwardat othersas an explanationfor one'sown behaviorhas been expressed,interestinglyenough, even in the psychology and IR literature,whereone might have expectedother emphasesas well, since psychologydealsto such an importantextentwith the internalcauses of behavior.Yet, in the psychologyand IR literature,"perceptionsof the enemy"has been the predominantfocus. To the extentthat this enterprise has penetratedconventionalmodesof analysisin IR, it has beenvia this construct. Self-imageshave been largely ignored. Thus, in Holsti's (1976, pp. is not evenmen130-43)thoroughliteraturereview,the concept"self-image" tioned. Nor has there been much considerationof self-imagerysince. Yet, class workin politicaland social psychology(Adornoet al., 1950;Erikson, 1950;Lane, 1962;Lasswell,1930;Leites,1953)andthe manyresultingstudies [reviewedby Sniderman(1975, pp. 12-15)]have indicatedthat self-images andself-esteemcan havecrucialbearinguponpoliticalattitudesandbehavior. It is a basic assumptionof the theorypresentedhere that the ways in whichpeoplesand groupswithinnationssee themselvesinfluencetheirconflict behaviorin directand indirect,consciousand unconsciousways. Particularlyimportantare the most salientaspectsof nationalself-imagery,the positiveand negativeboundariesof suchimagery-i.e., what a peoplelikes and dislikesabout itself, how it views its history, the resultant"lessons"it has learned,its aspirationsand desires,the ways in which it may want to change,its conceptionsof nationalpurposeandinterest,and its perceptions of its powers and limits. Nationalself-imagesare thus an importantdimensionof politicalculture and are transmittedby agentsof that culture.At the same time, they are also psychologicalvariablesin that they are ultimatelyheld by individuals. The explorationof nationalself-imagerywill thus integrateinto studies of internationalrelations,an importantcomponentof politicalculture.The level of analysis,however,will be imagesand perceptions-variableswhich can be moreeasilyoperationalized,and whoseinfluenceupon policycan be more readilydiscerned,than is the case regardingthe moregeneralconcept of political culture. Some of the earlierclassic worksin politicalpsychologypointedto a relationshipbetweenself-imagesand conflict behavior.Erikson(1950, pp. 48 Kaplowitz 365-82) presented intriguing hypotheses about Soviet behavior under Stalin as reactions to Bolshevik images of traditional Russia, which included fatalism, pessimism, soul-bearing, and self-pity. He suggested a connection between such images and the Soviet effort at building a new "imagery of steel [which] suggests incorruptiblerealism and enduring, disciplined struggle"and to "behavior which underscores without cessation the incorruptibility of Bolshevik perception, the long range of its vision, its steel-like clarity of decision, and the machine-like firmness of action." Leites (1953) also pointed to connections between self-images, perceptions of enemies, and strategies of conflict, although he did not explicitly use these terms or centrally organize his work around these concepts. He did, however, discuss a whole array of Russian self-images and Bolshevik perceptions of traditional Russia and its intelligentsia. These included tendencies toward persistent doubt, emotionalism, depressed passivity, substitution of talk for action, procrastination,credulity, and fatalism. Leites linked these images to such Bolshevik maxims, tactics, and "Operational Codes" as emphasizing the correctness of the party line; excluding feelings from political calculations; guarding against perceiving facts in terms of wishes and fears; being blunt, sharp, brief, and rude; actively struggling; and suspecting others. What counted in determining Bolshevik conflict behavior was not the degree of validity in these images-but rather that these were the Bolshevik images. Such images can change although, in most cases, quite slowly. In such cases, conflict behavior will also change. Ralph K. White (1970) later discussed virile and moral self-images among parties to the Vietnam War and their relationships to militant positions. Virile images include military overconfidence, preoccupation with prestige, fear of humiliation, and concern with declining status. Moral images comprise attributions of great virtue, truthfulness, blamelessness, and peaceableness to the self. Despite these suggestive examples, there has been very little work which relates national self-imagery to foreign policy and conflict processes. There are several reasons, I believe, why this crucial factor has been ignored. First, self-images are less obviously and directly related to foreign policy than are perceptions of others who are the objects of that policy. There is a natural, cognitively-rooted tendency, as attribution theory emphasizes, to point to others and to situational variables as explanations for one's own behavior. Haradstveit's work (1979, p. 55) suggests the refinement that actors tend to explain their own "good" behavior in dispositional terms but "bad"behavior in situational terms. Thus, there is still the tendency to ignore the self as an explanatory factor regarding that "bad" behavior which is so crucial in international relations. Second, there is often a powerful disposition, as depthpsychological theory emphasizes, to ignore the role of one's own underlying Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 49 needsand motivesin determiningattitudesand behavior,particularlywhen suchacknowledgmentwouldresultin discomfort,embarrassment,or anxiety. Third, the very idea that self-imagesaffect behaviorimpliesthat one's own nation may sometimesbe responsible,at least to some degree,for the intensity, severity,and even "irrational"natureof conflicts with othersconsiderations in the particularsensethat other-than-"objective" "irrational" or the behaviorof the opponentaccount for, or add a sharpedge to, the conflict.Fourth,earlierstudiesof self-imageryandconflictbehaviorby Adorno, Erikson, and Leites were to a substantialdegree psychoanalytically oriented-an approachsubsequentlyresistedin the social sciences. George (1969), who explicatedand effectivelymade the case for the continuingrelevanceof the "OperationalCode"construct,accordinglynoted that researcherscould explorethe relatedbeliefsand perceptionswithout necessarilyacceptingthe psychoanalytic premisesembeddedin the Leiteswork from whichthe approachwas derived.The "OperationalCode,"as formalized by George,stressesbeliefsaboutconflictand perceptionsof the enemy. It does not referto the nationalandgroupself-imagesor to the psychodynamic processesalludedto by Leites.At the sametimeGeorge(1969,pp. 195-96), in his accompanyingcommentary,notes the effects of unconsciousmotives and defenses againstthem upon manifest beliefs. National self-imagesmay also be exploredwithoutnecessarilyengaging in depth-psychological analysis.Thereare importantmanifestand cognitive aspectsof self-imagerywhich influenceinternationalbehavior, and there are sometimesquite directand readilyobservableconnectionsin this regard.The assumptionof a linkbetweenself-imagesand behavior,however, also pointsimplicitlyto underlyingdimensionsand processes.If self-images influencebehavior,they often do so indirectlyand in ways which are not so readilyrecognized.Moreover,particularself-imagesmay be generatedby underlyingneedsfor self-esteemandshapedby culturallyinfluencedrequirements for the achievementof such esteem. The expressionof such needs, in turn, may be affected by a varietyof defense mechanisms(Rosenberg, 1979, pp. 55-56). Otherunconsciousmotivesentailingneeds for powerand prestige-as ends in themselves-may also play a role in generatingselfimages.Widespreaddesiresto changewhat are perceivedto be undesirable characteristicsof the nation may generaterelatedneeds. These underlying aspectsof self-imagery,whichhave an importantaffectivecomponent,can be crucialin determiningpoliticalbehavior.National self-images,because they compriseimportantmanifestand underlying,cognitiveand affective dimensions,and becausethey influencebehaviorin directand indirect,conscious and unconsciousways, providea key constructfor bridgingthe gap betweencognitiveand depth-psychologicalanalysesof foreign policy attitudes and behavior. 50 Kaplowitz Analysis of self-images is required for a thorough understanding of the concerns which underlie policies. Such understanding, in turn, is essential for interpretingthe meaning of a state's behavior- a point increasinglyrecognized in psychology and IR studies (e.g., Hermann, 1985, pp. xviv, 3-21; Lebow and Stein, 1987, pp. 18, 45). Resultant knowledge may then be used in the formulation of appropriate strategies for dealing with adversaries. From work in political and social psychology, I have inferred two guiding propositions about the relationshipbetween self-images and conflict strategies: (1) the more grandiose the manifest, and negative the often underlying self-images, the greater the disposition toward a totalist strategy, one which requirestotal victory and implies total defeat for the other. Conversely, (2) the more positive the self-images, the greater the tendency toward a firm-bycooperative strategy, one which aims at mutually satisfying outcomes in conflict situations.7 These propositions are elaborated and refined in the discussion of conflict strategies below.8 It is not only the degree of grandiose or negative self-imagery which is important in determining conflict behavior. It is what is regarded as positive or what is disliked and why that is especially important in determining such behavior. Do leaders and peoples feel they have been too passive and weak, for example, too vulnerable to oppression, domination, humiliation, or even physical annihilation? Do they feel they can recoup periods of past greatness, that they deserve more in the present world, or that they can "do 7Forrelevantworkin politicaland socialpsychology,see Lane(1962,p. 156),citingRedland Wineman(1957)on the connectionbetweenweakego strengthandlow self-estimates,andaggressivepersonalities;Adornoet al. (1950),concerningthe relationshipbetweenego weakness and, by implication,low self-esteem,anda dispositiontowardoutgroupaggression;the related thesisin Levinson(1957);Erikson's(1956,p. 82) themeconcerningthe relationshipbetween whathe termsoutwornor unfinishedidentitiesandthe wagingof holywarsthroughunlimited worksuggeststhe means;Deutsch's(1962,p. 107)conversepropositionthat "Psychoanalytic capacityto understandratherthanto condemnis largelydeterminedby the individual'ssense of self-esteem,by his abilityto cope withthe externalproblemsfacinghim, and by his sense in overcominghis owndefects";Kohlberg's of resoluteness (1964,pp. 382-431)consonantfinding that self-esteemis relatedto the abilityto empathizewithothersandto moralconduct,as cited by Lane(1969, p. 193);White's(1970)discussionof virile(or omnipotent)and moral(or of militants;Etheredge's(1978)findings self-righteous)imagerywhichhe findscharacteristic regardingpersonalitycorrelatesof foreignpolicy attitudes;and the numerousreferencesin Sniderman(1975,pp. 12-15)concerningthe relationshipbetweenself-esteemand politicalbehavior. 