Unit V States of Consciousness PD Unit Overview We take our consciousness for granted. We like to think we are conscious and psychologically “present” at any given point in our day and confuse our wakefulness with our being conscious. Yet, our consciousness is not just being aware or being awake. Being conscious involves a complex mix of different levels of awareness and wakefulness—it even includes times during which we are not aware or even awake! This unit explores several types of consciousness beyond our being aware and awake: hypnosis, sleep, and psychoactive drug effects. Each state of consciousness has its own biological and psychological effects on our awareness and wakefulness. We consider each a different state of consciousness, but we do process information and can even solve problems in these “altered” states. So are we awake and aware even when we are not? This unit helps us start to answer that question. After reading this unit, students will be able to: • Describe how people have been fascinated with the study of consciousness throughout history. • Define hypnosis and how the process generally works. • Debate whether hypnosis is an altered state of consciousness. • Describe how the body’s biological rhythms influence daily life. • Outline the different stages of sleeping and dreaming. • Analyze the different biological and environmental influences on our sleep patterns. • • • • • • Explain why sleep is important. • • • • Understand common misconceptions about addiction. Evaluate the effects of sleep loss. Identify major sleep disorders. Understand common dream content. Explain why we might dream. Describe how tolerance and addiction work physiologically and psychologically. Identify and describe the effects of depressants. Identify and describe the effects of stimulants. Identify and describe the effects of hallucinogens. Alignment to AP® Course Description Topic 5: States of Consciousness (2–4% of AP® Examination) Module Topic Essential Questions Module 22 Defining Consciousness • What does it mean to be conscious? Hypnosis • What is hypnosis? Biological Rhythms and Sleep • How do biological rhythms affect everyday life? Sleep Theories • Why do we sleep? Sleep Deprivation and Sleep Disorders • If sleep is so important, why do some people avoid it? • How do sleep disorders affect daily life? Dreams • Are dreams important to daily life? Tolerance and Addiction • How does the misuse of drugs affect daily life? Types of Psychoactive Drugs • Why is it important to know about the different types of psychoactive drugs? Module 23 Module 24 Module 25 States of Consciousness MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 1 Unit V 217a 2/20/14 8:12 AM Unit Resources Module 22 Module 24 STUDENT ACTIVITY • Fact or Falsehood? • The Creative Imagination Scale • Hypnosis as Heightened Suggestibility TEACHER DEMONSTRATIONS • Hypnosis—A Stage Demonstration • The Relaxation Response Fact or Falsehood? School Start Times: An Informal Debate Visiting SleepNet Sleep Profile Sleep Strategies Dream Journal FLIP IT VIDEO FLIP IT VIDEO • Why Do We Dream? • Theories of Hypnosis Module 25 Module 23 STUDENT ACTIVITIES STUDENT ACTIVITIES • • • • STUDENT ACTIVITIES • • • • • • • • • • • The Sleep IQ Test Remembering Daydreams Sleep Deficit Scale Keeping a Sleep Diary FLIP IT VIDEO Fact or Falsehood? Signs of Drug Abuse Eyescube Drug Addiction Simulation The Internet Addiction Test Blood Alcohol Concentrations FLIP IT VIDEOS • REM Sleep • The Psychology of Addiction • Neurotransmitters and Drugs MyersAP_SE_2e_Mo 217b Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 2 States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM Unit V States of Consciousness Modules 22 Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis 23 Sleep Patterns and Sleep Theories 24 Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders, and Dreams 25 Psychoactive Drugs C onsciousness can be a funny thing. It offers us weird experiences, as when entering sleep or leaving a dream, and sometimes it leaves us wondering who is really in control. After zoning me out with nitrous oxide, my dentist tells me to turn my head to the left. My conscious mind resists: “No way,” I silently say. “You can’t boss me around!” Whereupon my robotic head, ignoring my conscious mind, turns obligingly under the dentist’s control. During my noontime pickup basketball games, I am sometimes mildly irritated as my body passes the ball while my conscious mind is saying, “No, stop! Sarah is going to intercept!” Alas, my body completes the pass. Other times, as psychologist Daniel Wegner (2002) noted in The Illusion of Conscious Will, people believe their consciousness is controlling their actions when it isn’t. In one experiment, two people jointly controlled a computer mouse. Even when their partner (who was actually the experimenter’s accomplice) caused the mouse to stop on a predetermined square, the participants perceived that they had caused it to stop there. Then there are those times when consciousness seems to split. Reading Green Eggs and Ham to one of my preschoolers for the umpteenth time, my obliging mouth could say the words while my mind wandered elsewhere. And if someone asks what you’re doing for lunch while you’re texting, it’s not a problem. Your thumbs complete the message as you suggest getting tacos. What do such experiences reveal? Was my drug-induced dental experience akin to people’s experiences with other psychoactive drugs (mood- and perceptionaltering substances)? Was my automatic obedience to my dentist like people’s 217 MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod22_B.indd 217 1/15/14 8:46 AM Pacing Guide Module Topic Module 22 Defining Consciousness Hypnosis Module 23 Biological Rhythms and Sleep Sleep Theories Module 24 Sleep Deprivation and Sleep Disorders Dreams Module 25 Tolerance and Addiction Types of Psychoactive Drugs Standard Schedule Days Block Schedule Days 1 1 1 1 1/2 States of Consciousness MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 217 Unit V 217 2/20/14 8:12 AM 218 Unit V States of Consciousness responses to a hypnotist? Does a split in consciousness, as when our minds go TEACH elsewhere while reading or texting, explain people’s behavior while under hypno- Discussion Starter sis? And during sleep, when do those weird dream experiences occur, and why? Use the Module 22 Fact or Falsehood? activity from the TRM to introduce the concepts from this module. TEACH Before considering these questions and more, let’s ask a fundamental question: What is consciousness? Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis Module Learning Objectives 22-1 Describe the place of consciousness in psychology’s history. 22-2 Define hypnosis, and describe how a hypnotist can influence a hypnotized subject. 22-3 Discuss whether hypnosis is an extension of normal consciousness or an altered state. TEACH E Concept Connections very science has concepts so fundamental they are nearly impossible to define. Biologists agree on what is alive but not on precisely what life is. In physics, matter and energy elude simple definition. To psychologists, consciousness is similarly a fundamental yet slippery concept. Be sure students understand the content presented in Unit III about the two-track mind and Unit IV about selective attention. The study of the brain and consciousness demonstrates that we are able to process many kinds of information at the same time, with much of it processed unconsciously. Defining Consciousness 22-1 “Psychology must discard all reference to consciousness.” -BEHAVIORIST JOHN B. WATSON (1913) MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod22_B.indd 218 218 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 218 Press, Help students see that what we know about unconscious processing is different from Freud’s ideas about the unconscious mind. Freud described the unconscious as a repository of childhood experiences that influenced behavior without our conscious knowledge. The modern understanding of the unconscious views it as a parallel processing system that enables us to deal with all sorts of information and stimuli outside the conscious. This system helps us to be more cognitively efficient. Inc./A Alamy Module 22 Common Pitfalls ZUMA TR M TRM What is the place of consciousness in psychology’s history? At its beginning, psychology was “the description and explanation of states of consciousness” (Ladd, 1887). But during the first half of the twentieth century, the difficulty of scientifically studying consciousness led many psychologists—including those in the emerging school of behaviorism (Module 26)—to turn to direct observations of behavior. By the 1960s, psychology had nearly lost consciousness and was defining itself as “the science of behavior.” Consciousness was likened to a car’s speedometer: “It doesn’t make the car go, it just reflects what’s happening” (Seligman, 1991, p. 24). After 1960, mental concepts reemerged. Neuroscience advances related brain activity to sleeping, dreaming, and other mental states. Researchers began studying consciousness 1/15/14 8:46 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis Daydreaming Drowsiness Dreaming Some are physiologically induced Hallucinations Orgasm Food or oxygen starvation Some are psychologically induced Sensory deprivation Hypnosis Meditation TEACH Note that our modern-day understanding of the unconscious is very different from Sigmund Freud’s theory of the unconscious (Module 55). Freud believed the unconscious was a hiding place for our most anxiety-provoking ideas and emotions, and that uncovering those hidden thoughts could lead to healing. Now, most psychologists simply view the unconscious track as one that operates without awareness. Make sure you keep these two ideas of the unconscious straight. Teaching Tip Point out the margin note about Unit VII’s coverage of reconstructed memories. It is a good idea to emphasize to students that memory is not a computer file that can be opened and reviewed at will. Memory is an active system that is reworked as memories are replayed in the conscious mind. Because of this fact, the claim that hypnosis enables the recall of memories with vivid detail is something to view with skepticism. ENGAGE What do contemporary researchers believe about hypnosis? For answers to this question, students can browse the Society for Psychological Hypnosis’s website www.apa.org/divisions/div30. Figure 22.1 States of consciousness What is hypnosis, and what powers does a hypnotist have over a hypnotized subject? Imagine you are about to be hypnotized. The hypnotist invites you to sit back, fix your gaze on a spot high on the wall, and relax. In a quiet voice the hypnotist suggests, “Your eyes are growing tired. . . . Your eyelids are becoming heavy . . . now heavier and heavier. . . . They are beginning to close. . . . You are becoming more deeply relaxed. . . . Your breathing is now deep and regular. . . . Your muscles are becoming more and more relaxed. Your whole body is beginning to feel like lead.” After a few minutes of this hypnotic induction, you may experience hypnosis. When the hypnotist suggests, “Your eyelids are shutting so tight that you cannot open them even if you try,” it may indeed seem beyond your control to open your eyelids. Told to forget the number 6, you may be puzzled when you count 11 fingers on your hands. Invited to smell a sensuous perfume that is actually ammonia, you may linger delightedly over its pungent odor. Told that you cannot see a certain object, such as a chair, you may indeed report that it is not there, although you manage to avoid the chair when walking around (illustrating once again that two-track mind of yours). But is hypnosis really an altered state of consciousness? Let’s start with some frequently asked questions. Frequently Asked Questions About Hypnosis Hypnotists have no magical mind-control power. Their power resides in the subjects’ openness to suggestion, their ability to focus on certain images or behaviors (Bowers, 1984). But how open to suggestions are we? 1/15/14 8:46 AM A P ® E x a m Ti p Online Activities Hypnosis 22-2 219 Stuart Franklin/Magnum Photos Some states occur spontaneously Maria Teijeiro/Getty Images altered by hypnosis and drugs. Psychologists of all persuasions were affirming the importance of cognition, or mental processes. Psychology was regaining consciousness. Most psychologists now define consciousness as our awareness of ourselves and our environment. As we saw in Module 13, our conscious awareness is one part of the dual processing that goes on in our two-track minds. Although much of our information processing is conscious, much is unconscious and automatic—outside our awareness. Module 16 highlighted our selective attention, which directs the spotlight of our awareness, allowing us to assemble information from many sources as we reflect on our past and plan for our future. We are also attentive when we learn a complex concept or behavior. When learning to ride a bike, we focus on obstacles that we have to steer around and on how to use the brakes. With practice, riding a bike becomes semi-automatic, freeing us to focus our attention on other things. As we do so, we experience what the early psychologist William James called a continuous “stream of consciousness,” with each moment flowing into the next. Over time, we flit between different states of consciousness, including sleeping, waking, and various altered states (FIGURE 22.1). Module 22 MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod22_B.indd 219 In addition to normal, waking awareness, consciousness comes to us in altered states, including daydreaming and meditating. ENGAGE TR M TRM consciousness our awareness of ourselves and our environment. hypnosis a social interaction in which one person (the subject) responds to another person’s (the hypnotist’s) suggestions that certain perceptions, feelings, thoughts, or behaviors will spontaneously occur. 1/15/14 8:46 AM ENGAGE Enrichment A stage hypnotist makes ready use of our openness to suggestion when announcing to audience members that they will begin to experience itching sensations on various parts of their bodies: the head . . . arms . . . back . . . legs. To underscore the suggestion, the performer may casually but not obtrusively scratch him- or herself. Before long, many in the audience will follow suit. Use Teacher Demonstration: Hypnosis—A Stage Demonstration from the TRM to show students some techniques used for this type of entertainment. Active Learning Some obstetricians have begun teaching their pregnant patients to self-hypnotize to alleviate the pain of labor. Some patients report no feelings of pain, even without the use of drugs. Research the extent to which women going through labor use self-hypnosis. What techniques are important, making success with self-hypnosis more likely? Are these results just as likely to occur with relaxation techniques? Why or why not? How popular is hypnosis? Have any studies been conducted on its effectiveness? Are there concerns about the use of drugs during labor that would prompt women to choose hypnosis over drugs? Does self-hypnosis pose dangers? How does one come out of a self-hypnotic state? Are there liability issues or ethical obligations for doctors who advocate this practice? Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 219 Module 22 219 2/20/14 8:12 AM 220 Unit V States of Consciousness • ENGAGE TR M TRM Enrichment The text notes that there are individual differences in hypnotic responsiveness and that the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS) provides a measure of a person’s hypnotizability. Sheryl Wilson and Theodore Barber designed the Creative Imagination Scale (CIT) as an alternative to earlier scales (including the SHSS), which they claimed were too authoritarian, usually required a trance induction, and could not be administered in a group setting. Given the emergence of a more democratic culture and a more educated public, few people, they reasoned, like to be told, “You are under my control and you will behave as I tell you.” Use Student Activity: The Creative Imagination Scale from the TRM to let students experience this measure for themselves. A P ® E x a m Ti p Psychological research corrects the mistaken popular belief that hypnosis or other methods can be used to tap into a pure and complete memory bank. You will learn much more about how memory really works when you get to Unit VII. • “Hypnosis is not a psychological truth serum and to regard it as such has been a source of considerable mischief.” -RESEARCHER KENNETH BOWERS (1987) FYI See Module 33 for a more detailed discussion of how people may construct false memories. So should testimony obtained under hypnosis be admissible in court? American, Australian, and British courts have agreed it should not. They generally ban testimony from witnesses who have been hypnotized (Druckman & Bjork, 1994; Gibson, 1995; McConkey, 1995). • Can hypnosis force people to act against their will? Researchers have induced hypnotized people to perform an apparently dangerous act: plunging one hand briefly into fuming “acid,” then throwing the “acid” in a researcher’s face (Orne & Evans, 1965). Interviewed a day later, these people emphatically denied their acts and said they would never follow such orders. Had hypnosis given the hypnotist a special power to control others against their will? To find out, researchers Martin Orne and Frederich Evans unleashed that enemy of so many illusory beliefs—the control group. Orne asked other individuals to pretend they were hypnotized. Laboratory assistants, unaware that those in the experiment’s control group had not been hypnotized, treated both groups the same. The result? All the unhypnotized participants (perhaps believing that the laboratory context assured safety) performed the same acts as those who were hypnotized. • Can hypnosis be therapeutic? Hypnotherapists try to help patients harness their own healing powers (Baker, 1987). Posthypnotic suggestions have helped alleviate headaches, asthma, and stress-related skin disorders. In one statistical digest of 18 studies, the average client whose therapy was supplemented with hypnosis showed greater improvement than 70 percent of other therapy patients (Kirsch et al., 1995, 1996). Hypnosis seemed especially helpful for the treatment of obesity. However, drug, alcohol, and smoking addictions have not responded well to hypnosis (Nash, 2001). In controlled studies, hypnosis speeds the disappearance of warts, but so do the same positive suggestions given without hypnosis (Spanos, 1991, 1996). TEACH TR M TRM Common Pitfalls Be sure to emphasize to students that high suggestibility does not mean one is weak minded or gullible. People who value respect for authority may submit to hypnotic suggestion because they want to please the hypnotist. Environment and attitude also play important roles in how suggestible someone may be. Use Student Activity: Hypnosis as Heightened Suggestibility from the TRM to demonstrate this phenomenon for students. “It wasn’t what I expected. But facts are facts, and if one is proved to be wrong, one must just be humble about it and start again.” -AGATHA CHRISTIE’S MISS MARPLE posthypnotic suggestion a suggestion, made during a hypnosis session, to be carried out after the subject is no longer hypnotized; used by some clinicians to help control undesired symptoms and behaviors. Can anyone experience hypnosis? To some extent, we are all open to suggestion. When people stand upright with their eyes closed and are told that they are swaying back and forth, most will indeed sway a little. In fact, postural sway is one of the items assessed on the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale. People who respond to such suggestions without hypnosis are the same people who respond with hypnosis (Kirsch & Braffman, 2001). Highly hypnotizable people—say, the 20 percent who can carry out a suggestion not to smell or react to a bottle of ammonia held under their nose—typically become deeply absorbed in imaginative activities (Barnier & McConkey, 2004; Silva & Kirsch, 1992). Many researchers refer to this as hypnotic ability—the ability to focus attention totally on a task, to become imaginatively absorbed in it, to entertain fanciful possibilities. Can hypnosis enhance recall of forgotten events? Most people believe (wrongly, as Module 32 will explain) that our experiences are all “in there,” recorded in our brain and available for recall if only we can break through our own defenses (Loftus, 1980). But 60 years of memory research disputes such beliefs. We do not encode everything that occurs around us. We permanently store only some of our experiences, and we may be unable to retrieve some memories we have stored. “Hypnotically refreshed” memories combine fact with fiction. Since 1980, thousands of people have reported being abducted by UFOs, but most such reports have come from people who are predisposed to believe in aliens, are highly hypnotizable, and have undergone hypnosis (Newman & Baumeister, 1996; Nickell, 1996). Without either person being aware of what is going on, a hypnotist’s hints—“Did you hear loud noises?”—can plant ideas that become the subject’s pseudomemory. TEACH MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod22_B.indd 220 1/15/14 8:46 AM Concept Connections Link the concept of lost or hidden memories with Freudian theory. Sigmund Freud developed the idea that people repress difficult experiences from childhood in the subconscious or unconscious, and that these memories must be brought to consciousness so the experiences do not interfere with adult life. Hypnosis and dream interpretation were two ways Freud believed these experiences and memories could be revealed. 220 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 220 States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM MyersAP_SE_2e • Can hypnosis relieve pain? Hypnosis can relieve pain (Druckman & Bjork, 1994; Jensen, 2008). When unhypnotized people put their arm in an ice bath, they feel intense pain within 25 seconds. When hypnotized people do the same after being given suggestions to feel no pain, they indeed report feeling little pain. As some dentists know, light hypnosis can reduce fear, thus reducing hypersensitivity to pain. Hypnosis inhibits pain-related brain activity. In surgical experiments, hypnotized patients have required less medication, recovered sooner, and left the hospital earlier than unhypnotized control patients (Askay & Patterson, 2007; Hammond, 2008; Spiegel, 2007). Nearly 10 percent of us can become so deeply hypnotized that even major surgery can be performed without anesthesia. Half of us can gain at least some pain relief from hypnosis. The surgical use of hypnosis has flourished in Europe, where one Belgian medical team has performed more than 5000 surgeries with a combination of hypnosis, local anesthesia, and a mild sedative (Song, 2006). Module 22 221 BSIP SA/Alamy Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis 1. A quiet, calm environment with as few distractions as possible. A quiet room or place of worship would be suitable. Hypnotherapy This therapy aims to help people uncover problem-causing thoughts and feelings, or to change an unwanted behavior. 2. A mental device to prevent “mind wandering” (a sound, a word, or a phrase repeated silently or aloud). Attending to the normal rhythm of breathing is also useful. Is hypnosis an extension of normal consciousness or an altered state? 3. A passive, “let-it-happen” attitude. Don’t worry about distracting thoughts or about how well you are doing. When distractions occur, simply return to repetition of the sound, word, or phrase. We have seen that hypnosis involves heightened suggestibility. We have also seen that hypnotic procedures do not endow a person with special powers but can sometimes help people overcome stress-related ailments and cope with pain. So, just what is hypnosis? Psychologists have proposed two explanations. HYPNOSIS AS A SOCIAL PHENOMENON Our attentional spotlight and interpretations powerfully influence our ordinary perceptions. Might hypnotic phenomena reflect such workings of normal consciousness, as well as the power of social influence (Lynn et al., 1990; Spanos & Coe, 1992)? Advocates of the social influence theory of hypnosis believe they do. Does this mean that subjects consciously fake hypnosis? No—like actors caught up in their roles, they begin to feel and behave in ways appropriate for “good hypnotic subjects.” The more they like and trust the hypnotist, the more they allow that person to direct their attention and fantasies (Gfeller et al., 1987). “The hypnotist’s ideas become the subject’s thoughts,” explained Theodore Barber (2000), “and the subject’s thoughts produce the hypnotic experiences and behaviors.” Told to scratch their ear later when they hear the word psychology, subjects will likely do so—but only if they think the experiment is still under way. If an experimenter eliminates their motivation for acting hypnotized—by stating that hypnosis reveals their “gullibility”—subjects become unresponsive. Such findings support the idea that hypnotic phenomena are an extension of normal social and cognitive processes. These views illustrate a principle that Module 75 emphasizes: An authoritative person in a legitimate context can induce people—hypnotized or not—to perform some unlikely acts. Or as hypnosis researcher Nicholas Spanos (1982) put it, “The overt behaviors of hypnotic subjects are well within normal limits.” 