ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH Labovitz School of Business & Economics, University of Minnesota Duluth, 11 E. Superior Street, Suite 210, Duluth, MN 55802 Effect of Goals Salience and Goals Conflict on Typicality: the Case of "Health Food" Hiba El Dahr, Universite Montpellier SupAgro, France Fatiha Fort, Universite Montpellier SupAgro, France The aim of this article is to highlight the role of consumer’s goals, in this case the « health » goal, in the cognitive representations and perception of food products, in particular « health foods ». On the basis of the goal theories used in a categorical approach in information processing, we show that the salience of health goals has a significant influence on the perceived typicality of the products in the health food category. The consumption context and the overall hierarchy of the goals sought constitute moderating variables. [to cite]: Hiba El Dahr and Fatiha Fort (2008) ,"Effect of Goals Salience and Goals Conflict on Typicality: the Case of "Health Food"", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 35, eds. Angela Y. Lee and Dilip Soman, Duluth, MN : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 282-288. [url]: http://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/13344/volumes/v35/NA-35 [copyright notice]: This work is copyrighted by The Association for Consumer Research. For permission to copy or use this work in whole or in part, please contact the Copyright Clearance Center at http://www.copyright.com/. Effect of Goal Salience and Goal Conflict on Typicality: The Case of “Health Food” Hiba El Dahr, IAM, UMR-MOISA Montpellier, France Fatiha Fort, SupAgro, UMR-MOISA Montpellier, France ABSTRACT The aim of this article is to highlight the role of consumer’s goals, in this case the “health” goal, in the cognitive representations and perception of food products, in particular “health foods.” On the basis of the goal theories used in a categorical approach in information processing, we show that the salience of health goals has a significant influence on the perceived typicality of the products in the health food category. The consumption context and the overall hierarchy of the goals sought constitute moderating variables. INTRODUCTION For several years, following a trend already seen in North America and Japan, “health” argument has provided an opportunity to develop products on the French food market. The increasing number of product launches claiming to be “healthy” illustrates consumers’ interest in “alicaments” (medicated foods) and a growing awareness of better nutrition. Treating the question of diet as an integral part of a general public health policy, awareness campaigns are undertaken by the authorities to promote among the consumers products with high nutritional value. Whether it is a matter of individual worries (as a result of food safety crisis) or firms’ marketing concerns, the socio-economic context thus appears to favour products with a strong health image. However, faced with the profusion of supply on the one hand, and the diversity of consumers’ expectations on the other (health, pleasure, well-being, ethics, ecological concerns etc.), consumer’s choice is becoming more and more complex. The increasing number of views on nutrition provided by media, the medical profession and authorities reinforce this trend, while messages concerning products “health” value are often contradictory. Therefore, it is worth examining how the consumer perceives “health foods” according to his own motivations and personal expectations. We should therefore pay particular attention to personal goals as individual determinants of food choices linked with health. The interest in goals is justified not only by their cognitive dimension directed towards action, but also by their motivational and affective content whose role is supported by theoretical models of health behaviour (Self-Regulation Models of Carver & Scheier, 1982; Cognitive Social Theory of Bandura, 2004). In this article, we are concerned with the role of goals in mental representations and in the organization of information about products (specifically health foods) in a category-based framework of information processing. We propose therefore to define the mechanisms whereby personal goals (i.e. health goals) affect products’ perception within cognitive categories called “goal-derived categories” (Barsalou, 1983, 1991) depending on goals organization and on goal salience. We will use the concept of typicality as a key construct in categorization theory. Our purpose is to show that, in the “health food” category: – – with concrete products and behaviour. Within the theoretical accounts of categorization, typicality concept will be used in order to introduce health goals salience as an antecedent of typicality in the health food category (defined as a goal-derived category). The results of a quantitative survey will then be presented in order to test the research hypotheses. THEORETICAL BACKGROUNDS Goal theories assume that goals are organized within a hierarchical structure (Carver & Scheier, 1982) based on instrumentality links. Goals of lower order, closely linked to action levels, constitute concrete means for