347 The British Psychological British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2010), 28, 347-368 ©2OiO The British Psychohsicai Society • ! * — f ^ Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk A comparison of dyadic interactions and coping with still-face in healthy pre-term and full-term infants Rosario Montirosso'*, Renato Borgatti', Sabina Trojan^, Rinaldo Zanini^ Ed Tronick^"*'^ 'Department of Child and Adolescent Neurology and Psychiatry and Italian NNNS Centre for Infant Neurobehavioural Study, Scientific Institute 'E. Medea', Bosisio Parini (Lecco), Italy ^Neonatal Intensive Care Unit, Manzoni Hospital, Lecco, Italy ^Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, USA "•child Development Unit, Children's Hospital Boston, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA ^Fielding Graduate University, Santa Barbara, California, USA Pre-term birth has a significant impact on infants' social and emotional competence, however, little is known about regulatory processes in pre-term mother-infant dyads during normal or stressful interactions. The primary goals of this study were to investigate the differences in infant and caregiver interactive behaviour and dyadic coordination of clinically healthy pre-term compared to full-term infant-mother dyads and to examine pre-term infants' capacity for coping with stress using the face-to-face still-face paradigm (FFSF). Fifty mother-infant dyads, including 25 pre-term infants and 25 full-term infants were videotaped during the FFSR All infants were 6-9 months ofage (corrected for gestational age in the pre-term group). Infant and maternal socioemotional expressivity and self-regulatory behaviours were coded and measures of dyadic coordination {Matching, Reparation Rate, and Synchrony) were calculated. There were no significant differences in infant and caregiver socio-emotional behaviours between the two groups and both groups demonstrated the still-face (SF) effect and the reunion effea. There was a difference in self-regulatory behaviour. Pre-term infants were more likely than full-term infants to use distancing (e.g., by turning away, twisting, or arching) from their mothers during the FFSF. Additionally, during the Reunion episode of the FFSF pre-term infants showed more social monitoring compared to full-term infants. Regardless of the birth status, the dyads showed less coordination and a slower rate of reparation during the Reunion episode than during the Play episode. The higher proportion of distancing in the pre-term group and the increase in social monitoring * Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Rosario Montirosso, Scientific Institute ' £ Medea', Via don Luigi Monza, 20 - 23842, ßosisio Parini. Lecco, Italy (e-mail: rosario.montirosso@bp.lnf.it). DOI: 10.1348/026151009X416429 348 R. Montírosso et al. suggest that even in normal interactions pre-term infants may experience a higher level of stress and have less capacity for self-regulation compared to the full-terms and that pre-term infants appear to use a compensatory strategy of increased social monitoring to cope with the stress of renegotiating the interaction during Reunion. The findings suggest that pre-term infants have different regulatory and interactive capacities than full-term infants. Pre-term birth is considered to have a subtle but potentially significant impact on infants' social and emotional development (Field, 1987; Lowe, Woodward, & Papile, 2005; Macey, Harmon, & Easterbrooks, 1987; Minde, Whitelaw, Brown, & Fitzhardinge, 1983), In addition, previous studies have found that pre-term infants' have difficulty regulating their behavioural states (Wolf et al, 2002), less capacity for self-quieting (Thoman & Graham, 1986), and limited capacities for coping with stress (Als, 1983), Taken together, these factors, along with others (e,g,, early separation), increase the risk for abnormal development of the mother-infant relationship in pre-term infant mother dyads (Feldman, Weiler, Sirota, & Eidelman, 2003; Muller-Nix et al, 2004), because they are likely to affect the quality and the dyadic coordination of the interaction (Feldman, 2006; Korja et al., 2008), Although, previous research has found that prematurity affects mother-itifant synchrony during face-to-face interactions (Feldman, 2006; Feldman & Eidelman, 2007; Lester, Hoffman, & Brazelton, 1985) little is known about the quality of dyadic processes such as matching, mismatching, and reparation in pre-term dyads. Thus, the primary goals of this study were to investigate differences in interactive behaviour and dyadic coordination of clinically healthy preterm compared to full-term infant-mother dyads as well as pre-term infants' capacity for coping with stress. Our thinking was framed by the mutual regulation model (MRM; Gianino & Tronick, 1988; also see Fogel, 1993; Sander, 2000). The MRM argues that the interaction is organized by a bidirectional exchange of communicative signals that are used by the infant and the caregiver to coordinate the interaction and to cope with the stress of inevitable interactive miscoordinations. From this perspective, the quality of the interaction is determined by the ability of each participant to cope with external Stressors, regulate his/her emotional states, express communicative messages, and respond to his/her partner's affective communications and regulatory needs, Caregivers' behaviour is guided by itifants' expressive displays (e,g,, gaze, facial expressions, gestures, and vocalizations). In turn, itifant behavioural and affective states are affected by the expressive displays of the caregiver, Tronick and Gianino (1986) hypothesized that infants, and especially pre-term infants (Als, 1983; Mouradian, Als, & Coster, 2000) have limited regulatory capacities and, therefore, need their caregivers' regulatory scaffolding to maintain affective regulation and cope with interactive stress. Thus, we expected differences in pre-term infant-caregiver expressivity and interactive coordination and differences in the pre-term infant's self-regulatory coping capacity compared to full term infants. Studies have found that during thefirstyear of life itifants born prior to term compared to full-term infants are less responsive and attentive, make fewer vocal and affective signals, are fussier, and have lower scores on the clarity of their social cues (Crawford, 1982; Crnic, Ragozin, Greenberg, Robinson, & Basham, 1983; McGehee & Eckerman, 1983), Pre-term infants evidence more negative affect and gaze aversion Pre-term infants in still-face paradigm 349 during dyadic interactions (Van Beek, Hopkins, Hoeksma, & Samsom, 1994) and they are more difficult to arouse to an attentive state and tend to avoid social stimulation (Field, 1981). In contrast, pre term infants have been described as relatively competent in their interactive behaviour (Schermann-Eizirik, HagekuU, Bohlin, Persson, & Sedin, 1997) and as more responsive to vocal and affective signals (Barratt, Roach, & Leavitt, 1992) compared to full-terms. Maternal social sensitivity is another factor influencing the infants' social experience, socio-emotional development and the infant-mother relationship. When the infantmother interaction is viewed as a process of mutual regulation (Tronick, 1989) and if pre-term infants are less clear in their affective communicative signals, one would expect pre-term infants to require more sensitive tuning of maternal emotional responsivity and regulatory scaffolding to maintain attention and positive affect (Als, 1983). However, research on the expressive behaviour and responsiveness of mothers of pre-term infants is conflicting. Researchers have found that mothers of pre-term infants compared to mothers of full-term itifants are more stimulating (Crawford, 1982; Crnic et al., 1983; McGehee & Eckerman, 1983) and display less sensitivity to their infants' affective cues (Feldman & Eidelman, 2007; Malatesta, Grigoryev, Lamb, Albin, & Culver, 1986). On the other hand, compared to mothers of full-term infants, mothers of pre-terms have been found to be more responsive (Barratt etal., 1992; Greene, Fox, & Lewis, 1983) and to show better performance on interactive measures (SchermannEizirik etal., 1997). Part of the discrepancy in ñndings may be related to differences in measurement and definitions. Mother-infant dyadic regulatory processes have been defined in a number of different ways in the literature but have not been examined in pre-term dyads. Studies of full-term mother-infant dyads have focused on mothers' and infants' ability to change affective or behavioural states in temporal coordination with one another (synchrony), to share the lead in interaction (bidirectionality) and to share joint attentional, affective, or behavioural states at the same moment in time (matching; Beebe & Lachmann, 2002; Cohn & Tronick, 1987; Feldman, Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, 1999; Jaffe, Beebe, Feldstein, Crown, & Jasnow, 2001; Tronick & Cohn, 1989). However, interactions are not always smoothly coordinated but are characterized by interactive mismatching (Gianino & Tronick, 1988). Mismatching occurs when the infant and the mother are in different behavioural states (e.g., infant looking and smiling at the mother and the mother looking away with a neutral facial expression). Tronick (1989) has shown that mismatches are associated with interactive stress and Tronick and Cohn (1989) found that mothers and infants spend most of their playtime in mismatching states. However, most