EXPLORATION OF THE BENEFITS OF AN ACTIVITY

advertisement
Psychological Reports, 2009, 105, 1-16. © Psychological Reports 2009
EXPLORATION OF THE BENEFITS OF AN ACTIVITYSPECIFIC TEST OF TEMPERAMENT1, 2
IRINA N. TROFIMOVA
Collective Intelligence Laboratory
Department of Psychiatry and Neuroscience
McMaster University
Summary.—The Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ) was proposed
‹¢ȱžœŠ•˜Ÿȱ’—ȱŗşŞşȱŠ—ȱœž‹œŽšžŽ—•¢ȱŽœŽȱ’—ȱ꟎ȱ•Š—žŠŽœǯȱ‘ŽȱšžŽœ’˜——Š’›Žȱ
assesses four temperamental traits (Ergonicity, Plasticity, Tempo, and Emotionality) in three separate areas of activity: physical, verbal-social, and intellectual. The
œŒŠ•Žœȱ Š›Žȱ Š••ȱ ŠŒ’Ÿ’¢Ȭœ™ŽŒ’ęŒǯȱ —ȱ ŝŝśȱ Š—Š’Š—ȱ œž‹“ŽŒœǰȱ  ˜ȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ Žœœȱ
were compared, both developed on the basis of Pavlovian studies of the nervous
œ¢œŽ–DZȱ ‘Žȱ ŠŒ’Ÿ’¢Ȭœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ Š™™›˜ŠŒ‘ȱ ǻǼȱ Š—ȱ ‘Žȱ —˜—œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ ŠŸ•˜Ÿ’Š—ȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—Š•ȱž›ŸŽ¢ȱǻǼǯȱ˜›Žȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱœŽ¡ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ Ž›Žȱ˜ž—ȱ˜—ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢Ȭ
œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱȱ‘Š—ȱ˜—ȱ‘Žȱ—˜—œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱȱœŒŠ•Žœǯȱ‘Žȱ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ˜ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ‘ŽȱȱœŒŠ•ŽœȱŠ—ȱ‘Žȱ’–ŽȱŠ”Ž—ȱ˜—ȱŠ—ȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—Š•ȱŠœ”ȱ›Žšž’›’—ȱ
a prolonged and intense word-assessment activity showed stronger correlations
’‘ȱ‘Žȱœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱȱ–ŽŠœž›’—ȱ‘Žȱ¢—Š–’ŒȱŠœ™ŽŒœȱ˜ȱœ˜Œ’Š•ȬŸŽ›‹Š•ȱ
ŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ǰȱŠ—ȱ—˜ȱ ’‘ȱ‘Žȱȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱœŒŠ•Žǰȱ ‘’Œ‘ȱ’œȱ‹ŠœŽȱ˜—ȱŠȱȃŽ—eral arousal” concept. The results supported the separation of temperament traits
›Ž•ŠŽȱ˜ȱ‘›ŽŽȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ¢™Žœȱ˜ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’’ŽœȱŠ—ȱ˜™™˜œŽȱ˜ȱȃŽ—Ž›Š•ȱŠ›˜žœŠ•Ȅȱ‘Ž˜›ies of temperament.
Many researchers consider temperament to comprise the contentfree, formal dimensions of behaviour, whereas personality is considered a
sociopsychological construct comprising the content characteristics of human behaviour (¢œŽ—Œ”ȱǭȱ¢œŽ—Œ”ǰȱŗşŜŞDzȱŽ‹¢•’œ¢—ǰȱŗşŝŘDz Gray, 1982;
Rusalov, 1989; Strelau & Angleitner, 1991). As Strelau and Angleitner
(1991ǰȱ™ǯȱŜǼȱ™˜’—Žȱ˜žȱ’—ȱ‘Ž’›ȱ›ŽŸ’Ž ǰȱȃ–˜œȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ›ŽœŽŠ›Œ‘Ž›œȱ
agree that temperament, whatever the traits and structure to which this
concept refers, has a strong biological determination.” This assumption
has its roots in the facts that temperament characteristics can be observed
›˜–ȱ‘Žȱꛜȱ ŽŽ”œȱ˜ȱ•’ŽȱŠ—ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ’—ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—Š•ȱ
traits have a strong genetic determination.” The European tradition in anŠ•¢œ’œȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱǻŠ—ǰȱŗŝşŞȦŗşŝŚDzȱŽ›—ǰȱŗşŖŖǰȱŒ’Žȱ›˜–ȱŠ–’Ž••ǰȱ
2003; Heymans, 1929; Pavlov, 1941; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968) described
›ŽœœȱŒ˜››Žœ™˜—Ž—ŒŽȱ˜ȱǯȱǯȱ›˜ę–˜ŸŠǰȱşŘȱ˜ –Š—ȱǯǰȱ
Š–’•˜—ǰȱ—Š›’˜ǰȱŠ—ŠŠȱŞȱ
ŘŜȱ˜›ȱŽȬ–Š’•ȱǻ’›˜ę–˜ŸŠȓœ¢–™Š’Œ˜ǯŒŠǼǯ
2
The author acknowledges the hard work of students in administration of the tests and of
the Semantic Task: Kristine Espiritu, Chandrima Bandyopadhyay, Samira Patel, and Vanita
Marques, and also the help of Dr. William Sulis in the work on the English STQ and in editing
the manuscript of this article.
1
DOI 10.2466/PR0.105.2.
2
I. N. TROFIMOVA
two basic components of temperament, Activity characteristics and Emotionality characteristics.
A two-component model of temperament was developed further in
the Russian psychological school, which studied the types and properties of the nervous system as the basis of the most consistent personal’¢ȱ›Š’œǯȱ’—ŒŽȱŠŸ•˜ŸȂœȱ’–ŽȱŠȱ‘Žȱ‹Ž’——’—ȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱŘŖ‘ȱŒŽ—ž›¢ǰȱŽ¡Ž—œ’ŸŽȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—Š•ȱ ˜›”ȱ ’‘ȱ‘ž–Š—ȱ™Š›’Œ’™Š—œȱ‘Šœȱ‹ŽŽ—ȱŒ˜—žŒŽȱ’—ȱ
the laboratories of Teplov and Nebylitsyn (see the review of Gray, 1964)
Š—ȱ žœŠ•˜Ÿȱ ǻŗşŝşǼǯȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ Ž¡™Ž›’–Ž—œȱ œ‘˜ Žȱ ‘Šȱ ‘Žȱ œ›Ž—‘ȱ ˜ȱ Ž¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱ˜›ȱ’—‘’‹’’˜—ȱ’—ȱ‘ŽȱŒŽ—›Š•ȱ—Ž›Ÿ˜žœȱœ¢œŽ–ȱǻǼȱ’œȱŽ¡™›ŽœœŽȱŠœȱ
how long the individual can sustain activation or inhibition of activation.
The mobility of the CNS processes is indicated as the plasticity of behav’˜ž›ǰȱ‘˜ ȱŽŠœ’•¢ȱ‘Žȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱŒŠ—ȱœŠ›ȱ˜›ȱœ˜™ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ǰȱŠ—ȱ‘˜ ȱ̎¡’‹•Žȱ
and adaptive the individual is to new circumstances or instructions. The
‹Š•Š—ŒŽȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱŽ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ’—‘’‹’’˜—ȱ Šœȱ‘˜ž‘ȱ˜ȱ‹Žȱ‘Žȱ‹Šœ’œȱ˜ȱ
emotionality, impulsivity, or detachment behaviour. The British psycholo’œȱŽě›Ž¢ȱ›Š¢ǰȱ ‘˜ȱŒ˜—žŒŽȱ–˜œȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ ˜›”ȱ˜—ȱ‘Žȱ›Š—œ•Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ
Š—Š•¢œ’œȱ˜ȱŠŸ•˜ŸȂœȱȃ¢™Žœȱ˜ȱȄȱǻ›Š¢ǰȱŗşŜŚǼǰȱ˜ž—ȱŠȱœ›˜—ȱ™Š›Š••Ž•ȱ
between the concept of arousal and the Pavlovian concept of the strength
of a nervous system. Elucidating the relationships between various brain
œ›žŒž›Žœǰȱ ›Š¢ȱ ™›˜™˜œŽȱ Šȱ –˜Ž•ȱ ‘Šȱ Ž¡™•Š’—œȱ ‹˜‘ȱ ‘Žȱ ˜ž›ȱ Œ•Šœœ’ŒŠ•ȱ
temperamental types and Pavlov’s types in terms of the relationship between approach and withdrawal systems (Gray, 1982).
•Š’–’›ȱžœŠ•˜Ÿǰȱ ‘˜ǰȱŠĞŽ›ȱŽ‹¢•’œ¢—ǰȱ’—‘Ž›’Žȱ‘ŽȱŠ‹˜›Š˜›¢ȱ˜ȱ
’쎛Ž—’Š•ȱœ¢Œ‘˜•˜¢ȱŠ—ȱ’쎛Ž—’Š•ȱœ¢Œ‘˜™‘¢œ’˜•˜¢ȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ—œ’žŽȱ
˜ȱœ¢Œ‘˜•˜¢ȱž—Ž›ȱ‘Žȱžœœ’Š—ȱŒŠŽ–¢ȱ˜ȱŒ’Ž—ŒŽœǰȱœŠ›Žȱ’—ȱ‘ŽȱŗşŝŖœȱ
Šȱ•Š›ŽȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—Š•ȱ™›˜“ŽŒȱŠ—Š•¢£’—ȱ¢™Žœȱ˜ȱȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ǰȱœ›Ž—‘ǰȱŠ—ȱ
mobility of nervous processes in various modalities, performance by humans in deterministic and probabilistic conditions, tempo of reading and
motor tasks, verbal activity, performance on tests of intelligence, and behavioral particularities associated with the various temperamental traits.
Based on this work, Rusalov concluded that temperamental traits are ac’Ÿ’¢Ȭœ™ŽŒ’ęŒDZȱŽ—Ž›Ž’Œȱ•ŽŸŽ•ȱ˜›ȱŽ–™˜ȱ˜ȱ™Ž›˜›–Š—ŒŽȱ–’‘ȱ‹Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ
for a given individual in physical, social, or intellectual activities, therefore the aspects of the performance of these activities should be assessed
and analysed separately.
ŠœŽȱ ˜—ȱ ‘’œȱ Ž¡™Ž›’–Ž—œǰȱ žœŠ•˜Ÿȱ ™›˜™˜œŽȱ ‘Žȱ ›žŒž›Žȱ ˜ȱ Ž–perament theory and developed his Structure of Temperament Question—Š’›Žǯȱ‘ŽȱꛜȱŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ—•’œ‘ȱŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ›žŒž›Žȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›ament Questionnaire (STQ; Rusalov, 1989) had four scales: Ergonicity
ǻŽ—ž›Š—ŒŽǰȱ ‘Žȱ Š‹’•’¢ȱ ˜ȱ ”ŽŽ™ȱ ’—Ž—œ’ŸŽȱ ˜›”Ǽǰȱ •Šœ’Œ’¢ȱ ǻ˜›ȱ ̎¡’‹’•’¢ǰȱ
‘ŽȱŠ‹’•’¢ȱ˜ȱŽěŽŒ’ŸŽ•¢ȱœ ’Œ‘ȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱŠœ”œȱ˜›ȱ˜ȱŒ‘Š—Žȱ‘Žȱ Š¢ȱ˜ȱ™Ž›-
ACTIVITY-SPECIFIC TEST OF TEMPERAMENT
3
formance), Tempo, Emotionality separately in physical-motor (Motor) activity, and social activity (such as reading, writing, speaking, or communiŒŠ’˜—Ǽǯȱœ’–ŠŽœȱ˜ȱ’—Ž›—Š•ȱ›Ž•’Š‹’•’¢ȱ˜›ȱ‘ŽȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ‘ŠŸŽȱ›Š—Žȱ›˜–ȱǯŝŖȱ
to .81. Then, a third set of four scales was added to measure aspects of intellectual activity, with the development of adult, teenage, middle school,
Š—ȱ™›ŽœŒ‘˜˜•ȱ¡Ž—ŽȱŸŽ›œ’˜—œȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱȱǻžœŠ•˜ŸǰȱŗşşŝǰȱŘŖŖŚǼǯȱ‘Žȱ¡Ž—Žȱȱ‘ŠœȱŠȱŚȬ™˜’—ȱ’”Ž›ȱœŒŠ•Žȱ˜›–ŠDzȱœŽŽȱ™™Ž—’¡ȱǰȱ™ǯȱȊȊȊǰȱ˜›ȱ
Ž¡Š–™•Žœȱ˜ȱ’Ž–œǯ A summary of the validation of the Structure of Tem™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱžŽœ’˜——Š’›Žȱ’œȱ™›˜Ÿ’Žȱ’—ȱ™™Ž—’¡ȱǰȱ™ǯȱȊȊȊǯ
‘Žȱ‹Ž—Žęœȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱžœŠ•˜Ÿȱ›žŒž›Žȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱžŽœ’˜——Š’›Žȱ
were that it žœŽȱ‘ŽȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢Ȭœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ–˜Ž•ȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ’—ȱ ‘’Œ‘ȱ‘Žȱ
œŒŠ•Žœȱ Ž›Žȱ›˜ž™Žȱ’—ȱŠŒ˜›œȱ‹¢ȱ‘Žȱ¢™Žœȱ˜ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ȱǻ˜›ȱŽ¡Š–™•Žǰȱ˜Œ’Š•ȱ
Ergonicity, Social Plasticity, and Social Tempo) and not by dynamic aspects
of activity. The previous models of temperament and personality did not
’œ’—ž’œ‘ȱŠ–˜—ȱŠ›ŽŠœȱ˜ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ǰȱŒ˜—œ’Ž›’—ǰȱ˜›ȱŽ¡Š–™•ŽǰȱŠ›˜žœŠ•ȱ’—ȱ
–˜˜›ȱŠ—ȱœ˜Œ’Š•ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ȱǻ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱ˜›ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ—Ž›Ÿ˜žœȱœ¢œŽ–Ǽȱ
Šœȱ Šȱ —˜—œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ Ž—Ž›Š•ȱ ŠŒ’ŸŠ’˜—ȱ ˜ȱ ‘Žȱ —Ž›Ÿ˜žœȱ œ¢œŽ–ȱ ǻŠŸ•˜Ÿǰȱ ŗşŚŗDzȱ
Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968; NebylitsynǰȱŗşŝŘDzȱ›Š¢ǰȱŗşŞŘDzȱ˜œŠȱǭȱŒ›ŠŽǰȱ
1992; Strelau, 1999).
