Judging Health Status: Effects of Perceived Prevalence and

advertisement
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1986, Vol. 50, No. 5, 899-905
Copyright 1986 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0022-3514/86/S00.75
Judging Health Status: Effects of Perceived Prevalence
and Personal Relevance
John B. Jemmott III and Peter H. Ditto
Princeton University
Robert T. Croyle
Williams College
In this article, we show that people's evaluations of the seriousness of a health disorder are influenced
by the perceived prevalence and personal relevance of that disorder. As part of a study ostensibly
concerned with college students' health characteristics, 60 undergraduates were "tested" for the presence
of a fictitious enzyme deficiency. The subjects discovered either that they had the deficiency (deficiencypresent subjects) or that they did not have it (deficiency-absent subjects), and were led to believe either
that 1 of the 5 people in the laboratory had the deficiency (low-prevalence subjects) or that 4 of them
had it (high-prevalence subjects). As predicted, the low-prevalence subjects evaluated the deficiency
as more serious than did the high-prevalence subjects. In addition, consistent with the view that
personal relevance affects perceptions of health disorders, the deficiency-present subjects evaluated
the deficiency as less serious than did the deficiency-absent subjects. The deficiency-present subjects
also derogated the validity of the test ostensibly used to diagnose the deficiency compared with other
subjects. Finally, the deficiency-present subjects requested more information about the deficiency than
did the deficiency-absent subjects.
Everyone at one time or another has had or has known someone with a health disorder. The mere pervasiveness of health
disorders makes them interesting as a psychological phenomenon.
People are constantly evaluating health disorders—both their own
and those of others—to determine the importance of the disorders
and the type of behavior warranted by them. The physiological
roots of physical health are undeniable; but judgments about
health are inherently psychological. These judgments, as such,
may be subject to certain cognitive and motivational influences—
a characteristic they share with myriad phenomena of interest
to social psychologists.
There are obvious parallels between health disorders and negatively valenced personal attributes or stigmas (Goffman, 1963;
Jones et al., 1984; Katz, 1981). People typically consider health
disorders to be undesirable. The afflicted person may experience
anxiety. Others react to victims of some disorders with ambivalence, avoidance, or even scorn. This is not to say that health
disorders are similar in all respects to other negative attributes.
Indeed the latter themselves are not entirely homogeneous (Jones
et al., 1984), and health disorders, too, differ on many dimensions
(D'Andrade, Quinn, Nerlove, & Romney, 1972; Linz, Penrod,
Siverhus, & Leventhal, 1982). Nevertheless, there are sufficient
similarities to make research on reactions to health disorders
potentially informative about more traditional social psychological concerns and vice versa.
In this article, two factors that might affect judgments about
a health disorder are considered: The perceived prevalence of
the disorder and its personal relevance. The specific judgment
examined is the evaluation of seriousness. Obviously there are
other aspects of the perception of health disorders one could
examine (e.g., D'Andrade et al., 1972; Linz et al., 1982). The
judgment of seriousness is interesting because it captures, in a
simple way, a critical part of what is on the layperson's mind
when considering a health disorder.
Little hard evidence exists on the determinants of judgments
of the seriousness of a health disorder; yet such judgments are
seen as important to many health-relevant behaviors, including
compliance with treatment regimens, the decision to seek medical
attention, and the tendency to delay before seeking such attention
(Becker, 1979; Safer, Tharps, Jackson, & Leventhal, 1979; Rosenstock & Kirscht, 1979). In short, researchers typically treat
seriousness judgments as independent rather than as dependent
variables. There is evidence that pain, sudden disability, interference with valued activities, and novelty all characterize health
disorders that are likely to prompt contact with medical professionals or with the informal lay referral network (Friedson, 1970;
Safer et al., 1979; Suchman, 1965; Twaddle, 1969; Zola, 1966).
But, on the issue of what affects seriousness judgments, such
evidence is at best indirect.
We hypothesize that the perceived prevalence of a negative
attribute such as a health disorder influences how it is evaluated,
namely, that people may be biased toward interpreting a disorder
as more serious to the extent that they perceive it as relatively
rare. Research in social cognition supports the notion that the
The authors gratefully acknowledge the helpful comments of Andy
Crider, Neil Lutsky, Gerry Sande, and Peggy Thoits on an earlier version
of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John
B. Jemmott III, Princeton University, Department of Psychology, Green
Hall, Princeton, New Jersey 08544.
899
900
J. JEMMOTT, P. DITTO, AND R. CROYLE
prevalence of an attribute can affect its evaluation. Individuals
whose social categories are relatively scarce in a setting are evaluated more extremely—more favorably or more unfavorably—
than are those whose social category is less scarce (MacArthur,
1981; Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff, & Ruderman, 1978). Commodity
theory also asserts that perceived prevalence influences evaluations, specifying that commodities (denned as attractive objects)
have higher valuation when they are perceived as less plentiful
(Brock, 1968; Fromkin & Snyder, 1980).
