Developmental Science 10:5 (2007), pp 654– 663 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00607.x PAPER Blackwell Publishing Ltd Do you believe in magic? Infants’ social looking during violations of expectations Tedra Walden, Geunyoung Kim, Carrie McCoy and Jan Karrass Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, USA Abstract Young infants tend to look longer at physical events that have unexpected outcomes than those that have expected outcomes, suggesting that they have knowledge of physical principles such as numerosity and occlusion (Baillargeon & Graber, 1987; Wynn, 1992). Although infants are typically tested in the presence of a caregiver, the social component of violations of expectations has received little attention. The present study investigated social looking during presumably expected and unexpected cognitive/ perceptual events. Two experiments replicated the results of well-known physical knowledge experiments on addition/subtraction and occlusion in 6- (Experiments 1 and 2) and 9-month-old infants (Experiment 1), in that infants at both ages looked longer at unexpected than at expected events. Furthermore, infants at both ages initiated more looks at their caregivers’ faces during unexpected than expected events. These findings are interpreted as suggesting that infants as young as 6 months of age actively seek to embed their experiences of unexpected physical/cognitive events in a social context. Introduction In 1934, Lev Vygotsky noted that understanding the interplay of cognition and the social world was critical in understanding the development of the individual as a whole. Decades later, there is still much to learn about the integration of social and cognitive development. The present study focused on social behaviors when young infants witnessed events that violated their expectations about how physical objects behave. Studies of infants’ nonsocial reactions to violations of their expectations regarding cognitive/perceptual phenomena have suggested that infants possess some understanding of physical events such as occlusion, containment, and support early in the first year of life. Baillargeon and Graber (1987) and Wynn (1992) have shown that infants look longer at unexpected events than at expected events, indicating that by 6 months of age infants have some knowledge of physical principles such as occlusion and numerosity. Both Baillargeon and Graber and Wynn used a violation of expectation (VOE) method, which compares infants’ visual attention to displays of physical objects and events as they usually occur (expected or possible events) to ‘tricks’ in which impossible physical events appear to occur (unexpected because they violate physical principles, although Luo and Baillargeon (2005) studied ordinary possible events that were inconsistent with infants’ limited knowledge). It is based on infants’ tendency to look longer at events that they find novel or surprising (Baillargeon, Spelke & Wasserman, 1985), which has been linked theoretically to increases in alertness and ‘orientational-investigative activity’ (Berlyne, 1960, 1978; Berlyne & Borsa, 1968; Sokolov, Nezlina, Polyanskii & Evtikhin, 2002). Baillargeon (2004) used the term ‘surprise’ to indicate the ‘state of heightened interest or attention induced by an expectation violation’ (p. 392), although others have used arousal- or emotion-based descriptions of the experience of viewing impossible events appear to occur (e.g. Camras, Meng, Ujlie, Dharamsi, Miyake, Oster, Wang, Cruz, Murdoch & Campos, 2002). VOE methods have been criticized because of possible confounding variables that may account for increased looking at unexpected events (e.g. Cohen & Marks, 2002, who presented data from three numeriosity experiments with 5-month-old infants that supported an explanation for addition/subtraction focusing on preferences for familiarity and larger numbers of objects, arguably less developmentally advanced mechanisms). Sorting out competing explanations for increased looking at the unexpected events is important for understanding infants’ Address for correspondence: Tedra Walden, GPC 512, Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, 230 Appleton Place, Nashville, TN 37203, USA; e-mail: tedra.walden@vanderbilt.edu © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. Infant social looking behavior following the events, but the present studies focused on the simpler question of whether infants engage in social behavior following the presumably unexpected events in VOE. Although infants are typically tested in the presence of a caregiver, possible social components of violations of expectations have received little attention (cf. Camras et al., 2002). We reasoned that infants may be aroused by violations of their expectations, and that this arousal occurs in a social context in which they seek to resolve their uncertainty or share their experience. Studies of infant social referencing have shown that 1-year-old infants faced with novel or ambiguous events (e.g. unusual robots) are likely to visually reference the emotional expressions of others and use these expressions to guide their own behavior toward the events (e.g. Sorce, Emde, Campos & Klinnert, 1985). For example, 12-month-old infants are more likely to approach and play with a toy associated with positive emotional messages than with a toy associated with fearful messages. Social referencing is said to provide information about how to interpret the meaning of events and