RELATIONAL DISENGAGEMENT STRATEGIES AND CONSEQUENCES by DEBORAH LEIGH PERRY, B.A. A THESIS IN SPEECH COMMUNICATION Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Texas Tech University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF ARTS Approved Accepted '^í'•2f h\^. :";^ ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am very grateful to Dr. Michael J. Cody for his assistance and guidance in the direction of this thesis and to other members of my committee, Dr. Margaret L. McLaughlin-Cody and Dr. William J. Jordan. I would like to give special thanks to my parents, John and Charlotte Perry for their continued love and support throughout this project. 11 TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ii * LIST OF TABLES I. v INTRODUCTION 1 Statement of Purpose 1 Review of Literature 2 Social Exchange Theory 2 Determinants of Disengagement 6 Disengagement Strategies 9 Typology of Strategies Strategy Selection 9 12 Trust 16 Dyadic Adjustment 18 Consequences of Relational Disengagement Hypotheses 11. III. METHODOLOGY 19 24 26 Subjects 26 Materials 26 RESULTS 29 Factor Analysis of Strategies 29 Interrelationships Among Variables 31 Hypotheses 38 Multiple Regression Analyses 43 111 TABLE OF CONTENTS cont'd IV. DISCUSSION 45 Strategy Selection 45 Consequences of Disengagement 48 Conclusion 51 REFERENCES 55 APPENDIX 58 IV LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Factor Analysis Solution 30 2. Correlations Between All Variables 32 3. Factor Analysis Solution for Intimacy, Constrained, Unwillingness to Compromise and Trust 35 4. Correlations Between Relational Variables, Strategies and Consequences of Disengagement ... 39 V CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Statement of Purpose While the initiation and development of pair relationships has long been a topic of interest in interpersonal communication, much less attention has been given to the dissolution of pair relationships. It has been argued that disengagements should be studied in order to understand the • complete life cycle of relationships (Baxter, 1979) . A considerable body of research exists concerning marital separation and divorce, but, as Hill, Rubin and Peplau (1976) point out, "for every recorded instance of the ending of a marriage, there are many instances of the ending of a pre-marital relationship" (p. 148). Despite the pervasiveness of pre-marital breakups, exploration into the disengagement of friendship dyads and dating couples has only recently begun to emerge. To date, the research has examined various factors associated with the precipitating causes of a breakup, the communication strategies used in the process of breaking up and the effects of the breakup on the individuals involved. some speculative comparisons have been made relating pre-marital breakups with marital breakups. 1 Also, The purpose of the present paper is to extend the work of previous research in an effort to understand more fully the disengagement of relationships. Utilizing a social exchange perspective, a review of literature will be presented which focuses on factors associated with which partner disengages and how disengagement is achieved. In an attempt to build upon the research base, additional variables which have remained unexplored will be discussed and hypotheses will then be proposed. Review of Literature Social Exchange Theory Within social exchange theory, human interaction is viewed as a voluntary exchange of mutually rewarding objects or activities. Levinger (1979a) notes that although critics claim this approach is too materialistic to apply to close interpersonal relationships, social exchange theory is indeed useful in examining phenomena that occur in close relationships because exchanges can be interpreted in either concrete or symbolic terms. Therefore, abstract concepts such as love and affection are not discounted, but reinterpreted within an exchange orientation. Five perspectives of social exchange theory are reviewed in Roloff (1981): Homans• operant psychology approach (1974); Blau's economic approach (1964); Thibaut and Kelley's theory of interdependence (1978); Foa and Foa's resource theory (1974); and Walster, Berscheid and Walster's equity theory (1978). Though each perspective approaches social exchange in a slightly different manner, there is agreement concerning the major constructs. The basic constructs of social exchange theory are rewards, costs, comparison level, comparison level for alternatives, distributive justice and reciprocity. The first important concept is that of resources (rewards and costs) which constitute what is exchanged. Foa and Foa (1974) posit six types of resources: love, status, services, goods, information and money. Love involves the expression of affection and warmth and is considered a particularistic resource as the value attached to love is largely dependent upon the particular person who provides it. communication of regard or esteem. Status is the Services and goods are more concrete resources since they usually deal with observable exchanges such as fixing someone's car or delivering a newspaper. Information takes the form of advice, opinions or instructions. Money is coin or currency assigned a standard value within a social system. Since it is unaffected by the person who provides it, money is considered a universal resource (Roloff, 1981). Resources are not equally valued: certain resources over others. some people desire Preferred resources may be thought of as rewards or "the pleasures, satisfactions and gratifications a person enjoys" (Thibaut and Kelley, 1959, p. 12). Also, the value a person attributes to a resource may vary. For example, if an individual has accumulated a great deal of one resource, it will become less valued (Roloff, 1981). Blau (1964) posits that some rewards may emerge spontaneously such as personal attraction, social approval and respect, while others are open to negotiation such as acceptance into a group, instrumental services and compliance. These types of social rewards may be distinguished by whether they are intrinsic or external to the relationship. Extrinsic rewards (personal attraction, social approval and instrumental services), though conveyed within the relationships exist independent of the relationship. Respect and compliance are internal, unilateral rewards in that when one complies with another, it is implicit that superiority is granted to one at the expense of the other's power. When an individual loses or is denied a preferred resource, it is considered a cost. A cost may be incurred as a function of foregoing rewards available elsewhere, or as the result of receiving an aversive stimulation (Roloff 1981) . Supposedly, a rewarding relationship will continue A costly relationship will eventually be terminated. The comparison level refers to one's subjective standard of satisfaction or to the attraction that the relationship holds for a member in terms of the rewards and costs a person feels should be received. Attraction to the relationship is determined by comparing outcomes of the current relationship with the comparison level which may represent a level of relational rewards received in previous associations. Comparison level for alternatives represents the current outcomes compared to outcomes expected from an available alternative. The comparison level for alternatives indicates a stability component within the relationship (Kelley and Thibaut, 1978; Levinger. 1979a; Roloff, 1981). By utilizing the constructs of comparison level and comparison level for alternatives, an idea as to how stable and attractive a relationship is can be obtained. According to Levinger (1979a), when a relationship's level of outcomes is below the comparison level of one member and below outcomes available elsewhere (comparison level for alternatives), the relationship is in a state of unattractive instability. The relationship is therefore perceived as a costly one and will probably be terminated. Distributive justice and reciprocity are both norms which serve to equalize outcomes and ensure fair exchange. It is assumed that participants within a relationship attempt to achieve equity or balance. If one member violates the norm of fair exchange, social disapproval may be expected. Within the social exchange perspective, the declining attraction of the present relationship, the rising attraction of alternative relationships and the erosion of normative restraints which serve to contain the relationship are viewed as probable determinants of pair dissolution. Determinants of Disengagement Simpson (1981a) synthesized social exchange theory with symbolic interactionism to create a framework for examining the development and/or decay of intimate relationships. Symbolic interactionism focuses on the analysis of shared meaning and the role of social interactions in the development of self (Kimmel, 1979; Simpson, 1981a). The integration of social exchange theory and symbolic interactionism—symbolic exchange—has as a central concern the exchange of symbolic meaning in the development of a "relationship world view." The relationship world view consists of a couple's common assumptions of life and perceptions of the degree of importance these assumptions hold for the couple. The extent to which a couple has constructed a shared view of the world is the level of symbolic interdependence. The relationship world view is especially applicable to highly intimate couples and married partners who have supposedly spent a great deal of time developing shared assumptions and values. Simpson 7 (1981a) found a couple's level of symbolic interdependence to be correlated positively with the degree of commitment and the level of satisfaction. In a study of pre-marital breakups, Simpson (1981a) used the concept of symbolic interdependence as a predictor of relational disengagement. His findings supported the prediction that couples with high levels of symbolic interdependence would be less likely to break up. However, the correlation was only -.13, and further analyses revealed that the portion of variance accounted for by symbolic interdependence was also accounted for by other predictors such as relational commitment and satisfaction. Many of the subjects had only dated for a short time (one week), which may explain the low correlation. Inherent in the concept of relationship world view is the fact that it takes time to develop. Numerous studies examining marital disruption have considered the level of relationship satisfaction and/or commitment as determinants of pair progress or dissolution (Fitzpatrick and Winke, 1979; Levinger, 1979b; Snyder, 1979). However, the major emphasis has been on how certain variables affect satisfaction under the assumption that satisfaction is the primary predictor of relational change. Particular concern has been given to the effects of children and child-rearing practices on satisfaction (Miller, 19^6; 8 Ryder, 1973; Thornton, 1977). These attempts to determine the underlying dimensions of marital satisfaction are problematic because global measures of marital satisfaction seem to lack reliability, appropriateness and validity (Snyder, 1979). Despite the methodological problems, researchers agree that the role of satisfaction in the progress and/or decay of relationships is extremely important. Hill, Rubin and Peplau (1976) conducted a longitudinal investigation of breakups before