mismatches were corrected in the next interactive step. The process of changing from mismatching to matching states is referred to as reparation (measured as the rate of change between matching and mismatching). From the perspective of the MRM (Tronick, 1989, 2003) reparation resolves the interactive stress of a mismatch and is a key interactive process that affects the development of the infant's sense of trust in the mother, self-regulation and coping with stress (Feldman, Greenbaum etal., 1999; Tronick, 2004). Based on the research on pre-term infants and their mothers, we expected that pre-term dyads would have less matching of their interactive states, fewer reparations and would likely develop different ways of coping with the stress of mismatching compared to full-terms. Many other factors also come into play that may disrupt the organization of the dyadic co-regulatory system. For example, maternal-itifant separation caused by standard incubator care along with the stress of medical procedures (Anand & Scalzo, 2000; Feldman, Weiler, Leckman, Kuint, & Eidelman, 1999) are among the negative 350 R. Montirosso et al. sequelae of pre-term birth. These conditions are Ukely to decrease the opportunities for mothers to act as effective external organizers or regulators of the infant's socio-emotional and behavioural states (Als et al, 2004; Eckerman, Hsu, Molitor, Leung, & Goldstein, 1999; Hofer, 1994; Spangler, Schieche, Ilg, Maier, & Ackermann, 1994). Thus, aversive conditions affecting early caregiving interactions can lower level of coordination observed between mothers and premature infants during the first 6 months of life (Feldman & Eidehnan, 2007). For example, in a group of low- to high-risk infants (3-5 months of age) Lester et al. (1985) found that pre-term infant-mother dyads were less able to coordinate their behavioural cycles of affect and attention during face-to-face social exchanges compared to full-term pairs. Feldman (2006) examined Unks between neonatal biological rhythms (sleep-wake cyclicity and cardiac vagal tone activity) and the emergence of interaction rhythms (mother-infant synchrony computed from microanalysis of face-to-face interactions at 3 months of age). She found that high-risk premature infants displayed disorganized biological rhythms, lower levels of dyadic synchrony, a more limited capacity for arousal modulation, and lower thresholds for negative emotionality. Although, the low risk group of pre-term infants did not differ from the full-term group on the continuous measures of synchrony and neonatal orientation, they scored significantly more poorly than the full-term group on negative emotionality and arousal modulation. In contrast, in a recent micro longitudinal study Korja et al (2008) found that mother-infant interaction was comparable between preterm and full-term infants at 6 and 12 months of age. Perhaps, the differences seen in younger infants disappear by the second half of the first year of life. Differences between full-term and pre-term infants in socio-emotional behaviours (SEB) have been examined during several types of adult-infant interactions: feeding (Minde et al, 1983; Singer et al, 2003), undressing the infant (Schermann-Eizirik et al, 1997), and face-to-face play interactions, including peek-a-boo (Eckerman, Oehler, & Medvin, 1994; Forcada-Guex, Pierrehumbert, Borghini, Moessinger, & Muller-Nix, 2006). However, few studies have studied a context that involves interactional stress or challenge, yet a stress places different demands on the communicative and social abilities of the infants, mothers, and dyads and may bring out differences not seen in non-stressful contexts. For example, Malatesta, Culver, Tesman, and Shepard (1989) recorded face-to-face interaction involving play and separation/reunion sessions in preterm and full-term infants at 2.5, 5, 7.5, and 22 months. They reported that stressful situations compared to non-stressful situations and that pre-term birth altered infant affective expressions and maternal contingency levels. However, almost no studies have specifically addressed pre-term infants' abilities to coordinate and repair interactions or their emotional regulation during a stressful situation. This lack of research is in part due to researchers having few paradigms for examining infants coping with stress in early infancy (Lewis, Hitchcock, & Sullivan, 2004). One paradigm that has been established as a standard method is the face-to-face still-face paradigm (FFSF; Adamson & Frick, 2003; Troniek, Als, Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, 1978). It is well established that the FFSF is a pow^erful paradigm for examining infants' socio-emotional competence and abuity to cope with stress (for an overview see, Adamson & Frick, 2003) and several studies have demonstrated that the effect of the still-face (SF) is robust among different groups of infants, including both lo-w- and high-risk samples (Adamson & Frick, 2003; Troniek et al, 2005). During the FFSF paradigm, infants engage in normal face-to-face interaction -with their caregiver and also confront the perturbation of the caregiver remaining poker-faced and unresponsive (SF). Thus, the FFSF paradigm allows for the evaluation of both infants and mothers' Pre-term infants in still-face paradigm 351 affective and regulatory behaviour and dyadic characteristics during normal interaction (Play episode), infants' coping capacities during the stress of the SF (SF episode), and their coping and regulation during the resumption of the normal interaction (Reunion episode), when mothers and infants must re-establish and repair the interaction following the stress of the SF. During the SF episode, infants show a typical signature response known as the SF effect (Adamson & Frick, 2003). They show a dramatic decrease in smiling, more gaze aversion, and an increase in negative affect. In addition, infants repeatedly attempt to re-engage the adult by smiling and gesturing while looking at the interaction partner. When these attempts fail, infants show an increase in negative affect, gazing away, and self-comforting behaviours. Following the SF in the Reunion episode, infants show an increase in smiling and eye-contact. However, some studies show that there is a carry-over of negative effect from the SF episode to the Reunion episode, indicating that the itifants have to cope with the stress from the SF during the Reunion episode. The carry-over of negative affect has been labelled the reunion effect (Adamson & Frick, 2003). To our knowledge, only one study has looked at the pre-term mother-infant pairs using the FFSF Segal etal. (1995) used it with groups of black pre-term and full-term motherinfant pairs from middle and lower income levels. No differences in negative affect were found between groups, but pre-term infants spent less time displaying smiles than fullterms. The premature infants showed the SF effect w'tíh a significant reduction in smiling from Play to the SF episode, confirming that healthy pre-term infants, like full-terms, have developed expectations about social interactions. Unfortunately, there was no evaluation of the mothers' interactive behaviour or the dyadic characteristics of the interactions. This study evaluated the effects of premature birth and interactional context on mother-infant affective expressiveness and the dyadic features of their interactions. Since, differences in levels of social engagement and the quality of maternal responsiveness have been found between premature infants born at low^ and high medical risk (Greene et al, 1983; Landry, Chapieski, Richardson, Palmer, & Hall, 1990; Landry, Smith, Miller-Loncar, & Swank, 1998), we chose to assess healthy pre-term infants with birth weight appropriate for gestational age, normal psychomotor development, and a documented absence of brain damages. At the time of testing infants ranged in age from 6 to 9 months (corrected for gestational age in the pre-term group). Though this is, a wider age range than used in other studies, we did not expect the age range to affect our results for several reasons. A number of studies have found that after the age of 6 months, the SF effect and reunion effect become stable (see Adamson & Frick, 2003). For example, Striano and Rochat (1999) found no significant age effects on infant behaviour in the FFSF between 7- and 10-month-olds suggesting that ways of coping become characteristic of the infant by 6 months of age. Mother-infant dyadic coordination also is very similar at 6 and 9 months of age (Tronick & Cohn, 1989). Furthermore, by the second half of theirfirstyear of life, pre-term infants have had more of a chance than younger pre-term infants to get catch-up developmentally with fullterm infants (Brachfeld, Goldberg, & Sloman, 1980). From the perspective of the MRM and the literature we expected that pre-terms, as compared to full-term infants, would evidence less social attentiveness, more negative and fewer positive affects, and fewer self-regulatory behaviours during the FFSF paradigm. Second, despite the contradictory empiricalfindingsabout the impact of preterm birth on the mothers' SEB, we expected that mothers of pre-term infants would use different strategies to help their infants regulate affective states compared with mothers of full-terms because of the expected differences in their itifants' communicative and 352 R. Montirosso et al. regulatory behaviours. The research, however, does not allow for a more specific hypothesis about the form of such