8Animplicitcorollaryto the firstpropositionassumesa relationshipbetweenmanifestgrandiose and underlyingnegativeself-images.The reasoningentailedis that grandioseself-images areoftencompensations or low self-esteem for, or reactionformationsto, negativeself-imagery which is usuallyrepressedbut may sometimesbecomemanifest. Dimensionsof International Relations Psychopolitical 51 anything"they want if only they have the "will"to do it? The exploration of self-imagerythereforerequiresqualitativeanalysis. Quantitativeassessments may be useful for furthermeasuringintensitiesand for precisecomparisons(Holsti, 1976,pp. 136-37;George, 1979,p. 105).Knowledgeof the contentof such imagesis also crucialto the formulationof effectivestrategies for dealingwith adversariesand for movingconflictpatternsin cooperative directions. Negativeself-imageshavea specialimpactuponconflictbehavior.They often generatecompensatingdefensiveattitudeswhichareespeciallyevoked in conflictsituations.Conflictalso providesopportunitiesto proveself-worth throughassertive,aggressive,and defiant behavior. Alternatively,what a peoplelikes about itself may affect its demands and expectationsregardingothers, and shape criteriafor evaluatingthem. Lebow(1981,pp. 202-20)notesthe flatteringimageswhichnationsoften have of themselvesand showshow suchimages,harboredby Americanand Indian decision-makers,gave riseto misleadingexpectationsconcerningChinese behaviorin the Koreanand Sino-IndianWars. Imagesof one'sown powersand limitsare also importantis determinconflict behavior.Underestimationscan resultin unnecessaryacquiesing cence to others. Exaggerations-more often the case in international relations- can give riseto policiesof overextensionandto nationaldisasters. Self-imagesbecomeoperativein the developmentof conflictorientations and strategieswhennationalleaders,importantdecision-makers,andprominent opinion elites reflect them. Often importantself-imagesare common to groups of leaders and elites, and characterizeparticular"schools of thought"regardingforeign policy. We may then speak of modal images. Perceptionsof History Perceptionsof historyare a second importantdimensionof national self-imagery.They can generatepowerfulneeds to avoid past experiences whichare felt as humiliating,dangerous,or deadly(Jonesand Rosen, 1982, pp. 130-50;Leites, 1953;May, 1973;Pye, 1968). Those aspectsof history aboutwhicha peoplefeelsmostdisturbedcangiveriseto demandsfor change whichare expressedparticularlyin conflict behavior.Theremay be a sense of inadequacyor powerlessnessabout periodsof oppression,colonialism, defeat, or even slippagefrom previousstatus. Theremay be shameabout one's forebearsand the waysin which, it is perceived,they behavedtoward oppressorsor conquerors. Such images can generate needs to prove to 52 Kaplowitz others -and most importantly to the self-that one's people is not inherently weak or inferior. Nations which have experiencedrepeated defeats or long periods of subjugation and persecution may pursue conflict-and pursue it in particular ways- in order to regain or maintain self-esteem. A similar process may occur if leaders and decision-makers are particularly concerned with honor, prestige, or declining status, or if they believe their nations have become too soft, passive, or undisciplined. Barber (1972, pp. 78-95, 129-42, 347-65, 424-42) provides some intriguing data in this regard. In such cases, personal concerns will reverberate with sensitive modal chords in the larger political culture which are reflected by significant groups within the nation. Perceptions of history which are humiliating can lead to quests for a veritable "New (or Renewed or Old-New) Man" who will restore honor, dignity, and strength to the nation - and who will also demand and gain respect from the rest of the world (Erikson, 1950, pp. 359-402; Leites, 1953; Pye, 1968). The quest for a "New Man"is rooted in universalneeds for self-esteem. There is widespreadagreementamong various schools in psychology regarding the need for such esteem (Sniderman, 1975). These needs are also shaped by particular cultures and histories. Needs for self-esteem may be aggravated by examples of technologically advanced, heavily industrialized, highly modernized, and militarily successful societies, images of which are now projected universally by a worldwide network of communications media. These needs for self-esteem are a largely unrecognized and unexplored but crucial source of international behavior. Perceptions of history can also generate desires to recapture periods of past glory. Such perceptions can ironically give rise to a questioning of present self-worth since the nation may no longer be perceived as great. But they can also be a source of encouragement for the future: "If once we were great, surely we can be great again." Harking back to periods of past glory can sometimes lead to grandiose blueprints for the future. Thus, the creation and recreation of peoples can have immense effects upon international behavior. Conceptions of National Interest Conceptions of national interest comprise a third dimension of national self-imagery. Traditional IR theory has tended to assume that the meaning of the term "national interest" is self-evident-that "interests" are an objective category and can be objectively determined (Morgenthau, 1960, pp. 5-8). Despite much criticism, this particular view continues to substantially influence scholars and practitioners, so that many still use the term Dimensionsof International Relations Psychopolitical 53 "nationalinterest"as if it referredsolelyto mattersof fact and not to preferences or values. It can be empiricallydemonstrated,however,that thereare often significantdifferencesconcerningwhatthe nationalinterestsare and how they should be ordered.Different states and leaderswithin states, in relatively similarsituations,often espousedifferentgoals. In addition,thereareusually competingsubgoalswhichcompriseoverarchingconceptionsof national interest and there are often serious disagreementsconcerningtrade-offs (Georgeand Keohane, 1980, p. 219). Now, thereare somegeneralconceptionsof interestwhicharevirtually universal,suchas maintainingthe existenceand securityof states.9If one asserts that "the state'sexistenceis a nationalinterest"and "a governmentis actingin the nationalinterestif it preservesthe state'sexistence,"one is close to statingmattersof fact sincealmostall peoplesand decision-makers desire that theirstatescontinueto exist. Oncewe get beyondthis not veryusefulor informativelevelof generality,however,we findsignificantdifferencesregarding nationalinterests.If there is widespreadagreementon preservationof the "nationalself,"thereis sometimesdisagreementconcerningthe "wayof life"to be preserved.At times thereis even controversyregardingthe territory of the "nationalself" whose existenceis so widely desired.There are also differencesconcerningthe amountsof securityactorsdesireandtheprices they are willingto pay (Jervis, 1978,p. 174).This is to say nothingof ubiquitous differencesconcerningmeans for achievingaims. Statementsaboutnationalinterestswhichseemto suggestthat suchinterestsarean objectivecategory,often stemimplicitlyfrompredictionsabout whatwouldhappenif the stateconcernedengagedin or refrainedfrom particularactions, and from the valueswhich would be realizedor damaged. Suchvaluesand predictions,in turn, are heavilyinfluencedby nationalselfimages, perceptionsof others, culturallyinducedbeliefs about appropriate behaviorin conflictsituations,andexpectationsaboutwhatis possible.These psychologicaland culturalfactorsthushelpto determinethe valenceattached to particulargoals- pieces of territoryfor example- and to the means for achievingthem. Failureto understandthe intensitiesof attachmentto goals often stems frominsufficientattentionto suchpsychologicalandculturalinfluencesupon policy. This inattention,in turn, can lead to crucialfailuresof deterrence (George,1980,pp. 69-72;Jervis,1979,p. 306;Lebow,1985a)to self-defeating policies, and even to strategicdisasters(Stein, 1985a, 1985b).Conversely, knowledgeof the psychologicaland culturalinfluencesupon an adversary's 91am indebtedto the late NathanLeitesfor commentsconcerningthe discussionin this and the followingparagraphon nationalinterests. Kaplowitz 54 policies can be immenselyuseful in the developmentof effective strategies for dealing with him (Kaplowitz,1984). Both the 1967and 1973warsin the MiddleEast occurred,in part, because Egypt, in the first case, did not initiallycomprehendthe intensityof Israel'sconvictionabout the necessityfor overcominga perceivedthreatto nationalexistence;and Israel,in the second case, did not sufficientlycomprehendthe degreeof Egypt'scommitmentto regainingterritoryandrecouping national honor. Accordingly,neitherparty adequatelyunderstoodthe extentto whichthe other was impelledto take greatrisks. Whilesome theoristshavearguedthat suchbreakdownsof deterrenceare exceptions,these exceptionshavegreatlyinfluencedthe shapeof internationalrelationstoday and may well influencethe fate of the worldtomorrow.Understandingthe other'sconceptionsof his nationalinterestsand the valenceshe attachesto particulargoals is crucialif actorsareto deviseeffectivepoliciesfor dealing with adversariesand if they are to enhancethe possibilitiesfor constructive outcomes. Criticsof psychologyand IR have often accusedthis enterpriseof ignoring "real"conflicts of interestand of assumingthat bitter disputesare "merely"the products of "misunderstanding." Psychopoliticalanalysis, however,adds a crucialdimensionto traditionalthinkingon these matters. It focuseson whatdeterminesconceptionsof nationalinterestsuchthat very realconflictsdo occurthat often lead to destructiveinteractions.In particular, it indicatesthe waysin whichpsychologicalandculturalfactorsinfluence conceptionsof interestand consequentinteractions.This mode of analysis is then able to suggestwaysin whichresultantknowledgecan be usedto design policies more likely to make for constructiveoutcomes. PERCEPTIONSOF ENEMIES Muchof the workin psychologyand IR until now has focusedon perceptionsof enemies.The most salientperceptionsand particularlythose of an enemy'saims, power,determination,andperseveranceareof obviousimportancein determiningbehaviortoward him. Misperceptions Therehas beenparticularemphasison misperceptionsor gapsbetween "imageand reality."To the extentthat specifichypotheseshavebeengenerated, they pertainlargelyto misperceptions(Jervis, 1968; McGowanand Shapiro, 1973, pp. 181-186).Much of this work has been concernedwith the waysin whichperceptionscan be distortedby cognitiveprocesses(Axelrod, 1976; Jervis, 1976; Larson, 1985;Steinbrenner,1974). These include Dimensionsof International Relations Psychopolitical 55 strivings for consistency, the role of central beliefs and expectations in this regard, the assimilation of incoming data to such pre-existing beliefs, and the ways in which discrepant information is denied, explained away, or ignored; misplaced analogies from the past; premature cognitive closure; postdecisional rationalization to reduce dissonance; the failure to recognize trade-offs; the avoidance of difficult choices; and incrementalchanges in images rather than fundamental rethinking (Lebow, 1981, pp. 102-107). Depth-psychological theories add to cognitive explanations, the role of underlying needs and related defense mechanisms. The needs most relevant to international attitudes are those for aggression, power, esteem, rectitude, and, to a lesser extent, affection. The most relevant defenses are the denial of unpleasant realities; reaction formation, in attitude and behavior, in a direction opposite from underlying feelings, images, or dispositions about which one is anxious or embarrassed, or which would evoke personal or social disapproval; the projection onto others of one's own underlying motives and attitudes, usually of an aggressivenature; and the rationalizationof egoistic or aggressive behavior in socially approved terms. The concern with misperceptions in the psychology and IR literature follows naturally from the focus upon decision-making as the primary dependent variable in most studies. It stems in particular from the premise that correct perceptions are the key to rational and effective policies. Now, it is indeed quite possible to correctly assess "reality"regardingsome tangible factors pertinent to policy formulation such as the level of armaments in a country or, more precisely, the numbers of tanks and planes it has. Even regarding certain intangible factors such as the intentions of nations or the character of leaders, it can sometimes be said, with varying degrees of assurance, that particular perceptions are distorted or incorrect. The intentions and attitudes of actors, however, are often manifold and multilayered, and sometimes confused, inconsistent, or ambivalent, as well as fluid and evolving. A certain perception may be correct regarding one aspect of an enemy's position but incorrect regarding another. Consider, for example, the perceptions "the Arab world or particularArab states are ready to coexist with Israel" and "the Arab world or particular Arab states and organizations are determined to ultimately destroy Israel"-two views which form the core of highly contentious policy debates among involved parties. The evidence in this regard, however, has been sufficiently complex and ambiguous - when