is thought to be developmentally adaptive because, as is characteristic of social learning strategies, it capitalizes on the experiences of others and does not require first-hand experience with uncommon, unexpected, or potentially dangerous events (Feinman, Roberts, Hsieh, Sawyer & Swanson, 1992). There has been little empirical work specifying conditions under which infants engage in social referencing (cf. Sorce et al., 1985); however, most theoretical accounts emphasize novelty or ambiguity (Feinman et al., 1992). Stenberg (2003) reported that 1-year-old infants look at other persons more in ambiguous situations than in unambiguous situations. Most experiments have used arousing stimuli that infants might perceive as dangerous or unpleasant (e.g. motorized robots, approach of strangers, crossing an apparent drop-off). Studies of social referencing embed these stimuli in a social and emotional context; caregivers are positioned within view and respond with emotional messages when the infants look toward them (e.g. Walden & Ogan, 1988). Social referencing has been reliably observed in infants as young as 12 months of age, so this is generally considered the age of onset (cf. Striano, Vaish & Benigno, in press). Walden and Ogan (1988) found evidence of social referencing in 10–12-month-old infants but not younger infants. They also found selectivity in social looking such that, especially for older infants, looks toward caregivers’ faces differed when caregivers expressed positive and negative emotion, whereas looks toward caregivers’ bodies did not differ. It has been difficult to investigate social referencing in younger infants because current methods require that infants are mobile (and © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 655 able to approach or avoid stimuli), that they understand the social-emotional messages about the event (as well as understanding that the messages refer to the event), and that they can regulate their responses accordingly. This requires coordination of cognitive, social, and motor behavior. However, components of social referencing, such as joint attention and social looking, begin to develop earlier in the first year of life (e.g. Butterworth & Jarrett, 1991; Carpenter, Nagell & Tomasello, 1998). For example, Striano and Rochat (2000) showed that 10month-old infants regulate their social looking following toy dog-barking events in response to variations in the availability of their social partner for interaction by looking more toward an attentive partner than one who is looking away. Another relevant development during the first year is joint attention between infants and other people regarding objects or events (e.g. Carpenter et al., 1998), a skill that Tomasello (1995) and Hobson (2002) described as two minds aware that they are focusing on the same topic. For example, Striano and Stahl (2005) reported that infants as young as 3 months show a sensitivity in social looking to triadic attention involving an adult stranger. Violations of cognitive expectations may set the stage for infants to engage in social referencing behaviors, or at least social looking, because the events may be novel or ambiguous. Violations of expectations differ from typical social referencing stimuli in that they involve interpretations of physical events, rather than potential danger or unpleasantness. Whether social behaviors, such as social looks, accompany infants’ reactions to these events has not been thoroughly investigated. Camras and colleagues (2002) studied emotional expressions of 11-month-old infants during a covert toy-switch event, which they argued was expectancy violating (see also Baillargeon, 2004). Despite the difficulty in coding surprise in infants and the fact that surprise expressions are infrequent during violation of expectancy procedures (Camras et al., 2002; Hiatt, Campos & Emde, 1979), they found that expressions of surprise and interest (but not social looking) were more frequent during unexpected than expected events. In the present study we focused on occurrences of social looking (infant initiated looks at others) that accompany infants’ responses to presumably expected and unexpected physical events. In sum, 1-year-old infants are likely to look to other persons during times of uncertainty. In addition, infants use others’ reactions to events as information guiding their own interpretation of events. Components of social referencing, such as social looking, emerge earlier than 12 months but we do not know whether social looking is used in conditions of novelty or uncertainty for young infants, especially for events that are nonsocial in nature 656 Tedra Walden et al. and do not involve emotional elements such as danger, disgust, or happiness. Studying infants’ social looking during violations of their expectations about physical events allows us to address some of these questions. It also might be a sensitive measure of infants’ reactions to uncertainty at younger ages than is typically observed. We conducted two experiments using well-known tasks of young infants’ knowledge of numerosity and occlusion (Baillargeon & Graber, 1987; Simon, Hespos & Rochat, 1995; Wynn, 1992). Unlike studies of infant social referencing, caregivers were nonresponsive to infant looks; therefore we did not address the question of how infants might have used social information or emotion to interpret the events. Instead, we focused on infant social looks to caregivers. We expected that social looking