differences (e.g., more or less maternal interest or positive affect). Third, we expected that the dyadic organization of the interaction would be different among pre-term dyads compared to full-term dyads. In line with previous SF research (Tronick & Cohn, 1989; Weinberg, Tronick, Cohn, & Olson, 1999) three measures of coordination (i.e., Matching, Reparation Rate, and Synchrony) were used to assess the mother-infant dyadic processes. We expected that pre-term dyads, as compared to full-term dyads, w^ould evidence less synchrony (Lester et al., 1985; Feidman, 2006). Based on the MRM, we also expected that pre-term dyads compared to full-term dyads would show less matching and fewer reparations of behavioural and affective states. Fourth, w^e sought to identify some of the factors (gender, birth status, developmental quotient, level of maternal depressive symptomatology, socio-economic status (SES), and maternal SEB) associated with infant SEB during SE Regardless of the extent to which the data support these hypotheses, thefindingsfrom this study will add to our understanding of the regulatory capacities of pre-term infants and mothers, the dyadic features of their interaction and the effects of stress on the infant and the mother. Method Participants Fifty mother-infant dyads, 25 healthy pre-term infants (11 females) and 25 full-term infants (12 females) participated in the study. All the infants were between 6.8 and 99 months of age (corrected for gestational age in the pre-term group). The pre-term infants were recruited consecutively from all the infants, born between August 2002 and February 2003 in the NICU at 'A. Manzoni' Hospital, Lecco Gtaly), w^ho met the following criteria: gestational age (GA) less than i6^^^ weeks (range: 26-36 weeks), birth weight less than 2,500 g (range: 845-2,450 g), and an absence of serious medical complications (e.g., grade I intraventricular hemorrhage, any degree of periventricular leukomalacia, any degree of hearing or visual impairments, congenital abnormalities). Their mothers were excluded if they were at psychosocial risk due to a history of drug abuse, mental illness, or had high levels of depressive symptoms (i.e., greater than the cut-off level on the Beck Depression Inventory [BDI]; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erlbaugh, 1961). Twenty-eight (93%) of the 30 eligible mothers of pre-term infants, we contacted agreed to participate. Three pre-terms were excluded because they became too distressed to complete the experimental procedures. Two w^ere members of twin pairs. Healthy full-term participants were recruited during the same time period from a list of possible full-term infants obtained from community paediatricians. Selection criteria for control subjects were: full-term gestation (> 37 weeks, GA range: 37-41), appropriate weight for GA (birth weight > 2,500, range: 2,540-3,840 g), Apgar scores of at least 7 at 1 min and 8 at 5 min, no congenital abnormalities, and uncomplicated prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal courses. Their mothers met the same criteria used for the mothers of the pre-term participants. Twenty-five (71%) of the 35 mothers of eligible full-term infants contacted agreed to participate. No fuU-term infants were members of twin pairs. No full-term infants had to be excluded because of distress during the procedures. The infant and maternal characteristics of both groups are reported in Table 1. As can be seen, the selection criteria for both groups resulted in clinically healthy samples. Pre-term infants in stili-face paradigm 353 Table I . Infants' and maternal characteristics Full-term dyads (N = 25; F = 11) Characteristics Infant G A at birth (v^eeks) Birth v^^eight (grams) Age (months)* Brunet-Lezine score Maternal Age (years) Education (years) SES BDI score Pre-term (dyads (N = 25; F = 12) Mean SD Mean SD 39.9 3,293 9,0 101.1 1.2 382 I.I 5.3 32,1 1,516 9.3 100,1 2.8 483 1,2 8,6 33 14.0 68 5.5 4.7 2.9 17.4 3.4 32 12,3 61 4,6 3,6 2,8 23,8 2,4 Note, *GA, gestational age corrected for GA in the pre-term infants; F, female; SES, socio-economic status; BDI, Beck depression inventory. Procedure Mothers of potential participants (pre-terms and full-terms) were recruited by phone. Participation was voluntary. They were told that the study concerned infant-mother interactive and communicative behaviour. Mothers who expressed an interest in participating in the study were scheduled to visit the laboratory of the 'E. Medea' Scientific Institute at a time when they thought their infants would be rested and alert, usually between 9 a.m. and noon. The protocol included the video-recording the mother-infant interaction in the FFSF, completion by the mother of a socio-demographic questionnaire, a clinical report form, and the BDI, The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the 'E, Medea' Scientific Institute and written informed consent was obtained from all the mothers. Measures SES SES was coded according to the information provided by caregivers on the basis of Hollingshead's (1975) classification for parental occupation. Developmental quotient Developmental quotient (DQ) was determined by the Brunet-Lézine's scale (Brunet & Lézine, 1955), a widely used, standardized measure that provides general indexes of mental and psychomotor development relative to group norms in itifants aged 4 months to 5 years. The Brunet-Lezine is based on early descriptive research by Gesell (Gesell & Amatruda, 1947), It has well-established reliability and validity. The scale evaluates the child's development in four areas: posture and gross motor function, eye-hand and finemotor coordination, language, and social reactions. Scores above 90 are considered normal, 80-90 are below average (i,e,, lo'w normal), 70-79 are considered borderline, and scores below 70 represent some degree of mental retardation. 354 R. Montirosso et al. Maternai depression Maternal depression was evaluated with the BDI (Beck etal., 1961). The BDI is a 21-item measure of depressive symptoms. Items are rated on a four-point rating scale, indicating the absence/presence and the range of severity of depressed feelings/behaviours/symptoms. The BDI demonstrates good internal consistency and has concurrent and discriminant validity in clinical and non-clinical samples (Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). A score of greater than 12 was used as the cut-off for the clinical range (O'Hara, Rehm, & Campbell, 1983). Laboratory setting FFSF interactions Caregivers and infants were videotaped during the FFSF procedure, based on the original paradigm developed by Tronick et al. (1978). The video room was equipped with a high chair, an adjustable sw^ivel stool for the caregiver approximately 40 cm from the infant, two cameras (one focused on the infant, the other on the caregiver), and a microphone. The paradigm is made up of three 2 min episodes: (a) a 2 min face-to-face play interaction (Play); (b) a 2 min SF interaction (SF), and (c) a 2 min reunion play interaction (Reunion). Prior to the video-recording, the mothers were instructed to play with their infants during the Play and Reunion episodes just as they would normally do at home. For the SF, the mothers were instructed to pose a neutral expressionless face to their infants while looking at them, but not to smile, talk, or touch their infants. The signals from the two cameras were edited off-line into a video-recorder to produce a single image with a simultaneous frontal vie'w of the face, hands and torso of the infant and mother. For analysis purposes, a computerized digital format was created for each infant-mother dyad's video clip. Behavioural coding Infants' and caregivers' behaviours w^ere coded using the infant and caregiver engagement phases (ICEP; Weinberg & Tronick, 1998; see also Tronick et al., 2005). The ICEP system is made up of a set of mutually exclusive infant and caregiver phases of interactive engagement and several additional codes for regulatory behaviours. Definitions for ICEP and infant self-regulation codes are provided in Table 2. ICEP were coded in two separate viewings of the videotape. A third viewing of the tape was devoted to coding infant regulatory behaviours (i.e., self-comforting/mouthing, hand clasping, and distancing/turning away from the caregiver). The coding was done from the digital video by coders masked to the pre-term status of the infants. A digital time display was used to track time intervals and the time code was automatically entered into a datafile.Software developed by the E. Medea Scientific Institute's Bioengineering Laboratory allowed coders to indicate the time measured to within 1-s w^hen a particular behaviour occurred. This coding procedure produced an absolute frequency count of the ICEP codes and regulatory behaviours and maintained their temporal sequence within a 1-s interval. Reliability Inter observer reliability values for both the ICEP and the additional codes were determined through percentage agreement and Cohen's kappa (Cohen, I960). Fifty percent of the Play, SF, and Reunion episodes w^ere selected randomly and were assessed for agreement between two independent coders. The mean percentage of Pre-tern) infants in still-face paradigm 355 Table 2. Outline of the encoding system of the ICEP (Weinberg & Troniek, 1998). Infant engagement Negative engagement: The infant is negative or protesting. The infant displays negative facial expressions (e.g., sadness, distress, crying, or grinnacing), complaining, being fussy, crying vocalizations. When