attainable and reliable - that resolution of the question, as stated, has been difficult if not impossible. Most importantly, if one takes into account latent as well as manifest attitudes and they turn out to differ, both of the above perceptions can be true to a degree. This may even be the case if one asks these questions with respect to particular actors - for example, Egypt and the PLO-entities to which many observers have been more disposed to attach one of the above perceptions. Students of the Middle East are familiar with disagreements among decision-makers and scholars regard- Kaplowitz 56 ing the positions of the PLO and Egypt, even when they are exposed by and large to the same data, even after the peace treaty between Egypt and Israel, and even following the declarations of the PLO in 1988. Similar observations could be made about Soviet attitudes regarding the West. This is not to say that strong arguments cannot be made one way or the other on such matters. Indeed, policy-makershave to make and weigh such arguments. At the same time, interpretations of relevant data can be substantially affected by underlying assumptions, central beliefs, previously held theories about conflict (Jervis, 1976), core perceptions of the enemy, prior experiences, and related cognitive schema (Larson, 1985), as well as by anxiety, stress, and exposure to threat (George, 1980, pp. 47-49). Moreover, such interpretationsmay be importantly influenced by an actor's own needs, goals, and aspirations which stem, in part, from its self-images. Finally, the adversary's intentions may themselves be affected by, and change as a result of, one's own attitudes and behavior. Differences in Perception Such analyticalproblems notwithstanding,studies of misperceptionhave greatly enriched our understanding of foreign policy decision-making and the difficulties involved. If one's primary concern, however, is to understand the dynamics of confict and the strategies most likely to be effective in transforming destructive into constructive processes, then it is also crucial to focus upon the differences in perceptions of parties to conflict. Then one needs to analyze the differences with which parties perceive the same issues, events, policies, and peoples. To be exclusively concerned with which position on such complex matters is more in accord with reality-or, as Stagner (1965, p. 49) puts it, "Who is really right about this?"- will not adequatelyilluminate conflict dynamics or suggest strategies that can yield constructive outcomes. The actors involved in the Soviet-American and Arab-Israeli conflicts, for example, have perceived and interpreted capitalism, socialism, Palestinian nationalism, and Zionism in different ways. The same is the case regarding perceptionsof formative events such as World War II or the 1948 Arab-Israeli War, important issues such as nuclear strategies or the territories taken by Israel in the 1967 War, and various policies concerning these issues such as the Strategic Defense Initiative or the Begin Autonomy Plan. These views may well entail distorted perceptions and "selective inattention." Both cognitive processes and motivational dynamics play a part in generating such misperceptions. Illuminating these sources of misperception can further our understanding of policy-making processes among the respective actors. If one wants to understandthe dynamics of these conflicts and the requirements for more productive outcomes, however, one also needs to analyze the differ- Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 57 ences in the actors'perceptionsof the above symbols, ideologies, political movements,and socioeconomicsystems,and why the actorsperceivethem as they do. Analysis of such differencesis crucialto a thoroughunderstandingof conflict patterns. Attemptingto assess gaps between"imageand reality"can, in some respects,be an irrelevantand evenmisleadingfocus regardingsuchmatters. Often more useful and feasible, for purposesof analyzingconflict dynamics, is to portraythe gaps in perceptionbetweenthe parties- the interacting subjectivities.How do they perceiveand evaluatetheirown and the other's strength,character,intentions,trustworthiness,perseverance,and attitudes towardissues?What is the meaningwhichthey attachto events?And why aretheredifferencesin this regard?Understandingthesegapscan be of crucial importancein findingwaysout of interlockingbinds, devisingstrategies for alteringconflictprocesses,and openingup newpossibilitiesfor constructive outcomes. The Boundariesof Perceptions What each party most dislikesabout the other can be a particularly importantsourceof behavior.Negativeperceptionsarealso the most salient ones for actorsin conflict.Moreover,suchperceptionsareespeciallyresilient (Jervis,1976,pp. 310-15;White, 1970,pp. 310-19).Positiveperceptionscan pointto potentialgroundsfor cooperation.In bitterdisputes,however,even what is liked or admiredabout the other can heightenenvy and exacerbate conflict. Perceptionsof History Perceptionsof the historyof relationswith others can influencecurrent attitudesand add a decisiveedge to them. Historymay teach "lessons" about the need for caution, suspicion,and toughness.It may teach special "lessons"about particularenemiesor categoriesof otherssuch as "aggresor "imperialists." These"lessons"arethen sors,""capitalists," "Communists," appliedto currentpolicies, sometimesappropriately,but at othertimes on the basis of misleadinganalogies(Brewer,1975;Jervis, 1976, pp. 217-82). Legitimacyand Authenticityof the Other One of the most importantaspectsof enemy perceptions,largelyignoredin the literature,is the degreeof legitimacywhicheach partyaccords the other (for exceptions see Bronfenbrenner,1961; Rapoport, 1960, p. 58 Kaplowitz 299-367; and Snyder and Diesing, 1977, pp. 299-310). This aspect of perceptions of the enemy feeds back into the formulation of one's own aims and choice of means. In extreme cases, parties may completely derogate the legitimacy of the other's claims and even the authenticity of his identity and existence. Such positions give rise to total conflict in which each side attempts to completely eliminate the other's political and sometimes physical existence. In less extreme cases, accordance of legitimacy ranges from minimal to substantial, with important differential effects on conflict strategies and interaction dynamics. Knowledge of how parties to conflict view themselves and each other, and of differences in perceptions of the same issues and objects, the dimensions of which we have explicated here, is requiredfor an understandingboth of the behavior of actors and of the interaction dynamics between them. Such knowledge may then be used to inform strategies for coping more effectively and constructively with adversaries. It is to an analysis of such strategies that we now turn. STRATEGIES OF CONFLICT The Policy Relevance of Psychology and IR Scholars who advocate the study of psychological and cultural dimensions of international relations have had a difficult time answering the question, "what can be done with such information?" To be sure, this is not the only approach to IR which has experienced such difficulties. At the same time, most scholars in this subfield are decidedly interested in analyses which will have a salutary effect upon international conflict and they expect that this domain will be policy-relevant. Early works which suggested the ways in which perceptions could be distorted by defense mechanisms had difficulty in indicating what could be feasibly done about such processes that would have beneficial effects on the international level. There were suggestions for changes in child-rearing, for emotional as well as intellectual understanding of the aggressive impulses which presumably result in war, and for the complete suppression of these impulses altogether through the establishment of collective security systems (Durbin and Bowlby, 1939; Glover, 1933). These prescriptions,however, were either ineffective, unfeasible, or at best, entailed very long-term projects (Claude, 1962, pp. 94-204, is the classic analysis of the failure of collective security). One of the results was what might be termed an "if only" approach to policy-if only peoples and leaders would be less defensive, more under- Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 59 standing, and more realistic in their perceptions of enemies: "If the people on each side could recognize the extent to which their own actions serve to provoke and frighten the other side, instead of placing all the blame on the 'enemy'. . .. " (Gladstone, 1959, p. 136). Critics reflecting the Realist tradition in IR took particular aim at these "if only" implications (Waltz, 1954: 42-79). More recent, largely cognitive analyses, interestingly and surprisingly enough, have not proceeded very much beyond a contemporary version of these earlier prescriptions, as they suggest that decision-makers become more thoughtful, aware of perceptual errors, and sensitive to alternative explanations (Axelrod, 1976, pp. 224-25; Jervis, 1976, pp. 410-15, 423-24; Kinder and Weiss, 1978, pp. 228-30). Such suggestionscan perhaps aid policy-makers, if they are reflected upon, and help to render decision-making processes more sophisticated. At the same time, virtually all schools of psychology would agree that urging others to be more thoughtful and reflective is not a sufficient prescription for changing deeply rooted or substantially reinforced processes. Brodie (1973, p. 307) asserted in this vein that even if one knew what emotions were at work in making for war, this would not make any difference since "repressedmaterial is not available for being swayed by arguments of reason. .. ." To know such sources of international behavior, however, can make a difference. Such knowledge, most importantly, affords an understanding of the underlying determinants of behavior. This understanding can then be used to shape strategies for dealing with others more effectively and in ways more likely to lead to constructive outcomes. The underlying sources of an adversary'sbehavior may not be amenable to verbal persuasion and reasoned argument (Janis and Smith, 1965, pp. 188-235). But they can be influenced by one's own behavior. The most fundamental way in which states can mitigate or aggravate the psychological and cultural sources of hostility in adversariesis through their own strategies of conflict. These are still the most important levers in international relations. In this respect, psychopolitical analysis necessarilydovetails with the more traditionalstudy of conflict strategies. The small-group problem-solving workshop can be an effective setting for exploring, in depth, the concerns of parties to conflict and for generating new ideas about conflict resolution (Kelman, 1972, pp. 168-204). Coming to better comprehend the underlying psychological and cultural sources of conflict behavior can be one of the benefits of such workshops, both for the participants and the social scientists who conduct them. As Kelman himself has observed, however, such a setting cannot substitute for ultimate interaction at the governmental level. It is at this point that conflict strategies come decisively into play. 60 Kaplowitz We proceed,therefore,to explicatesuch strategies.In this explication we emphasizethe psychologicaldynamicsandthe consequencesfor strategy whichstem fromparticularkindsof self-imagesand perceptionsof enemies, and elaborateupon previouswork (Kaplowitz,1984, pp. 376-84). Conflictorientationsaredispositionsto achieveparticularaimsand to act towardsadversariesin particularways. Conflict strategiesare the plans and actions designedto achievethese aims. They are defined here by such aimswhichcan rangefrom total victory,throughmutualsatisfaction,to appeasementof the otherparty.Theyarefurtherdistinguishedby whetherfundamentalaimsareto be achievedin the shortor long run, andby differences between manifest aims and latent attitudes. Strategiesare colored by the meansemployed,whichrangefrom negativesanctionsthroughpositiveinducements. Thetheorypropoundedhereassumesthatconflictorientationsinfluence strategies,though there is not necessarilya one-to-onerelationshipin this regard.A varietyof situationaland