‘Žȱ‘Ž˜›Ž’ŒŠ•ȱ›Š’˜—Š•Žȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ™›ŽœŽ—ȱœž¢ȱ Šœȱ˜ȱŽ¡™•˜›Žȱ‘Žȱ‹Ž—Žęœȱ˜ȱŠ—Š•¢œ’œȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ›Š’œȱœŽ™Š›ŠŽ•¢ȱ’—ȱ™‘¢œ’ŒŠ•ǰȱœ˜Œ’Š•ȬŸŽ›‹Š•ǰȱ
and intellectual activities. Many models of temperament and personality
Œ˜—’—žŽȱ˜ȱ˜••˜ ȱ‘Žȱœ˜ȬŒŠ••ŽȱȃŽ—Ž›Š•ȱŠ›˜žœŠ•ȄȱŠ™™›˜ŠŒ‘ǰȱŒ˜—œ’Ž›’—ȱ
that only one general trait is related to the energetic component of behav’˜ž›ǰȱ—Š–Ž•¢ȱȃœ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱŽ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȄȱǻŠŸ•˜ŸǰȱŗşŚŗDzȱ›Ž•ŠžǰȱŗşşşǼǰȱȃ•’ŸŽ•’—ŽœœȄȱǻŠĴŽ••ǰȱŗşŜśǼǰȱȃŽ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—Ȅȱǻ¢œŽ—Œ”ȱǭȱ¢œŽ—Œ”ǰȱŗşŜŞDzȱ˜‘‹Š›ǰȱ
ŗşŞŞDzȱ ’ȱ ’ŸŽȱ –˜Ž•ǰȱ ’—Œ•ž’—ȱ ˜œŠȱ ǭȱ Œ›ŠŽǰȱ ŗşşŘǼǰȱ ȃŠŒ’Ÿ’¢Ȅȱ ǻŽhavioural Approach System of Gray, 1982; Buss & Plomin, 1984; Windle
ǭȱ Ž›—Ž›ǰȱ ŗşŞŜǼǰȱ ȃ›’ŸŽȱ ™Ž›œ’œŽ—ŒŽȄȱ ǻŠ›ŸŽ›ȱ ǭȱ ‘’Žǰȱ ŗşşŚDzȱ •˜—’—Ž›ǰȱ
›£¢‹ŽŒ”ǰȱŸ›Š”’ŒǰȱǭȱŽ£Ž•ǰȱŗşşŚǼǰȱ˜›ȱ“žœȱȃŠ›˜žœŠ•ȄȱǻŽ‘›Š‹’Š—ȱǭȱŠ—”ǰȱ
ŗşŝŞǼǯȱ ‘Žȱ œŠ–Žȱ ’œȱ ›žŽȱ ˜›ȱ –˜‹’•’¢ȱ ǻ’ǯŽǯǰȱ ‘˜ ȱ ŽŠœ’•¢ȱ ‘Žȱ ŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ȱ ŒŠ—ȱ ‹Žȱ
started and carried out). Previous studies using the STQ showed that the
arousal-related traits of temperament correlate with the personality traits
’—ȱŠȱ’œŒ›’–’—Š˜›¢ȱ–Š——Ž›ǯȱ˜›ȱŽ¡Š–™•Žǰȱ‘’‘ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ˜ȱ¡›ŠŸŽ›sion, as measured by the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, are found
with the Social Ergonicity (rœȶƽȶǯŜŞȮǯŝŚǼǰȱ˜Œ’Š•ȱ•Šœ’’Œ’¢ȱǻrœȶƽȶǯŚřȮǯŜśǼǰȱŠ—ȱ
Social Tempo (rœȶƽȶǯřşȮǯśŗǼȱ œŒŠ•Žœȱ ˜ȱ ‘Žȱ ȱ ǻžœŠ•˜Ÿǰȱ ŗşŞşDzȱ ›Ž‹—Ž›ȱ ǭȱ
Stough, 1993; Dumenci, 1995; Zin’ko, 2006), but not with the scales of Mo˜›ȱ˜›ȱ—Ž••ŽŒžŠ•ȱ›˜—’Œ’¢ǯȱ’–’•Š›•¢ǰȱ‘’‘ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ˜ȱ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—ǰȱ
as measured by the Big Five, are found with the Social Ergonicity (rȶƽȶǯŘşǼȱ
and Social Tempo (rȶƽȶǯŘşǼȱœŒŠ•Žœǰȱ ‘’•Žȱ‘ŽȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ ’‘ȱ‘Žȱ˜˜›ȱ›˜—’Œ’¢ȱŠ—ȱ˜˜›ȱŽ–™˜ȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ Ž›Žȱ—˜ȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱǻž–Ž—Œ’ǰȱŗşşśǼǯ
4
I. N. TROFIMOVA
In this study, a test measuring temperament traits separately in three
Š›ŽŠœȱ ˜ȱ ŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ȱ ǻ’ǯŽǯǰȱ ›žŒž›Žȱ ˜ȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ žŽœ’˜——Š’›ŽǼȱ Šœȱ Ž¡pected to yield more knowledge about biologically based individual difŽ›Ž—ŒŽœȱ ‘Š—ȱ Šȱ —˜—œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ Žœȱ ˜ȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ǯȱ Ž¡ȱ ’œȱ ˜—Žȱ ˜ȱ ‘Žȱ –Š’—ȱ
‹’˜•˜’ŒŠ•ȱŠŒ˜›œȱ›Ž•ŠŽȱ˜ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœǰȱ˜ĞŽ—ȱŠœœ˜Œ’ŠŽȱ ’‘ȱ
particular temperaments. Previous studies using the STQ have shown
that separation of temperament traits related to verbal-social and physiŒŠ•ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’’Žœȱ™›˜Ÿ’Žœȱ’–™˜›Š—ȱ’—˜›–Š’˜—ȱŠ‹˜žȱœŽ¡ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱǻŠœ¢Ȭ
Vasyž›ŠǰȱŘŖŖŞDzȱ›˜ę–˜ŸŠǰȱŘŖŖşǼǯ The second main factor universally listed as
a temperament dimension is arousal within the nervous system, helping
Š—ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ˜ȱœŠ¢ȱŠŒ’ŸŽȱ˜—ȱŠȱŠœ”ǯȱȱ‘Ž›Žȱ’œȱŠȱȃŽ—Ž›Š•ȱŠ›˜žœŠ•ȄȱŠŒ˜›ȱǻŽœŒ›’‹ŽȱŠœȱȃœ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱŽ¡Œ’Š’˜—Ȅȱ˜›ȱȃŽ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—Ȅȱ˜›ȱȃŠŒ’Ÿ’¢Ȅȱ
˜›ȱ ȃ›’ŸŽȱ ™Ž›œ’œŽ—ŒŽȄȱ ˜›ȱ “žœȱ ȃŠ›˜žœŠ•ȄǼȱ Š—ȱ ’ȱ ‘Ž›Žȱ ’œȱ —˜ȱ —ŽŽȱ ˜›ȱ ‘Žȱ
separation of arousal by type of activity, then performance on any task
›Žšž’›’—ȱ Œ˜—œŠ—ȱ ŠŒ’ŸŠ’˜—ȱ ˜ž•ȱ œ‘˜ ȱ —˜—œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ Œ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ ’‘ȱ
temperament scales measuring arousal. On the other hand, if the specif’ŒȱŠœ”ȱŠěŽŒŽȱ‘Žȱ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ˜ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ
traits and speed of performance on this task, then that would demonstrate
the need to use temperament scales designed to measure the dynamic aspects of activity separately in physical, social-verbal, and intellectual areas. For this study, a task requiring a prolonged semantic estimation of the
abstract words (i.e., requiring arousal in verbal and intellectual activities)
was performed under time pressure. This
his task was chosen over other social activities to measure the ability to sustain prolonged repetitive activity associated with verbal material based upon its duration and intensity.
The use of other social activities would bring unnecessary variance to the
ŠŠȱŠ—ȱŠ•œ˜ȱ–’‘ȱ‹Žȱ–˜›Žȱœ’–ž•Š’—ȱ˜›ȱœ˜–Žȱœž‹“ŽŒœǰȱ™˜Ž—’Š••¢ȱ‹’asing the results.
In summary, the goals of the present study were (a) to assess the benŽęœȱ ˜ȱ Š—ȱ ŠŒ’Ÿ’¢Ȭœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ Žœȱ ˜ȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ ǻǼȱ ’—ȱ ‘Žȱ Š—Š•¢œ’œȱ ˜ȱ
œŽ¡ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœǰȱǻ‹Ǽȱ˜ȱ’—ŸŽœ’ŠŽȱ‘Žȱ’쎛Ž—’Š•ȱ™˜ Ž›ȱ˜ȱ ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Šment tests developed within the Pavlovian tradition (the activity-specif’Œȱ ȱ Š—ȱ ‘Žȱ —˜—œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ ŠŸ•˜Ÿ’Š—ȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ ž›ŸŽ¢Dzȱ Ǽȱ žœ’—ȱ
Š—ȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—Š•ȱŠœ”ȱ›Žšž’›’—ȱ’—Ž—œŽȱŠ—ȱŠœȱŸŽ›‹Š•ȱ’—Ž••ŽŒžŠ•ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ǰȱŠ—ȱǻŒǼȱ˜ȱŒ˜–™Š›Žȱ‘ŽœŽȱ ˜ȱŽœœȱ‹¢ȱꗍ’—ȱ‘ŽȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ
‘Ž’›ȱœŒŠ•Žœǯȱ‘ŽȱŒ˜››Žœ™˜—’—ȱ‘¢™˜‘ŽœŽœȱ Ž›Žȱ‘ŠȱǻŠǼȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ’쎛ences would be observed between the scores of men and women on the
scales of the STQ when the scales assess dynamic properties separately
’—ȱ ‘›ŽŽȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ ¢™Žœȱ ˜ȱ ŠŒ’Ÿ’’ŽœDzȱ ǻ‹Ǽȱ Š–˜—ȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ  ˜ȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ
Žœœȱ ŽŸŽ•˜™Žȱ ’‘’—ȱ ‘Žȱ ŠŸ•˜Ÿ’Š—ȱ ›Š’’˜—ǰȱ ‘Žȱ ŠŒ’Ÿ’¢Ȭœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ Žœȱ
of temperament would have stronger correlations with the time required
to complete a prolonged word-assessment task than a nonspŽŒ’ęŒȱŽœȱ˜ȱ
ACTIVITY-SPECIFIC TEST OF TEMPERAMENT
5
Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȯœ™ŽŒ’ęŒŠ••¢ǰȱ‘Žȱž›Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘’œȱŠœ”ȱ ˜ž•ȱŒ˜››Ž•ŠŽȱ—ŽŠtively mostly with the dynamic aspects of verbal-social and possibly intellectual activity, but not with the aspects of physical activity; and (c) STQ
scales measuring the arousal aspects of activity (i.e., Ergonicity) would
Œ˜››Ž•ŠŽȱ™˜œ’’ŸŽ•¢ȱ ’‘ȱ‘ŽȱȱœŒŠ•Žȱ˜ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ǰȱ ‘’•Žȱ‘Žȱ
STQ scales of Plasticity and Tempo would correlate positively with the
PTS Mobility scale, and the STQ scale of Intellectual Ergonicity would correlate positively with the PTS scale of Strength of Inhibition.
ђѡѕќё
Participants
Canadian participants (NȶƽȶşŜŜDzȱ Řřřȱ –Ž—ǰȱ ŝřřȱ ˜–Ž—Ǽȱ ŠŽœȱ ŗŝȱ ˜ȱ řśȱ
years (MȶƽȶŘŖǯŘǰȱSDȶƽȶřǯřǼǰȱ˜ȱ ‘˜–ȱŗŝŗȱ Ž›ŽȱŸ˜•ž—ŽŽ›œȱŠ—ȱŝŚřȱ Ž›Žȱ™œ¢chology students at McMaster University and 52 were psychology students at Brock University (both universities are located in Southern OnŠ›’˜ǰȱŠ—ŠŠǼȱ˜˜”ȱ™Š›ȱ’—ȱ‘’œȱœž¢ȱ’—ȱŗşşşȱ˜ȱŘŖŖŜǯȱȱ‘ŽœŽǰȱŝŝśȱ˜ȱşŜŜȱ
™Š›’Œ’™Š—œǰȱŗŝśȱ–Ž—ȱŠ—ȱŜŖŖȱ ˜–Ž—ȱǻMȱŠŽȶƽȶŗşǯŞȱ¢›ǯǰȱSDȶƽȶřǯŖǼǰȱŜřȱŸ˜•ž—ŽŽ›œȱǻŞƖǼȱŠ—ȱŝŗŘȱœžŽ—œǰȱŒ˜–™•ŽŽȱŠ••ȱ–ŽŠœž›ŽœDzȱŝŚȱœžŽ—ȱ™Š›’Œ’pants (MȱŠŽȶƽȶŘŖǯŗȱ¢›ǯǰȱSDȶƽȶřǯŘǼȱŒ˜–™•ŽŽȱ‘ŽȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—Š•ȱŠœ”ȱŠ—ȱ‘Žȱ
ǰȱŠ—ȱŗŗŝȱŸ˜•ž—ŽŽ›œȱŒ˜–™•ŽŽȱ‘Žȱȱ˜—•¢ȱǻ‘Ž¢ȱ Ž›Žȱ—˜ȱ’—Ÿ’Žȱ
˜ȱ™Š›’Œ’™ŠŽȱ’—ȱ‘ŽȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—Ǽǯ
Measures
Participants (ȶƽȶŝŝśǼȱ Œ˜–™•ŽŽȱ ‘Žȱ Ž¡Ž—Žȱ —•’œ‘ȱ ŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱ ˜ȱ
žœŠ•˜ŸȂœȱ›žŒž›Žȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱžŽœ’˜——Š’›ŽȱǻžœŠ•˜Ÿȱǭȱ›˜ę–˜ŸŠǰȱŘŖŖŝǼǰȱŠ—ȱ‘Žȱ—•’œ‘ȱŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱŠŸ•˜Ÿ’Š—ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱž›ŸŽ¢ȱ
(Strelau, 1999).
Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (Rusalov, 1989; Rusalov & TroꖘŸŠǰȱ ŘŖŖŝǼǯȯȱ ‘’œȱ šžŽœ’˜——Š’›Žȱ ‘Šœȱ ŗśŖȱ œŠŽ–Ž—œȱ ˜ȱ ‹Žȱ Š—œ Ž›Žȱ
using a 4-point Likert-type scale with labels 1: Strongly disagree, 2: DisŠ›ŽŽǰȱřDZȱ›ŽŽǰȱŠ—ȱŚDZȱ›˜—•¢ȱŠ›ŽŽǯȱ’¡ȱ’Ž–œȱŠ›ŽȱŠœœ’—Žȱ˜ȱŠȱŸŠ•’Ȭ
ǯȱ’¡ȱ’Ž–œȱŠ›ŽȱŠœœ’—Žȱ˜ȱŠȱŸŠ•’ity scale, and 144 items to 12 temperamental scales (12 items each) measuring the four traits of Ergonicity, Plasticity, Tempo, and Emotionality in
each of three areas of activity (motor-physical, social-verbal, and intellecžŠ•Ȭ–Ž—Š•Ǽǯȱ‘Žȱ›˜—‹ŠŒ‘ȱŠ•™‘Šȱ›Ž•’Š‹’•’¢ȱŒ˜ŽĜŒ’Ž—ȱ˜›ȱ‘ŽȱȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ
›Š—Žȱ›˜–ȱǯŝŖȱ˜ȱǯŞŚȱǻžœŠ•˜Ÿȱǭȱ›˜ę–˜ŸŠǰȱŘŖŖŝǼǯȱ
Pavlovian Temperament Survey (Strelau, 1999).—This survey has 66
statements to be answered on a 4-point Likert-type scale with labels 1:
Strongly disagree, 2: Disagree, 3: Agree, and 4: Strongly agree. There are
‘›ŽŽȱ œŒŠ•Žœǰȱ ›Ž—‘ȱ ˜ȱ ¡Œ’Š’˜—ǰȱ ›Ž—‘ȱ ˜ȱ —‘’‹’’˜—ǰȱ Š—ȱ ˜‹’•’¢ȱ
(22 items each), for which ›˜—‹ŠŒ‘ȱŒ˜ŽĜŒ’Ž—œȱŠ•™‘Šȱwere reported to
range from .81 to .84.
6
I. N. TROFIMOVA
Semantic task.—To complete this task, participants rated 30 abstract
Œ˜—ŒŽ™œȱǻ ˜›œǼȱ˜—ȱŜŖȱŝȬ™˜’—ȱ‹’™˜•Š›ȱœŒŠ•Žœȱǻ’ǯŽǯǰȱ Š›–ȱŸœȱŒ˜•ǰȱœ˜ĞȱŸœȱ
hard, interesting vs uninteresting, etc.). Each concept was presented as a
word on a computer monitor along with each of the bipolar scales.
Procedure
Š›’Œ’™Š—œȱ Ž›ŽȱŽ‹›’ŽŽȱŠ‹˜žȱ‘Žȱž›Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ—Šž›Žȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱŽ¡periment. They were instructed to work as fast as possible, and their time
˜—ȱ ‘’œȱ Šœ”ȱ Šœȱ ›ŽŒ˜›Žǯȱ ‘Žȱ Œ˜–™žŽ›ȱ ™›˜›Š–ȱ ¡™Š—ȱ ǻ™›˜Ÿ’Žȱ ‹¢ȱ
HR-Laboratory Human Technologies)3 detected whether a participant
was giving random or inconsistent answers. The procedure took 1 to 3
hours depending on the speed of the participant’s performance. All participants signed an informed consent form before testing and participa’˜—ȱ’—ȱ‘ŽȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—ǯȱЎ› Š›ǰȱž—’ŸŽ›œ’¢ȱœžŽ—œȱ›ŽŒŽ’ŸŽȱ‘Žȱ™›ŠŒ’Œž–ȱŒ›Ž’ȱ˜›ȱ‘Ž’›ȱ™Š›’Œ’™Š’˜—ǯȱ••ȱœž‹“ŽŒœȱ Ž›Žȱ̞Ž—ȱ’—ȱ—•’œ‘ǯȱ
ђѠѢљѡѠȱюћёȱіѠѐѢѠѠіќћ
Š‹•Žȱŗȱœ‘˜ œȱ–ŽŠ—œǰȱœŠ—Š›ȱŽŸ’Š’˜—œǰȱŠ—ȱŒ˜—ꍮ—ŒŽȱ’—Ž›ŸŠ•œȱ
for the applied measures. Each STQ scale had a normal distribution of
scores with a range of 12 to 48.ȱ‘ŽȱœŽ¡ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ’—ȱ‘ŽȱœŒ˜›Žœȱ˜—ȱȱ
œŒŠ•Žœȱ Ž›Žȱ –žŒ‘ȱ œ›˜—Ž›ȱ ‘Š—ȱ ˜—ȱ ȱ œŒŠ•Žœǰȱ œž™™˜›’—ȱ ‘Žȱ ꛜȱ ‘¢™˜‘Žœ’œǯȱ—Š•¢œ’œȱ˜ȱœŽ¡ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱžœ’—ȱ˜—ŽȬ Š¢ȱȱshowed that
˜–Ž—ȱ™Ž›˜›–Žȱ‘ŽȱŽ–Š—’ŒȱŠœ”ȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—•¢ȱŠœŽ›ȱ‘Š—ȱ–Ž—ǰȱ ‘’Œ‘ȱ
Šœȱ Œ˜—œ’œŽ—ȱ ’‘ȱ œ’—’ęŒŠ—•¢ȱ ‘’‘Ž›ȱ œŒ˜›Žœȱ ˜›ȱ ˜–Ž—ȱ ‘Š—ȱ –Ž—ȱ
on Social Ergonicity and Social Tempo scales (Table 1). Men had higher scores on Motor Ergonicity, Plasticity, Tempo, and Emotionality scales,
Š—ȱŠ•œ˜ȱ˜—ȱ—Ž••ŽŒžŠ•ȱ•Šœ’Œ’¢ȱŠ—ȱŽ–™˜ȱœŒŠ•ŽœǯȱŽ¡ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ’—ȱȱ
œŒ˜›Žœȱ Ž›Žȱ˜ž—ȱ˜ȱ‹ŽȱœŠ’œ’ŒŠ••¢ȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ˜—•¢ȱ˜›ȱ‘Žȱ˜‹’•’¢ȱœŒŠ•Žȱ
( pȶǀȶǯŖŗǼǯȱ‘ŽœŽȱ›Žœž•œȱŠ›Žȱ’—ȱ•’—Žȱ ’‘ȱꗍ’—œȱ›˜–ȱstudies showing that
men have higher physical activity than women, especially in activities
requiring upper body strength (Thomas & French, 1985; Eaton & Enns,
ŗşŞŜǼǰȱŠ—ȱ‘Šȱ˜—ȱ‘ŽȱŠŸŽ›ŠŽȱ–Ž—ȱ™Ž›˜›–ȱ‹ŽĴŽ›ȱ‘Š—ȱ ˜–Ž—ȱ˜—ȱŽœœȱ˜ȱ
visual-spatial abilities but more poorly than women on verbal tests (Hyde
& Linn, 1988; Halpern, 2000).
