Running Head: RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS Relational Dialectics in Roommate Relationships Clarissa Hunter Huntington University 1 RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 2 Abstract Relational dialectics is often examined within the context of romantic relationships. However, in their theory, Baxter and Montgomery specify that it can also apply to close friendships and family relationships. The theory gives three central dialectics: integration vs. separation, expression vs. nonexpression, and stability vs. change. The purpose of this study was to discover the value of these dialectics within the context of roommates. The research questions and hypothesis are as follows: RQ1: Which dialectic is most valued in a roommate relationship? RQ2: Does the gender of the roommates affect the value of the three dialectics? H1: The openness-closedness dialectic will be most valued in shorter roommate relationships and the connection-autonomy and certainty-uncertainty dialectics will be more valued in longer relationships. This study answers these questions via self-report data from students at a small, Midwestern university. Using descriptive statistics, it was found that relational dialectics are very apparent in roommate relationships. RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 3 Relational Dialectics in Roommate Relationships When roommates first meet, they do their best to immediately assess how their relationship will pan out as they spend each day cohabitating. This is especially true in college, where freshmen and their roommates are often given worksheets and guides to help them figure out how to relate to one another. During orientation weekend at our university, my roommate and I went through this very ordeal. When we finished moving in all of our things and getting settled into our new room, we sat on the floor, looking over the “Get to Know Your Roommate” handouts given to us by our Resident Assistants. On it, there were questions about how we handled conflict, how much we would like to disclose to each other, and other questions about our general living patterns. As our freshmen year went on, my roommate and I proved our answers to those questions discussed on that first day to be both true and completely false. There was no way that we could anticipate the many situations and emotions that would arise from living in a small space with someone new while dealing with the stresses and joys of school, family, friends, and jobs. There were times when we bonded over games and knitting, and times we chose to spend alone. There were times that we stuck to our routines and times we went and had impromptu picnics together. There were times we listened to each other’s troubles, joys, and sadness, and times we kept our cares to ourselves. The dynamics of this relationship show the contradictory nature of all close relationships that have often been studied in dating or marriage. However, these dynamics have not yet been studied in roommate relationships. Since roommate relationships have an impact on college students’ social and academic success, the way the relationship works should be examined (Duran & Zakahi, 1988). No relationship flows in one steady stream; there is an ebb and flow between two opposite desires whenever people in close relationships attempt to relate to one RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 4 another. Navigating those areas well is how relationships grow and strengthen. They also impact relational satisfaction. What are the main contradictory influences that pull and push relational parties in separate or joint directions? Do they change over time as relationships lengthen? What is their impact on roommate relationships like that of mine and my roommate’s? These questions will be the central focus of this paper. Setting out to examine roommate relationships in the context of relational dialectics, a communication theory by Baxter and Montgomery (1996; 1998), this paper will examine the contradictions present in close relationships, which ones are most prominent, and whether time or gender is a determining factor in their prevalence between roommates. Literature Review Relational dialects came about because of a perceived lack of insight in the communications field as it pertains to the study of relationships. Baxter and Montgomery felt that contradictions ever present in relationships were overlooked and not addressed in an appropriate manner. The field that did study this, however, was dialectics. As Baxter and Montgomery examined this field, they began to see a new way of looking at relationships (1996, p. xiii). The field of dialectics goes as far back as the ancient Greek philosophers, but Baxter and Montgomery built their theory around the “dialogism theory” of Mikhail Bakhtin, a Russian cultural theorist (1895-1975) who studied dialogue between people as a unified yet separate process (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996). Contrary to many other scholars, Bahktin described social life as “an open dialogue characterized by multivocality and the indeterminacy inherent when those multiple voices interpenetrate” (Baxter, 2004, p. 2). Thus, dialogism holds central to dialogue the fact that communicators must be at the same time fused with one another while also RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 5 each contributing their own perspective to the situation or conversation. Dialogue cannot be solely an independent or a dependent process—it must be both. This same dynamic interplay of unity versus difference is fundamental in Baxter and Montgomery’s relational dialectics (1996). Relational dialectics, built on Bahktin’s work, sees relating as a process of ongoing flux, it emphasizes change in the relationship. There is no equilibrium as long as two people are both engaged in the relationship—“relating is a complex knot of contradictory interplays” (Baxter, 2004, p. 8). In relational dialectics, this is not seen as detrimental to the relationship. Unlike many theories, such as uncertainty reduction theory (e.g.