organizationalvariablesat the international and statelevelsof analysisand whicharewell-knownto international relationistsalso come into play. These includedomesticpolitical and economicfactors;bureaucraticandorganizationalstruggles;powercapabilities; the availabilityof particularmeans;the geographicalproximityof enemies; the attitudesof importantthirdparties;and regionaland internationalconfigurationsand constraints.All of these variablesmay influencethe timing of specific actions and may make difficult or precludebehavingin accord with a particularconflict orientation. This pluralityof determinantsnotwithstanding,theorycan best be advancedby focusingupon eachparticulardomainof variablesthat influences behaviorandby probingas deeplyandextensivelyas possibleits effects.Most of the advancesin social sciencetheoriesin general,and IR theoriesin particular,have been achievedin this way. It is then that the influenceof each domain may be placed in a broadercontext. Particularconflictorientationsaredeterminedby differentkindsof selfimagesand perceptionsof enemies.Theseorientations,as mediatedby situational variables,give rise to strategies.Core self-imagesand perceptions of enemiesheld by individualsand groups,whichare then reflectedin broad "schoolsof thought"and relatedapproachesto foreignpolicy, tend to be relativelystable(Gamsonand Modigliani,1971,pp. 92-108;George, 1980, p. 57; Rokeach,1968;Steinbrenner,1974,pp. 102-03).Yet suchimagesand perceptionscan also change-usually over considerableperiodsof time but occasionallyas the resultof traumaticor catastrophicevents(Deutschand Merrit, 1965). They then give rise to changedorientationsand strategies. Since differentpoliticalgroupswithin nations often hold differentimages and perceptions,conflict orientationsand strategiesmay sometimeschange Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 61 to a certainextent, and occasionallyeven to a considerabledegree, when governmentschange. Totalist Strategy A totalist strategyis characterizedby the aim of, and often obsession with, total wins regardingbasic issuesin conflict. Nothing less will suffice. Totalism thus often entails complete eliminationor subordinationof the other. Proponentsof suchstrategiesemphasizethreatsandpunishmentsalmost exclusivelyfor both logical and psychologicalreasons:logical, becauseit would be difficultif not impossibleto promiseor rewardan adversarywith anythingthat would make worthwhileacquiescencein his own demise;and psychological,becausepositive inducementswould threatento undermine the singlenessof purposeand completeindifferenceto the fate of the other which are necessaryto the sustainingof totalist positions. Positive inducements would be threateningto the very psychologyof totalism. Totalistpositionsderivefrom highlynegativeand monolithicpercepviewed as intrinsicallyevil, intions of the enemy. He is characteristically no rightto the attainmentof with herentlyaggressive,or basicallyinferior, the other's of legitimacyand authenany of his aims. Absolutederogation often becomeentotalist actors ticity arethe hallmarksof totalism. Indeed, own nations who their accordeventhe within ragedby individualsor groups slightestdegreeof legitimacyto the other'spositions.Suchaccordance,they deeplyfear, will not only projectirresolutionto the enemy, but even more importantly,will erode the validityof their own totalist demands. Totalismis also generatedby exaggerated positive,grandiose,andsometimes even megalomaniacself-images.Totalist actorsbelievenot only that they are completelyrightand deservingof total victory, but also that they are virtually omnipotent and always capable of achieving such victory. Moreover,thereis often a finalityto totalistexpectations;the resultsof victorywillbe perpetuatedforever.Thesegrandioseaspectsof self-imagerymay serveto compensatefor negativeself-imageswhichpertainto victimization, passivity,fatalism,and weakness,and whichgive riseto damagedself-esteem (Erikson, 1950, pp. 326-402; Leites, 1953). The negative aspects of selfimageryare often repressed,suppressed,or denied,thoughthey may sometimes become manifest. Totalismcan thusdevelopas a reactionto underlyingfeelingsof powerlessnessor to recentperiodsof degradation,frustration,and humiliation. withthe past,especiallyif coupledwithbitternessabout Intensedissatisfaction the presentand compoundedby uncertaintyaboutthe future,is particularly 62 Kaplowitz conduciveto the adoptionof totalist positionsin determinedbut also frantic questsfor compensation,certainty,and greatness(Erikson,1956,p. 82). Long-RunTotalist Strategy Occasionally,totalistactorscometo believethat it is necessaryto postpone completevictory.The enemy,it is thought,has too muchstayingpower and any attemptsto completelydefeat him in the short run would entail an unacceptablepriceor resultin one's own defeat. This is especiallyso in the nuclearera when much of internationalconflict occursbetweenthe superpowers,betweenstates at least one of whichpossessesnuclearweapons, or betweenactors who are associatedwith the superpowersas allies, proteges, or clients.Thus, bilateral,regional,and internationalpowerconfigurationsmay be perceivedas preventingthe immediaterealizationof totalist aims.Suchaimsmustthereforebe approachedgraduallyandvictoryachieved in stages. Long-runtotalistsstill emphasizethreatand force, however,sinceadversarieswill be reluctantto move even graduallytowardtheir own demise merelybecause of an actor'swillingnessto negotiateover interimstages. Moreover,negativesanctionsas well as stridentpropagandaare also affectively consonantwith, and help to sustain, the underlyingtotalist disposition. Thus, althoughan attenuatedform of coexistencemay be tolerated for some time, full satisfactionrequiresthe other'scompleteeliminationor defeat. Perceptionsof the enemyarehighlynegative,but they differregarding the enemy'sstaying power from those of totalists. Moreover,perceptions are not so totally monolithicthat even interimagreementsare impossibleto suffer. Still, little if any legitimacyis accordedthe enemy. Ultimately,it is believed,he does not deserveto exist-at least not in his odious political, sociocultural,or economic form. Self-imagestend to be grandiose,though not so exaggeratedas to induce a belief that total victory can be achievedin the short run. As with totalists,grandiositymayserveto compensatefor negativeself-images,usually underlying.These self-imagesare also not so exaggeratedas to generatea compulsionto act immediatelyin a totalist fashion, in orderto compensate for innerfeelingsof weaknessor to gain someimmediatesenseof self-worth. Thenegativeself-imagesmayevenhavebecomerecentlyattenuated,andsuch actors may be better able to postpone the achievementof victorybecause they have experiencedsome partialvictories,increasedpower,and recognition by others. Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 63 and Deterrent-PunitiveStrategies Competitive-Use-of-Force A third set of strategiesaims at maximizingone's gains regardlessof lossesto the other.At the sametime, proponentswill acceptsomethingless, and usuallysubstantiallyless, than total victory. The literatureon conflict strategieshastraditionallyemphasizedthosethat entailcompetitiveaimsand negativemeans,both as objectsof studyandas prescriptionsfor policy.This thruststemsfromthe orthodoxand still influentialRealistparadigmin IRone whichbecameparamountfollowingthe traumaof WorldWar II, embodied a reactionto the idealism,pacifism, and isolationismof the interwaryears,and was particularlysuitedto the Cold War.Thisparadigmhighsystemand lightsthe conditionsof anarchyand insecurityin the international the if it does not exclude the de-emphasizes importance, altogether, possibility of cooperativerelationsamongnations.At the veryleast,it downgradesthe canbecomepredominant possibilitythatthismodeof relationship amongstates. The minimalprescriptiveimplicationis the pursuitof a balanceof powerof power(Claude,1962,pp. frequentlydefinedin practiceas preponderance and its in extension the nuclear era-deterrence. 13-16) Sometimes,the logical literaturehasgone furtherand emphasizedthe winningof conflicts(Schelling, 1960). The maximizingof gains, especially in relativelyanarchic systems characterizedby the "securitydilemma,"usuallyinvolvesattemptsto compel the otherto do somethingor, at the veryleast,to deterhim fromengaging in particularactions.In a competitivestrategy,actorsstriveto win as much as possible. In a deterrentstrategy,they aim at preventingthe enemyfrom taking as much as he can. Winningas much as possible almost alwaysinvolves the use of force. Deterringothers from taking as much as they can often involves the use of punishments.In both competitiveand deterrent strategies,the thrustis upon demandingandforcing the otherto do or not to do something.Both emphasizenegativesanctionssincethe fundamental influenceprocessis one of threateningand/or coercingthe enemy. In practice,one strategysometimesleadsto the otherand occasionally it is difficultto distinguishbetweenthem. If an actor is convincedhe faces a ruthlessand aggressiveenemy, he will not only rely upon threatto deter, but will also deem it necessaryto use force upon occasion. He may also go beyonddeterrenceand defense, as in the case of the Americanintervention in Vietnam,whichwas neverthelessexplainedand justified in termsof the deterrencepolicy of the 1950s. Implicitin competitiveand deterrentstrategiesare perceptionsof the enemy as incorrigibleregardingone's own vital interests,at least for the foreseeablefuture(Deutsch,1965;HoaglandandWalker,1979,p. 143;Snyder 64 Kaplowitz and Diesing, 1977,pp. 188, 299), thoughtheremay be some hope that if he is deterredor coercedlong enough,he may changeover the long run. Promises and rewardsare thereforeregardedas largelyirrelevant.They may even be seenas dangerousandlikelyto encouragethe enemy(Jervis,1978,p. 196). Theyarenot alwaysentirelyexcluded,however,as meansof influence.Negotiationsmaybe usedto tryto consolidategains,stabilizebalances,postponeconflict, or reassurenervousadversaries.Threatsand punishments,however,are the meansemphasizedfor deterringand coercingothers. The credibilityof a deterrentstrategynecessitatespunishmentfor enemy transgressionsregardingboth explicitand implicitlines. If he is allowed to go unpunishedand permittedto derivegains from his behavior,he will be reinforcedin his aggression.In this respect, deterrencetheory-albeit unwittingly-incorporatesa fundamental principleof behaviorist psychology. Deterrencestrategiesalso presumethatthe enemyis basicallyrationalat the veryleast it is hopedhe is so. Therefore,he will be inhibitedand will yield when faced with sufficientthreatand force. If deterrenceor compellence does not work, it is usuallyassumedthat the threatswerenot credible or the punishmentsnot massiveenough. Analysisof the impactof strategy upon the adversaryis typicallystraightforward,basedon rationalactor assumptions,and couchedin termsof manifestcosts and benefits(Georgeand Smoke, 1974, pp. 54, 73-74;Morgan, 1977, pp. 79-80;Rapoport, 1964, p. 166;Snyderand Diesing, 1977,pp. 304-05).Thoughdeterrencetheorydoes not necessarilyassumerationalityon the part of the deterringactor (Morgan, 1977,pp. 115, 116, 119),it does presumethat the enemywill be rational enoughnot to acceptcoststhat (the actorthinks)areunacceptableto him. Competitiveand deterrentactorstend to take little accountof the possibilitythat threatand force may fail becauseof the adversary'sunderlying needsto savefaceandrecoupself-esteem.Accordingly,suchactorshaveoften failedto adequatelyestimatethe degreeto whichan adversarywill be determinedto resistthreatsand to take risks to attain a position of equality,if not dominance,in a conflict,preciselybecauseof internalpsychologicaland culturalneeds whichgive rise to politicalimperatives.Takinginto account such underlyingneeds and motives requires,in essence, subtly integrating considerationsof a depth-psychologicalnatureinto more generaltheories of influence. Lebow(1981)hasshown,analogously,how internalpoliticalweaknesses can impel statesto pursueaggressivepoliciesagainstpowerfuladversaries. Whilehe stressesthe weaknessesof leaders,the politicalsystem,andthe state itself, these are amongthe factorswhichcan give rise to the damagedselfimagesand the needsto recoupself-esteemwhichwe