would occur during unexpected physical events. Because previous social referencing research has emphasized the importance of infants’ looks at parents’ faces, rather than other parts of the body, we examined them separately. We were especially interested in infants’ looks toward their caregivers’ faces, since faces have been considered key in providing social information, and studies of social referencing in 1-year-old infants have suggested that looks to the face are associated with searches for social information, whereas body looks are not (e.g. being more often associated with security- or comfort-seeking; see Walden & Ogan, 1988, for discussion of the meaning of looks at faces and nonface looks). Experiment 1 Six- and 9-month old infants’ social looking was compared during unexpected and expected events involving numerosity, using methods reported in Wynn (1992) and Simon et al. (1995). We predicted an age by event interaction such that older infants would engage in more social looking during unexpected than expected events, with younger infants engaging in little social looking under either condition; furthermore, we expected this effect to be primarily accounted for by looks to caregivers’ faces rather than nonface looks. Method Participants Twenty-five 6- and 26 9-month-old infants were recruited through phone calls from birth records available from the State of Tennessee. Three participated with fathers; the remainder with mothers. The sample was predominantly Caucasian and middle class. Data from nine 6-month-old (three had experimental errors, three were terminated © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. early, one was fussy, one was inattentive, and one mother failed to follow instructions) and 10 9-month-old infants (three had experimental errors, four were terminated early, one was fussy, one was inattentive, and one mother failed to follow instructions) were removed. The final sample included 16 6- (M = 5.7 months of age, range 5;15 to 6;14) and 16 9-month-old infants (M = 9.0 months of age, range 8;15 to 9;14). No sex differences were found in any analysis. Procedure We followed procedures reported in Wynn (1992) and Simon et al. (1995). Children sat on their parents’ laps at a viewing distance of one meter away from a puppet stage (about 100 cm high, 130 cm wide, and 70 cm deep) with an opening 36 cm high and 90 cm wide in the center. A rotating screen 50 cm wide and 30 cm high was positioned in the center of the stage. A large screen was lowered to cover the opening between the infants and the stage. There were flaps cut on the sides of the stage to allow experimenters to manipulate objects. The experiment contained four events. First, during baseline events, each infant saw both a 15-cm high single Elmo puppet and two Elmo puppets (twice each, with order counterbalanced). There was no significant difference in looking at one vs. two puppets during baseline. Second, pre-test events acquainted infants with movements of the experimenter’s hand and rotating screen and were identical to test events but did not include Elmos. Specifically, infants saw an empty stage, then the screen rotated up, the experimenter’s hand reached behind the screen and came out empty, and finally the screen rotated down. Following four pre-test events, six test trials occurred. The test events were of two types: expected and unexpected, and two categories: addition and subtraction. In the addition condition, infants first saw a single Elmo on stage. After 5 sec of infant gaze fixation, the rotating screen raised and occluded the object. Then the experimenter’s hand emerged from the flap on the side of the stage, holding an additional Elmo puppet. She slowly moved the object behind the occluder and showed the empty hand to infants. Immediately following, the occluder rotated down to reveal one (Unexpected: 1 + 1 = 1) or two Elmos (Expected: 1 + 1 = 2) on the stage. Orders of stimulus presentation were counterbalanced, and no order effect was found in any subsequent analyses. The subtraction condition was similar to the addition condition, except that infants first saw two Elmos on stage. The experimenter showed the empty hand after the rotating screen raised, moved the hand behind the screen, and took one of the objects. Again, infants saw either one (Expected: Infant social looking 657 Figure 1 Test events for Experiment 1. 2 – 1 = 1) or two puppets (Unexpected: 2 – 1 = 2) when the rotating screen lowered (see Figure 1). There were three expected and three unexpected events for each infant (addition/subtraction randomly assigned across infants). A second observer who was unaware of the events on stage or the experimental condition watched a live video of the infant’s face from a separate room. When the infant looked away from the stage for 2 consecutive sec, she signaled the main experimenter to lower the large screen between infants and the stage to end the trial. Caregivers closed their eyes and did not interact with the child during the study. and upward, involving head and chin movement were counted as a look toward face. Looks to the side alone or looks down were counted as a look toward body (see Walden & Ogan, 1988, for further details). All social looks were actively initiated by the infants, and caregivers did not respond to any looks. To control for differences in trial duration, rates of each type of social look (number of looks/trial time in sec) were used for analyses. Two observers blind to trial condition coded