4. A comfortable position to prevent muscular tension. A sitting position is probably best. If you lie down, you may fall asleep. ENGAGE TR M TRM Other hypnosis researchers believe hypnosis is more than inducing someone to play the role of “good subject.” How, they ask, can we explain why hypnotized subjects sometimes carry out suggested behaviors on cue, even when they believe no one is watching (Perugini et al., 1998)? And why does distinctive brain activity accompany hypnosis (Oakley & Halligan, 2009)? In one TEACH MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod22_B.indd 221 Flip It Students can get additional help understanding the different theories of hypnosis by watching the Flip It Video: Theories of Hypnosis. 1/15/14 8:46 AM What happens? Are the students salivating? Why or why not? How does this demonstration mimic hypnotic suggestion? What about this demonstration was particularly vivid? Use Teacher Demonstration: The Relaxation Response from the TRM for another way to demonstrate this response. Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 221 Active Learning Tell your students to close their eyes and imagine they are cutting a lemon . . . a large . . . sour . . . bitter lemon . . . so full of juice that it drips over their fingers onto the floor. Now imagine sucking the juice from the same fruit. HYPNOSIS AS DIVIDED CONSCIOUSNESS 1/15/14 8:46 AM Enrichment Herbert Benson suggests that 4 basic components are necessary to elicit the relaxation response: Explaining the Hypnotized State 22-3 ENGAGE Module 22 221 2/20/14 8:12 AM 222 Unit V States of Consciousness ENGAGE Active Learning Divide students into groups, and have them research how hypnosis is portrayed in different types of movies and television shows. If possible, have them bring in clips to demonstrate their conclusions. How do legal shows and movies portray hypnosis? How do comedic and cartoon shows and movies portray hypnosis? Do either type of media cite the drawbacks or limitations of hypnosis? What claims are made about what hypnosis is capable of providing? dissociation a split in consciousness, which allows some thoughts and behaviors to occur simultaneously with others. experiment, deeply hypnotized people were asked to imagine a color, and areas of their brain activated as if they were really seeing the color. To the hypnotized person’s brain, mere imagination had become a compelling hallucination (Kosslyn et al., 2000). In another experiment, researchers invited hypnotizable and nonhypnotizable people to say the color of letters. This is an easy task, but it slows if, say, green letters form the conflicting word RED, a phenomenon known as the Stroop effect (Raz et al., 2005). When easily hypnotized people were given a suggestion to focus on the color and to perceive the letters as irrelevant gibberish, they were much less slowed by the word-color conflict. (Brain areas that decode words and detect conflict remained inactive.) These results would not have surprised famed researcher Ernest Hilgard (1986, 1992), who believed hypnosis involves not only social influence but also a special dual-processing state of dissociation—a split between different levels of consciousness. Hilgard viewed hypnotic dissociation as a vivid form of everyday mind splits—similar to doodling while listening to a lecture or typing the end of a sentence while starting a conversation. Hilgard felt that when, for example, hypnotized people lower their arm into an ice bath, as in FIGURE 22.2, the hypnosis dissociates the sensation of the pain stimulus (of which the subjects are still aware) from the emotional suffering that defines their experience of pain. The ice water therefore feels cold—very cold—but not painful. Figure 22.2 Dissociation or role playing? A hypnotized woman tested by Ernest Hilgard exhibited no pain when her arm was placed in an ice bath. But asked to press a key if some part of her felt the pain, she did so. To Hilgard, this was evidence of dissociation, or divided consciousness. Proponents of social influence theory, however, maintain that people responding this way are caught up in playing the role of “good subject.” Divided-consciousness theory: Concept Connections What other explanations can students come up with to explain hypnotic behavior? Have students research the following social psychology concepts related to the social influence theory. Students should apply their research by explaining orally or in writing how these concepts reinforce the social influence theory: Conformity Peer pressure Groupthink Social desirability Social influence theory: The subject is so caught up in the hypnotized role that she ignores the cold. Hypnosis has caused a split in awareness. Courtesy Elizabeth Jecker TEACH Attention is diverted from a painful ice bath. How? Biological influences: • distinctive brain activity • unconscious information processing Another form of dual processing—selective attention—may also play a role in hypnotic pain relief. PET scans show that hypnosis reduces brain activity in a region that processes painful stimuli, but not in the sensory cortex, which receives the raw sensory input (Rainville et al., 1997). Hypnosis does not block sensory input, but it may block our attention to those stimuli. This helps explain why an injured athlete, caught up in the competition, may feel little or no pain until the game ends. Although the divided-consciousness theory of hypnosis is controversial, this much seems clear: There is, without doubt, much more to thinking and acting than we are conscious of. Our information processing, which starts with selective attention, is divided into simultaneous conscious and nonconscious realms. In hypnosis as in life, much of our behavior occurs on autopilot. We have two-track minds (FIGURE 22.3). Psychological influences: • focused attention • expectations • heightened suggestibility • dissociation between normal sensations and conscious awareness Hypnosis Social-cultural lturall iinfluences: nflue • presence of an authoritative person in legitimate context • role playing “good subject” Figure 22.3 Levels of analysis for hypnosis Using a biopsychosocial approach, researchers explore hypnosis from complementary perspectives. ENGAGE Enrichment Ernest Hilgard is considered to be one of the most influential psychologists of the 20th century. After joining the faculty at Stanford in 1933, he pursued many interests, one of which was hypnosis. Hypnosis was viewed largely as the tool of magicians and quacks, but Hilgard’s scientific studies of the phenomenon helped shed light on why it worked for some people and what applications were beneficial. He developed the Stanford Hypnotic Suggestibility Scale, which helped to standardize the study of hypnosis. 222 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 222 TEACH MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod22_B.indd 222 1/15/14 8:46 AM Concept Connections The problems of hypnotic suggestion have called into question the diagnosis of dissociative identity disorder (DID), formerly known as multiple personality disorder. Have students research the criticism against DID as a legitimate psychological disorder: Why has the legitimacy of DID come into question in recent years? What are some arguments for and against its legitimacy? What role does hypnosis play in this debate? What position has the American Psychiatric Association taken on this disorder? Is it still included in the current version of DSM? Are there plans to remove it from the manual? States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis Module 22 223 Before You Move On ENGAGE 䉴 ASK YOURSELF Enrichment You’ve read about two examples of dissociated consciousness: talking while texting, and thinking about something else while reading a child a bedtime story. Can you think of another example that you have experienced? Irving Kirsch and Steven Jay Lynn provide an excellent review of the literature on hypnosis, including the question of whether hypnosis produces an out-of-the-ordinary, trancelike, “altered” state of consciousness. Research indicates the following: 䉴 TEST YOURSELF When is the use of hypnosis potentially harmful, and when can hypnosis be used to help? Answers to the Test Yourself questions can be found in Appendix E at the end of the book. The ability to be hypnotized does not indicate gullibility or weakness. Participants retain the ability to control their behavior during hypnosis. Spontaneous posthypnotic amnesia is relatively rare. Hypnosis is not a dangerous procedure when practiced by qualified researchers and clinicians. Hypnosis does not increase the accuracy of memory. Hypnosis does not foster a literal reexperiencing of childhood events. Module 22 Review 22-1 • • After initially claiming consciousness as its area of study in the nineteenth century, psychologists had abandoned it in the first half of the twentieth century, turning instead to the study of observable behavior because they believed consciousness was too difficult to study scientifically. Since 1960, under the influence of cognitive psychology and neuroscience, consciousness (our awareness of ourselves and our environment) has resumed its place as an important area of research. 22-2 1/15/14 8:46 AM What is the place of consciousness in psychology’s history? 22-3 Is hypnosis an extension of normal consciousness or an altered state? • Many psychologists believe that hypnosis is a form of normal social influence and that hypnotized people act out the role of “good subject” by following directions given by an authoritative person. • Other psychologists view hypnosis as a dissociation—a split between normal sensations and conscious awareness. Selective attention may also contribute by blocking attention to certain stimuli. Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S. J. (1995). The altered state of hypnosis: Changes in the theoretical landscape. American Psychologist, 50, 846–858. What is hypnosis, and what powers does a hypnotist have over a hypnotized subject? • Hypnosis is a social interaction in which one person suggests to another that certain perceptions, feelings, thoughts, or behaviors will spontaneously occur. • Hypnosis does not enhance recall of forgotten events (it may even evoke false memories). • It cannot force people to act against their will, though hypnotized people, like unhypnotized people, may perform unlikely acts. • Posthypnotic suggestions have helped people harness their own healing powers but have not been very effective in treating addiction. Hypnosis can help relieve pain. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod22_B.indd 223 CLOSE & ASSESS Exit Assessment Have students write a paragraph comparing and contrasting the 2 main theories of hypnosis. Ask them to decide which one is more convincing and why. 1/15/14 8:46 AM Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 223 Module 22 223 2/20/14 8:12 AM Unit V States of Consciousness 224 Answers to Multiple-Choice Questions Multiple-Choice Questions 1. What do we call awareness of our environment and ourselves? 1. e 2. a 3. b 4. d a. b. c. d. e. Selective attention Hypnotism Posthypnotic suggestion Dissociation Consciousness 2. Which of the following is true about daydreaming? a. b. c. d. e. Answer to Practice FRQ 2 1 point: Biological influence: either distinctive brain activity or unconscious information processing. 1 point: Psychological influence: focused attention, expectations, heightened suggestibility, or dissociation between normal sensations and conscious awareness. It occurs spontaneously. It is physiologically induced. It is psychologically induced. It is considered the same as waking awareness. It is more like meditation than it is like dreaming. 3. Which of the following states of consciousness occurs when one person suggests to another that certain thoughts or behaviors will spontaneously occur? a. b. c. d. e. Dreaming Hypnosis Daydreaming Hallucination Waking awareness 4. Which of the following is the term most closely associated with the split in consciousness that allows some thoughts and behaviors to occur simultaneously with others? a. b. c. d. e. Consciousness Hypnosis Hallucination Dissociation Meditation Practice FRQs 1. Identify two states of consciousness that are psychologically induced and two that occur spontaneously. Answer 2. According to the biopsychosocial approach, identify a biological, a psychological, and a social-cultural influence on hypnosis. (3 points) 1 point: For any two psychologically induced states: sensory deprivation, hypnosis, or meditation. 1 point: For any two spontaneously occurring states: daydreaming, drowsiness, or dreaming. 1 point: Social-cultural influence: either presence of an authoritative person or role-playing a “good subject.” MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod22_B.indd 224 224 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 224 1/15/14 8:46 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mo Sleep Patterns and Sleep Theories Module 23 225 TEACH Module 23 my s/Ala Service light V Active Learning Catch Module Learning Objectives 23-1 Describe how our biological rhythms influence our daily functioning. 23-2 Describe the biological rhythm of our sleeping and dreaming stages. 23-3 Explain how biology and environment interact in our sleep patterns. 23-4 Describe sleep’s functions. Have students calculate roughly how much of their lives they have spent sleeping. Have students interview their parents to find out how many hours of sleep they averaged when they were infants and toddlers. Also have them recall their daily bedtimes and wake-up times during the school week. They should also keep track of the average daily amount of sleep they get for a week. S leep—the irresistible tempter to whom we inevitably succumb. Sleep—the equalizer of teachers and teens. Sleep—sweet, renewing, mysterious sleep. While sleeping, you may feel “dead to the world,” but you are not. Even when you are deeply asleep, your perceptual window is open a crack. You move around on your bed, but you manage not to fall out. The occasional roar of passing vehicles may leave your deep sleep undisturbed, but a cry from a baby’s room quickly interrupts it. So does the sound of your name. Electroencephalograph (EEG) recordings confirm that the brain’s auditory cortex responds to sound stimuli even during sleep (Kutas, 1990). And when you are asleep, as when you are awake, you process most information outside your conscious awareness. Many of sleep’s mysteries are now being solved as some people sleep, attached to recording devices, while others observe. By recording brain waves and muscle movements, and by observing and occasionally waking sleepers, researchers are glimpsing things that a thousand years of common sense never told us. Perhaps you can anticipate some of their discoveries. Are the following statements true or false? 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Discussion Starter ENGAGE isual Sleep Patterns and Sleep Theories TR M TRM Use the Student Activity: The Sleep IQ Test activity from the TRM to introduce the concepts from this module. “I love to sleep. Do you? Isn’t it great? It really is the best of both worlds. You get to be alive and unconscious.” -COMEDIAN RITA RUDNER, 1993 When people dream of performing some activity, their limbs often move in concert with the dream. Older adults sleep more than young adults. Sleepwalkers are acting out their dreams. Sleep experts recommend treating insomnia with an occasional sleeping pill. Some people dream every night; others seldom dream. All these statements (adapted from Palladino & Carducci, 1983) are false. To see why, read on. Biological Rhythms and Sleep Like the ocean, life has its rhythmic tides. Over varying periods, our bodies fluctuate, and with them, our minds. Let’s look more closely at two of those biological rhythms—our 24hour biological clock and our 90-minute sleep cycle. 1/15/14 8:46 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 225 1/15/14 8:47 AM SleepConsciousness Patterns and Sleep Theories Understanding and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 225 Module 23 22 225 2/20/14 8:12 AM 226 Unit V States of Consciousness Circadian Rhythm ENGAGE 23-1 Active Learning There are actually 3 types of body rhythms: Circadian rhythms occur once each day. Our circadian rhythm spans 24 hours and is responsible for our varying levels of arousal throughout the course of a day. Ultradian rhythms occur more than once each day and include the cycles of appetite and hormonal release. Infradian rhythms occur once per month or season and include the menstrual cycle. FYI Dolphins, porpoises, and whales sleep with one side of their brain at a time (Miller et al., 2008). ENGAGE Active Learning Have students conduct a sleep survey with a representative sample of students at your school. They should ask how much sleep students get each night, whether they nap in class or feel extremely tired during the day, and why they may or may not get enough sleep each night. Be sure to get approval from an institutional review board and obtain informed consent from subjects before embarking on any research project. Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 226 The rhythm of the day parallels the rhythm of life—from our waking at a new day’s birth to our nightly return to what Shakespeare called “death’s counterfeit.” Our bodies roughly synchronize with the 24-hour cycle of day and night by an internal biological clock called the circadian rhythm (from the Latin circa, “about,” and diem, “day”). As morning approaches, body temperature rises, then peaks during the day, dips for a time in early afternoon (when many people take siestas), and begins to drop again in the evening. Thinking is sharpest and memory most accurate when we are at our daily peak in circadian arousal. Try pulling an allnighter or working an occasional night shift. You’ll feel groggiest in the middle of the night but may gain new energy when your normal wake-up time arrives. Age and experience can alter our circadian rhythm. Most teens and young adults are evening-energized Eric Isselée/Shutterstock “owls,” with performance improving across the day (May & Hasher, 1998). Most older adults are morning-loving “larks,” with performance declining as the day wears on. By mid-evening, when the night has hardly begun for many young adults, retirement homes are typically quiet. At about age 20 (slightly earlier for women), we begin to shift from being owls to being larks (Roenneberg et al., 2004). Women become more morning oriented as they have children and also as they transition to menopause (Leonhard & Randler, 2009; Randler & Bausback, 2010). Morning types tend to do better in school, to take more initiative, and to be less vulnerable to depression (Randler, 2008, 2009; Randler Peter Chadwick/Science Source & Frech, 2009). Sleep Stages 23-2 circadian [ser-KAY-dee-an] rhythm the biological clock; regular bodily rhythms (for example, of temperature and wakefulness) that occur on a 24-hour cycle. REM sleep rapid eye movement sleep; a recurring sleep stage during which vivid dreams commonly occur. Also known as paradoxical sleep, because the muscles are relaxed (except for minor twitches) but other body systems are active. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 226 226 How do our biological rhythms influence our daily functioning? What is the biological rhythm of our sleeping and dreaming stages? Sooner or later, sleep overtakes us and consciousness fades as different parts of our brain’s cortex stop communicating (Massimini et al., 2005). Yet the sleeping brain remains active and has its own biological rhythm. About every 90 minutes, we cycle through four distinct sleep stages. This simple fact apparently was unknown until 8-year-old Armond Aserinsky went to bed one night in 1952. His father, Eugene, a University of Chicago graduate student, needed to test an electroencephalograph he had repaired that day (Aserinsky, 1988; Seligman & Yellen, 1987). Placing electrodes near Armond’s eyes to record the rolling eye movements then believed to occur during sleep, Aserinsky watched the machine go wild, tracing deep zigzags on the graph paper. Could the machine still be broken? As the night proceeded and the activity recurred, Aserinsky realized that the periods of fast, jerky eye movements were accompanied by energetic brain activity. Awakened during one such episode, Armond reported having a dream. Aserinsky had discovered what we now know as REM sleep (rapid eye movement sleep). Similar procedures used with thousands of volunteers showed the cycles were a normal part of sleep (Kleitman, 1960). To appreciate these studies, imagine yourself as a participant. As the hour grows late, you feel sleepy and yawn in response to reduced brain metabolism. (Yawning, which can be socially contagious, stretches your neck muscles and increases your heart rate, which increases your alertness [Moorcroft, 2003].) When you are ready for bed, a 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Sleep Patterns and Sleep Theories Left eye movements Right eye movements EMG (muscle tension) Module 23 227 Figure 23.1 Measuring sleep activity Sleep ENGAGE researchers measure brain-wave activity, eye movements, and muscle tension by electrodes that pick up weak electrical signals from the brain, eye, and facial muscles. (From Dement, 1978.) TR M TRM EEG (brain waves) Hank Morgan/Science Source Figure 23.2 Brain waves and sleep stages The beta waves of an alert, waking state and the regular alpha waves of an awake, relaxed state differ from the slower, larger delta waves of deep NREM-3 sleep. Although the rapid REM sleep waves resemble the nearwaking NREM-1 sleep waves, the body is more aroused during REM sleep than during NREM sleep. 100 nV Rebecca Spencer, University of Masssachusetts, assisted with this figure. researcher comes in and tapes electrodes to your scalp (to detect your brain waves), on your chin Waking Beta (to detect muscle tension), and just outside the corners of your eyes (to detect eye movements) Waking Alpha (FIGURE 23.1). Other devices will record your heart rate, respiration rate, and genital arousal. When you are in bed with your eyes closed, the researcher in the next room sees on the REM EEG the relatively slow alpha waves of your awake but relaxed state (FIGURE 23.2). As you adapt to all this equipment, you grow tired and, in an unremembered moment, slip into sleep NREM-1 (FIGURE 23.3). The transition is marked by the slowed breathing and the irregular brain waves of non-REM stage 1 sleep. Using the new American NREM-2 Academy of Sleep Medicine classification of sleep stages, this is called NREM-1 (Silber et al., 2008). In one of his 15,000 research participants, William Dement (1999) observed the moment NREM-3 (Delta waves) the brain’s perceptual window to the outside world slammed shut. Dement asked this sleepdeprived young man, lying on his back with eyelids taped open, to press a button every time a strobe light flashed in his eyes (about every 6 seconds). After a few minutes the young man missed one. Asked why, he said, “Because there was no flash.” But there was a flash. He missed 6 sec it because (as his brain activity revealed) he had fallen asleep for 2 seconds, missing not only the flash 6 inches from his nose but also the awareness of the abrupt moment of entry into sleep. Enrichment The waves people exhibit when they are awake and active are called beta waves. These are high-frequency, lowamplitude waves. Theta waves characterize the transition from NREM-1 to NREM-2. Theta waves are slower, so they have a higher amplitude and lower frequency than alpha waves, the type of waves that are present as we move from relaxation to deeper sleep. Use Student Activity: Remembering Daydreams from the TRM to help students see that daydreams can be just as helpful to our lives as night dreams. ENGAGE Enrichment alpha waves the relatively slow brain waves of a relaxed, awake state. Another type of wave found in sleep NREM-2 is the K-complex. As you can see in Figure 23.2, in NREM-2 there is a sharp upturn followed by a rapid downturn of the wave, which looks similar to a heartbeat pattern on an EKG. This wave is the precursor to the large delta waves that characterize NREM-3 sleep. sleep periodic, natural loss of consciousness—as distinct from unconsciousness resulting from a coma, general anesthesia, or hibernation. (Adapted from Dement, 1999.) Figure 23.3 The moment of sleep We seem unaware of the Sleep 1/15/14 8:47 AM 1 second TEACH MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 227 moment we fall into sleep, but someone watching our brain waves could tell. (From Dement, 1999.) 