emphasizehere, as determinantsof strategy.Onlyveryrecentlyhaveseveralstudiesand commentariesbegunto suggestthis problematicaspectof deterrencetheory,as they Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 65 point to the unexpected effects upon adversary behavior of intense frustration, wishful thinking, defensive denial, and desires for self-respect (Jervis, 1985a, pp. 21, 25, 28; Lebow, 1985a, p. 107; 1985b, pp. 182-83; Stein, 1985a, pp. 45, 57; 1985b, pp. 82, 86, 88). Since credibility is a crucial component of effective deterrenceand compulsion, there is typically great concern with demonstrating one's own resolve (Snyder and Diesing, 1977, p. 188). Accordingly, there is a disposition toward confrontation on issues even if they do not touch upon vital interests (Jervis, 1976, pp. 58-61; Morgan: 1985; Snyder and Diesing, 1977, p. 298). This, in turn, involves the danger of creating asymmetriesof motivation-situations in which an adversary becomes much more highly motivated regarding matters he perceives as vital than does one's own side (George et al., 1971, pp. 22-26; George and Smoke, 1974, p. 221). In this respect, too, deterrencetheory tends to ignore the strength of underlying motivations and has not been sufficiently attuned to considerations of a depth-psychological nature. A deterrent strategy presumes that the adversary will perceive the actor, interpret his behavior, and understand his intent in the same way the actor does (or wants the adversary to see him). In some cases, with respect to some adversaries, and regarding some issues the deterring actor is right. Threat and force do, indeed, entail costs which can have an inhibitory effect on undesirable behavior. Moreover, a deterrent stance intended as defensive is sometimes seen that way. In other cases, however, the assumption that the adversary will perceive and respond to deterrence attempts in the way the actor might, or thinks the adversary will, is incorrect. A variety of psychological, cultural, historical, and political factors which impinge upon the adversary can give rise to belief systems, perceptual predispositions, and behavioral propensities which lead to actions the deterrer does not expect. Thus, deterrence theory is not always adequate regarding its implicit cognitive assumptions. Understanding how and why states react to others as they do, requires taking into account how they perceive the other's intent and interpret his behavior.?0 Competitive and deterrent strategies are generated by negative perceptions of enemies who are characteristically seen as aggressive and incorrigible. They also stem from particular kinds of positive and sometimes ambivalent self-images. The self-images of actors using these strategies are not so exaggerated as to lead to the belief that total and final wins are possible in most cases. Nor are these actors driven by either grandiose or negative self-images to aim at such outcomes. Rather, they have positive enough images to make some demands upon others and to assert themselves to try to '?Lebow and Stein (1987) have recently offered a thorough critique of deterrence theory from both cognitive and motivational points of view. 66 Kaplowitz realizetheirgoals. They may also harborambivalentself-images,however, of havingbeentoo soft, naive,or insufficientlyvigilant.Relatedly,theymay thinkthatothers-both externalenemiesand, as Morgan(1985,p. 149)points out, domesticopponents-perceive them as passive, irresolute,or lacking in courage.In such cases, in additionto calculationsderivedfrom negative perceptionsof the enemy'sintentions,therewill be internallygeneratedneeds to demonstrateto othersand to one's own people, that one is active, decisive, and courageous, and to avoid behaviorwhich may be perceivedas projectingweaknessor lack of resolve.Thereis then a dispositionto make and punishmentsmoremassive.Suchself-images threatsmoredemonstrative may play a role in generatingcompetitiveand deterrentstrategies,and will in any case add a sharp edge to them. Firm-But-Cooperative Strategy Oureveryday,intuitivesenseof conflictsuggeststhatthereareordinarily winnersand losersin such situations,that the involvedpartiesaim at maximizingtheirgains, and are often, at best, indifferentto, if not activelyinterestedin negativeoutcomes for, their opponents. This sense of conflict is consonantwith the frequentuse of orthodox competitiveand deterrent strategies. Actors may also aim, however,at achievingmutuallysatisfyingresolutions in conflict situations.Implicitin the very envisioningof such outcomesis the accordanceof at leastsomelegitimacyto someof the adversary's positions and claims. Such a vision also presumesthat the adversarymay eventuallybe broughtto at leastsomerecognitionof one'sown vitalinterests. Otherwise,mutuallysatisfyingoutcomeswould not be possible. The fundamentalthrustof such a strategyis producingand eliciting, ratherthan demandingand forcing, changein an adversary.This strategy emphasizespromisesand rewardswalicharemeantto demonstrateto the adversary that changes in a cooperative direction will entail net benefits (Deutsch, 1965, pp. 151-57;Georgeand Smoke, 1974, pp. 604-13).At the same time, capabilitiesfor negative sanctions are developedand held in reserve,largelyfor defensivepurposes,but also to indicatethat whatever gainsthe adversarymay derivefromhis hostileorientationwill not outweigh the costs (Deutsch, 1965,p. 151). Suchcosts will resultfrom negativesanctions for oversteppingimplicitor explicitlines regardingunacceptablebehavior. A firm-but-cooperative strategyis thus meantto indicatenot only the possibilitiesfor cooperation,but also the costs of belligerence. Such a strategyimplicitlyentails the belief that reliancesolely upon threatsand punishmentswill not be sufficientfor movinghostileadversaries Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 67 in cooperativedirections.Evenif, in particularcases, it is militarilypossible to totallydefeat opponents- increasinglydifficultin contemporaryinternational conflict-negative sanctionswill have to be supplementedwith positive inducementsif thereis to be fundamentaland lastingchange.TheAllied policiestowardthe defeatedpartiesin WorldWarsI and II reflectcontrasting examplesof negativesanctionsalone, and the supplementationof such sanctionswith positive inducements. Threatsandpunishmentsmaycertainlyinhibitor repressaggressivebehavior.They may also servethe veryimportantfunctionof graduallyeroding the totalismof particularadversariesby repeatedlydemonstratingthat completevictoryis not possible.Butnegativesanctionscanalso increasefrustration,anger,rage, and a senseof injustice-all of whichcan heightenaggression.Meanwhile,the underlyingneeds,motivations,or aspirationsremain unchanged,so that there will be a dispositionto revertto the originalaggressivebehavior(Berkowitz,1969;Dollardet al., 1939;DeutschandKrauss, 1965, p. 83; Holsti, 1977, p. 102; Milburn, 1961). Threatshave also been found to reducethe rangeof alternativesconsideredin decision-making,to decreaseplanning,and to simplifythoughtprocesses(Milburn,1973,citing Holsti, 1971),all of whichare conduciveto reflexivelyaggressiveresponses. Thus, negativesanctionsalone will haveonly limitedeffect in fundamentally changinghostile attitudes.This is all the more so if such attitudesserve historicallyinducedand culturallyreinforcedneedsfor self-esteemandproof of power-not infrequentneeds of actorsin highly chargedconflict situations (Jerviset al., 1985;Kaplowitz, 1976). Thus, negativesanctions,unby positiveinducements,willnot producecooperationin hostile supplemented adversaries. A firm-but-cooperativestrategyimplicitlytakes into accountthe adversary'sunderlyingneeds and motives, as well as the tangible costs and benefitswhichresultfrom his behavior.In this sense, it integratesconsiderations of a depth-psychologicalnature.The earlyStanfordStudieson Conflict and Integrationwhich focused on the perceptionsof partiesto World War I, found along these lines, that nationsmay sometimesgo to war even if they do not believethey can prevail,providingtheirperceptionsof adversarialhostilityaregreatenough(HolstiandNorth, 1965;Zinneset al., 1961). motivationis powerfulenough, Deterrencemayalso fail whenthe challenger's whenhe is possessedwith a senseof desperation(Georgeand Smoke, 1974, pp. 527ff; Stein, 1985a,b),or whenhe has an overpoweringneedfor revenge. Thesefindingshighlightthe importanceof psychologicalneedsand motives, influencedby history, culture,and politics, as determinantsof conflict behavior. Such needs may sometimesoutweighthe rationalcalculusof tangi- 68 Kaplowitz ble costs and gains, and must be taken into account when analyzingthe 1 requirementsfor producingchange in adversaries. strategiesalso integratean importantdimension Firm-but-cooperative of behavioralpsychologywhichviewsboth rewardsandpunishmentsas conditionersof behavior,but highlightsthe formerand deemphasizesthe latter (Freedmanand Freedman,1975,pp. 33-86;George, 1980, p. 257; Rachlin, 1970;Rosecrance,1981,p. 32). The assumptionthatnegativesanctionsalone are effective,not only for repressingundesirablebehavior,but for basically changinghostile attitudes,amountsto an overly simplifiedapplicationof behaviorism,though it is often made, wittinglyand unwittingly,by leaders involved in bitter conflicts. A firm-but-cooperative approachcautionsagainstsimplisticallyassumwill that the ing adversary necessarilyperceiveand react to negativesanctions in a salutaryfashion. Rather,it entails taking into account how the other perceivesand is likely to react to the behaviorof the actor. Thus, it also incorporatesan importantdimensionof cognitivepsychology. One can drawfrom the latterthe implicationthat strategieswhichuse negativesanctions exclusivelyareeasilyconsonantwith, and assimilatedto an adversary's perceptionsof, a hostile environment.Conversely,actions dissonantwith such perceptionsare morelikelyto induceadversarialchangein a cooperative direction.'2 At the sametime, the indiscriminateuse of a firm-but-cooperative approachwould be discordantwith cognitivepsychology'sinjunctionto careof others.Totalistandlong-runtotalist fullytakeinto accountthe perspectives adversarieshavedeeplyrootedvestedinterestsin believingthat the actorwill eventuallygive up or be defeated. Thus, the projectionof cooperationto such enemies, even if accompaniedby firmness,is likely to be interpreted as weakness.They will "selectout"the firmness,"homein" on the conciliation, and be reinforcedin their positions (Kaplowitz,1984, pp. 390-91). A firm-but-cooperative stanceis facilitatedby positiveself-images.In suchcases, actorsrespecttheirown valuesenoughto be firm, to makesome 'Though usuallyso, it is not alwaysthe casethata rationalcalculusof costs andgainsdictates that a partyrefrainfrom warif it thinksit may well lose the militarybattle. It still may be the case, as Jervispointsout in a personalcommunication,thatthe costs of not goingto war outweighthoseof doingso and losing- at leastlosingto a certainextent- shortof forfeiting one'sindependenceand endangeringone'sphysicalexistence.An exampleof this wouldbe Sadat'sdecisionto go to warwith Israelin 1973in orderto stun the latter,arosethe superpowers,and jar a stagnantpoliticalprocessinto motion, even thoughthereis no evidence he thoughtEgyptcoulddecisivelywinsucha war.At thesametime,whenhumilitation,desperation, anger,an obsessionwithrevenge,or the needto provepowerareparamount,theycan often outweighthe rationalcalculusof tangiblecosts and gains. 