reaching higher, more abstract and more stable goals (Gutman, 1982). The closeness between goals and behaviour appears at three levels: through goal structure; goal accessibility and goal content. In fact, the hierarchical structure defines different goal’s levels of abstraction: abstract, central and operational (Bagozzi & Dabholkar, 1994), which exert a driving role on behaviour by guiding towards actions or towards concrete products which conform with goals. Goal accessibility (Higgins & King, 1981; Wyer & Srull, 1981; Förster et al., 2005) or goal “salience” (Ratneshwar et al., 2001) refers to the “level of awareness of the goal” through levels of attention and intention of working memory (Austin and Vancouver, 1996). Acting in a significant way on perception and cognitive representations (Ratneshwar et al., 2001), goal salience is a necessary condition for the commitment to behaviour, notably as regards health. Previous studies show that goal salience depends on motivation (Förster et al., 2005), on situation (Vallacher & Wegner, 1985; Ratneshwar et al., 2001), and on frequency and recency of prior activations (Förster et al., 2005). The notion of content of goals refers to classifications of outcomes or states that individuals approach or avoid in their behaviour according to the origin of these states (internal or external), their nature, or to their meaning. Although the number of goals that an individual is able to pursue is relatively limited (Austin & Vancouver, 1996), studies show that the pursuit of multiple goals may lead to goals conflict when these are of a contradictory or incompatible nature, which may have many consequences on behaviour. Two possibilities may therefore be envisaged as regards the use of goal theories in the field of consumer behaviour: – one is focused on motivation and its effect on behaviour, – the other corresponds to knowledge about products organization in memory. This aspect has been particularly developed by Barsalou (1991) who has shown that goals influence products’ cognitive representations and form “goal-derived categories” (example: category of products to eat when dieting). Goals will therefore be involved in categorization process and thus in consumers’ choice. the perception of product typicality is significantly influenced by health goals salience and by other competing goals pursued by the consumer, the influence of health goals salience on product typicality varies according to the consumption context (Figure 1). RESEARCH HYPOTHESES Goal theories will be explored in order to highlight the hierarchical organization of goals in memory and their instrumental links Typicality is a central concept in categorization theory. Cognitive psychology shows that, apart from its role in determining object membership and positioning within a category (Mervis & Rosch, 1981), there is a strong positive relationship between 282 Advances in Consumer Research Volume 35, © 2008 Advances in Consumer Research (Volume 35) / 283 FIGURE 1 The role of health goals in the health food category typicality and attitude (Loken & Ward, 1987; Nedungadi & Hutchinson, 1985), preferences, and the product choice within a category (Changeur & Chandon, 1995). Typicality judgments are very useful in understanding consumer’s choice. Much research has been carried out on variables determining the level of perceived typicality. Among these variables, the literature gives the following determinants: familiarity (Loken & Ward, 1990), frequency of instantiation (Barsalou, 1985), family resemblance, attributes structure (Loken & Ward, 1990), ideal (Barsalou, 1985) and coherence. Behind of these determinants of typicality, we postulate in this research that, in the case of goal-derived categories (Barsalou, 1991): H1: Pursued goals salience, which represents the ideal of the category determines products’ typicality. Thus, in the “health food” categories, products’ level of typicality is determined by the salience (and activation level) of health goals. In fact, according to Barsalou (1991), the ideal of the category influences both the category’s membership and its members’ typicality. Moreover, Förster et al. (2005) suggest that great accessibility of goals and the constructs related to them make it possible to detect the most effective environmental stimuli in the pursuit of these goals, thus improving the probability of their attainment. According to this point of view, goal salience seems to be correlated with typicality, insofar as the most typical members are, according to Barsalou (1983, 1991), those which best serve to reach the goal sought. In addition, cognitive psychology and marketing show that for each consumption situation, there are corresponding goals sought by the individual. The activation of situational goals occurs, according to Förster et al. (2005) under the effect of environmental variables which automatically trigger the process of their pursuit (Schwarz et al., 2003). Goals salience is thus influenced by the context, which can make certain goals more accessible and more prioritized than others. Furthermore, Ratneshwar et al. (1991; 1997) have shown that for each situational goal there are corresponding specific benefits which are rendered