infants’ behaviors. Interobserver agreement for duration of infants’ looks at the stimuli was .89. Intraclass correlations were .99 for face looks, and .98 for nonface looks. Measures Results and discussion Two types of infant looking behaviors were coded from videotape by observers who were unaware of experimental condition: duration of looking at the stimuli during each event, a typical measure of interest or discrimination (see Simon et al., 1995), and number of infant looks toward the caregiver, a typical measure of social referencing. Duration of social looking was not used because social looks longer than 2 sec ended a trial and truncated coding the duration of these looks. In addition, infant social looks were differentiated into two types: looks that would have brought the caregiver’s face into focal view and nonface looks directed at the caregiver’s body. Only looks to the side First, duration of looking at expected and unexpected events was compared. Repeated measures analysis of variance with two within-subject factors, trial (6 presentations) and type of event (2: expected or unexpected), and one between-subject factor, age (2), indicated that both 6- and 9-month old infants looked at the stimulus longer during the unexpected events (1 + 1 = 1 or 2 – 1 = 2; F(1, 30) = 6.74, p < .05, partial η2 = .18; see Table 1). Individuals showed the same pattern, with 12 of 16 infants in each age group looking longer at unexpected events. These findings replicated previous findings reported by Wynn (1992) and Simon et al. (1995) and showed that infants discriminated the events. © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 658 Tedra Walden et al. Table 1 Means (Standard Deviations) for looks to targets and social looks Experiment 1 Condition Experiment duration (sec) 6 months 9 months Target looks (duration in sec) 6 months 9 months Frequency of social looks at faces 6 months 9 months Frequency of nonface social looks 6 months 9 months Rate of social looks at facea 6 months 9 months Rate of nonface social looksa 6 months 9 months Expected events Unexpected events 36.31 (14.74) 35.43 (9.66) 45.61 (21.03) 40.25 (21.53) 25.21 (11.33) 24.74 (6.49) 34.19 (16.57) 29.13 (17.22) 1.06 (1.43) .75 (1.00) 1.56 (2.09) 1.19 (1.11) 1.63 (1.86) 2.00 (1.90) 1.56 (1.26) 2.31 (1.82) .03 (.04) .02 (.03) .04 (.06) .04 (.05) .04 (.05) .06 (.05) .04 (.05) .07 (.08) Figure 2 Experiment 1: Infants’ looks to caregivers during expected and unexpected events. Note: a Due to unequal duration of trials across infants, all analyses of social looks were based on rates (frequency/trial duration). Twenty-seven of the 32 participants engaged in some type of social looking, with 25 engaging in nonface looking and 20 looking toward caregivers’ faces. Thirteen of 16 6-month-old and 14 of 16 9-month-old infants initiated social looks, with 63% (17/27) of those engaging in both face and nonface looking toward caregivers. Overall ANOVA for social looks with 2 within- (type of event, type of social look) and one between-subject factors (age) showed a significant main effect for the type of social look, F(1, 30) = 5.71, p < .05, partial η2 = .16, in that infants tended to look at non-face areas more than face areas. A marginally significant main effect for the type of event, F(1, 30) = 3.15, p = .08, partial η2 = .10, was found, in that infants looked at caregivers more during unexpected events than during expected events. In this omnibus analysis, the interaction between type of event and type of social look was not significant, F(1, 30) = .30, ns (see Figure 2). Since looks toward caregivers’ faces was of special interest for the present study, looks to face and those toward body parts were separately analyzed (preplanned comparisons). For looks toward faces, infants at both ages looked at a higher rate at their caregivers’ faces during unexpected than expected events, F(1, 30) = 4.23, p < .05, partial η2 = .12. A separate planned comparison at 6 and 9 months tested whether the effect held at each age. Due to the loss of statistical power by splitting the data, it did not reach significance. However, both younger (1.57 vs. 1.063) and older (1.19 vs. .75) infants © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. Figure 3 Experiment 1: Infants’ face and nonface looks to caregivers. * p < .05 for expected vs. unexpected. tended to look at caregivers’ faces more often during unexpected events (see Figure 3). There was no significant interaction with age. There was no difference in nonface looking for expected and unexpected events, F(1, 30) = 1.45, ns. This pattern indicates that infants were not simply ‘looking around’ more during the unexpected events, but focused selectively on the target stimulus or caregivers’ faces. The relations among social looks, nonsocial looks, and trial time were further examined. First, correlations between looks toward target and social looks indicated that duration of looks to target was negatively correlated with social looks only during unexpected events, r = .42, p < .05 for looks to face, r = −.52, p < .01 for nonface looks. The correlations were not significant during expected events, r = −.22, ns for looks to face, r = −.13, ns, for nonface looks, suggesting that infants were more systematically coordinating their target looking with social looks during unexpected events. Second, the number Infant social looking of trials ended by social looks accounted for about 30% of all trials. Specifically, an average of 1.31 trials of