In the health domain, some evidence is consistent with the
view that the perceived prevalence of a disorder may affect reactions to that disorder. Zola (1966) argued that the prevalence
of a disorder affects decisions about seeking medical treatment.
He cited examples of populations where particular medically
significant disorders were widespread, yet were not seen as especially pathologic and were seldom treated medically. One interpretation of these data is that owing to the perceived high prevalence of the disorders, they were evaluated as benign and consequently treatment was not sought.
More direct evidence on the role of perceived prevalence in
evaluations of health disorders is provided by Jemmott, Croyle,
and Ditto (1984) who found that the lower college students estimated the prevalence of a familiar symptom or disease to be,
the higher were their ratings of that disorder's seriousness and
vice versa. Although the prevalence estimates may have affected
the seriousness judgments, it is possible that the reverse occurred
or that some unmeasured variable influenced both kinds of
judgments. The present research attempts to clarify this relation.
This research also examines whether the personal relevance
of a disorder affects judgments of its seriousness. That relevance
can affect people's judgments is suggested both by studies (Chaiken, 1980; Petty &Cacioppo, 1979) demonstrating that personally relevant information is processed differently from information that is not personally relevant and by correspondent inference theory (Jones & Davis, 1965), which holds that the
relevance to the perceiver of an actor's behavior will affect evaluations of that behavior.
We hypothesize that a disorder is seen as less serious if it has
high personal relevance than if it has low personal relevance. If
one assumes that a disorder has greater relevance to the people
who have it than to those who do not, then the hypothesis is
consistent with research indicating that people are less likely to
interpret a hypothetical bodily sensation as indicating disease
(Campbell, 1975) and to say it is appropriate to consult a physician about it (Rosenstock & Kirscht, 1979) if those decisions
concern themselves rather than others. It is also supported by
research indicating that people who report having had a disorder
rate it lower in seriousness than do other people (Jemmott et al.,
1984).
These differential judgments may stem at least in part from
defensiveness. Although some people may perceive benefits in
having a disorder and some circumstances may foster such perceptions (Barsky & Klerman, 1983; Jones & Berglas, 1978; Visotsky, Hamburg, Goss, & Lebovitz, 1961), for most people most
of the time, the perception that one has, may have, or is susceptible to a health disorder arouses anxiety. It threatens self-image,
including self-perceptions of invulnerability, and one's customary
life style (Cohen & Lazarus, 1979; Weinstein, 1982). To cope
with anxiety-provoking health threats, people often deny having
their diagnosed disorder or downplay its probable consequences
(Cohen & Lazarus, 1979; Janis, 1958; Kubler-Ross, 1969; Katz,
Weiner, Gallagher, &Hellman, 1970; Visotsky et al., 1961). That
a disorder need not be extremely serious to prompt denial-like
responses is suggested by Campbell's (1975) finding of such responses among subjects judging rather mild disorders. More generally, social psychologists have reported defensiveness phenomena in a variety of contexts where individuals are threatened by
negative feedback (Burger, 1981; Zuckerman, 1979).
The present research brings a novel approach to the study of
subjective judgments of health disorders: the experimental design.
A fictitious disorder was created for use in the experiment. It
was ostensibly a deficiency in a recently discovered enzyme that
affects pancreatic functioning. For half of the subjects the enzyme
deficiency had relatively high personal relevance: They were led
to believe they had the enzyme deficiency (deficiency-present
subjects). For the others, the deficiency had relatively low personal
relevance: They were led to believe they did not have it (deficiency-absent subjects). The advantages of an experimental design
over case study or correlational designs are numerous. However,
the possible negative consequences for the subjects were also
recognized, and precautions were taken throughout the experiment to minimize them.
In devising the prevalence manipulation, we assumed that laypersons' perceptions of the prevalence of a disorder are probably
more often influenced by the number of people they encounter
or hear about who have that disorder than by numerically summarized prevalence statistics. In addition, following social evaluation theories, we assumed that the effect of prevalence information would be greatest if it concerned people similar to the
perceiver (Festinger, 1954; Kelley, 1968; Sanders, 1982). Accordingly, we manipulated perceptions of prevalence by varying
the number of fellow students present in the laboratory who
apparently had the deficiency. Half the subjects were led to believe
that 1 of the 5 people in the laboratory had the deficiency (lowprevalence subjects); half were led to believe that 4 of the 5 had
it (high-prevalence subjects).
In summary, we predicted that the subjects in the low-prevalence condition would give higher ratings of the seriousness of
the deficiency than would those in the high-prevalence condition.
