Law and Human Behavior, Vol 22, No. 1, 1998 Female Juvenile Delinquency: Misunderstood by the Juvenile Justice System, Neglected by Social Science Stephanie Hoyt1 and David G. Scherer2,3 The study of juvenile delinquency has focused primarily on conduct disorder and aggression in males, while relatively little attention has been paid to females who commit delinquent acts. This article offers a critical review of the existing theories of and research on female delinquency and the juvenile justice system's response to female delinquency. The inadequacies and persistence of historical theories and the conceptual and methodological strengths and weaknesses of contemporary perspectives in female delinquency are reviewed. Understanding and treatment of female and male delinquency could be enhanced through the adoption of a gender-integrated theory of delinquency that is informed by the comprehensive study of developmental, psychological, and social-ecological determinants. Delinquency and youth violence have been growing by epidemic proportions. From 1983 to 1993, juvenile murder and manslaughter arrests leaped 128% (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 1993) and from 1986 to 1995, violent crime arrests among juveniles rose 67% (Snyder, 1996). This escalation in violent crime by adolescents has raised concerns about the number of juveniles victimized by youth violence, prompted projections of a doubling in juvenile arrests for violent crime by 2010, and fueled anxieties about a future crime wave as these juvenile delinquents mature into adults (Snyder & Sickmund, 1995; Snyder, Sickmund, & Poe-Yamagata, 1996). Until recently, the research and theories of social scientists and the attention of policy makers and the public have centered on male delinquency (Chamberlain & Reid, 1994; Hirschi, 1969; Zoccolillo, 1993). However, the issue of female delinquency is forcing its way into social awareness. From 1983 to 1992, arrests of female adolescents rose over 25% (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 1993), and in 1994 and 1995 girls accounted for one fourth of all juvenile arrests (Girls Incorporated, 1996; Snyder, 1996; Snyder et al., 1996). Moreover, from 1985 to 1994, arrests of females for violent offenses more than doubled (Girls Incorporated, 1996). Fur1 University of South Carolina. 2University of New Mexico. 3Correspondence should be addressed to David Scherer, Ph.D., 114 Simpson Hall, College of Education, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131 (e-mail: scherer@unm.edu). 81 0147-7307/98/0200-0081J15.00/1 © 1998 American Psychology-Law Society/Division 41 of the American Psychological Association 82 Hoyt and Scherer thermore, epidemiological studies have found that conduct disorder (diagnostically akin to juvenile delinquency) is among the most common psychiatric diagnoses for girls, has a similar prevalence rate in females and males by adolescence, and is predictive of poor long-term psychological outcomes for females (Chamberlain & Reid, 1994; Dembo, Williams, & Schmeidler, 1993; Webster-Stratton, 1996; Zoccolillo, 1993). Despite growing concern, relatively little is known empirically about female delinquency (Calhoun, Jurgens, & Chen, 1993). Social scientists have excluded females from their studies of delinquents, apparently suspecting that delinquency among females is simply a subset or minor variation of delinquency among males (Figueira-McDonough, 1992; Rhodes & Fischer, 1993). What literature exists on female delinquency betrays a variety of biases and fallibilities (Girls Incorporated, 1996). Moreover, these biases in the study of delinquency also appear to be reflected in the applications of juvenile justice (Bergsmann, 1989; Girls Incorporated, 1996). This review examines these themes. We begin with a look at the precedents established by the nascent studies of female delinquency and how the predilections that emerged from this literature have been paralleled in juvenile justice practices toward females. In the second section, we chronicle contemporary efforts to understand the phenomena of female delinquency by surveying some of the more prominent theses that have emerged from research on female delinquents. These topics span the range of human ecology from individual factors (e.g., developmental considerations, history of victimization), to the effects of social context (e.g., family, peer, and academic correlates of female delinquency) and larger social environmental considerations (e.g., socioeconomic status and opportunity, the impact of the women's movement). We conclude that, without renewed effort from social scientists, female delinquents are apt to continue to encounter misguided justice and intervention. FEMALE DELINQUENCY, SEXUALITY, AND THE DOUBLE STANDARD The Stigma of Female Delinquency Early theories of female delinquency emphasized sexuality as either the cause or expression of females' delinquent behavior (Lombroso & Ferrero, 1895; Thomas, 1928). For example, the "deceitful" nature of women (derived from practice at faking sexual arousal), along with precocious biological maturity, were proposed to account for female delinquency (Pollak, 1950; Pollak & Freidman, 1969). By the 1960s, researchers were documenting the prevalence of "sex delinquencies," vague status-type offenses like incorrigibility, or simply "needing care and protection" as the