marriage. This study represents a first attempt in examining relational disengagement of dating couples rather than married couples. The authors argue that an understanding of pre-marital breakups would greatly facilitate the understanding of divorce. Despite the different social contexts in which the two events occur, the psychological bonds of attachment created in intimate dating couples resemble the bonds of attachment within married dyads and "thus, the requirements and difficulties of 'uncoupling' in the two cases may show similarities" (Hill et a_l. , 1976, p. 148). The results of H i U et aj,. (1976) revealed several factors associated with breakups before marriage. Included were unequal involvement in the relationship, geographic vicinity, pressure from parents, boredom and the desire for independence. Other variables correlated with breaking up were differences in intelligence, backgrounds and interests, conflicting ideas about marriage and sex, and age discrepancy. These factors can be considered costs within social exchange theory. For example, if one's relational partner lives sixty miles away, the cost of driving the distance to see the partner may outweigh the pleasure derived from the partner's company. If a comparable alternative is available without the cost of distance, the relationship is likely to be terminated. Disengagement Strategies Recently, there has em.erged an interest in studying the actual process of disengagement. The communication strategies and variables that affect the use of these strategies have been the focus of investigations. Further, while Hill et a_l. (1976) reported some findings concerning the consequences of thé parties of the breakup (e.g., who stayed friends), it is clear that how people break up influences subsequent feelings and orientations. Typology of strategies. Baxter (1979b) developed a typology of disengagement strategies in a study involving responses indicating the likelihood that subjects would use 35 strategies. Four primary clusters emerged: withdraw/ avoidance (strategy of indirectness), Machiavellian (intentional manipulation of the other), positive tone (concern for the other's feelings) and openness (concern with honesty and openness of communication). 10 In another study, Baxter and Philpott (1980) examined disengagement as a process rather than an event which occurs at a single point in time. Subjects were asked to complete a hypothetical story concerning the ending of a relationship by providing step-by-step dynamics of what occurred between the interactants. Results suggest a pattern of tactics which oscillate between approach/avoidance extremes. For example, a disengager might express verbally his/her desire to end the relationship, but the other party is unwilling to let go making further encounters necessary to complete the disengagement. In this case, Baxter and Philpott (1980) reported a tendency for the disengager to avoid further interactions. Likewise, when the disengager initially withdrew, the other party's reactions of hurt, anger or confusion usually forced the disengager to confront the other party. Baxter (1979a) also examined the verbal strategy of self-disclosure as a potential disengagement strategy. The findings showed an avoidance of discussion of the state of the relationship and an avoidance of direct confrontation. Furthermore, as hypothesized, an effort tc disengage was accompanied by less willingness to disclose on the part of the disengager. Another typology of disengagement strategies was developed by Cody (1981). Whereas Baxter (1979b) focused on the disengagement of friendship dyads, Cody (1981) developei a typology generalizable to more intimate dyads, reasoning 11 that withdraw/avoidance and Machiavellian tactics were less likely to be used in achieving disengagement from romantic partnerships. In such a case, "the disengager must at least recognize that the partner has the right to request an accounting of changes in the disengager's behavior and that she/he is obligated to give some type of account" (Cody, 1981, p. 2 ) . The development of emotional attachments, the disclosing of intimate information, the propensity to make long term plans and the willingness to sacrifice personal goals for the good of the relationship are all likely to increase as intimacy between two people increases. Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that "disengagers will feel obligated to report verbally their disengagement intentions" (Cody, 1981, p. 11). The five strategies reported by Cody (1981) were: (1) behavioral de-escalation (withdraw/avoidance); (2) de-escalation (possible reconciliation, relationship fault); (3) positive tone (expressed caring, grief about disengagement and desire to be fair); (4) negative identity management (failure to attend to the partner's needs of rejection) and (5) justification (provided reasons and implied consequences). In conjunction with Baxter (1979b), these five strategies are generalizable from less intimate to very intimate relationships. 12 Strategy selection. In an effort to determine what variables might affect the choice of certain disengagement strategies, Baxter (1979b) investigated the role of relationship closeness (friendship/very close friendship) and relational intention (partial versus total disengagement). The findings showed that very close friends were less likely to choose withdraw/avoidance and Machiavellian strategies of disengagement than friends, and the results suggest a tendency for very close friends to be more concerned with the other, presumably because people in more intimate relationships are more likely to feel an obligation to explain their actions to intimates. The effect of relational intention was not significant. Communicator age and sex role orientation also affect the choice of strategy (Baxter, 1981). Fifth graders and adults prefer strategies of confrontation (explicit declaration of the intent to disengage) as opposed to avoidance tactics (reduced interaction). The basis for this result is that children "have a relative lack of social perspective-taking skills" (Baxter, 1981, p. 5) and are not likely to anticipate the reaction of the other; thus, children are less motivated to avoid confrontation. Adults select confrontation tactics because of their broader social experience and their capability of "realizing the delayed and hidden costs afforded by an initial avoidance tactic" (Baxter, 1981, p. 6). A social desirability factor may also 13 explain these results. It is more socially acceptable to show concern to others; therefore, people may prefer to indicate on questionnaires the use of more direct tactics of disengagement. Furthermore, subjects were imagining a disengagement situation, rather than recalling an actual event, which might not reflect actual strategy choice. Confrontation can be viewed as a masculine tactic because initiative taking and assertiveness are stereotypically masculine traits. However, confrontation is also characterized by concern for the other and attentiveness to the needs of the other which are stereotypically feminine traits. Therefore, androgynous persons, who are equally comfortable with masculine or feminine behaviors, demonstrate more preference for confrontation strategies than either masculine sex-typed or feminine sex-typed persons (Baxter, 1981). Cody (1981) explored the relationship between intimacy, reactions to inequity and factors of relational problems with the selection of disengagement strategies. Intimacy correlated positively with the use of justification, de-escalation and positive tone tactics, and negatively with the use of behavioral de-escalation, indicating that the use of behavioral de-escalation is not often employed between intimates. High levels of intimacy imply that emotional bonds have formed. Therefore, the use of 14 behavioral de-escalation would not be acceptable. Again, the disengager probably feels obligated to give a verbal account of intent. The perceptions of inequity in a relationship deal with disparities in investments between partners. The "under- benefited" partner receives fewer gains from the relationshlp and is more likely to feel angry when realizing that the partner does not reciprocate at the same level of intensity. The emotional reaction of anger to perceived inequity predicted the selection of justification and behavioral de-escalation strategies, but the results concerning guilt reactions were not significant. A partial explanation might be that disengagers wish to feel as if they had done the right thing and will rationalize away any guilty feelings. Although the relational problems examined by Cody (1981) do not represent a comprehensive model of relational problems, each of the three factors examined (disengager felt constrained, personal faults of target and target's failure to compromise) influenced the choice of disengagement strategies. These types of costs within social exchange theory may cause the outcomes received in the current relationship to fall below the comparison level. The factor labeled "Contrained" dealt with the perception that the disengager felt "constrained," a "lack of freedom," "suffocated" by a partner making "too many contributions" and by a partner who was "too possessive." Such a factor 15 would appear to deal with a major discrepancy in expectations for the relationship, with the partner perceived as desiring a more serious or more intense commitment than was desired by the disengager. In such a relationship (where the disengager is being pursued), behavioral de-escalation strategies are not necessarily an effective means by which to disengage, and disengagers were thus likely to employ either positive tone, de-escalation, justification or negative identity management strategies. factor included such perceptions as: The "Faults" the disengager felt the partner publicly embarrassed him/her, was "too demanding," had "personality problems" and possessed a personality which was incompatible with the disengager's. Disengagers are not likely to desire to continue future interactions with partners who introduce such costs into the relationship. Thus, disengagers avoided using positive tone and used justification strategies. The "Failure to Compromise" factor included perceptions that the partner was "unwilling to make enough contributions," took the disengager "for granted" and "no longer behaved romantically toward the disengager," and the partner was "unwilling to compromise for the good of the relationship." This orientation provided the opposite theoretical orientation to that of the Constrained factor because disengagers who rated their partners higher on this factor are probably the more inves- 16 partner. Since these disengagers desired a serious relationship that was not fulfilled, it is unlikely that they would use negative identity management strategies to disengage. Disengagers in this situation were more likely to use de-escalation and justification strategies. To date, the research does not represent a comprehensive view of the variables that influence strategy selection; therefore, further investigations are necessary. Trust Interpersonal trust, though an integral feature of close relationships, has been virtually ignored in research (Larzelere and Huston, 1980) . Trust increases relational security, reduces inhibitions and defensiveness, and allows people the freedom to share feelings (Stinnett