1/15/14 8:47 AM Teaching Tip Use Figure 23.2 to help students identify the different types of brain waves characterizing the different stages of sleep. A diagram like this may show up on the AP® exam. Students could need to identify which waves correspond to each stage. SleepConsciousness Patterns and Sleep Theories Understanding and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 227 Module 23 22 227 2/20/14 8:12 AM 228 Unit V States of Consciousness hallucinations false sensory experiences, such as seeing something in the absence of an external visual stimulus. TEACH Teaching Tip delta waves the large, slow brain waves associated with deep sleep. Be sure to point out the name for the hallucinations people experience when they are in the early stages of sleep: hypnagogic sensations. This term may show up on the AP® exam. NREM sleep non–rapid eye movement sleep; encompasses all sleep stages except for REM sleep. ENGAGE Active Learning During this brief NREM-1 sleep you may experience fantastic images resembling hallucinations—sensory experiences that occur without a sensory stimulus. You may have a sensation of falling (at which moment your body may suddenly jerk) or of floating weightlessly. These hypnagogic sensations may later be incorporated into your memories. People who claim to have been abducted by aliens—often shortly after getting into bed— commonly recall being floated off of or pinned down on their beds (Clancy, 2005). You then relax more deeply and begin about 20 minutes of NREM-2 sleep, with its periodic sleep spindles—bursts of rapid, rhythmic brain-wave activity (see Figure 23.2). Although you could still be awakened without too much difficulty, you are now clearly asleep. Then you transition to the deep sleep of NREM-3. During this slow-wave sleep, which lasts for about 30 minutes, your brain emits large, slow delta waves and you are hard to awaken. Ever say to classmates, “That thunder was so loud last night,” only to have them respond, “What thunder?” Those who missed the storm may have been in delta sleep. (It is at the end of the deep, slow-wave NREM-3 sleep that children may wet the bed.) TEACH Concept Connections The device used to measure sleep waves is the EEG—electroencephalogram—which was discussed in Unit III. This device captures the electrical activity given off by the neurons as they fire action potentials. The speed with which the action potentials occur creates the brain wave patterns seen during wakefulness and sleep. Sleep researchers use other devices during sleep studies to measure eye movements and muscle activity to gain a better understanding of the physiology of sleep. ScienceCartoonsPlus.com REM SLEEP Students have likely experienced hypnagogic sensations during school when they have fallen asleep in class. Ask students if they have ever jerked awake as they dozed off. If they have, they may recall what they were dreaming about at that moment—perhaps tripping on a sidewalk or falling from a chair. Their dream experience is then translated into behavior since they are not fully into REM sleep. About an hour after you first fall asleep, a strange thing happens. You start to leave behind the stages known as NREM sleep. Rather than continuing in deep slumber, you ascend from your initial sleep dive. Returning through NREM-2 (where you spend about half your night), you enter the most intriguing sleep phase—REM sleep (FIGURE 23.4). For about 10 minutes, your brain waves become rapid and saw-toothed, more like those of the nearly awake NREM-1 sleep. But unlike NREM-1, during REM sleep your heart rate rises, your breathing becomes rapid and irregular, and every halfminute or so your eyes dart around in momentary bursts of activity behind closed lids. These eye movements announce the beginning of a dream—often emotional, usually “Boy are my eyes tired! I had REM sleep story-like, and richly hallucinatory. Because anyone watching a sleeper’s eyes can notice these all night long.” REM bursts, it is amazing that science was ignorant of REM sleep until 1952. Figure 23.4 The stages in a typical night’s sleep People pass through a multistage sleep cycle several times each night, with the periods of deep sleep diminishing and REM sleep periods increasing in duration. As people age, sleep becomes more fragile, with awakenings common among older adults (Kamel et al., 2006; Neubauer, 1999). (a) Young Adults REM NREM-1 NREM-2 NREM-3 1 4 5 6 7 8 5 6 7 8 (b) Older Adults REM A P ® E x a m Ti p Study this cycle of sleep carefully. One common mistake that students make is to believe that REM sleep comes directly after deep NREM-3 sleep. As you can see, it does not. Generally, NREM-2 follows NREM-3. Then comes REM. NREM-1 NREM-2 NREM-3 Teaching Tip Figure 23.4 is a good example to use for identifying elements of a diagram. Students need to identify the stages of sleep and explain the relationship between REM and NREM stages throughout the night. MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 228 3 Awake TEACH Unit V 2 Hours of sleep MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 228 228 REM increases as night progresses Awake 1 2 3 4 Hours of sleep TEACH 1/15/14 8:47 AM Flip It Students can get additional help understanding paradoxical sleep by watching the Flip It Video: REM Sleep. States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Sleep Patterns and Sleep Theories 229 FYI ENGAGE People rarely snore during dreams. When REM starts, snoring stops. Enrichment FYI Uriel Sinai/Getty Images Horses, which spend 92 percent of each day standing and can sleep standing, must lie down for REM sleep (Morrison, 2003). Safety in numbers? Why would communal sleeping provide added protection for those whose safety depends upon vigilance, such as these soldiers? ANSWER: With each soldier cycling through the sleep stages independently, it is very likely that at any given time at least one of them will be awake or easily wakened in the event of a threat. Except during very scary dreams, your genitals become aroused during REM sleep. You have an erection or increased vaginal lubrication, regardless of whether the dream’s content is sexual (Karacan et al., 1966). Men’s common “morning erection” stems from the night’s last REM period, often just before waking. Your brain’s motor cortex is active during REM sleep, but your brainstem blocks its messages. This leaves your muscles relaxed, so much so that, except for an occasional finger, toe, or facial twitch, you are essentially paralyzed. Moreover, you cannot easily be awakened. (This immobility may occasionally linger as you awaken from REM sleep, producing a disturbing experience of sleep paralysis [Santomauro & French, 2009].) REM sleep is thus sometimes called paradoxical sleep: The body is internally aroused, with waking-like brain activity, yet asleep and externally calm. The sleep cycle repeats itself about every 90 minutes. As the night wears on, deep NREM-3 sleep grows shorter and disappears. The REM and NREM2 sleep periods get longer (see Figure 23.4). By morning, we have spent 20 to 25 percent of an average night’s sleep—some 100 minutes—in REM sleep. Thirty-seven percent of people report rarely or never having dreams “that you can remember the next morning” (Moore, 2004). Yet even they will, more than 80 percent of the time, recall a dream after being awakened during REM sleep. We spend about 600 hours a year experiencing some 1500 dreams, or more than 100,000 dreams over a typical lifetime— dreams swallowed by the night but not acted out, thanks to REM’s protective paralysis. Module 23 TEACH What Affects Our Sleep Patterns? 23-3 Teaching Tip How do biology and environment interact in our sleep patterns? The idea that “everyone needs 8 hours of sleep” is untrue. Newborns often sleep two-thirds of their day, most adults no more than one-third. Still, there is more to our sleep differences than age. Some of us thrive with fewer than 6 hours per night; others regularly rack up 9 hours or more. Such sleep patterns are genetically influenced (Hor & Tafti, 2009). In studies of fraternal and identical twins, only the identical twins had strikingly similar sleep patterns and durations (Webb & Campbell, 1983). Today’s researchers are discovering the genes that regulate sleep in humans and animals (Donlea et al., 2009; He et al., 2009). Sleep patterns are also culturally influenced. In the United States and Canada, adults average 7 to 8 hours per night (Hurst, 2008; National Sleep Foundation, 2010; Robinson & Martin, 2009). (The weeknight sleep of many students and workers falls short of this average [NSF, 2008].) North Americans are nevertheless sleeping less than their counterparts a century ago. Thanks to modern lighting, shift work, and social media and other diversions, those who would have gone to bed at 9:00 P.M. are now up until 11:00 P.M. or later. With sleep, as with waking behavior, biology and environment interact. Bright morning light tweaks the circadian clock by activating light-sensitive retinal proteins. These proteins control the circadian clock by triggering signals to the brain’s suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN)—a pair of grain-of-rice-sized, 10,000-cell clusters in the hypothalamus (Wirz-Justice, 2009). The SCN does its job in part by causing the brain’s pineal gland to decrease its production of the sleep-inducing hormone melatonin in the morning and to increase it in the evening (FIGURE 23.5 on the next page). 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 229 ENGAGE Dreams do not usually occur in NREM sleep, but they can and do happen. Have you ever fallen asleep and started “dreaming” about something mundane—like walking down the sidewalk or talking with a friend—only to jerk awake when you dreamed that you are tripping or falling? This experience can be explained as an NREM dream. Normally in REM sleep, the body is completely relaxed, so people do not react physically to their dreams. But, if we dream during NREM, then our bodies may interpret our dreams as reality and react to them, causing us to jump or startle at an apparent fall. The term paradoxical sleep is a good one for students to know. If students can recall this term as a synonym for REM sleep, they will remember the nature of this stage of sleep—that brain wave patterns resemble wakefulness, but the body is at rest. TEACH Common Pitfalls suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) a pair of cell clusters in the hypothalamus that controls circadian rhythm. In response to light, the SCN causes the pineal gland to adjust melatonin production, thus modifying our feelings of sleepiness. TEACH Students may need help understanding why REM is called paradoxical sleep. A paradox is something that seems to contradict itself. It seems illogical for your brain to be so active while it is asleep, but this is true during REM. When we are at our most relaxed physically, we are experiencing one of our most active times mentally. 1/15/14 8:47 AM Enrichment Concept Connections REM is also known as emergent stage 1 sleep. As we go through the sleep cycle each night, the body will start with the twilight sleep of stage 1 and move into deeper, slow-wave sleep patterns. When the body cycles back up to stage 1, it again experiences brain waves similar to those of stage 1, only this time it is in REM, the most restful sleep. So while the body appears to be coming out of sleep every 90 minutes, it is actually cycling into emergent stage 1 or REM sleep. Connect the sleep stages with information discussed later in this unit related to sleep disorders. People with sleep disorders experience a disruption in the regular patterns of sleep, making their experience of them vastly different. The most disruption occurs with their REM patterns. SleepConsciousness Patterns and Sleep Theories Understanding and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 229 Module 23 22 229 2/20/14 8:12 AM Unit V States of Consciousness 230 ENGAGE Figure 23.5 The biological clock Light striking Enrichment the retina signals the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) to suppress the pineal gland’s production of the sleep hormone melatonin. At night, the SCN quiets down, allowing the pineal gland to release melatonin into the bloodstream. The stages of both non-REM and REM occur at the usual hours and last the usual amount of time. Most users do not seem to feel drowsy or experience side effects. Melatonin appears to promote sleep at any time of day. Repeated doses at certain times of day or night can alter circadian body rhythms. The data suggest that melatonin may be helpful for people: who need to sleep during the day (that is, night shift workers or those who fly across time zones). who are taking drugs that inhibit normal melatonin synthesis. who are patients with diseases that cause insomnia. Tr y T h i s If our natural circadian rhythm were attuned to a 23-hour cycle, would we instead need to discipline ourselves to stay up later at night and sleep in longer in the morning? FYI A circadian disadvantage: One study of a decade’s 24,121 Major League Baseball games found that teams who had crossed three time zones before playing a multiday series had nearly a 60 percent chance of losing their first game (Winter et al., 2009). Students should recall from Unit III that the hypothalamus is responsible for behaviors and mental processes governed by hormones, such as eating, sex, and the stress response. Point out this unit reinforces that idea by showing that yet another behavior governed by hormones—sleep—is regulated by the suprachiasmatic nucleus, a small section of the hypothalamus. Melatonin produced f lo w Blo Being bathed in light disrupts our 24-hour biological clock (Czeisler et al., 1999; Dement, 1999). Curiously—given that our ancestors’ body clocks were attuned to the rising and setting Sun of the 24-hour day—many of today’s young adults adopt something closer to a 25-hour day, by staying up too late to get 8 hours of sleep. For this, we can thank (or blame) Thomas Edison, inventor of the light bulb. This helps explain why, until our later years, we must discipline ourselves to go to bed and force ourselves to get up. Most animals, too, when placed under unnatural constant illumination will exceed a 24-hour day. Artificial light delays sleep. Sleep often eludes those who stay up late and sleep in on weekends, and then go to bed earlier on Sunday evening in preparation for the new school week (Oren & Terman, 1998). They are like New Yorkers whose biology is on California time. For North Americans who fly to Europe and need to be up when their circadian rhythm cries “SLEEP,” bright light (spending the next day outdoors) helps reset the biological clock (Czeisler et al., 1986, 1989; Eastman et al., 1995). Sleep Theories What are sleep’s functions? So, our sleep patterns differ from person to person and from culture to culture. But why do we have this need for sleep? Psychologists believe sleep may have evolved for five reasons. “Sleep faster, we need the pillows.” -YIDDISH PROVERB 1. 2. Figure 23.6 Animal sleep time Would you Concept Connections Melatonin production suppressed Light 23-4 Haimov, I., & Lavie, P. (1996). Melatonin—A soporific hormone. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 106–111. TEACH Pineal gland od In contrast to the sleep induced by most drugs, melatonin-induced sleep seems remarkably normal. Suprachiasmatic nucleus rather be a brown bat and sleep 20 hours a day or a giraffe and sleep 2 hours daily (data from NIH, 2010)? Kruglov_Orda/Shutterstock; Courtesy of Andrew D. Myers; Utekhina Anna/Shutterstock; Steffen Foerster Photography/ Shutterstock; The Agency Collection/Punchstock; Eric Isselée/Shutterstock; pandapaw/Shutterstock 20 hours 16 hours Sleep protects. When darkness shut down the day’s hunting, food gathering, and travel, our distant ancestors were better off asleep in a cave, out of harm’s way. Those who didn’t try to navigate around rocks and cliffs at night were more likely to leave descendants. This fits a broader principle: A species’ sleep pattern tends to suit its ecological niche (Siegel, 2009). Animals with the greatest need to graze and the least ability to hide tend to sleep less. (For a sampling of animal sleep times, see FIGURE 23.6.) Sleep helps us recuperate. It helps restore and repair brain tissue. Bats and other animals with high waking metabolism burn a lot of calories, producing a lot of free radicals, molecules that are toxic to neurons. Sleeping a lot gives resting neurons time to repair themselves, while pruning or weakening unused connections (Gilestro et al., 2009; Siegel, 2003; Vyazovskiy et al., 2008). Think of it this way: When consciousness leaves your house, brain construction workers come in for a makeover. 12 hours TEACH MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 230 10 hours 8 hours 4 hours 2 hours 1/15/14 8:47 AM Concept Connections Point out how different qualities have evolved in animals and humans due to their sleep habits. Nocturnal animals typically have better night vision and larger ears than daytimeloving animals. Animals who sleep a lot also tend to have faster metabolisms. 230 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 230 States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Sleep Patterns and Sleep Theories 3. 4. 5. Sleep helps restore and rebuild our fading memories of the day’s experiences. Sleep consolidates our memories—it strengthens and stabilizes neural memory traces (Racsmány et al., 2010; Rasch & Born, 2008). People trained to perform tasks therefore recall them better after a night’s sleep, or even after a short nap, than after several hours awake (Stickgold & Ellenbogen, 2008). Among older adults, more sleep leads to better memory of recently learned material (Drummond, 2010). After sleeping well, seniors remember more. And in both humans and rats, neural activity during slow-wave sleep re-enacts and promotes recall of prior novel experiences (Peigneux et al., 2004; Ribeiro et al., 2004). Sleep, it seems, strengthens memories in a way that being awake does not. Sleep feeds creative thinking. On occasion, dreams have inspired noteworthy literary, artistic, and scientific achievements, such as the dream that clued chemist August Kekulé to the structure of benzene (Ross, 2006). More commonplace is the boost that a complete night’s sleep gives to our thinking and learning. After working on a task, then sleeping on it, people solve problems more insightfully than do those who stay awake (Wagner et al., 2004). They also are better at spotting connections among novel pieces of information (Ellenbogen et al., 2007). To think smart and see connections, it often pays to sleep on it. Sleep supports growth. During deep sleep, the pituitary gland releases a growth hormone. This hormone is necessary for muscle development. A regular full night’s sleep can also “dramatically improve your athletic ability,” report James Maas and Rebecca Robbins (see Close-up: Sleep and Athletic Performance). As we age, we release less of this hormone and spend less time in deep sleep (Pekkanen, 1982). Module 23 231 TEACH “Corduroy pillows make headlines.” -ANONYMOUS TR M TRM ENGAGE Given all the benefits of sleep, it’s no wonder that sleep loss hits us so hard. TR M TRM Sleep and Athletic Performance when the body’s natural cooling is most efficient. Early Exercise improves sleep. What’s not as widely known, remorning workouts are ill-advised, because they increase port James Maas and Rebecca Robbins (2010), is that the risk of injury and rob athletes of valuable sleep. Heavy sleep improves athletic performance. Well-rested workouts within three hours of bedtime should also be avoidathletes have faster reaction times, more energy, ed because the arousal disrupts falling asleep. Preciand greater endurance, and teams that build 8 to sion muscle training, such as shooting free throws, 10 hours of daily sleep into their training show may, however, benefit when followed by sleep. improved performance. Top violinists also report Maas has been a sleep consultant for college and sleeping 8.5 hours a day on average, and rate professional athletes and teams. On Maas’ advice, the Orpractice and sleep as the two most important imlando Magic cut early morning practices. He also advised provement-fostering activities (Ericsson et al., 1993). one young woman, Sarah Hughes, who felt stymied in Slow-wave sleep, which occurs mostly in the her efforts to excel in figure-skating competition. “Cut first half of a night’s sleep, produces the human the early morning practice,” he instructed, as part of growth hormone necessary for muscle developthe recommended sleep regimen. Soon thereafter, ment. REM sleep and NREM-2 sleep, which Hughes’ performance scores increased, ultioccur mostly in the final hours of a long night’s mately culminating in her 2002 Olympic gold sleep, help strengthen the neural connections AP Photo/Lionel Cironneau medal. that build enduring memories, including the “muscle memories” learned while practicing Ample sleep supports skill learning and high tennis or shooting baskets. performance This was the experience of Olympic gold The optimal exercise time is late afternoon medalist Sarah Hughes. or early evening, Maas and Robbins advise, MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 231 1/15/14 8:47 AM SleepConsciousness Patterns and Sleep Theories Understanding and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 231 Applying Science Have students record their sleep habits for a week, including a weekend. Then ask them to calculate the average number of hours of sleep they get each night, comparing weekend sleep with weeknight sleep. Discuss with students how their sleep patterns may be affecting their daily performance. Ask them what prevents them from getting more sleep, and suggest how even 1 more hour a night would affect their quality of life. Use Student Activity: Keeping a Sleep Diary from the TRM to help students assess their own sleep habits. Close-up 1/15/14 8:47 AM Teaching Tip Have students brainstorm about possible reasons why teenagers resist getting 8–10 hours of sleep a night. Have them consider work, academic, familial, physiological, and social reasons for the prevalence of sleep deprivation among teens. Discuss ways they can counter these negative influences on getting a good night’s sleep. Use Student Activity: Sleep Deficit Scale from the TRM to help students assess the effects of sleeplessness on their own lives. Module 23 22 231 2/20/14 8:12 AM 232 Unit V States of Consciousness CLOSE & ASSESS Before You Move On Exit Assessment 䉴 ASK YOURSELF Would you consider yourself a night owl or a morning lark? When do you usually feel most energetic? What time of day works best for you to study? Provide students with either Figure 23.2 or 23.4, and have them label the graph. This activity will help you assess each student’s knowledge of the different stages of sleep and give all students practice working with graphs. 䉴 TEST YOURSELF What five theories explain our need for sleep? Answers to the Test Yourself questions can be found in Appendix E at the end of the book. Module 23 Review 23-1 How do our biological rhythms influence our daily functioning? • Our bodies have an internal biological clock, roughly synchronized with the 24-hour cycle of night and day. • This circadian rhythm appears in our daily patterns of body temperature, arousal, sleeping, and waking. Age and experiences can alter these patterns, resetting our biological clock. 23-3 • Biology—our circadian rhythm as well as our age and our body’s production of melatonin (influenced by the brain’s suprachiasmatic nucleus)—interacts with cultural expectations and individual behaviors to determine our sleeping and waking patterns. 23-4 23-2 What is the biological rhythm of our sleeping and dreaming stages? • We cycle through four distinct sleep stages about every 90 minutes. • Leaving the alpha waves of the awake, relaxed stage, we descend into the irregular brain waves of non-REM stage 1 sleep (NREM-1), often with the sensation of falling or floating. • NREM-2 sleep (in which we spend the most time) follows, lasting about 20 minutes, with its characteristic sleep spindles. • We then enter NREM-3 sleep, lasting about 30 minutes, with large, slow delta waves. • About an hour after falling asleep, we begin periods of REM (rapid eye movement) sleep. • Most dreaming occurs in this REM stage (also known as paradoxical sleep) of internal arousal but outward paralysis. • During a normal night’s sleep, NREM-3 sleep shortens and REM and NREM-2 sleep lengthens. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 232 232 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 232 How do biology and environment interact in our sleep patterns? What are sleep’s functions? • Sleep may have played a protective role in human evolution by keeping people safe during potentially dangerous periods. • • Sleep also helps restore and repair damaged neurons. • • Sleep promotes creative problem solving the next day. REM and NREM-2 sleep help strengthen neural connections that build enduring memories. During slow-wave sleep, the pituitary gland secretes human growth hormone, which is necessary for muscle development. 