the infant is protesting, he/she often displays facial expressions of anger, grimaces, and fussing/crying. Withdrawn: The infant is withdrawn and minimally engaged with the caregiver. This phase often includes sad facial expressions, whimpering vocalizations, slumped posture, and gaze aversion Object/environment engagement: The infant is looking at objects that are either proximal (e.g., infant seat) or distal (e.g., camera). The infant may manipulate proximal objects (e.g., infant chair strap; caregiver's hands) Social monitoring: Gazing at caregiver's face/eyes with a neutral or interested facial expression. Infant may vocalize in a neutral manner Sociai positive engagement Gazing at caregiver's face/eyes while smiling Caregiver engagement Negative engagement: The adult is negative, intrusive or hostile Withdrawn: The adult is minimally engaged and withdrawn with the infant (disengagement from infant for gaze direction, facial expressions and/or vocalizing) Non-infant focused engagement: The adult is not attending to the infant and is involved in a non-infant focused activity Social monitor: Gazing at infant with neutral/interested expression or with positive vocalizing. Social positive engagement: Positive affect (smiles, laughter, exaggerated expressions) Infant self-regulation Mouthing: Infant sucks on his/her body (e.g., thumb or wrist) Seif<lasping: The infant's two hands are touching Distancing: The infant's shoulders and trunk are rotated sideways from the caregiver and the infant's head is averted agreement and Cohen's kappa were .85 and .73 for infants' behaviour, .89 and .75 for caregivers' behaviour, and .88 and .74 for the regulatory codes. Data reduction and dependent measures Dependent measures for the infant, caregiver, and dyad were: (1) The proportion of time the infant or caregiver were in each ICEP phase was obtained by dividing the total time of each code by the total length of the episode. (2) The proportion of time for self-regulatory behaviours was obtained by dividing the total time of each code by the total length of the episode. (3) Three measures of coordination were analyzed: Matching, Reparation rate, and Synchrony. Following procedures from previous studies (Troniek & Cohn, 1989; Weinberg et al, 1999), measures of coordination were defined as follows: (a) Matching: the extent to which caregivers and infants shared joint ICEP codes; infant and mother simultaneously in Negative, Withdraw^n, Looking a-way, Social Monitor, or Social Positive states, (b) Reparation Rate: the rate of change between mismatching to matching states, (c) Synchrony: the extent to w^hich mothers and infants simultaneously increased or decreased their level of engagement in the same direction (more or less engagement) w^ith each other. Synchrony is quantified by first scaling the infant and mother ICEP codes from less to more engaged and 356 R. Montirosso et al. then calculating the proportion of shared variance at LagO, as indexed by the square of the cross-correlation between each mother and infant's time series of mean engagement scale scores. Matching and Synchrony measures differ in that Matching focuses on the shared or non-shared content of the behaviours of mothers and infants (they are doing the same thing at the same time), whereas Synchrony evaluates how mothers and infants change their levels of engagement with each other over time, regardless of the content of their behaviour (getting more or less engaged even if they are doing different things). Thus, some dyads might seldom be in matching states but nonetheless have high-synchrony scores because they tended to change their level of engagement in the same direction. Statistical analysis Pearson correlations were calculated in order to evaluate possible relations between the infant's age and infant ICEP phases and infant self-regulatory behaviours and measures of coordination. To assess the effects of group (pre-term vs. full-term) and interactional context (Play, SF, and Reunion) on infant and maternal ICEP phases and self-regulatory behaviours, we conducted separate 2 (group) X 3 (episode) ANOVAs with episodes as repeated measures. For the caregiver, negative engagement, withdrawn, and non-infant focused engagement were not included in the ANOVA because of the low proportion of occurrence. Following the techniques used in previous studies (Tronick & Cohn, 1989; Weinberg etal., 1999), coordination measures w^ere analyzed as follows: Matching, the adjusted proportions were transformed using and arcsine transformation' and then examined in a 2 (group) X 2 (episode) ANOVA w^ith episodes as repeated measures; the Reparation Rate per second was arcsine transformed and analyzed in a 2 (group) X 2 (episode) ANOVA with the Play and Reunion play episodes as repeated measures; and Synchrony was evaluated using the cross-correlations of the Mother and Infant Scale scores in a 2 (group) X 2 (episode) ANOVA with Play and Reunion play as repeated measures. Because all mothers were instructed to behave in the same manner during the SF episode, the caregiver and dyadic variables were analyzed only for the Play and Reunion episodes using a repeated measures ANOVA. Significant episode effects were evaluated in pair wise comparisons using post hoc tests w^ith the critical p value for significance adjusted w^ith Bonferroni correction to control for multiple tests. Effect size was evaluated using the partial Eta square (Tip), which evaluates the proportion of variance accounted for by each variable. The conventional cut-offs for T|p = .01, .06, and. 14 for small, medium, and large effect sizes were used (Green & Salldnd, 2003). Separate forward stepwise linear regressions were done to identify the significant predictors of each itifant's SEB (infant ICEP phases and self-regulatory behaviours). The effect of the following variables were exatnined for each episode of the FFSF procedure: gender, birth status (full-term or pre-term birth), DQ, BDI scores, and SES. The maximum number of variables entered into the regression analysis was five, insuring that there were at least 10 participants for each independent variable (Munro, 1997). All analyses were performed at a significance level/J < .05. Recause the dependent variables were expressed as proportions, the data were arcsine transformed. The arcsine transformation was performed to make the distribution of the variable approximately normal for statistical analysis. Cohen and Cohen (1983) suggested this transformation for uncorrelating the means and variance of proportional data. Pre-terw infants in stiii-face paradigm 357 Results As expected, comparisons (t test) between the full-term and the pre-term groups' medical and demographic variables revealed that only the infants' GA and birth weight were significantly different, i(48) = 11.50, p < .001, and i(48) = 12.91, p < 001, respectively. No associations were found for the correlation of corrected age and DQ. A chi-square test revealed that gender was not significantly associated with group. There were no significant differences in age, education, SES, and BDI scores between the parents of fuU-term and pre-term infants. No significant correlations were found between the infants' age and infants' behaviour, self-regulation. Matching, Reparation Rate, or Synchrony. Consistent with findings from previous SF research, these results suggest that the age range used in the present study w^as not associated w^ith the infant variables or coordination measures. Infant and maternal ICEP phases and infant self-regulation Table 3 presents the adjusted proportion means and SD of infant and maternal ICEP phases and self-regulatory behaviours for pre-term and full-term infants. Contrary to expectations, the analysis revealed no significant differences in ICEP codes between groups across the three episodes for infant and maternal ICEP phases. As expected, a significant effect was found for self-regulatory behaviours, i='(l,48) = 8.88, p < .01, T|p = .19. The partial Eta squared level indicates a large effect size (Green & Salkind, 2003): pre-term infants compared to full-term infants showed higher levels of distancing/turning away (0.10 vs. 0.03, respectively). No differences were found for oral self-comforting/mouthing and self-clasping. There were significant differences in infant ICEP phases and self-regulatory behaviours for the different phases of the EESF: Negative engagement, F(2,86) = 11.63, p < .00, f]l = .23; Withdrawn, F(_2,86) = 4.80,p < .05, iq^ = .11; Object/environment engagement, F = (2,86)17.60, p < .001, TI^ = .32; Social monitor, F(2,86) = 4.70, p < .05, Tip = .11; Social positive engagement, 7^(2,86) = 13.72, p < .001, Tip = .26, and Distancing/turning away from caregiver, F(2,86) = 6.