younger and 2.19 trials of older infants out of six test trials were terminated because the infants were involved in social looking. The age difference was not statistically significant, t(30) = 1.20, ns. Results of Experiment 1 are consistent with Simon et al. (1995) and Wynn (1992), showing longer looking at targets during unexpected events. In addition, infants used a social strategy (social looks) when they encountered cognitive/ perceptual ambiguity. There were no age differences. Experiment 2 To replicate and extend the results of Experiment 1, a new task was identified for a group of 6-month-old infants. Baillargeon and Graber’s (1987) violationof-expectation task involving infants’ expectations about the height of objects behind occluders was used. Based on the results of Experiment 1, we predicted that infants would look toward caregivers’ faces, especially during unexpected events. Method Participants Twenty 6-month-old infants and their mothers (19) or full-time babysitters (1) were recruited through phone calls from state birth records. Half were male. The sample was predominantly Caucasian and middle class. Four infants were excluded (two had experimental errors, and two caregivers failed to follow instructions). The final sample included 16 infants (M = 6.1 mos, range 5;16 to 6;14). Procedure Children sat on their caregivers’ laps facing the same puppet stage used for the Study 1, with the caregiver’s body and chair angled 45 degrees either to the left or right of the center of the stage (counterbalanced). This positioning allowed the infant to look at his or her caregiver more easily, if desired. Caregivers closed their eyes and did not interact with the child. An occlusion task tested 6-month-old infants’ expectations about tall (27 cm) and short (13 cm) toy carrots that passed behind a screen following the method described in Baillargeon and DeVos (1991). The carrots were made of orange plush fur and each had two green leaves, black thread eyes, and a black thread mouth. Each carrot was sewn to an L-bracket carrier used to © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 659 slide the carrots across the stage out of view of the infant. The rotating screen used in Study 1 was replaced with two freestanding screens, one full occluder used for pre-test events and one screen with a cutout used for test trials. Each infant saw four pre-test events, which alternated between short and tall carrots that moved across the stage (left to right from the infant’s view) and passed behind a full occluder (no cutout). The pre-test events demonstrated the carrots and their trajectories and made it possible to determine if the infants preferred looking at the short or tall carrot (test of the difference was F(1, 15) = .56, ns). After the pre-test trials, each infant viewed six test events, which alternated between expected (short carrot passed behind a short occluder unseen) and unexpected events (tall carrot passed behind a short occluder unseen) or the reverse (see Figure 4). The test events were identical to the pre-test events, except that the cutout screen was used. A short and a tall carrot traversed the puppet stage and passed behind a screen with a 20-cm × 15-cm cutout in the top half, a height that revealed the tall but not the short carrot as it passed behind. It is expected that the short carrot could pass behind the occluder unseen (expected event), but when the tall carrot passed behind the occluder unseen, it was unexpected (unexpected event). In order to produce the illusion, two carrots of each size were actually used, one which started moving across the stage and passed behind the tall portion of the near side of the screen and a second carrot that had been hidden behind the screen and appeared from behind the tall portion of the far side of the screen. Each event took approximately 12 sec and the cycle was repeated until the signaled end of the trial (infant looked away from stage for at least 2 sec). A metronome beat assisted the experimenter in timing the events to appear as one continuous movement across the stage. Each trial continued until the infant looked away from the stage for 2 consecutive sec, as judged by an observer blind to experimental condition. There were three expected and three unexpected events for each infant. Measures Measures are as described in Experiment 1. Interobserver agreement was .94 for duration of infants’ looks at the stimuli, with intraclass correlations of .99 for both face and nonface looks. Results and discussion Results replicated findings from previous studies, with infants looking longer at unexpected than expected events, F(1, 15) = 6.92, p < .05, partial η2 = .32. Individuals 660 Tedra Walden et al. Figure 4 Test events for Experiment 2. showed the same pattern, with 12 of 16 infants looking longer at the unexpected events. These findings are consistent with Baillargeon and Graber (1987) and Baillargeon and DeVos (1991) and support the position that the unexpected events elicited special interest. Fifteen of 16 participants engaged in some type of social looking, with 13 engaging in nonface looking and nine engaging in face looking. Infants were separated into two groups: those who looked at the target more during unexpected events and those who did not. A repeated measures ANOVA did not show a significant interaction of event (expected vs. unexpected) and targetlook group on the number of social looks toward the caregiver’s face, F(1, 15) = 2.08, p = .17 (see Table 2). As