Contrary to the common view of women being more emotional than
men, the results showed that while women had higher scores on Social
Š—ȱ —Ž••ŽŒžŠ•ȱ –˜’˜—Š•’¢ȱ œŒŠ•Žœǰȱ ’ȱ Šœȱ –Ž—ȱ ‘˜ȱ ‘Šȱ œ’—’ęŒŠ—•¢ȱ
‘’‘Ž›ȱœŒ˜›Žœȱ˜—ȱ‘ŽȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ˜˜›ȱ–˜’˜—Š•’¢ǰȱ’ǯŽǯǰȱ–Š•Žȱœž‹“ŽŒœȱ›Žported being more sensitive to success or failure in physical activities than
’ȱ ˜–Ž—ǯȱ‘’œȱ›Žœž•ȱœž™™˜›œȱ‘Žȱ’ŽŠȱ˜ȱœŽ™Š›Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ ˜ȱŽę—’’˜—œȱ
˜ȱ Ž–˜’˜—Š•’¢ǰȱ Ž¡™›Žœœ’˜—ȱ ˜ȱ Š—ȱ Ž–˜’˜—ȱ ˜›ȱ Ž¡™Ž›’Ž—ŒŽȱ ˜ȱ Š—ȱ Ž–˜’˜—ǯȱ
ŽŸŽ›Š•ȱœž’Žœȱ‘ŠŸŽȱ˜ž—ȱ™›ŽŸ’˜žœ•¢ȱ‘Šȱ‘ŽȱœŽ¡ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ Ž›ŽȱœŠ’œ3
‘Ĵ™DZȦȦ ǯ‘ǯ›žȦŠ‹˜žȦ’—Ž¡ȏŽ—ǯ™‘™ǯ
Ș™ȶǀȶ.05. †™ȶǀȶ.01. ‡™ȶ>ȶǯ001.
Time on Semantic Task, min.
PTS scales
ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—
Strength of Inhibition
Mobility
STQ scales
Motor Ergonicity
Intellectual Ergonicity
Social Ergonicity
Motor Plasticity
Intellectual Plasticity
Social Plasticity
Motor Tempo
Intellectual Tempo
Social Tempo
Motor Emotionality
Intellectual Emotionality
Social Emotionality
Measure
33.5
31.4
34.4
32.2
řŖǯŝ
29.6
řřǯŝ
34.4
33.6
26.9
29.6
28.1
58.4
śŞǯŝ
58.4
84.5
M
Men, —ȶƽȶ202
25.5
Men, —ȶƽȶŗŝś
ŝǯś
6.4
Ŝǯŝ
Men, —ȶƽȶ233
ŝǯŚ
5.5
ŝǯŗ
śǯŝ
5.4
6.3
6.0
5.6
5.9
śǯŝ
śǯŝ
6.1
SD
ŝśǯŝ
śŝǯŚ
58.3
56.8
30.9
30.8
36.0
řŖǯŝ
29.2
28.8
32.8
32.1
36.5
25.4
30.8
29.6
śŝǯŘȮśşǯś
śŝǯŝȮśşǯŜ
śŝǯŚȮśşǯŚ
řŘǯśȮřŚǯŚ
řŖǯŝȮřŘǯŗ
řřǯśȮřśǯŚ
řŗǯśȮřŘǯş
řŖǯŖȮřŗǯŚ
ŘŞǯŞȮřŖǯŚ
řřǯŖȮřŚǯś
řřǯŝȮřśǯŘ
řŘǯŞȮřŚǯř
ŘŜǯŗȮŘŝǯŜ
ŘŞǯŞȮřŖǯř
ŘŝǯřȮŘŞǯŞ
M
ŞŖǯşȮŞŞǯŖ
95%CI
Women, —ȶƽȶŜŚŝ
20.4
Women, nȶƽȶŜŖŖ
6.9
6.4
6.8
Women, —ȶƽȶŝřř
ŝǯŖ
5.6
Ŝǯŝ
5.5
5.2
6.6
5.3
5.3
5.5
6.0
6.0
śǯŝ
SD
řŖǯŚȮřŗǯŚ
řŖǯŚȮřŗǯŘ
řśǯśȮřŜǯŚ
řŖǯřȮřŗǯŗ
ŘŞǯŞȮŘşǯŜ
ŘŞǯřȮŘşǯř
řŘǯŚȮřřǯŘ
řŗǯŝȮřŘǯś
řŜǯŗȮřŜǯş
ŘśǯŖȮŘśǯŞ
řŖǯŚȮřŗǯř
ŘşǯŘȮřŖǯŖ
śŜǯşȮśŞǯŖ
śŝǯŞȮśŞǯŞ
śŜǯřȮśŝǯŚ
ŝŚǯŗȮŝŝǯř
95%CI
ȶƽȶŗǰȱŞŚŝ
24.80‡
ȶƽȶŗǰȱŝŝř
2.45
0.41
6.83†
ȶƽȶ1, 964
ŘŘǯŝ؇
1.69
ŞǯŝŜ‡
12.05‡
14.61‡
2.51
ŚǯŝŞȘ
34.16‡
ŚŝǯśŞ‡
10.96‡
8.16‡
11.85‡
F
TABLE 1
ђюћѠǰȱѡюћёюџёȱђѣіюѡіќћѠǰȱşśƖȱќћѓіёђћѐђȱћѡђџѣюљѠǰȱюћёȱFȱюљѢђѠȱќѓȱћђȬѤюѦȱћюљѦѠђѠȱќѓȱюџюіћѐђȱќџȱђѥȱќџȱюѐѕȱђюѠѢџђ
ACTIVITY-SPECIFIC TEST OF TEMPERAMENT
ŝ
8
I. N. TROFIMOVA
’ŒŠ••¢ȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ ‘Ž—ȱœ’žŠ’˜—Š•ȱŠŒ˜›œȱ–˜Ž•Žȱž—Ž›•¢’—ȱœŽ¡ȱœŽ›Ž˜¢™ŽœDZȱ ˜–Ž—ȱ›Ž™˜›Žȱ–˜›Žȱ’—Ž—œŽȱŽ–˜’˜—Š•ȱŽ¡™Ž›’Ž—ŒŽœȱ‘Š—ȱ–Ž—ȱ’—ȱ
situations which involved interpersonal rather than impersonal emotion
Ž•’Œ’˜›œȱ ǻŠ›Š—ŒŽȱ ǭȱ Š—Š“’ǰȱ ŗşşŘDzȱ ’œŒ‘Ž›ǰȱ ŗşşřǼǯȱ —ȱ ‘’œȱ œŽ—œŽǰȱ œŽ—œ’’Ÿity to failure in relations and social activities might be greater for women,
and sensitivity to failure in physical activity might be greater for individžŠ•œȱ ‘˜ȱŠ›ŽȱŽ¡™ŽŒŽȱ’—ȱœ˜Œ’Ž¢ȱ˜ȱ‹Žȱœ›˜—Ž›ȱ˜›ȱ–˜›Žȱ™‘¢œ’ŒŠ••¢ȱęǰȱ’ǯŽǯǰȱ
’—ȱŽ—Ž›Š•ȱ–Ž—ǯȱ—Ž›Žœ’—•¢ǰȱ‘Ž›Žȱ ŠœȱŠȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ—ŽŠ’ŸŽȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—ȱ
( pȶǀȶǯŖŖŗǼȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ‘Žȱȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ—‘’‹’’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ˜Œ’Š•ȱ–˜’˜—Š•’¢ȱ
scales (Table 2). It is possible that men consider behavior related to social
Ž–˜’˜—Š•’¢ȱŠœȱ’œ’—‘’‹’ŽȱŠ—ȱ’—Š™™›˜™›’ŠŽȱ˜›ȱ‘Ž’›ȱœŽ¡ǯ
Ž¡ȱ ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ Š–˜—ȱ Œ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ ˜ȱ ‘Žȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ œŒŠ•Žœȱ ’‘ȱ
ž›Š’˜—ȱ˜—ȱ‘ŽȱŽ–Š—’ŒȱŠœ”ȱ Ž›Žȱ—˜ȱ›Š–Š’Œǰȱ‹žȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱǻŠ‹•Žȱ
2). The temperament traits associated in men with faster performance on
‘ŽȱŽ–Š—’ŒȱŠœ”ȱ Ž›Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ›˜–ȱ‘˜œŽȱ˜›ȱŠœŽ›ȱ™Ž›˜›–Š—ŒŽȱŠ–˜—ȱ
˜–Ž—ǯȱ‘Žȱž›Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘’œȱŠœ”ȱ˜›ȱ ˜–Ž—ȱœ‘˜ Žȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱŒ˜››Ž•Štions with all three Tempo scales, Intellectual Plasticity, and Motor Ergonicity scales; these were not observed for men. The duration of the Semantic Task for men had the strongest correlation with the STQ Social
Plasticity scale, which assesses how easily an individual generates, stops,
˜›ȱœ ’Œ‘Žœȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱŸŽ›‹Š•Ȭœ˜Œ’Š•ȱŠŒ’˜—œǯȱȱ’œȱ™˜œœ’‹•Žȱ‘Šȱ‘Žȱœ’£Žȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ
–Š•ŽȱœŠ–™•Žȱ ŠœȱŠȱŠŒ˜›ȱŒ˜—›’‹ž’—ȱ˜ȱ‘ŽœŽȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœǯȱȱ’œȱŠ•œ˜ȱ™˜œœ’‹•Žǰȱ ‘˜ ŽŸŽ›ǰȱ ‘Šȱ –Ž—ȱŠ—ȱ ˜–Ž—ȱžœŽȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ Š‹’•’’Žœȱ ˜ȱ ™Ž›˜›–ȱ
‘Žȱ Šœ”ȱ Š—ȱ œ˜ȱ ‘Šȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ œ¢•Žœȱ ˜ȱ ˜›”’—ȱ ˜—ȱ ’ǯȱ ȱ ˜–Ž—ȱ ’—ŽŽȱ
are more accustomed to working with words, then for them the Semantic
Šœ”ȱ’œȱŠȱ–ŠĴŽ›ȱ˜ȱœ™ŽŽȱ’—ȱ˜’—ȱ›Š‘Ž›ȱŠž˜–Š’Œǰȱ Ž••Ȭ”—˜ —ȱ ˜›”ǯȱȱ
men on average report lower speed and endurance in verbal-social activities than women (as noted above), then men might compensate through
‘’‘Ž›ȱ̎¡’‹’•’¢ȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱȃœ ’Œ‘ŽœȄȱ›Žšž’›Žȱ’—ȱœžŒ‘ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’’ŽœȱŠ—ȱžœŽȱ‘Ž’›ȱ
verbal-social plasticity to succeed on the Semantic Task.
–˜—ȱ Š••ȱ Š™™•’Žȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ œŒŠ•Žœǰȱ œ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ Œ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ ǻpȶǀȱ
.001) of performance times on the semantic task were found with the scales
measuring dynamic aspects of social-verbal activity, i.e., Social Ergonicity,
Social Plasticity, and Social Tempo, and much less with other scales. Higher scores on these scales were associated with faster performance on the
task, which involves rating of abstract concepts. The correlations of other
scales with the time required to complete the Semantic Task were not conœ’œŽ—ȱŠŒ›˜œœȱœŽ¡ŽœȱǻŠ‹•ŽȱŘǼȱŠ—ȱ—˜ȱŠœȱ˜ĞŽ—ȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ǯȱ‘Žȱ‘›ŽŽȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ
of the STQ related to verbal-social activity (Social Ergonicity, Social Plas’Œ’¢ǰȱŠ—ȱ˜Œ’Š•ȱŽ–™˜Ǽȱ–žœȱ›ŽĚŽŒȱ‘ŽȱŠ‹’•’¢ȱ˜›ȱ’—Ž—œŽȱŠ—ȱŠœȱŸŽ›‹Š•ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ȱ–žŒ‘ȱ‹ŽĴŽ›ȱ‘Š—ȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ’—Ž••ŽŒžŠ•ȱŠ—ȱ–˜˜›ȱŠŒ’Ÿity. The semantic task required a prolonged, intense activation of nervous
processes related to verbal activity and the inhibition of unrelated behav-
.00
ȮǯŖŗ
ȮǯŗŚȘ
ȮǯŖŘ
ȮǯŖŝ
Ȯǯŗކ
ȮǯŖŞ
Ȯǯŗř
ȮǯŗŚȘ
.04
Ȯǯŗŗ
.06
ȮǯŖŜ
ȮǯŖŗ
ȮǯŖŝ
ȮǯŖś
.01
Ȯǯŗއ
.03
ȮǯŖŜ
ȮǯŗŚ‡
ȮǯŗŖ†
ȮǯŖŞȘ
ȮǯŘŖ‡
.02
ȮǯŖŘ
.00
ȮǯŖŞȘ
.05
ȮǯŖś
Total
ȮǯŗŖȘ
.06
ȮǯŖŝ
Ȯǯŗŗ†
.00
Ȯǯŗއ
.01
ȮǯŖşȘ
ȮǯŗŚ‡
Ȯǯŗř†
Ȯǯŗŗ†
Ȯǯŗއ
ȮǯŖŗ
.04
.01
Time on STQ
Men
Women
ǯŖşȘ
.38‡
.33‡
.33‡
.35‡
.41‡
ǯřŝ‡
.43‡
.40‡
.40‡
ȮǯŖŗ
ȮǯŖşȘ
ȮǯŘŗ‡
Ȯ
Total
.52‡
.13‡
.22‡
.23‡
.31‡
.38‡
.45‡
.39‡
.33‡
.34‡
.32‡
ȮǯŖŗ
Ȯǯŗŗ†
ȮǯŗŚ‡
Ȯ
Total
.04
.15‡
ȮǯŖşȘ
.12‡
.02
ȮǯŘŝ‡
.05
.02
ȮǯŖşȘ
ȮǯŖś
.00
ȮǯŖŝ
Total
ȮǯŖŚ
ǯŗŜȘ
ȮǯŖş
.09
.09
ȮǯŘŞ‡
ȮǯŖŜ
ǯŖŝ
Ȯǯŗŗ
ȮǯŖŜ
ȮǯŖř
ȮǯŗŜ‡
Ȯ
Men
.06
.15‡
ȮǯŖşȘ
.13‡
ȮǯŖŗ
ȮǯŘŞ‡
ǯŖşȘ
.00
ȮǯŖşȘ
ȮǯŖś
.01
ȮǯŖŚ
Women
Note.—Ȯȶƽȶȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱœŒŠ•ŽDzȱȮȶƽȶ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ—‘’‹’’˜—ȱœŒŠ•ŽDzȱȮȶƽȶ˜‹’•’¢ȱœŒŠ•ŽǯȱȘpȶǀȶǯŖśǯȱ†pȶǀȶǯŖŗǯȱ‡pȶǀȶǯŖŖŗǯȱ
Structure of Temperament Questionnaire
Motor Ergonicity
Intellectual Ergonicity
Social Ergonicity
Motor Plasticity
Intellectual Plasticity
Social Plasticity
Motor Tempo
Intellectual Tempo
Social Tempo
Motor Emotionality
Intellectual Emotionality
Social Emotionality
Pavlovian Temperament Survey
ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱǻnȶƽȶŝŘŘǼ
Strength of Inhibition (nȶƽȶŝŘŘǼ
Mobility (nȶƽȶŝŘŘǼ
Scales
TABLE 2
ќџџђљюѡіќћѠȱђѡѤђђћȱѡѕђȱѡџѢѐѡѢџђȱќѓȱђњѝђџюњђћѡȱѢђѠѡіќћћюіџђȱǻǼȱѐюљђѠǰȱѢџюѡіќћȱ
ќѓȱђњюћѡіѐȱюѠјȱǻњіћǯǼȱǻnȶƽȶŞŚşǼǰȱюћёȱюѣљќѣіюћȱђњѝђџюњђћѡȱѢџѣђѦȱǻǼȱѐюљђѠȱǻnȶƽȶŝŝśǼ
ACTIVITY-SPECIFIC TEST OF TEMPERAMENT
9
10
I. N. TROFIMOVA
’˜›œȱ’—ȱ˜›Ž›ȱ˜ȱœŠ¢ȱ˜ŒžœŽDzȱ‘˜ ŽŸŽ›ǰȱ‘Ž›Žȱ Ž›Žȱ—˜ȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱŒ˜—œ’œŽ—ȱ’—ȱŽ’‘Ž›ȱ›˜ž™ȱ‹¢ȱœŽ¡ǰȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ‘Žȱȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’tation or Strength of Inhibition scales and the duration of performance on
‘ŽȱœŽ–Š—’ŒȱŠœ”ǯȱ‘Ž›Žȱ Šœȱ˜—•¢ȱ˜—Žȱ—ŽŠ’ŸŽȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱœ–Š••ȱŽěŽŒȱ
œ’£ŽȱǻpȶǀȶǯŖśǼȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ‘Žȱȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱœŒŠ•ŽȱŠ—ȱ‘Žȱž›Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱŽ–Š—’ŒȱŠœ”ȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ ˜–Ž—ǰȱ ‘’Œ‘ȱŠěŽŒŽȱ‘ŽȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—ȱ‹Žtween the same in the whole sample. The PTS Mobility scale did not show
œ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—ȱ ’‘ȱ‘Žȱž›Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱŽ–Š—’ŒȱŠœ”, in spite of
‘ŽȱŠŒȱ‘Šȱ‘ŽȱŠœ”ȱ›Žšž’›Žȱ̎¡’‹’•’¢ȱ˜ȱ™Ž›˜›–Š—ŒŽȱ’—ȱœ ’Œ‘’—ȱ›˜–ȱ
one concept to another.