^0, p < .01, Tip = .14. These size effects were moderate to large. As expected, regardless of group membership, a Bonierroni post hoc analysis found that infants exhibited a greater proportion of negative affect and withdrawn behaviour during the SE Moreover, there •was an increase in the infant's negative affect in the Reunion, which did not return to the levels found in the Play episode. In the SE episode infants also engaged in more self-regulatory behaviours and showed a reduction of Social Monitoring and Social Positive engagement, both of which returned in the Reunion episodes to the levels observed in the Play episode. These findings show that both groups reacted with negative affect to the SF and both had a carry-over effect of negative affect from the SE into the Reunion episode as well as an increase in positive affect reflecting the ambivalent nature of the infant's behaviour during the Reunion play. There was one significant group X episode interaction for Social Monitoring, F(2,86) = 397, p < .05, Tip = .10. Separate t tests for each episode revealed that during the Reunion episode, the pre-term dyads showed greater social monitoring than full-term dyads (respectively, 0.28 vs. 0.17), i(48) = -2.21,p < .05. There were no significant episode effects (Play vs. Reunion) for the Caregiver Engagement Phases. The regression analysis evaluating the predictors of infant SEB during the SF paradigm is presented in Table 4. None of the predictor variables were correlated w^ith each other at greater than r = .38 indicating little multi-coUinearity. The significant R. Montirosso et al. lN00>O INOIN OCSci mnvo 1 <00^ —O ÖÖ o —— O O O O O O LOLO f^. "^ OÖ (v.)f^|•T^ —O— O O Ö <NLo O O O mt^ ON — o ÖÖÖ ÖÖ ÖÖÖ OÖ ÖÖÖ Lororo INOIN O O O >o<N —— O'O OO — O O O O O O lo-o ININ CiO' — pLO <~iC> pop 00 — — OOO O —p ^ ÖÖÖ <s — . ÖO p p p ÖÖÖ lo IN -o w-i CN rM ~ IN ~ O I títítí títí O I O I '^'"i P° — rviNOO — OO OOCi OO <N — LO i^f^ ÖÖ — p p ÖÖÖ ro IN O„ — O — I ' títítí L O v O r O l O — ^~ ^ 3 >O —^ ^ 3 os — LO ^ 5 ^ 3 ^— ÖÖÖ ÖÖ I I I "^ ^ i rsj •^-. ^^ ^5 ^^ ^^ rsj fNj OOO ÖO ocio' ÖÖ ^^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ¿ ^ l,^ ^ ^ ÖÖ Ö Ö Ö ÖÖ ^ ^ ^O ^ ^ ^^ ^ ^ ^^ f^ ^ ^ p^ QQ —o O O O ININ — O O ^N( P O ÖÖÖ 00 Po ^ ^ IN~IN II Ö Ö Ö ^i ^i ~^ OÖÖ ^ ^ ^^p ^ ^ Ö Ö Ö títítí títí títítí títí títítí J: a. U bo se -o c « O^ LO fN ^— ^5 LO Ö Ö Ö ^~ ' ^ fN ^3 OÖ ^3 ^3 ^3 ^3 ^3 ^5 Ö Ö Ö r**» ro r**» rS ÖÖ t*v —~ r o ^3 ^3 ^3 Ö Ö Ö ^O ro Lo ro ""^ fN ^í* f^ ~"^ ^5 ^i ^ ^ ^3 ^3 ^i ^i ^— •"" fNj rs4 ^ ^ •"- r o ••^ ^i ^i CiCÍO Ö Ö ÖO'Ö Ö Ö Ö Ö Ö f*J ^ r o í 00 LO ^3 ^3 C3 r**. fN| p*>i, ^3 — rop^ ÖÖÖ —p ÖÖ p p p ÖÖÖ f^f^ p p p ÖÖ Ö Ö Ö "<O ' O Ov f Ñ —• rsj "^ — — O Ö Ö Ö Ö Ö íN p lO Ö Ö Ö — p Ö Ö < (U -O tu -o 3 O 5 tu c lU O) E c P títítí 00 — vO § §ö o o o o tí o tí o o IN tí I I rs| ro tí 888 IN IN tí O es o o o o T3 C IN O O 05 02 01 IN I I I 76 24 a E o o o .03 .61 2 T3 C o. O ,28 ,05 E C O t o a. lU <u E T3 C S) ve en rawn :/envii 358 •5 ,Si. | z XI lU (U E o. L. •"t'§ tu > tu ü tM) U o o u (J «_i S (^ (8 ^ ,Í2 5 Pre-terw infants in still-face paradigm 359 predictor was birth status, accounting for 22% of the variance in Distancing during the Play and accounting for 9% of the variance in Social Monitoring during the Reunion. Thus, pre-term status was predictive of greater distancing during Play and Social Monitoring during the Reunion. Table 4. Linear regression for variables predicting infant SE B during SF paradigm Episode Infant behaviour Predictor B Play Distancing Birth status 0.07 Reunion Social monitoring Birth status 0.13 SEB 0.03 Adjusted« ^ = .22 0.05 Adjusted R^ = .09 ß 0.43* 0.43* Note. SE B, standard error of B; *f> < .05. Measures of coordination The measures of coordination were assessed in the Play and Reunion episodes. Table 5 presents the adjusted proportion means and SD of measures of coordination for preterm and term infants. There w^as no main effect of group for Social Monitor Match and Social Positive Match. There were significant episode effects. There was a reduction of Look Away from Partner Match, P(l,46) = 11.43,/» < .01, T|p = .24, during Reunion (0.23) as compared to Play (0.34) suggesting that dyads of both groups were more attentive to their partner or looked less at objects after the stressful condition. There were fewer Total matches states during Reunion (0.30) than during Play (0.40), F(l,47} = 7.60, p < .01, Tip = .17, suggesting more difficulty in coordination when dyads resume interaction after the SF episode. There w^as a significant effect for Reparation Rate between mismatching and matching states suggesting reparation rate was slower during Reunion (.11 per second) than during Play (.09 per second), P(l, 47) = 4.10, p < .05, Tip = .09. There was also a significant episode effect for Synchrony, F(l, 45) = 4.50, p < .04, r\^ = .11. Across the groups, mothers and infants showed a higher synchrony score during Reunion (0.32) than during Play (0.25), indicating that after the perturbation of the SF the direction of their changes in engagement level was more coordinated. The effect sizes were moderate to large (Green & Salkind, 2003), suggesting that the Reunion episode is a stressful interaction that requires a complex mutual dyadic adjustment for the mothers and infants. Discussion The findings demonstrate similarities as well as differences in the social behaviour, coping and qualities of dyadic interactions of pre-term and full-term infants and their mothers. The lack of findings of group differences fails to support the prediction that pre-terms would be less socially capable and display less social attentiveness, less positive expressive behaviour and greater negative emotion than full-terms. These findings are consistent with previous studies that found no differences in interactive SFB between pre-term and full-term infants in the second half of the first year (Brachfeld et al., 1980; Schermann-Eizirik et al, 1997). Nonetheless, as we expected based on the MRM and the literature there appear to be differences in regulatory behaviours between the groups. Pre-term infants displayed more distancing from their mothers (e.g., by turning away, twisting, or arching) than full-term infants across all the episodes. 360 R. Montirosso et al. I I ~ fN 1^ o ^^ O> <N o — I rs Ö Ö C) ci O O ro m 0^ O ^ iN ro — — "^ o rn I Ö Ö Ö Ö Ö O 00 IN 00 O. ~: - ~ •* -: O o o o oo I 00 ^^ ^O ^^ ^^ 00 rs — IN ro p ro I Ö Ö Ö Ö Ö Ö I I O O O O O I I ci Ö O ci O I I O Ö Ö Ö Ö Ö 00 "^ ro IN o >O 00 l/^ *O o O Lr) o* O ro Os rs IN O i/ï 3 T3 o O •¡3 ' ' ci ci o' o' ci ci I I 00 L/^ ro LO O^ NO , — — — fS p f^ I o o o o o o o 'c J u <u -u 3 O- -o 00 — u-i 00 rs o 00 r s I N ON — •<»• I N r s — ro — I N I Ö Ö Ö Ö o o T3 C o a E Q. 2 n) ë 1- p n) t Q. v2 -5 JZ i ii Q (1) td -F, E ifli XI .£ _ u Q § _ XI Pre-term infants in still-face paradigm 361 In particular, there was greater distancing during Play. The infant's attempts to increase physical distance from the caregiver are a sign of stress and a failure of self-regulation (Gianino & Tronick, 1988). Consequently, a greater use of distancing in the pre-term group may indicate a greater level of stress and a lower capacity for self-regulation compared to their full-term counterpart even in the normal interactions. This result and along withfindingsby others (Mouradian et al., 2000; Wolf eí al., 2002) suggests deficits of self-regulation in pre-term infants during the first year of life. Interestingly, pre-term infants also evidenced more social monitoring than full-term dyads in the Reunion episode subsequent to the stress of SF and prematurity was a significant predictor of social monitoring during the Reunion. These findings suggest that pre-term infants may try to deal with the stress of the Reunion with the compensatory strategy of increasing their social monitoring, a less aroused state than either positive or negative affective states (Greene et al., 1983; Weinberg & Trotiick, 1997). In other words, they use an other-directed strategy (Gianino & Tronick, 1988) in an attempt to obtain regulatory support from the adult to modulate their stress because their own self-directed regulatory mechanisms are inadequate. It should be noted that the differences found here were not associated to gender, developmental quotient, level of maternal depressive symptomatology, SES, or maternal SEB. These findings suggest that these low-risk pre-term infants do not have comparable emotional regulatory functions to their full-term peers and it may be important to monitor their negative affect, regulation, arousal, and stress reactivity especially in stressful environments in order to facilitate their optimal development (Feldman, 2006). Pre-term infants, like full-term infants, displayed the SF effect: more negative affect and withdra'wn behaviour, less social and positive engagement, and an increase in distancing from the caregiver in the SF episode than in the Play episode. These results are in partial disagreement with the study by Segal et al. (1995). They found no differences in negative affect between pre-term and full-term infants, but also reported that pre-term infants spent proportionally less time displaying smiles than full-terms. This difference may be due to methodological differences. Their coding scheme for positive affect may have been more sensitive than the coding scheme used in this study, whereas the coding scheme for negative affect was similar (Segal et al., 1995). There was also a reunion effect (Weinberg & Tronick, 1996) for both groups: the pre-term as well as the full-term infants displayed an increase in negative affect, less interest in objects and the environment, a decreased distancing from the caregiver and the prototypical rebound of social positive engagement. These results confirm Weinberg and Tronick's (1996) hypothesis that the process of reparation in the Reunion episode is a stressful and complex task for the infants because of the ongoing effects of the SF experience and the resumption of positive interaction. Thefindingof SF and reunion effects for the pre-term infants attest to their capacity to form social expectations, their sensitivity to stress and their capacities for coping with stress; that is it speaks to social emotional and regulatory Wellness. Several studies suggest that in thefirstyear of life mothers