major factors distinguishing female from male delinquents (Cowie, Cowie, & Slater, 1968; Konopka, 1966; Vedder & Somerville, 1970). Cowie et al. noted that many of the girls in their study claimed they ran away from home because of sexual advances by relatives, but they did not examine this finding for its implication for explaining female delinquency. Vedder and Somerville accounted for reports like Female Juvenile 83 these by suggesting that adolescent females ran away from home because they feared the incestuous consequences of their sexual impulses. Konopka also attributed the sometimes harsh treatment that female delinquents received to the agitation and ambivalence engendered in others by female delinquents' sexual behavior. Differential Treatment of Male and Female Delinquents Within the Juvenile Justice System As a consequence of these early studies that assumed that female delinquency was sexual in nature, female delinquency acquired a stigma not associated with male delinquency. Historically, female delinquents were charged with immorality, waywardness, or status offenses (i.e., offenses such as running away, truancy, or underage drinking, that are illegal for juveniles, but not adults) because of concern about their sexual behavior and moral depravity (Schlossman & Wallach, 1978; Shelden, 1981), and subsequently treated more harshly than males who committed the same or similar crimes (Chesney-Lind, 1973; Datesman & Scarpitti, 1977; Shelden, 1981). This led some to conclude that a sexual double standard existed in the juvenile justice system (Chesney-Lind, 1977; Pope & Feyerherm, 1982). Over the past 15 years, the question of a double standard in the administration of juvenile justice for females and males has become more complex. There are considerable gender differences in "official" delinquency, the picture of delinquency derived from statistics maintained by law enforcement agencies. Most notably, adolescent females are arrested less frequently than male adolescents, and are more likely than males to have their cases handled informally rather than through formal adjudication hearings. Females arrested for delinquency and formally petitioned to court are less likely to be adjudicated and are less likely to be remanded to adult courts than males (Girls Incorporated, 1996; Poe-Yamagata & Butts, 1996). However, female adolescents are arrested and involved in the juvenile justice system for less serious offenses more often than male adolescents (Girls Incorporated, 1996; Poe-Yamagata & Butts, 1996). One interpretation of these data is that female delinquents are treated more leniently than male delinquents. Others have argued, however, that the juvenile justice system continues to discriminate against female delinquents, holding them more accountable than males for the commission of status offenses (Girls Incorporated, 1996). The perception of a negative bias toward arresting female adolescents more often for less serious offenses is accented by self-report delinquency data which indicate that females and males commit similar rates of status offenses and minor index crimes (e.g., larceny-theft, vandalism, and prostitution) (Canter, 1982a; Kruttschnitt, 1996; Steffensmeier & Steffensmeier, 1980). The juvenile justice system's commitment to the parens patrie concept and the language of status offense provisions allow discretionary application by law enforcement and juvenile court personnel to hold females legally accountable for behavior (frequently behavior interpreted as sexual acting out) that would not be considered serious if committed by males (Rosenbaum & Chesney-Lind, 1994; Sussman, 1977; Teitelbaum & Gough, 1977). This has raised the specter that the juvenile justice system has evidenced a 84 Hoyt and Scherer concern for controlling the sexuality of females not seen in the handling of male delinquents (Bergsmann, 1989; Krohn, Curry, & Nelson-Kilger, 1983; Rosenbaum & Chesney-Lind, 1994). The disparity between females and males in arrest and referral rates for status offenses continues to be found in studies of delinquents (Dembo et al., 1993; Girls Incorporated, 1996; Hancock & Chesney-Lind, 1982; Horowitz & Pottieger, 1991; Rhodes & Fischer, 1993; Rosenbaum & Chesney-Lind, 1994; Smith, 1980). Over the past 15 years, the procedure of "bootstrapping" (the rearrest and detention of previously adjudicated minors upon a violation of a court order) has raised particular concern. Bootstrapping, some contend, has resulted in disproportionately more females being adjudicated and incarcerated for status offenses or violation of non-offense conditions of the original court order (Girls Incorporated, 1996). In fact, much like the case with delinquency crimes, the arrest and incarceration rates of female delinquents for status offenses has been rising (Bergsmann, 1989; Rosenbaum & Chesney-Lind, 1994), and the rates of growth of female petitioned status offenses exceeds that of males (Butts, Snyder, Finnegan, Augenbaugh, & Poole, 1996). In addition to bias in arresting female delinquents, the juvenile justice system also has been accused of bias against female delinquents in determining their disposition (Girls Incorporated, 1996). The evidence is ambiguous. On the one hand, some social scientists contend that adolescent females have a greater possibility of being incarcerated for less serious delinquent activity than