'Lebow's and Stein's(1987)recentexplicationof reassurancestrategies,whichhavesome features of the approachwe discusshere, is predicatedupon such assumptions. Dimensionsof International Relations Psychopolitical 69 demands upon the adversary, and to draw lines regarding unacceptable behavior. At the same time, they feel strong, secure, and confident enough to project the possibility of being cooperative without fear of appearing weak, eigher in the other's or in their own eyes. Such actors may also be motivated by images of their nations as having been aggressive and uncompromising with adversaries. A firm-but-cooperativestrategywill be precluded, however, by the kinds of negative self-images which give rise to and intensify compensatory needs to prove power, bravery, and perseverance. Such needs are more easily expressed in totalist or highly competitive aims and are assuaged by an emphasis upon threat and force, rather than promise and reward or conciliation and compromise. Furthermore, such negative self-images may stimulate fears of appearing weak through gestures of cooperation. A firm-but-cooperative strategy is also predicated on perceptions of the enemy which are open and contain at least some positive elements. Because the enemy is not completely amenable even to one's minimal demands, however, such perceptions are likely to be mixed. Heradstveit (1979, p. 114) finds, in line with this proposition, that pluralistic or mixed perceptions of the enemy are associated with a propensity to support policies of compromise in the Arab-Israeli conflict. George and Smoke (1974, pp. 597-599) provide examples of changes in American perceptions of the Soviet Union and China from negative to mixed, which accompanied the evolution of policies in what are termed here a firm-but-cooperative direction. Conversely, if the enemy is perceived as fundamentally evil and aggresan actor will have and should have no incentive to even think about sive, mutual satisfaction as an aim and positive inducements as means, leave alone any disposition to test such an approach. At the same time, perceptions of the enemy as totally unacceptable may, in some cases, be cognitively linked to totalist conceptions of one's own national interests- regardless of the enemy's intentions and whether or not he changes. In such cases, the possibility of according some legitimacy to the enemy's positions is precluded from the outset. A firm-but-cooperative strategy requires that, at some time, actors attempt to intervene in cycles of hostility with positive inducements to change. They must also indicate to the adversary those aspects of his positions which they regard as at least somewhat legitimate. Belief in the efficacy of such actions does not necessarily entail the assumption that much of the incompatibility with the enemy is illusory (Jervis, 1976, pp. 75-76, 80). The actors' conceptions of their interests are such that they are substantially incompatible. A firm-but-cooperative approach aims at altering the other's conceptions of interests and goals. This strategy presumes that the accordance of some legitimacy to the adversary's positions, coupled with adequate firmness, may gradually have such an effect with certain kinds of adversaries. 70 Kaplowitz ConciliatoryStrategy Occasionallyactorsin internationalrelationsand, more often, groups withinnationsemphasizesatisfactionof the otherto the point of all but ignoringtheir own state'sneeds. Conflict, as they see it, resultsfrom factors that can be changedthroughconciliationand offers of friendship.Better education, more information,and increasedcommunicationare not only necessary,but sufficientconditionsfor changinghostilerelationships.Since conflictcan be eliminatedthroughthe redressof legitimategrievances,which such actors tend to think is possible in almost all instances,promisesand rewardsare used exclusivelywhile militarypower is de-emphasized,and threatsand punishmentseschewed. Sucha purelyconciliatoryapproachstemsfromperceptionsof the adversarywhichemphasizethe positiveand downplaythe negative.The other's aggressionoccurssolely becausehis legitimateinterestshavenot been satisfied. Oncethis dissatisfactionis eliminatedthroughappropriateconcessions, the enemywill be appeased,his hostilitywill dissolve, and he will become more cooperative. The conciliatorypositionis also facilitatedby particularkindsof negative and ambivalentimages. Proponentstend to regardtheir own state as too aggressive,militaristic,or imperialistic.Thereforedemandsshouldnot be madeuponthe adversary,but ratherconcessionsgranted.One'sown state may also be seen as weak or potentiallyweak. Thus, self-imagesentailing guiltand/or powerlessnesscan giveriseto sucha positionandprecludefirmness as a complementto conciliation. Latent Acceptanceof Enemies Theconflictpositionsdelineatedaboveareratherstraightforward. There is is anotherintriguingpossibility,however,withimportantimplicationsfor emergingfrom the bindswhichcharacterizeseeminglyintractableconflicts. Partiesmay sometimesvehementlyrejectthe idea of compromiseand anythingless than total victorybut neverthelessshowevidenceof expectingthat the enemywill not be totally defeatedand, sometimes,not defeatedat all. Thus, the actor latentlyacceptsthe enemy in the sense of not believinghe can be eliminated.The actorbecomesresignedto some formof coexistence, whetherhe likes it or not. There is no embracingin friendship,however, and overt hostility continuesto be intense. The rhetoricof such actorsis zero-sumand they may pay lip service to totalistgoals. Theyhave substantiallygivenup, however,on the possibility of achievingsuch goals. Thus, thereis a lessercompulsiontowardideological purity and a somewhatgreaterpull toward pragmatism. Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 71 This orientationraisesa numberof intriguingtheoreticaland policyrelevantquestions:(1) why is therelatentacceptancein the first placeif the manifestaims are totalist or long-runtotalist?(2) why does the acceptance remainlatent-why is it not acknowledged?and (3) what can othersdo to help make manifestthis latencyso that mutuallysatisfyingconflict resolution becomes possible? Latent acceptancedevelopsbecauseone party becomes convincedof the other'sstayingpower. No matterwhat he does, the enemycontinuesto exist, cannotbe defeated,and may even be thriving.Acceptanceof the enemy and some of his positions remainslatent, first of all, because of the difficultyin acknowledgingdefeatregardinglong-standing,deeplycherished, and ideologicallyingrainedgoals. Moreover,such acceptance,especiallyin the contextof militaryinferiorityor defeatwhichthe latentlyacceptingparty mayhaveexperienced,often has the meaningfor theseactorsof abjectyielding and unconditionalsurrender-of kneelingbeforethe enemyin a posture of submission(Kaplowitz,1976). Thus, the very idea of acceptanceis experiencedas humiliatingand as an intolerableblow to self-esteem.These blockagesto open acceptanceare of a depth-psychologicalnature.In addition, openacceptanceof the enemywouldproducetoo muchdissonancewith the centralbeliefs of the prevailingideology. Hence, a cognitivebarrieris also present.Suchpartiesarealso likelyto havebeenreinforcedwithintheir nations and groups for maintaininga manifeststance of totalism or longruntotalism,and to havebeenpunishedfor any hints of a softeningin their position.Hence,a behavioralreinforcementprocessis also at workin blocking open acceptanceof the enemy. Partieswho only latentlyacceptenemiesemphasizethreatand punishment, sincetheymanifestlyrejectpermanentsolutionsbasedon compromise regardingbasic issues. They may eventuallybecomeopen to some negotiations, however,and to whatthey assertare interimcompromises.Theymay even come to favor such agreementsif these are designedso that the attendanthumiliationis minimized.Butsuchactorswillnot makepermanentpeace unlessand until theirlatent acceptanceof the enemyhas becomemanifest. Blockageof open acceptanceis generatedand reinforcedby ambivalent self-imageswhichmay involveconcernswith passivity,fatalism,weakfor bolsteringself-esteem ness, or lackof courage.Theresultingrequirements entaildemonstrationsof activism,persistence,andbravery-all of whichare servedby continuedrejection,on a manifestlevel, of the other. The negativeperceptionsof the enemy'sodious characterand evil intentionsmakeit all the moredifficultto openlyacknolwedgehis stayingpower. Such perceptionsare similarto those of long-runtotalistsexceptfor the crucialexpectationthat the enemycannotbe eliminated.It is this particular combinationof negativeself-imagesand perceptionsof the enemy which makes for the recalcitrantbut intriguinglatent orientation. 72 Kaplowitz Kelman's remarks (1978, pp. 180-81) about distinguishing between dreams - what we would here call long-run totalist dreams - and more pragmatic operational programs are germane to the discussion of latency. As explicated here, latency implies a potential though not actual operational program which permits compromise with the enemy, and a potential recession of manifest totalism to the category merely of dreams. But the orientation of latent acceptance is not equivalent to such a pragmatic and open program of compromise. Rather, it embodies substantial psychological and hence political resistance to compromise. Moreover, the mere assertion on the part of an actor, of a distinction between dreams and operational programs, is not by itself a sufficiently convincing indicator of latent acceptance and could well be a manipulative deception compatible with long-run totalism. The development of latent acceptance in contrast to long-run totalism turns on the strengthof expectationsthat complete victory cannot be achieved, as opposed to beliefs about the necessity for some interim compromise now in order to continue the march toward total victory later. A'lII'UDES TOWARD ISSUES Attitudes toward specific issues are embodied in the aims that define conflict orientations and strategies. These aims, as we have seen, can range from total victory, through mutual satisfaction, to appeasement of the other. The assessment of possibilities for constructive conflict resolution requires mapping areas of overlap and convergence in attitudes regarding particular issues. This task entails analysis of positions concerning optimal, satisfactory, and acceptable outcomes. In cases where there appears to be no overlap whatsoever, it is crucial to explore latent attitudes as well as manifest aims. An important indicator of differences, in this regard, is evidence that the expectations of a party about what can be achieved diverge from stated aims. Such evidence suggests that the actor himself has substantial, though perhaps repressed, doubts about the efficacy of his policies. Latent attitudes may be further explored by analyzing positions on issues which do not directly tap raw emotions, elicit ego defensive attitudes, or involve heavily reinforced group taboos, as is the case in highly intense conflicts regarding ideological tenets, positions in principle, and the efficacy of force. Focusing only upon manifest attitudes in seemingly intractable conflicts regarding ideological tenets, positions in principle, and the efficaoutcomes other than defeat for one side or continued conflict into the indefinite future, with the possibility of mutual catastrophe. Attitudes on other issues, however, may indirectly suggest a willingness to accept something less than a total win, even though the idea of compromise per se may evoke negative and defensive reactions. Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 73 Table I. Conflict Strategies:Aims and Means Means Emphasisupon Threatsand Punishments Positive Inducements and Negative Sanctions Aims Total win, short run Totalism Total win, long run Long-Run Totalism Manifestaim: total win in long run; but latent acceptanceof enemy Latent Acceptance Maximizationof interests; offensive stance Competitiveuse-of-force (Usual) Competitiveuse-of-force (Occasional) Maximizationof interests; defensivestance Deterrentpunitive (usual) Deterrentpunitive (occasional) Mutual satisfaction Emphasisupon satisfactionof the other Firm-butcooperative (occasional) Emphasisupon Positive Inducements Firm-butcooperative (usual) Conciliatory, appeasement Particular strategies of conflict on the part of the adversary may either aggravate or attenuate blockages to compromise which result from the underlying psychological needs and processes, as shaped by history and culture, which we have elaborated here. The mapping of potential overlap through the exploration of latent as well as manifest attitudes may point to possibilities for mutually acceptable outcomes. This procedure opens up the possibility of and is required for analyzing the question-what strategies would be most effective in making manifest the latent attitudes and thus the areas of potential overlap, which would then create the possibility for constructive conflict resolution (Kaplowitz, 1984)? HYPOTHESES CONCERNING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SELF-IMAGES, PERCEPTIONS OF ENEMIES, AND CONFLICT STRATEGIES The foregoing analysis leads to the following hypotheses, which are also schematized in Table II. Kaplowitz 74 Table II. Conflict Strategies:Self-Imagesand Perceptionsof Enemies Conflict Strategies Self-Images Perceptionsof Enemy Totalism Highlynegative Highlygrandiose manifest;and negative (may be underlying) Highlynegative,with the beLong-runtotalism Grandiosemanifest; lief enemycannot be defeated and negative(may be in short-run underlying) Manifestlong-run Ambivalentor Negative totalism;latent acceptanceof enemy Negative,with expectations cannot be totally defeated Competitive-use-of Positiveor Ambivalent force, deterrentpunitive Positive or Ambivalent Firm-but-cooperative Negative Conciliatoryappeasement Negativeor Ambivalent Mixed Positiveor Mixed 1. Totaliststrategiesaregeneratedby highlygrandioseself-imagesand negativeimages which may be underlying;and by highly negativeperceptions of the enemy. 