and Walters, 1977). The reciprocity of trust occurs more frequently between partners than either the reciprocity of love or the depth of self-disclosure (Larzelere and Huston, 1980). The conceptualization of trust is obviously a pertinent aspect of human relationships. According to Larzelere and Huston (1980), trust is composed of perceptions of benevolence and honesty. Benevolence is an attribution of motivation: is a partner genuinely interested in the welfare of the other, or is the partner motivated to seek individual gains? concerned with sincerity and truthfulness: Honesty is to what extent 17 can a partner be believed in his/her promises? If a partner is perceived as benevolent and honest, then the partner is worthy of trust. Dyadic trust, or trust referring specifically to the benevolence and honesty of a significant other, may be a prerequisite for the development of intimacy. Increased intimacy may cause feelings of vulnerability which can be counterbalanced if the partner is trusted. However, the feelings of vulnerability may inhibit the growth of trust if it is not previously established. Dyadic trust is associated with characteristics of relational intimacy such as love, self-disclosure and commitment (Larzelere and Huston, 1980). Altman and Taylor (1973) suggest that trust is necessary for self-disclosure because reciprocity of disclosure must be based on reciprocity of trust. commitment: Trust may be a prerequisite for higher levels of trust are necessary for higher levels of commitment. However, it has been noted that it "takes time to develop high levels of trust prior to an initial commitment" (Larzelere and Huston, 1980, p. 602). When considering the role trust plays in the selection of disengagement strategies, it is important to note the process of breaking up involves a deterioration of dyadic trust (Larzelere and Huston, 1980). It is not known whether the decaying of trust takes place before or after the breakup, but it diminishes regardless of the prior level of 18 intimacy. This deterioration may be explained by the assumption that the development of trust is accompanied by self-disclosure. Therefore, subsequent to breaking up, the ex-partner still has the information that was disclosed in the relationship. Uncertainty as to if or how the ex-partner may use that knowledge may cause deterioration of trust. It seems fair to assume that relationships characterized by high levels of trust are probably characterized by high levels of self-disclosure, commitment and intimacy as well. In such cases, the partner who wishes to disengage will likely feel obligated to account for this desire as was previously reported for intimate couples. If a partner is viewed as sincere and honest, it would precipitate a concern for the other's feelings. Therefore, it seems reasonable to expect that highly trusting couples would not utilize disengagement strategies that were indirect or manipulative. Dyadic Adjustment Dyadic adjustment is a prominent concept in the study of marital relationships but has only recently been generalized to unmarried couples (Spanier, 1976). The concept is utilized in an effort to obtain an evaluation of the characteristics and interactions of a relationship. Dyadic adjustment is comprised of four empirically verified components: dyadic satisfaction, dyadic consensus (the extent to which a couple agrees on matters important to 19 dyadic functioning), dyadic cohesion (the extent a couple engages together in various activities) and affection (the verbal or physical expression of caring). Evaluating the quality of a relationship may be useful in predicting relational growth or decay. Dyadic adjustment measures should also be predictive of how a couple will disengage. The components of dyadic adjustment are related to intimacy and commitment in relationships. Although it may be argued that the components are redundant, Spanier (1976) justified the use of the separate components in obtaining a total dyadic adjustment score. Dyadic adjustment may also be related in some aspects to the "relationship world view" (Simpson, 1981a) because couples who are more cohesive may have more opportunity to develop shared assumptions and values. It seems reasonable to expect that the more adjusted a couple is, the more likely a partner wishing to disengage would use strategies which show concern for the other and avoid the use of manipulative strategies. The concept of adjustment implies that time is required to adjust. Therefore, increases in trust, self-disclosure and commitment are also likely to occur. Consequences of Relational Disengagement As with the antecedents of disruption, the study of ^he effects of disengagement has focused on the effects of 20 marital breakup. It is well documented that divorce is a stressful event. Individuals must readjust to a different lifestyle (Newman and Langer, 1981; Spanier and Lachman, 1980) . Some research suggests that ex-partners often find the readjustment period too difficult and consequently seek or require mental health care (Bloom, Asher and White, 1978). Disruption has been shown to affect sleeping, health, work efficiency, memory and loneliness (Nye and Berardo, 1973). Also, conflict concerning child custody and financial matters can lead to greater dissatisfaction with the breakup as well as continued hostility between ex-partners (Levinger, 1979b) . It is difficult to assess the similarities that might exist between breakups before and after marriage due to the fundamental differences in the social contexts. However, in order to determine intrinsic features that may characterize the ending of close relationships in general, it is necessary to study breakups before marriage and the effects on the members of the relationship. Hill et al. (1976) suggested that pre-marital breakups are generally less stressful than marital disruption, but Simpson (1982) reported major impacts in the areas of psychological effects and the effect on self-opinion. Another area affected by pre-marital breakups was social life. Often, partners establish mutual friendships and construct their social lives around a common network of 21 friends. Following a disruption, one or both of the members may experience stress when facing the task of restructuring their social lives, which often entails forming new friendships. Hill et aj.. (1976) reported that feelings of depression, loneliness and freedom are experienced following a breakup. were noted. Furthermore, some sex differences For example, when the man precipitated the disengagement, the couple was more likely to remain friends than when the woman initiated the breakup. Also, men reported more feelings of depression and rejection than did women (Hill et. a2.. , 1976) . The available literature concerning the consequences of breaking up indicate that similarities do exist between pre-marital and marital disruption. Hill et aj^. (1976) reported several similarities between pre-marital breakups and divorce. Although there are exceptions, breaking up is characterized by two-sidedness. "It is very rare for any sort of breakup to be entirely mutual" (Hill et al., 1976, p. 165). Consequently, one will rarely find two people who have the same perceptions of their breakup. There is also the possibility that sex differences in orientations to breakups are similar before and after marriage. Women cite more reasons for ending dating relationships and are more likely to initiate the disengagement of dating relationships, just as women cite more marital complaints when divorcing and seem to be more likely 22 to first suggest getting a divorce (Hill et aj^. , 1976). The feelings of failure, guilt and anxiety that often follow marital disruption may also occur in pre-marital breakups (Weiss, 1975) where men often find it more difficult to "let go" and are usually unwilling to withdraw from the relationship (Hilletal., 1976). However, the effects of the disengagement strategies on the feelings following a breakup have not been examined. Guilt and depression are often experienced following a breakup (Simpson, 1982) . Guilt may result when one uses an avoidance strategy when the target deserved an account. Of course, the relational problems precipitating the breakup are important. For example, if an "underbenefited" partner grows angry and uses strategies such as behavioral deescalation or negative identity management, depression may occur because the partner may still desire rewards from the relationship. The use of negative identity management may be an attempt to regain "face." If the cause of the relational problem was the disengager's feelings of constrainment, less positive strategies may be used in an effort to dissolve the bonds. However, if the "overbenefited" partner feels guilty, positive tone may be employed to lessen the guilt.- In any case, feelings of freedom are likely to be experienced because the disengager is no longer constrained. 23 The distress experienced when restructuring one's social life may be lessened if strategies which increase the likelihood of future contact are employed. If the ex- partners remain friends, it would be more probable that the mutual friendships formed during the relationship could be maintained. Showing concern for the other should facilitate the relationships ending on a positive note and should be conducive to the partners' remaining friends. The effect of variables on strategy selection and the effect of strategies on the consequences of disengagement warrant further attention. Based on this review of research, it is apparent that breaking up affects feelings of depression, guilt and freedom, but the effects of relational problems (i.e., constrained) and strategy selection on these variables remain undetermined. Also, whether or not the couple maintains contact (remains friends) should be affected by the manner in which the couple disengages. These four consequences (depression, guilt, freedom, staying friends) will be examined in the present study. In sum, it seems fair to assume that the use of positive tone and de-escalation strategies w i U facilitate the likelihood of future contact and lead to reductions in feelings of guilt, whereas the use of behavioral de-escalation, negative identity management and justification strategies will reduce the likelihood of future interaction and lead to increases 24 in guilt feelings. In cases where the relationship is characterized by greater trust and adjustment, one would expect a disengager to select positive tone, justification and de-escalation strategies because the disengager might feel obligated to account for the disengagement. Also, the more the partner is perceived as unwilling to compromise, the more likely the disengager will feel depressed, whereas the more the disengager feels constrained, the more likely the disengager will feel free. Hypotheses Replicating Cody (1981), the following hypotheses are advanced: H,: As intimacy increases, the selection of justification, positive tone, de-escalation and negative identity management strategies will increase, while the selection of behavioral de-escalation strategies will decrease. H^: The more the partner is perceived as unwilling to compromise, the more likely the disengager will be to select behavioral de-escalation, justification and negative identity management strategies. H^: The more the disengager feels that the partner was personally responsible for the disengagement by introducing personality problems (i.e., Faults) into the relationship, the more likely the disengager will select justification and de-escalation strategies. 