We also predicted that the subjects in the deficiency-present condition, those for whom the deficiency had great personal relevance, would rate the deficiency as less serious than would those
in the deficiency-absent condition, for whom it had relatively
little personal relevance.
Method
Subjects
The subjects were 64 undergraduates who volunteered for a study of
the health characteristics of college students. The results from 4 subjects
were discarded after their participation, 2 because they voiced suspicions
about the experimental manipulations and 2 because they misinterpreted
the deficiency-present versus absent manipulation. The final sample,
therefore, consists of 60 subjects, 37 men and 23 women.
Procedure
The subjects were initially contacted by phone and discreetly administered a hypochondriasis scale (Pilowsky, 1967). Those scoring 2 or more
standard deviations above the normal mean were excluded, and, because
901
JUDGING HEALTH STATUS
of the use of glucose in the procedure, so were diabetics and hypoglycemics.' The subjects were told the study was jointly sponsored by the
psychology department and the "Health Awareness Project (HAP)," ostensibly a federally funded health organization attempting to gauge the
health characteristics of undergraduates and to increase their awareness
of health-related issues. The experiment was conducted in a laboratory
replete with medical trappings—health posters, stethoscopes, eye charts,
and the like—and the subjects were met by a male experimenter wearing
a white lab coat.
The subjects were told that they would complete health questionnaires
and would undergo several medical tests, some standard, like blood pressure, eyesight, and hearing tests, and some very recently developed, like
the Thioamine Acetylase (TAA) Saliva Reaction Test and the Auditory
Reaction Time Test. The experimenter measured the subjects' blood
pressures and pulse rates, veridically reported them to the subjects, and
then ushered each subject into one of five soundproof booths, where a
computer-assisted hearing test was to be administered. Two or 3 subjects
were run at a time, but they were led to believe 5 subjects were present
in the booths. With the subjects in their booths, the experimenter explained over an audio system that due to a microcomputer problem the
hearing test would be delayed, that they should start completing the Cornell
Medical Index (a lengthy questionnaire; Brodman, Erdmann, & Wolff,
1949), and that he would contact them when the hearing test was ready.
He then individually contacted each subject over the audio system while
leading him or her to believe that they all were being addressed. In this
way, the subjects were individually randomly assigned to conditions. He
stated that the malfunction was taking longer to repair than he had anticipated and that they would therefore proceed to the TAA Saliva Reaction
Test. The test was described in a booklet, which the experimenter told
the subjects to read while he read it to them over the audio system.
The booklet indicated that Thioamine Acetylase or TAA is an enzyme
that is active in pancreatic functioning and that some people have the
enzyme but others do not. It indicated that researchers have recently
discovered that people who lack TAA seem to be relatively susceptible
to a complex of mild but irritating pancreatic disorders. If TAA is present
in the body, the booklet stated, it can be detected in saliva with a chemically
coated paper that was developed 18 months ago. Reference was then
made to the similarity of the TAA Saliva Reaction Test to the Phenylthiocarbamide (PTC; Stedman, 1972) taste test. At this point, personal
relevance was manipulated.
Manipulating Personal Relevance
The high personal relevance subjects, those randomly assigned to the
deficiency-present conditions, were told that the yellow TAA test paper
would turn green if placed in contact with saliva that does not contain
TAA. The low personal relevance subjects, those in the deficiency-absent
conditions, were told that the paper would turn green if placed in contact
with saliva that contains TAA. The experimenter then told the subjects
how to self-administer the test. They were told to rinse their mouths with
a cup of mouthwash, purportedly to help generate saliva and to remove
any food residue that might contaminate the test. They were told next
to place a small amount of saliva in a cup and then to take out a TAA
test strip and rub it in the saliva so that both sides were uniformly moistened but the end of the strip held between fingers was dry. They were
told to wait 30 s for color development to be completed. In fact, the strip
always turned green because, unknown to the subjects, it was glucosesensitive paper and a small amount of glucose had been added to the
mouthwash. Thus, all subjects saw the color reaction, but, depending on
the condition to which they had been randomly assigned, interpreted it
differently.
Manipulating the Prevalence of the Deficiency
At this point, the perceived prevalence of the deficiency was manipulated. The experimenter stated that he needed a tally of the number of
subjects who actually had TAA deficiency and told them to flip on their
indicator lights if the test indicated they had the deficiency. If the subject
was in the low-prevalence condition, the experimenter said the lights
indicated that only 1 out of the 5 of them had the deficiency. If the subject
was in the high-prevalence condition, he said the lights indicated that 4
of the 5 had the deficiency. Fifteen subjects were randomly assigned to
each of the four conditions formed by crossing personal relevance and
prevalence of deficiency.