2. Long-runtotalist strategiesare generatedby grandioseself-images and negativeimageswhichmay be underlyingand somewhatattenuatedas the resultof partialvictories,increasedpower, and recognitionby others; and by negativeperceptionsof the enemy, which neverthelessattributeto him sufficient power so that he cannot be defeatedin the short-run. 3. Latentacceptanceof the enemydespitemanifestlong-runtotalism is generatedby ambivalentor negativeself-imageswhichpertainto passivity, weakness,and fatalism;and by negativeperceptionsof the enemy,which neverthelessinclude underlyingexpectationsthat he cannot be defeated. 4. Competitiveanddeterrentstrategiesaregeneratedby positiveor ambivalentself-images,the latterof whichmay involveconcernswith softness, naivete, insufficientvigilance, or lack of resolve;and by negativeperceptions of the enemywhichincludeincorrigibilityfor the foreseeablefuture. 5. Firm-but-cooperative strategiesaregeneratedby positiveor ambivalent self-images,the latterinvolvingconcernswith havingbeen too aggressive or uncompromising;and by mixed perceptionsof the enemy, which include hostility but also corrigibility. 6. Conciliatoryand appeasementstrategiesare generatedby negative or ambivalentself-imageswhichpertainto aggressiveness,militarism,chauvinism, or imperialism;and by positive perceptionsof the enemy. Relations Dimensionsof International Psychopolitical 75 SUMMARYAND CONCLUSIONS Thetheorypresentedherehas introducedand emphasizednationalselfimageryas a crucialvariabledeterminingconflictbehavior.Variousdimensionsof self-imageryareexplicatedandlinkedto conflictstrategies.Regarding perceptionsof enemies,the theory stressesanalysisof the differenceswith whichactorsperceivethe sameissues, events,policies, and nations. Studies of such "gaps"in perceptionand interactingsubjectivitiesshould complement the hithertoprevalentconcernwith misperceptionsas a primarydependentvariable.Analyzingthese differenceswill illuminatethe dynamics of conflict and may also indicateways out of interlockingbinds. A typology of conflict strategiesis elaborated,rangingfrom totalism to conciliation.The possibilityis stressedthat an actor may manifestlyreject the idea of acceptingthe enemyand of compromisewith him while, at the same time, evidenceemergesthat he does not believethe enemycan be defeatedexcept, perhaps,at an unacceptableprice. The theory thus introducesthe idea of latent acceptanceof enemies. The uncoveringof this latencyand the makingof it manifestare crucialin conflicts from whichthereseemsno exit, short of defeat by one side of the other or catastrophefor all. The explicationof each strategyentailsan analysisof the kindsof selfimagesand perceptionsof enemieswhichare its sources.A relatedseriesof hypothesesis presentedregardingthese connections.Such imagesand perceptionsmust be categorizedalong a positive-negativedimensionfor purposes of theoryconstructionand hypothesistesting. The understandingof conflictdynamicsandthe derivationof policy-relevant implications,however, requiresknowledgeof the content of these imagesand the particularways in which they influence conflict behavior, and thus, in-depth qualitative analysis. Assessment of the possibilities for constructiveoutcomes requires analyzingattitudestowardissuesin conflict, both manifestand latent, and mappingareasof overlap.Whilethe analysisof such attitudescan indicate areasof convergenceor potentialoverlap,the strategiesof the partiesmay neverthelesshavereciprocaleffects suchthat they becomeevermoretightly locked into mutuallydestructiveinteractions.Analysisof these interaction effects is thereforean essentialpart of the psychopoliticalstudy of international relations,one which is focused upon in a relatedwork (Kaplowitz, 1984).The influenceof an actor'sstrategyupon an adversaryis determined not only by its impactupon the latter'smanifestand tangiblegains and losses, but also by its impactupon the imagesand perceptionswhichdetermine his behavior.Analysisof reciprocalinteractioneffectsand of strategiesmost appropriatefor producingadversarial changein a cooperativedirection,there- Kaplowitz 76 fore requires assessing and taking into account the self-images and perceptions of enemies elaborated here. It is in this way that knowledge of the psychological and cultural sources of international behavior can ultimately make a crucial difference in enhancing possibilities for constructive conflict resolution. APPENDIX: RESEARCH GUIDELINES The theory presented here suggests the following guidelines for study of the variables and processes explicated. These guidelines are meant to facilitate the construction of interview schedules, content analysis categories, and schema for behavioral observations. Self-Images 1. What are the most salient aspects of a people's, group's, or leadership's self-imagery? 2. Are the self-images grandiose, positive, ambivalent, or negative, and in what particular ways? 3. About what are a people or group most disturbed concerning themselves? What do they most dislike? 4. In what ways do they want to change and what means are regarded as most appropriate for producing these changes? 5. What do they most like about themselves and how do these images influence the demands they make and the expectations they have regarding others? 6. How do they perceive their nation's history and what are the most salient images in this regard? 7. What aspects of this history are regarded as most disturbing and what "lessons" have been learned? 8. What periods of the nation's history are most liked? In what ways are these attitudes projected onto present aims and expectations for the future? 9. What are the conceptions of national purpose and interest? How are these influenced by other aspects of self-imagery? 10. What are the perceptions of the state's capabilities and power? Perceptions of Enemies 1. What are the most salient perceptions of the enemy? 2. Are the perceptions highly negative, negative, mixed, or positive? Psychopolitical Dimensions of InternationalRelations 77 3. What is most dislikedor distrusted? 4. What is most liked, preferred,or admired? 5. What are the perceptionsof the historyof one's relationswith the other?What"lessons"havebeenlearnedaboutthe otherand how to behave toward him? 6. Whatarethe perceptionsof the other'sinterests,positions,andgoals, and what degree of legitimacyis accordedthese? To what extent is the legitimacyand authenticityof the other'sidentityand existenceacceptedor derogated? 7. What are the perceptionsof the other'sattitudestoward oneself? 8. What are the perceptionsof the other'spower, determination,and perseverance? 9. What are the differencesbetweenPartyA's perceptionof Party B and B's perceptionof itself; betweenB's perceptionof A and A's perception of itself; betweenA's perceptionsof B's attitudestowardissues in conflict and B'sattitudes;and betweenB'sperceptionsof A's attitudestowardissues and B's attitudes? Conflict Strategies 1. Are the aims of partiestotal victory, total victoryin the long run, maximizationof gains regardlessof losses to the other, mutuallysatisfying outcomes, or satisfactionof the other'sinterestswith little regardfor one's own? 2. Are thereindicationsthat expectationsaboutwhatis possiblediffer from manifest aims? 3. Is there latent acceptanceof the other and some of his positions? 4. Whatare regardedas the most appropriateand effectivemeansfor achievingaims? Is there an emphasisupon negativesanctions,positiveinducements,or a mix of means? 5. Whatarethe beliefsaboutthe utilityof threatandforce?Whatkinds of force are regardedas most effective?And what are the perceivedeffects upon the adversary? 6. Whatarethe beliefsaboutthe utilityof positiveinducements?What promisesand rewardsare regardedas most effective?And whatarethe perceived effects upon the adversary? 7. Are particularmeans supportedeven though there are indications that proponentsdo not believe them to be achievingstated aims? 8. If so, what do the attitudesof the partiesindicateabout support for such meansservingto fulfill psychologicalneeds, as affectedby history and culture-such as needs for self-esteem, prestige, power, aggression, revenge, and morale? 78 Kaplowitz 9. What are the attitudes toward the idea of compromise and negotiations with the enemy? 10. If compromise and negotiations are rejected in principle, are there psychological needs and cultural patterns which give rise to connotations of compromise and negotiations as meaning abject yielding, unconditional surrender, and kneeling before the enemy? Attitudes Toward Issues 1. What are the attitudes of the parties toward the issues in conflict? What are regarded as optimal, satisfactory, and acceptable outcomes? 2. What areas of overlap, if any, are indicated by the manifest attitudes of the parties? 3. Do attitudes on some issues indirectly suggest a willingness to compromise and accept something less than a total win, even though the idea of compromise per se may evoke negative and defensive reactions? 4. If so, do blockages to compromise result from psychological needs and cultural patterns? 5. What areas of potential overlap are indicated by latent attitudes toward issues? 6. In what ways do the strategies of the other aggravate or attenuate the blockages to compromise? 7. What strategies on the part of the other are most likely to make manifest the areas of potential overlap?'3 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writing of this essay was facilitated by the support of the Institute of International Studies, University of California, Berkeley, and its Director, Carl Rosberg. I wish to thank Alexander George and the late Nathan Leites for comments on an earlier draft; three anonymous reviewers for posing important questions of synthesis and additional lines of inquiry; and Robert Jervis for his helpful synthesis of critical suggestions. REFERENCES Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswick, E., Levinson,D. J., and Sanford, R. N. (1950). The AuthoritarianPersonality,Harperand Row, New York. 'The answersto the last two questionsrequireanalysisof the reciprocalinteractioneffects of partiesusing differentstrategies(Kaplowitz,1984). PsychopoliticalDimensionsof InternationalRelations 79 Almond,G., andVerba,S. (1963).TheCivicCulture,PrincetonUniversityPress,Princeton,N.J. Axelrod,R. (ed.)(1976).Structureof Decision:TheCognitiveMapsof PoliticalElites,Princeton UniversityPress, Princeton,N.J. Axelrod,R. (1980).Moreeffectivechoicein the prisoner'sdilemma.J. Confl.Res. 24: 379-403. Barber,J. D. (1972). The PresidentialCharacter,Prentice-Hall,EnglewoodCliffs, N.J. of theFrustration-Aggression Berkowitz,L. (ed.)