žŒ‘ȱŠȱœŽ•ŽŒ’ŸŽȱ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ˜ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱœž™™˜›œȱ‘ŽȱœŽŒ˜—ȱ‘¢™˜‘esis, showing that the STQ scales, designed to measure dynamic aspects
˜ȱœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱŠŒ’Ÿ’’Žœǰȱ‘ŠŸŽȱœ›˜—Ž›ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ ’‘ȱ‘Žȱœ™ŽŽȱ˜ȱ™Ž›˜›–Š—ŒŽȱ’—ȱŒ˜››Žœ™˜—’—ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’’Žœȱ‘Š—ȱ‘Žȱ—˜—œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱȱœŒŠ•ŽœǯȱŸŽ›Š••ǰȱ
the selectivity of correlations of temperament scales with the duration of
‘ŽȱœŽ–Š—’ŒȱŠœ”ȱŠ—ȱ‘Žȱꗍ’—œȱ˜—ȱœŽ¡ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱœ‘˜ ȱ‘Žȱ‹Ž—Žęœȱ˜ȱ
separation of temperamental aspects according to the type of activity in
‘ŽȱŠ—Š•¢œ’œȱ˜ȱ‹’˜•˜’ŒŠ•ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœǯ
Correlations between the STQ and PTS scales were in accord with the
‘’›ȱ‘¢™˜‘Žœ’œDZȱ‘Ž›Žȱ Ž›Žȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ™˜œ’’ŸŽȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ‘Žȱ
ȱ›˜—’Œ’¢ȱœŒŠ•ŽœȱŠ—ȱ‘ŽȱȱœŒŠ•Žȱ˜ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ǰȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ
the STQ Plasticity and Tempo scales and the PTS Mobility scale, and between the STQ Intellectual Ergonicity scale and the PTS Strength of Inhib’’˜—ȱœŒŠ•ŽȱǻŠ‹•ŽȱŘǼǯȱ‘Žȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ŒŽȱ˜ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ
Š—ȱ‘ŽȱȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ˜‹’•’¢ȱ Šœȱ’Ž—’ŒŠ•ȱ˜›ȱ
–Ž—ȱŠ—ȱ ˜–Ž—ǰȱœ˜ȱ˜—•¢ȱȃŠ••ȱœŠ–™•ŽȄȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱŠ›Žȱ’ŸŽ—ȱ˜—ȱ‘ŽœŽȱ ˜ȱ
PTS scales. Overall pŠĴŽ›—œȱ˜ȱcorrelations between of STQ and PTS scales,
‘˜ ŽŸŽ›ǰȱ Ž›Žȱ—˜—œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱŠ—ȱ Ž›Žȱœ’–’•Š›ȱ˜ȱ‘˜œŽȱ›Ž™˜›Žȱ‹¢ȱ›Ž•Šžȱ
(1999) for Polish and German samples, and by Ruch, Angleitner, and Strelau (1991) for a German sample. All STQ scales measuring characteristics
of activity (i.e., Ergonicity, Plasticity, Tempo) in all three areas of activity
ǻ–˜˜›ǰȱ œ˜Œ’Š•ǰȱ ’—Ž••ŽŒžŠ•Ǽȱ ‘Šȱ œŠ’œ’ŒŠ••¢ȱ œ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ (pȶǀȶǯŖŖŗǼȱ ™˜œ’’ŸŽȱ
Œ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ˜ȱŸŠ›’˜žœȱ–Š—’žŽœȱ ’‘ȱ‘ŽȱȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ˜‹’•’¢ǯȱžŒ‘ȱŠȱ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ˜ȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ’—’ŒŠŽœȱ‘ŠȱŠ•–˜œȱ
all STQ scales measure arousal and mobility aspects of activity, but PTS
œŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ—˜ȱ’쎛Ž—’ŠŽȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱŠœ™ŽŒœȱ˜ȱŠ›˜žœŠ•ȱ˜›ȱ¢™Žœȱ˜ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ǯȱ
The scales of Emotionality in Social and Intellectual activities (which
–ŽŠœž›ŽȱœŽ—œ’’Ÿ’¢ȱ˜ȱŠȱ™Ž›œ˜—ȱ˜ȱŠ’•ž›Žȱ˜›ȱœžŒŒŽœœǼȱ‘Šȱœ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ—ŽŠ’ŸŽȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ ’‘ȱ‘Žȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ˜‹’•’¢ȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ
PTS. Such correlation suggests that people with low arousal (i.e., with the
low ability to sustain intense or prolonged work) and low mobility of be‘ŠŸ’˜›ȱ–’‘ȱ‘ŠŸŽȱ›ŽšžŽ—ȱŽ¡™ŽŒŠ’˜—œȱ˜ȱŠ’•ž›Žǰȱ’—Œ›ŽŠœŽȱ—Žž›˜’Œ’œ–ȱ
’—ȱ œ˜Œ’Š•ȱ œŽĴ’—œǰȱ Š—ȱ ˜ŸŽ›Š••ȱ ™›˜‹•Ž–œȱ ’—ȱ œ˜Œ’Š•ȱ ŠŠ™Š’˜—ǯȱ ’—’ęŒŠ—ȱ
ACTIVITY-SPECIFIC TEST OF TEMPERAMENT
11
—ŽŠ’ŸŽȱŒ˜››Ž•Š’˜—ȱŒ˜—œ’œŽ—ȱ˜›ȱ‹˜‘ȱœŽ¡Žœȱ ŠœȱŠ•œ˜ȱ˜ž—ȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ‘Žȱ
scores on the STQ Social Plasticity scale and the PTS Strength of Inhibition
scale, which suggests that inhibitory behavior as measured by PTS relates
to hesitancy in social activity as measured by the STQ or that the content
˜ȱ‘Žȱȱ˜Œ’Š•ȱ•Šœ’Œ’¢ȱœŒŠ•Žȱ˜ȱŠȱ•Š›ŽȱŽ¡Ž—ȱŽœŒ›’‹Žœȱ’œ’—‘’‹’Žȱ
‹Ž‘ŠŸ’˜›ȱŠ—ȱœ‘˜ž•ȱ‹ŽȱŽ¡Š–’—Žȱž›‘Ž›ǯȱ
In conclusion, STQ and PTS both emerge from the Pavlovian tradi’˜—ȱ˜ȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—Š•ȱœž¢ȱ˜ȱ™›˜™Ž›’Žœȱ˜ȱ—Ž›Ÿ˜žœȱœ¢œŽ–œȱŠ—ȱœ‘Š›Žȱ‘Žȱ
tenet that these properties have a biological basis and appear as dynamic
aspects of human behaviour: energetic, mobility, and regulatory aspects.
The results illustrate the importance of separation of such dynamic aspects into three main types of activity—physical, verbal, and intellectual—as such separation provides more sensitive and detailed analysis of
‹’˜•˜’ŒŠ••¢ȱ‹ŠœŽȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœǯȱ
REFERENCES
ђђџђǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱіѐюǰȱǯȱǻŗşşśǼȳ‘Žȱ™›Ž’œ™˜œ’’˜—ȱ˜ȱ’œœ˜Œ’ŠŽDZȱ‘ŽȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—Š•ȱ›Š’œȱ
˜ȱ̎¡’‹’•’¢Ȧ›’’’¢ǰȱŠ’•¢ȱ›‘¢‘–ǰȱŽ–˜’˜—Š•’¢ǰȱŠ—ȱ’—Ž›ŠŒ’˜—Š•ȱœ™ŽŽǯȱDissociation, 8, 236-240.
іѠѕќѝǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱ
ђџѡђћѠѡђіћǰȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŚǼȳȱŒ˜—ę›–Š˜›¢ȱŠŒ˜›ȱŠ—Š•¢œ’œȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ›žŒž›Žȱ
of Temperament Questionnaire. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64,
1019-1029.
іѠѕќѝǰȱǯǰȱюѐјѠǰȱ
ǯǰȱ&ȱюћёѦǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşşřǼȳ›žŒž›Žȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱžŽœ’˜——Š’›Žȱ
(STQ): results from a U.S. sample. Ž›œ˜—Š•’¢ȱŠ—ȱ—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœǰȱŗŚǰȱŚŞśȬŚŞŝǯ
ќёѢћќѣǰȱ ǯȱ ǯǰȱ ђѧёђћђѧѕћѦјѕǰȱ ǯȱ ǯǰȱ &ȱљђѥюћёџќѣǰȱ ǯȱ ǯȱ ǻŗşşŜǼȳ‘Š›Š”Ž›’œ’”Šȱ
˜ŸŽ˜Ÿȱ—ŠȱŽœ˜Ÿ’Žȱ£ŠŠ—’Šȱ™œ’‘˜•˜’Œ‘Žœ”’‘ȱ–Ž˜’”ȱ’ȱœ›žŒž›Šȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•Ȃ—˜˜ȱ
opita [Peculiarities of psychodiagnostic test item responses and the structure of
’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱŽ¡™Ž›’Ž—ŒŽǾǯȱPsikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological JournalǾǰȱŗŝǰȱŞŝȬşŜǯȱ
ǽ’—ȱžœœ’Š—Ǿ
џђяћђџǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱѡќѢєѕǰȱǯȱǻŗşşřǼȳ‘Žȱ›Ž•Š’˜—œ‘’™ȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ‘Žȱ›žŒž›Žȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱŠ—ȱŽ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ—Žž›˜’Œ’œ–ǯȱŽ›œ˜—Š•’¢ȱŠ—ȱ—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœ. 14,
623-626.
ѢѠѠǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱљќњіћǰȱǯȱǻŗşŞŚǼȳTemperament: early developing personality trait. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Ѧѧќѣюǰȱ ǯȱ ǯȱ ǻŗşşŝǼȳŠ–˜ŠŒžŠ•’£ŠŒ’Šȱ ™›ŽœŠŸ’Ž•Ž¢ȱ ˜–’ȱ ’ȱ žœœ”˜˜ȱ Ž—˜œ˜Ÿȱ
ǽŽ•ȬŠŒžŠ•’£Š’˜—ȱ’—ȱ›Ž™›ŽœŽ—Š’ŸŽœȱ˜ȱ˜–’ȱŠ—ȱžœœ’Š—ȱŽ‘—’Œȱ›˜ž™œǾǯȱPsikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological JournalǾǰȱŗŞǰȱŜŗȬŜşǯȱǽ’—ȱžœœ’Š—Ǿ
юџѣђџǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱѕіѡђǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşşŚǼȳŽ‘ŠŸ’˜›Š•ȱ’—‘’‹’’˜—ǰȱ‹Ž‘ŠŸ’˜›Š•ȱŠŒ’ŸŠ’˜—ǰȱŠ—ȱ
ŠěŽŒ’ŸŽȱ ›Žœ™˜—œŽœȱ ˜ȱ ’–™Ž—’—ȱ ›Ž Š›ȱ Š—ȱ ™ž—’œ‘–Ž—DZȱ ‘Žȱ Ȧȱ ŒŠ•Žœǯȱ
Journal of Personality and Social PsychologyǰȱŜŝǰȱřŗşȬřřřǯȱ
юѡѡђљљǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŜśǼȳ‘ŽȱœŒ’Ž—’ęŒȱŠ—Š•¢œ’œȱ˜ȱ™Ž›œ˜—Š•’¢ǯ London: Penguin.
љќћіћєђџȱǯȱǯǰȱџѧѦяђѐјǰȱǯȱǯǰȱѣџюјіѐǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱђѡѧђљǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşşŚǼȳThe temperament and character inventory (TCI): a guide to its development and use. St Louis, MO:
Center for Psychobiology of Personality.
ќѠѡюǰȱǯȱǯǰȱџǯǰȱ&ȱѐџюђǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşşŘǼȳRevised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI–R)
and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO–FFI): professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
12
I. N. TROFIMOVA
Ѣњђћѐіǰȱ ǯȱ ǻŗşşśǼȳ‘Žȱ ›Ž•Š’˜—ȱ ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱ ‘Žȱ ›žŒž›Žȱ ˜ȱ Ž–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ žŽœ’˜—naire and other personality domains. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
śśǰȱŞśŖȬŞśŝǯȱ
ѢњђћѐіǰȱǯȱǻŗşşŜǼȳŠŒ˜›’Š•ȱŸŠ•’’¢ȱ˜ȱœŒ˜›Žœȱ˜—ȱ‘Žȱ›žŒž›Žȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱžŽœtionnaire. Educational and Psychological MeasurementǰȱśŜǰȱŚŞŝȬŚşřǯȱ
юѡќћǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱћћѠǰȱǯȱǻŗşŞŜǼȳŽ—Ž›ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ’—ȱ‘ž–Š—ȱ–˜˜›ȱŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ȱ•ŽŸŽ•ǯȱPsychological Bulletin, 100, 19-28.
ѝѢѡюђѣǰȱǯȱǯǰȱјќћћіјќѣюǰȱǯȱǯǰȱєюџјќѣǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱюџюяџіћюǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖřǼȳDissociative states and formal-dynamical properties of individual: materials of the 7th Multidisciplinary Conference “Stress and Behavior.” Moscow: IPRAN Press. Pp. 121-123.
ѦѠђћѐјǰȱ
ǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱѦѠђћѐјǰȱǯȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŜŞǼȳManual for the Eysenck Personality Inventory.
San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
іѠѐѕђџǰȱǯȱ
ǯȱǻŗşşřǼȳŽ—Ž›ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ’—ȱŽ–˜’˜—Š•’¢DZȱŠŒȱ˜›ȱœŽ›Ž˜¢™ŽǵȱFeminism
and Psychology, 3, 303-318.
џюѦǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŜŚǼȳPavlov’s typology: recent theoretical and experimental developments from
the laboratory of B. M. Teplov. New York: Macmillan.
џюѦǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŞŘǼȳThe neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septohippocampal system.ȱŽ ȱ˜›”DZȱ¡˜›ȱ—’ŸŽ›ǯȱ›Žœœǯ
юљѝђџћǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŖǼȳŽ—Ž›ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ’—ȱŒ˜—’’ŸŽȱŠ‹’•’’Žœǯ Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ђѦњюћѠǰȱǯȱǻŗşŘşǼȳInleiding tot de speciale psychologie [—›˜žŒ’˜—ȱ˜ȱ’쎛Ž—’Š•ȱ™œ¢chologyǾǯȱ
ŠŠ›•Ž–ǰȱ‘ŽȱŽ‘Ž›•Š—œDZȱŽȱ›ŸŽ—ȱǯȱ˜‘—ǯ
Ѧёђǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱіћћǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŞŞǼȳŽ—Ž›ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ’—ȱŸŽ›‹Š•ȱŠ‹’•’¢DZȱŠȱ–ŽŠȬŠ—Š•¢œ’œǯȱ
Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53-69.