salient by situational factors, for which specific products can be used in order to attain fixed goals. Starting from the principle that, in a categorization context, products which best satisfy the category goal are the most typical, this suggests that typical products should correspond best with the consumption context in order to respond to the category objective. We therefore postulate that: H2: The influence of goal salience on products’ typicality in goal-derived categories is moderated by the appropriateness level of these products for the context. However typicality does not depend only on personal goal salience but also on the accessibility of the other goals in the hierarchy. Decision theories postulate that the individual gives more attention and subjective value to products capable to satisfy a need or a given goal as long as this need is active (Lewin, 1935; Klinger, 1975; Förster et al., 2005). From this point of view, attention to and usefulness of products vary according to the intensity or the importance of goals, which they serve (Brendl et al., 2003). Studies have focused on the influence of active goals on the attractiveness of products or behaviour related to these goals (Klinger, 1975; Brendl & Higgins, 1996; Ratneshwar et al., 1997; Ratneshwar et al., 2000; Brendl et al., 2000). Theses works suggest that the level of goal salience has an influence on product preference, and that a “devaluation effect” may be driven by products that are perceived as irrelevant as regards active goal (Brendl et al., 2003; Förster, et al., 2005). Moreover, the effect of active goals is reinforced by their protection against environmental stimuli which do not compete with their attainment (Brendl et al., 2000; Brendl et al., 2003; Förster, et al., 2005). Among these factors, we suggest that competing goals could affect main goal’s effect in the evaluation of products according to their level of importance or intensity. If we assume that, in goal-derived categories typicality is influenced by goal salience level, and by taking into account that a given individual is able to pursue several consumption goals simultaneously, we assume that: H3.a: The effect of goal salience on typicality is significant when competing goals are not highly rated H3.b: The effect of goal salience on typicality is not significant when at least one of the competing goals is highly rated If we take the case of health goals as an example, their effect (assumed to be significant) on typicality can be seen as follows (Figure 2). METHODOLOGY To test these hypotheses, we have chosen the health food category. The interest in food products is justified by their close link 284 / Effect of Goals Salience and Goals Conflict on Typicality: The Case of “Health Food” FIGURE 2 Conceptual model and research hypothesis TABLE 1 Products retained for the final experiment BREAKFAST Chocolate croissant1, Organic wholemeal bread, Orange, Pure fruit juice, Butter SPORT High-energy bars, Kiwi fruit, Chocolate bar, Fruit juice, Soda SNACKING Granola bar, Apple, Chocolate bar, Chips, Hamburger 1French pastry containing strips of chocolate with health goals and by the diversity of objectives, which underlie their consumption (biological, cultural, economic, social and psychological). An exploratory qualitative study was made as a first step among 22 individuals in order to identify perceptions of health foods. A lexical analysis showed that health goal could be broken down into three main subordinates goals: the need for a balanced diet, the prevention of illness and the desire for food security (in terms of health risks). The analysis also revealed 3 consumption contexts in which health goals were particularly salient: breakfast, sport and snacking context. Associated with these 3 situations, a list of 145 products was submitted to 3 judges to select products belonging to the health food category. 44 products were selected unanimously by the 3 judges to be used in a quantitative study in order to test our research hypotheses. 15 out of these products (Table 1) were retained for the final experiment (5 products per situation). 404 participants took part in the 2nd phase of the survey (the quantitative phase), spread uniformly over 3 experimental groups according to the usual socio-demographic criteria (sex, age, educational level and size of housing area). As a first step, the salience of health goals was evaluated in order to understand how this variable could influence food perception and product classification into categories. Thus, we submitted the participants to a product categorization task based on the work of Murphy and his colleagues (1999, 2001, 2003) and Berger & Bonthoux (2000) in cognitive psychology. This task succeeded in defining the nature of associations made among the products and in distinguishing the nutritional and health links (e.g. calcium and milk) of the taxonomic categorizations (e.g. milk and cheese). Another task, suggested by the work of Ratneshwar et al. (2001), was given to the participants in order to evaluate the degree of similarity between products pairs (on a 7-point scale (from “very different” to “very similar”). The authors had checked that perceived similarity between health-related