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:12 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Sleep Patterns and Sleep Theories Module 23 233 Multiple-Choice Questions 1. Which of the following represents a circadian rhythm? a. A burst of growth occurs during puberty. b. A full Moon occurs about once a month. c. Body temperature rises each day as morning approaches. d. When it is summer in the northern hemisphere, it is winter in the southern hemisphere. e. Pulse rate increases when we exercise. 2. In which stage of sleep are you likely to experience hypnagogic sensations of falling? a. b. c. d. e. Alpha sleep NREM-1 NREM-2 NREM-3 REM 3. What is the role of the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in sleep? a. It induces REM sleep approximately every 90 minutes during sleep. b. It causes the pineal gland to increase the production of melatonin. c. It causes the pituitary gland to increase the release of human growth hormone. d. It causes the pituitary gland to decrease the release of human growth hormone. e. It causes the pineal gland to decrease the production of melatonin. sleep can • provide protection. • promote physical growth. a. b. c. d. e. Memory Protection Growth Recuperation Creativity Answer to Practice FRQ 2 2. Name and briefly describe three stages of sleep when 1 point: NREM-1, a brief stage of light sleep that may include hallucinations and sensations of falling. rapid eye movements are not occurring. (3 points) 1 point: NREM-2, moderately deep sleep that includes rapid bursts of brain activity called sleep spindles. 1 point: Sleep kept our ancestors safe from nighttime dangers. 1 point: Sleep promotes the release of pituitary growth hormone. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod23_B.indd 233 1 point: NREM-3, deep sleep, characterized by large, slow delta waves, from which it is difficult to awaken. 1/15/14 8:47 AM SleepConsciousness Patterns and Sleep Theories Understanding and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 233 3. e 4. d role in restoring and repairing brain tissue? Answer 1/15/14 8:47 AM 1. c 2. b 4. Which of the following sleep theories emphasizes sleep’s Practice FRQs 1. Sleep serves many functions for us. Briefly explain how Answers to Multiple-Choice Questions Module 23 22 233 2/20/14 8:12 AM 234 ENGAGE TR M TRM Active Learning The importance of sleep cannot be ignored, especially in today’s fastpaced society. Use the following activity to help students evaluate how sleep may positively affect their lives. On the left half of a piece of paper, have students list 3 ways in which they would like to improve in school subjects or extracurricular and social activities. After you discuss the importance of getting enough sleep, ask students to speculate on how the 3 items they listed might improve if they changed or modified some of their sleep habits. Encourage them to write down the possible improvements on the right side of the piece of paper. These comparisons may help students determine how they can correct unhealthy sleep habits. Use Student Activity: School Start Times: An Informal Debate from the TRM to help students apply research on sleep deficits to a topic interesting to them. Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders, and Dreams Module Learning Objectives 24-1 Describe the effects of sleep loss, and identify the major sleep disorders. 24-2 Describe the most common content of dreams. 24-3 Identify proposed explanations for why we dream. Sleep Deprivation and Sleep Disorders 24-1 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 234 How does sleep loss affect us, and what are the major sleep disorders? When our body yearns for sleep but does not get it, we begin to feel terrible. Trying to stay awake, we will eventually lose. It’s easy to spot students who have stayed up late to study for a test or finish a term paper: They are often fighting the “nods” (their heads bobbing downward in seconds-long “microsleeps”) as they fight to stay awake. In the tiredness battle, sleep always wins. In 1989, Michael Doucette was named America’s Safest Driving Teen. In 1990, while driving home from college, he fell asleep at the wheel and collided with an oncoming car, killing both himself and the other driver. Michael’s driving instructor later acknowledged never having mentioned sleep deprivation and drowsy driving (Dement, 1999). Effects of Sleep Loss Today, more than ever, our sleep patterns leave us not only sleepy but drained of energy and feelings of well-being. After a succession of 5-hour nights, we accumulate a sleep debt that need not be entirely repaid but cannot be satisfied by one long sleep. “The brain keeps an accurate count of sleep debt for at least two weeks,” reported sleep researcher William Dement (1999, p. 64). Obviously, then, we need sleep. Sleep commands roughly one-third of our lives—some 25 years, on average. But why? Allowed to sleep unhindered, most adults will sleep at least 9 hours a night (Coren, 1996). With that much sleep, we awake refreshed, sustain better moods, and perform more efficient and accurate work. The U.S. Navy and the National Institutes of Health have demonstrated the benefits of unrestricted sleep in experiments in which volunteers spent 14 hours daily in bed for at least a week. For the first few days, the volunteers averaged 12 hours of sleep a day or more, apparently paying off a sleep debt that averaged 25 to 30 hours. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 234 234 Images s Use the Module 24 Fact or Falsehood? activity from the TRM to introduce the concepts from this module. Module 24 Getty Discussion Starter Laska/ TR M TRM Lukhas TEACH Unit V States of Consciousness 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e That accomplished, they then settled back to 7.5 to 9 hours nightly and felt energized and happier (Dement, 1999). In one Gallup survey (Mason, 2005), 63 percent of adults who reported getting the sleep they needed also reported being “very satisfied” with their personal life (as did only 36 percent of those needing more sleep). And when 909 working women reported on their daily moods, the researchers were struck by what mattered little (such as money, so long as the person was not battling poverty), and what mattered a lot: less time pressure at work and a good night’s sleep (Kahneman et al., 2004). Perhaps it’s not surprising, then, that when asked if they had felt well rested on the previous day, 3 in 10 Americans said they had not (Pelham, 2010). College and university students are especially sleep deprived; 69 percent in one national survey reported “feeling tired” or “having little energy” on several or more days in the last two weeks (AP, 2009). In another survey, 28 percent of high school students acknowledged falling asleep in class at least once a week (Sleep Foundation, 2006). The going needn’t get boring before students start snoring. (To test whether you are one of the many sleep-deprived students, see TABLE 24.1.) Sleep loss is a predictor of depression. Researchers who studied 15,500 young people, 12 to 18 years old, found that those who slept 5 or fewer hours a night had a 71 percent higher risk of depression than their peers who slept 8 hours or more (Gangwisch et al., 2010). This link does not appear to reflect sleep difficulties caused by depression. When children and youth are followed through time, sleep loss predicts depression rather than Module 24 235 MARK RALSTON/AFP/Getty Images Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders, and Dreams Sleepless and suffering These fatigued, sleep-deprived earthquake rescue workers in China may experience a depressed immune system, impaired concentration, and greater vulnerability to accidents. FYI In a 2001 Gallup poll, 61 percent of men, but only 47 percent of women, said they got enough sleep. Table 24.1 Cornell University psychologist James Maas has reported that most students suffer the consequences of sleeping less than they should. To see if you are in that group, answer the following true-false questions: True False 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. I need an alarm clock in order to wake up at the appropriate time. It’s a struggle for me to get out of bed in the morning. Weekday mornings I hit snooze several times to get more sleep. I feel tired, irritable, and stressed out during the week. I have trouble concentrating and remembering. I feel slow with critical thinking, problem solving, and being creative. I often fall asleep watching TV. I often fall asleep in boring meetings or lectures or in warm rooms. I often fall asleep after heavy meals. I often fall asleep while relaxing after dinner. I often fall asleep within five minutes of getting into bed. I often feel drowsy while driving. I often sleep extra hours on weekend mornings. I often need a nap to get through the day. I have dark circles around my eyes. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 235 Teaching Tip Relate to students the story of Randy Gardner, a high school student who in 1969 attempted to stay awake for as long as possible. Gardner eventually stayed awake for 11 days, but he experienced frightening symptoms as a result of his sleep loss. He developed a heart murmur, slurred his speech, and couldn’t attend to a conversation for more than a few moments. He hallucinated that street signs were people, and he believed himself to be an African American football star, even though he was not African American and did not play football. When people confronted him with the truth, he called them racist. Once Gardner began sleeping again, his symptoms disappeared. He was back to his regular sleep pattern within 3–4 days of ending his marathon of wakefulness. ENGAGE TR M TRM 1/15/14 8:47 AM Sleep Deprivation, Consciousness Sleep Disorders,and andHypnosis Dreams Understanding MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 235 Active Learning Have students take the sleep deprivation quiz in Table 24.1. Have them evaluate whether the quiz indicates that they are experiencing sleep deprivation. Then ask them to reflect on why they may be sleep deprived. Challenge them to consider ways in which lack of sleep has adversely affected their lives and what they can realistically do to add more sleep to their daily schedules. Use Student Activity: Visiting SleepNet from the TRM to give students more information about the importance of sleep. If you answered “true” to three or more items, you probably are not getting enough sleep. To determine your sleep needs, Maas recommends that you “go to bed 15 minutes earlier than usual every night for the next week—and continue this practice by adding 15 more minutes each week—until you wake without an alarm clock and feel alert all day.” (Sleep Quiz reprinted with permission from James B. Maas, “Sleep to Win!” (Bloomington, IN: AuthorHouse, 2013).) 1/15/14 8:47 AM TEACH Module 24 22 235 2/20/14 8:13 AM 236 Unit V States of Consciousness ENGAGE Critical Questions If the benefits of sleep are so clear, why does Western culture seem to disdain it? Have students consider the factors that keep people in the United States and other industrialized cultures from sleeping. How do economic factors play into the lack of sleep? What about social influences? How can policy makers encourage people to get more sleep? TEACH TR M TRM Teaching Tip Ask students if they have ever fallen asleep when bored. Most students will raise their hands. Tell them that boredom actually does not cause drowsiness. In fact, boredom causes restless behavior, like fidgeting or impatience. When people fall asleep while bored, it is an indication of sleep deprivation. If students are falling asleep in class, this is a sure sign they are not getting enough sleep during the night. Use Student Activity: Sleep Profile from the TRM to help students keep track of their own sleep habits. A P ® E x a m Ti p Many students try to get by on less and less sleep to try to fit everything in. The irony is that if you stay up too late studying, it can be counterproductive. Sleep deprivation makes it difficult to concentrate and increases the likelihood you will make silly mistakes on tests. The impact on your immune system means you are more likely to get sick. To be the best student you can be, make sleep a priority. “So shut your eyes Kiss me goodbye And sleep Just sleep.” -SONG BY MY CHEMICAL ROMANCE MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 236 236 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 236 vice versa (Gregory et al., 2009). Moreover, REM sleep’s processing of emotional experiences helps protect against depression (Walker & van der Helm, 2009). After a good night’s sleep, we often do feel better the next day. And that may help to explain why parentally enforced bedtimes predict less depression, and why pushing back school start time leads to improved adolescent sleep, alertness, and mood (Gregory et al., 2009; Owens et al., 2010). Even when awake, students often function below their peak. And they know it: Four in five teens and three in five 18- to 29-year-olds wish they could get more sleep on weekdays (Mason, 2003, 2005). Yet that teen who staggers glumly out of bed in response to an unwelcome alarm, yawns through morning classes, and feels half-depressed much of the day may be energized at 11:00 P.M. and mindless of the next day’s looming sleepiness (Carskadon, 2002). “Sleep deprivation has consequences—difficulty studying, diminished productivity, tendency to make mistakes, irritability, fatigue,” noted Dement (1999, p. 231). A large sleep debt “makes you stupid.” It can also make you fatter. Sleep deprivation increases ghrelin, a hunger-arousing hormone, and decreases its hunger-suppressing partner, leptin (more on these in Module 38). It also increases cortisol, a stress hormone that stimulates the body to make fat. Sure enough, children and adults who sleep less than normal are fatter than those who sleep more (Chen et al., 2008; Knutson et al., 2007; Schoenborn & Adams, 2008). And experimental sleep deprivation of adults increases appetite and eating (Nixon et al., 2008; Patel et al., 2006; Spiegel et al., 2004; Van Cauter et al., 2007). This may help explain the common weight gain among sleep-deprived students (although a review of 11 studies reveals that the mythical college student’s “freshman 15” is, on average, closer to a “first-year 4” [Hull et al., 2007]). In addition to making us more vulnerable to obesity, sleep deprivation can suppress immune cells that fight off viral infections and cancer (Motivala & Irwin, 2007). One experiment exposed volunteers to a cold virus. Those who had been averaging less than 7 hours sleep a night were 3 times more likely to develop a cold than were those sleeping 8 or more hours a night (Cohen et al., 2009). Sleep’s protective effect may help explain why people who sleep 7 to 8 hours a night tend to outlive those who are chronically sleep deprived, and why older adults who have no difficulty falling or staying asleep tend to live longer than their sleep-deprived agemates (Dement, 1999; Dew et al., 2003). When infections do set in, we typically sleep more, boosting our immune cells. Sleep deprivation slows reactions and increases errors on visual attention tasks similar to those involved in screening airport baggage, performing surgery, and reading X-rays (Lim & Dinges, 2010). Similarly, the result can be devastating for driving, piloting, and equipment operating. Driver fatigue has contributed to an estimated 20 percent of American traffic accidents (Brody, 2002) and to some 30 percent of Australian highway deaths (Maas, 1999). One two-year study examined the driving accident rates of more than 20,000 Virginia 16- to 18-year-olds in two major cities. In one city, the high schools started 75 to 80 minutes later than in the other. The late starters had about 25 percent fewer crashes (Vorona et al., 2011). Consider, too, the timing of four industrial disasters—the 1989 Exxon Valdez tanker hitting rocks and spilling millions of gallons of oil on the shores of Alaska; Union Carbide’s 1984 release of toxic gas that killed thousands in Bhopal, India; and the 1979 Three Mile Island and 1986 Chernobyl nuclear accidents. All occurred after midnight, when operators in charge were likely to be drowsiest and unresponsive to signals requiring an alert response. Likewise, the 2013 Asiana Airlines crash landing at San Francisco Airport happened at 3:30 A.M. Korea time, after a 10-hour flight from Seoul. When sleepy frontal lobes confront an unexpected situation, misfortune often results. Stanley Coren capitalized on what is, for many North Americans, a semi-annual sleepmanipulation experiment—the “spring forward” to “daylight savings” time and “fall backward” to “standard” time. Searching millions of records, Coren found that in both Canada and the United States, accidents increased immediately after the time change that shortens sleep (FIGURE 24.1). 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders, and Dreams Number of accidents 2800 Less sleep, more accidents 2700 4200 More sleep, fewer accidents 4000 2500 3800 2400 237 Figure 24.1 Canadian traffic accidents On the Monday Number of accidents 2600 Module 24 ENGAGE Enrichment after the spring time change, when people lose one hour of sleep, accidents increased, as compared with the Monday before. In the fall, traffic accidents normally increase because of greater snow, ice, and darkness, but they diminished after the time change. (Adapted from Coren, 1996.) According to legend, Leonardo da Vinci slept a mere 90 minutes a day, in catnaps of 15 minutes every 4 hours. Salvador Dali liked to doze off while sitting up with a spoon in his hand. As he fell asleep, the spoon would fall and clatter to the ground, and he would wake rejuvenated. Thomas Edison and Winston Churchill also seemed to thrive on catnaps. Lyndon B. Johnson put on his pajamas in the middle of the day and slept for 30 minutes. Bill Clinton naps in cars, buses, trains, and planes. He jokes that Arkansans might come naturally to sleeping anywhere because “most of us don’t have to go very far back to find a family without a bed.” 3600 Spring time change (hour of sleep lost) Monday before time change Fall time change (hour of sleep gained) Monday after time change FIGURE 24.2 summarizes the effects of sleep deprivation. But there is good news! Psychologists have discovered a treatment that strengthens memory, increases concentration, boosts mood, moderates hunger and obesity, fortifies the disease-fighting immune system, and lessens the risk of fatal accidents. Even better news: The treatment feels good, it can be self-administered, the supplies are limitless, and it’s available free! If you are a typical high school student, often going to bed near midnight and dragged out of bed six or seven hours later by the dreaded alarm, the treatment is simple: Each night just add 15 minutes to your sleep. Ignore that last text, resist the urge to check in with friends online, and succumb to sleep, “the gentle tyrant.” Brain Diminished attentional focus and memory consolidation, and increased risk of depression Immune system Suppression of immune cell production and increased risk of viral infections, such as colds Figure 24.2 How sleep deprivation affects us Heart Increased risk of high blood pressure Stomach Increased hunger-arousing ghrelin and decreased hunger-suppressing leptin Fat cells Increased production and greater risk of obesity Joints Increased inflammation and arthritis 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 237 Muscles Reduced strength, and slower reaction time and motor learning 1/15/14 8:47 AM Sleep Deprivation, Consciousness Sleep Disorders,and andHypnosis Dreams Understanding MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 237 Module 24 22 237 2/20/14 8:13 AM 238 Unit V States of Consciousness MAJOR SLEEP DISORDERS ENGAGE Enrichment Gregg Jacobs and his colleagues report much better success in treating insomnia when a combination of behavioral techniques is used. Subjects who had serious difficulty falling asleep were told to try the following strategies: Sleep restriction: Do not spend more than 7 hours in bed. Avoid naps, and arise at the same time every morning, including weekends. Stimulus control: Go to bed only when sleepy, and use the bed exclusively for sleep or relaxing activities. If you cannot fall asleep within 20 minutes, stop trying and do something relaxing. Relaxation response training: Use soothing visual imagery, rhythmic breathing, and muscle relaxation to calm yourself. Jacobs, G., Benson, H., & Friedman, R. (1993). Home-based central nervous system assessment of a multifactor behavioral intervention for chronic sleep-onset insomnia. Behavior Therapy, 24, 159–174. “The lion and the lamb shall lie down together, but the lamb will not be very sleepy.” -WOODY ALLEN, IN THE MOVIE LOVE AND DEATH, 1975 “Sleep is like love or happiness. If you pursue it too ardently it will elude you.” -WILSE WEBB, SLEEP: THE GENTLE TYRANT, 1992 insomnia recurring problems in falling or staying asleep. narcolepsy a sleep disorder characterized by uncontrollable sleep attacks. The sufferer may lapse directly into REM sleep, often at inopportune times. Common Pitfalls Point out to students that insomnia is not just an inability to fall asleep quickly: it is also an inability to stay asleep. Patients with insomnia often cannot sleep through the night, waking up in the middle of the night and then having trouble falling back asleep. Use Student Activity: Sleep Strategies from the TRM to give students more tools to help them fall asleep when needed. Table 24.2 Some Natural Sleep Aids • Exercise regularly but not in the late evening. (Late afternoon is best.) • Avoid caffeine after early afternoon, and avoid food and drink near bedtime. The exception would be a glass of milk, which provides raw materials for the manufacture of serotonin, a neurotransmitter that facilitates sleep. • Relax before bedtime, using dimmer light. • Sleep on a regular schedule (rise at the same time even after a restless night) and avoid naps. • Hide the clock face so you aren’t tempted to check it repeatedly. • Reassure yourself that temporary sleep loss causes no great harm. • Realize that for any stressed organism, being vigilant is natural and adaptive. A personal conflict during the day often means a fitful sleep that night (Åkerstedt et al., 2007; Brissette & Cohen, 2002). And a traumatic stressful event can take a lingering toll on sleep (Babson & Feldner, 2010). Managing your stress levels will enable more restful sleeping. (See Modules 43, 44, and 84 for more on stress.) • If all else fails, settle for less sleep, either by going to bed later or getting up earlier. TEACH TR M TRM No matter what their normal need for sleep, 1 in 10 adults, and 1 in 4 older adults, complain of insomnia—not an occasional inability to sleep when anxious or excited, but persistent problems in falling or staying asleep (Irwin et al., 2006). From middle age on, awakening occasionally during the night becomes the norm, not something to fret over or treat with medication (Vitiello, 2009). Ironically, insomnia is worsened by fretting about one’s insomnia. In laboratory studies, insomnia complainers do sleep less than others, but they typically overestimate—by about double—how long it takes them to fall asleep. They also underestimate by nearly half how long they actually have slept. Even if we have been awake only an hour or two, we may think we have had very little sleep because it’s the waking part we remember. The most common quick fixes for true insomnia—sleeping pills and alcohol—can aggravate the problem, reducing REM sleep and leaving the person with next-day blahs. Such aids can also lead to tolerance—a state in which increasing doses are needed to produce an effect. An ideal sleep aid would mimic the natural chemicals that are abundant during sleep, without side effects. Until scientists can supply this magic pill, sleep experts have offered some tips for getting better quality sleep (TABLE 24.2). Falling asleep is not the problem for people with narcolepsy (from narco, “numbness,” and lepsy, “seizure”), who have sudden attacks of overwhelming sleepiness, usually lasting less than 5 minutes. Narcolepsy attacks can occur at the most inopportune times, perhaps just after taking a terrific swing at a softball or when laughing loudly, shouting angrily, or having sex (Dement, 1978, 1999). In severe cases, the person collapses directly into a brief period of REM sleep, with loss of muscular tension. People with narcolepsy—1 in 2000 of us, estimated the Stanford University Center for Narcolepsy (2002)—must therefore live with extra caution. As a traffic menace, “snoozing is second only to boozing,” says the American Sleep Disorders Association, and those with narcolepsy are especially at risk (Aldrich, 1989). Researchers have discovered genes that cause narcolepsy in dogs and humans (Miyagawa et al., 2008; Taheri, 2004). Genes help sculpt the brain, and neuroscientists are searching the brain for narcolepsy-linked abnormalities. One team discovered a relative absence of a hypothalamic neural center that produces orexin (also called hypocretin), a neurotransmitter linked to alertness (Taheri et al., 2002; Thannickal et al., 2000). (That discovery has led to the clinical testing of a new sleeping pill that works by blocking orexin’s arousing activity.) TEACH MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 238 1/15/14 8:47 AM Concept Connections Narcolepsy is often treated with powerful stimulants, called amphetamines, to keep the patient’s brain from going into the REM patterns that are most restful. Amphetamines are discussed in greater detail later in the unit. 238 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 238 States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders, and Dreams Foundation (2009) survey found 27 percent of people reporting sleeplessness related to the economy, their personal finances, and employment, as seems evident in this man looking for work. ENGAGE sleep apnea a sleep disorder characterized by temporary cessations of breathing during sleep and repeated momentary awakenings. Active Learning night terrors a sleep disorder characterized by high arousal and an appearance of being terrified; unlike nightmares, night terrors occur during NREM-3 sleep, within two or three hours of falling asleep, and are seldom remembered. 