males (Bergsmann, 1989; Chesney-Lind, 1977, 1988; Horowitz & Pottieger, 1991; Rhodes & Fischer, 1993; Schwartz, Steketee, & Schneider, 1990). For example, the decision to process female delinquents informally or to not adjudicate them when formally referred to court has not led necessarily to less serious or less restrictive dispositions (Girls Incorporated, 1996). Following the Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention Act of 1974, which emphasized diversion and deinstitutionalization of juvenile delinquents, the adjudication and rate of incarceration of juvenile delinquents for status offenses declined. This result chiefly affected the adjudication and incarceration of female delinquents. There is evidence, however, that these delinquents were merely redirected or shifted from public correctional facilities to confinement in private treatment centers (Bergsmann, 1989; Girls Incorporated, 1996; Weithorn, 1988). This incarceration occurred absent even the limited access juveniles have to due process and under conditions that cast doubt on the voluntarinesss of their consent. Another consideration is that female status offenders are much more often referred to juvenile court for running away, while male status offenders are more often referred for liquor law violations. As a result, female status offenders more often received formal probation than male status offenders, who more often received less formal judicial dispositions such as fines and referrals to counseling (Butts et al., 1996). However, from 1989 to 1993, the percentages of female and male status offenders detained and subsequently placed out-of-home for status offenses were quite comparable. Consequently, other researchers assert that when the type of offense and a variety of additional covariates are accounted for in analyses of the incarceration of female delinquents, females receive lenient treatment for the commission of less serious offenses and that no differences exist in adjudication of females and males Female Juvenile 85 for serious crimes against persons (D. R. Johnson & Scheuble, 1991; Kruttschnitt, 1996). However, in maintaining a narrow, status-offense, sexuality-dominated perspective on female delinquents, the juvenile justice system is missing the larger picture of serious offending committed by female delinquents. The largest gender differences in both official and self-report delinquency occur for violent and serious index crimes (Kruttschnitt, 1996). Though official delinquency rates indicate that many more males than females are arrested for both offenses against persons and property offenses, the percentages of adolescent females arrested for these crimes have been increasing dramatically and at greater rates than males (Poe-Yamagata & Butts, 1996). For example, between 1989 and 1993, the growth rate of female juvenile arrests was more than twice the growth rate in male juvenile arrests (PoeYamagata & Butts, 1996). From 1985 to 1994, violent crime arrests have increased 125% for females compared to 67% for males, mostly due to arrests for robbery and aggravated assault (male juveniles continue to exceed female juveniles in growth rates for homicide arrests) (Poe-Yamagata & Butts, 1996; Snyder & Sickmund, 1995; Snyder et al., 1996). Property crime arrest rates for female juveniles continue to increase (Snyder & Sickmund, 1995; Snyder et al., 1996). From 1989 to 1993, female arrests for property crimes climbed 22%, while arrests of males for property crimes dropped 3% (Poe-Yamagata & Butts, 1996). Even for most categories of non-index offenses the growth rate of female juvenile arrests exceeds that of male juvenile arrests (Poe-Yamagata & Butts, 1996). Because the juvenile justice system is oriented toward coping with female status offenders, it quite possibly may be employing ineffective or inappropriate correctional practices (Horowitz & Pottieger, 1991). Treatment interventions for delinquent females are distinctly lacking and have fallen behind the development of intervention programs for males and adult females (Bergsmann, 1989). Calls to intervene with the communities, families, and mental health needs of delinquent females have a long history (Vedder & Somerville, 1970) and continue to be the focus of entreaties on behalf of delinquent females (Bergsmann, 1989; Dembo et al., 1993). Yet, the juvenile justice system has failed to provide services tailored for the needs of female delinquents. When intervention programs are available, incarcerated female delinquents are apt to receive intervention focused on acting-out conduct rather than the internalizing concerns that are potentially more salient (Bergsmann, 1989; Chamberlain & Reid, 1994). Furthermore, those female delinquents who are removed from their homes and placed in residential treatment, foster care, or group homes also may not be receiving appropriate treatment. Chamberlain and Reid (1994) reported that female delinquents respond to therapeutic placement quite differently than males. While males generally responded to Treatment Foster Care with steady improvement after a troubled start, female delinquents appear to begin quietly and then express more problem behaviors, theoretically after gaining some measure of comfort or intimacy in foster care. Consequently, the course and strategy of treatment used for male delinquents in outof-home placements may not