products were correlated with health goal salience. Thus, when products sharing few common traits were judged similar as regards a given goal, this shows a high level of goal salience in the consumer’s cognitive representations. We also included in the measurement of health goals salience a motivational dimension which, according to Higgins et al. (1981), is an important determinant of goal accessibility which has been excluded from earlier research. We evaluated motivation in the tasks linked to health foods by way of a 7-point scale adapted from Maheswaran (1994), also used by Odou (2000), ranging from “don’t agree at all” to “agree completely”: “When I choose health foods: I read carefully the information on the pack I am keen to read the product description I am keen to find out more about the product” Finally, a score for health goals salience is obtained by the average of the measurements of motivation, similarity and associations between products. The suitability of products in the consumption context was measured on a scale adapted from Jean (2000) going from 1 (not at all suitable) to 7 (completely suitable) in response to the following question: “Please indicate to what extent each of the following products seems to you suitable for [context]’. In order to measure the hierarchy of goals pursued by consumers, we have made use of goal theories where objects and concrete Advances in Consumer Research (Volume 35) / 285 TABLE 2 Results of linear regressions of health goals salience as antecedent of the typicality in different contexts R2 t β Breakfast 55.44 % 1.97 0.209* Sport 15.88 % 0.52 0.159 Snacking 21.76 % 2.45 0.449* * significant at 5% level goals represent means of reaching abstract goals. So we have used some criteria sought in food products consumption in general, which reflect higher pursued goals, including “health”, namely: price, taste, freshness, origin, packaging, respect for the environment, ease of preparation, label and health. For each of the 3 studied contexts, we established a classification of these criteria according to their perceived importance by the participants. This variable (importance) was measured using an Osgood scale with 7 points, ranging from “not at all important” to “very important” in response to the following question: “Can you indicate to what extent you attach importance to each of the following criteria in your choice of foods?” Typicality was evaluated by the way of the representativeness of products as regards health food category. A 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all representative) to 7 (completely representative) was used in response to the following question: “In your opinion, to what extent is each of these products representative of the health food category?” RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Table 2 shows results of linear regressions of health goals salience as an antecedent of typicality in the 3 studied contexts. The regression model was as follows: Typicality=constant+salience+conformity+coherence+attributes structure+sharing of attributes+subjective expertise1+E where “salience” is the variable which describes the salience of health goals; “conformity” is the conformity of the products with the ideal of the category (Barsalou, 1985) or the degree to which products permit health goals attainment; “coherence” describes perceived coherence between products and category (Dawar & Anderson, 1994); “attributes structure” describes the degree to which products possess the category attributes (Odou, 2000); “sharing of attributes” describes the difference between products and category in the degree of possession of these attributes; “subjective expertise” refers to the perception that the individual has of his level of competence in the health food category. Although the prediction quality is lower in sport and snacking than in breakfast situation (where R2=55.68 %), a large part of typicality is correctly predicted by health goals salience. 1 According to our results expertise variable has no significant influence on typicality. The other variables used in the regression model are the classical typicality determinants in marketing and psychological literature. Regression coefficients are all positive but not significant in all contexts, notably in sport context (p=0.605). Hence, we find that health goal salience is not a significant determinant of typicality in every situation. Consequently, (H1) is only partially supported by theses findings. To test the moderating effect of products adequacy for the context, we first tested the distribution normality of this variable before dividing observations into two groups. Actually, it is assumed that normally distributed variables can be transformed into nominal variables with a threshold effect around the mean (obtained for the variable context adequacy). ANOVA analysis is then used to compare typicality effects in the two groups (Group 1, has considered products as inappropriate for the situation, and Group 2 as appropriate). ANOVA results (Table 3) show that the differences in typicality evaluation in the two groups were significant at the 5% level threshold in the 3 situations (p=0.05; 0.007 et 0.000 respectively). This result