—Uncertainty Reduction Theory; see Griffin (2012), pp. 125-137 for a summary) or social penetration theory (e.g.—Social Penetration Theory; see Griffin (2012), pp. 113-124 for a summary), relational dialectics does not simply examine one side of relating by focusing on just openness, certainty, and connection factors that supposedly make relationships stronger. Instead, these factors only have meaning “by examining the broader contradictions of which they are a part” (Baxter, 1990, p. 70). Relationships must be examined by looking at the push and pull of contradictions on each other. Looking at openness, certainty, and connection also requires looking at their opposites: closedness, uncertainty, and autonomy. Before looking at the main dialectics Baxter and Montgomery outline, however, the four concepts that relational dialectics builds upon must be examined. These concepts— contradiction, change, praxis, and totality—are present in every dialectical contradiction. Without understanding these, relational dialectics would be very difficult to grasp. Contradiction and Change Contradiction is essentially a unity between opposites where two irreconcilable concepts are at the same time interdependent and simultaneously present. There are many different “oppositions” that could take place. Oppositions must be “actively compatible yet mutually RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 6 negate one another” (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996, p. 8). In essence, things that are inherently contradictory must go together—“the forces complete a whole that is incomplete without one or the other” (Montgomery, 1993, p. 207). These contradictions are not damaging to relationships, but are constantly ingrained in and shaping relationships. Contradictions can be either individuallevel—within the mind of someone in the relationship, or relationship level—occurring between the members of the relationship (Baxter & Montgomery, 1998). Change refers to the ongoing fluidity of relationships. Relationships are never constantly the same, but always re-shaping in response to the contradictions within them. Thus, the contradictions make it essential for relationships to be in a constant state of change. Baxter and Montgomery state, “It is not useful conceptually to separate change from contradiction because it is the interplay or tension of opposites that results in ongoing fluidity for any relationship” (1998, p. 7). Contradiction cannot exist without change. Praxis and Totality Praxis is the action-reaction part of relationships. Baxter and Montgomery state that praxis is the principle that “people are at once actors and objects of their own actions” (1996, p. 13). This category explains how people respond to the contradictions taking place within their relationships. Praxis includes the strategies that are employed to deal with relational contradictions in the social world. Totality is “the assumption that phenomena can be understood only in relation to other phenomena” (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996, p. 14). In the field of dialectics, totality focuses on three things: where contradictions are located, interdependencies among contradictions, and contextualization of contradictory interplay. The location of contradictions refers to the fact that dialectical contradictions are not independently experienced phenomena; they occur because of RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 7 interaction with others. Because of this, in many cases, contradictions can be viewed separately by each party but are not the equivalent of interpersonal conflict (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996). Interdependencies among contradictions refer to the fact that contradictions can be either internal or external. Internal contradictions take place within the relationship—between the parties of the relationship. External contradictions are between the relationship and the society in which it occurs. According to Baxter and Montgomery, “couples and society sustain a relationship of sorts, and in so doing they engage inherent contradictions of such relationships” (1996, p. 16). Internal and external contradictions relate in certain ways. For example, a couple’s standard for how they should act in a relationship can influence the way that they handle the dialectical pulls that take place (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996). Finally, the contextualization of dialectical interplay is in reference to the fact that these contradictions in relationships have to be viewed in the context in which they take place. Contradictions must be studied “at both universal and particular levels” (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996, p. 17). Within relational dialectics, Baxter and Montgomery identify three major contradictions with both external and internal fronts. These contradictions are: integration-separation, stabilitychange, and expression-nonexpression (Baxter, 2004). (A) Integration-separation includes (A1) connection versus autonomy on the internal front and (A2) inclusion versus seclusion on the external front. (B) Stability versus change includes (B1) certainty versus uncertainty on the internal front and (B2) conventionality versus uniqueness on the external front. Finally, (C) Expression versus nonexpression includes (C1) openness versus closedness on the internal front and (C2) revelation versus concealment on the external front (Griffin, 2012). These tensions are not necessarily acknowledged and expressed within a relationship. Montgomery states, “They may work backstage in a relationship beyond partners’ mindful awareness or ability to identify RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 8 and describe them, but still contributing to a sense of unsettledness or instability in a relationship” (Montgomery, 1993, p. 206). Integration-Separation Connection-autonomy is what Baxter identifies as the primary struggle on the internal front of relationships. Obviously, there can be no relationship without connection between the relational parties, however, with excessive connection, the individuality