25 H^: The more the disengager feels constrained by the partner, the more likely the disengager will be to select behavioral de-escalation, positive tone and de-escalation strategies. In expandinq the work of Baxter (1979a,b; 1980; 1981) and Cody (1981), the foUowing hypotheses are offered based on the review of literature: H^: As trust and dyadic adjustment increase, the selection of justification, positive tone and de-escalation strategies will increase and the selection of negative identity management and behavioral de-escalation strategies will decrease. H^: The use of positive tone and de-escalation strategies will lead to "increases in the likelihood of future contact and will be negatively associated with guilt. H-,: The use of behavioral de-escalation, negative identity management and justification strategies will lead to decreases in the likelihood of future contact and increases in the feelings of guilt. HQ: o The more the partner is perceived as unwilling to compromise, the more likely the disengager will feel depressed. Hp.: 9 The more the disengager feels constrained, the more likely the disengager will feel "free." CHAPTER II METHODOLOGY Subjects Subjects were 333 undergraduates enrolled in freshman, sophomore and junior level Speech Communication courses at Texas Tech University. One hundred forty-five males and 188 females received extra credit for voluntary participation. The average length of reported relationships was 62.10 weeks, ranging from one week to 312 weeks. Materials A copy of the questionnaire is included in the Appendix. The questionnaire consisted of four parts: perceptions of the relationship, causes precipitating the breakup, how the breakup was executed and consequences of the breakup. Part I (perceptions of the relationship) included questions concerning intimacy, trust and dyadic adjustment. Items 1-5 were used to measure perceived intimacy. items were previously used in Cody (1981). were used to measure perceived trust. These Items 17-22 These items constitute six of the eight items developed by Larzelere and Huston (1980). Subjects were asked to indicate the extent to which each item applied to the relationship they 26 27 recalled using a 7-point scale ranging from Strongly Agree to Strongly Disagree. components: Dyadic adjustment consisted of four consensus (items 23-34), satisfaction (items 35-41), cohesion (items 42-46) and affection (items 47-50). The items measuring consensus, satisfaction and cohesion were developed by Spanier (1976). The items dealing with affection were developed by the author to index the expression of affection. Consensus was measured on a 7- point scale ranging from Always Disagreed to Always Agreed. The remaining components were scored using a 7-point scale ranging from All the Time to Never. Part II (causes precipitating the breakup) included items concerning the relational problems investigated by Cody (1981): faults, unwilling to compromise and constrained. The faults factor was measured by items 1,5,9,13 and 17. The unwilling to compromise factor was measured by items 2,6,10,14 and 18. The constrained factor was measured by items 3,7,11,15 and 19. Subjects were asked to respond to the items according to how important each item was in the decision to break up. Part III (how the breakup was executed) included statements concerning verbal strategies or actions that could be used when breaking off a relationship. Subjects were asked to rate the degree to which they used these strategies on a 7-point scale ranging from "I never did/saití this" to "I definitely did/said this." Items 1-15 28 represent the statements concerning disengagement strategies. These items were previously used in Cody (1981). Part IV (consequences of the breakup) included statements developed by the author to index subsequent feelings of guilt, depression, freedom and the amount of future contact. Items 1,6 and 11 measured guilt; items 2,7 and 12 measured depression; items 4,9 and 14 measured freedom; items 5,10 and 15 measured the amount of future contact. (Refer to the Appendix for detailed items). Correlations were computed to test the hypotheses. Multiple regression analyses were also performed. CHAPTER III RESULTS This chapter presents the results of analyses conducted to confirm the existence of the five proposed methods of disengaging and to assess the internal consistency and interrelationships among the independent and dependent variables. Since the preliminary analyses indicated fairly high correlations between the independent variables (i.e., the Faults factor correlated with all other independent variables except Intimacy), a series of factor analyses was conducted in order to identify items that were pure loading items so that more independent indices could be constructed. presented. The solutions of the factor analyses are The results of analyses conducted to test the nine hypotheses are then presented. Factor Analysis of Strategies Table 1 presents the results of a Principle Components (with Varimax rotation) factor analysis of the 15 items employed to measure the five methods of disengaging. Factor one (withdraw/avoidance) was defined by"didn't say anything," "avoided future meetings" and "discouraged seeing each other." This factor accounted for 29.9% of the variance. 29 30 TABLE 1 FACTOR ANALYSIS SOLUTION ITEMS Didn't Say Anything Avoided Future Meetings Discouraged Seeing Each Other Going to Date Others Life is too short Wanted to be happy Fully Explained Both should be happy Changing inside May get back together Break off for awhile Call quits for now Very Sorry Regretted Very Much Cared Very Much Eigen values % of variance Alphas FACTOR 1 FACTOR 2 FACTOR 3 FACTOR 4 FACTOR 5 .74 -.08 -.15 -.13 -.21 .71 .02 -.13 -.05 -.11 .75 -.09 -.19 -.06 -.18 -.22 .63 .05 .15 .13 .11 .56 .14 .12 .06 -.06 .80 .24 .19 -.03 -.27 .09 .70 .10 .06 -.17 .12 .72 .11 .07 -.03 .27 .58 .18 .01 -.12 .06 .21 .62 . 12 -.05 .20 .15 .70 .16 -.07 -.24 .28 .12 .03 .10 .63 .11 .08 .66 -.16 .07 .01 .16 .87 -.33 4.48 -.05 2.09 13.9 .73 .06 1.56 10.4 .75 .30 .36 1.22 8.1 .74 1.03 6.8 .7'^ 29.9 .81 31 The alpha was .81. Factor two (negative identity management) accounted for 13.9% of the variance and was defined by "going to date others," "life is too short" and "want to be happy." The alpha was .73. Factor three (justification) accounted for 10.4% of the variance and was defined by "fully explained," "both should be happy" and "changing inside." The alpha was .75. Factor four (de-escalation) was defined by "may get back together "break off for awhile" and "call quits for now." factor accounted for 8.1% of the variance. .74. This The alpha was Factor five (positive tone) was defined by "very sorry,""regretted very much" and "cared very much." factor accounted for 6.8% of the variance. This The alpha was .79. Interrelationships Among Variables Table 2 presents the correlations among all variables included in this study. presented. The alpha coefficients are also Several problems can be noted. First, the items written to measure the Faults construct correlated with all other variables except the Intimacy construct (r; = .05). Second, the Dyadic Adjustment measure correlated with all other variables except the Unwillingness to Compromise variable (r = -.16). Third, the Trust measure correlated very highly with both the Unwillingness to Compromise construct (r = .56) and with the Faults construct f^ k 1 1 1 5 •H (0 co 1 a / <D Q —( j •H (0 / rM ^ TT in • . u^ V£> .1 0\ r-l CTV r«- r^ CN ' -o •H 0 l' / / > < 0 •H JJ / . 1 m / 0) - H (Q U / l' ' ' i^ -< «< <D 1 1 ' 1 r^ (N iT) (N eo O / c • • / 1 O • • co • 1 1 1 / rn rM -H -H • • • 1 { •,,] rvj . "^ '"".1 1 rvj O j ^ r- / 0} i < 1 M c cc: 1 / 1 0 •-U H - ÍH; 03 « < > 1 1 3 U ^ 1-5 - H vu < > / U •H / • 0 / 0^ -1 1 1 O 0^ 5 cs / • • aoQomrNu^ -. (<n « o • • • • in O • ^ r, 1 • ng F a u l t / a> rc) í^ / m • o vo in -. .^ • 1 1• • • V -• • p-i r^ • f* c5 in 00 • . (N • 1 ví ' C' V O • inl c tr -< -1 -^ •H • (n rsi vo (N • . . 1 r\ 00 (N 1 1 o\ O rsi —. —. rsi rsi —. f1 (>i vO rsi (N -^ 'jC 1 1 1 1 <3\ QC ^ r^, I I in rvj rsi o I —< 1 VO — I VÛ tn ^ — (•" o I VC 1 1 "*1 I 1 I r^ in "^ ^ l inl o1 o o O —< -n 1 1 1 / 1 "0 (U 1 -H (0 U —" 1 r^ í^ "* ^ 1 ^ O (N O —• m <N ÍN ^l in / 1 1 1 >< ' 5 c - ! ! 1 z. 1i 1 1 1 1 1 ^. - T j c 3 X i^ > :2 < s 3 u •~ H — î:. ^ iP O 1 ^' -sl 1 t ^t 1 XI 1 i 1 ' ' 1 r^-l ' rMi 1 —.1 LO ssl xl 1 1' > , n _. - : : ; i' ^ O 1 1 1 -£>| - n — 1 .-N ! (U > -^ ;- vo (vj — o II rr 1 '-' C 2 - • H c 11 p^ Cfl 3 * j -n -^ co ol Ti o fNI !N (NI / 1 E I i / u HJ o "OÍ ^ l 1 1 co 1 1 rvi 1 •9 1 .u —>j 1 j 1 1 1 c 1 r^r ' 1 If) m t :s in I 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 •H « e/ 03 ^"-1 1 c -< >, 0 03 O m / Cfl 1 •H 4J C . 1 m — Q C 0 U • 1 1 / IT3 3 • 1 C> Ot / >i o u •j 1 / •0 oi oi / H < ^ o / co 3 t^ -< M • / •iJ l-H r* / / cn H 0 &H 00 m( - . / > •H •i-' a) c 2 O inl 1 / • z cn tn r • / / 0 CQ í^ m • 1 / 0« (U IS e-i / 1 (0 CLI / / •H JJ 2 6H / / / / 03 < • n / tu 0) 03 00 - / 4J C/3 h^ ' / > Q / / 3 Pcj / / 4J w 32 1 1 c 0 — - - '— - -— -i^ 3 á "^ ^*^ J: ^^ .- c .X «^ T 1 -^ 1 l^ c - v: :2 :; > < w c 'ií* >. — - r 1 •: •- '— •X. •'z £_ ^ ; •j'. < < 33 (r = -.45). Subjects felt that partners who had faults and who would not reciprocate investments could not be trusted. Fourth, the assessment of internal consistency for the Guilt items was low (.54) indicating that these items poorly measured the construct. For the purpose of this study, however, the Gui-lt variable was retained in order to test hypotheses six and seven. It is conceded that better items should be employed to measure Guilt. Since many of these independent variables were intercorrelated, there was a problem of multicolinearity. Factor analyses were conducted to explore whether deleting some items and/or combining some factors would result in the construction of more orthogonal indices for subsequent analyses. These analyses revealed that each of the five items written to measure the Faults construct correlated with the items of other variables. Thus, it was not possible to select fewer items as pure loading items measuring Faults. In fact, a multiple regression analysis revealed that 40% of the variance in Faults was accounted for by Dyadic Adjustment, Trust, Constrained and Unwilling to Compromise. Thus, the Faults variable was eliminated from subsequent analyses. It should be noted, however, that Faults did correlate .25 with the use of justification strategies, but only -.02 with the use of de-escalation strategies, thus providing qualified support for hypothesis three. Faults also correlated -.24 with the use of positive 34 tone strategies and .19 with the use of avoidance tactics indicating that when the faults of the partner were perceived as the cause of the breakup, disengagers used justification and avoidance tactics and avoided using positive tone strategies. Further factor analyses revealed that only two Trust items ("My partner was perfectly honest and truthful with me" and "I felt that I could trust my partner completely") were identified as pure loading items, distinct from the Intimacy and Unwilling to Compromise variables. However, even when these two items were averaged together to form a new Trust variable, this Trust variable correlated -.41 with the Unwilling to Compromise variable. Subsequently, a scree line test was employed in a factor analysis of the Intimacy, Unwilling to Compromise, Constramed and Trust items. (Dyadic Adjustment was not included because the Dyadic Adjustment measure is itself multi-dimensional, See Spanier, 1976) The purpose of the analysis was to provide a means for purifying the Intimacy, Unwilling to Compromise, Constrained and Trust items. The three factor solution is presented in Table 3. The analysis revealed that the following items were pure loading items measuring both Trust and UnwiUing to Compromise: "My partner was perfectly honest and truthful with me," "I felt that I could trust my partner completely," TABLE 3 FACTOR ANALYSIS SOLUTLON, FOR IJSITIMACY/ CONSTRMNED, _UNWILLJNGNESS TO COMPROMISE AND TRUST ITEMS FACTOR 1 FACTOR 2 FACTOR 3 Honest and Truthful .64 .02 .05 Could be Trusted .63 .13 -.02 Truly Sincere .74 .10 .04 Fair and Just .75 .01 -.02 Unwilling to Contribute .70 -.13 .10 Took Me for granted .68 -.10 -.03 I made more investments .61 .23 -.22 Do not show consideration .60 .10 -.07 Very Intimate -.03 .86 -.07 Disclosed intimate information -.02 .84 -.09 Exclusive Dating -.07 .71 -.19 Emotionally Attached -.10 .86 -.16 .22 -.13 .65 Too Demanding -.18 -.03 .68 Too Possessive -.05 -.04 .76 .21 .24 .65 5.59 29.4 3.45 18.1 Suffocated Wanted More Eigen Value % of variance 2. 