юћѡǰȱǯȱǻŗŝşŞȦŗşŝŚǼȳAnthropology from a pragmatic point of view (Mary Gregor, Transl.).
‘Žȱ
ŠžŽǰȱ‘ŽȱŽ‘Ž›•Š—œDZȱŠ›’—žœȱħ‘˜ěȱǻ”ǯȱǼǯȱ
юџюћѐђǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱюћюїіǰȱǯȱǻŗşşŘǼȳ˜ Š›œȱ›ŽŒ˜—œ’Ž›Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱŽ—Ž›ȬŽ–˜’˜—ȱ
relationship. In M. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior: review of personality and
social psychologyǯȱ˜•ǯȱŗŚǯȱŽ ‹ž›¢ȱŠ›”ǯȱDZȱŠŽǯȱ™ǯȱŗŝŞȬŘŖŗǯ
юњіђљљǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖřǼȳŽ¢˜—ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱŠ—ȱ›˜ž™ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœDZȱ‘ž–Š—ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•’¢ǰȱœŒ’Ž—’ęŒȱ™œ¢Œ‘˜•˜¢ǰȱŠ—ȱ’••’Š–ȱŽ›—ȂœȱŒ›’’ŒŠ•ȱ™Ž›œ˜—Š•’œ–ǯ Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
ђѕџюяіюћǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱюћјǰȱǯȱǻŗşŝŞǼȳȱšžŽœ’˜——Š’›Žȱ–ŽŠœž›Žȱ˜ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ
in achieving tendency. Educational and Psychological MeasurementǰȱřŞǰȱŚŝśȬŚŝŞǯȱ
ђяѦљіѡѠѦћǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŝŘǼȳFundamental properties of the human nervous system. New York:
Plenum.
юѣљќѣǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŚŗǼȳŽŒž›Žœȱ˜—ȱŒ˜—’’˜—Žȱ›ŽĚŽ¡Žœǰȱv. IIi: types of the higher nervous activity, their interdependence with neuroses and psychoses, and the physiological mechanism
of neurotic and psychotic symptoms. New York: International Publishers.
ќѝќѣǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŜǼȳ›ŽŸ˜“—˜œȂȱŸȱ™œ¢Œ‘˜Ȭ™ŽŠ˜˜Œ‘Žœ”˜–ȱ™˜››ŽŽȱœžŽ—Šȱ”˜••ŽŠȱ
ǽ—¡’Ž¢ȱ ’—ȱ ™œ¢Œ‘˜Ȭ™ŽŠ˜’ŒŠ•ȱ ™˜››Š’ȱ ˜ȱ Šȱ Œ˜••ŽŽȱ œžŽ—Ǿǯȱ Professional’noye
Obuchenie [Professional EducationǾǰȱŞǰȱŘŚȬŘśǯȱǽ’—ȱžœœ’Š—Ǿ
юѡѕђђǰȱ ǯǰȱ &ȱ іћєѕǰȱ ǯȱ ǻŘŖŖŗǼȳ˜‹’•’¢ȱ ˜›ȦŠ—ȱ •Š‹’•’¢ȱ ˜ȱ ‘Žȱ —Ž›Ÿ˜žœȱ ™›˜ŒŽœœŽœȱ Šœȱ
temperamental trait(s). Ž›œ˜—Š•’¢ȱŠ—ȱ—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœ, 31, 1091-1104.
ќѡѕяюџѡǰȱ ǯȱ ǯȱ ǻŗşŞŞǼȳŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ Š—ȱ ‘Žȱ ŽŸŽ•˜™–Ž—ȱ ˜ȱ ’—‘’‹’Žȱ Š™™›˜ŠŒ‘ǯȱ
Child Development, 59, 1241-1250.
Ѣѐѕǰȱǯǰȱћєљђіѡћђџǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱѡџђљюѢǰȱǯȱǻŗşşŗǼȳ‘Žȱ›Ž•ŠžȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ—ŸŽ—˜›¢Ȯ
ŽŸ’œŽȱǻȮǼDZȱŸŠ•’’¢ȱœž’ŽœǯȱEuropean Journal of PersonalityǰȱśǰȱŘŞŝȬřŖŞǯ
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŝşǼȳBiologicheskiye osnovi individual’no-psichologicheskih razlichiy [Bio•˜’ŒŠ•ȱ‹Šœ’œȱ˜ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ™œ¢Œ‘˜•˜’ŒŠ•ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœǾǯȱʂ˜œŒ˜ DZȱŠž”Šǯȱǽ’—ȱžœœ’Š—Ǿ
ACTIVITY-SPECIFIC TEST OF TEMPERAMENT
13
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŞşǼȳ˜˜›ȱŠ—ȱŒ˜––ž—’ŒŠ’ŸŽȱŠœ™ŽŒœȱ˜ȱ‘ž–Š—ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—DZȱŠȱ
new questionnaire of the structure of temperament. Personality and Individual DifferencesǰȱŗŖǰȱŞŗŝȬŞŘŝǯ
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱ ǯȱ ǯȱ ǻŗşşŝǼȳOprosnik formal’no-dynamicheskih svoystv individual’nosti: rukovodstvo [Questionnaire of formal-dynamical properties of individuality: manualǾǯȱ˜œŒ˜ DZȱȱ›Žœœǯȱǽ’—ȱžœœ’Š—Ǿ
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŚǼȳFormal’no-dynamicheskiye svoystva individual’nosti (temperament)
[Formal-dynamical properties of individual (temperament)Ǿǯȱ˜œŒ˜ DZȱȱ›Žœœǯȱǽ’—ȱ
žœœ’Š—Ǿ
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱѢёіћȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşşśǼȳŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ’ȱ’—Ž••ŽŒDZȱ˜‹Œ‘’Žȱ‹ȱœ™ŽŒ’ęŒ‘Žœ”’¢Žȱ
ŠŒ˜›’ȱ›ŠœŸ’’¢ŠȱǽŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱŠ—ȱ’—Ž••’Ž—ŒŽDZȱŽ—Ž›Š•ȱŠ—ȱœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱŠŒ˜›œȱ˜ȱŽŸŽ•˜™–Ž—ǾǯȱPsikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological JournalǾǰȱŗŜǰȱŗŘȬŘřǯȱǽ’—ȱžœœ’Š—Ǿ
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱюѢњќѣюǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşşşǼȳ—ȱ‘Žȱ›Ž•Š’˜—œ‘’™ȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱŽ—Ž›Š•ȱŠ‹’•’’Žœȱ
Š—ȱȃ’—Ž••ŽŒžŠ•ȄȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ǯȱPsikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological
JournalǾǰȱŘŖǰȱŝŖȬŝŝǯ
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱќљѡюѣѡѧђѣюǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşşŝǼȳŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱŠœȱŠȱ™›Ž›Žšž’œ’Žȱ˜ȱŒ›ŽŠ’ŸŽȱ
abilities. Pavlov Journal of Higher Nervous ActivityǰȱŚŝǰȱŚśŗȬŚŜŖǯ
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱ ǯȱ ǰȱ ѢѠюљќѣюǰȱ ǯȱ ǯǰȱ &ȱ ѡџђљȂћіјќѣюǰȱ ǯȱ ǯȱ ǻŘŖŖŖǼȳŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ Œ‘Ž•˜ŸŽ”Šȱ’ȱ˜œ˜‹Ž——˜œ’ȱŸ’‹˜›Šȱ–Ž“žȱŸŽ›˜¢Š—˜œ¢žȱ˜œħŽ—’¢ŠȱœŽ•’ȱ’ȱŽŽȱ£Ž——˜œ¢žȱ
[Temperament of man and peculiarities of choice between the probability of goal
ŠŒ‘’ŽŸŽ–Ž—ȱŠ—ȱ’œȱŸŠ•žŽǾǯȱZhurnal Vysshey Nervnoy Deyatel’nosti [ Journal of Higher Nervous ActivityǾǰȱśŖǰȱřŞŞǯȱǽ’—ȱžœœ’Š—Ǿ
ѢѠюљќѣǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱџќѓіњќѣюǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŝǼȳStructure of temperament and its measurement.
Toronto: Psychological Services Press.
ѡќѢєѕǰȱǯǰȱџђяћђџǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱќќѝђџǰȱǯȱǻŗşşŗǼȳ‘ŽȱžœŠ•˜Ÿȱ›žŒž›Žȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ
Questionnaire (STQ): results from an Australian sample. Personality and Individual
’쎛Ž—ŒŽœǰȱŗŘǰȱŗřśśȬŗřśŝǯ
ѡџђљюѢǰȱǯȱǻŗşşşǼȳThe Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS): an international handbook.
žĴŠ›ǰȱŽ›–Š—¢DZȱ
˜›ŽŽȱǭȱ
ž‹Ž›ǯȱ
ѡџђљюѢǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱћєљђіѡћђџǰȱǯȱǻœǯǼȱǻŗşşŗǼȳExplorations in temperament: international
perspectives on theory and measurement. New York: Plenum.
ѕќњюѠǰȱǯȱǯǰȱ&ȱџђћѐѕǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŞśǼȳŽ—Ž›ȱ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱŠŒ›˜œœȱŠŽȱ’—ȱ–˜˜›ȱ™Ž›˜›mance: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 260-282.
џќѓіњќѣюǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşşşǼȳ
˜ ȱ™Ž˜™•Žȱ˜ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱŠŽǰȱŽ—Ž›ǰȱŠ—ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱŽœ’mate the world. Psychological Reports, 85, 533-552.
џќѓіњќѣюǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖşǼȳAuthor: please add reference, cited on p. 4.
юѠѦѢџюǰȱ ǯȱ ǯȱ ǻŘŖŖŞǼȳœ¢Œ‘˜•˜¢ȱ ˜ȱ –Š•Žȱ Š—ȱ Ž–Š•Žȱ Œ˜––ž—’ŒŠ’ŸŽȱ ŠŒ’Ÿ’¢ǯȱ The
Spanish Journal of Psychology, 11, 289-300.
ќџќяіђѣюǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŚǼȳ˜Ž›—ȱ™œ¢Œ‘˜Ž—Ž’Œȱœž’Žœȱ˜ȱ’—Ž••’Ž—ŒŽȱŠ—ȱ‘Ž˜›¢ȱ˜ȱ
motivation for achievements. Journal of Applied Psychology, 3, 53-59.
іћёљђǰȱǯǰȱ&ȱђџћђџǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŗşŞŜǼȳŽŠœœŽœœ’—ȱ‘Žȱ’–Ž—œ’˜—œȱ˜ȱŽ–™Ž›Š–Ž—ȱ’—dividually across the life span: the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey
ǻȮǼǯȱJournal of Adolescent Research, 1, 213-229.
іћȂјќǰȱǯȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŜǼȳ‘Š›Š”Ž›’œ’”’ȱœŠ–˜˜ŒŽ—”’ȱ’ȱž›˜Ÿ—¢Šȱ˜œħŽ—’¢ȱ’ȱ’‘ȱœ˜™˜•˜“Ž—’Žȱ
ǽ‘Š›ŠŒŽ›’œ’Œœȱ˜ȱœŽ•Ȭ’–ŠŽȱŠ—ȱ•ŽŸŽ•ȱ˜ȱŠœ™’›Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ’œȱ›Ž•Š’˜—œ‘’™œǾǯȱPsikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological JournalǾǰȱřǰȱŗŞȬřŖDzȱŚǰȱŗśȬŘśǯȱǽ’—ȱžœœ’Š—Ǿ
14
I. N. TROFIMOVA
Accepted July 29, 2009.
APPENDIX A
ѥюњѝљђѠȱќѓȱѡѕђȱѡђњѠȱќѓȱѡѕђȱѥѡђћёђёȱ
ѡџѢѐѡѢџђȱќѓȱђњѝђџюњђћѡȱѢђѠѡіќћћюіџђȱǻǼ
STQ Scale
Motor Ergonicity
Item
4. I get pleasure from doing physical work in my free time.
32. It is easy for me to do hard physical work.
Intellectual Ergonicity
ȱȱŗŝǯȱȱŽȱ’›Žȱ˜ȱ™›˜•˜—Žȱ–Ž—Š•ȱ ˜›”ǯ
82. I carry out my mental activities with pleasure.
Social Ergonicity
33. I would feel uncomfortable if I didn’t communicate with
people for a long time.
98. I’m silent even among my friends.
Motor Plasticity
11. I quickly move into high gear.
ȱȱŚŖǯȱȱœžŒŒŽœœž••¢ȱŒŠ››¢ȱ˜žȱŠœ”œȱ›Žšž’›’—ȱœž‹•Žȱ˜›ȱꗎȱ
movements.
Intellectual Plasticity
8. It is easy for me to simultaneously do several things, for
Ž¡Š–™•Žǰȱ˜ȱ ŠŒ‘ȱȱŠ—ȱ›ŽŠǯ
18. I easily switch from solving one problem to another.
Social Plasticity
15. It’s easy for me to enter into conversation with strangers.
51. It’s easy for me to make new acquaintances.
Motor Tempo
118. I work slowly when I make something by hand.
122. I prefer to do my physical work at a fast rate.
Intellectual Tempo
23. I like intellectual games that require fast decision making.
ȱȱřŞǯȱ¢ȱ‘˜ž‘œȱ̘ ȱœ•˜ •¢ǯ
Social Tempo
50. It’s hard for me to talk fast.
ȱȱŝŗǯȱȂ–ȱŠȱŠ•”Š’ŸŽȱ™Ž›œ˜—ǯ
Motor Emotionality
52. I get annoyed if I am not agile during recreational games.
ȱȱŝřǯȱ¢ȱ–˜˜ȱ’œȱ˜ĞŽ—ȱœ™˜’•Žȱ ‘Ž—ȱ‘Žȱ‘’—œȱ–ŠŽȱ‹¢ȱ–¢œŽ•ȱ
are not as good as I hoped.
Intellectual Emotionality ȱȱŗŘǯȱȱŠ–ȱž™œŽȱꗍ’—ȱŠ—ȱŽ››˜›ȱ’—ȱ–¢ȱŽŒ’œ’˜—œǰȱŽŸŽ—ȱœ–Š••ȱ
ones.
25. I fear that I cannot perform work requiring mental strain.
Social Emotionality
56. I’m upset if the people whom I’m talking with do not
understand me.
109. I worry a lot when I must discuss relationships with friends.
ACTIVITY-SPECIFIC TEST OF TEMPERAMENT
15
APPENDIX B
юљіёюѡіќћȱ
іѠѡќџѦȱќѓȱѡѕђȱѡџѢѐѡѢџђȱќѓȱђњѝђџюњђћѡȱѢђѠѡіќћћюіџђ
During ‘ŽȱŽ¡™Ž›’–Ž—Š•ȱŸŠ•’Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱǰȱ‘Žȱ™Ž›˜›–Š—ŒŽȱ‹¢ȱ
participants on the following measures was compared with scores on
STQ scales in a series of studies in the 1980s: speed of writing; reading
Š—ȱ œ™ŽŽȱ ˜ȱ Ž—Ž›Š’˜—ȱ ˜ȱ ˜›œDzȱ –Š¡’–Š•ȱ Š—ȱ ˜™’–Š•ȱ Ž–™˜ȱ ˜ȱ ™Ž›formance in sensory-motor tasks and intellectual (including unsolvable)
tasks; performance on nonverbal tasks, with which participants were unfamiliar; rigidity of perception in tactile and visual modalities; duration of
the switch between one method of solving a task and another; mobility in
ŠĴŽ—’˜—DzȱŠ—ȱŸŠ›’Š‹’•’¢ȱ’—ȱ•’—Žȱ›Š ’—ȱǻžœŠ•˜ŸǰȱŗşŝşǰȱŗşşŝǰȱŘŖŖŚDzȱžœŠ•˜Ÿȱǭȱ›˜ę–˜ŸŠǰȱŘŖŖŝǼǯȱ›˜—’Œ’¢ȱœŒŠ•Žœȱ˜ȱ‘ŽȱȱŒ˜››Ž•ŠŽȱ™˜œ’’ŸŽ•¢ȱ
’‘ȱ‘Žȱ¢œŽ—Œ”ȱŽ›œ˜—Š•’¢ȱžŽœ’˜——Š’›ŽȂœȱ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱœŒŠ•ŽȱǻžœŠ•˜Ÿǰȱ
ŗşŞşDzȱ›Ž‹—Ž›ȱǭȱ˜ž‘ǰȱŗşşřDzȱ’—Ȃ”˜ǰȱŘŖŖŜǼǰȱ ’‘ȱ‘Žȱ’ȱ’ŸŽȱ¡›ŠŸŽ›Ȭ
›Ž‹—Ž›ȱǭȱ˜ž‘ǰȱŗşşřDzȱ’—Ȃ”˜ǰȱŘŖŖŜǼǰȱ ’‘ȱ‘Žȱ’ȱ’ŸŽȱ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱœŒŠ•Žȱǻ˜ž—˜ŸǰȱŽ£Ž—Ž£‘—¢”‘ǰȱǭȱ•Ž¡Š—›˜ŸǰȱŗşşŜDzȱžœŠ•˜Ÿȱǭȱ›˜Ȭ
Ž£Ž—Ž£‘—¢”‘ǰȱǭȱ•Ž¡Š—›˜ŸǰȱŗşşŜDzȱžœŠ•˜Ÿȱǭȱ›˜Ȭ
, 1996; Rusalov & TroꖘŸŠǰȱŘŖŖŝǼǰȱ ’‘ȱ›Ž•ŠžȂœȱȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱœŒŠ•ŽȱǻRuch, et al.,
1991; Bodunov,et al., 1996; Strelau, 1999), with Torrance’s Nonverbal Tests
˜ȱ›ŽŠ’ŸŽȱ‘’—”’—ȱǻžœŠ•˜Ÿȱǭȱ˜•ŠŸ£ŽŸŠǰȱŗşşŝǼǰȱ ’‘ȱ˜ĴŽ›Ȃœȱ˜Œžœȱ
˜ȱ˜—›˜•ȱœŒŠ•Žȱǻ¢£˜ŸŠǰȱŗşşŝǼǰȱ ’‘ȱ˜Š•Ȭ›’ŸŽ—ȱŒ‘˜’ŒŽȱ˜ȱ™›˜Žœœ’˜—ȱŠœȱ
opposed to accessibility of profession (Rusalov, Rusalova, & Strel’nikova,
2000), and with the Motivation for Achievement scale (Vorobieva, 2004).