of each person involved will be lost (Baxter, 1990). This contradiction can be manifested in everyday things, such as how much time is spent together or apart, how many things each wants to share with the other, or how many activities they choose to participate in alone or together. Baxter asserts that the timemanagement dilemma—deciding how much time to spend together or on other obligations— seems to be “particularly salient in romantic and friendship relationships” (Baxter, 2004, p. 9). This tension may be viewed by relationship participants in different lights. Baxter and Montgomery write, “relationship partners are quite likely to be in various degrees of synchrony at any given moment with respect to their perceptions of the connection-separation dynamic” (1996, p. 98). Because of this fact, if this contradiction is not recognized and managed appropriately, it can cause harm to the relationship. Inclusion-seclusion is the external struggle of a relationship within its surrounding community. It takes time alone together as a couple, friendship, or family to solidify such relationships and make them strong. For example, the things shared in intimate conversations are rarely shared within a large group. Privacy is essential for intimacy. However, it is impossible for a relationship to exist in a space all-its-own. Every relationship must be able to function both separately and within society. If relationship parties do not figure out how to navigate their private and public lives, the relationship will suffer (Griffin, 2012). RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 9 Stability-Change Certainty-uncertainty refers to the need of both predictability and spontaneity in relationships—“People require stability for predictability so they know what to expect and how to behave. However, people require variation as well. Relationships must have degrees of stability for people to coordinate their behavior and accomplish joint goals” (Baxter & Montgomery, 1998, p. 127). If there is too much certainty in a relationship, though, it can lead to a dulling of emotion between relational parties (Baxter & Simon, 1993). In essence, being able to predict every part of the relationship can eventually lead to boredom. On the other hand, if the relationship contains too much uncertainty and spontaneity, there would not be the consistency necessary to construct a solid close relationship. That said, certainty-uncertainty is a dialectic that that can only be “assessed in terms of some other aspect of the relationship” (Baxter & Montgomery, 1998, p. 126). Because of that, this contradiction can show up in a variety of facets within the relationship, such as predicting the other’s personality, beliefs, attitudes, or behavior towards making plans, the emotional excitement of romance, or the state of the relationship (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996). Conventionality-uniqueness is the external tension that appears between the relationship and the society in which it exists. There are certain relational constructs created by societal influence that may clash with the reality of the relationship. According to Baxter and Montgomery, “relationship partners have been socialized with cultural norms and idealizations of relationships that invariably are challenged in the unique, concrete practices of everyday relating” (1996, p. 124). Essentially, the real relationship will never be just like the picture people hold cognitively or what the culture around them expects—this can create a pull back and forth between society’s relational expectations and the characteristics of the real relationship. RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 10 Expression-Nonexpression Openness-closedness is “the tension experienced between the two partners in what to say and what not to say to one another” (Baxter & Simon, 1993, p. 228). This dialectic focuses on the intricacies between self-disclosure and keeping information private within a personal relationship. Self-disclosure can include such things as personal facts, feelings, opinions, and judgments, as well as informative statements or evaluations of the relationship (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996). While self-disclosure is important in building a relationship or friendship, there may be some things that the parties chose not to share. There is a pull between being vulnerable by expressing themselves and avoiding the risk involved in sharing their private or important information with another. On a related note, this dialectic can also show up between a person’s desire to be honest about their evaluations of the other and wanting to preserve the other person if the message may hurt them (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996). Conflict can occur when “one party wants to disclose and the other doesn’t want to listen, or when one party doesn’t want to disclose and the other wants to receive such disclosure” (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996, p. 141). As with the other contradictions, if openness and closedness are not managed, they could bring harm to the relationship. Revelation-concealment involves public disclosure between a personal relationship and those around them. In romantic relationships this would be very strong when determining whether or not to tell others about the relationship. This can happen in other ways as well. For example, when a couple is going to have a baby, they typically wait for what they think is the opportune time to tell others. If they did not tell those around them, it could result in considerable amounts of confusion for them and those surrounding them. Often, the couple may not want to tell everyone that they are having a baby right away, however, “community members RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 11 need to know about relationship realities in order to respond to them” (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996, p. 173). Just as there is risk in self-disclosure, there is risk in sharing relational information with society; the couple has to navigate what to reveal