18 11.5 36 "My partner was truly sincere in his/her promises," "I felt that my partner did not show me enough consideration," "My partner treated me fairly and justly/' "I realized he/ she was unwilling to make enough contributions into the relationship," "l felt that he/she took me for granted," and "I made many more investments to the relationship than my partner." This factor accounted for 29.4% of the variance in the solution. The alpha was .86. The factor will be referred to as Trust for the remainder of the study. Since the original Trust and Unwilling to Compromise constructs were inversely related, the Unwilling to Compromise items were recoded so that the new Trust construct measured perceived trust and willingness to compromise. The second factor in the solution contained four of the original Intimacy items and accounted for 18.1% of the variance. The alpha was .89. The third factor accounted for 11.5% of the variance and included four of the Constrained items. The alpha was .73. The pure loading items identified above were averaged together to form the indices of Trust, Intimacy and Constrained. Finally, the preliminary analyses revealed that the four affection items written for this study as part of the Dyadic Adjustment measure correlated highly with the Intimacy items, as did the following three items: consensus agreement on "aims, goals and things beleived important," consensus agreement on "making major decisions" and the cohesion item which dealt with "engaging in outside interests together." Eliminating these items reduced the interrelationship between Intimacy and Dyadic Adjustment (r_ = .25). The Dyadic Adjustment measure correlated .36 with the new Trust construct. The alpha for Dyadic Adjustment was .87. The descriptive statistics for the four independent variables measuring aspects of the relationships were as follows: Intimacy, mean = 2.31, median = 1.756, standard deviation = 1.52; Constrained, mean = 4.10, median = 4.00, standard deviation = 1.66; Trust, mean = 3.42, median = 3.185, standard deviation = 1.570; Dyadic Adjustment, mean = 3.351, median = 3.327, standard deviation = .843. The descriptive statistics for the five disengagement strategies were as follows: Positive Tone, mean = 4.035, median = 3.898, standard deviation = 2.043; Justification, mean = 3.914, median = 3.675, standard deviation = 1.759; Negative Identity Management, mean = 4.499, median = 2.946, standard deviation = 1.716; Avoidance, mean = 5.027, median = 5.474, standard deviation = 1.900; De-escalation, mean = 4.225, median = 1.785, standard deviation = 4.053. 38 Hypotheses Table 4 presents the correlations between the relational variables and strategies (hypotheses 1-5), between strategies and the consequences of disengagement (hypotheses 6 and 7) and between relational variables and the consequences of disengagement (hypotheses 8 and 9 ) . Hypothesis one predicted that as Intimacy increased, disengagers would be more likely to employ positive tone, justification, de-escalation and negative identity management strategies and less likely to employ avoidance tactics. Although the correlations were smaller than anticipated, significant support was obtained for the first hypothesis with the exception of the correlation between intimacy and negative identity management (£ = .02). Intimacy was negatively associated with avoidance (r = -.24, p < .001) and positively associated with positive tone {r = .16, p < .01), de-escalation (r = .17, p < .01) and justification (£ = .19, p < .001). Hypothesis two predicted that the more a partner was perceived as unwilling to compromise, the more likely the the disengager would be to select avoidance, justification and negative identity management strategies. As noted earlier the original Unwilling to Compromise and Trust variables were subjected to factor analysis in order to distinguish pure loading items. The resulting construct (Trust) measured perceived trust and willingness to 39 C O •H cn w CN (N O o o CN o o o æ m m æ o 0) cu C/3 Q W u 2 CN W 13 o w C/3 2 O o u I EH ^ 03 W J 03 < 1—t CC < > J 2 w s w o < o 2 w -p C/3 M CU W •H 03 U •P Q c NCE w 2 W W O s &H w CO 2 O ES AND C EH 0:í TIONS BE < ELA EH o æ < M W o w os o: o u RELATIONAL VAR][ABLES M o (N LD CN cn < 2 O CN E-t < U3 (N O O O (N co c 0 u •r3 LD CTl un (N H Q CX3 CN (N O CN CN I (N (N (-n (N CN O O CN O (N CN o I H U (Tî E •H -U Oí EH cn (T> <£> CN "^ o C co Q; w c 0 M o W < Oí E-t cn C 0 -l-í •H cn 0 Oi 0 •H •H (tJ -U (T3 EH 0) > •H C r—( 03 u w o; 1 <D Q U •H 4H •H -M æ r-í ^ C 0 •H 0) > cn æ •rH 4-J 4-) (T3 CP -H > 1 cu 2 0 > < (T3 -P 03 T! (—H 0) Q; M •H 3 CL, O o; ui a o; Q 40 compromise and was used to test the second hypothesis. The new construct correlated -.21 with the use of justification strategies. That is, trust and willingness to compromise were negatively related to justification. This result provides indirect support for the prediction that Unwillingness to Compromise and justification would be positively associated. The correlations with negative identity management and avoidance were not significant (r = .06, -.05, respectively). Hypothesis three dealt with the Faults variable which was not included in the final analysis because the variable was not independent of other predictor variables. Hypothesis four predicted that the more the disengager felt constrained by the partner, the more likely the disengager would be to select avoidance, positive tone and de-escalation strategies. The Constrained variable correlated positively with de-escalation [r = .12, p < .05). The correlations with positive tone and avoidance were not significant (r = -.03, .01, respectively). Significant correlations were obtained between Constrained and justification [r = .15) and between Constrained and negative identity management (£ = .26). Hypothesis five predicted that as Trust and Dyadic Adjustment increased, the disengager would be more likely to employ justification, positive tone and de-escalation strategies and less likely to employ negative identity 41 management and avoidance tactics. As Trust increased, disengagers were more likely to select positive tone (x = .22, p < .001) and de-escalation (r = .12, p < .05). A negative correlation was obtained between Trust and justification (r = -.21). The correlations with negative identity management and avoidance were not significant (X = .06, -.05, respectively). As Dyadic Adjustment increased, disengagers were more likely to select positive tone (r; = .15, p < .01). Significant negative associations were obtained between Dyadic Adjustment and justification (£ = -,19), negative identity management (r = -.11) and avoidance {r = -.15). The correlation with de-escalation was not significant (jr = .10), though in the predicted direction. Hypothesis six predicted that the use of positive tone and de-escalation strategies would increase the likelihood that partners would remain friends. A negative association between positive tone and de-escalation and guilt was also predicted. Significant support was obtained for the first part of the hypothesis six. Positive tone correlated positively with "staying friends" {_r = .16, p <• .01); de-escalation correlated positively with "staying frier.ds" (r = .27, p < .001). was not supported. The second part of hypothesis six Positive tone and de-escala^ion both correlated .25 with guilt. 42 Hypothesis seven predicted that the selection of avoidance, negative identity management and justification strategies would lead to decreases in the likelihood of partners remaining friends and increases in feelings of guilt. Partial support was obtained for the first part of hypothesis seven. Avoidance was negatively associated with "staying friends" {r_ = -.17, p < .01). Justification correlated -.07 with "staying friends". Negative identity management was positively associated with "staying friends" {r_ = .12). The second part of hypothesis seven was not supported though the correlations between negative identity management and guilt and between avoidance and guilt were in the predicted direction (£ = .05, .004, respectively). Justification correlated -.04 with guilt. Hypothesis eight predicted that the more the partner was perceived as unwilling to compromise, the more likely the disengager would be to feel depressed. The new Trust construct (perceptions of willingness to compromise) correlated -.07 with depression. Willingness to compromise correlated more strongly with subsequent feelings of guilt (£ = .26, p < .001) and "staying friends" (r = .23,p<.001). Hypothesis nine predicted that the more the disengager felt constrained by the partner, the more likely the disengager would be to feel free. for this hypothesis. Strong support was found The Constrained factor correlated .46 with "feeling free" (p<.001). Also, disengagers who felt 43 constrained by their partners were less likely to experience subsequent feelings of depression (r = -.27). Multiple Reqression Analyses Multiple regression analyses were performed to test the predictive power of the independent variables to the dependent variables. Regression onto the strategies of disengagement revealed that 14% of the variance in the selection of justification strategies was accounted for on the basis of the combination of four variables: Trust, Constrained, Intimacy and Dyadic Adjustment. Intimacy and Constrained accounted for six percent of the variance in the selection of de-escalation strategies and eight percent of the variance in the selection of negative identity management strategies. Trust and Intimacy accounted for eight percent of the variance in the selection of positive tone strategies, whereas Intimacy, Dyadic Adjustment and Constrained accounted for seven percent of the variance in the selection of avoidance strategies. Although the portions of variance accounted for in the regression analysis were small, they were significant at the .05 level. Regression onto the consequences of disengagement revealed that 23% of the variance in subsequent feelings of depression was accounted for by Intimacy and Constrained. Twenty one percent of the variances in feelings of freedom was accounted for by Constrained and Dyadic Adjustment. 