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 239 Some people have theorized that sleep apnea is related to SIDS or Sudden Infant Death Syndrome, which occurs when infants seem to die without cause in their sleep. Parents have been given conflicting advice throughout the years on how to avoid this disturbing and controversial problem. Have students explore what researchers know about SIDS and its link to sleep disorders: What are some possible causes of SIDS? How might one explanation be related to apnea? What is the current advice to parents about preventing SIDS? How has this advice differed over the years? What are some recent research projects that explore the causes and prevention of SIDS? Did Brahms need his own lullabies? Cranky, overweight, and nap-prone, Johannes Brahms exhibited common symptoms of sleep apnea (Margolis, 2000). Brian Chase/Shutterstock Narcolepsy, it is now clear, is a brain disease; it is not just “in your mind.” And this gives hope that narcolepsy might be effectively relieved by a drug that mimics the missing orexin and can sneak through the blood-brain barrier (Fujiki et al., 2003; Siegel, 2000). In the meantime, physicians are prescribing other drugs to relieve narcolepsy’s sleepiness in humans. Although 1 in 20 of us have sleep apnea, it was unknown before modern sleep research. Apnea means “with no breath,” and people with this condition intermittently stop breathing during sleep. After an airless minute or so, decreased blood oxygen arouses them and they wake up enough to snort in air for a few seconds, in a process that repeats hundreds of times each night, depriving them of slow-wave sleep. Apnea sufferers don’t recall these episodes the next day. So, despite feeling fatigued and depressed—and hearing their mate’s complaints about their loud “snoring”—many are unaware of their disorder (Peppard et al., 2006). Sleep apnea is associated with obesity, and as the number of obese Americans has increased, so has this disorder, particularly among overweight men, including some football players (Keller, 2007). Other warning signs are loud snoring, daytime sleepiness and irritability, and (possibly) high blood pressure, which increases the risk of a stroke or heart attack (Dement, 1999). If one doesn’t mind looking a little goofy in the dark (imagine a snorkeler at a slumber party), the treatment—a masklike device with an air pump that keeps the sleeper’s airway open—can effectively relieve apnea symptoms. Unlike sleep apnea, night terrors target mostly children, who may sit up or walk around, talk incoherently, experience doubled heart and breathing rates, and appear terrified (Hartmann, 1981). They seldom wake up fully during an episode and recall little or nothing the next morning—at most, a fleeting, frightening image. Night terrors are not nightmares (which, like other dreams, typically occur during early morning REM sleep); night terrors usually occur during the first few hours of NREM-3. Sleepwalking—another NREM-3 sleep disorder—and sleep talking are usually childhood disorders, and like narcolepsy, they run in families. (Sleep talking—usually garbled or nonsensical—can occur during any sleep stage [Mahowald & Ettinger, 1990].) Occasional childhood sleepwalking occurs for about one-third of those with a sleepwalking fraternal twin and half of those with a sleepwalking identical twin. The same is true for sleep talking (Hublin et al., 1997, 1998). Sleepwalking is usually harmless. After returning to bed on their own or with the help of a family member, few sleepwalkers recall their trip the next morning. About 20 percent of 3- to 12-year-olds have at least one episode of sleepwalking, 239 The Granger Collection, New York AP Photo/Paul Sakuma, File Economic recession and stress can rob sleep A National Sleep Module 24 ENGAGE Now I lay me down to sleep Enrichment For many with sleep apnea, a continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) machine makes for sounder sleeping and better quality of life. Some studies have shown a link between Type 2 diabetes and apnea. Dr. Arthur Friedlander (2000) published a study suggesting that diabetic patients were 3 times more likely to develop sleep apnea. The link may be correlational rather than causal; that is, being overweight may be a factor in both disorders. 1/15/14 8:47 AM ENGAGE Active Learning Divide your students into groups, and have each one research a different sleep disorder. Have the groups collect information on the causes, treatments, and symptoms of their disorder. Challenge students to go beyond the information in the text. If possible, have them find a patient with this particular disorder to see what his or her day-to-day life is like. (Alternatively, they could find a written account by someone who describes living with the sleep disorder that the students have been assigned.) Groups can then present their findings in several ways: Create a poster for display to educate other students about the sleep disorder. Write a newspaper item about the disorder for the school or local newspaper. Create a PowerPoint presentation for a seminar about sleep disorders sponsored by the psychology club. Sleep Deprivation, Consciousness Sleep Disorders,and andHypnosis Dreams Understanding MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 239 Module 24 22 239 2/20/14 8:13 AM 240 Unit V States of Consciousness usually lasting 2 to 10 minutes; some 5 percent have repeated episodes (Giles et al., 1994). Young children, who have the deepest and lengthiest NREM-3 sleep, are the most likely to experience both night terrors and sleepwalking. As we grow older and deep NREM-3 sleep diminishes, so do night terrors and sleepwalking. After being sleep deprived, we sleep more deeply, which increases any tendency to sleepwalk (Zadra et al., 2008). ENGAGE Enrichment Stanley Krippner and his colleagues compared the dreams of more than 400 Argentinean, Brazilian, and American adults: Americans tended to dream about animals and food, whereas Brazilians had more sexual and emotional dreams. Americans and Argentineans reported more dreams about architecture than Brazilians did. Argentineans reported more dreams about aggression and good fortune than Brazilians or Americans did. Inception creatively played off our interest in finding meaning in our dreams, and in understanding the layers of our consciousness. It further explored the idea of creating false memories through the power of suggestion—an idea we will explore in Module 33. dream a sequence of images, emotions, and thoughts passing through a sleeping person’s mind. Dreams are notable for their hallucinatory imagery, discontinuities, and incongruities, and for the dreamer’s delusional acceptance of the content and later difficulties remembering it. The dreams of Americans from different parts of the country differed in content: Northeasterners have dreams with images of time, activity, streets, and architecture. Southerners dream of nature, good fortune, emotion, and family members. Westerners dream about architecture, objects, negative emotions, and indoor settings. “I do not believe that I am now dreaming, but I cannot prove that I am not.” -PHILOSOPHER BERTRAND RUSSELL (1872–1970) FYI Would you suppose that people dream if blind from birth? Studies in France, Hungary, Egypt, and the United States all found blind people dreaming of using their nonvisual senses—hearing, touching, smelling, tasting (Buquet, 1988; Taha, 1972; Vekassy, 1977). In the United States, men were more likely to dream about aggression and tools; women were more likely to dream about children, clothes, food, and friendly interactions. There were relatively few gender differences in dreams within the Latin American sample. Other researchers have found that Mexican women’s dreams are significantly more emotional than men’s. Casto, K. L., Krippner, S., & Tartz, R. (1999). The identification of spiritual content in dream reports. Anthropology of Consciousness, 10(1), 43–53. 240 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 240 “For what one has dwelt on by day, these things are seen in visions of the night.” -MENANDER OF ATHENS (342–292 B.C.E.), THE PRINCIPAL FRAGMENTS Dreams Now playing at an inner theater near you: the premiere showing of a sleeping person’s vivid dream. This never-before-seen mental movie features captivating characters wrapped in a plot so original and unlikely, yet so intricate and so seemingly real, that the viewer later marvels at its creation. Waking from a troubling dream, wrenched by its emotions, who among us has not wondered about this weird state of consciousness? How can our brain so creatively, colorfully, and completely construct this alternative world? In the shadowland between our dreaming and waking consciousness, we may even wonder for a moment which is real. Discovering the link between REM sleep and dreaming opened a new era in dream research. Instead of relying on someone’s hazy recall hours or days after having a dream, researchers could catch dreams as they happened. They could awaken people during or within 3 minutes after a REM sleep period and hear a vivid account. What We Dream 24-2 Krippner also found some differences between the genders: Photofest/Warner Bros. Pictures A dreamy take on dreamland The 2010 movie What do we dream? Daydreams tend to involve the familiar details of our life—perhaps picturing ourselves explaining to a teacher why a paper will be late, or replaying in our minds personal encounters we relish or regret. REM dreams—“hallucinations of the sleeping mind”(Loftus & Ketcham, 1994, p. 67)—are vivid, emotional, and bizarre—so vivid we may confuse them with reality. Awakening from a nightmare, a 4-year-old may be sure there is a bear in the house. We spend six years of our life in dreams, many of which are anything but sweet. For both women and men, 8 in 10 dreams are marked by at least one negative event or emotion (Domhoff, 2007). Common themes are repeatedly failing in an attempt to do something; of being attacked, pursued, or rejected; or of experiencing misfortune (Hall et al., 1982). Dreams with sexual imagery occur less often than you might think. In one study, only 1 in 10 dreams among young men and 1 in 30 among young women had sexual content (Domhoff, 1996). More commonly, the story line of our dreams incorporates traces of previous days’ nonsexual experiences and preoccupations (De Koninck, 2000): • After suffering a trauma, people commonly report nightmares, which help extinguish daytime fears (Levin & Nielsen, 2007, 2009). One sample of Americans recording their dreams during September 2001 reported an increase in threatening dreams following the 9/11 terrorist attacks (Propper et al., 2007). • After playing the computer game Tetris for 7 hours and then being awakened repeatedly during their first hour of sleep, 3 in 4 people reported experiencing images of the game’s falling blocks (Stickgold et al., 2000). • Compared with city-dwellers, people in hunter-gatherer societies more often dream of animals (Mestel, 1997). Compared with nonmusicians, musicians report twice as many dreams of music (Uga et al., 2006). MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 240 1/15/14 8:47 AM ENGAGE TR M TRM Enrichment Students are fascinated by dream analysis. Popularized by Sigmund Freud as being the key to understanding our unconscious thoughts, dream analysis is still used by some psychoanalysts to help clients understand the causes of their current life problems. Typically, however, these professionals leave most of the interpretation up to the client, forgoing the practice of imposing symbolic interpretation on the patient. Most dream interpretation books on the market today are not based on scientific testing and shouldn’t be used as evaluation tools. Use Student Activity: Dream Journal from the TRM to help students assess their nightly dreaming experiences. States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Module 24 241 Our two-track mind is also monitoring our environment while we sleep. Sensory stimuli—a particular odor or a phone’s ringing—may be instantly and ingeniously woven into the dream story. In a classic experiment, researchers lightly sprayed cold water on dreamers’ faces (Dement & Wolpert, 1958). Compared with sleepers who did not get the cold-water treatment, these people were more likely to dream about a waterfall, a leaky roof, or even about being sprayed by someone. So, could we learn a foreign language by hearing it played while we sleep? If only it were so easy. While sleeping we can learn to associate a sound with a mild electric shock (and to react to the sound accordingly). But we do not remember recorded information played while we are soundly asleep (Eich, 1990; Wyatt & Bootzin, 1994). In fact, anything that happens during the 5 minutes just before we fall asleep “Uh-oh. I think I’m having one of those dreams again.” is typically lost from memory (Roth et al., 1988). This explains why sleep apnea patients, who repeatedly awaken with a gasp and then immediately fall back to sleep, do not recall the episodes. It also explains why dreams that mo“Follow your dreams, except for mentarily awaken us are mostly forgotten by morning. To remember a dream, get up and that one where you’re naked at work.” -ATTRIBUTED TO COMEDIAN stay awake for a few minutes. TEACH © The New Yorker Collection, 2008, Jack Ziegler from cartoonbank.com.All Rights Reserved. Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders, and Dreams Enrichment Freud believed that dreams were the “royal road” to the unconscious, shedding light on the forbidden thoughts and feelings we keep hidden from ourselves and the world. Why We Dream What are the functions of dreams? Dream theorists have proposed several explanations of why we dream, including these: To satisfy our own wishes. In 1900, in his landmark book The Interpretation of Dreams, Sigmund Freud offered what he thought was “the most valuable of all the discoveries it has been my good fortune to make.” He proposed that dreams provide a psychic safety valve that discharges otherwise unacceptable feelings. He viewed a dream’s manifest content (the apparent and remembered story line) as a censored, symbolic version of its latent content, the unconscious drives and wishes that would be threatening if expressed directly. Although most dreams have no overt sexual imagery, Freud nevertheless believed that most adult dreams could be “traced back by analysis to erotic wishes.” Thus, a gun might be a disguised representation of a penis. Freud considered dreams the key to understanding our inner conflicts. However, his critics say it is time to wake up from Freud’s dream theory, which is a scientific nightmare. Based on the accumulated science, “there is no reason to believe any of Freud’s specific claims about dreams and their purposes,” observed dream researcher William Domhoff (2003). Some contend that even if dreams are symbolic, they could be interpreted any way one wished. Others maintain that dreams hide nothing. A dream about a gun is a dream about a gun. Legend has it that even Freud, who loved to smoke cigars, acknowledged that “sometimes, a cigar is just a cigar.” Freud’s wish-fulfillment theory of dreams has in large part given way to other theories. To file away memories. The information-processing perspective proposes that dreams may help sift, sort, and fix the day’s experiences in our memory. Some studies support this view. When tested the next day after learning a task, those deprived of both slow-wave and REM sleep did not do as well on their new learning as those who slept undisturbed (Stickgold et al., 2000, 2001). People who hear unusual phrases or learn to find hidden visual images before bedtime remember less the next morning if awakened every time they begin REM sleep than they do if awakened during other sleep stages (Empson & Clarke, 1970; Karni & Sagi, 1994). Brain scans confirm the link between REM sleep and memory. The brain regions that buzz as rats learn to navigate a maze, or as people learn to perform a visual-discrimination 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 241 Sleep serves many functions, allowing us to process many things at once. We are great multitaskers when we sleep—protecting, recuperating, making memories, thinking creatively, and growing all at the same time. ENGAGE HENNY YOUNGMAN 24-3 Concept Connections FYI TEACH A popular sleep myth: If you dream you are falling and hit the ground (or if you dream of dying), you die. (Unfortunately, those who could confirm these ideas are not around to do so. Some people, however, have had such dreams and are alive to report them.) Flip It Students can get additional help understanding the function of dreams by watching the Flip It Video: Why Do We Dream? “When people interpret [a dream] as if it were meaningful and then sell those interpretations, it’s quackery.” -SLEEP RESEARCHER J. ALLAN HOBSON (1995) manifest content according to Freud, the remembered story line of a dream (as distinct from its latent, or hidden, content). latent content according to Freud, the underlying meaning of a dream (as distinct from its manifest content). 1/15/14 8:47 AM Sleep Deprivation, Consciousness Sleep Disorders,and andHypnosis Dreams Understanding MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 241 Module 24 22 241 2/20/14 8:13 AM 242 Unit V States of Consciousness ENGAGE Critical Questions Does the sleep bulimia as described by Robert Stickgold occur among your students? Have them discuss whether they use the weekend to play catch-up on their sleep or to become night owls, staying up extremely late participating in social activities. Have them discuss the effect of this erratic sleep behavior on their daily performance. FYI Rapid eye movements also stir the liquid behind the cornea; this delivers fresh oxygen to corneal cells, preventing their suffocation. ENGAGE Applying Science Have students correlate GPA with hours of sleep to try to replicate the findings of the Wolfson and Carskadon study. Ask them to hypothesize the reasons why those with higher GPAs may get more sleep than those with lower GPAs. Have students share their experiences with sleep and academic achievement. FYI Question: Does eating spicy foods cause one to dream more? Answer: Any food that causes you to awaken more increases your chance of recalling a dream (Moorcroft, 2003). Figure 24.3 Sleep across the life span As we age, TEACH our sleep patterns change. During our first few months, we spend progressively less time in REM sleep. During our first 20 years, we spend progressively less time asleep. (Adapted from Snyder y & Scott,, 1972.)) Concept Connections Point out to students how sleep differences occur in a developmental pattern, with infants spending up to 16 hours a day sleeping. Older adults actually sleep about 1 hour less a night than their younger counterparts. This was described in more detail earlier in the unit, but students should be aware of the developmental differences in sleep patterns. Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 242 24 Average daily sleep 16 (hours) Marked drop in REM during infancy 14 Waking 12 REM sleep 10 8 6 4 Non-REM sleep 2 0 1– 3–5 6–23 1–15 days mos. mos. da 2 3–4 5–13 14–18 19–30 yrs. yrs. yrs. yrs. yrs. 31–45 yrs. 90 yrs. swissmacky/Shutterstock Infancy MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 242 242 task, buzz again during later REM sleep (Louie & Wilson, 2001; Maquet, 2001). So precise are these activity patterns that scientists can tell where in the maze the rat would be if awake. Others, unpersuaded by these studies, note that memory consolidation may also occur during non-REM sleep (Siegel, 2001; Vertes & Siegel, 2005). This much seems true: A night of solid sleep (and dreaming) has an important place in our lives. To sleep, perchance to remember. This is important news for students, many of whom, observed researcher Robert Stickgold (2000), suffer from a kind of sleep bulimia—binge-sleeping on the weekend. “If you don’t get good sleep and enough sleep after you learn new stuff, you won’t integrate it effectively into your memories,” he warned. That helps explain why high school students with high grades have averaged 25 minutes more sleep a night than their lower-achieving classmates (Wolfson & Carskadon, 1998). To develop and preserve neural pathways. Perhaps dreams, or the brain activity associated with REM sleep, serve a physiological function, providing the sleeping brain with periodic stimulation. This theory makes developmental sense. As you will see in Unit IX, stimulating experiences preserve and expand the brain’s neural pathways. Infants, whose neural networks are fast developing, spend much of their abundant sleep time in REM sleep (FIGURE 24.3). To make sense of neural static. Other theories propose that dreams erupt from neural activation spreading upward from the brainstem (Antrobus, 1991; Hobson, 2003, 2004, 2009). According to one version, dreams are the brain’s attempt to make sense of random neural activity. Much as a neurosurgeon can produce hallucinations by stimulating different parts of a patient’s cortex, so can stimulation originating within the brain. These internal stimuli activate brain areas that process visual images, but not the visual cortex area, which receives raw input from the eyes. As Freud might have expected, PET scans of sleeping people also reveal increased activity in the emotion-related limbic system (in the amygdala) during REM sleep. In contrast, frontal lobe regions responsible for inhibition and logical thinking seem to idle, which may explain why we are less inhibited in our dreams than when awake (Maquet et al., 1996). Add the limbic system’s emotional tone to the brain’s visual bursts and—voila!—we dream. Damage either the limbic system or the visual centers active during dreaming, and dreaming itself may be impaired (Domhoff, 2003). Childhood Adolescence Adulthood and old age 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders, and Dreams To reflect cognitive development. Some dream researchers dispute both the Freudian and neural activation theories, preferring instead to see dreams as part of brain maturation and cognitive development (Domhoff, 2010, 2011; Foulkes, 1999). For example, prior to age 9, children’s dreams seem more like a slide show and less like an active story in which the dreamer is an actor. Dreams overlap with waking cognition and feature coherent speech. They simulate reality by drawing on our concepts and knowledge. They engage brain networks that also are active during daydreaming. Unlike the idea that dreams arise from bottom-up brain activation, the cognitive perspective emphasizes our mind’s topdown control of our dream content (Nir & Tononi, 2010). TABLE 24.2 compares major dream theories. Although today’s sleep researchers debate dreams’ function—and some are skeptical that dreams serve any function—there is one thing they agree on: We need REM sleep. Deprived of it by repeatedly being awakened, people return more and more quickly to the REM stage after falling back to sleep. When finally allowed to sleep undisturbed, they literally sleep like babies—with increased REM sleep, a phenomenon called REM rebound. Withdrawing REM-suppressing sleeping medications also increases REM sleep, but with accompanying nightmares. Most other mammals also experience REM rebound, suggesting that the causes and functions of REM sleep are deeply biological. That REM sleep occurs in mammals—and not in animals such as fish, whose behavior is less influenced by learning—also fits the information-processing theory of dreams. So does this mean that because dreams serve physiological functions and extend normal cognition, they are psychologically meaningless? Not necessarily. Every psychologically meaningful experience involves an active brain. We are once again reminded of a basic principle: Biological and psychological explanations of behavior are partners, not competitors. Module 24 243 ENGAGE Enrichment What happens when we don’t get REM sleep? Students may have seen movies or TV shows that depict people becoming delusional if deprived of dreaming. Some studies have shown that REM deprivation can lead to confusion, irritability, and lack of concentration. Others have shown that very few ill effects occur when one is deprived of REM. Studies of animals suggest that they will behave as though acting out dreams when REM deprived. Regardless, when people or animals are deprived of REM, they experience REM rebound: falling more easily into REM and staying in REM longer than normal. REM rebound the tendency for REM sleep to increase following REM sleep deprivation (created by repeated awakenings during REM sleep). Table 24.2 Dream Theories Theory 1/15/14 8:47 AM Explanation Critical Considerations Freud’s wishfulfillment Dreams provide a “psychic safety valve”—expressing otherwise unacceptable feelings; contain manifest (remembered) content and a deeper layer of latent content—a hidden meaning. Lacks any scientific support; dreams may be interpreted in many different ways. Informationprocessing Dreams help us sort out the day’s events and consolidate our memories. But why do we sometimes dream about things we have not experienced? Physiological function Regular brain stimulation from REM sleep may help develop and preserve neural pathways. This does not explain why we experience meaningful dreams. Neural activation REM sleep triggers neural activity that evokes random visual memories, which our sleeping brain weaves into stories. The individual’s brain is weaving the stories, which still tells us something about the dreamer. Cognitive development Dream content reflects dreamers’ cognitive development—their knowledge and understanding. Does not address the neuroscience of dreams. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 243 1/15/14 8:47 AM Sleep Deprivation, Consciousness Sleep Disorders,and andHypnosis Dreams Understanding MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 243 Module 24 22 243 2/20/14 8:13 AM 244 Unit V States of Consciousness Before You Move On CLOSE & ASSESS 䉴 ASK YOURSELF Exit Assessment In some places, the school day for teenagers runs from 9:00 A.M. to 4:00 P.M. But in the United States, the teen school day often runs from 8:00 A.M. to 3:00 P.M., or even 7:00 A.M. to 2:00 P.M. Early to rise isn’t making kids wise, say critics—it’s making them sleepy. For optimal alertness and well-being, teens need 8 to 9 hours of sleep a night. So, should early-start schools move to a later start time, even if it requires buying more buses or switching start times with elementary schools? Or is this impractical, and would it do little to remedy the tired-teen problem? Use Table 24.2 to help students keep the theories of dreaming distinct in their minds. Note how Freud’s theory differs from the other theories, especially the neural activation theory. A useful exercise may be to have students write a compare-and-contrast essay on the theories. 䉴 TEST YOURSELF Are you getting enough sleep? What might you ask yourself to answer this question? Answers to the Test Yourself questions can be found in Appendix E at the end of the book. Module 24 Review 24-1 • • How does sleep loss affect us, and what are the major sleep disorders? • • There are five major views of the function of dreams. Sleep disorders include insomnia (recurring wakefulness); narcolepsy (sudden uncontrollable sleepiness or lapsing into REM sleep); sleep apnea (the stopping of breathing while asleep; associated with obesity, especially in men); night terrors (high arousal and the appearance of being terrified; NREM-3 disorder found mainly in children); sleepwalking (NREM-3 disorder also found mainly in children); and sleep talking. • Information-processing: Dreams help us sort out the day’s events and consolidate them in memory. • Physiological function: Regular brain stimulation may help develop and preserve neural pathways in the brain. • Neural activation: The brain attempts to make sense of neural static by weaving it into a story line. • Cognitive development: Dreams reflect the dreamer’s level of development. • Most sleep theorists agree that REM sleep and its associated dreams serve an important function, as shown by the REM rebound that occurs following REM deprivation in humans and other species. What do we dream? • We usually dream of ordinary events and everyday experiences, most involving some anxiety or misfortune. • Fewer than 10 percent (and less among women) of dreams have any sexual content. • Most dreams occur during REM sleep; those that happen during NREM sleep tend to be vague fleeting images. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 244 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 244 What are the functions of dreams? Sleep deprivation causes fatigue and irritability, and it impairs concentration, productivity, and memory consolidation. It can also lead to depression, obesity, joint pain, a suppressed immune system, and slowed performance (with greater vulnerability to accidents). 24-2 244 24-3 Freud’s wish-fulfillment: Dreams provide a psychic “safety valve,” with manifest content (story line) acting as a censored version of latent content (underlying meaning that gratifies our unconscious wishes). 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Disorders, and Dreams Module 24 245 Multiple-Choice Questions 1. Sleep deprivation can lead to weight gain, reduced muscle strength, suppression of the cells that fight common colds, and most likely which of the following? dreams help us sort out the day’s events and consolidate our memories? a b. c. d. e. a. b. c. d. e. Increased productivity Depression Decreased mistakes on homework Increased feeling of well-being Sleep apnea 2. What do we call the sleep disorder that causes you to 1. b 2. c Information-processing Wish-fulfillment Physiological function Neural activation Neural disconnection most likely to experience after sleep deprivation? a. b. c. d. e. a. b. c. d. e. Night terrors Sleep apnea Manifest content dreams Narcolepsy REM rebound Answer to Practice FRQ 2 Practice FRQs 1. Identify and briefly describe the three major sleep disorders experienced by adults. Answer 2 points: Sleep apnea: stops breathing during sleep. 3. a 4. e 4. According to research, which of the following are we stop breathing and awaken in order to take a breath? Narcolepsy Insomnia Sleep apnea Nightmares Night terrors Answers to Multiple-Choice Questions 3. Which of the following dream theories states that 2. Explain the following two theories regarding why we 2 points: Freud’s wish-fulfillment theory states that dreams are a psychic safety valve to express otherwise unacceptable feelings. Criticism: It has no scientific support. dream. Include a criticism each faces: • Freud’s theory • Neural activation theory (4 points) 2 points: Narcolepsy: falls asleep suddenly. 2 points: Insomnia: can’t fall asleep. 2 points: The neural activation theory states that REM evokes random visual images and the brain turns them into stories. Criticism: The individual’s brain is weaving stories, which still tells us something about the dreamer. 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod24_B.indd 245 1/15/14 8:47 AM Sleep Understanding Deprivation, Consciousness Sleep Disorders,and andHypnosis Dreams MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 245 Module 24 22 245 2/20/14 8:13 AM 246 Module 25 TEACH Discussion Starter Use the Module 25 Fact or Falsehood? activity from the TRM to introduce the concepts from this module. ock Psychoactive Drugs TEACH TR M TRM Module Learning Objectives Teaching Tip If students approach you about drugrelated problems they or someone they care about has, help them understand your obligations as a teacher. While you can assure them a certain level of confidentiality, you may be obligated to report behaviors that may cause them personal harm to a guidance counselor, school psychologist, and/or parent. Inform them upfront that you will do everything in your power to help, but for their safety, that help may involve other people more qualified than you. Use Student Activity: Signs of Drug Abuse from the TRM to help students recognize when someone might be addicted to drugs. 25-1 Define substance use disorders, and explain the roles of tolerance, withdrawal, and addiction. 25-2 Identify the depressants, and describe their effects. 25-3 Identify the stimulants, and describe their effects. 25-4 Identify the hallucinogens, and describe their effects. L et’s imagine a day in the life of a legal-drug-using business executive. It begins with a wake-up latte. By midday, several cigarettes have calmed frazzled nerves before an appointment at the plastic surgeon’s office for wrinkle-smoothing Botox injections. A diet pill before dinner helps stem the appetite, and its stimulating effects can later be partially offset with a glass of wine and two Tylenol PMs. And if performance needs enhancing, there are beta blockers for onstage performers, Viagra for middle-aged men, hormonedelivering “libido patches” for middle-aged women, and Adderall for those hoping to focus their concentration. Before drifting off into REM-depressed sleep, our hypothetical drug user is dismayed by news reports of pill-sharing, pill-popping students. Tolerance and Addiction 25-1 TEACH substance use disorder continued substance craving and use despite significant life disruption and/or physical risk. Concept Connections Drugs can have such an effect on the body because they are chemically similar to the brain’s natural neurotransmitters. Here is a list of some commonly known drugs and the neurotransmitters they are similar to: Cocaine is chemically similar to dopamine. LSD is chemically similar to serotonin. Opiates are chemically similar to endorphins. Even the name endorphins is a combination of the NT’s technical name, endogenous morphines. Endogenous means “naturally occurring.” 246 Gang Liu/Shu tterst TR M TRM Unit V States of Consciousness Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 246 psychoactive drug a chemical substance that alters perceptions and moods. tolerance the diminishing effect with regular use of the same dose of a drug, requiring the user to take larger and larger doses before experiencing the drug’s effect. What are substance use disorders, and what role do tolerance, withdrawal, and addiction play in these disorders? Most of us manage to use some nonprescription drugs in moderation and without disrupting our lives. But some of us develop a self-harming substance use disorder (TABLE 25.1). In such cases, the substances being used are psychoactive drugs, chemicals that change perceptions and moods. A drug’s overall effect depends not only on its biological effects but also on the psychology of the user’s expectations, which vary with social and cultural contexts (Ward, 1994). If one culture assumes that a particular drug produces euphoria (or aggression or sexual arousal) and another does not, each culture may find its expectations fulfilled. In Module 81, we’ll take a closer look at these interacting forces in the use and potential abuse of particular psychoactive drugs. But here let’s consider how our bodies react to the ongoing use of psychoactive drugs. Why might a person who rarely drinks alcohol get buzzed on one can of beer while a long-term drinker shows few effects until the second six-pack? The answer is tolerance. With continued use of alcohol and some other drugs (marijuana is an exception), the user’s brain chemistry adapts to offset the drug effect (a process called neuroadaptation). To experience the TEACH MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 246 TR M TRM 1/15/14 8:47 AM Common Pitfalls Drug use can be a difficult topic to discuss since the social stigma of drug use is so profound. If students are using drugs illegally, they or their parents may be in denial of a problem, or their parents may be unaware of it. In discussing drug use and addiction, be sensitive about accusing someone of being a drug user but be open to discussing privately any personal concerns students may have about their own problems or those of family and friends. Use Student Activity: Eyescube Drug Addiction Simulation from the TRM to help students empathize with those who might be addicted to drugs. States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Psychoactive Drugs Module 25 247 TEACH Table 25.1 When Is Drug Use a Disorder? Teaching Tip A person may be diagnosed with substance use disorder when drug use continues despite significant life disruption. Resulting changes in brain circuits may persist after quitting use of the substance (thus leading to strong cravings when exposed to people and situations that trigger memories of drug use). The severity of substance use disorder varies from mild (two to three symptoms) to moderate (four to five symptoms) to severe (six or more symptoms) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Contact a local psychologist who specializes in substance dependence to get his or her thoughts on the various addictions found in the U.S. population. Impaired Control 1. 2. 3. 4. Uses more substance, or for longer, than intended. Tries unsuccessfully to regulate substance use. Spends much time gaining, using, or recovering from substance use. Craves the substance. Does the psychologist believe addictions to the Internet, shopping, or sex have the same fundamental basis as substance dependence? Why or why not? Does the psychologist treat all addictions to substances the same way? Why or why not? Social Impairment 5. Use disrupts obligations at work, school, or home. 6. Continues use despite social problems. 7. Use causes reduced social, recreational, and work activities. Risky Use 8. Continues use despite hazards. 9. Continues use despite worsening physical or psychological problems. Drug Action 10. Experiences tolerance (needing more substance for the desired effect). 11. Experiences withdrawal when attempting to end use. TEACH TR M TRM same effect, the user requires larger and larger doses (FIGURE 25.1). In chronic alcohol abuse, for example, the person’s brain, heart, and liver suffer damage from the excessive amounts of alcohol being “tolerated.” Ever-increasing doses of most psychoactive drugs can pose a serious threat to health and may lead to addiction: The person craves and uses the substance despite its adverse consequences. (See Thinking Critically About: Addiction on the next page.) The World Health Organization (2008) has reported that, worldwide, 90 million people suffer from such problems related to alcohol and other drugs. Regular users often try to fight their addiction, but abruptly stopping the drug may lead to the undesirable side effects of withdrawal. FYI The odds of getting hooked after using various drugs: Tobacco 32% Heroin 23% Alcohol 15% Marijuana 9% Source: National Academy of Science, Institute of Medicine (Brody, 2003). Figure 25.1 Drug tolerance With repeated exposure Big effect Drug effect to a psychoactive drug, the drug’s effect lessens. Thus, it takes larger doses to get the desired effect. Response to first exposure After repeated exposure, more drug is needed to produce same effect Little effect Small Large addiction compulsive craving of drugs or certain behaviors (such as gambling) despite known adverse consequences. withdrawal the discomfort and distress that follow discontinuing an addictive drug or behavior. Drug dose 1/15/14 8:47 AM TEACH MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 247 Concept Connections Teens often feel as though their own behaviors do not qualify as addictive or destructive. They attribute them to situational factors and the behavior of others to dispositional factors—a type of attribution known as the fundamental attribution error (discussed in Unit XIV). Students will say they drink or take drugs due to factors such as stress or social pressure. They will say that others drink or take drugs due to weakness or bad decision making. Help students evaluate their own behaviors critically, asking themselves whether their own behavior would qualify as destructive. Use Student Activity: The Internet Addiction Test from the TRM to help students see that many types of behaviors are addictive behaviors. 1/15/14 8:47 AM Flip It Students can get additional help understanding why people become addicted to drugs by watching the Flip It Video: The Psychology of Addiction. Psychoactive Drugs Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 247 Module 25 22 247 2/20/14 8:13 AM Unit V States of Consciousness 248 TEACH Common Pitfalls Addiction Battling student misconceptions can be difficult because beliefs tend to persist even in the face of disconfirming evidence. Student beliefs about drug use and addiction can be powerfully persistent since their popular culture often seems to validate such myths. Use this opportunity, along with the Thinking Critically About box, to dispel some of these persistent myths. In recent years, the concept of addiction has been extended to cover many behaviors formerly considered bad habits or even sins. Psychologists debate whether the concept has been stretched too far, and whether addictions are really as irresistible as commonly believed. For example, “even for a very addictive drug like cocaine, only 15 to 16 percent of people become addicted within 10 years of first use,” observed Terry Robinson and Kent Berridge (2003). Addictions can be powerful, and many addicts do benefit from therapy or group support. Alcoholics Anonymous has supported millions of people in overcoming their alcohol addiction. But viewing addiction as an uncontrollable disease can undermine people’s self-confidence and their belief that they can change. And that, critics say, would be unfortunate, for many people do voluntarily stop using addictive drugs, without any treatment. Most ex-smokers, for example, have kicked the habit on their own. The addiction-as-disease-needing-treatment idea has been offered for a host of driven, excessive behaviors—eating, shopping, gambling, work, and sex. However, critics suggest that “addiction” can become an all-purpose excuse when used not as a metaphor (“I’m a science fiction addict”) but as reality. Moreover, they note that labeling a behavior doesn’t explain it. Attributing serial adultery, as in the case of Tiger Woods, to a “sex addiction” does not explain the sexual impulsiveness (Radford, 2010). Sometimes, though, behaviors such as gambling, video gaming, or online surfing do become compulsive and dysfunctional, TEACH Concept Connections Many antidepressants are known as SSRIs or selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. These drugs block the reabsorption of serotonin so it remains in the synapse longer. Some disorders that these drugs are effective in treating include depression, bulimia nervosa, obsessive-compulsive disorder, anorexia nervosa, and panic disorder. David Horsey/Hearst Newspapers Thinking Critically About A social networking addiction? much like abusive drug taking (Gentile, 2009; Griffiths, 2001; Hoeft et al., 2008). Thus, psychiatry’s manual of disorders now includes behavior addictions such as “gambling disorder” and proposes “Internet gaming disorder” for further study (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Some Internet users, for example, display an apparent inability to resist logging on, and staying on, even when this excessive use impairs their work and relationships (Ko et al., 2005). Stay tuned. Debates over the nature of addiction continue. Types of Psychoactive Drugs AP TEACH ® E x a m Ti p These three categories— depressants, stimulants, and hallucinogens—are important. There are likely to be questions on the AP® exam that will require you to know how a particular psychoactive drug is classified. Flip It Students can get additional help understanding the biology of drug use by watching the Flip It Video: Neurotransmitters and Drugs. The three major categories of psychoactive drugs are depressants, stimulants, and hallucinogens. All do their work at the brain’s synapses, inhibiting, stimulating, or mimicking the activity of the brain’s own chemical messengers, the neurotransmitters. Depressants 25-2 What are depressants, and what are their effects? Depressants are drugs such as alcohol, barbiturates (tranquilizers), and opiates that calm neural activity and slow body functions. ALCOHOL depressants drugs (such as alcohol, barbiturates, and opiates) that reduce neural activity and slow body functions. True or false? In small amounts, alcohol is a stimulant. False. Low doses of alcohol may, indeed, enliven a drinker, but they do so by acting as a disinhibitor—they slow brain activity that controls judgment and inhibitions. Alcohol is an equal-opportunity drug: It increases (disinhibits) helpful tendencies, as when tipsy restaurant patrons leave extravagant tips (Lynn, 1988). MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 248 ENGAGE 1/15/14 8:47 AM Active Learning Have students explore the advertising techniques alcohol distributors use: 248 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 248 Is there a common theme in the type of scenes depicted in the ads? Describe it. To what type of audience is the ad directed? Are there laws that limit what alcohol advertising can depict? Are there limits to where distributors can advertise? States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Psychoactive Drugs Module 25 249 Ray Ng/Time & Life Pictures/Getty Images And it increases harmful tendencies, as when sexually aroused men become more disposed to sexual aggression. Alcohol + sex = the perfect storm. When drinking, both men and women are more disposed to casual sex (Cooper, 2006; Ebel-Lam et al., 2009). The urges you would feel if sober are the ones you will more likely act upon when intoxicated. SLOWED NEURAL PROCESSING Low doses of alcohol relax the drinker by slowing sympathetic nervous system activity. Larger doses cause reactions to slow, speech to slur, and skilled performance to deteriorate. Paired with sleep deprivation, alcohol is a potent sedative. Add these physical effects to lowered inhibitions, and the result can be deadly. Worldwide, several hundred thousand lives are lost each year in alcohol-related accidents and violent crime. As blood-alcohol levels rise and judgment falters, people’s qualms about drinking and driving lessen. In experiments, virtually all drinkers who had insisted when sober that they would not drive under the influence later decided to drive home from a bar, even when given a breathalyzer test and told they were intoxicated (Denton & Krebs, 1990; MacDonald et al., 1995). Alcohol can also be life threatening when heavy drinking follows an earlier period of moderate drinking, which depresses the vomiting response. People may poison themselves with an overdose that their bodies would normally throw up. Dangerous disinhibition Alcohol consumption leads to feelings of invincibility, which become especially dangerous behind the wheel of a car, such as this one totaled by a teenage drunk driver. This Colorado University Alcohol Awareness Week exhibit prompted many students to post their own anti-drinking pledges (white flags). TR M TRM REDUCED SELF-AWARENESS AND SELF-CONTROL In one experiment, those who consumed alcohol (rather than a placebo beverage) were doubly likely to be caught mind-wandering during a reading task, yet were less likely to notice that they zoned out (Sayette et al., 2009). Alcohol not only reduces self-awareness, it also produces a sort of “myopia” by focusing attention on an arousing situation (such as a provocation) and distracting attention from normal inhibitions and future consequences (Giancola et al., 2010; Hull et al., 1986; Steele & Josephs, 1990). Reduced self-awareness may help explain why people who want to suppress their awareness of failures or shortcomings are more likely to drink than are those who feel good about themselves. Losing a business deal, a game, or a romantic partner sometimes elicits a drinking binge. 