and what to conceal. Research on Relational Dialectics Many studies have been done to define how the contradictions posed by Baxter and Montgomery appear in relationships and how they are managed. Although no studies have been done on relational dialectics and roommate relationships specifically, Baxter conducted one examining relational dialectics in relationship development (1990). In this study, the dialectical contradictions of connection-autonomy, certainty-uncertainty, and openness-closedness were examined retrospectively throughout relationship stages. The study sought to discover whether the length of the relationship affected which dialectical contradictions were present. It also examined which response strategies were employed to deal with the contradictions. Baxter found that the three dialectics were not equally prevalent in all stages of relationships. In beginning stages, the openness-closedness dialectic was most common. As the relationships developed, openness-closedness tended to fade to the background and connection-autonomy and certaintyuncertainty became the primary dialectical contradictions (Baxter, 1990). Thus, based on this research, as the length of a relationship expands and people get to know each other better, openness-closedness becomes less pressing and connection-autonomy and certainty-uncertainty become stronger tensions. Baxter and Simon (1993), examined how the three main “contradictions of relating mediate the relationship between satisfaction and perceived partner maintenance strategies” (p. 225). Testing preservation strategies in relationships and perceived partner satisfaction during the dialectical tensions of connection-autonomy, certainty-uncertainty, and openness-closedness, RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 12 Baxter and Simon found that when the relationship was dominated by one pole of these contradictions, maintenance strategies using the other pole were most effective. This was discovered to be true in autonomy, predictability, and closedness dominated moments. For example, when one partner feels that the relationship is too autonomous and perceives that their partner is responding by increasing contact, this builds relationship satisfaction (Baxter & Simon, 1993). This information is important for seeing the effects of relational contradictions on relationship satisfaction in any relationship. Research on Roommate Relationships Roommate relationships have been examined in a variety of contexts. Duran and Zakahi, (1988) examined the connection between communication competence and roommate satisfaction. Results indicate that roommate satisfaction has an impact on college performance and enjoyment and that communicative competence has an impact on roommate relationships. Moreover, this study found “strong evidence for a causal relationship between communicative competence and roommate satisfaction” (p. 142). Communicative competence in this framework means communicative satisfaction—the ability to perceive and fulfill the communicative expectations of another—and communicative adaptability—the ability to predict another’s response and adjust communication appropriately (Duran & Zakahi, 1988). Thus, roommates were more likely to room together a second time when they found satisfaction in their communication and were able to adapt to the communicative needs of the other. Martin and Anderson, (1995) examined roommate similarity and communication traits. This study asserts that in roommate relationships “communication strategies and communication traits are a key factor in determining personal satisfaction with the other” (p. 46). Looking at willingness to communicate, interpersonal communication competence, and verbal aggression, RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 13 this study examined similarities in roommate communication and the connection between these similarities and roommate satisfaction. It found that when roommates are both willing to communicate or are high in their ability to interpersonally communicate, they express satisfaction. On the other hand, as would be expected, if roommates are high in verbal aggressiveness, satisfaction does not occur (Martin & Anderson, 1995). Further studies were done in the context of bettering the practices used to match roommates together. Erlandson (2009)—similar to the studies previously mentioned—looked at the similarity of interpersonal needs between roommates and their effect on roommate satisfaction. Using a survey, this study assessed the roommates’ needs for inclusion, openness, and control and then measured the relationship of those needs to the satisfaction between roommates. The results found that only control had a significant relationship to satisfaction; when both roommates were low in their need for control, there was more satisfaction in the relationship than if both had a high need for control (Erlandson, 2009). A second study by Erlandson (2012) examined the relationship between nonverbal behavior and relationship satisfaction amongst roommates. Looking at immediacy (nonverbals signaling approachability or liking) and territoriality (the use and division of shared living space), this study sought to find out if similarity in these traits led to satisfaction in roommate relationships. Observational methods to determine immediacy and survey research methods to determine territoriality were used in this study. Coders of the immediacy observation looked at proxemics distancing, body orientation, altercentrism, body relaxation, and positive affect (Erlandson, 2012). The data gathered indicated that roommates who were both high in territoriality had less satisfaction. With regards to immediacy, only positive affect seemed to have a strong correlating relationship with roommate satisfaction (Erlandson, 2012). RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 14 Though these studies do not look at roommate relationships in the context of relational dialectics, they do offer insight into the dynamics between roommates. Clearly, communication is an important factor in the satisfaction and maintenance of roommate