of pre-term infants are less responsive to infants' signals than mothers of full-term infants (Harrison, 1990; Harrison & Magill-Evans, 1996). By contrast, other studies have found that mothers of pre-term infants are particularly responsive to their infants' affective signals (Barratt etal, 1992) and several reported that they may actually exhibit more over stimulating behaviour (Brown & Bakeman, 1980; DiVitto & Goldberg, 1979; Field, 1979). The results of the present study do not support the findings of either greater or lesser responsiveness of mothers of pre-terms compared to mothers of full-terms. Mothers of both pre-terms and full-terms had similar levels of social monitoring and positive affect. Specifically, in both 362 R. Montírosso et al. groups the mothers' gaze was focused on their infants' or their infants' activities and the mothers expressed positive affect (e.g., by vocalizations, motherese, making kissing, or clicking sounds, and singing) at similar levels. These findings are consistent with previous research in which mothers of pre-term infants were described as relatively competent in their interactive behaviour with their infants (Schermann-Eizirik et al., 1997; Stjernqvist & Svenningsen, 1990). Our results on maternal behaviour are consistent with the evidence that the differences in maternal social behaviour, observed in the first 6 months of life between the pre-term and full-term groups, tend to fade during the second half of thefirstyear of age (Brachfeld et al., 1980). It seems likely that in the second 6 months of life, full-term as well as pre-term infants' behaviours (e.g., social skills) tend to consolidate and mothers' adjustment to their infants, as well as their increased experience with them may overcome stressful factors such as anxiety about the infants' health and concerns about depression. Consequently, similar tofindingsby Landry, Chapieski, & Schmidt (1986), our results suggest that maternal adaptation to pre-term infants tends to be more appropriate towards the end of the first year of life. This interpretation is further supported by the lack of differences between the groups on the three measures of coordination (Matching, Synchrony, and Reparation Rate). These results were not expected. Previous findings suggested that pre-term dyads show lower levels of synchrony. However, previous research (Eeldman, 2006; Feidman & Eidelman, 2007; Lester etal., 1985) quantified social interaction during thefirst6 months of life (3-5 months). In home observational research. Watt (1986) reported that at 3 months of age, pre-term dyads had lower levels of interaction and synchrony in comparison with their full-term counterparts but by 6 months ofage the pre-term dyads showed higher levels of simultaneous interaction, mutual gaze and were more synchronized than full-term dyads. Thus, it is possible that, in spite of the differences between full-term and pre-term dyads in their first months of life, the mutual regulatory capacities of the pre-term become more effective during the second half of the first year of life, and the quality of interaction becomes comparable between the two groups (Korja et al., 2008). Ourfindingindicated that overall for both groups, mother-infant synchrony showed a mean value of 0.09, which is consistent with earlierfindings.Tronick and Cohn (1989) found that at 6 and at 9 months ofage, synchrony varied from 0.09 to 0.23 for different mother-infant pairs and Weinberg etal. (1999) reported a mean value of 0.08. Thus, our results suggest that from 6 months of age, pre-term dyads may be able to synchronize their engagements in a similar way of fullterm dyads. With regard to Matching and Reparation Rate thefindingsof current study suggest that pre-term infants and their mothers maintained levels of mutual engagement and reparation similar to those of full-term dyads. In line with previous studies of this age range (Cohn & Tronick, 1989), the dyads of both groups spent most of their interaction time in mismatching states (more than 60%). Withdrawn behaviour and negative match were extremely rare (less than 1%) while social monitor and positive match occurred on average about 17% and 15% of the time. Furthermore, for both groups the data indicated that the dyads spent most of the time in a Look Away from Partner Match (25% and 33%, respectively). Both mother and infant looked at objects that were either proximal (e.g., infant seat) or distal (e.g., light switch). This result suggests that the partners are not only interested in the dyadic context, but also clearly show other behaviours, oriented 'outside the relationship'. A possible explanation for this result is that the behaviours refiect the infant's growing interest in objects and that the dyadic focus on other objects is a positive developmental change (Feidman, 2007). For example, between 3 and 9 months of age, episodes of shared gaze decrease to about a third of the time while shared attention to objects increases dramatically (Landry, 1995). Pre-term infants in still-face paradigm 363 In this study, the mean reparation rate across the groups was 0,09 per second. This finding is consistent with Weinberg etal.'s (1999) study, which reported a 0,10 reparation rate during normal face-to-face interactions at 6 months of age. Thus, in both studies, the repair rate was about once every 10 s, suggesting that reparations are typical features of the interaction. Consistent with the theoretical perspective of the MRM, these findings confirm that in pre-term and full-term dyads, interactions are organized into a matching and mismatching pattern with reparations resolving the mismatches (Tronick, 1989), As this mismatch-repair-match organization is similar in both groups, it appears that from 6 months of age neither healthy pre-term infants nor their mothers are compromised in their capacity to coordinate face-to-face interactions, even when they had to deal with a stressful condition (i,e,, the SF), Thus, our results on coordination suggest that a favourable infant-mother relationship may function to attenuate the effects of prematurity on early socio-emotional delays and/or that healthy pre-term infant dyads are able to achieve a normal quality of interactive regulation in the second half year of life. Another sign of the normalcy of these pre-term infants was that, as in the fullterm group, the amount of shared engagement (Total matches) decreased in the reunion play compared to the first play As has been argued by Weinberg and Tronick (1996), it is likely that after the stress of the SF, both infants and mothers find it more difficult to coordinate their social behaviour and maintain mutual engagement. This is corroborated by the finding that the rate of reparations was slower during Reunion than during the first play interaction. The finding suggests that mothers and infants made fewer attempts at reparation during Reunion than during Play and/or found it more difficult to make repairs of mismatching states. These data appear to be compatible with interpretations suggesting that the processes of reparation and mutual regulation are more stressful and complex in the Reunion episode than in the Play episode preceding the SF (Kogan & Carter, 1996; Weinberg & Tronick, 1996), Dyads showed a higher synchrony score during the Reunion episode than in the first play, suggesting that after the SF perturbation these mothers and infants were more sensitive or vigilant to changes in each other's engagement. Thus, while synchrony and matching are often seen as measuring similar phenomena they may not be, A higher synchrony score may indicate that the dyads tracked each other's behaviour more carefully whereas a lower matching scores may indicate that they are less able to coordinate their behaviours successfully. In this perspective, a higher level of synchrony may be indicative of reduced flexibility in interaction (Tronick & Cohn, 1989) and suggestive of greater dyadic effort to create more predictability in the interaction (Jaffe et al, 2001), Thus, while synchrony is typically thought of as an indication of a 'good' interaction (Stern, 1985), it may actually be a measure of hypervigilance, possibly being used as a strategy for renegotiating the interaction after a stress. In summary, the lack of group differences indicates that the pre-term infant-mother dyads reacted typically to the additional stress of the Reunion episode. There are several limitations in the present study. The sample size though typical for many micro analytic studies of interactions is relatively small. The pre-term infants w^ere relatively healthy so the results cannot be generalized to more at-risk populations of infants. Replications with a larger sample and greater variations of SES and health status are needed. The study did not follow-up these infants so that we cannot assess the relations between the differences in regulatory behaviours and later outcome. It would also be interesting to have other measures of stress (e,g,, skin conductance. Ham & Tronick, 2008) on the infants and mothers. 