confirms that context adequacy of products has a moderating effect on their typicality in health food category, which partly explains the variability in the effect of health goal salience on typicality. In order to analyze the influence of goals conflict in the hierarchy, we established a classification of the different product criteria sought by participants in the 3 studied contexts (Table 4). We selected health competing factors according to their “perceived importance” ratings. Thus, we assumed as competing with health factors those that obtained a greater “importance” rating. According to this classification, we notice that, in breakfast situation, “taste” and “freshness” get better ratings (first and second position respectively) and thus seem to be more valued than health considerations (3rd position). They are therefore considered as competing with health concerns. By introducing “taste” and “freshness” concerns with health in the regression model, we were able to study the combined effects of all these factors on typicality. Table 5 shows that, in breakfast situation, “taste” and “freshness” variables has not a statistically significant influence on product typicality. However, health factor has a significant influence at the 5% threshold (p=0.030). Therefore, our (H3.a) hypothesis is consistently supported. Actually, sport situation allows us to analyze the influence of health criterion with interaction with highly valued competing criteria which have significant influence on the dependent variable (typicality). Table 5 shows that in this situation (sport), taste, freshness, environmental aspects and price are more valued than health goals (more highly rated by participants). Whereas health contribution is not significant in sport situation (p=0.881), taste variable (p=0.050) and respect for the environment (p=0.002) are significant at the 5% threshold. Freshness and price variables have 286 / Effect of Goals Salience and Goals Conflict on Typicality: The Case of “Health Food” TABLE 3 Differences in typicality ratings according to context adequacy (on 7-point scale) Adequacy Mean Typicality Mean ANOVA Group 1 Group 2 Group 1 Group 2 Sum of squares ddl F test Breakfast 3.86 5.99 4.94 5.24 2.80 1 p=0.050 Sport 4.16 5.59 4.94 5.24 4.87 1 p=0.007 Snacking 4.18 5.95 4.19 4.78 10.54 1 p=0.000 TABLE 4 Classification of criteria sought in food consumption according to their perceived importance Breakfast Sport Snacking Row Criterion Mean Criterion Mean Criterion Mean 1 Taste* 6.22 Taste* 6.02 Taste* 6.27 2 Freshness* 6.03 Freshness* 5.71 Freshness* 6.14 3 Health 5.12 Environment* 4.85 Health 5.23 4 Price 4.99 Price* 4.81 Environment 5.10 5 Environment 4.67 Health 4.64 Price 4.91 * Higher-order factors compared with health TABLE 5 Combined effects of the different food requirements sought on typicality β t Health 0.209* 1.94 Taste 0.028 1.12 Freshness 0.025 1.19 Health 0.013 0.15 Taste 0.175* 1.94 Freshness 0.091 0.95 Respect for the environment 0.293* 3.18 Price 0.116 1.38 Health 0.489* 1.54 Taste 0.130 1.18 Freshness 0.098 1.33 CRITERIA CLASSIFICATION Breakfast Sport Snacking * significant at 5% level no significant influence on typicality. These findings show that taste and respect for the environment factors may have an inhibitory effect on health considerations. Introduced into the regression model, they contribute significantly to explain typicality variance. The hypothesis (H3.b) is supported by these results. In snacking situation, table 5 shows that health dimension influences typicality (p=0.006) whereas the competing criteria, i.e. taste and freshness, do not contribute in the same way. Their influence on typicality is in fact not significant (p=0.240 and 0.739 respectively). Considered as a whole, we have therefore validated the hypothesis (H1) and shown that salient health goals influence perceived typicality in health food category in certain situations. Theses findings are particularly relevant when goal-related products are assumed to match the consumption context (H2). This Advances in Consumer Research (Volume 35) / 287 influence of health goal salience is more important as health competing goals are less influential. (H3.a). Once other goals of the hierarchy are valued compared with health criteria (when the individual considers other criteria in his food choice), health goals influence on typicality becomes less important (H3.b). CONCLUSION The main conclusion of this research offers evidence and insights on how goals impact categorization process and health food evaluation by way of two dimensions: goal hierarchy and goal salience. Our findings offer additional evidence that the consumer does not pursue a health goal alone, but that there may be interactions between different goals according to individual priorities and context. These goals interferences may influence product judgments. Our findings are consistent with psychological literature (Abraham & Sheeran, 2003), which suggests that goal theories consider the possibility of goals conflicts, and the influence of salient goals. Moreover, the theory of action identification (Vallacher and Wegner, 1985) emphasizes that context is able to alter the salience of certain goals in a given