Dependent Measures
Prevalence and seriousness. The subjects next completed a questionnaire concerning 15 symptoms and illnesses, one of which was TAA
deficiency. It contained the following questions: (a) Have you ever heard
of this disorder? (b) Have you ever had this disorder? (c) What is your
estimate of the percentage of the college-age population that has had this
disorder? (d) How serious a threat to health is this disorder? They answered
this last question on a scale from not serious (0) to very serious (100).
The questionnaire included a few rather obscure disorders so that inclusion
of TAA deficiency would not seem unusual. The other disorders were
laryngitis, hypoglycemia, migraine headache, systemic lupus erythematosus, bronchitis, hypertension, anemia, ulcers, mononucleosis, astigmatism, hemmorhoids, skin cancer, asthma, and conjunctivitis.
Information seeking. The subjects then completed the HAP Services
Form, which assessed their interest in additional information about TAA
deficiency. Services were listed concerning several disorders, including
TAA deficiency, and the subjects were asked to indicate the ones they
desired.
Diagnostic test accuracy. The subjects next took the Quick Health
Quiz, which included, among filler questions, a scale for them to rate
the accuracy of the TAA Saliva Test from very inaccurate (1) to very
accurate (9).
Debriefing
The subjects were then ushered out of the booths and put through a
careful process debriefing (Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975). They were
told that TAA did not exist, that the saliva test had been engineered so
that the test strip always turned green, that the test had no bearing on
their susceptibility to pancreatic disorders, and that the interpretation
they had been given depended only on the experimental condition to
which they had been randomly assigned. They were shown the table of
random numbers used to assign them to conditions and heard the reactions
of other subjects assigned to different conditions. The reasons for the
deception were explained, any remaining questions the subjects had were
sensitively answered, and they were paid for their participation. The subjects revealed little if any evidence of distress about the procedures during
the debriefing.
Results
Prevalence (low, high) X Deficiency (present, absent) X Sexof-Subject (male, female) analyses of variance (ANOVA) revealed
no sex-of-subject main effects or interactions on any dependent
variable; accordingly, this factor will not be discussed. The prevalence manipulation was successful. The subjects in the high-
1
Three potential subjects were excluded because they scored very high
on hypochondriasis and one was excluded because he was a diabetic. One
disadvantage of the systematic exclusion of subjects scoring high in hypochondriasis should be noted: It may lead to an underestimation of the
size of the experimental effects.
902
J. JEMMOTT, P. DITTO, AND R. CROYLE
prevalence conditions (M = 52.33, SD = 20.07) estimated that
a greater percentage of the college-age population has TAA deficiency than did those in the low-prevalence conditions (M =
19.87, SD = 13.85), F(l, 52) = 50.29, p < .0001.2 There were
no other significant effects on prevalence estimates.
Table 1
Mean Ratings of the Accuracy of the TAA Saliva Reaction Test
by Deficiency Presence and Prevalence
Prevalence of the
deficiency
Prevalence and Perceived Seriousness
Deficiency presence
As predicted, the subjects in the low-prevalence conditions
(M = 37.00, SD = 18.22) rated TAA deficiency as more serious
than did those in the high-prevalence conditions (M = 29.33,
SD = 17.00), F(l, 52) = 3.77, p < .058. The Prevalence X Deficiency interaction was not significant. The correlation between
subjects' estimates of the percentage of the college-age population
that has TAA deficiency and their ratings of the seriousness of
the deficiency provides additional evidence supporting the hypothesis. It is negative and significant, r(58) = —.39, p < .002,
indicating that the lower a subject's estimate of the prevalence
of TAA deficiency, the higher is his or her rating of its seriousness.3
The data from the other 14 health disorders on the H questionnaire also support the view that the lower the perceived prevalence
of a disorder, the more serious it is perceived to be. All 14 correlations are negative, and 6 are significant (ps < .05, one-tailed).
A Fisher Z transformation (Cohen & Cohen, 1975) was applied
to the correlation coefficients, and the mean was calculated. It
was negative (-.23) and significantly different from 0, t(\3) =
5.56, p<. 0001.
Deficiency-Present
M
SD
Low
High
6.60
2.10
7.46
1.30
7.66
7.60
.90
.91
Deficiency-Absent
M
SD
Note. Higher numbers indicate greater accuracy.
Information Seeking
Seeking information is one way of actively coping with a health
threat (Cohen & Lazarus, 1979). It was widely used by the subjects in the deficiency-present condition, where 83% requested
at least one HAP informational service concerning TAA and
where such requests were independent of ratings of diagnostic
test accuracy and TAA seriousness. ANOVA reveals that the deficiency-present subjects (M = 1.53, SD = 1.07) requested a
greater number of such services than did the deficiency-absent
subjects (M = .23, SD = .50), F(l, 52) = 20.58, p < .0001. No
other effects were significant.