44 Trust and Dyadic Adjustment accounted for eight percent of the variance in "staying friends." Trust and Intimacy accounted for eight percent of the variance in feelings of guilt. Again, these were significant at the .05 level. CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION Strateqy Selection The results of the present study provided support for several of the hypotheses concerning variables which affect the choice of disengagement strategies. Replicating the findings of Cody (1981), intimacy correlated positively with the use of justification, de-escalation and positive tone and negatively with the use of avoidance strategies. When intimate bonds have been formed, disengagers probably feel obligated to account for the disengagement. Furthermore, trust correlated positively with the use of positive tone and de-escalation tactics as predicted. Trust is associated with characteristics of intimacy; therefore, when higher levels of trust were present, disengagers selected strategies which showed concern for the partners' feelings. The results concerning dyadic adjustment were also consistent with expectations. Dyadic adjustment correlated positively with the use of positive tone tactics and negatively with the use of avoidance and negative identity management strategies. Again, when a couple is more adjusted, it is more probable that trust and mtimacy have developed; therefore, disengagers selected strategies which showed concern for their partners. 45 46 The factors of relational problems (constrained and unwilling to compromise) influenced strategy selection. When the disengager felt that the partner did not contribute to the relationship (unwilling to compromise), justification strategies were employed. In such a case, the disengager is the more invested partner and it would be difficult to merely withdraw from the relationship. The use of justi- fication strategies provided reasons for the termination. When disengagers felt constrained, de-escalation strategies were employed. Disengagers who feel constrained usually receive more benefits from partners who desire a more serious relationship. The disengager may wish to continue to receive rewards without feeling as if he/she were taking advantage of the partner. Therefore, de-escalation tactics, which show concern for the partner while lessening the amount of present contact were employed. Some of the expectations were not confirmed. For example, intimacy was not correlated with the use of negative identity management. It was originally expected that an individual would not manipulate his/her partner's image were they not intimate with one another (Cody, 1981), However, when terminating an intimate relationship, the disengager may feel obligated to show concern for the target or to at least account verbally for the disengagement without managing a negative image of the partner. Contrary to expectations, justification correlated negatively with trust 47 and dyadic adjustment (r = -.21, -.19, respectively). When higher levels of trust and dyadic adjustment are present, it is probable that the disengager feels that target deserves more than a mere explanation. Whereas justification strategies often assign blame for the disengagement (disengager or partner) and request a full termination, positive tone and de-escalation strategies show concern for the other and enhance the probability of remaining friends. (Though the correlation between dyadic adjustment and de-escalation was smaller than anticipated, r = .10, it approached significance in the predicted direction.) Consequently, disengagers would avoid the use of justification strategies which might indicate an unwillingness to discuss the partner's perceptions and reactions. The trust construct, derived to measure trust and willingness to compromise, did not correlate significantly with negative identity management or avoidance tactics. Dyadic adjustment was more strongly correlated with both strategies. Contrary to expectations, disengagers who felt constrained selected negative identity management and justification strategies. It was expected that when disengagers felt constrained, they would be motivated to justify their intentions in a positive manner because they were receiving more benefits without contributing to the relationship and would be likely to feel guilty. 48 However, it is probable that overbenefited individuals who felt bored and suffocated attempted to lessen the intense feelings of the other by using less positive strategies. The desire to terminate the relationship, and therefore eliminate continued guilt feelings, may be greater than the desire to continue to receive rewards from the partner. In this case, the benefits are no longer perceived as rewards since guilt feelings accompany them because the disengager cannot reciprocate at the same level of intensity. Therefore, less attention is given to the needs of the partner. Consequences of Disengagement Several of the expectations concerning the consequences of disengagement were confirmed. When disengagers used positive tone and de-escalation strategies, the couple was more likely to remain friends. Positive tone and de-escalation strategies express caring for the partner and express possibilities for future reconciliation. The likelihood of maintaining a friendship is enhanced. When disengagers used avoidance and justification strategies, the couple was less likely to remain friends. Avoidance tactics leave the partner with no reason or explanation for the disengagment and are likely to cause confusion, hurt and anger. Justification provides an explanation, but does not express concern for the rejection needs of the partner. 49 Thus, avoidance and justification tactics do not ensure the continuation of friendship. The expectation that disengagers who felt constrained would experience subsequent feelings of freedom was strongly confirmed. Disengagers who felt suffocated and tied down in the relationship felt freer after the disengagement than disengagers who did not feel constrained in the relationship. It is interesting to note that disengagers who felt constrained were less likely to experience subsequent depression (r = -.27). it seems probable that the cost of remaining in a relationship which is constraining is greater than the cost of foregoing the relationship, especially if the disengager has an available alternative. The results concerning guilt were unexpected. It was predicted that the use of positive tone and the de-escalation strategies would be negatively associated with guilt because the disengager would feel as if he/she had attended to the needs of the partner. However, the use of positive tone and de-escalation tactics correlated positively with guilt indicating that when these strategies were used, greater feelings of guilt were experienced subsequent to the breakup. It is probable that some positive tone and de-escalation strategies are used because they are prosocial in nature: "I really care for you, but.. ../' "maybe we'll get back together..." The disengager may not have sincere intentions of maintaining a friendship in 50 which case he/she may experience guilt due to the lack of honesty. However, it is also probable that the disengager has sincere concern for the partner but feels guilty because he/she is responsible for terminating the relationship and thereby hurting the other because the desire to disengage is discrepant with the partner's desire to continue the relationship. The subsequent feelings of guilt may partially explain why the use of positive tone and de-escalation strategies is correlated positively with staying friends. The disengager may be motivated initially by guilt to maintain the friendship, regardless of the sincerity of the disengagement attempt. The trust construct correlated positively with staying friends and guilt, indicating that when a partner was perceived as trustworthy and willing to compromise for the good of the relationship the couple was more likely to stay friends and the disengager was more likely to feel guilty. When the partner is perceived as willing to invest in the relationship, the disengager may feel guilty because he/she did not desire to contribute further to the relationship. However, the trust variable did not correlate significantly with depression. Feelings of depression may be more dependent upon levels of intimacy and the degree of love in the relationship than upon perceptions of a partner's willingness to compromise. 51 Contrary to expectations, the use of negative identity management strategies correlated positively with staying friends. It was originally expected that attempts to negatively altercast the partner would hinder any possibilities of continuing the relationship on a friendship level. However, as noted earlier, disengagers who felt constrained were more likely to use negative identity management strategies. It seems probable that the sense of freedom following the disengagement from a constraining relationship would allow the disengager to maintain the relationship at a lesser level of intensity provided the partner is also willing. Conclusion The present study provides support for the claim that relational variables influence disengagement strategy selection. Although some of the correlations were smaller than anticipated, the majority were in the predicted directions. Generally, when a relationship is characterized by higher levels of intimacy, trust and dyadic adjustment, disengagers will select strategies which show concern for the partner, offer possible reconciliation and provide reasons for the disengagement. When the partner was perceived as unwilling to compromise, disengagers selecfed justification strategies, whereas when disengagers felt constrained by the partner, they selected de-escalation, 52 justification and negative identity management strategies. Relational variables also affect subsequent feelings. Disengagers who felt constrained experienced greater feelings of freedom and were less likely to experience depression. When partners were perceived as willing to compromise, the couple was more likely to remain friends and the disengager was more likely to feel guilty. The results also supported the claim that the choice of disengagement strategy influenced subsequent feelings. Positive tone and de-escalation strategies were positively associated with staying friends and feelings of guilt. Negative identity management correlated positively with staying friends whereas justification correlated negatively with staying friends. The multiple regression analyses revealed that significant portions of the variance in strategy selection were accounted for by various combinations of trust, intimacy, dyadic adjustment and the constrained construct. Also, significant portions of the variance in the consequences of disengagement were accounted for by the same relational variables. The multiple regression analyses were performed to determine if one or more linear combinations of the independent variables significantly predicted the variance in the dependent variables. However, the portions of variance accounted for were small indicating that 53 other variables need to be investigated in order to increase predictive abilities. The