situation. Ratneshwar and his colleagues (1997, 2001) support these findings and show that, according to situational factors, different goals may take priority, thus affecting products perception and representations. Our results are congruent with these assumptions in the sense that, for each situation there is a different hierarchy of goals and a different perception of products typicality in health food category. These results may have important implications in consumer behaviour research. First, by manipulating health situational factors (by means of ads for example) it is possible to activate (or inhibit) other goals, which would lead to different product evaluations according to health competing goals. On the operational level, notably that of product management, typicality concept could be used to respond to consumer needs. To improve a product “health” image, it is possible to reinforce its perceived typicality as regards health food category or specific goals sought by the consumer. This could be done for example by devaluating its perceived typicality as regards goals that compete with health. Such a strategy favours a better positioning, especially when products serve double needs: nutrition and pleasure, health and “terroir”, health and naturality etc. This research accentuates the need to consider different consumer concerns specially those to which he gives priority. Since, for example, for one consumer pleasure is the main motive in food consumption, to lessen products typicality as regards health goals seems to be a prudent strategy by reinforcing enjoyment image, taste and flavour instead of product medicinal aspects. Finally, although analyzing consumer behaviour using goal theories may appear a deterministic and functionalist approach, this study shows that it is possible to guide consumer choice through marketing by using the concept of “goal salience”. By encouraging a suitable nutritional discourse and by controlling the actual “food cacophony”, the consumer can be guided towards a balanced diet, favorable both to his health and to his various expectations. Some limits can be drawn at the end of this research and open doors to further investigations: Analyzing food behaviors as being determined by goals can rise some interrogations as for the rigidity of this deterministic and functionalist approach. In order to understand the reality of food consumption and the consumer as a socialized individual, it would be interesting to include into the model the ecological variables underlining the role of the agents of influence, the culture or the social environment which can have as much influence on perceptions and choices (Steptoe et al., 1995; Pliner & Mann, 2004) as personal motivations. Another limit concerns the experimental procedure and the categorization task in particular which, in our case, requires reasoning in terms of products and “ health food “ rather than of “healthy eating”. However, healthy behavior analysis requires to think food not only in terms of combinations of products but in terms of balance, preparation process and consumption, which could make difficult the task of products evaluation in our experimentation and limit the external validity of the results. It would be useful, in a future research, to associate with the categorization tasks, others aspects, as for example, the origin of the products, their mode of preparation or their consumption. In our research, we focused on the intrinsic attributes of the products related to their functionality and their degree of conformity to health goals. While being conscious that other extrinsic attributes can influence the evaluation of the products, it would be interesting to study the categorization schemes privileged by the consumer that can be related to the goal, the brand, the product category or the area of origin. REFERENCES Abraham, C. and P. Sheeran (2003). “Implications of goal theories for the theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour.” Current psychology: developmental, learning, personality, social. Autumn 2003, 22(3): 264-280 Austin, J. T. and J. B. Vancouver (1996). “Goal constructs in psychology: structure, process, and content.” Psychological Bulletin. 120(3): 338-375. Bagozzi, R. P. and P. A. Dabholkar (1994). “Consumer recycling goals and their effect on decisions to recycle: a means-end chain analysis.” Psychology & marketing. July-august 1994, 11 (4): 313-340. Bandura, A. (2004). “Health Promotion by Social Cognitive Means.” Health Education & Behavior. 31 (2): 143-164. Barsalou, L. W. (1983). “Ad-hoc categories.” Memory and cognition. 11(3): 211-227. Barsalou, L. W. (1985). “Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories.” Journal of experimental psychology: learning, memory and cognition. October 1985, 11(4): 629-654. Barsalou, L. W. (1991). “Deriving categories to achieve goals.” The psychology of learning and motivation. 27: 1-64. Berger, C. and F. Bonthoux (2000). “Accès aux catégories par les propriétés: influence de la tâche et des connaissances chez le jeune enfant.” Psychologie française. (45): 123-130. Brendl, C. M. and E. T. Higgins (1996). “Principles of judging valence: what makes events positive or negative.” Advances in experimental social psychology. 