FACULTY OF ENGLISH LANGUAGE AND LITERATURE ACADEMIC DISCOURSE 2015-2016 ASSIGNMENT (10/100) Deadline: December for section A–Kαρ (Instructor: A. Tzanne) 14th December for section Κασ–Παν (Instructor: B. Mitsikopoulou) 16th December for section Παξ–Ω (Instructor: E. Ifantidou) 17th PLEASE NOTE: No assignments will be accepted beyond the designated deadlines No assignments will be accepted via e-mail Assignments must be typed and not hand-written TASK 1 The following extract is from a chapter entitled Sign Language Acquisition and it has been written by Vivian Gramley (pages 187-204). This chapter appears in a book entitled Handbook of Communication Competence which has been edited by Gert Rickheit and Hans Strohner. The book was published in Berlin in 2008 by Mouton de Gruyter Publishers. Read the extract and: (a) prepare an outline of the characteristic features of sign languages presented in this text, (b) based on the outline, write a summary of approximately 100 words, and (c) provide a full bibliographical reference for the above chapter. Contrary to some people’s belief there is not one universal sign language. There is not one sign language that is shared by all deaf people in the world. There are many different sign languages which have evolved independently of each other. They are all very different but also share similar elements. The features that are shared by all sign languages is the existence of a lexicon and grammatical rules. The lexicons and grammatical rules are different for every sign language, comparable to spoken languages. Just like spoken language, sign languages differ in their historical relationships. A well-known example in this respect is the difference between American Sign Language (ASL) and British Sign Language (BSL). Although both languages are surrounded by the same spoken language, English, these two sign languages are historically very different and mutually unintelligible (Emmorey 2002: 2). French Sign Language and ASL, on the other hand, are more alike due to the influence of French Sign Language on American Deaf people in the eighteenth and early nineteenth centuries, when Laurent Clerc and Thomas Gallaudet, two French teachers of the Deaf, established the first public school for the Deaf in the United States. Nowadays, the two sign languages are clearly distinct languages (Lane 1984) sharing lexical features. 1 Another misbelief is that sign languages are made up of pictorial signs, similar to pantomime, and are not as expressive as a spoken language. This is not true. Sign languages have an intricate structure and enable the speakers/signers to express complex ideas just like speakers of spoken languages do. At times it seems difficult to describe a sign language linguistically because hearing linguists, who usually are the ones who describe sign languages, have to learn not only to look for features in a sign language that they know from spoken languages but recognize and acknowledge that sign languages also use a different modality, namely the visual one. When taking a closer look at sign languages it becomes obvious that grammatical rules and lexicons are only two general features that sign languages have in common, even though each language has its own set of rules and its own lexicon. Above and beyond this, however, sign languages share the use of movement, handshape, position of the hands, as well as simultaneity. The main difference that can be seen between a signed and a spoken language is its mode. While spoken languages use articulatory organs located in the vocal tract in order to produce sounds, sign languages use a completely different set of articulators, namely the hands and the face. While spoken languages are organized linearly in time, sign languages make use of simultaneity and space. Simultaneity is the expression of more than one lexical and grammatical feature at the same time – meaning that different elements are incorporated in one sign, similar to polysynthetic languages (Bellugi and Klima 1982). Furthermore, spoken languages are perceived with the ear while sign languages are perceived with the eye. Needless to say, spoken languages also employ manual and facial gestures but do not rely solely on these for the transmission of lexical information. TASK 2 The following article appears in the newspaper The Independent and reports on recent scientific research findings concerning the effects of worrying on people waiting for important results. Read it carefully and extracting all the necessary and relevant scientific information, adopt the identity of one of the academics who conducted the research (Kate Sweeny and her team) and write the Abstract for a research paper on this topic (also suggesting a title). The research paper will be published in an academic journal and therefore your Abstract will have to exhibit the appropriate discourse style and structure (about 200 words). Worrying can be good for you, say scientists Specifically, you may be in a better position to deal with both good and bad news when it finally arrives By Shana Lebowitz The Independent, Thursday 12 November 2015 If after you've finished a job interview, you tend to bite your nails to shreds and mentally replay the questions you fumbled until you've convinced yourself that you absolutely, positively didn't get the job, take heart. You may have a leg up on everyone else. Specifically, you may be in a better position to deal with both good and bad news when it finally arrives. 2 That's according to new research led by Kate Sweeny, Ph.D. at the University of California, Riverside. Sweeny and her colleagues wanted to specifically investigate the effects of worrying while waiting for important, potentially life-changing results. So they looked at 230 law school graduates taking the July 2013 California bar exam, starting two weeks before the exam and ending soon after the results were released four months later. Study participants filled out questionnaires at several points throughout the study period, indicating things like how anxious they felt and how consumed they were thinking about the exam; how often they tried to distract themselves or hide their feelings; how confident they felt that they would pass the exam; and how much time they spent planning to cope with potential failure. Results suggested that there were three general strategies for dealing with the waiting period. While some people tried to distract themselves and pretend that they weren't freaking out, others tried to think of a silver lining to failure. The third group actively anticipated the possibility of failure. This last strategy is similar to what researchers call "defensive pessimism," or embracing and preparing for the worst while still hoping for the best. Eighty-five percent of participants passed the exam. The 33 people who failed were asked whether they could believe that they had failed and whether they could accept the situation. Those who passed were asked whether they felt relieved. Two fascinating findings emerged. First, participants' efforts to distract themselves and hide their feelings weren't particularly helpful at alleviating their anxiety. In fact, those who tried to distract themselves often ended up feeling more anxious. "Our findings suggest that the advice to 'just try to distract yourself' or to 'take your mind off it' is not necessarily a recipe for a distress-free waiting period," the authors write, adding that "perhaps a better alternative would be to simply experience and express emotions in a natural way without attempting to suppress them." Participants who had struggled through the waiting period were pleasantly surprised when they found out they'd passed. Second, worrying seemed to be productive. If they failed, participants who had ruminated and acted pessimistic during the waiting period "were more motivated to spring into action, presumably with an eye on retaking the exam at the next available opportunity," the researchers write. On the other hand, people who tried to stay positive and optimistic "responded to news of failure with a sense of disbelief and denial." If they passed, participants who had struggled through the waiting period were more pleasantly surprised, while participants who had made it through easily were relatively underwhelmed. The researchers say their findings "reveal an emotional trade-off." Whether you're waiting to hear back about a job or an academic exam, worrying for a few weeks or months may predict a better experience receiving the results. In other words, don't worry about worrying; know that you're engaging in a totally natural process that may even be productive in the long run. 3