Scores of the Motor and Social Plasticity and Tempo scales of the STQ
correlated positively with Strelau’s PTS Mobility scale (Ruch, et al., 1991;
Bodunov, et al., 1996; Strelau, 1999), with adaptivity of behaviour on the
Ž–‹˜Ȭ
˜™™Žȱ ŽŸŽ•ȱ ˜ȱœ™’›Š’˜—ȱ Ž¡™Ž›’–Ž—ȱ ǻ’—Ȃ”˜ǰȱ ŘŖŖŜǼǰȱ ’‘ȱ ˜››Š—ŒŽȂœȱ ˜—ŸŽ›‹Š•ȱ Žœœȱ ˜ȱ ›ŽŠ’ŸŽȱ ‘’—”’—ȱ ǻžœŠ•˜Ÿȱ ǭȱ ˜•ŠŸ£ŽŸŠǰȱ
ŗşşŝǼǰȱ ’‘ȱ ˜ĴŽ›Ȃœȱ ˜Œžœȱ˜ȱ ˜—›˜•ȱ œŒŠ•Žȱǻ¢£˜ŸŠǰȱ ŗşşŝǼǰȱ Š—ȱ ’‘ȱ ‘Žȱ
Motivation for Achievement scale (Vorobieva, 2004). Rathee and Singh
(2001) reported a comparison of 25 measures of Mobility, including the
Plasticity and Tempo scales of the English version of the STQ. The authors
˜ž—ȱ ‘’‘ȱ Œ˜››Ž•Š’˜—œȱ ˜ȱ œŒ˜›Žœȱ ˜—ȱ ‘Žȱ ¡Ž—Žȱ ȱ •Šœ’Œ’¢ȱ œŒŠ•Žœȱ
’‘ȱ‘˜œŽȱ˜›ȱ•Ž›Š’˜—ȱŠœ”ǰȱ•Ž¡’‹’•’¢ȱ˜ȱŠĴŽ—’˜—ǰȱ™›˜˜Ȭ›ŽŠ’—ȱŠ‹’•ity, the number of trials needed for participants to reach the optimal reac’˜—ȱ ’–Žȱ ŠĞŽ›ȱ Š•Ž›Š’˜—ȱ ˜ȱ Šȱ œ’–ž•žœǰȱ Š—ȱ ˜‹’•’¢ȱ –ŽŠœž›Žȱ ‹¢ȱ ›Žlau’s Pavlovian Temperamental Survey (PTS). Tempo in Motor Activity
as measured by the English STQ in this study was correlated with EEG
Į-general speed, time taken to generate simultaneous contrast, duration
˜ȱȃŠĞŽ›Ȭ’–ŠŽȄȱ›ŽŠŒ’˜—ǰȱŒ›’’ŒŠ•ȱ̒Œ”Ž›ȱžœ’˜—ǰȱŠ—ȱœ’£Žȱ˜ȱž—ŒŽ›Š’—¢ȱ’—terval. Scores on the Plasticity scales also positively correlated with those
on ¢œŽ—Œ”Ȃœȱ ȱ ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱ œŒŠ•Žȱ ǻžœŠ•˜Ÿǰȱ ŗşŞşDzȱ Brebner & Stough,
ŗşşřDzȱ’—Ȃ”˜ǰȱŘŖŖŜǼǰȱ˜—ȱ‘Žȱ’ȱ’ŸŽȱ¡›ŠŸŽ›œ’˜—ȱœŒŠ•Žȱǻž–Ž—Œ’ǰȱŗşşśDzȱ˜-
16
I. N. TROFIMOVA
dunov, et alǯǰȱŗşşŜǼǰȱŠ—ȱ˜—ȱ‘Žȱȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱœŒŠ•ŽȱǻRuch, et
al., 1991; Strelau, 1999).
Emotionality scales scores correlated positively with those on the
Neuroticism scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Rusalov,
1989; Brebner & Stough, 1993; Zin’ko, 2006), with the Big Five Neuroticism scale (Dumenci, 1995; Bodunov, et alǯǰȱŗşşŜǼǰȱ˜—ȱ‘ŽȱŠŽȬ›Š’ȱ—¡’Ž¢ȱ—ŸŽ—˜›¢ȱǻ˜™˜ŸǰȱŘŖŖŜǼǰȱ ’‘ȱŠ¢•˜›ȂœȱŠ—’Žœȱ—¡’Ž¢ȱŒŠ•Žȱǻ˜™˜Ÿǰȱ
2006; Zin’ko, 2006), and use of alcohol (Bodunov,et al., 1996), and correlated negatively with scores on the ’œœ˜Œ’Š’ŸŽȱ¡™Ž›’Ž—ŒŽœȱŒŠ•Ž (Beere &
’ŒŠǰȱŗşşśDzȱ™žŠŽŸǰȱ”˜——’”˜ŸŠǰȱŠ›”˜ŸǰȱǭȱŠ›Š‹›’—ŠǰȱŘŖŖřǼǰȱ‘Žȱ˜œŽ—£Ȭ
™žŠŽŸǰȱ”˜——’”˜ŸŠǰȱŠ›”˜ŸǰȱǭȱŠ›Š‹›’—ŠǰȱŘŖŖřǼǰȱ‘Žȱ˜œŽ—£Ȭ
”˜——’”˜ŸŠǰȱŠ›”˜ŸǰȱǭȱŠ›Š‹›’—ŠǰȱŘŖŖřǼǰȱ‘Žȱ˜œŽ—£Ȭ
ǰȱŘŖŖřǼǰȱ‘Žȱ˜œŽ—£Ȭ
˜œŽ—£ŸŽ’ȱŽœȱǻ’—Ȃ”˜ǰȱŘŖŖŜǼǰȱ‘Žȱȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ—Ȭ
ǻ’—Ȃ”˜ǰȱŘŖŖŜǼǰȱ‘Žȱȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ¡Œ’Š’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ›Ž—‘ȱ˜ȱ—hibition scales (Ruch, et al.ǰȱŗşşŗDzȱ›Ž•ŠžǰȱŗşşşǼǰȱœŒ˜›Žœȱ˜—ȱŠĴŽ••Ȃœȱǰȱ
ǰȱ
Q2, and Q4 factors (Vasyura, 2008), Torrance’s Nonverbal Tests of Creative
‘’—”’—ȱǻžœŠ•˜Ÿȱǭȱ˜•ŠŸ£ŽŸŠǰȱŗşşŝǼǰȱŠ—ȱ‘Žȱ˜’ŸŠ’˜—ȱ˜›ȱŒ‘’ŽŸŽ–Ž—ȱœŒŠ•Žȱǻ˜›˜‹’ŽŸŠǰȱŘŖŖŚǼǯȱ›˜ę–˜ŸŠȱǻŗşşşǼȱŠ™™•’Žȱ‘ŽȱœŽ–Š—’Œȱ’ferential method to contrast temperamental groups selected on the basis of
ȱœŒ˜›Žœǯȱ‘Žȱ›Ž™˜›Žȱ‘Šȱœž‹“ŽŒœȱ ’‘ȱ‘Žȱ‘’‘ŽœȱŠ—ȱ•˜ ŽœȱœŒ˜›Žœȱ
˜—ȱ ‘Žȱ ȱ œŒŠ•Žœȱ œ‘˜ ȱ ’쎛Ž—ŒŽœȱ ’—ȱ ‘Ž’›ȱ ™Ž›ŒŽ™’˜—œȱ ˜ȱ œŽ–Š—’ŒŠ••¢ȱ
—Žž›Š•ȱ˜‹“ŽŒœǰȱ’—Œ•ž’—ȱœŽ•Ȭ™Ž›ŒŽ™’˜—ǯȱ‘ŽȱSTQ scales which measure
dynamic aspects of intellectual activity had positive correlations with such
measures of intelligence as the Wechsler and Shepard tests, including the
Šœ”œȱ–ŽŠœž›’—ȱŒ•Šœœ’ę
–ŽŠœž›’—ȱŒ•Šœœ’ęŒŠ’˜—ȱŠ‹’•’’Žœȱǻȃ¡Œ•ž’—ȱ‘Žȱ‘’›ȄǼȱŠ—ȱ™•Šœ’ŒȱŒŠ’˜—ȱŠ‹’•’’Žœȱǻȃ¡Œ•ž’—ȱ‘Žȱ‘’›ȄǼȱŠ—ȱ™•Šœ’ŒȬ
ity in nonverbal thinking (Rusalov & Dudin, 1995; Rusalov & Naumova,
1999). Intellectual activity scales had positive correlations with scores on
‘Žȱ˜Œžœȱ˜ȱ˜—›˜•ȱœŒŠ•Žȱǻ¢£˜ŸŠǰȱŗşşŝǼǰȱŠ—ȱ˜Š•Ȭ˜›’Ž—ŽȱŒ‘˜’ŒŽȱ˜ȱ™›˜Ȭ
˜Œžœȱ˜ȱ˜—›˜•ȱœŒŠ•Žȱǻ¢£˜ŸŠǰȱŗşşŝǼǰȱŠ—ȱ˜Š•Ȭ˜›’Ž—ŽȱŒ‘˜’ŒŽȱ˜ȱ™›˜fession (Rusalov, et al., 2000), and negative correlations with translations
˜ȱ‘ŽȱŠŽȬ›Š’ȱ—¡’Ž¢ȱ—ŸŽ—˜›¢ȱǻ˜™˜ŸǰȱŘŖŖŜǼǰȱŠ¢•˜›ȂœȱŠ—’Žœȱ—¡Ȭ
ŠŽȬ›Š’ȱ—¡’Ž¢ȱ—ŸŽ—˜›¢ȱǻ˜™˜ŸǰȱŘŖŖŜǼǰȱŠ¢•˜›ȂœȱŠ—’Žœȱ—¡iety Scale (Popov, 2006; Zin’ko, 2006), and the access-oriented choice of
profession (Rusalov, et al., 2000). Intellectual plasticity correlated with 25
measures of mobility in Rathee and Singh’s study (2001).
The administration of the English version of the STQ to American,
Australian, and Canadian samples showed the factor structure of this version similar to the Russian language version and that the English version
possessed good reliability and internal consistency (Stough, Brebner, &
Cooper, 1991; Bishop, Jacks, & Tandy, 1993; Dumenci, 1995, 1996; Bishop
& Hertenstein, 2004; Rusalov, 2004; žœŠ•˜Ÿȱǭȱ›˜ę–˜ŸŠǰȱŘŖŖŝǼǯ
Activity-specific test of temperament
Exploration of the benefits of an activity-specific test of temperament 1,2
Irina N. Trofimova
Collective Intelligence Laboratory
Department of Psychiatry and Neuroscience
McMaster University
1
Address correspondence to I. N. Trofimova, 92 Bowman St., Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 2T6
(itrofimova@sympatico.ca).
2
The author acknowledges the hard work of students in administration of the tests and of the Semantic Task:
Kristine Espiritu, Chandrima Bandyopadhyay, Samira Patel, Vanita Marques, and also the help of Dr. William Sulis
in the work on English STQ and in editing the manuscript of this article.
Summary.—The Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ) was proposed by
Rusalov in 1989 and subsequently tested in five languages. The questionnaire assesses four
temperamental traits (Ergonicity, Plasticity, Tempo, and Emotionality) in three separate areas of
activity: physical, verbal-social and intellectual. The scales are all activity-specific. In 775
Canadian subjects, two temperament tests were compared, both developed on the basis of
Pavlovian studies of the nervous system: the activity-specific approach (STQ) and the
nonspecific Pavlovian Temperamental Survey (PTS). More significant sex differences were
found on activity-specific scales of the STQ than on the nonspecific PTS scales. The pattern of
correlations between the STQ scales and the time taken on an experimental task requiring a
prolonged and intense word-assessment activity showed stronger correlations with the specific
scales of the STQ measuring the dynamical aspects of social-verbal activity, and not with the
PTS Strength of Excitation scale, which is based on a “general arousal” concept. The results
supported the separation of temperament traits related to three different types of activities and
opposed to “general arousal” theories of temperament.
2
Many researchers consider temperament to comprise the content-free, formal dimensions
of behaviour, whereas personality is to be considered a sociopsychological construct comprising
the content characteristics of human behaviour (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968; Nebylitsyn, 1972;
Gray, 1982; Rusalov, 1989; Strelau & Angleitner, 1991). As Strelau and Angleigtner (1991, p. 6)
pointed out in their review, “most temperament researchers agree that temperament, whatever the
traits and structure to which this concept refers, has a strong biological determination.” This
assumption has its roots in the facts that temperament characteristics can be observed from the
first weeks of life and individual differences in temperamental traits have a strong genetic
determination.” The European tradition in analysis of temperament (Kant, 1798; Stern, 1900;
cited from Lamiell, 2003; Heymans, 1929; Pavlov, 1941; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968) described
two basic components of temperament, Activity characteristics and Emotionality characteristics.
A two-component model of temperament was developed further in the Russian
psychological school, which studied the types and properties of nervous system as the basis of
the most consistent personality traits. Since Pavlov’s time at the beginning of the 20th century
extensive experimental work with human participants was conducted in the laboratories of
Teplov and Nebylitsyn (see the review of Gray, 1964), and Rusalov (1979). These experiments
showed that the strength of excitation or inhibition in the central nervous system (CNS) is
expressed as how long the individual can sustain activation or inhibition of activation. The
mobility of the CNS processes is indicated as the plasticity of behaviour, how easily the
individual can start or stop activity, and how flexible and adaptive the individual is to new
3
circumstances or instructions. The balance between excitation and inhibition was thought to be
the basis of emotionality, impulsivity, or detachment behaviour. The British psychologist Jeffrey
Gray, who conducted most of the work on the translation and analysis of Pavlov’s “types of
CNS” (Gray, 1964), found a strong parallel between the concept of arousal and the Pavlovian
concept of the strength of a nervous system. Elucidating the relationships between various brain
structures, Gray proposed a model that explains both the four classical temperamental types and
Pavlov’s types in terms of the relationship between approach and withdrawal systems (Gray,
1982).
Vladimir Rusalov who, after Nebylitsyn, inherited the Laboratory of Differential
Psychology and Differential Psychophysiology in the Institute of Psychology under the Russian
Academy of Sciences, started in the 1970s a large experimental project analyzing types of EEG
activity, strength and mobility of nervous processes in various modalities, performance by
humans in deterministic and probabilistic conditions, tempo of reading and motor tasks, verbal
activity, performance on tests of intelligence and behavioral particularities associated with the
various temperamental traits. Based on this work, Rusalov concluded that temperamental traits
are activity-specific: energetic level or tempo of performance might be different for a given
individual in physical, social or intellectual activities, therefore the aspects of the performance of
these activities should be assessed and analysed separately.
Based on his experiments, Rusalov proposed the Structure of Temperament theory and
developed his Structure of Temperament Questionnaire. The first version of the English version
of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ: Rusalov, 1989) had four scales Ergonicity
(endurance, the ability to keep intensive work), Plasticity (or flexibility, the ability to effectively
switch between tasks or to change the way of performance), Tempo, and Emotionality separately
4
in physical-motor (Motor) activity, and social activity (such as reading, writing, speaking,
communication). Estimates of internal reliability for the scales has ranged from .70 to .81. Then
a third set of four scales was added to measure aspects of intellectual activity, with the
development of adult, teenage, middle school and preschool Extended versions of the STQ
(Rusalov, 1997, 2004). The Extended STQ has 4-point Likert scale format; see Appendix A for
Comment [SAI1]:
Appendix A
examples of items. A summary of the validation of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire
is provided in Appendix B.