be effective for female delinquents. In sum, the original theories about the sexual etiology of female delinquency were stigmatizing and led to stereotyping of female delinquents. This stereotyping 86 Hoyt and Scherer of female delinquents has been reflected in the practices of the juvenile justice system, which, in spite of reform efforts, still is accused of unfair bias by handling female delinquents more harshly than male delinquents. Absent more informed social science input, juvenile justice is liable to continue to disregard the unique needs of female delinquents. Over the past 15 years, a variety of correlates, covariates, and theories of female delinquency have been empirically explored. However, the current state of the science of female delinquency is rife with methodological complications and theoretical ambiguity. CONTEMPORARY PERSPECTIVES ON FEMALE DELINQUENCY Models of female delinquency that generally emphasized status offenses and female sexuality may never have been accurate and certainly do not account for the full range of delinquent behavior exhibited by contemporary adolescent females. Only recently have social scientists attempted to correct common misconceptions about the nature of female delinquency by emphasizing developmental and socioecological determinants of female delinquency. In doing so, they have encountered considerable uncertainty about how to proceed theoretically and empirically with studies of female delinquency. Many investigations reflect popular misconceptions and stereotypes about females and their lives. Others assume "gender-sameness" and attempt to understand female delinquency by applying the same concepts that dominate research on male delinquency. Still others have argued that female delinquency is a unique phenomenon requiring separate constructs and hypothesizing unique causal paths to the development of female delinquency. As a consequence, basic issues such as whether to use cross-gender analyses or same-sex comparisons are the subject of considerable debate. Although empirical interest in female delinquency has grown in recent years (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992; Chesney-Lind & Sheldon, 1992; Rosenbaum & Lashley, 1990; Seydlitz, 1991), this research has suffered from the basic limitations imposed by biased sampling, inadequate sample descriptions, measurement inadequacies, and design restrictions. For example, studies of female delinquency have sampled Caucasians primarily, despite concerns about delinquency in minority populations, have omitted certain types of family structures such as mother-only families (e.g., Brown, 1984; Henggeler, Edwards, & Borduin, 1987), and have neglected chronic offenders (Cernkovich, Giordano, & Pugh, 1985). Often, researchers have failed to specify the actual numbers of females and males in the sample (e.g., Rankin, 1980; Shover, Norland, James, & Thorton, 1979). Some studies (e.g., Seydlitz, 1991) carried out secondary analyses on regional or national surveys like the Seattle Youth Study (Hindelang, Hirschi, & Weis, 1981) or the National Youth Survey (Elliott, 1993) confining what variables could be studied to those collected in the original survey. Some of these data sets were collected in the early 1970s and may not reflect more contemporary trends in female delinquency. Moreover, several studies have used the same data set repeatedly in attempts to test modifications of a theory (e.g., Hagan, Simpson, & Gillis, 1987; R. Johnson, 1986). Female Juvenile 87 Problems with measurement instruments also have hindered research on female delinquency. Reliance on self-report delinquency inventories has been criticized because they underrepresent truly serious offenses, while overrepresenting relatively minor offenses, typically inquire about behaviors that are not sufficiently described, and aggregate high-frequency offenders in the same category in spite of the severity of criminal conduct (Cernkovich et al, 1985). The lack of standardization in self-report measures used to assess various correlates of delinquency has resulted in researchers using one-item measures and idiosyncratic definitions and constructs, and failing to capture the dynamic and multidimensional nature of female delinquency. Few systematic attempts have been made to design research or collect data specifically on female delinquents. Research in the field of delinquency has been dominated by cross-sectional designs that provide information about correlates of offending, but offer little firm evidence about the causes of offending. Moreover, statistical analyses that are sensitive to prevalence, such as correlations, may yield lower effect sizes because of the relatively low base rate of female offending. Consequently, while many studies have shown relationships between certain variables and female delinquency, few are able to account for any significant amount of variation in delinquent behavior. In spite of these methodological complications, several topics have stood out in contemporary theories and research on female delinquents. These themes span the range of biopsychosocial variables from individual traits to larger socio-ecological influences. For example, research on the developmental characteristics of conduct-disorder and delinquency has begun to emerge. An empirical literature on the effects of abuse and sexual victimization and their