Personal Relevance and Perceived Seriousness
It was also hypothesized that subjects to whom TAA deficiency
was highly personally relevant would evaluate it as less serious
than would the other subjects. This prediction is supported, F( 1,
52) = 9.98, p < .003. The deficiency-present subjects (M = 25.67,
SD = 18.13) rated the deficiency as less serious than did the
deficiency-absent subjects (M = 40.67, SD = 14.37).4
Diagnostic Test Accuracy
We were concerned that the subjects in the high-prevalence
conditions, where the TAA Saliva Reaction Test indicated that
4 of the 5 people had the deficiency, might doubt the test's validity.
Instead we found that, though the difference is statistically nonsignificant, F(l, 52) = 1.12, the mean rated accuracy of the test
actually is slightly higher in the high-prevalence conditions
(M = 7.53) than in the low-prevalence conditions (M = 7.13).
The Prevalence X Deficiency interaction was not significant.
Interestingly, high personal relevance occasioned lower estimates of the accuracy of the diagnostic test: The deficiency-present subjects (M = 7.03) rated the test's accuracy as lower than
did the deficiency-absent subjects (M = 7.63), F(\, 52) = 3.55,
p < .06. Inspection of the means in Table 1 provides evidence
of defensiveness. The test-derogation effect is especially pronounced in the low-prevalence, deficiency-present cell. Its mean
is significantly lower than the others, F(l, 52) = 5.56, p < .03.
This is precisely the condition where anxiety about having the
deficiency should be greatest and where therefore the desire to
deny having the disorder should be strongest.
Discussion
The results indicate that perceptions of the prevalence of a
health disorder influence judgments of its seriousness. The subjects who thought only 1 of the 5 people in their lab session had
TAA deficiency gave higher ratings of the deficiency's seriousness
than did those who thought 4 of them had the deficiency. To the
subjects in this experiment, TAA deficiency was a novel disorder—indeed it is bogus—yet the results concerning perceptions
2
Why the mean prevalence estimate of the high-prevalence subjects
(52.33%) substantially deviates from the sample prevalence (80%), whereas
the mean estimate of the low-prevalence subjects (19.87%) is virtually
identical to the sample prevalence (20%) is unclear. One possible explanation is the operation of the representativeness heuristic (Kahneman &
Tversky, 1972). The sample prevalence of 20% may have seemed more
reasonable as reflecting a population prevalence than did the sample
prevalence of 80%. Indeed, the median prevalence estimate the subjects
gave for the other 14 diseases ranged from 5% to 40% with a median of
18%. Thus a sample-based estimate of 80% probably seemed unrepresentative, and the subjects probably questioned its reliability and adjusted
their population estimates downward.
3
The within-cell correlations, which are strongest among deficiencypresent subjects, are as follows: deficiency present, low prevalence, r =
-.53, p < .04; deficiency present, high prevalence, r = —.39, p < .15;
deficiency absent, low prevalence, r = —.20, ns; and deficiency absent,
high prevalence, r = —.02, ns.
4
The sample was too small to obtain a reliable test of the relation of
having had a disorder to ratings of its seriousness for the 14 other diseases
and symptoms. Too few subjects reported having had the disorders.
JUDGING HEALTH STATUS
of prevalence and seriousness mirror those in Jemmott et al.
(1984), a correlational study on judgments of familiar health
disorders.
It might be asserted that the subjects in the high-prevalence
condition rated the disorder's seriousness as lower, not simply
because of the prevalence of the deficiency, but also because of
its unfamiliarity. In this view, the high-prevalence subjects might
have reasoned that one usually hears about health disorders that
are serious and widespread but not about those that are benign
and widespread; hence, because they had not heard of TAA deficiency, it must not be serious. That these subjects actually reasoned in this manner, however, is unlikely because all the subjects
were told that the problematic nature of TAA deficiency had
been only recently discovered. This was stressed in both the experimenter's initial introduction of the study and the booklet for
the TAA Saliva Reaction Test and provided an adequate and
salient explanation for their lack of prior knowledge of TAA
deficiency.