28: 95–160. Brendl, C. M., A. B. Markman, et al. (2000). “La comptabilité mentale comme autorégulation: représentativité pour les catégories dirigées par un but.” Recherche et applications en marketing. 15(1): 81-96. Brendl, C. M., A. B. Markman, et al. (2003). “The devaluation effect: activating a need devalues unrelated objects.” Journal of consumer research. March 2003, 29: 463-473. Carver, C. and M. Scheier (1982). “Control theory: a useful conceptual framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology.” Psychological Bulletin. 92 (1): 111-135. 288 / Effect of Goals Salience and Goals Conflict on Typicality: The Case of “Health Food” Changeur, S. and J. L. Chandon (1995). “Le territoire-produit: étude des frontières cognitives de la marque.” Recherche et applications en marketing. 10(2): 31-52. Dawar, N. and P. Anderson (1994). “The effects of order and direction on multiple brand extensions.” Journal of business research. New York, 30: 119-129 Förster, J., N. Liberman, et al. (2005). “Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals.” Journal of experimental social psychology. (41): 220-239. Gutman, J. (1982). A Means-End Chain Model Based on Consumer Categorization Processes. Journal of Marketing, 46 (2), Spring 1982, 60-72. Higgins, E. T. and G. A. King (1981). Accessibility of social constructs: Information-processing consequences of individual and contextual variability. In: N. Cantor and J. Kihlstrom. Personality, cognition and social interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 69-122. Jean, S. (2000). Un modèle intégrateur des effets de l’ordre d’entrée sur la prise en considération des marques. Université Montpellier II. IAE.Montpellier, 316. Klinger, E. (1975). “Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives.” Psychological review. January 1975, 82: 1-25. Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality: selected papers. New York, McGraw-Hill. Lin, E. et G. L. Murphy (2001). “Thematic relations in adults’ concepts.” Journal of experimental psychology: General. 130(1): 3-28. Loken, B. and J. Ward (1987). “Measures of the attribute structure underlying product typicality.” Advances in consumer research. 14: 22-26. Loken, B. and J. Ward (1990). “Alternative approaches to understanding the determinants of typicality.” Journal of consumer research. September 1990 (17): 111-126. Maheswaran, D. (1994). “Country of Origin as a Stereotype: Effects of Consumer Expertise and Attribute Strength on Product Evaluations.” Journal of Consumer Research. September, 21: 354-365. Mervis, C. B. and E. Rosch (1981). “Categorization of natural objects.” Annual review of psychology. (32): 89-115. Nedungadi, P. and W. Hutchinson (1985). “The Prototypicality of Brands: Relationships with Brand Awareness, Preference and Usage.” Advances in Consumer Research. 12: 498-503. Nguyen, S. P. and G. L. Murphy (2003). “An apple is more than just a fruit: cross classification in children’s concepts.” Child development. 74(6): 1783-1806. Odou, P. (2000). L’utilisation des schémas catégoriels dans le processus dévaluation d’un produit: une application aux catégories associées au pays d’un produit. Thèse de doctorat de Sciences de gestion. Université du Littoral Côte d’Opale, 391 Plier, P. and N. Mann (2004). Influence of social norms and palatability on amount consumed and food choice. Appetite. (42): 227-237. Ratneshwar, S. and A. D. Shocker (1991). “Substitution in use and the role of product usage context in product category structures.” Journal of marketing research. August 1991, 28: 281-295. Ratneshwar, S., L. Warlop, et al. (1997). “Benefit salience and consumers’ selective attention to product features.” International Journal of research in marketing. (14): 245-259. Ratneshwar, S., D. Mick, et al. (2000). Introduction: The ‘Why’ of Consumption. In: S. Ratneshwar, D. Mick and C. Huffman. The Why of Consumption: Contemporary Perspectives on Consumer Motives, Goals, and Desires. London, Routledge, 1–35. Ratneshwar, S., L. W. Barsalou, et al. (2001). “Goal-derived categories: the role of personal and situational goals in category representations.” Journal of consumer psychology. 10(3): 147-157. Ross, C. E. and G. L. Murphy (1999). “Food for thought: CrossClassification and category organization in a complex realworld domain.” Cognitive psychology. (38): 495-553 Schwarz, N., H. Bless, et al. (2003). Accessibility Revisited. In: G. V. Bodenhausen and A. J. Lambert. Foundations of Social Cognition: A Festschrift in Honor of Robert S. Wyer, Jr. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 51-77. Steptoe, A., T. Pollard and J. Wardle (1995). Development of a measure of the motives underlying the selection of food: the food choice questionnaire. Appetite. 25: 267–284. Vallacher, R. and D. Wegner (1985). A theory of action identification. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wyer, R. and T. Srull (1981). Category accessibility: Some theoretical and empirical issues concerning the processing of social stimulus information. In: E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman and M. Zanna. Social Cognition: The Ontario Symposium. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. I, 161-198.