Comment [SAI2]:
Appendix B
The benefits of the Rusalov Structure of Temperament Questionnaire were that it used the
activity-specific model of temperament in which the scales were grouped in factors by the types
of activity (for example, Social Ergonicity, Social Plasticity and Social Tempo) and not by
dynamic aspects of activity. The previous models of temperament and personality did not
distinguish among areas of activity, considering, for example, arousal in motor and social
activity (Extraversion or Strength of the nervous system) as a nonspecific general activation of
the nervous system (Pavlov, 1941; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968; Nebylitsyn, 1972; Gray, 1982;
Costa & McCrae, 1992; Strelau, 1999).
The theoretical rationale of the present study was to explore the benefits of analysis of
temperament traits separately in physical, social-verbal, and intellectual activities. Many models
of temperament and personality continue to follow the so-called “general arousal” approach,
considering that only one general trait is related to the energetic component of behaviour, namely
“strength of excitation” (Pavlov, 1941; Strelau, 1999), “liveliness” (Cattell, 1965),
“extraversion” (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968; Rothbart, 1988; Big Five model, including Costa &
McCrae, 1992), “activity” (Buss & Plomin, 1984; Windle & Lerner, 1986; Behavioural
Approach System of Gray, 1982), “drive persistence” (Carver & White, 1994; Cloninger,
5
Przbeck, Svrakic, & Wetzel, 1994) or just “arousal” (Mehrabian & Bank, 1978). The same is true
for mobility (i.e., how easily the activity can be started and carried out). Previous studies using
the STQ showed that the arousal-related traits of temperament correlate with the personality
traits in a discriminatory manner. For example, high correlations of Extraversion, as measured by
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire are found with the Social Ergonicity (rs=.68–.74), Social
Plasiticity (rs=.43-.65) and Social Tempo (rs=.39-.51) scales of the STQ (Rusalov, 1989;
Brebner & Stough, 1993; Dumenci, 1995; Zin’ko, 2006), but not with the scales of Motor or
Intellectual Ergonicity. Similarly, high correlations of Extraversion, as measured by Big Five are
found with the Social Ergonicity (r = .29) and Social Tempo (r = .29) while the correlations with
Motor Ergonicity and Motor Tempo were not significant (Dumenci, 1995)
In this study, a test measuring temperament traits separately in three areas of activity (i.e.,
Structure of Temperament Questionnaire) was expected to yield more knowledge about
biologically-based individual differences than a nonspecific test of temperament. Sex is one of
the main biological factors related to individual differences, often associated with particular
temperaments. Previous studies using the STQ have shown that separation of temperament traits
related to verbal-social and physical activities provides important information about sex
differences (Vasyura, 2008; Trofimova, 2009). The second main factor universally listed as a
temperament dimension is arousal within the nervous system, helping an individual to stay active
on a task. If there is a “general arousal” factor (described as “strength of excitation”, or
“extraversion”, or “activity”, or “drive persistence”, or just “arousal”) and if there is no need for
the separation of arousal by type of activity, then performance on any task requiring constant
activation would show nonspecific correlations with temperament scales measuring arousal. On
the other hand, if the specific task affected the pattern of correlations between specific
6
temperament traits and speed of performance on this task, then that would demonstrate the need
to use temperament scales designed to measure the dynamic aspects of activity separately in
physical, social-verbal, and intellectual areas. For this study, a task requiring a prolonged
semantic estimation of the abstract words (i.e., requiring arousal in verbal and intellectual
activities) was performed under time pressure. This task was chosen over other social activities
to measure the ability to sustain prolonged repetitive activity associated with verbal material
based upon its duration and intensity. The use of other social activities would bring unnecessary
variance to the data and also might be more stimulating for some subjects, potentially biasing the
results.
In summary, the goals of the present study were: (a) to assess the benefits of an activityspecific test of temperament (STQ) in the analysis of sex differences; (b) to investigate the
differential power of two temperament tests developed within the Pavlovian tradition (the
activity-specific Structure of Temperament Questionnaire and the nonspecific Pavlovian
Temperament Survey) using an experimental task requiring intense and fast verbal intellectual
activity; (c) to compare these two tests by finding the correlations between their scales. The
corresponding hypotheses were that (a) significant differences would be observed between the
scores of men and women on the scales of the STQ when the scales assess dynamic properties
separately in three different types of activities; (b) among these two temperament tests developed
within the Pavlovian tradition, the activity-specific test of temperament would have stronger
correlations with the time required to complete a prolonged word-assessment task than a
nonspecific test of temperament—specifically, the duration of this task would correlate
negatively mostly with the dynamic aspects of verbal-social and possibly intellectually activity,
but not with the aspects of physical activity; (c) STQ scales measuring the arousal aspects of
7
activity (i.e., Ergonicity) would correlate positively with the PTS scale of Strength of Excitation,
while the STQ scales of Plasticity and Tempo would correlate positively with the PTS Mobility
scale, and the STQ scale of Intellectual Ergonicity would correlate positively with the PTS scale
of Strength of Inhibition.
Method
Participants
Canadian participants (N=966; 233 men, 733 women) ages 17 to 35 years (M = 20.2, SD =
3.3), of whom 171 were volunteers and 743 were psychology students of McMaster University
and 52 were psychology students of Brock University (both universities are located in Southern
Ontario, Canada) took part in this study in 1999 to 2006. Of these, 775 of 966 participants, 175
men and 600 women (M age = 19.8 yr., SD = 3.0), 63 volunteers (8%) and 712 students
completed all measures; 74 student participants (M age = 20.1 yr., SD = 3.2) completed the
experimental task and the STQ, and 117 volunteers completed the STQ only (they were not
invited to participate in the experiment).
Measures
Participants (N=775) completed the extended English version of Rusalov's Structure of
Temperament Questionnaire (Rusalov & Trofimova, 2007), and the English version of Pavlovian
Temperament Survey (Strelau, 1999).
Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ: Rusalov, 1989; Rusalov & Trofimova,
2007).— This questionnaire has 150 statements to be answered using a 4-point Likert-type scale
with labels 1: Strongly disagree 2: Disagree, 3: Agree, and 4: Strongly agree. Six items are
assigned to a validity scale, and 144 items to 12 temperamental scales (12 items each) measuring
8
the four traits of Ergonicity, Plasticity, Tempo, and Emotionality in each of three areas of activity
(motor-physical, social-verbal, and intellectual-mental). The Cronbach alpha reliability
coefficient for the STQ scales ranged from .70 to .84 (Rusalov & Trofimova, 2007).
Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS: Strelau, 1999).—This survey has 66 statements to
be answered on a 4-point Likert-type scale with labels 1: Strongly disagree 2: Disagree, 3:
Agree, and 4: Strongly agree. There are three scales, Strength of Excitation, Strength of
Inhibition, and Mobility (22 items each), for which Cronbach coefficients alpha were reported to
range from .81 to .84.
Semantic task.— To complete this task, participants rated 30 abstract concepts (words) on
60 7-point, bipolar scales (i.e., warm vs. cold, soft vs. hard, interesting vs. uninteresting, etc.).
Each concept was presented as a word on a computer monitor along with each of the bipolar
scales.
Procedure
Participants were debriefed about the duration and nature of the experiment. They were
instructed to work as fast as possible and their time on this task was recorded. The computer
program Expan (provided by HR-Laboratory “Human Technologies”) 3 detected whether a
participant was giving random or inconsistent answers. The procedure took 1 to 3 hours
depending on the speed of the participant’s performance. All participants signed an informed
consent form before testing and participation in the experiment. Afterwards university students
received the practicum credit for their participation. All subjects were fluent in English.
Results and Discussion
3
http://www.ht.ru/about/index_eng.php
9
Table 1 shows means, standard deviations and confidence intervals for the applied
measures. Each STQ scale had a normal distribution of scores with range 12 to 48. The sex
differences in the scores on STQ scales were much stronger than on PTS scales, supporting the
first hypothesis. Analysis of sex differences using one-way ANOVA showed that women
performed the Semantic Task significantly faster than men, which was consistent with
significantly higher scores for women than men on Social Ergonicity and Social Tempo scales
(Table 1). Men had higher scores on Motor Ergonicity, Plasticity, Tempo, and Emotionality, and
also on Intellectual Plasticity and Tempo. Sex differences in PTS scores were found to be
statistically significant only for the Mobility scale (p < .01). These results are in line with
findings from studies showing that men have higher physical activity than women, especially in
activities requiring upper body strength (Thomas & French, 1985; Eaton & Enns, 1986), and that
on the average men perform better than women on tests of visual-spatial abilities but more
poorly than women on verbal tests (Hyde & Linn, 1988; Halpern, 2000).
Contrary to the common view of women as being more emotional than men, the results
showed that while women had higher scores on Social and Intellectual Emotionality scales, it
was men who had significantly higher scores on the scales of Motor Emotionality, i.e., male
subjects reported being more sensitive to success or failure in physical activities than did the
women. This result supports the idea of separation of two definitions of emotionality, expression
of an emotion or experience of an emotion. Several studies have found previously that the sex
differences were statistically significant when situational factors modeled underlying sex
stereotypes: women reported more intense emotional experiences than men in situations which
involved interpersonal rather than impersonal emotion elicitors (LaFrance & Banaji, 1992;
Fischer, 1993). In this sense, sensitivity to failure in relations and social activities might be
10
Comment [SAI3]:
Table 1
greater for women, and sensitivity to failure in physical activity might be greater for individuals
who are expected in society to be stronger or more physically fit, i.e., in general men.
Interestingly, that there was a significant negative correlation (p < .001) between the PTS
Strength of Inhibition and Social Emotionality scales (Table 2). It is possible that men consider
behavior related to social emotionality as disinhibited and inappropriate for their sex.
Sex differences among correlations of the temperament scales with duration on the
Semantic Task were not dramatic, but significant (Table 2). The temperament traits associated in
men with faster performance on the Semantic Task were different from those for faster performance
among women. The duration of this task for women showed significant correlations with all three
Tempo scales, Intellectual Plasticity, and Motor Ergonicity scales; these were not observed for
men. The duration of the Semantic Task for men had the strongest correlation with the STQ
Social Plasticity scale, which assesses how easily an individual generates, stops or switches between
verbal-social actions. It is possible that the size of the male sample was a factor contributing to
these differences. It is also possible, however, that men and women used different abilities to perform
the task and so had different styles of working on it. If women indeed are more accustomed to working
with words, then for them the Semantic Task is a matter of speed in doing rather automatic, well-known
work. If men on average report lower speed and endurance in verbal-social activities than women (as
noted above), then men might compensate through higher flexibility in the “switches” required in such
activities, and use their verbal-social plasticity to succeed on the Semantic Task.
Among all applied temperament scales, significant correlations (p < .001) of performance
times on the semantic task were found with the scales measuring dynamic aspects of socialverbal activity, i.e., Social Ergonicity, Social Plasticity, and Social Tempo, and much less with
other scales. Higher scores on these scales were associated with faster performance on the task,
which involves rating of abstract concepts. The correlations of other scales with the time
11
Comment [SAI4]:
Table 2
required to complete the Semantic Task were not consistent across sexes (Table 2), and not as
often significant. The three scales of the STQ related to verbal-social activity (Social Ergonicity,
Social Plasticity, and Social Tempo) must reflect the ability for intense and fast verbal activity
much better than do the scales of intellectual and motor activity. The semantic task required a
prolonged, intense activation of nervous processes related to verbal activity and the inhibition of
unrelated behaviors in order to stay focused; however, there were no significant correlations
consistent in either group by sex, between the PTS Strength of Excitation or Strength of
Inhibition scales and the duration of performance on the semantic task. There was only one
negative correlation of small effect size (p < .05) between the PTS Strength of Excitation scale
and the duration of the Semantic Task in the women, which affected the correlation between the
same in the whole sample. The PTS Mobility scale did not show significant correlation with the
duration of the Semantic Task, in spite of the fact that the task required flexibility of
performance in switching from one concept to another.
Such a selective pattern of correlations supports the second hypothesis, showing that the
STQ scales, designed to measure dynamic aspects of specific activities, have stronger
correlations with the speed of performance in corresponding activities than the nonspecific PTS
scales. Overall, the selectivity of correlations of temperament scales with the duration of the
semantic task and the findings on sex differences show the benefits of separation of
temperamental aspects according to the type of activity in the analysis of biological individual
differences.
Correlations between the STQ and PTS scales were in accord with the third hypothesis:
there were significant positive correlations between the STQ Ergonicity scales and the PTS scale
of Strength of Excitation, between the STQ Plasticity and Tempo scales and the PTS Mobility
12
scale, and between the STQ Intellectual Ergonicity scale and the PTS scale Strength of Inhibition
(Table 2). The significance of correlations between STQ scales and the PTS scales of Strength of
Excitation and Mobility was identical for men and women, so only “all sample” correlations are
given on these two PTS scales. Overall patterns of correlations between of STQ and PTS scales,
however, were nonspecific and were similar to those reported by Strelau (1999) for Polish and
German samples, and by Ruch, et al. (1991) for a German sample. All STQ scales measuring
characteristics of activity (i.e., Ergonicity, Plasticity, Tempo) in all three areas of activity (motor,
social and intellectual) had statistically significant (p < .001) positive correlations of various
magnitudes with the PTS scales of Strength of Excitation and Mobility. Such a pattern of
correlations indicates that almost all STQ scales measure arousal and mobility aspects of activity,
but PTS scales do not differentiate between aspects of arousal or types of activity.
The scales of Emotionality in Social and Intellectual activities (which measure sensitivity
of a person to failure or success) had significant negative correlations with the Strength of
Excitation and Mobility scales of the PTS. Such correlation suggests that people with low
arousal (i.e., with the low ability to sustain intense or prolonged work) and low mobility of
behavior might have frequent expectations of failure, increased neuroticism in social settings,
and overall problems in social adaptation. Significant negative correlation consistent for both
sexes was also found between the scores on the STQ Social Plasticity scale and the PTS Strength
of Inhibition, which suggests that inhibitory behavior as measured by PTS relates to hesitancy in
social activity as measured by the STQ, or that the content of the STQ Social Plasticity scale to a
large extent describes disinhibited behavior and should be examined further.
In conclusion, STQ and PTS both emerge from the Pavlovian tradition of experimental
study of properties of nervous systems and share the tenet that these properties have biological
13
basis and appear as dynamic aspects of human behaviour: energetic, mobility, and regulatory
aspects. The results illustrate the importance of separation of such dynamic aspects into three
main types of activity—physical, verbal, and intellectual—as such separation provides more
sensitive and detailed analysis of biologically based individual differences.
14
References
Beere, D., & Pica, M. (1995) The predisposition to dissociate: the temperamental traits of
flexibility/rigidity, daily rhythm, emotionality and interactional speed. Dissociation, 8, 236240.
Bishop, D., & Hertenstrein, M. (2004) A confirmatory factor analysis of the Structure of
Temperament Questionnaire. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 10191029.
Bishop, D., Jacks, H., & Tandy, S. B. (1993) Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ):
results from a U.S. sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 485-487.
Bodunov M. V., Bezdenezhnykh B. N., & Alexandrov Y. I. (1996) Charakteristika otvetov na
testovie zadania psihologicheskih metodik i structura individual’nogo opita [Peculiarities of
psychodiagnostic test item responses and the structure of individual experience].
Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 17, 87-96. [in Russian]
Brebner, J., & Stough, C. (1993). The relationship between the Structure of Temperament and
extraversion and neuroticism. Personality and Individual Differences. 14, 623-626.
Buss, A. & Plomin, R. (1984) Temperament: early developing personality trait. Hillsdale:
Erlbaum.
Buss, A., & Plomin, R. (1984) Temperament: early developing personality trait. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Byzova, V. M. (1997) Samoactualizacia predstaviteley Komi i russkogo etnosov [Selfactualization in representatives of Komi and Russian ethnic groups]. Psikhologicheskiy
Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 18, 61-69. [in Russian]
15
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994) Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective
responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333.
Cattell, R. B. (1965) The Scientific analysis of personality. London: Penguin.
Cloninger C. R., Przybeck, T. R., Svrakic, D. M. & Wetzel, R. D. (1994) The temperament and
character inventory (TCI): a guide to its development and use. St Louis, MO: Center for
Psychobiology of Personality.
Costa, P. T., Jr.,, & McCrae, R. R. (1992) Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO Pl-R) and
NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
Dumenci, L. (1995) The relation between the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire and other
personality domains. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55, 850-857.
Dumenci, L. (1996) Factorial validity of scores on the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 56, 487-493.
Eaton, W., & Enns, L. (1986) Gender differences in human motor activity level. Psychological
Bulletin, 100, 19–28.
Eputaev E. Y., Ikonnikova M. E., Agarkov B. A., & Tarabrina N. V. (2003) Dissociative states
and formal-dynamical properties of individual. Materials of 7th Multidisciplinary
Conference “Stress and Behavior”. Moscow: IPRAN Press. Pp. 121-123.