Zola (1966) argued that laypersons' perceptions of the prevalence of a disorder influence whether they seek medical attention
concerning it, that those who believe that a disorder is common
are unlikely to seek such attention. In the present study, lowand high-prevalence subjects in the deficiency-present condition
did not differ in interest in additional information about TAA
deficiency. This may have occurred because the subjects perceived
the enzyme deficiency as untreatable. If they did, additional information would serve only to exacerbate anxiety about having
it (cf. Leventhal, 1970; Rogers, 1983) and therefore may have
been avoided in the low prevalence condition where the deficiency
was seen as particularly serious.5
The deficiency-present subjects, those to whom TAA deficiency
had great personal relevance, rated the deficiency lower in seriousness than did the deficiency-absent subjects. Consistent with
a defensiveness explanation of this effect are the results concerning
ratings of the accuracy of the test used to diagnose TAA deficiency. The deficiency-present subjects judged the test to be less
accurate than did the deficiency-absent subjects. Considerable
research suggests that denial is a common initial reaction when
a person is confronted with a health threat (Breznitz, 1983). The
deficiency-present subjects, by derogating the validity of the diagnostic test relative to other subjects, may have buttressed hopes
that they did not lack the enzyme after all. The defensiveness
explanation is further supported by the fact that this test-derogation effect was particularly pronounced in the condition where
the subjects should have been the most anxious about the test's
results: the deficiency-present, low-prevalence condition. A serendipitous and more anecdotal finding also suggests that the deficiency-present subjects resisted the diagnosis of TAA deficiency.
Only two subjects misinterpreted the deficiency-present versus
deficiency-absent manipulation; both had been assigned to the
deficiency-present condition. Their misinterpretation of the test's
result, in effect, permitted them to deny having the enzyme deficiency.6
An intriguing question raised by our findings is why a health
disorder is judged as more serious if it is perceived to be relatively
low in prevalence. It may occur because people reason that serious
prevalent disorders are precisely the ones that medical science
seeks to eradicate, which decreases the probability that a prevalent
disorder is serious.
903
Another possibility is that through their experiences with disorders people come to perceive an illusory correlation between
illness prevalence and seriousness as a result of paired distinctiveness. An illusory correlation (Chapman, 1967) is the perception of a relation between two classes of events that is greater
than warranted by the facts. Chapman (1967) found that experience with word pairs led subjects to overestimate the number
of times distinctive (i.e., longer) words were paired. Later, Hamilton and GifFord (1976) found that exposure to descriptions of
the behavior of members of different groups led to an overestimation of the number of times the distinctive (i.e., less frequent)
behavior occurred among members of the distinctive (i.e.,
smaller) group. By definition, low-prevalence disorders are distinctive; they occur relatively infrequently. Very serious disorders,
too, are distinctive by virtue of their above average seriousness.
Experience with disorders varying in prevalence and seriousness
might lead to an illusory correlation, an overestimation of the
number of disorders that are both serious and low in prevalence.
Furthermore, the perceived correlation might be reinforced by
media attention to rare serious disorders instead of rare benign
disorders (Combs & Slovic, 1979).
It is also possible, of course, that the subjects accurately perceived a true relation between the seriousness and prevalence of
disorders. This account is not incompatible with the others. It
should be emphasized, however, that the possible existence of a
true relation does not alter the fact that two individuals who
have the identical health disorder can arrive at substantially different judgments of that disorder simply because of their different
beliefs about its prevalence.
The results of the present experiment may have implications
for research on evaluations of personal characteristics more generally. Stigma researchers, for instance, might do well to investigate how a stigma is perceived by stigmatized individuals. Such
individuals may hold a more benign view of their stigma than
other people. In addition, stigmatized individuals may evaluate
their stigma as more negative if they believe it is scarce than if
they believe it is highly prevalent. Finally, when viewed in light
of the commodity theory maxim that positively evaluated objects
increase in valuation according to their scarcity, the present results suggest the existence of a more general rarity-evaluative5
In fact, unpublished data (Ditto & Jemmott, 1984) supports this
explanation. In a study using the TAA deficiency paradigm deficiencypresent, low-prevalence subjects were significantly (p < .02) more likely
to request a booklet entitled "TAA Deficiency: It Really Can Be Treated"
than were deficiency-present, high-prevalence subjects.
6
One might argue that the deficiency-present subjects minimized the
seriousness of TAA deficiency and the accuracy of the diagnostic test
because they were unable to recall having symptoms consistent with the
diagnosis. However, this explanation would not predict that the deficiencypresent, low-prevalence group would rate the test's accuracy lower than
would the other groups. Second, none of the information the subjects
received about TAA indicated that recent health experience was pertinent.
In fact, they were told the deficiency might affect their future health, that
it might increase the likelihood of their developing pancreatic disorders.
Third, other research suggests that even if the deficiency-present subjects
took their recent health into account, they probably found symptom
evidence to confirm rather than refute the diagnosis of TAA deficiency
(Croyle & Sande, 1985; Leventhal, Meyer, & Nerenz, 1980; Leventhal,
Nerenz, & Steele, 1984).
904
J. JEMMOTT, P. DITTO, AND R. CROYLE
extremity bias such that rare characteristics, whether negative
or positive, are evaluated extremely (Jemmott & Ditto, 1984).