relationships. The first study hints on a few key qualities of relational dialectics in its examination of communicative adaptability. With the constant push and pull of contradictions in relationships, the ability to sense the other person’s inclination and communicate accordingly is important and constant. The purpose of this study is to observe the way the dialectics posed by Baxter and Montgomery appear in the relationships of college roommates and their impact on roommate satisfaction in relation to the length of time they have roomed together, the gender of the dyad. Specifically, this study will examine the internal dialectics of connection-autonomy, opennessclosedness, and certainty-uncertainty. Much research has been done on these dialectics in the context of romantic and familial relations, but not on roommates. Similarly, much research has been done on roommates, but not in the context of relational dialectics. According to the research, all three dialectics occur in romantic or familial relations (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996). Further, the contradictions have shown various amounts of strength at different stages in the relationships (Baxter, 1990). Thus, the type and length of the roommate relationship may indicate which contradictions appear and how strongly. The research questions are as follows: RQ1: Which dialectic is most valued in a roommate relationship? RQ2: Does the gender of the roommates affect the value of the three dialectics? The hypothesis is as follows: H1: The openness-closedness dialectic will be most valued in shorter roommate relationships and the connection-autonomy and certainty-uncertainty dialectics will be more valued in longer relationships. RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 15 Method Subjects/Participants The participants for this study are 98 students from a small, private, Midwestern university. There were 100 surveys passed out, but only 98 were returned. There were 61 females and 36 males. The participants were selected via convenience, purposive and cluster sampling. Anyone participating in this study was required to have a roommate in order to fill out the survey. Procedure For the purpose of this study, roommates were considered people one shares a room with—not simply a house or an apartment. After confirming that participants had roommates, they were given a survey to fill out with questions pertaining to their roommate relationships. The survey is structured to assess the three dialectics of connection-autonomy, certaintyuncertainty, and openness-closedness in roommate relationships. These dialectics show the inherent contradictions present in close relationships. To determine which dialectic occurs most strongly in roommate relationships, this survey asked a series of behavior questions on a 5-point Likert scale. The points on the scale ranged from low to high: “Strongly disagree,” “Disagree,” “Neutral,” “Agree,” and “Strongly agree.” For each question, one side of the scale was geared toward one side of the dialectic and the other side of the scale was geared toward the other dialectic. The scale will illustrate the participants’ behavior regarding the varying dialectics. For example, depending on which part of the scale they circle, they could be either more autonomous than connected or vice versa— this applies to the other two dialectics as well. The survey also contained some attitude questions that used a 5-point Likert scale to assess participants’ preferences towards the different poles of the dialectical contradictions. The attitude questions RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 16 serve to help reinforce the behavior questions and show which dialectic is most valued. The survey also had one question about the length of the roommate relationship to measure whether the dialectical tensions are equally present in various stages of the relationship. Finally, to determine whether gender has an influential effect on which dialectics are valued, the participants were asked to mark their gender at the end of the survey. This survey accurately detects the preference of the dialectics because it not only looks at behavior but attitude as well. Additionally, the structure of the questions is fixed so that there are reliability checks in several places throughout. This set-up prevents someone from simply working through the entire survey and circling one side of the scale for every question—that would mean that they have not paid attention. Finally, the survey answers the relationship length and gender questions necessary for RQ2 and H1 by having participants identify both of these items. Descriptive statistics are used for data analysis. For an example survey, see appendix. Results The research questions and hypothesis are as follows: RQ1: Which dialectic is most valued in a roommate relationship? RQ2: Does the gender of the roommates affect the value of the three dialectics? And H1: The openness-closedness dialectic will be most valued in shorter roommate relationships and the connection-autonomy and certainty-uncertainty dialectics will be more valued in longer relationships. It was discovered that RQ1 and RQ2 ultimately produced similar results, regardless of gender. H1 was partially supported but not fully corroborated. In answer to RQ1, the most favored dialectic was discovered to be the dialectic of openness-closedness with a mean score of 4.18 on a 5-point Likert scale. Certainty-uncertainty was the second most valued dialectic with a mean score of 3.85. Connection-autonomy was the least valued with a mean score of 3.57. These results are shown in Chart 1. RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 17 Chart 1 The Most Valued Dialectic of Total Sample Additionally, based on the preference questions, the poles of connection, openness, and certainty were more valued than their opposites—especially in the case of connection versus autonomy. Closedness was the least preferred, followed by autonomy, and then certainty. These results are shown in Chart 2. Chart 2 Oder of Preferences toward Each Dialectical Pole RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 