(1969).Rootsof Aggression:A Re-examination Hypothesis,AthertonPress, New York. Brecher,M. (1980).Decisionsin Crisis:Israel, 1967and 1973, Universityof CaliforniaPress, Berkeley,Calif. Brecher,M., Steinberg,B., and Stein, J. (1969).A frameworkfor researchon foreignpolicy behavior.J. Confl. Res. 13: 75-101. Brewer,T. L. (1975).LearningProcessesin ForeignPolicymaking.Paperpresentedat the annual meetingof the AmericanPoliticalScienceAssociation,San Francisco. Brodie, B. (1973). Warand Politics, MacMillan,New York. Bronfenbrenner,U. (1961).The mirrorimagein Soviet-Americanrelations.J. SocialIssues 17: 45-56. Claude, I. (1962).Power and InternationalRelations,RandomHouse, New York. Deutsch,K., and Merrit,R. (1965). Effects of eventson nationaland internationalimages. Behavior:A Social-PsychologicalAnalysis, In Kelman,H. (ed.),International Holt, Rinehart and Winston,New York, pp 130-187. Deutsch,M. (1962). Psychologicalalternativesto war. J. SocialIssues 18: 97-119. Deutsch,M. (1965).Producingchangein an adversary.In Fisher,R. (ed.), InternationalConflict and BehavioralScience,Basic Books, New York, pp. 145-160. Deutsch,M. (1973). The Resolutionof Conflict, Yale UniversityPress, New Haven. Deutsch,M., Epstein,Y., Canavan,D., andGumpert,P. (1967).Strategiesof inducingcooperation: An experimentalstudy. J. Confl. Res. 11: 345-360. Deutsch,M., andKrauss,R. M. (1965).Theoriesin SocialPsychology,BasicBooks,NewYork. Dollard,J., Doob, L. W., Miller,N. E., Mowrer,O. H., and Sears,R. N. (1939).Frustration and Aggression,Yale UniversityPress, New Haven. and War,ColumbiaUniverDurbin,E. F. M., andBowlby,J. (1939).PersonalAggressiveness sity Press, New York. East,M. A., Salmore,S. A., andHermann,C. F. (eds.)(1979).WhyNationsAct, Sage,Beverly Hills, Calif. Erikson,E. (1950). Childhoodand Society, Norton, New York. Erikson,E. (1956). Problemsof ego identity.J. Am. PsychoanalyticAssoc. 4: 58-121. Etheredge,L. (1978).A Worldof Men: ThePrivateSourcesof AmericanForeignPolicy, MIT Press, Cambridge,Mass. Freedman,A. E., and Freedman,P. E. (1975). ThePsychologyof PoliticalControl,St. Martin's, New York. Gamson,W. A., andModigliani,A. (1971). Untanglingthe Cold War:A Strategyfor Testing Rival Theories,Little, Brown, Boston. George,A. (1969).The'OperationalCode':A neglectedapproachto the studyof politicalleaders and decision-making.Int. StudiesQ. 13: 190-222. behavior: George,A. (1979).The causalnexusbetweencognitivebeliefsand decision-making The 'OperationalCode'belief system.In Falkowski,L. S. (ed.), PsychologicalModels in InternationalPolitics, Westview,Boulder,Colo., pp. 95-124. George,A. (1980).PresidentialDecisionmakingin ForeignPolicy, Westview,Boulder,Colo. George,A., Hall, D. K., andSimons,W. R. (1971).TheLimitsof CoerciveDiplomacy,Little, Brown, Boston. George,A., andKeohane,R. 0. (1980).Theconceptof nationalinterest:Usesandlimitations, In George,A., Presidential in ForeignPolicy,Westview,Boulder,Colo., Decisionmaking pp. 217-237. George,A., and Smoke,R. (1974).Deterrencein AmericanForeignPolicy: Theoryand Practice, ColumbiaUniversityPress, New York. Gladstone,A. (1959). The conceptionof the enemy.J. Confl. Res. 3: 132-137. Glover,E. (1933). War,Sadism,and Pacifism, George,Allen and Unwin, London. 80 Kaplowitz Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry (1978). Self-Involvement in the Middle East Conflict, Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry, New York. Heradstveit, D. (1979). The Arab-Israeli Conflict: Psychological Obstacles to Peace, Universitetsforlaget, Oslo. Hermann, M. (1980). Explaining foreign policy behavior using the personal characteristics of political leaders. Int. Studies Q. 24: 7-46. Hermann, R. K. (1985). Perceptions and Behavior in Soviet Foreign Policy, University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, Pa. Hoagland, S. W., and Walker, S. G. (1979). Operationalcodes and crisisoutcomes. In Falkowski, L. S. (ed.), Psychological Models in International Politics. Westview, Boulder, Colo., pp. 125-168. Holsti, 0. (1971). Crisis, stress, and decision-making. Int. Social Sci. J. 22: 53-67. Holsti, 0. (1976). Foreign policy decision-makers viewed psychologically: 'Cognitive process' approaches. In Rosenau, J. N. (ed.), In Search of Global Patterns, Free Press, New York, pp. 120-143. Holsti, 0. (1977). The 'Operational Code': A coding manual. In Holsti, O. (ed.), The 'Operational Code' as an Approach to the Analysis of Belief Systems, Final Report to the National Science Foundation, Grant SOC 75-15368, Duke University. Holsti, 0., and North, R. (1965). The history of human conflict. In McNeil, E. B. (ed.), The Nature of Human Conflict, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N. J., pp. 155-171. Janis, I., and Smith, M. B. (1965). Effects of education and persuasion on national and international images. In Kelman, H. (ed.), International Behavior: A Social-Psychological Analysis, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York, 188-235. Jervis, R. (1968). Hypotheses on misperception. World Polit. 20: 454-479. Jervis, R. (1976). Perception and Misperception in International Politics, Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J. Jervis, R. (1978). Cooperation under the security dilemma. World Politics, 30: 167-214. Jervis, R. (1979). Deterrence theory revisited. World Politics, 21: 289-324. Jervis, R. (1982a). Perception and misperception: An updating of the analysis, Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Society of Political Psychology, Washington, D.C. Jervis, R. (1982b). Security regimes. In Krasner,S. (ed.), International Regimes, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, N. Y., pp. 173-194. Jervis, R. (1985a). Introduction: Approaches and assumptions. In Jervis, R., Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (eds.), Psychology and Deterrence, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Md., pp. 1-12. Jervis, R. (1985b). Perceiving and coping with threat. In Jervis, R., Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (eds.), Psychology and Deterrence, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Md., pp. 13-33. Jervis, R., Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (eds.), (1985). Psychology and Deterrence, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Md. Jones, W. S., and Rosen, S. J. (1982). The Logic of International Relations (fourth edition), Winthrop, Cambridge, Mass. Kaplowitz, N. (1976). Psychopolitical dimensions of the Middle East conflict: Policy implications. J. Confl. Res. 20: 279-318. Kaplowitz, N. (1984). Psychopolitical dimensions of international relations: The reciprocal effects of conflict strategies. Int. Studies Q. 28: 373-406. Kelman, H. (1972). The problem-solving workshop in conflict resolution. In Merrit, R. (ed.), Communication in International Politics, University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ill., pp. 168-204. Kelman, H. (1978). Israelis and Palestinians: Psychological prerequisitesfor mutual acceptance. Int. Sec. 3: 162-186. Kinder, D. R., and Weiss, J. A. (1978). In lieu of rationality: Psychological perspectives on decision making. J. Confl. Res. 22: 707-735. Kohlberg, L. (1964). Development of moral character and moral ideology. In Hoffman, M. L., and Hoffman, L. W. (eds.), Child Development Research, Vol. 1, Russell Sage, New York, pp. 383-431. Lane, R. (1962). Political Ideology, Free Press, New York. Psychopolitical Dimensions of International Relations 81 Lane, R. (1969). Political Thinking and Consciousness, Markham, Chicago. Larson, D. B. (1985). Originsof Containment:A Psychological Explanation, Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J. Lasswell, H. (1930). Psychopathology and Politics, University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Lebow, R. N. (1981). Between Peace and War:The Nature of InternationalCrisis, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, N.Y. Lebow, R. N. (1985a). Miscalculation in the South Atlantic: The origins of the Falklands War. In Jervis, R., Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (eds.), Psychology and Deterrence, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Md., pp. 89-124. Lebow, R. N. (1985b). The deterrence deadlock: Is there a way out? In Jervis, R., Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (eds.), Psychology and Deterrence, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Md., pp. 180-202. Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (1987). Beyond deterrence. J. Social Issues 43(4): 5-71. Leites, N. (1953). A Study of Bolshevism, Free Press, New York. Leng, R. J., and Wheeler, H. G. (1979). Influence strategies, success, and war. J. Confl. Res. 23: 655-684. Levinson, D. J. (1957). Authoritarian personality and foreign policy. J. Confl. Res. 1: 37-47. May, E. R. (1973). 'Lessons' of the Past, Oxford University Press, New York. McGowan, P. M., and Shapiro, H. B. (1973). The Comparative Study of Foreign Policy: A Survey of Scientific Findings, Sage, Beverly Hills, Calif. Milburn, T. W. (1961). The concept of deterrence: Some logical and psychological considerations. J. Social Issues 17: 3-11. Milburn, T. W. (1973). When do threats provoke violent responses? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Studies Association, New York. Morgan, P. M. (1977). Deterrence: A Conceptual Analysis, Sage, Beverly Hills, Calif. Morgan, P. M. (1985). Saving face for the sake of deterrence. In Jervis, E., Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (eds.), Psychology and Deterrence, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Md., pp. 125-152. Morgenthau, H. (1960). Politics Among Nations, third edition, Knopf, New York. Pye, L. (1962). Politics, Personality, and Nation Building, Yale University Press, New Haven. Pye, L. (1965). Introduction: Political culture and political development. In Pye, L., and Verba, S. (eds.), Political Culture and Political Development, Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J., pp. 3-26. Pye, L. (1968). The Spirit of Chinese Politics: A Psychological Study of the Authority Crisis in Political Development, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass. Rachlin, H. (1970). Introduction to Modern Behaviorism, Freeman, San Francisco. Rapoport, A. (1960). Fights, Games, and Debates, University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor. Rapoport, A. (1964). Strategy and Conscience, Schocken, New York. Redl, F., and Wineman, D. (1957). The Aggressive Child, Free Press, New York. Rokeach, M. (1968). Beliefs, Attitudes, and Values, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco. Rosecrance, R. (1981). Reward, punishment, and interdependence. J. Confl. Res. 25: 31-46. Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the Self, Basic Books, New York. Schelling, T. (1960). The Strategy of Conflict, Oxford University Press, New York. Shapiro, M. J., and Bonham, G. M. (1973). Cognitive process and foreign policy decision-making. Int. Studies Q. 17: 147-176. Sniderman, P. (1975). Personality and Democractic Politics, University of California Press, Berkeley. Snyder, G. H., and Diesing, P. (1977). Conflict Among Nations: Bargaining, Decision Making, and System Structure in International Crises, Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J. Stagner, R. (1965). The psychology of human conflict. In McNeil, E. B. (ed.), The Nature of Human Conflict, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N.J., pp. 45-63. Stein, J. G, (1985a). Calculation, miscalculation, and conventional deterrence I: The view from Cairo. In Jervis, R., Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (eds.), Psychology and Deterrence, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Md., pp. 34-59. Stein, J. G. (1985b). Calculation, miscalculation, and conventional deterrenceII: The view from Jerusalem. In Jervis, R., Lebow, R. N., and Stein, J. G. (eds.), Psychology and Deterrence, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Md., pp. 60-88. 82 Kaplowitz Steinbrenner,J. D. (1974). The CyberneticTheoryof Decision, PrincetonUniversityPress, Princeton,N.J. Strozier,C. B. (ed.) (1985).Self-Psychologyand the Humanities:Reflectionson a New PsychoanalyticApproach. Suedfeld,P., Tetlock,P. E., and Rodriguez,C. (1977).Peace, war,and integrativecomplexity. J. Confl. Res. 21: 427-442. Verba,S. (1965).Conclusion:Comparativepoliticalculture.In Pye, L., and Verba,S. (eds.), PoliticalCultureandPoliticalDevelopment,PrincetonUniversityPress,Princeton,N.J. 512-560. Waltz, K. (1954).Man, the State, and War,ColumbiaUniversityPress, New York. Waltz, K. (1979). Theoryof InternationalPolitics, Addison-Wesley,Reading,Mass. in VietnamandOtherWars,DoubleWhite,R. K. (1970).Nobody WantedWar:Misperception day, New York. Wilkenfeld,J., Hopple, G. W., Rossa, P. J., and Andriole,S. J. (1980).ForeignPolicy Behavior, Sage, BeverlyHills, Calif. Zinnes,D., North, R., and Koch, Jr., H. (1961).Capability,threat,and the outbreakof war. In Rosenau,J. N. (ed.),International PoliticsandForeignPolicy,FreePress,New York, pp. 469-482.