364 R. Montirosso et al. Nevertheless, the results from this study suggest that it is important to evaluate preterm infants and their mothers in different interactive contexts (normal vs. stressful conditions) and to obtain measures of their coordination in addition to measures of each partner's interactive behaviours in order to obtain a better understanding of the mother-infant interaction. The pre-term infant-mother dyads were able to coordinate their interactions in a similar manner to full-term dyads. Pre-term dyads also demonstrated similar capacities for the reparation of interactive matches, which suggests that pre-term infants experience normal levels of reparation. Thesefindingson coordination and reparation, along with the findings on the typicality of the pre-terms' reaction to the SF, demonstrate the coping and social competence of the pre-term infants and their mothers. In the MRM it is argued that reparation is one of the primary mechanisms underlying normal development, including the development of trust and attachment, mastery, the experience that negative affect or stress can be transformed into positive affect and the elaboration of coping capacities (Tronick, 1989, 2007). The social competence observed in these healthy pre-term itifant-mother dyads suggests that their interactive experience is typical and likely to lead to positive developmental outcomes. These findings can be used clinically to reassure parents, which can further reduce their anxiety and consequently improve their interaction with their pre-term infant. A technique may be to explain to parents that miscoordination and mismatching in their interactions with their itifants is normative along with the value of reparation for their infants' development. Understanding the importance of the inherent messiness of interactions may help parents overcome their search for the perfect, wished-for interaction (Brazelton, 1999) which only serves to make them anxious when the fail to achieve it. Clinicians also need to see reparation and miscoordinations as normal interactive processes. Indeed, the findings suggest that the clinician can expect pre-term infant-mother pairs to be interacting w^ell by 6 months of age, but 'well' must be seen as involving interactive mismatches and reparations as indicators of normalcy and not as indicators of something going awry and requiring intervention (Trotiick, 2007). Acknowledgement This research was supported by funds from the Italian Health Minister (Ricerca Corrente 2003), partially supported by an unrestricted educational grant from Chiesi Farmaceutici, and from grants from the National Institute Child Health & Human Development (R01HD37138 & RO1HDO5O459; E. T, PI) and the National Science Foundation (NSF 06-511; E. T., PI). The authors wish to thank the nursing and neonatology staff from the nurseries of the Manzoni Hospital NICU (Lecco) and community pediatricians in Lecco for their support. We thank Patrizia Cozzi for their support in the data collection. We also wish to thank Sara Averna, Gloria Mauri, Sonia Monticelli, Viviana Sabadini, Elisa Zoboli for their help in data coding; when the research was conducted they were graduate psychology students at the Catholic University of Milan. Furthermore, we are grateful to Carlo Magni from Scientific Institute Medea's Bioengineering Laboratory for the software and the computer program used for the video analyses. Thanks are due to Dr Uberto Pozzoli for his support in the matching analyses. Finally, special thanks go to all infants and their mothers participating in this study. References Adamson, L. B., & Frick, J. E. (2003). The still-face: A history of a shared experimental paradigm. Infancy, 4(4), 451-473. Pre-term infants in still-face paradigm 365 Als, H. (1983). Infant individuality: Assessing patterns of very early development. In J. D. Call, E. Galenson, & R. L. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry. New York, NY: Basic Books. Als, H., Duffy, E H., MeAnulty, G. B., Rivkin, M. J., Vajapeyam, S., Mulkern, R. V, etal (2004). Early experience alters brain function and structure. Pediatrics, 113(/î), 846-857. Anand, K. J., & Scalzo, E M. (2000). Can adverse neonatal experiences alter brain development and subsequent behaviour? Biology of the Neonate, 77(2), 69-82. Barratt, M. S., Roach, M. A., & Leavitt, L. A. (1992). Early channels of mother-infant communication: Preterm and term infants./oMma/ of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33(7), 1193-1204. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of Beck depression inventory: TWenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(1), 77-100. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erlbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571. Beebe, B., & Lachmann, E M. (2002). Infant research and adult treatment: Co-constructing interactions. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press. Brachfeld, S., Goldberg, S., & Sloman, J. (1980). Parent infant interaction in free play at 8 and 12 months: Effects of prematurity and immaturity. Infant Behaviour and Development, 3(1), 289-305. Brazelton, T. B. (1999). How to help parents of young children: The touchpoints model.Joumal of Perinatoiogy, 19(6), 6-7. Brown, J., & Bakeman, R. (1980). Relationships of human mothers with their infants during the first year of Ufe. In R. Bell & W. Smotherman (Eds.), Maternal influences on eariy behaviour (pp. 437-447). New York: Spectrum. Brunet, C, & Lézine, I. (1955). Échelle de développement psychomoteur de tapremière enfance. Manuel d'instructions. [Scaie of the psychomotor deveiopment of early childhood. Instruction Manual]. Clamart, France: Éditions Scientifiques et Psychotechniques. Cohen, J. (I960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educationai and Psychoiogicai Measurement, 20(1), 37-46. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Appiied regression/correiation analysis for behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cohn, J. E, & Troniek, E. Z. (1987). Mother-infant face-to-face interaction: the sequence of dyadic states at 3-6 and 9 months. Developmental Psychology, 23, 68-77. Crawford, J. W. (1982). Mother-infant interaction in premature and full-term infants. Child Development, 53('i), 957-962. Crnic, K. A., Ragozin, A. S., Greenberg, M. T., Robinson, N. M., & Basham, R. B. (1983). Social interaction and developmental competence of preterm and full-term infants during the first year of life. Child Development, 54(5), 1199-1210. DiVitto, B., & Goldberg, S. (1979). The effects of newborn medical status on early parent-infant interaction. In T. M. Eield, A. M. Sostek, S. Goldberg, & H. H. Shuman (Eds.), Infants bom at risk (pp. 311-332). New York, NY: Spectrum Books. Eckerman, C. O., Hsu, H. C, Molitor, A., Leung, E. H., & Goldstein, R. E (1999). Infant arousal in an en-face exchange with a new partner: Effects of prematurity and perinatal biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 282-293. Eckerman, C. O., Oehler, J. M., & Medvin, M. B. (1994). Premature newborns as social partners before term age. Infant Behaviour and Development, 17(\), 55-70. Eeldman, R. (2006). Erom biological rhythms to social rhythms: Physiological precursors of mother-infant synchrony. Developmental Psychology, 42, 175-188. Eeldman, R. (2007). On the origins of background emotions: Erom affect synchrony to symbolic expression. Emotion, 7(3), 601-611. Eeldman, R., & Eidelman, A. 1. (2007). Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infantmother and infant-father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: The role of neonatal vagal tone. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 290-302. 366 R. Montirosso et al. Feldman, R., Greenhaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 223-231. Feldman, R., Weiler, A., Leckman, J. F., Kuint, J., & Eidelman, A. I. (1999). The nature of the mother's tie to her infant: Maternal bonding under conditions of proximity, separation, and potential loss, foumal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(6), 929-939. Feldman, R., Weiler, A., Sirota, L., & Eidelman, A. I. (2003). Testing a famUy intervention hypothesis: The contribution of mother-infant skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) to family interaction, proximity and touch, foumal of Family Psychology. 17iV), 94-107. Field, T. M. (1979). Interaction patterns of preterm and term infants. In T. M. Field, A. M. Sostek, S. Goldberg, & H. H. Shuman (Eds.), Infants bom at risk (pp. 333-356). New York, NY: Spectrum Books. Field, T. M. (1981). Infant arousal, attention, and affect during early interactions. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. Rovee-Collier (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 1, pp. 57-100). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Field, T. M. (1987). Interaction and attachment in normal and atypical infants, foumal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55(6), 853-859. Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Borghini, A., Moessinger, A., & MuUer-Nix, C. (2006). Early dyadic patterns of mother-infant interactions and outcomes of prematurity at 18 months. Pediatrics, 118(1), 107-114. Gesell, A., & Amatruda, C. S. (1947). Developmental diagnosis (2nd ed.). New York: HoeberHarber. Gianino, A., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). The mutual regulation model: The infant's self and interactive regulation and coping and defense capacities. In T. Field, P. McCabe, & N. Schneiderman (Eds.), Stress and coping (Vol. 2, pp. 47-68). HiUsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Green, S. B., & Salkind, N. J. (2003). Using SPSS for Windows and Macintosh: Analyzing and understanding data (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Greene, J. G., Fox, N. A., & Lewis, M. (1983). The relationship between neonatal characteristics and three-month mother-infant interaction in high-risk infants. Child Development, 54(5), 1286-1296. Ham, J., & Tronick, E. (2008). A procedure for the measurement of infant skin conductance and its initial validation using clap induced startle. Developmental Psychophysiology, 50(6), 626-631. Harrison, M. J. (1990). A comparison of parental interactions with term and preterms infants. Research in Nursing and Health, 73(3), 173-179. Harrison, M. J., & Magill-Evans, J. (1996). Mother and father intemctions over the first year with term and preterm infants. Research in Nursing and Health, 19(6), 451-459. Hofer, M. A. (1994). Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 55(2-3), 192-207. Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Pour-factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University, Department of Sociology, Unpublished manuscript. Jaffe, F, Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66, 1-151. Kogan, N., & Carter, A. S. (1996). Mother-infant reengagement following the still-face: The role of maternal emotional availability in infant affect regulation. Infant Behaviour and Devetopment, 19(5), 359-370. Korja, R., Maunu, J., Kirjavainen, J., Savonlahti, F., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., et al. (2008). Mother-infant interaction is influenced by the amount of holding in preterm infants. Early Human Development, 84(4), 257-267. Landry, S. H. (1995). The development of joint attention in premature low birth weight infants: Effects of early medical complications and maternal attention-directing behaviors. In C. Moore & P J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hülsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Pre-term infants in stiii-face paradigm 367 Landry, S. H., Chapieski, M. L., & Schmidt, M. (1986). Effects of maternal attention-directing strategies on preterms' response to toys. Infant Behaviour and Development, 5(4), 257-269. Landry, S. H., Chapieski, M. L., Richardson, M. A., Palmer, J., & Hall, S. (1990). The social competence of children born prematurely: Effects of medical complications and parent behaviors. Child Development, 61(5'), l605-l6l6. Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C. L., & Swank, P. R. (1998). The relation of change in maternai interactive style to the developing social competence of full-term and preterm chUdren. Child Development, 69, 105-123. Lester, B. M., Hoffman, J., & Brazelton, T. B. (1985). The rhythmic structure of mother-infant interaction in term and preterm infants. Child Development, 56(V), 15-27. Lewis, M., Hitchcock, D. F. A., & Sullivan, M. W. (2004). Physiological and emotional reactivity to learning and frustration. Infancy, 6(1), 121-143. Lowe, J., Woodward, B., & Papüe, L. A. (2005). Emotional regulation and its impact on development in extremely low birth weight infants. Joumal of Developmental and Behavioural Pediatrics, 26(5), 209-213. Macey, T. J., Harmon, R. J., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1987). Impact of premature birth on the development of the infant in the famäy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55(6), 846-852. Malatesta, C. Z., Culver, C, Tesman, J. R., & Shepard, B. (1989). The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs for the Society of Research in Child Development, 54, 1-2. Malatesta, C. Z., Grigoryev, P, Lamb, C, Albin, M., & Culver, C. (1986). Emotion socialization and expressive development in preterm and full-term infants. Child Development, 57(2), 316-330. McGehee, L. J., & Eckerman, C. O. (1983). The preterm infant as a social partner: Responsive but unreadable. Infant Behaviour and Development, 6(4), 461-470. Minde, K., Whitelaw, A., Brown, J., & Fitzhardinge, P (1983). Effect of neonatal complications in premature infants on early parent-infant interactions. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25(6), 763-777. Mouradian, L. E., Als, H., & Coster, W. J. (2000). Neurobehavioural functioning of healthy preterm infants of varying gestational ages. Developmental and Behavioural Pediatrics, 21(6), 408-416. MuUer-Nix, C, Eorcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A., & Ansermet, E (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother-child interactions. Early Human Development, 79(2), 145-158. Munro, B. (1997). Statistical methods for health care research. Philadelphia, PA: J.B. Lippincott. O'Hara, M., Rehm, L. P, & Campbell, S. B. (1983). Postpartum depression: A role for social network and life stress variables. The Joumal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 171(6), 336-341. Sander, L. W. (2000). >XTiere are we going in thefieldof infant mental health? Infant Mental Health Joumal, 21(1-2), 3-20. Schermann-Eizirik, L., Hagekull, B., Bohlin, G., Persson, K., & Sedin, G. (1997). Interaction between mothers and infants born at risk during the first six months of corrected age. Acta Paediatrica, 86(8), 864-872. Segal, L. B., Oster, H., Cohen, M., Caspi, B., Myers, M., & Brown, D. (1995). Smiling and fussing in seven-month-old preterm and full-term black infants in the still-face situation. Child Development, 66(6), 1829-1843. Singer, L. T., Eulton, S., Davillier, M., Koshy, D., Salvator, A., & Baley, J. E. (2003). Effects of infant risk status and maternal psychological distress on maternal-infant interactions during the first year of life. Joumal of Developmental and Behavioural Pediatrics, 24(4), 233-241. Spangler, G., Schieche, M., llg, U., Maier, U., & Ackermann, C. (1994). Maternal sensitivity as an organizer for biobehavioural regulation in infancy. Deveiopmental Psychobiology, 27(7), 425-437. Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York, NY: Basic Books. 368 R. Montirosso et al. Stjernqvist, K,, & Svenningsen, N, W, (1990), Neurobehaviour development at term of extremely low-birth weight infants (< 901 g). Development Medicine and Child Neurology, 32(8), 679-688, Striano, T,, & Rochat, P, (1999), Developmental link between dyadic and triadic social competence in infancy, British Joumat of Developmental Psychotogy, 17(4), 551-562, Thoman, E, B,, & Graham, S, E, (1986), Self-regulation of stimulation by premature infants. Pediatrics, 78(5), 855-860, Tronick, E, Z, (1989), Emotions and emotional communication in infants, American Psychologist, 44(2), 112-119, Tronick, E, Z, (2003), 'Of course all relationships are unique': How co-creative processes generate unique mother-infant and patient-therapist relationships and change other relationships, Psychoanatytic Inquiry, 23(5), 473-491, Tronick, E, Z, (2004), '\X'hy is connection with others so critical? The formation of dyadic states of consciousness and the expansion of individuals' states of consciousness: Coherence governed selection and the co-creation of meaning out of messy meaning making'. In J, Nadel & D, Muir (Eds,), Emotionat development (pp, 293-315), New York, NY: Oxford University Press, Tronick, E, Z, (2007), The neurobehavioral and sociat-emotionat devetopment of infants and chitdren. New York, NY: W,W Norton and Co, Tronick, E, Z,, & Cohn, J, F, (1989), Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: Age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination, Chitd Devetopment, 60(\), 85-92, Tronick, E, Z,, & Gianino, A, (1986), Interactive mismatch and repair: Challenges to the coping infant. Zero to Three. Buttetin of the National Centerfor Clinical Infant Programs, 5(5), 1 -6, Tronick, E, Z,, Als, H,, Adamson, L,, Wise, S,, & Brazelton, T, B, (1978), The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction, foumat of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17(1), 1-13, Tronick, E, Z,, Messinger, D, S,, Weinberg, M, K,, Lester, B, M,, Ugasse, L,, Seifer, R,, etal (2005), Cocaine exposure is associated with subtle compromises of infants' and mothers' socialemotional behaviour and dyadic features of their interaction in the face-to-face stiU-face paradigm. Developmental Psychotogy, 41(5), 711-722, Van Beek, Y, Hopkins, B,, Hoeksma, J, B,, & Samsom, J, F, (1994), Prematurity, posture and the development of looking behaviour during early communication,/owr««/ of Chitd Psychotogy and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 35(6), 1093-1107, Watt, J, (1986), Interaction and development in the first year: I, The effects of prematurity. Early Human Development, 13(2), 195-210, Weinberg, M, K,, & Tronick, E, Z, (1996), Infant affective reactions to the resumption of maternal interaction after the stUl-face, Chitd Devetopment, 67(5'), 905-914, Weinberg, M, K,, & Tronick, E, Z, (1997), Maternal depression and infant maladjustment: A failure of mutual regulation. In J, D, Noshpitz (Ed,), The handbook of child and adolescent psychiatry: Vol 1. Infancy and preschool: Development and syndromes (pp. 177-191), New York, NY: Wiley, Weinberg, M, K,, & Tronick, E, Z, (1998). Infant and Caregiver Engagement Phases system. Boston, MA: Children's Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Unpublished manuscript, Weinberg, M, K,, Tronick, E, Z,, Cohn, J, F, & Oison, K, L, (1999), Gender differences in emotional expressivity and self-regulation during early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 175-188, Wolf, M, J,, Koldewijn, K,, Beelen, A,, Smit, B,, Hedlund, R,, & de Groot, I, J, (2002), Neurobehavioural and developmental profile of very low birth weight preterm infants in early infancy. Acta Paediatrica, 91(S), 930-938, Received 14 February 2008; revised version received 15 January 2009 Copyright of British Journal of Developmental Psychology is the property of British Psychological Society and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.