18 For RQ2, the data found that gender does not affect the order of preferences for dialectics. Males and females both preferred openness-closedness above the other two dialectics. Females had a mean score of 4.34 and males had a mean score of 3.91 on a 5-point Likert scale. The other two dialectics ranked the same as they did for the entire sample: certainty-uncertainty second and connection-autonomy third. This data is shown in Chart 3. Chart 3 The Most Valued Dialectic of Males versus Females Though both sexes preferred the same dialectic, there was statistical significance between how much they valued the dialectics. The females scored significantly higher than the males on their openness-closedness preference, ANOVA (F= 8.735; 1, 96, p= 0.004). There was also statistical significance in the preference of certainty-uncertainty between females and males such that females preferred this dialectic more than males, ANOVA (F= 10.381; 1, 96, p= 0.002). Though both sexes value the dialectics, they seem more important to females overall. H1 was only partially supported. While the data did support that the openness-closedness dialectic was most valued in shorter relationships, it was also the strongest dialectic in longer relationships. The order of the other two dialectics was also the same throughout the length of RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 19 the relationship with certainty-uncertainty being the second most valued and connectionautonomy the least valued. These results are shown in Chart 4. Chart 4 The Most Valued Dialectic According to Relationship Length Discussion The purpose of this study was to determine whether relational dialectics are valued in roommate relationships in the same way that they are valued in romantic and family relationships. Moreover, this study looked at whether the duration of the relationship or sex of the roommates affects which dialectic is most valued. There was no previous research done on relational dialectics in this particular area. The data from this study shows that the three dialectics of connection-autonomy, openness-closedness, and certainty-uncertainty are all present in roommate relationships, supporting the view of Baxter and Montgomery that relational dialectics occurs in all close relationships. The most valued dialectic, according to this study— regardless of length of relationship or gender— is openness-closedness, followed by certaintyuncertainty, and then connection-autonomy. The openness, certainty, and connection sides of the dialectics were all more valued than their opposites, showing a preference for advancement of RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 20 the relationship and potentially adding support for theories such as the Uncertainty Reduction Theory and the Social Penetration Theory that look at relational development. Based on previous research on relational dialectics in romantic relationships done by Baxter, this study hypothesized that as the roommate relationship progressed, the most valued dialectic would change from openness-closedness at first to certainty-uncertainty and connection-autonomy once the roommates had become better acquainted later in the relationship. Since openness-closedness was the most preferred dialectic regardless of the length of the relationship, this hypothesis was not completely supported. This finding could mean that the preferred dialectic only changes in relationships of a certain nature— or that the study itself could not accurately assess the hypothesis posed. Either factor might be at play in the results, but the true cause cannot be determined by the research at hand. This study contained several limitations. One such limitation was the amount of participants. There would be a larger predictive validity to this research if there was a greater and more varied sample being tested. Most of the participants in this study marked that they considered their relationships “healthy” on the survey, but a greater dynamic between dialectics would be shown in some unhealthy roommate relationships. Another limitation to this study is that the data gathered was self-report and therefore subject to a certain amount of error. Finally, this study only assessed the participants at the current point in their roommate relationship. Had the study been longitudinal, examining the participants over a length of time, there might have been a greater difference shown between longer and shorter roommate relationships and perhaps the hypothesis would have been fully supported. If this study were to be replicated, an improved research tactic and methodology would create more comprehensive results. A future examination of relational dialectics in roommate RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 21 relationships would produce more in-depth results if a longitudinal study was conducted, examining the roommates’ preferences at different points in the relationship, throughout their time rooming together. This study lacked richness because it only examined each relationship at its current point from one side of the relationship, not over a span of time and from both sides. A future study, to provide greater insight into the impact and value of the dialectics, would need to assess each roommate and the satisfaction between the roommates as a pair; both roommates should fill out the survey and the researcher should analyze their results as compared to each other. Additionally, a future study should include more nuanced questions to gauge both sides of each dialectic more fully. Lastly, future studies should also examine the maintenance strategies determined by Baxter and Montgomery and how those work between roommates. Would it be different than in romantic relationships? Examination of relational dialectics in roommate relationships, and maintenance strategies associated with them may provide beneficial information for college housing departments when creating policies and dealing with roommate conflict. Studies like this one would assist universities in better understanding the dynamics of roommate relationships and help roommates to recognize the tensions and contradictions occurring in their own relationships. Once people understand what is happening, they will be more fully equipped to manage potential struggle and create a healthier relationship overall. In short, research on relational dialectics in roommate relationships could be a powerful communication aid and resource. RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 22 Appendix Roommate Relationship Survey Directions: Circle the best answer that applies. Please do not discuss this survey with your roommate until you have both finished taking it. Note: You must currently have a roommate to participate in this research. 