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 249 Why have efforts to prohibit alcohol’s sale and consumption failed in the past? Are current laws that regulate the sale and consumption of alcohol effective? Why or why not? What laws or enforcement techniques should be enacted for better effect? What are some of the health benefits of alcohol consumption? What recommendations do doctors give for healthful alcohol consumption? Should there be different blood alcohol limits for men and women considering how the drug affects each gender differently? Why or why not? Use Student Activity: Blood Alcohol Concentrations from the TRM to help students understand the physical effects of different levels of alcohol. TEACH alcohol use disorder (popularly known as alcoholism). Alcohol use marked by tolerance, withdrawal, and a drive to continue problematic use. Concept Connections 1/15/14 8:47 AM ENGAGE Critical Questions Alcohol is a controversial drug. Its misuse has led to countless tragedies. Moderate, thoughtful use is linked to significant health benefits. Have students explore the following questions related to alcohol consumption: Daniel Hommer, NIAAA, NIH, HHS MEMORY DISRUPTION Alcohol can disrupt memory formation, and heavy drinking can have long-term effects on the brain and cognition. In rats, at a developmental period corresponding to human adolescence, binge drinking contributes to nerve cell death and reduces the birth of new nerve cells. It also impairs the growth of synaptic connections (Crews et al., 2006, 2007). In humans, heavy drinking may lead to blackouts, in which drinkers are unable to recall people they met the night before or Scan of woman with Scan of woman without what they said or did while intoxicated. These blackouts result partly from alcohol use disorder alcohol use disorder the way alcohol suppresses REM sleep, which helps fix the day’s experiFigure 25.2 ences into permanent memories. Disordered drinking shrinks The prolonged and excessive drinking that characterizes alcohol use disorder can the brain MRI scans show brain shrink the brain (FIGURE 25.2). Girls and young women (who have less of a stomach shrinkage in women with alcohol use enzyme that digests alcohol) can become addicted to alcohol more quickly than boys and disorder (left) compared with women in a control group (right). young men do, and they are at risk for lung, brain, and liver damage at lower consumption levels (CASA, 2003; Wuethrich, 2001). ENGAGE Alcohol has been shown to affect the hippocampus, the area of the brain that is responsible for forming new memories. This effect helps explain why people who drink heavily lose their memories of the period during which they were intoxicated. Active Learning Have students explore the rates of violent crime that occur when people are acting under the influence of alcohol: What percentage of murders occurs while either the victim or assailant is under the influence? What percentage of sexual assaults occurs while either the victim or assailant is under the influence? Does this percentage change depending on the age of either person? How does the influence of alcohol affect other violent crimes such as theft, battery, or domestic abuse? Psychoactive Drugs Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 249 Module 25 22 249 2/20/14 8:13 AM 250 Unit V States of Consciousness EXPECTANCY EFFECTS As with other drugs, expectations influence behavior. When people believe that alcohol affects social behavior in certain ways, and believe, rightly or wrongly, that they have been drinking alcohol, they will behave accordingly (Moss & Albery, 2009). In a classic experiment, researchers gave Rutgers University men (who had volunteered for a study on “alcohol and sexual stimulation”) either an alcoholic or a nonalcoholic drink (Abrams & Wilson, 1983). (Both had strong tastes that masked any alcohol.) In each group, half the participants thought they were drinking alcohol and half thought they were not. After watching an erotic movie clip, the men who thought they had consumed alcohol were more likely to report having strong sexual fantasies and feeling guilt free. Being able to attribute their sexual responses to alcohol released their inhibitions—whether or not they had actually consumed any alcohol. Alcohol’s effect lies partly in that powerful sex organ, the mind. TEACH Concept Connections Explain to students that expectancy effects are similar to the placebo effect. Both involve the mind convincing the body that an effect will occur. The effects that the body expects may or may not be the actual effects of the drug or treatment, or the effects may be an exaggerated effect of the drug or treatment. BARBITURATES Like alcohol, the barbiturate drugs, or tranquilizers, depress nervous system activity. Barbiturates such as Nembutal, Seconal, and Amytal are sometimes prescribed to induce sleep or reduce anxiety. In larger doses, they can impair memory and judgment. If combined with alcohol—as sometimes happens when people take a sleeping pill after an evening of heavy drinking—the total depressive effect on body functions can be lethal. ENGAGE Enrichment OPIATES Laudanum was a common medicine used in the Victorian era to treat a variety of different ailments. Most successfully, it was used as a painkiller and cough suppressant. It was also found to help with loose stool. Mothers used it often to soothe their fussy babies. What made laudanum so effective? It was a special mixture of alcohol and opium. Doctors during the Victorian period were so impressed with the medicinal properties of opium and laudanum that they neglected to tell their patients about its addictive properties. Officials in the United States banned the practice of prescribing opium in 1914. TEACH Concept Connections Link information about amphetamines to neuroscience by pointing out that recent research has shown that detoxified former methamphetamine users often lose some key dopamine transporters crucial to movement, verbal performance, and memory. Patients with Parkinson’s disease experience similar decreases in these same dopamine transporters. The opiates—opium and its derivatives—also depress neural functioning. When using the opiates, which include heroin, a user’s pupils constrict, breathing slows, and lethargy sets in as blissful pleasure replaces pain and anxiety. For this short-term pleasure, opiate users may pay a long-term price: a gnawing craving for another fix, a need for progressively larger doses (as tolerance develops), and the extreme discomfort of withdrawal. When repeatedly flooded with an artificial opiate, the brain eventually stops producing endorphins, its own opiates. If the artificial opiate is then withdrawn, the brain lacks the normal level of these painkilling neurotransmitters. Those who cannot or choose not to tolerate this state may pay an ultimate price—death by overdose. Opiates include the narcotics, such as codeine and morphine, which physicians prescribe for pain relief. barbiturates drugs that depress central nervous system activity, reducing anxiety but impairing memory and judgment. opiates opium and its derivatives, such as morphine and heroin; they depress neural activity, temporarily lessening pain and anxiety. stimulants drugs (such as caffeine, nicotine, and the more powerful amphetamines, cocaine, Ecstasy, and methamphetamine) that excite neural activity and speed up body functions. amphetamines drugs that stimulate neural activity, causing speeded-up body functions and associated energy and mood changes. nicotine a stimulating and highly addictive psychoactive drug in tobacco. Stimulants 25-3 What are stimulants, and what are their effects? A stimulant excites neural activity and speeds up body functions. Pupils dilate, heart and breathing rates increase, and blood sugar levels rise, causing a drop in appetite. Energy and self-confidence also rise. Stimulants include caffeine, nicotine, the amphetamines, cocaine, methamphetamine (“speed”), and Ecstasy (which is also a mild hallucinogen). People use stimulants to feel alert, lose weight, or boost mood or athletic performance. Unfortunately, stimulants can be addictive, as you may know if you are one of the many who use caffeine daily Vasca/Shutterstock in your coffee, tea, soda, or energy drinks. Cut off from your usual dose, you may crash into fatigue, headaches, irritability, and depression (Silverman et al., 1992). A mild dose of caffeine typically lasts three or four hours, which—if taken in the evening—may be long enough to impair sleep. NICOTINE One of the most addictive stimulants is nicotine, found in cigarettes and other tobacco products. Imagine that cigarettes were harmless—except, once in every 25,000 packs, an occasional innocent-looking one is filled with dynamite instead of tobacco. Not such a bad MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 250 ENGAGE 1/15/14 8:47 AM Active Learning Have students research the effects of caffeine. They should investigate both the negative effects and health benefits of caffeinated products. While caffeine may not be dangerous in and of itself, the products that contain caffeine (coffee, sodas, and so on) are not considered healthful products. 250 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 250 States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Psychoactive Drugs risk of having your head blown off. But with 250 million packs a day consumed worldwide, we could expect more than 10,000 gruesome daily deaths (more than three times the 9/11 fatalities each and every day)—surely enough to have cigarettes banned everywhere.1 The lost lives from these dynamite-loaded cigarettes approximate those from today’s actual cigarettes. A teen-to-the-grave smoker has a 50 percent chance of dying from the habit, and each year, tobacco kills nearly 5.4 million of its 1.3 billion customers worldwide. (Imagine the outrage if terrorists took down an equivalent of 25 loaded jumbo jets today, let alone tomorrow and every day thereafter.) By 2030, annual deaths are expected to increase to 8 million. That means that 1 billion twenty-first-century people may be killed by tobacco (WHO, 2008). Eliminating smoking would increase life expectancy more than any other preventive measure. Those addicted to nicotine find it very hard to quit because tobacco products are as powerfully and quickly addictive as heroin and cocaine. Attempts to quit even within the first weeks of smoking often fail (DiFranza, 2008). As with other addictions, smokers develop tolerance, and quitting causes nicotine-withdrawal symptoms, including craving, insomnia, anxiety, irritability, and distractibility. Nicotine-deprived smokers trying to focus on a task experience a tripled rate of mind-wandering (Sayette et al., 2010). When not craving a cigarette, they tend to underestimate the power of such cravings (Sayette et al., 2008). All it takes to relieve this aversive state is a cigarette—a portable nicotine dispenser. Within 7 seconds, a rush of nicotine signals the central nervous system to release a flood of neurotransmitters (FIGURE 25.3). Epinephrine and norepinephrine diminish appetite and boost alertness and mental efficiency. Dopamine and opioids calm anxiety and reduce sensitivity to pain (Nowak, 1994; Scott et al., 2004). 1 This analogy, adapted here with world-based numbers, was suggested by mathematician Sam Saunders, as reported by K. C. Cole (1998). Module 25 251 ENGAGE Active Learning Tobacco companies have come under intense scrutiny over the last decade as states sued them for reimbursement of the medical expenses of tobacco users. The companies signed a record-setting agreement to settle the claims brought by the states as their legal costs began to mount. FYI Smoke a cigarette and nature will charge you 12 minutes—ironically, just about the length of time you spend smoking it (Discover, 1996). Humorist Dave Barry (1995) recalling why he smoked his first cigarette the summer he turned 15: “Arguments against smoking: ‘It’s a repulsive addiction that slowly but surely turns you into a gasping, gray-skinned, tumor-ridden invalid, hacking up brownish gobs of toxic waste from your one remaining lung.’ Arguments for smoking: ‘Other teenagers are doing it.’ Case closed! Let’s light up!” What were the details of this settlement? What did the companies have to do differently as a result? What benefits did the states get by settling? Did they relinquish any rights regarding future litigation? ENGAGE Enrichment Figure 25.3 Where there’s smoke . . . : The physiological effects of nicotine Nicotine 1. Arouses the brain to a state of increased alertness 4. Reduces circulation to extremities reaches the brain within 7 seconds, twice as fast as intravenous heroin. Within minutes, the amount in the blood soars. 2. Increases heart rate and blood pressure 3. At high levels, relaxes muscles and triggers the release of neurotransmitters that may reduce stress 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 251 Some of the carcinogenic chemicals inhaled by tobacco smokers include formaldehyde, ammonia, and tar— all of which are potentially lethal if ingested into the body separately. Over time, the buildup of these chemicals in the lungs is what leads to lung cancer. 5. Suppresses appetite for carbohydrates 1/15/14 8:47 AM Psychoactive Drugs Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 251 Module 25 22 251 2/20/14 8:13 AM Unit V States of Consciousness Nic-A-Teen Virtually nobody starts ENGAGE Active Learning Tobacco companies were banned from advertising on television in the 1960s. Their advertising efforts, however, center on print media and sponsoring sports events. The recent tobacco settlement put limits on such advertising since those ads could be seen by teens. Divide students into groups to study advertising by tobacco companies today. One group can study magazine ads. In what magazines do tobacco ads regularly appear? To what demographic do they seem to be appealing? Do antismoking ads also appear in the same magazines? Another group can study tobacco companies’ sponsorship of sports events. What events do tobacco products directly sponsor? Whom do these events appeal to? What events do subsidiaries of tobacco companies sponsor? Is their relationship to a tobacco company ever mentioned? TEACH Concept Connections Help students see that operant conditioning is a factor in nicotine and other drug addiction. The faster the high, the more reinforcing the drug-taking behavior is. For many drug users, the cycle of taking drugs and withdrawal from them is a negatively reinforcing system. Once the drug’s effects start to wane, painful withdrawal symptoms occur. Taking more of the drug eliminates the negative symptoms, reinforcing the drug-taking behavior even more. smoking past the vulnerable teen years. Eager to hook customers whose addiction will give them business for years to come, cigarette companies target teens. Portrayals of smoking by popular actors, such as Robert Pattinson in Remember Me, entice teens to imitate. “Cocaine makes you a new man. And the first thing that new man wants is more cocaine.” -COMEDIAN GEORGE CARLIN (1937–2008) cocaine a powerful and addictive stimulant, derived from the coca plant, producing temporarily increased alertness and euphoria. methamphetamine a powerfully addictive drug that stimulates the central nervous system, with speeded-up body functions and associated energy and mood changes; over time, appears to reduce baseline dopamine levels. These rewards keep people smoking, even among the 8 in 10 smokers who wish they could stop (Jones, 2007). Each year, fewer than 1 in 7 smokers who want to quit will be able to. Even those who know they are committing slow-motion suicide may be unable to stop (Saad, 2002). Asked “If you had to do it all over again, would you start smoking?” more than 85 percent of adult smokers have answered No (Slovic et al., 2002). Nevertheless, repeated attempts seem to pay off. Half of all Americans who have ever smoked have quit, sometimes aided by a nicotine replacement drug and with encouragement from a counselor or a support group. Success is equally likely whether smokers quit abruptly or gradually (Fiore et al., 2008; Lichtenstein et al., 2010; Lindson et al., 2010). For those who endure, the acute craving and withdrawal symptoms gradually dissipate over the ensuing 6 months (Ward et al., 1997). After a year’s abstinence, only 10 percent will relapse in the next year (Hughes et al., 2010). These nonsmokers may live not only healthier but also happier lives. Smoking correlates with higher rates of depression, chronic disabilities, and divorce (Doherty & Doherty, 1998; Vita et al., 1998). Healthy living seems to add both years to life and life to years. James Devaney/WireImage 252 COCAINE The recipe for Coca-Cola originally included an extract of the coca plant, creating a cocaine tonic for tired elderly people. Between 1896 and 1905, Coke was indeed “the real thing.” But no longer. Cocaine is now snorted, injected, or smoked. It enters the bloodstream quickly, producing a rush of euphoria that depletes the brain’s supply of the neurotransmitters dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine (FIGURE 25.4). Within the hour, a crash of agitated depression follows as the drug’s effect wears off. Many regular cocaine users chasing this high become addicted. In the lab, cocaine-addicted monkeys have pressed levers more than 12,000 times to gain one cocaine injection (Siegel, 1990). In situations that trigger aggression, ingesting cocaine may heighten reactions. Caged rats fight when given foot shocks, and they fight even more when given cocaine and foot shocks. Likewise, humans who voluntarily ingest high doses of cocaine in laboratory experiments impose higher shock levels on a presumed opponent than do those receiving a placebo (Licata et al., 1993). Cocaine use may also lead to emotional disturbances, suspiciousness, convulsions, cardiac arrest, or respiratory failure. In national surveys, 3 percent of U.S. high school seniors and 6 percent of British 18- to 24-year-olds reported having tried cocaine during the past year (ACMD, 2009; Johnston et al., 2011). Nearly half had smoked crack, a faster-working crystallized form of cocaine that produces a briefer but more intense high, followed by a more intense crash. After several hours, the craving for more wanes, only to return several days later (Gawin, 1991). Cocaine’s psychological effects depend in part on the dosage and form consumed, but the situation and the user’s expectations and personality also play a role. Given a placebo, cocaine users who thought they were taking cocaine often had a cocaine-like experience (Van Dyke & Byck, 1982). MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 252 Active Learning The United States has a strained relationship with some countries due to cocaine trafficking. In Peru, for example, cultivation of the coca bush is legal, as long as farmers sell their crop to the state. But studies show that farmers can earn much more money selling to illegal markets, making cocaine a problem in the developing world. Some leaders in foreign countries and groups within the United States argue that if the U.S. government cracked down on the demand of people who use drugs, then drug traffickers would not find dealing to be 252 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 252 1/22/14 8:08 AM ENGAGE as profitable, thus ending the drug problem altogether. What should the U.S. government do to curb the drug trade? Have such efforts been successful in the past? Should the U.S. government cut off diplomatic and economic ties with drugtrafficking countries? Why or why not? States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Psychoactive Drugs Module 25 253 ENGAGE Sending neuron Enrichment Action potential Reuptake Synaptic gap Receiving neuron Neurotransmitter molecule Cocaine Receptor sites (a) (b) (c) Neurotransmitters carry a message from a sending neuron across a synapse to receptor sites on a receiving neuron. The sending neuron normally reabsorbs excess neurotransmitter molecules, a process called reuptake. By binding to the sites that normally reabsorb neurotransmitter molecules, cocaine blocks reuptake of dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin (Ray & Ksir, 1990). The extra neurotransmitter molecules therefore remain in the synapse, intensifying their normal moodaltering effects and producing a euphoric rush. When the cocaine level drops, the absence of these neurotransmitters produces a crash. Figure 25.4 Cocaine euphoria and crash ECSTASY National Pictures/Topham/The Image Works Methamphetamine is chemically related to its parent drug, amphetamine (NIDA, 2002, 2005) but has even greater effects. Methamphetamine triggers the release of the neurotransmitter dopamine, which stimulates brain cells that enhance energy and mood, leading to eight hours or so of heightened energy and euphoria. Its aftereffects may include irritability, insomnia, hypertension, seizures, social isolation, depression, and occasional violent outbursts (Homer et al., 2008). Over time, methamphetamine may reduce baseline dopamine levels, leaving the user with depressed functioning. 1/22/14 8:08 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 253 ENGAGE Figure 25.4 is an excellent review of how neurotransmitters work. If there is any part of this that you don’t understand, head back to Module 9 for a complete explanation. Dramatic drug-induced Ecstasy, a street name for MDMA ( m e t hy l e n e d i o x y m e t h a m p h e t amine), is both a stimulant and a mild hallucinogen. As an amphetamine derivative, Ecstasy triggers dopamine release, but its major effect is releasing stored serotonin and blocking its reuptake, thus prolonging serotonin’s feel-good flood (Braun, 2001). Users feel the effect about a half-hour after taking an Ecstasy pill. For three or four hours, they experience high energy, emotional elevation, and (given a social context) connectedness with those around them (“I love everyone”). During the 1990s, Ecstasy’s popularity soared as a “club drug” taken at nightclubs and all-night raves (Landry, 2002). The drug’s popularity crosses national borders, with an estimated 60 million tablets consumed annually in Britain (ACMD, 2009). There are, however, reasons not to be ecstatic about Ecstasy. One is its dehydrating effect, which— when combined with prolonged dancing—can lead to severe overheating, increased TEACH Teaching Tip Reinforce your study of neurotransmitters and action potentials with Figure 25.4, which shows how drugs affect the action at the synapse. You will know here whether students learned the material covered in Unit III. If students can understand this material after having learned about action potentials, they will have successfully transferred their learning from one unit to another. A P ® E x a m Ti p METHAMPHETAMINE Sigmund Freud was a strong advocate of the use of cocaine in his early years. He took the drug himself on a daily basis and advised its use as a treatment for mental and physical disorders (such as asthma), as an aphrodisiac, and as a local anesthetic. Freud was even hired by two pharmaceutical firms, Merck and Parke-Davis, to promote the use of their cocaine-based products. decline This woman’s methamphetamine addiction led to obvious physical changes. Her decline is evident in these two photos, taken at age 36 (left) and, after four years of addiction, at age 40 (right). ENGAGE Active Learning The use of methamphetamine (aka “crystal meth”) has been on the increase in the United States in recent years. Have students conduct research on the prevalence of crystal meth use in their local community. Also have them consider the economic impact that crystal meth use has had on the community. Discuss with them how information about addiction explains the virulent use of meth in some communities. Ecstasy (MDMA) a synthetic stimulant and mild hallucinogen. Produces euphoria and social intimacy, but with short-term health risks and longer-term harm to serotonin-producing neurons and to mood and cognition. 1/15/14 8:47 AM Enrichment Ecstasy, or MDMA, is a highly addictive and dangerous drug. As Myers points out, studies show that taking Ecstasy only once can lead to significant degeneration of serotonin neurons in the brain. This finding seems to be the basis of the popular notion that taking Ecstasy causes “holes in the brain.” While the drug does not produce visible holes, it does cause significant damage, which is about as close to making holes in the brain as you can get. Psychoactive Drugs Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 253 Module 25 22 253 2/20/14 8:13 AM 254 Unit V States of Consciousness Bill Greenblatt UPI Photo Service/Newscom Meth bust As use of the dangerously addictive stimulant methamphetamine has increased, enforcement agencies have increased their efforts to snuff out the labs that produce it. blood pressure, and death. Another is that long-term, repeated leaching of brain serotonin can damage serotonin-producing neurons, leading to decreased output and increased risk of permanently depressed mood (Croft et al., 2001; McCann et al., 2001; Roiser et al., 2005). Ecstasy also suppresses the disease-fighting immune system, impairs memory, slows thought, and disrupts sleep by interfering with serotonin’s control of the circadian clock (Laws & Kokkalis, 2007; Pacifici et al., 2001; Schilt et al., 2007). Ecstasy delights for the night but dispirits the morrow. ENGAGE Hallucinogens Enrichment 25-4 Flashbacks are associated with LSD use. These experiences can occur days, months, or years after an initial “trip.” Flashbacks can be as intense as the original trip, but because of their unpredictable nature, they are often terrifying for the user. hallucinogens psychedelic (“mind-manifesting”) drugs, such as LSD, that distort perceptions and evoke sensory images in the absence of sensory input. LSD a powerful hallucinogenic drug; also known as acid (lysergic acid diethylamide). MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 254 254 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 254 What are hallucinogens, and what are their effects? Hallucinogens distort perceptions and evoke sensory images in the absence of sensory input (which is why these drugs are also called psychedelics, meaning “mind-manifesting”). Some, such as LSD and MDMA (Ecstasy), are synthetic. Others, including the mild hallucinogen marijuana, are natural substances. LSD Chemist Albert Hofmann created—and on one Friday afternoon in April 1943 accidentally ingested—LSD (lysergic acid diethylamide). The result—“an uninterrupted stream of fantastic pictures, extraordinary shapes with intense, kaleidoscopic play of colors”—reminded him of a childhood mystical experience that had left him longing for another glimpse of “a miraculous, powerful, unfathomable reality” (Siegel, 1984; Smith, 2006). The emotions of an LSD trip vary from euphoria to detachment to panic. The user’s current mood and expectations color the emotional experience, but the perceptual distortions and hallucinations have some commonalities. Whether provoked to hallucinate by drugs, loss of oxygen, or extreme sensory deprivation, the brain hallucinates in basically the same way (Siegel, 1982). The experience typically begins with simple geometric forms, such as a lattice, cobweb, or spiral. The next phase consists of more meaningful images; some may be superimposed on a tunnel or funnel, others may be replays of past emotional experiences. As the hallucination peaks, people frequently feel separated from their body and experience dreamlike scenes so real that they may become panic-stricken or harm themselves. 