Just as an undesirable characteristic, such as a health disorder,
is seen as less desirable if its prevalence is perceived to be low, a
desirable characteristic may be seen as even more desirable if its
prevalence is perceived to be low. This view is consistent with a
common theme in social psychology expressed in the notion of
a "big frog in a small pond" and in social evaluation theories,
including social comparison theory (Davis, 1966; Festinger, 1954;
Pettigrew, 1967). The basic idea is that self evaluations involve
not only objective assessments of the self, but also comparative
assessments of the self in relation to others. What is emphasized
in this article is the prevalence of a characteristic among those
comparison others.
In conclusion, the present research suggests that people who
perceive that a disorder has a relatively low prevalence evaluate
that disorder as more serious. In addition, those for whom a
disorder has relatively great personal relevance judge it to be less
serious, and this seems due to defensiveness. More generally, this
research also suggests that concepts and methods from experimental social psychology can be fruitfully applied to the study
of health-related judgments.
References
Barsky, A. J., & Klerman, G. L. (1983). Overview: Hypochondriasis,
bodily complaints and somatic styles. American Journal of Psychiatry,
140, 273-283.
Becker, M. H. (1979). Psychosocial aspects of health-related behavior. In
H. E. Freeman, S. Levine, & L. G. Reeder (Eds.), Handbook of medical
sociology (pp. 253-274). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Breznitz, S. (1983). The denial of stress. New York: International Universities Press.
Brock, T. C. (1968). Implications of commodity theory for value change.
In A. G. Greenwald, X C. Brock, & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological
foundations of attitudes (pp. 243-276). New York: Academic Press.
Brodman, K., Erdmann, A. J., Jr., & Wolff, H. (1949). The Cornell Medical
Index: An adjunct to medical interviews. Journal of the American
Medical Association, 140, 530-534.
Burger, J. M. (1981). Motivational biases in the attribution of responsibility
for an accident: A meta-analysis of the defensive attribution hypothesis.
Psychological Bulletin, 90, 496-512.
Campbell, J. D. (1975). Attribution of illness: Another double standard.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 16, 114-126.
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing
and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752-766.
Chapman, L. J. (1967). Illusory correlation in observational report.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 151-155.
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlation
analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen, F., & Lazarus, R. S. (1979). Coping with the stresses of illness.
In G. C. Stone, F. Cohen, & N. E. Adler (Eds.), Health psychology: A
handbook (pp. 217-254). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Combs, B., & Slovic, P. (1979). Causes of death: Biased newspaper coverage
and biased judgments. Journalism Quarterly, 56, 837-843, 849.
Croyle, R. T., & Sande, G. N. (1985). Denial and confirmatory search:
Paradoxical consequences of medical diagnoses. Unpublished manuscript.
D'Andrade, R. G., Quinn, N. R., Nerlove, S. B., & Romney, A. K. (1972).
Categories of disease in American-English and Mexican-Spanish. In
A. K. Romney, R. N. Shepard, & S. B. Nerlove (Eds.), Multidimensional
scaling: Theory and applications in the behavioral Sciences (Vol. 2, pp.
9-54). New York: Seminar Press.
Davis, J. (1966). The campus as a frog pond. An application of the theory
of relative deprivation to career decisions of college men. American
Journal of Sociology, 72, 17-31.
Ditto, P. H., & Jemmott, J. B. III. (1984). [Valence, prevalence, and
reactions to a medical condition: Information-seeking scores]. Unpublished raw data.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human
Relations, 1, 114-140.
Freidson, E. (1970). Profession of medicine. New \brk: Harper & Row.
Fromkin, H. L., & Snyder, C. R. (1980). The search for uniqueness and
valuation of scarcity. In K. J. Gergen, M. S. Greenberg, & R. H. Willis
(Eds.), Social exchange (pp. 57-76). New York: Plenum.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Hamilton, D. L., & Gifford, R. K. (1976). Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 392-407.
Janis, I. L. (1958). Psychological stress. New York: Wiley.
Jemmott, J. B. Ill, Croyle, R. T, & Ditto, P. (1984, May). Subjective
judgments of illness prevalence and seriousness. Paper presented at the
Annual Meeting of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, Philadelphia,
PA.
Jemmott, J. B. Ill, & Ditto, P. (1984, August). Rarity and evaluative
extremity: From illness prevalence to illness seriousness. Paper presented
at The Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. (1978). Control of attributions about the self
through self-handicapping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and the
role of underachievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
4, 200-206.
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219-266). New \brk:
Academic Press.
Jones, E. E., Farina, A., Hastorf, A. H., Markus, H., Miller, D. T., &
Scott, R. A. (1984). Social Stigma: The psychology of marked relationships. New York: Freeman.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment
of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430-454.