1. How long have you lived with your roommate? Less than 6 months 6 to 12 months 13 months to 2 years 2 plus years 2. Most of the activities (hobbies or interests) that I participate in involve my roommate. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 3. How often do you and your roommate participate in mutual activities? Once a day Once a week Once a month Once every several months Not applicable 4. In reference to question 3, I appreciate the amount of time that I and my roommate spend on these activities. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 5. I am involved in most of the activities (hobbies or interests) that my roommate participates in. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 6. There are certain activities (hobbies or interests) that I and my roommate participate in together outside of our room. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 7. How often do you participate in these shared activities? Once a day Once a week Once a month Once every several months 8. I enjoy having time away from my roommate. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree Not applicable RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 23 9. My relationship with my roommate would be better if we spent more time with each other. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 10. My roommate shares personal information with me. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 11. In reference to question 10, it benefits our relationship when my roommate shares personal information with me. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 12. I often share personal information with my roommate. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 13. In reference to question 12, it benefits our relationship when I share personal information with my roommate. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 14. My relationship with my roommate would be better if we were more open with each other. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 15. I would prefer that my roommate and I not discuss personal issues. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 16. My roommate and I have established routines (sleeping schedules, cleaning, etc.). Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 17. I appreciate having routines in my roommate relationship. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree Agree Strongly agree 18. I rarely know the schedule of my roommate. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 19. I like to know what to expect from my roommate. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 20. In my roommate relationship, I prefer unity. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 21. In my roommate relationship, I prefer to be separate from my roommate. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 22. In my roommate relationship, I prefer we share information with each other. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 23. In my roommate relationship, I prefer to keep to myself. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 24. In my roommate relationship, I prefer predictability. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 25. In my roommate relationship, I prefer spontaneity. Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 26. I consider my roommate relationship healthy. Strongly disagree 27. I am… Male Female Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 24 RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 25 References Baxter, L. A. (1990). Dialectical contradictions in relationship development. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 69-88. doi: 10.1177/0265407590071004 Baxter, L. A. (2004). Relationships as dialogues. Personal relationships, 11, 1-22. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2004.00068.x Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B.M. (1996). Relating: Dialogues and dialectics. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (Eds.) (1998). Dialectical approaches to studying personal relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Baxter, L. A., & Simon, E. P. (1993). Relationship maintenance strategies and dialectical contradictions in personal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 225-242. doi: 10.1177/026540759301000204 Duran, R. L., & Zakahi, W. R. (1988). The influence of communicative competence upon roommate satisfaction. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 52, 135-146. doi: 10.1080/10570318809389631 Erlandson, K. (2009). Similarity of interpersonal needs and roommate satisfaction. Journal of College and University Student Housing, 36(2), 10-23. Erlandson, K. (2012). Stay out of my space! Territoriality and nonverbal immediacy as predictors of roommate satisfaction. Journal of College and University Student Housing, 38(2), 46-61. Griffin, E. (2012). Relational dialectics. A first look at communication theory (8th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. RELATIONAL DIALECTICS IN ROOMMATE RELATIONSHIPS 26 Martin, M. M., & Anderson, C. M. (1995). Roommate similarity: Are roommates who are similar in their communication traits more satisfied? Communication Research Reports, 12(1), 46-52. doi: 10.1080/08824099509362038 Montgomery, B. M. (1993). Relationship maintenance versus relationship change: A dialectical dilemma. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 205-223. doi: 10.177/026540759301000203