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:13 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Psychoactive Drugs Module 25 255 Figure 25.5 Near-death vision or hallucination? Psychologist These sensations are strikingly similar to the near-death experience, an altered state of consciousness reported by about 15 percent of patients revived from cardiac arrest (Agrillo, 2011; Greyson, 2010). Many describe visions of tunnels (FIGURE 25.5), bright lights or beings of light, a replay of old memories, and out-of-body sensations (Siegel, 1980). Given that oxygen deprivation and other insults to the brain are known to produce hallucinations, it is difficult to resist wondering whether a brain under stress manufactures the near-death experience. Following temporal lobe seizures, patients have reported similarly profound mystical experiences. So have solitary sailors and polar explorers while enduring monotony, isolation, and cold (Suedfeld & Mocellin, 1987). ENGAGE Ronald Siegel (1977) reported that people under the influence of hallucinogenic drugs often see “a bright light in the center of the field of vision. . . . The location of this point of light create[s] a tunnel-like perspective.” This is very similar to others’ near-death experiences. MARIJUANA For 5000 years, hemp has been cultivated for its fiber. The leaves and flowers of this plant, which are sold as marijuana, contain THC (delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol). Whether smoked (getting to the brain in about 7 seconds) or eaten (causing its peak concentration to be reached at a slower, unpredictable rate), THC produces a mix of effects. Synthetic marijuana (also called K2 or Spice) mimics THC. Its harmful side effects, which can include agitation and hallucinations, led to its ingredient becoming illegal under the U.S. Synthetic Drug Abuse Prevention Act of 2012. Marijuana is a difficult drug to classify. It is a mild hallucinogen, amplifying sensitivity to colors, sounds, tastes, and smells. But like alcohol, marijuana relaxes, disinhibits, and may produce a euphoric high. Both alcohol and marijuana impair the motor coordination, perceptual skills, and reaction time necessary for safely operating an automobile or other machine. “THC causes animals to misjudge events,” reported Ronald Siegel (1990, p. 163). “Pigeons wait too long to respond to buzzers or lights that tell them food is available for brief periods; and rats turn the wrong way in mazes.” Marijuana and alcohol also differ. The body eliminates alcohol within hours. THC and its by-products linger in the body for a week or more, which means that regular users experience less abrupt withdrawal and may achieve a high with smaller amounts of the drug than would be needed by occasional users. This is contrary to the usual path of tolerance, in which repeat users need to take larger doses to feel the same effect. A user’s experience can vary with the situation. If the person feels anxious or depressed, using marijuana may intensify these feelings. The more often the person uses marijuana, especially during adolescence and in today’s stronger, purified form, the greater the risk of anxiety or depression (Bambico et al., 2010; Hall, 2006; Murray et al., 2007). Daily use bodes a worse outcome than infrequent use. Marijuana also disrupts memory formation and interferes with immediate recall of information learned only a few minutes before. Such cognitive effects outlast the period of smoking (Messinis et al., 2006). Heavy adult use for over 20 years is associated with a shrinkage of brain areas that process memories and emotions (Yücel et al., 2008). Prenatal exposure through maternal marijuana use impairs brain development (Berghuis et al., 2007; Huizink & Mulder, 2006). To free up resources to fight crime, some states and countries have passed laws legalizing the possession of small quantities of marijuana. In some cases, legal medical marijuana use has been granted to relieve the pain and nausea associated with diseases such 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 255 near-death experience an altered state of consciousness reported after a close brush with death (such as by cardiac arrest); often similar to drug-induced hallucinations. Enrichment Students may have heard before that marijuana is considered to be a “gateway” drug, which means that it leads users to try harder drugs like heroin. A study of 311 sets of identical and fraternal twins in Australia found that those teens who used marijuana early (before age 17) were 2–5 times more likely to use harder drugs later in life. In each of these sets of twins, one twin was a user while the other was not, helping to control for confounding factors like genetics and environment. ENGAGE THC the major active ingredient in marijuana; triggers a variety of effects, including mild hallucinations. Enrichment Marijuana smokers typically take longer drags on a marijuana cigarette than on a traditional cigarette. Marijuana cigarettes are not usually filtered, as most tobacco cigarettes are. ENGAGE Enrichment A study released by the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) showed that teen marijuana use increased by about 20 percent during the last half of the 1990s. In 1994, 43 percent of teens treated for substance abuse used marijuana, but in 1999, that number was 60 percent. 1/15/14 8:47 AM Psychoactive Drugs Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 255 Module 25 22 255 2/20/14 8:13 AM 256 Unit V States of Consciousness as AIDS, glaucoma, and cancer (Munsey, 2010; Watson et al., 2000). In such cases, the Institute of Medicine recommends delivering the THC with medical inhalers. Marijuana smoke, like cigarette smoke, is toxic and can cause cancer, lung damage, and pregnancy complications. ENGAGE Active Learning Cancer and AIDS advocacy groups have been pushing for the legalization of marijuana for medicinal purposes. Marijuana has been shown to help curb the nausea associated with chemotherapy for cancer and AIDS, helping patients to keep up their strength as they endure uncomfortable treatments. Explore laws across the United States that address the medical use of marijuana. How many states authorize the medical use of marijuana? How does the federal government handle cases of people authorized to use marijuana medicinally? *** Despite their differences, the psychoactive drugs summarized in TABLE 25.2 share a common feature: They trigger negative aftereffects that offset their immediate positive effects and grow stronger with repetition. And this helps explain both tolerance and withdrawal. As the opposing, negative aftereffects grow stronger, it takes larger and larger doses to produce the desired high (tolerance), causing the aftereffects to worsen in the drug’s absence (withdrawal). This in turn creates a need to switch off the withdrawal symptoms by taking yet more of the drug (which may lead to addiction). Table 25.2 A Guide to Selected Psychoactive Drugs Does the federal government supply the drug to people authorized to use marijuana medically? Why or why not? Drug Type Pleasurable Effects Adverse Effects Alcohol Depressant Initial high followed by relaxation and disinhibition Depression, memory loss, organ damage, impaired reactions Heroin Depressant Rush of euphoria, relief from pain Depressed physiology, agonizing withdrawal Caffeine Stimulant Increased alertness and wakefulness Anxiety, restlessness, and insomnia in high doses; uncomfortable withdrawal Methamphetamine Stimulant Euphoria, alertness, energy Irritability, insomnia, hypertension, seizures Cocaine Stimulant Rush of euphoria, confidence, energy Cardiovascular stress, suspiciousness, depressive crash Nicotine Stimulant Arousal and relaxation, sense of well-being Heart disease, cancer Ecstasy (MDMA) Stimulant; mild hallucinogen Emotional elevation, disinhibition Dehydration, overheating, depressed mood, impaired cognitive and immune functioning Marijuana Mild hallucinogen Enhanced sensation, relief of pain, distortion of time, relaxation Impaired learning and memory, increased risk of psychological disorders, lung damage from smoke To learn about the influences on drug use, see Module 81. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 256 256 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 256 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:14 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Psychoactive Drugs Module 25 257 Before You Move On CLOSE & ASSESS 䉴 ASK YOURSELF Exit Assessment Do you think people can become addicted not only to psychoactive drugs but also to other repetitive, pleasure-seeking behaviors (such as gambling or “Internet game playing”)? Use Table 25.2 as an assessment tool. Have students fill in important details from this chart from memory. You can use this exercise to assess their knowledge of psychoactive drugs. 䉴 TEST YOURSELF Why do tobacco companies try so hard to get customers hooked as teens? Answers to the Test Yourself questions can be found in Appendix E at the end of the book. Module 25 Review 25-1 What are stimulants, and what are their effects? Stimulants—including caffeine, nicotine, cocaine, the amphetamines, methamphetamine, and Ecstasy—excite neural activity and speed up body functions, triggering energy and mood changes. All are highly addictive. Psychoactive drugs alter perceptions and moods. • These drugs may produce tolerance—requiring larger doses to achieve the desired effect—and withdrawal— significant discomfort accompanying efforts to quit. Nicotine’s effects make smoking a difficult habit to kick, yet the percentage of Americans who smoke has been dramatically decreasing. • Addiction is compulsive craving and use of drugs or certain behaviors (such as gambling) despite known adverse consequences. Cocaine gives users a fast high, followed within an hour by a crash. Its risks include cardiovascular stress and suspiciousness. • Use of methamphetamines may permanently reduce dopamine production. • Ecstasy (MDMA) is a combined stimulant and mild hallucinogen that produces euphoria and feelings of intimacy. Its users risk immune system suppression, permanent damage to mood and memory, and (if taken during physical activity) dehydration and escalating body temperatures. Those with a substance use disorder may exhibit impaired control, social disruption, risky behavior, and the physical effects of tolerance and withdrawal. • • • What are depressants, and what are their effects? • Depressants, such as alcohol, barbiturates, and the opiates (which include narcotics), dampen neural activity and slow body functions. • Alcohol tends to disinhibit, increasing the likelihood that we will act on our impulses, whether harmful or helpful. It also impairs judgment, disrupts memory processes by suppressing REM sleep, and reduces self-awareness and self-control. • 25-3 • • 25-2 1/15/14 8:47 AM What are substance use disorders, and what role do tolerance, withdrawal, and addiction play in these disorders? 25-4 • Hallucinogens—such as LSD and marijuana—distort perceptions and evoke hallucinations—sensory images in the absence of sensory input. The user’s mood and expectations influence the effects of LSD, but common experiences are hallucinations and emotions varying from euphoria to panic. • Marijuana’s main ingredient, THC, may trigger feelings of disinhibition, euphoria, relaxation, relief from pain, and intense sensitivity to sensory stimuli. It may also increase feelings of depression or anxiety, impair motor coordination and reaction time, disrupt memory formation, and damage lung tissue (because of the inhaled smoke). User expectations strongly influence alcohol’s behavioral effects. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 257 What are hallucinogens, and what are their effects? 1/15/14 8:47 AM Psychoactive Drugs Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 257 Module 25 22 257 2/20/14 8:14 AM Unit V States of Consciousness 258 Answers to Multiple-Choice Questions 1. d 2. c Multiple-Choice Questions 1. Which of the following represents drug tolerance? a. Hans has grown to accept the fact that his wife likes to have a beer with her dinner, even though he personally does not approve of the use of alcohol. b. Jose often wakes up with a headache that lasts until he has his morning cup of coffee. c. Pierre enjoys the effect of marijuana and is now using the drug several times a week. d. Jacob had to increase the dosage of his pain medication when the old dosage no longer effectively controlled the pain from his chronic back condition. e. Chau lost his job and is now homeless as a result of his drug use. 3. e 4. d 2. Which of the following drugs is classified as an opiate? a. b. c. d. e. Answer to Practice FRQ 2 2 points: Alcohol is classified as a depressant. Effects include reduced self-awareness. 2 points: Caffeine is classified as a stimulant. Effects include impaired sleep. Nicotine Marijuana Heroin Methamphetamine Cocaine 3. Which of the following drugs produces effects similar to a near-death experience? a. b. c. d. e. Ecstasy Nicotine Barbiturate Methamphetamine LSD 4. Which of the following statements is true of alcohol? a. Alcohol is a stimulant because it produces insomnia. b. Alcohol is a depressant because it produces bipolar disorder. c. Alcohol is a stimulant because people do foolish things while under its influence. d. Alcohol is a depressant because it calms neural activity and slows body function. e. Alcohol is a stimulant because it increases instances of casual sex. Practice FRQs 1. Name and compare the effects of the two hallucinogens discussed in the text. Answer 1 point: LSD creates vivid hallucinations and strong emotions. 2. Three of the most widely used psychoactive drugs— alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine—are legal for large segments of the population. Name the category that each of these drugs belongs to, and describe one effect of each. (6 points) 1 point: Marijuana creates mild hallucinations, enhanced sensory experiences, and impaired judgment. 2 points: Nicotine is classified as a stimulant. Effects include diminished appetite. MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 258 258 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 258 1/15/14 8:47 AM States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:14 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Review Unit V 259 Unit V Review Key Terms and Concepts to Remember consciousness, p. 219 narcolepsy, p. 238 barbiturates, p. 250 hypnosis, p. 219 sleep apnea, p. 239 opiates, p. 250 posthypnotic suggestion, p. 220 night terrors, p. 239 stimulants, p. 250 dissociation, p. 222 dream, p. 240 amphetamines, p. 250 circadian [ser-KAY-dee-an] rhythm, p. 226 manifest content, p. 241 nicotine, p. 250 latent content, p. 241 cocaine, p. 252 REM sleep, p. 226 REM rebound, p. 243 methamphetamine, p. 253 alpha waves, p. 227 substance use disorder, p. 246 Ecstasy (MDMA), p. 253 sleep, p. 227 psychoactive drug, p. 246 hallucinogens, p. 254 hallucinations, p. 228 tolerance, p. 246 LSD, p. 254 delta waves, p. 228 addiction, p. 247 near-death experience, p. 255 NREM sleep, p. 228 withdrawal, p. 247 THC, p. 255 suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), p. 229 depressants, p. 248 insomnia, p. 238 alcohol use disorder, p. 249 Key Contributors to Remember William James, p. 219 Ernest Hilgard, p. 222 Sigmund Freud, p. 241 AP® Exam Practice Questions Answers to Multiple-Choice Questions Multiple-Choice Questions 1. Sudden sleep attacks at inopportune times are symptomatic of which sleep disorder? a. b. c. d. e. Sleep apnea Insomnia Night terrors Sleepwalking Narcolepsy 2. Deep sleep occurs in which stage? a. b. c. d. e. 1/15/14 8:47 AM Hypnagogic REM Alpha NREM-1 Delta MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 259 3. Recurring problems in falling asleep or staying asleep are characteristic of which sleep disorder? a. b. c. d. e. 1. e 2. e 3. c 4. b Sleep apnea Narcolepsy Insomnia Sleep talking Sleepwalking 4. What is the pineal gland’s role in sleep? a. b. c. d. e. Activating the suprachiasmatic nucleus The production of melatonin The location of hypnagogic images Remembering dreams upon waking Emitting alpha waves 1/15/14 8:47 AM Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis Review Module Unit22 V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 259 259 2/20/14 8:14 AM 260 5. 6. 7. 8. d e b a 9. 10. 11. 12. a c b d 13. e 14. d 15. b Rubric for Free-Response Question 2 1 point: Posthypnotic suggestions have been used by therapists to help people overcome health issues. Patients typically don’t remember these suggestions that are made during a hypnotic state, but such suggestions may influence a patient’s behavior after the hypnosis session. These deliberate posthypnotic suggestions are unique to hypnosis and contradict Phil’s claim that dreams and hypnosis are equivalent states of consciousness. pp. 220–221 1 point: Some physiological studies indicate that hypnotic states are associated with unique patterns of activation in the brain. If brain scans indicate specific patterns unique to hypnotic states that are different from those associated with dreaming or others states of consciousness, Phil’s claim may not be accurate. pp. 221–222 1 point: Social influence theory explains the impact of hypnosis through the powerful social pressures experienced by people being hypnotized. Some studies show that people pretending to be hypnotized and people who are “really” hypnotized behave in similar ways, indicating that hypnosis may not lead to a “different state of consciousness,” as claimed by Phil. pp. 221–222 Unit V States of Consciousness 5. What are bursts of rapid, rhythmic brain-wave activity that occur during NREM-2 sleep? a. b. c. d. e. Hallucinations Circadian rhythms Alpha waves Sleep spindles Delta waves 6. Increasing amounts of paradoxical sleep following a period of sleep deprivation is known as what? a. b. c. d. e. Circadian sleep Sleep shifting Narcolepsy Sleep apnea REM rebound 7. Which of these drugs, which acts as both a stimulant and a hallucinogen, can also cause dangerous dehydration? a. b. c. d. e. LSD Ecstasy Alcohol Cocaine Caffeine 8. Recent research most consistently supports the effectiveness of hypnosis in which of the following areas? a. b. c. d. e. Pain relief Recovery of lost memories Reduction of sleep deprivation Forcing people to act against their will Cessation of smoking 9. What are the three major categories of drugs? a. b. c. d. e. Hallucinogens, depressants, and stimulants Stimulants, barbiturates, and hallucinogens Amphetamines, barbiturates, and opiates MDMA, LSD, and THC Alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine 10. Jarod’s muscles are relaxed, his body is basically paralyzed, and he is hard to awaken. Which sleep state is Jarod probably experiencing? a. b. c. d. e. 11. The effects of opiates are similar to the effects of which neurotransmitter? a. b. c. d. e. Barbiturates Endorphins Tranquilizers Nembutal Acetylcholine 12. Slowed reactions, slurred speech, and decreased skill performance are associated with abuse of which drug? a. b. c. d. e. Nicotine Methamphetamine Caffeine Alcohol Ecstasy 13. What term did Ernest Hilgard use to describe a split between different levels of consciousness? a. b. c. d. e. Hypnagogic imagery REM sleep Delta waves Spindles Dissociation 14. Psychologists who study the brain’s activity during sleep are most likely to use which of these technologies? a. b. d. d. e. MRI CT scan PET scan EEG EKG 15. What term describes the brain’s adaptation to a drug’s chemistry, requiring larger and larger doses to experience the same effect? a. b. c. d. e. Withdrawal Tolerance Addiction Substance use disorder Disinhibiting Sleep apnea Hypnagogic Paradoxical Delta Sleep deprivation MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 260 1/15/14 8:47 AM 1 point: Hilgard’s studies in support of the divided-consciousness theory indicate that dissociation may occur during hypnosis. If there is a “split” between different levels of consciousness, and one level may be aware of information that another level is ignorant of, Phil’s claim that hypnosis is similar to dreaming needs to be modified. p. 222 260 Unit V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 260 States of Consciousness 2/20/14 8:14 AM MyersAP_SE_2e Review Unit V 261 Free-Response Questions 1. Different biological changes are associated with different states of consciousness. Explain the biological changes (if any) typically associated with the following consciousness-related concepts: • Sleep deprivation • REM • Tolerance • Opiates his roommate, Phil. Phil says: “I can’t believe so many people fall for that hypnosis stuff. Hypnosis is just like dreaming. It’s just a different state of consciousness, and a dream can affect someone just like a supposed hypnotic state can.” • Posthypnotic suggestion • Divided-consciousness theory 1 point: Sleep deprivation causes a wide range of biological changes in the body, all associated with decreased performance while awake. These biological changes include lack of energy, falling asleep during the day, changes in appetite, suppression of the immune system, decreased focus and attention, and depressed mood. Pages 234–237 • Social influence theory 1 point: After repeated use of some drugs, humans develop tolerance for those substances, meaning that increasing dosages of those drugs are needed to produce the same effect. Tolerance occurs because of biological changes in the brain. The brain’s chemistry changes when some psychoactive drugs are repeatedly ingested, interfering with the brain’s ability to produce or use some neurotransmitters. Pages 246–247 1 point: Drugs categorized as opiates cause a range of biological changes in the body. Some of the changes mentioned in the text are: pupil constriction, slower breathing, lethargy, and eventually, painful withdrawal symptoms as the brain Page 250 loses its ability to produce “natural” endorphins. 1 point: Students should establish a definition that includes the idea that consciousness is related to awareness of our internal and external environments. pp. 218–219 Explain how Ernest might use the following terms as he discusses the validity of Phil’s claims. Rubric for Free Response Question 1 1 point: The REM stage of the sleep cycle is associated with dramatic biological changes. Brain waves and breathing become irregular, heart rate increases, and eyes dart back and forth beneath the eyelids. Pages 228–229 Rubric for Free-Response Question 3 2. Ernest, a psychology major, is discussing hypnosis with 1 point: Hypnosis relates to this definition because hypnotic states can influence awareness of both environments. Hypnotized individuals can be given suggestions that lead them to forget events that occurred while hypnotized, indicating a loss of awareness of the environment during hypnosis. pp. 219–222 • Dissociation (4 points) 3. Consciousness has been defined and studied differently throughout the history of psychology. In your own words, explain how modern psychologists define consciousness, and explain how the following “altered” states of consciousness relate to your definition. • Hypnosis • Sleep stages • Dreams 1 point: As we pass through the stages of sleep, we become less and less aware of our outside environment. During deeper stages of sleep, we are less likely to be awakened by noise in our environment, indicating a change in consciousness according to this definition. pp. 226–229 • Psychoactive drugs (5 points) Multiple-choice self-tests and more may be found at www.worthpublishers.com/MyersAP2e 1 point: Environmental stimuli are often incorporated into dreams, indicating that we are partially aware of our outside environment even during this sleep stage. pp. 240–241 1/15/14 8:47 AM MyersAP_SE_2e_Mod25_B.indd 261 1/15/14 8:47 AM 1 point: Psychoactive drugs by definition alter our perceptions of the world. These altered perceptions influence our awareness of the external and internal worlds. Changes in perception may influence us, causing us to ignore some environmental stimuli, react powerfully to others, or even react to stimuli that we hallucinate because of the influence of the drugs. Module 25 Understanding Consciousness and Hypnosis Review Module Unit22 V MyersPsyAP_TE_2e_U05.indd 261 261 2/20/14 8:14 AM