in Experiment 1, looks toward the caregiver’s face were analyzed separately. A repeated measures ANOVA indicated that the infants were more likely to reference their caregivers’ faces during unexpected events than expected events; mean rates per sec .017 and .009, respectively; F(1, 15) = 5.28, p < .05, partial η2 = .26 (see Table 2 and Figure 5). However, nonface looks toward the caregiver did not differ for expected and unexpected events, F(1, 15) = .39, ns. The results of Experiment 2 replicated the results of Experiment 1, using a new task with 6-month-old infants. The effects were replicated even though the new task elicited higher overall durations of target looks and lower rates of social looks; thus, task influenced amount of looking but the effects of unexpected events on social and target looks remained. Infants were more likely to look at caregivers’ faces during unexpected than expected events, whereas nonface looks did not differ. Findings from the two experiments suggest that social looking toward caregivers’ faces is a reliable phenomenon for young infants faced with the unexpected events in the VOE procedure. © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. Table 2 Means (Standard Deviations) for looks to targets and social looks Experiment 2 Condition Expected events Experiment duration (sec) Duration of target looks (sec) Frequency of social looks at faces Frequency of nonface social looks Rate of social looks at facesa Rate of nonface social looksa 71.78 60.73 .81 1.93 .01 .03 (28.78) (26.21) (1.60) (2.35) (.02) (.03) Unexpected events 89.76 77.76 1.56 1.88 .02 .02 (23.34) (21.66) (1.96) (1.71) (.02) (.02) Note: a Due to unequal trial durations across infants, all analyses of social looks were based on rates (frequency/trial duration). Figure 5 Experiment 2: Infants’ face and nonface looks to caregivers. * p < .05 for expected vs. unexpected. General discussion In her 2000 address to the APA for her Award for Distinguished Scientific Contributions, Elizabeth Spelke Infant social looking asked, ‘How do humans develop and deploy complex, species-specific and culture-specific cognitive skills such as reading, mathematics, mapmaking, myriad forms of tool use, and reasoning about the physical and social world?’ She argued that we should broaden our search for insights into the development of these skills. Similarly, in 1991 Baillargeon and DeVos noted that the richness and sophistication of young infants’ knowledge and abilities about the physical world raises important questions about the origins and development of this world knowledge. The two studies described here broaden that search to include social interactional contributions to understanding physical concepts. The present studies replicated findings of two well-known phenomena in infant cognitive development, one involving numerosity and one involving occlusion, showing that infants looked longer at events that are presumed to violate their expectations about how physical objects behave. In both experiments, infants also initiated more social looks to their caregivers when faced with unexpected physical events than when the events were expected. Although the two tasks elicited different rates of target and social looks, unexpected events elicited more of both types of looks in both experiments. These results suggest that social strategies are in the repertoire of infants and that referring to others may be a part of infants’ experience of events in the physical world. We expected that we might find social looking among 9-month-old infants, even though the tasks were cognitive and relatively nonsocial. We found that both 6- and 9-month-old infants actively engaged the social environment following violations of expectations. Infants sat on their motionless and unresponsive caregivers’ laps. To turn around and look back toward them and bring them into focal view (peripheral looks were not coded), especially upward at their faces, was no small task for these young infants (try it yourself!). That social looks occurred at all under these conditions speaks to the depth of the infants’ motivation to look at their caregivers. These conditions probably reduced the amount of social looking; as well as resulting in some infants’ looks that may have been intended toward the caregiver’s face being directed below the face, and thus coded as nonface looks and reducing the discriminant power of the two categories of looks. Because of the low rate of social looking in the present studies, we were unable to examine trial-by-trial effects, which may be important for understanding the function of social looking and in understanding what individual children do when faced with unexpected physical events. Use of an eye-tracker in future studies may help identify sequences of looking. In the present studies, we attempted to replicate published procedures as exactly as © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 661 possible, but future studies should use procedures that make it easier for infants to look at their parents (and their faces), perhaps by positioning the caregiver to the side of the infant. In addition, responsive caregivers who have their eyes open might elicit higher rates of social looking, especially for infants in the age range (9 months) of the older infants in the present Experiment 1 (Striano & Rochat, 2000). Variations in the responsiveness of social partners will help us to understand the function of infants’ social looks. Finally, longer