measures of trust, unwillingness to compromise and guilt used in the present study were problematic. It was difficult to assess the effects of trust and unwillingness to compromise because the variables were not independent of each other. Furthermore, the items used to measure the' consequences of disengagement may actually have measured what the subjects felt during the disengagement process rather than after the disengagement was complete (with the exception of the amount of future contact). This may account for the low correlations with depression which is likely to be a long lasting effect. We must concede that this study was based on recalled instances of disengaging. Recall of how the breakup occurred may be painted in ways which make the disengager look better than he/she actually is by overestimating the use of positive tone strategies. Ideally, future research with sufficient time and resources may wish to employ a longitudinal design that studies the growth, deterioration and disengagement as a communication process. (See Hill, Rubin and Peplau, 1976 and Simpson, 1982.) Future research should investigate personality variables which might affect strategy usage and subsequent consequences. For example, a person with high self-esteem might not use manipulative strategies because it woulc be 54 inconsistent with his/her self-image. Further, variables such as locus of control, Machiavellianism and cognitive complexity have been shown to influence communication strategies (Cody, 1981) . Also, this study did not address issues such as physical attraction and dominance within the relationship. Although theoretically sound constructs from social exchange were utilized, the constructs of comparison level and comparison level for alternatives were not assessed. It would also be interesting to determine how the integrated social networks (mutual friendships) of relational partners might affect strategy selection and consequences. Future consideration might be given to cultural differences, as well as socio-economic and educational differences. For example, an individual from a high socio- economic background might be expected to select more prosocial strategies than an individual from a low socioeconomic 'background who may not place as high a priority on appearing socially adept. Finally, it would be more conducive to accurate research if the perceptions of both partners could be obtained. Because relational breakups are characterized by two-sidedness, it would be interesting to compare the reactions of the disengager with those of the partner. REFERENCES Altman, I. and Taylor, D. A. Social penetration: the development of interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1973. Baxter, L. A. Self-disclosure as a relationship disengagement strategy: an exploratory investigation. Human Communication Research, 1979, 5^, 215-222. Baxter, L. A. Relationship closeness, relational intention and disengagement strategies. Paper presented to S.C.A., San Antonio, Texas, November, 19 79. Baxter, L. A. and Philpott, J. Relational disengagement: a process view. Paper presented to S.C.A., San Antonio, Texas, November, 1980. Baxter, L. A. and Philpott, J. Communicator age and sex role orientation differences is preferred relationship termination strategies. Paper presented to S.C.A., Anaheim, California, November, 1981. Blau, P. Exchanqe and power in social life. John Wiley, 1964. New York: Bloom, B., Asher, S., and White, S. Marital disruption stressor: a review and analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 1978, 8_5, 867-894. Cody, M. A typology of disengagement strategies and an examination of the role intimacy, reactions to inequity and relational problems play in strategy selection. Paper presented to S.C.A., Ãnaheim, California, November, 1981. Fitzpatrick, M. A. and Winke, J. You always hurt the one you love: strategies and tactics in interpersonal conflict. Communication Quarterly, 1979, 3-11. Foa, E. and Foa, V. Resource theory of social exchange, in M.E. Roloff, Interpersonal communiation: the social exchange approach. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, 1981, pp. 52-56. Hill, C. T., Rubin, Z. and Peplau, L. A. Breakups before marriage: the end of 103 affairs. Journal of Social Issues, 1976, 3^, 147-168. 55 56 Homans, G. Social behavior: Its elementary forms. York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1974. New Kelley, H. and Thibaut, J. Interpersonal relations: a theory of interdependence. New York: John Wiley, 1978. Kimmel, D. Relationship initiation and development: a life span developmental approach, in R. L. Burgess and T. L. Huston (Eds.) Social exchange in developing relationships. New York: Academic Press, 1979, pp. 351-377. Larzelere, R. E. and Huston, T. L. The dyadic trust scale: toward understanding interpersonal trust in close relationships. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 1980, 595-605. Levinger, G. A social exchange view on the dissolution of pair relationships, in R. L. Burgess and T. L. Huston (Eds.) Social exchange in developing relationships. New York: Academic Press, 1979, pp. 169-191. Levinger, G. Toward the analysis of close relationships. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1980, 16, 510-544. Miller, B. C. A multivariate developmental model of marital satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 1976, 643-657. Newman, H. M. and Langer, J. Post-divorce adaptationsand the attribution of responsibility. Sex Roles, 1981, 1_, 223-232. Roloff, M. E. Interpersonal communication: the social exchange approach. Eeverly Hills: Sage Publications, 1981. Ryder, R. G. Longitudinal data relating marriage satisfaction and having a child. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 1973, 604-606. Simpson, T. A symbolic exchange framework for the development of intimate relationships. Paper presented to S.C.A., Anaheim, California, November, 1981. Simpson, T. Premarital breakups and post-breakup adjustment: a longitudinal study. Unpublished manuscript, University of West Virginia, 1982. 57 Snyder, D. Multidimensional assessment of marital satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 1979, 813-823. Spanier, G. B. Measuring dyadic adjustment: new scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 1976, 15-28. Spanier G. and Lachman, M. Factors associated with adjustment to marital separation. Sociological Focus, 1980, n_, 369-381. Stinnett, N. and Walters, J. Relationships in marriage and family. New York: MacMillan, 1977. Thornton, A. Children and marital stability. Marriage and the Family, 1977, 531-540. Journal of Walster, E., Walster, G., and Berscheid, E. Eguity: theory and research. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1978. Weiss, R. Marital separation, New York: Basic Books, 1975, cited in T. Simpson, Pre-marital break-ups and post-break-up adjustment: a longitudinal study. Unpublished manuscript, University of West Virginia, 1982. APPENDIX DO NOT PUT YOUR NAME ON THIS QUESTIONNAIRE. Dear Participant: This questionnaire deals with how people "break off" their dating relationships. Occasionally, due to incompatibilities, conflicts, different desires or interests or due to the fact that people move great distances from each other, people will desire to break off the relationships they have with person(s) they date. We would like you to help us come to understand how people break off relationships by telling us how you perceived the relationship, why you decided to break off the relationship, how you went about breaking off the relationship, and how you felt after breaking off the relationship. To help us do this, please recall a relationship that you were in where you decided to break off the relationship. You can think of a relationship that lasted any length of time, from one or several dates to one that lasted several years. With this relationship in mind, please answer the following 4 parts of this questionnaire. All answers are strictly confidential. Please read the instructions for each part carefully and MAKE SURE THAT YOU ANSWER EVERY QUESTION. Even if the relationship you recall and report on only lasted one date, please answer every guestion. Part I. Perceptions of the relationship. In this part, we will ask a number of quesrions dealing with how you perceived the relationship. Please write the appropriate answer in the blank space to the left of the statement. Use the following numbers to indicate the extent to which each statement was true for the relationship you recalled: If If If If If If or If you you you you you you you Stronqly Agree, write 1 in the space provided, Somewhat Agree, write 2 in the space provided, Sliqhtly Aqree, write 3 in the space provided, are Neutral, write 4 in the space provided, Sliqhtly Disaqree, write 5 in the space provided, Somewhat Disagree, write 6 in the space provided, Stronqly Disagree, write 7 in the space providei. 58 59 Again, make sure that you answer every item. _and coop^era_tioj\_ are very much appreciated. 1- Your honesty The relationship was a very intimate one. _2. I disclosed very intimate information about myself to this partner. _3. I yielded to the wishes of the partner and compromised for the good of the relationship. 4. I expressed an interest in exclusively dating the partner. _5. I was emotionally attached to this partner. _6. I made nearly all the decisions concerning the relationship. _7. My partner was extremely attractive. _8. This partner and I shared many of the same friends. _9. My partner was extremely popular. _10. In this relationship, I had nearly all the power. 11. Most of my closest friends knew this partner. 12. I controlled much of the behavior of my partner. 13. My partner was extremely desireable. 14. In this relationship, I found it easy to dominate my partner. 15. My partner had many positive qualities. 16. This partner and I shared the same set of acquaintances. 17. There were times when my partner could not be trusted. 18. My partner was perfectly honest and truthful with me. 19. I felt that I could trust my partner completely. 60 20. My partner was truly sincere in his/her promises 21. I felt that my partner did not show me enough consideration. 22. My partner treated me fairly and justly. Indicate the extent to which you and your partner agreed or disagreed on: 23. Money matters Always Disagreed IJJJJJJJ 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Always Agreed 24. Matters of recreation Always Disagreed IJJJJJJJ 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Always Agreed 25. Religious matters Always Disagreed IJJJJJJJ 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Always Agreed 26. D e m o n s t r a t i o n s of Affection Always D i s a g r e e d /JJJJJJJ Always Agreed 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 27. Friends Always Disagreed IJ J JJ JJ J 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Always Agreed 28. Sex relations Always Disagreed IJJJJJJJ 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Always Agreed 29. Conventionality (correct or proper behavior) Always , , , , , , , , Disagreed /JJJJJJJ 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 ^^"^^^^ Agreed 30. Philosophy of life (mine or his/hers) Always , , , , , , , . Disagreed /JJJJJJJ 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 ^^""^^^ Agreed 31. Ways of dealing WaVS o r u t í c i J . x i i y „ i t h parents Alwavs Always rtxwci_/o , Aims: goals: things believed important , , Disagreed I I I J J l 7 32. , , , , ^ j • I Agreed D b 4 J z i Always . . . , . , / / ^^^^^^ Disagreed / / / /J /J ' Agree. 