Eysenck, H. J.,, & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968) Manual for the Eysenck Personality Inventory. San
Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
Fischer, A. H. (1993) Gender differences in emotionality: fact or stereotype? Feminism and
Psychology, 3, 303-318.
16
Gray, J. A (1964) Pavlov's typology; recent theoretical and experimental developments from the
laboratory of B. M. Teplov. New York: Macmillan.
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septohippocampal system. New York: Oxford Univer. Press.
Halpern, D. F. (2000) Gender differences in cognitive abilities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Heymans, G. (1929). Inleiding tot de speciale psychologie. [Introduction to differential
psychology]. Haarlem, The Netherlands: De Erven F. Bohn.
Hyde, J. S,, & Linn, M. C. (1988) Gender differences in verbal ability: a meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69.
Kant, I. (1798/1974) Anthropology from a pragmatic point of view (Mary Gregor, Transl.). The
Hague, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff (Ak. VII).
LaFrance, M., & Banaji, M. (1992) Towards reconsideration of the gender-emotion relationship.
In M. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior: review of personality and social
psychology. Vol. 14. Newbury Park. CA: Sage. Pp. 178-201.
Lamiell, J. T. (2003) Beyond individual and group differences: human individuality, scientific
psychology, and William Stern's critical personalism. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Mehrabian, A., & Bank, L. (1978) A questionnaire measure of individual differences in
achieving tendency. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 38, 475-478.
Nebylitsyn, V. D. (1972) Fundamental properties of the human nervous system. New York:
Plenum.
Pavlov, I. P. (1941) Lectures on conditioned reflexes, v. IIi: types of the higher nervous activity,
their interdependence with neuroses and psychoses and the physiological mechanism of
neurotic and psychotic symptoms. New York: International Publishers.
17
Popov Y. A. (2006) Trevojnost’ v psycho-pedagogocheskom portrete studenta kolledga [Anxiety
in psycho-pedagogical portrait of a college student]. Professional’noye obuchenie
[Professional Education]. 8, 24-25. [in Russian]
Rathee N., & Singh, R. (2001) Mobility or/and lability of the nervous processes as
temperamental trait(s). Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 1091-1104.
Rothbart, M. K. (1988) Temperament and the development of inhibited approach. Child
Development, 59, 1241-1250.
Ruch, W., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1991) The Strelau Temperament Inventory – Revised
(STI-R): validity studies. European Journal of Personality, 5, 287-308.
Rusalov, V. M. (1979) Biologicheskiye osnovi individual’no-psichologicheskih razlichiy
(Biological basis of individual psychological differences). ɆRVFRZ: Nauka. [in Russian]
Rusalov, V. M. (1989) Motor and communicative aspects of human temperament: a new
questionnaire of the structure of temperament. Personality and Individual Differences, 10,
817-827.
Rusalov, V. M. (1997) Oprosnik formal’no-dynamicheskih svoystv individual’nosti.
Rukovodstvo. [Questionnaire of formal-dynamical properties of individuality. Manual].
Russian Academy of Sciences. Moscow: IPAN Press. [in Russian]
Rusalov, V. M. (2004) Formal’no-dynamicheskiye svoystva individual’nosti (temperament)
[Formal-dynamical properties of individual (temperament)]. Russian Academy of
Sciences, Moscow: IPAN Press. [in Russian]
Rusalov, V. M., & Dudin C. I. (1995) Temperament i intellect: obchie b specificheskiye factori
rasvitiya [Temperament and Intelligence: general and specific factors of development].
Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 16, 12-23. [in Russian]
18
Rusalov, V. M., & Naumova, E. R. (1999) On the relationship between general abilities and
"intellectual" scales of temperament. Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal],
20, , 70-77
Rusalov, V. M., & Poltavtzeva, L. I. (1997) Temperament as a prerequisite of creative abilities.
Pavlov Journal of Higher Nervous Activity, 47, 451-460.
Rusalov, V. M, Rusalova, M. N., & Strel’nikova, E. V. (2000) Temperament cheloveka i
osobennosti vibora mejdu veroyatnostyu dostijeniya tseli i ee tzennostyu [Temperament of
man and peculiarities of choice between the probability of goal achievement and its value.
Zhurnal Vysshey Nervnoy Deyatel’nosti [Journal of Higher Nervous Activity], 50, 388. [in
Russian]
Rusalov, V. M., & Trofimova, I. N. (2007) Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement.
Toronto: Psychological Services Press.
Stough, C., Brebner, J., & Cooper, C. (1991) The Rusalov Structure of Temperament
Questionnaire (STQ): results from an Australian sample. Personality and Individual
Differences, 12, 1355-1357.
Strelau, J. (1999) The Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS): an international handbook.
Stuttgart, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.
Strelau, J., & Angleitner, A. (Eds.) (1991) Explorations in temperament: international
perspectives on theory and measurement. New York: Plenum.
Thomas, J. R., & French, K. E. (1985) Gender differences across age in motor performance a
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 260-282.
Trofimova, I. N. (1999) How people of different age, gender and temperament estimate the
world. Psychological Reports, 85, 533-552.
19
Vasyura, S. A. (2008) Psychology of male and female communicative activity. The Spanish
Journal of Psychology, 11, 289-300.
Vorobieva, E. V. (2004) Modern psychogenetic studies of intelligence and theory of motivation
for achievements. Journal of Applied Psychology. 3, 53-59.
Windle, M., & Lerner, R. M. (1986) Reassessing the dimensions of temperament individually
across life time span: the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R). Journal
of Adolescent Research, 1, 213-230.
Zin’ko, E. V. (2006) Kharakteristiki samootcenki i urovnya dostijeniy i ih sopolojenie
[Characteristics of self-image and of level of aspiration and its relationships].
Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Psychological Journal], 3, 18-30; 4, 15-25. [in Russian]
Accepted July 29, 2009.
20
Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, 95% Confidence Intervals and F Values of One-way
Analyses of Varaince for Sex For Each Measure
Measure
M
SD
Time on Semantic
Men, n= 202
Task, min.
84.5
PTS scales
Men, n= 175
25.5
95%CI
80.9–88.0
M
SD
95%CI
F()
Women, n= 647
df=1, 847
75.7
24.80‡
20.4
74.1-77.3
Women, n = 600
df= 1, 773
Strength of Excitation 58.4
7.5
57.2-59.5
57.4
6.9
56.9-58.0
2.45
Strength of Inhibition
58.7
6.4
57.7-59.6
58.3
6.4
57.8-58.8
0.41
Mobility
58.4
6.7
57.4-59.4
56.8
6.8
56.3-57.4
6.83†
STQ scales
Motor Ergonicity
Intellectual Ergonicity
Social Ergonicity
Motor Plasticity
Intellectual Plasticity
Social Plasticity
Motor Tempo
Intellectual Tempo
Social Tempo
Motor Emotionality
Intellectual
Emotionality
Social Emotionality
Men, n = 233
Women, n = 733
df=1, 964
33.5
7.4
32.5-34.4
30.9
7.0
30.4-31.4
22.72‡
31.4
5.5
30.7-32.1
30.8
5.6
30.4-31.2
1.69
34.4
7.1
33.5-35.4
36.0
6.7
35.5-36.4
8.76‡
32.2
5.7
31.5-32.9
30.7
5.5
30.3-31.1
12.05‡
30.7
5.4
30.0-31.4
29.2
5.2
28.8-29.6
14.61‡
29.6
6.3
28.8-30.4
28.8
6.6
28.3-29.3
2.51
33.7
6.0
33.0-34.5
32.8
5.3
32.4-33.2
4.78*
34.4
5.6
33.7-35.2
32.1
5.3
31.7-32.5
34.16‡
33.6
5.9
32.8-34.3
36.5
5.5
36.1-36.9
47.58‡
26.9
5.7
26.1-27.6
25.4
6.0
25.0-25.8
10.96‡
29.6
5.7
28.8-30.3
30.8
6.0
30.4-31.3
8.16‡
28.1
6.1
27.3-28.8
29.6
5.7
29.2-30.0
11.85‡
*p<.05. †p<.01. ‡p>001.
21
Comment [SAI5]: All en dashes
Table 2. Correlations Between the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ) Scales,
Duration of Semantic Task (min.) (n=849) and Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS)
scales (n=775).
Scales
Time on ST
Total
PTSM
Total
Total
Men Women
PTS-E
PTS-I
Total
Men Women
-.05
.00
-.11†
.38‡
.22‡
.04
-.04
.06
.01
-.01
.00
.33‡
.23‡
.15‡
.16*
.15‡
-.18‡
-.14*
-.18‡
.33‡
.31‡
-.09*
-.09
-.09*
.03
-.02
.01
.35‡
.38‡
.12‡
.09
.13‡
-.06
-.07
-.09*
.41‡
.45‡
.02
.09
-.01
-.14‡
-.18†
-.14‡
.37‡
.39‡
-.27‡
-.28‡
-.28‡
Motor Tempo
-.10†
-.08
-.13†
.43‡
.33‡
.05
-.06
.09*
Intellectual Tempo
-.08*
-.13
-.11†
.40‡
.34‡
.02
.07
.00
Social Tempo
-.20‡
-.14*
-.18‡
.40‡
.32‡
-.09*
-.11
-.09*
.02
.04
-.01
-.01
-.01
-.05
-.06
-.05
-.02
-.11
.04
-.09*
-.11†
.00
-.03
.01
.00
.06
.01
-.21‡
-.14‡
-.07
-.16‡
-.04
-.08*
-.06
-.10*
-
.52‡
.05
-.01
.06
.09*
.13‡
-.05
-.07
-.07
STQ
Motor Ergonicity
Intellectual
Ergonicity
Social Ergonicity
Motor Plasticity
Intellectual
Plasticity
Social Plasticity
Motor Emotionality
Intellectual
Emotionality
Social Emotionality
PTS
Strength of
Excitation (n=722)
Strength of
Inhibition (n=722)
Mobility (n=722)
22
Note.— PTS-E=PTS Strength of Excitation scale, PTS-I =Strength of Inhibition scale, PTS-M=
Mobility scale. *p < .05. †p < .01. ‡p < .001.
23
Appendix A
Examples of the items of the Extended Structure of Temperament Questionnaire.
STQ Scale
Item
Motor Ergonicity
4. I get pleasure from doing physical work in my free time.
32. It is easy for me to do hard physical work.
17. I get tired of prolonged mental work.
82. I carry out my mental activities with pleasure.
33. I would feel uncomfortable if I didn't communicate with
people for a long time.
98. I'm silent even among my friends.
11. I quickly move into high gear.
40. I successfully carry out tasks requiring subtle or fine
movements.
8. It is easy for me to simultaneously do several things, for
example, to watch TV and read.
18. I easily switch from solving one problem to another.
15. It's easy for me to enter into conversation with strangers.
51. It's easy for me to make new acquaintances.
118. I work slowly when I make something by hand.
122. I prefer to do my physical work at a fast rate.
23. I like intellectual games that require fast decision making.
38. My thoughts flow slowly.
50. It's hard for me to talk fast.
71. I'm a talkative person.
52. I get annoyed if I am not agile during recreational games.
73. My mood is often spoiled when the things made by myself
are not as good as I hoped.
12. I am upset finding an error in my decisions, even small
ones.
25. I fear that I cannot perform work requiring mental strain.
56. I'm upset if the people whom I'm talking with do not
understand me.
109. I worry a lot when I must discuss relationships with
friends.
Intellectual Ergonicity
Social Ergonicity
Motor Plasticity
Intellectual Plasticity
Social Plasticity
Motor Tempo
Intellectual Tempo
Social Tempo
Motor Emotionality
Intellectual Emotionality
Social Emotionality
24
Appendix B
Validation history of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire
During the experimental validation of the STQ the performance by participants on the
following measures was compared with scores on STQ scales in a series of studies in the 1980s:
speed of writing, reading and speed of generation of words, maximal and optimal tempo of
performance in sensory-motor tasks and intellectual (including unsolvable) tasks, performance
on nonverbal tasks, with which participants were unfamiliar, rigidity of perception in tactile and
visual modalities, duration of the switch between one method of solving a task and another,
mobility in attention, variability in line drawing (Rusalov, 1979, 1997, 2004; Rusalov &
Trofimova, 2007). Ergonicity scales of the STQ correlated positively with Eysenck Personality
Questionnaire’s Extraversion scale (Rusalov, 1989; Brebner & Stough, 1993, Zin’ko, 2006), with
the Big-Five Extraversion scale (Bodunov, Bezdenezhnykh, & Alexandrov, 1996; Rusalov &
Trofimova, 2007), with Strelau’s PTS Strength of Excitation scale (Ruch, Angleitner, & Strelau,
1991; Bodunov,et al., 1996; Strelau, 1999), with Torrance’ Nonverbal Tests of Creative
Thinking (Rusalov & Poltavtzeva, 1997), Rotter’s Locus of Control scale (Byzova, 1997), with
goal-driven choice of profession as opposed to accessibility of profession (Rusalov, Rusalova, &
Strel’nikova, 2000), and with the Motivation for Achievement scale (Vorobieva, 2004).
Scores of the Motor and Social Plasticity and Tempo scales of the STQ correlated
positively with Strelau’s PTS Mobility scale (Ruch, et al., 1991; Bodunov, et al., 1996; Strelau,
1999), with adaptivity of behaviour on the Dembo-Hoppe Level of Aspiration experiment
(Zin’ko, 2006), with the Torrance’ Nonverbal Tests of Creative Thinking (Rusalov &
Poltavtzeva, 1997), Rotter Locus of Control scale (Byzova, 1997) and with the Motivation for
25
Achievement scale (Vorobieva, 2004). Rathee and Singh (2001) reported a comparison of 25
measures of Mobility, including the Plasticity and Tempo scales of the English version of the
STQ. The authors found high correlations of scores on the Extended STQ Plasticity scales with
those for Alteration task, Flexibility of attention, proof-reading ability, the number of trials
needed for participants to reach the optimal reaction time after alteration of a stimulus, and
Mobility measured by Strelau’s Pavlovian Temperamental Survey (PTS). Tempo in Motor
Activity as measured by the English STQ in this VWXG\ZDVFRUUHODWHGZLWK((*Į-general speed,
time taken to generate simultaneous contrast, duration of “after-image” reaction, critical flicker
fusion and size of uncertainty interval. Scores on the Plasticity scales also positively correlated
with those on Eysenck’s EPQ Extraversion scale (Rusalov, 1989; Brebner & Stough, 1993;
Zin’ko, 2006), on the Big-Five Extraversion scale (Dumenci, 1995; Bodunov,et al., 1996), and
on PTS Strength of Excitation scale (Ruch, et al., 1991; Strelau, 1999).
Emotionality scales scores correlated positively with those on the Neuroticism scale of the
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Rusalov, 1989; Brebner & Stough, 1993; Zin’ko, 2006),
with the Big-Five Neuroticism scale (Dumenci, 1995; Bodunov, et al., 1996), on the State Trait
Anxiety Inventory (Popov, 2006), with Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale (Popov, 2006; Zin’ko,
2006), and use of alcohol (Bodunov,et al., 1996), and correlated negatively with scores on the
Dissociative Experiences Scale (Beere & Pica, 1995; Eputaev, Ikonnikova, Agarkov, &
Tarabrina, 2003), the Rosenzveig test (Zin’ko, 2006), PTS Strength of Excitation and Strength of
Inhibition scales (Ruch, et al., 1991; Strelau, 1999), scores on Cattell’s A, H, Q2 and Q4 factors
(Vasyura, 2008), Torrance’ Nonverbal Tests of Creative Thinking (Rusalov & Poltavtzeva,
1997), and the Motivation for Achievement scale (Vorobieva, 2004). Trofimova (1999) applied
the semantic differential method to contrast temperamental groups selected on the basis of STQ
26
scores. She reported that subjects with the highest and lowest scores on STQ scales show
differences in their perceptions of semantically neutral objects, including self-perception. The
STQ scales which measure dynamic aspects of intellectual activity had positive correlations with
such measures of intelligence as the Wechsler and Shepard tests, including the tasks measuring
classification abilities (“Excluding the third”) and plasticity in nonverbal thinking (Rusalov &
Dudin, 1995; Rusalov & Naumova, 1999). Intellectual activity scales had positive correlations
with scores on the Locus of Control scale (Byzova, 1997), and goal-oriented choice of profession
(Rusalov, et al., 2000), and negative correlations with translations of the State Trait Anxiety
Inventory (Popov, 2006), Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale (Popov, 2006; Zin’ko, 2006) and the
access-oriented choice of profession (Rusalov, Rusalova, & Strel’nikova, 2000). Intellectual
plasticity correlated with 25 measures of mobility in Rathee and Singh’s study (2001).
The administration of the English version of the STQ to American, Australian, and
Canadian samples showed the factor structure of this version similar to the Russian language
version, and that the English version possessed good reliability and internal consistency (Stough,
Brebner, & Cooper, 1991; Bishop, Jacks, & Tandy, 1993; Dumenci, 1995, 1996; Bishop &
Hertenstein, 2004; Rusalov, 2004; Rusalov & Trofimova, 2007).
27
Download