fixed-length trials may capture more infant social (and other) behavior than the procedures used in the current studies, in which looks away from the target lasting longer than 2 sec terminated the trials. Infant target looks and social looks must be interpreted in the context of a fuller consideration of the many ways that infants may behave in response to events that violate their expectations. Infants’ social looking was selective, in that they differentially looked at their caregivers’ faces following the unexpected events. Although we cannot determine the function of looks in these studies, for older infants (i.e. 12–24 months) in social referencing situations, looks at faces have been related to information seeking, whereas nonface looks are related to comfort-seeking responses (Walden & Kim, 2005; Walden & Ogan, 1988). Berlyne (1960) cited novelty, ‘surprisingness’, and uncertainty as factors that create cognitive conflict, motivating a search for information. If perception involves an active process of detecting and making sense of information (e.g. Gibson & Spelke, 1983), then social sources can be useful and efficient in interpreting events and may contribute to knowledge about those events. Looks at caregivers’ faces might also reflect affect-sharing attempts. For example, infants may have felt surprised by the unexpected events and wanted to share that experience (or even to share a positive experience of liking or preference, see Cohen & Marks, 2002). Or, infants may have learned an association between the occurrence of an arousing or surprising event and a caregiver response and they turned to the caregiver in anticipation of a response. Infants may also have been interested in knowing whether the caregiver was paying attention to the same interesting event (joint or coordinated attention; e.g. Tomasello, 1995). An important task for future research will be sorting out which of these processes (or which combination) motivates social looking. We have taken an admittedly cognitively rich interpretation of infant behavior. Increased looking at unexpected events was interpreted as reflecting increased interest as a result of violations of infants’ expectations, and social looking was suggested to be a strategy invoked to help resolve uncertainty. Cohen and Marks (2002) and Bogartz and colleagues (Bogartz, Shinsky & Speaker, 1997) offered 662 Tedra Walden et al. competing explanations for behavior in VOE procedures, focusing on preferences (a positive reaction) for familiarity and numerosity. A concern for numerosity may apply to our first experiment (though we controlled this), but the second experiment did not include numerosity; however, familiarity may have been produced by the baseline events in both experiments. Therefore, future work should include other VOE tasks. Furthermore, infants in the present studies were older and should be less influenced by these factors (Baillargeon, 2004; Cohen & Marks, 2002). Finally, if mildly positive experiences such as preferences prompt infant social looking, then the range of situations in which infants might initiate social interaction is expanded. Future studies can incorporate these factors in research designs to understand their impact and to take them into account in our theories of social cognitive development. That is, social looking that is motivated by a desire to share positive experiences may reflect different processes than motives to resolve uncertainty. Future studies might incorporate variations in tasks and in the usefulness of social information in order to identify factors that might encourage (or discourage) social looking (e.g. Kim & Walden, 2006). The present study was not a study of social referencing, in that no caregiver–child communication occurred and there is no test of whether infants might have been influenced by communications received during unexpected events (these infants received no responses from their caregivers at all, which might be expected to decrease the rate of social looking). Yet, social referencing is one likely explanation for the present results, suggesting that more sensitive methods might identify social referencing at earlier ages than has been typically found and in situations that are less social and more cognitive. The findings of the present study offer a possible alternative interpretation of infants’ looks away from the events in violation-of-expectation experiments. These looks have been interpreted as reflecting loss of interest or boredom for expected events (Baillargeon, 1999). The present findings suggest that at least some of the looks away from the display are attempts to engage the social environment, rather than reflecting simply loss of interest. Such social looks, which terminated a trial when they lasted 2 sec or more (30% of trials in Experiment 1 and 20% in Experiment 2), may have truncated, thus underestimated, duration of looking at unexpected events, as well as estimates of social looking. The occurrence of social looks also suggests that the testing situations used to study violations of cognitive/perceptual expectations may have a larger social component than previously appreciated. Finally, it is noteworthy that infants actively initiated looks at their caregivers during two different events – violations © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. of expectations about numerosity and occlusion. The two tasks have been said to reside in different ‘core knowledge systems’ that are domain specific, task