Katz, I. (1981). Stigma: A social psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Katz, J. L., Weiner, H., Gallagher, T. G., & Hellman, L. (1970). Stress,
distress, and ego defenses. Archives of General Psychiatry, 23, 131142.
Kelley, H. H. (1968). Two functions of reference groups. In H. H. Hyman
& E. Singer (Eds.), Readings in reference group theory and research
(pp. 77-83). New York: Free Press.
Kubler-Ross, E. (1969). On death and dying. New York: MacMillan.
Leventhal, H. (1970). Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 119-186). New \brk: Academic Press.
Leventhal, H., Meyer, D., & Nerenz, D. (1980). The common sense representation of illness danger. In S. Rachman (Ed.), Contributions to
medical psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 7-30). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.
Leventhal, H., Nerenz, D. R., & Steele, D. J. (1984). Illness representations
and coping with health threats. In A. Baum, S. E. Taylor, & J. E. Singer
(Eds.), Handbook of psychology and health (Vol. 4, pp. 219-252).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Linz, D., Penrod, S., Siverhus, S., & Leventhal, H. (1982). The cognitive
organization of disease and illness among lay persons. Unpublished
manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin,
Madison, WI.
MacArthur, L. Z. (1981). What grabs you? The role of attention in
impression formation and causal attribution. In E. T. Higgins, C. P.
Herman, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 1, pp. 201-246). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
JUDGING HEALTH STATUS
Pettigrew, T. F. (1967). Social evaluation theory: Convergencies and applications. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation
(Vol. 15, pp. 241-311). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. (1979). Issue involvement can increase or
decrease persuasion by enhancing message-relevant cognitive responses.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1915-1926.
Pilowsky, I. (1967). Dimensions of hypochondriasis. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 113, 89-93.
Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals
and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In
J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.), Socialpsychophysiology: A sourcebook (pp. 153-176). New York: Guilford.
Rosenstock, I. M., & Kirscht, J. P. (1979). Why people seek health care.
In G. C. Stone, F. Cohen, & N. E. Adler (Eds.), Health psychology (pp.
161-188). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self
perception and social perception processes. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 32, 880-892.
Safer, M. A., Tharps, Q. J., Jackson, T. C., & Leventhal, H. (1979). Determinants of three stages of delay in seeking care at a medical clinic.
Medical Care, 17, 11-29.
Sanders, G. S. (1982). Social comparison and perceptions of health and
illness. In G. S. Sanders & J. Suls (Eds.), Social psychology of health
and illness (pp. 129-160). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
905
Stedman, T. L. (1972). Stedman's medical dictionary (22nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins.
Suchman, E. A. (1965). Stages of illness and medical care. Journal of
Health and Social Behavior, 10, 105-114.
Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T, Etcoff, N., & Ruderman, A. (1978). The categorial
and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 778-793.
Twaddle, A. C. (1969). Health decisions and sick role variations: An
exploration. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 10, 105-114.
Visotsky, H. M., Hamburg, D. A., Goss, M. E., & Lebovitz, B. A. (1961).
Coping under extreme stress: Observations of patients with severe poliomyelitis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 5, 423-448.
Weinstein, N. (1982). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health
problems. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 5, 441-460.
Zola, I. K. (1966). Culture and symptoms—an analysis of patients' presenting complaints. American Sociological Review, 31, 615-630.
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Attributions of success and failure revisited, or:
The motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory. Journal
of Personality, 47, 245-287.
Received February 21, 1985
Revision received November 27, 1985
Low Publication Prices for APA Members and Affiliates
Keeping You Up-to-Date
All APA members (Fellows, Members, and Associates) receive — as part of their annual dues — subscriptions to
the American Psychologist, the APA Monitor, and Psychology Today.
High School Teacher and Student Affiliates receive subscriptions to the APA Monitor and Psychology Today, and
they can subscribe to the American Psychologist at a significantly reduced rate.
In addition, all members and affiliates are eligible for savings of up to 50% on other APA journals, as well as
significant discounts on subscriptions from cooperating societies and publishers (e.g.. the British Psychological
Society, the American Sociological Association, and Human Sciences Press).
Essential Resources
APA members and affiliates receive special rates for purchases of APA books, including the Publication
Manual of the APA, the Master Lectures, and APA's Guide to Research Support.
Other Benefits of Membership
Membership in APA also provides eligibility for low-cost insurance plans covering life; medical and income
protection; hospital indemnity; accident and travel; Keogh retirement; office overhead; and student/school,
professional, and liability.
For more information, write to American Psychological Association,
Membership Records, 1200 Seventeenth Street NW, Washington, DC 20036
Download