7 6 5 4 J 2 ... 61 33. Amount of time spent together Always Always Disagreed /JJJJJJJ Agreed 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 34. Making major decisions Always Disagreed /JJJJJJJ 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 PLEASE MAKE SURE THAT YOU HAVE ANSWERED EVERY ITEM. YOU. Always Agreed THANK Use the following scale for the next 16 questions: Write 7 if your Write 6 if your FREQUENTLY. Write 5 if your Write 4 if your TIME. Write 3 if your Write 2 if your Write 1 if your 35. answer to the question is ALL THE TIME. answer to the question is VERY answer to the question is FREQUENTLY. answer to the question is MUCH OF THE answer to the question is OCCASIONALLY. answer to the question is RARELY. answer to the question is NEVER. How often did you consider separation or terminating the relationship? 36. How often did you or your partner fight? 37. In general, how often did you think things between you and your partner were going well? 38. Did you confide in your partner? 39. Did you ever regret that you dated? 40. How often did you and your partner quarrel? 41. 44. How often did you and your partner "get on each other's nerves"? Did you and your partner engage in outside interests together? Did you and your partner have a stimulating exchange of ideas? Did you and your partner laugh together? 45. Did you and your partner calmly discuss thmgs? 42. 43. 62 46. Did you and your partner work together on projects? 47. Did you and your partner kiss each other? 48. Did you express how much you cared for each other' _49. Did you show how much you liked each other? 50. Did you show affection for each other? Part II. Causes precipitating the break up. How important were each of the following reasons in deciding to break off this relationship? Use the following to report your answers: If you Strongly Agree, write 1 in the space provided, If you Somewhat Agree, write 2 in the space provided, If you Slighlty Agree, write 3 in the space provided, If you are Neutral, write 4 in the space provided, If you Slightly Disagree, write 5 in the space provided, If you Somewhat Disagree, write 6 in the space provided, If you Strongly Disagree, write 7 in the space provided. 1. I realized that he/she had too many faults (personality and/or otherwise). 2. I realized he/she was unwilling to make enough contributions into the relationship. 3. I simply felt that the relationship was beginning to constrain me and I felt a lack of freedom. 4. I wanted to date another (others) who had more to offer. 5. I felt his/her personality was incompatible with mine. PLEASE MAKE SURE THAT YOU HAVE ANSWERED EVERY QUESTION. THANK YOU. 6. I felt that he/she no longer behaved towards me as romantically as he/she once did. 7. The partner made too many contributions and I started to feel suffocated. 63 ,8. I knew I could never fall "in love" with him/her, so I wanted to date another (others). I felt that he/she was too demanding. 10 I felt that he/she took me for granted. 11* I felt that he/she was becoming too possesive of me. 12. I wanted to start seeing another (others) who was (were) more desireable. 13. The partner behaved in ways that embarrassed me. 14. I felt that he/she wasn't ready to maké the necessary commitments to a serious relationship. 15. I felt the partner wanted more out of the relationship than I wanted to give. 16. I wanted to start seeing another (others) who was (were) more attractive. 17. The partner's behaviors and/or personality was more to blame for the break up than anything else 18. I made many more investments to the relationship than my partner. 19. My partner wanted a more serious relationship than I did. 20. I wanted to start seeing another (others) who was (were) more popular. We have tried to provide a number of reasons that may have led up to the break up. If you felt there were reasons for why you broke up that we didn't list, please take the opportunity to state these reasons in the space provided for below. Thank you. (Use the back of this page if you want more space.) 64 Part III. How the break up was executed. People use a number of ways to break off the relationships they have. Some people break off relationships by never seeing the partner again, never returning phone calls, avoiding the person, etc. Others tell their partners of their desires to break up or to see less of each other. Some examples are: "I'm going to date others and I think you should also," OR "I'm really changing inside and I don't feel good about our relationship anymore. I think we'd better stop seeing so much of each other," OR "It is unfair on my part and would be unfair to you to continue this relationship if one of us had to fake it. I care a great deal about you but I don't feel as strongly as I used to. I think it would be wise if we stopped seeing quite so much of each other." Instructions for this part of the questionnaire continue to the next page. Thank you! On occasion, people may use several tactics over a period of time. For example, you may have decided to break off the relationship at (say) midterm—at which time you tried to avoid seeing the partner or reduced contact with the partner without having said anything to him/her. Later, you may have said something like: "Don't you think we are too young for a serious relationship? I think what would be best for me is that we see other people and date around and then see if we wanted to be together." Still later the partner may not have dated others and continued to call you frequently. Then, as a third tactic you may have said something like: "Look, I don't want you to bother me anymore. We made a decision...." You annoy me. WE WOULD BE VERY APPRECIATIVE IF YOU REPORTED EACH STEP YOU USED TO BREAK OFF THE RELATIONSHIP (IF YOU USED MORE THAN ONE STEP). 65 In the space provided for below, please write, as best as you can remember, the process you went through to break off this relationship. TRY TO BE AS ACCURATE AND COMPLETE AS YOU CAN WHEN REPORTING WHAT YOU DID AND/OR SAID. (NOTE: If you said nothing to the partner when you broke off the relationship, simply write that you avoided the partner, or report how you went about trying to discourage future interactions. We appreciate your effortl) Another way to analyze messages people use is to have them rate the degree to which they used certain message elements or tactics in the messages they employed. The following set of questions represent statements that you could have made when you requested to break off the relationship with the partner (or used to discourage future interactions with the partner). For each statement, think about whether you had made a statement similar to the one in the question' and mark an X in the appropriate category for the 7-point scale. 1. I told him/her that I was very, very sorry about breaking off the relationship. I never did/said this 2. / / / / / / / I told him/her that I was going to date other people and that I though he/she should date others also. I never did/said this 3. / I definitely did/ said this to my partner / / / / / / / / 1 definitely did/ ^^^^ ^^^^ ^° ^^ partner I fully explained why I felt dissatisfied with the relationship, that it hasn't been growing and that I believe we will both be happier if we didn't date anymore. ^ ^ 1 '^fl^^"''l^ / / /_/_/_/_/—/ ^^^^ -^^^ partner I told him/her that there should be mutual love understanding in a relationship and that at the I didn't feel as close as I should. I then said I never did/said this ^^^^ ^° "^^ 4. and moment that I 66 think we should lay off awhile and see if we wanted to get back together. If we wanted to get back together, we wi11. I never did/said this 5. / / / / / / / / / / / / / I definitely did/ said this to my partner / / / / / / / / I definitely did/ said this to my partner I told him/her that life was too short and that we should date other people in order to enjoy life. I never did/said this 8. / I told him/her that I regretted very much having to break off the relationship. I never did/said this 7. / I didn't say anything to the partner, I avoided contact with him/her as much as possible. I never did/said this 6. / I definitely did/ said this to my partner / / / / / / / / I definitely did/ said this to my partner Without explaining my intentions to break off the relationship, I avoided scheduling future meetings with him/her. I never did/said this / / / / / / / / I definitely did/ said this to my partner 9. I told him/her that I cared very, very much for him/her. I never did/said this I definitely did/ / / / / / / / / said this to my partner 10. I fully explained how I felt and that I wanted to break things off. I explained that a relationship was no good unless it makes both people happy and that I wasn't happy and that I didn't want to date anymore. I never did/said I definitely did/ this / / / / / / / / said this to my partner PLEASE MAKE SURE THAT YOU HAVE ANSWERED EVERY ITEM. THANK YOU. 11. 67 I said that I was really changing inside and I didn't quite feel good about our relationship anymore. I said that we'd better stop seeing each other. I never did/said ^^^^ 12. I told him/her that I needed to be honest with him/her and suggested that we break it off for awhile and see what happens. I never did/said ^^^2 13. / /_/ /_/_/_/_/ I definitelv did/ said this tô my partner I told him/her that I wanted to be happy and that we should date other people. I never did/said this 15. /—/_/_/_/__/_/_/ I definitely did/ said this to my partner I never verbally said anything to the partner, but I discouraged our seeing each other again. I never did/said this 14. I—lU_/_/_/U_/ I definitely did/ said this to my partner I definitely did/ / / / / / / / / said this to my partner I told him/her that while I was happy most of the time I sometimes feltthat I can't do all the things I wanted to. I then said that we should call it quits for now and if we still wanted to get back together we wi11. I never did/said this I definitely did/ / / / / / / / / said this to my partner Part IV. Consequences of the break up. How did you feel about the break up during the time immediately after the breakup? For the following 15 questions, Write 1 in the space provided if you Strongly Agree with the statement, Write 2 if you Somewhat Agree, Write 3 if you Slightly Agree, Write 4 if you are Neutral or are Undecided, 58 Write 5 if you Slightly Disagree, Write 6 if you Somewhat Disagree, OR Write 7 if you Stronqly Disagree. _1. I felt extremely guilty. _2. I felt extremely lonely. _3. I felt extremely angry. _4. I felt extremely free. 5. My partner and I continued to see each other frequently. 6. 1 felt very much as if I had done the wrong thing. 7. I felt extremely depressed. 8. I felt verv mad. 9. I felt I could do all the things I want to do, but couldn't do while dating this partner. 10. My partner and I remained close after the breakup. 11. I felt as if I should have been more honest and open with the partner. 12. I felt extremely unhappy. 13. I felt very revengeful (wanted revenge). 14. I felt I was no longer tied down or constrained. 15. My partner and I still spent some time together. Are you a Male or Female (Circle one) How long did you and this partner date? (Try to answer in numbers of weeks. If one date, answer "1".)