specific, and operate independently from other systems (Spelke, 2000). The findings of the present study suggest that social interaction may be a strategy for dealing with uncertainty in a variety of domains. That is, attempts to engage the social world may be an early strategy at the service of many cognitive, perceptual and social understandings. Gibson and Spelke (1983) noted that infants are ‘richly endowed with the means of finding out about the environment’ and the present studies suggest that involving other persons may be one such means. The finding that infants actively sought to embed their experiences of cognitively puzzling events in a social context expands our view of how infants experience these events in the world and suggests the possibility of new explanatory mechanisms. Acknowledgements We are indebted to Susan Hespos, who provided videotapes of the procedure in Experiment 1 and facilities and assistance for Experiment 2 (McCoy’s undergraduate Honors thesis under the direction of Walden), and to Daniel Ashmead for comments on the manuscript. Many thanks to the parents and infants from the Nashville area who participated. Portions of the paper were presented at the 2004 ICIS meeting in Chicago, IL. References Baillargeon, R. (1999). Young infants’ expectations about hidden objects: a reply to three challenges. Developmental Science, 2, 115–132. Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants’ reasoning about hidden objects: evidence for event-general and event-specific expectations. Developmental Science, 7, 391–424. Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young infants: further evidence. Child Development, 62, 1227–1246. Baillargeon, R., & Graber, M. (1987). Where’s the rabbit? 5.5-month-old infants’ representation of the height of a hidden object. Cognitive Development, 2, 375–392. Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191– 208. Berlyne, D.E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill. Berlyne, D.E. (1978). Curiosity and learning. Motivation and Emotion, 2, 97–175. Berlyne, D.E., & Borsa, D.M. (1968). Uncertainty and the orientation reaction. Perception and Psychophysics, 3, 77–79. Infant social looking Bogartz, R., Shinskey, J., & Speaker, C. (1997). Interpreting infant looking: the event set × event set design. Developmental Psychology, 33, 408 – 422. Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common in space: spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55 – 72. Camras, L., Meng, Z., Ujlie, T., Dharamsi, S., Miyake, K., Oster, H., Wang, L., Cruz, J., Murdoch, A., & Campos, J. (2002). Observing emotion in infants: facial expression, body behavior, and rater judgments of responses to an expectancyviolating event. Emotion, 2, 179 –193. Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 63 (4, Serial No. 231). Cohen, L.B., & Marks, K.S. (2002). How infants process addition and subtraction events. Developmental Science, 5, 186 – 201. Feinman, S., Roberts, D., Hsieh, K., Sawyer, D., & Swanson, D. (1992). A critical review of social referencing in infancy. In S. Feinman (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality (pp. 15 – 54). New York: Plenum Press. Gibson, E.J., & Spelke, E.S. (1983). The development of perception. In Handbook of child psychology (4th edn.): Cognitive Development (Vol. 3; pp. 1– 76). New York: Wiley. Hiatt, S., Campos, J., & Emde, R. (1979). Facial patterning and infant emotional expression: happiness, surprise, and fear. Child Development, 50, 1020 –1035. Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought. London: Macmillan. Kim, G., & Walden, T. (2006). Do infants understand others’ expertise? A social referencing study. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the International Conference on Infant Studies, June, Kyoto, Japan. Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16, 601– 608. Simon, T., Hespos, S., & Rochat, P. (1995). Do infants © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 663 understand simple arithmetic? A replication of Wynn (1992). Cognitive Development, 10, 253–269. Sokolov, E.N., Nezlina, N.I., Polyanskii, V.B., & Evtikhin, D.V. (2002). The orienting reflex: the ‘targeting reaction’ and ‘searchlight of attention’. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 32, 347–362. Sorce, J., Emde, R., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195– 200. Spelke, E. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55, 1233–1243. Stenberg, G. (2003). Effects of maternal inattentiveness on infant social referencing. Infant and Child Development, 12, 399–419. Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (2000). Emergence of selective social referencing in infancy. Infancy, 1, 253–264. Striano, T., & Stahl, D. (2005). Sensitivity to triadic attention in early infancy. Developmental Science, 4, 333–343. Striano, T., Vaish, A., & Benigno, J.P. (in press). The meaning of infants’ looks: information seeking and comfort seeking? British Journal of Developmental Psychology. Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Vygotsky, L. (1934/1962). Thought and language. New York: MIT Press and Wiley. Walden, T., & Kim, G. (2005). Infants’ social looking toward mothers and strangers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 356–360. Walden, T., & Ogan, T. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child Development, 59, 1230–1240. Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749–750. Received: 17 April 2006 Accepted: 30 August 2006