relationship to female delinquency has been developing. Promising research has addressed how family factors and peer influences play a role in the etiology and maintenance of female delinquency. Academic correlates of female delinquency have been examined along with the significance of broader sociological considerations such as socioeconomic status and opportunity. Even the influence of the women's movement has been studied. The following is a brief review of each of these influences. Table 1 provides summary information on key studies for the contemporary perspectives discussed below. The studies included in the table were selected because they provided data about factors contributing to the etiology of female delinquency or tested a theoretical perspective on female delinquency. Studies (regardless of sample size and type of participants) were included in the table if their designs were either quasiexperimental or longitudinal. While several crosssectional studies are mentioned in the text, only those cross-sectional studies that tested a model of female delinquency versus cross-sectional studies that are descriptive in nature are included in the table. These studies represent the vanguard in efforts to understand empirically the phenomenon of female delinquency and pave the way for a more comprehensive understanding of female delinquency in the future. 88 Hoyt and Scherer Female Juvenile 89 90 Hoyt and Scherer Female Juvenile 91 92 Hoyt and Scherer Female Juvenile 93 94 Hoyt and Scherer Developmental Correlates of Female Delinquency Delinquency researchers often contend that the early predictors of male conduct disorders and delinquency are not as effective when applied to females (Chamberlain & Reid, 1994; Stattin & Magnusson, 1989; Tremblay et al., 1992; Zoccolillo, 1993). The childhood externalizing behaviors, conduct problems, and aggressiveness that predict subsequent delinquency for males do not seem to manifest themselves in female delinquents until adolescence (Robins, 1986; Stattin & Magnusson, 1989; Tremblay et al., 1992; J. L. White, Moffitt, & Silva, 1989). However, others argue that the quantitative difference between boys' and girls' aggression is exaggerated and that girls' aggression is expressed qualitatively differently than boys' aggression (Hamalainen & Pulkkinen, 1995; Krueger et al., 1994; Webster-Stratton, 1996; J. W. White & Kowalski, 1994). Supporting this view are Hamalainen and Pulkkinen's (1995) findings that lower prosocial behavior and higher bullying-type aggression in girls of age 8 years correlated with criminality in adult women. Research supporting both perspectives (i.e., Hamalainen & Pulkkinen, 1995; Tremblay et al., 1992; Webster-Stratton, 1996) has employed longitudinal or quasiexperimental designs, sampled large populations, and included male participants as a comparison group. Hamalainen and Pulkkinen (1995) and Webster-Stratton (1996) collected data from multiple informants which provides a more comprehensive picture and adds credibility to the contention that the differences between boys' and girls' aggression are not so great. However, the issue of the early predictors of female delinquency is most likely a much more complex issue. For example, the research of Caspi, Lynam, Moffitt, and Silva (1993) suggests that interactions between biological factors, such as age of menarche, and social context variables, such as enrollment in a mixed-sex or all-girl school, account for familiarity with delinquent peers, norm-violating behavior, and the incidence of female delinquency. For each of these studies (Caspi et al., 1993; Hamalainen & Pulkkinen, 1995; Tremblay et al., 1992; Webster-Stratton, 1996), the reliance on either self-reported delinquency or official delinquency statistics, rather than analyzing data from both sources, is a shortcoming. It hinders efforts to compare the results of the studies and limits our picture of female delinquency, which varies depending on the source used to quantify delinquency. Female Delinquency and Sexual Abuse The impact of sexual abuse has been absent from formal theories on female delinquency (Gray, 1988), despite empirical studies that have found significant correlations between delinquency and female victimization (e.g., Simons & Whitbeck, 1991). This may be, in part, because research on the sequelae of child maltreatment, in general, is problematic and limited by poor sampling procedures, descriptive rather than inferential statistics, and reliance on official statistics (Dembo et al., 1987). However, some studies have provided a general picture of the link between sexual abuse and female delinquency. For example, females arrested for ungovernability and running away frequently have cited family violence (Koroki & Ches- Female Juvenile 95 ney-Lind, 1985; McCormack, Janus, & Burgess, 1986; Rush, 1980) and being emotionally, physically, and sexually abused (McCormack et al., 1986; Rhodes & Fischer, 1993; Silbert & Pines, 1981) as their motivation for leaving home. Sexually abused female runaways reported more trouble with school officials, employers, and the law, and participation in more acts of physical violence than nonabused female runaways. Running away often leads these young women to commit a variety of crimes, such as shoplifting and prostitution, to survive (McCormack et al., 1986; Phelps, Mclntosh, Jesudason, Warner, & Pohlkamp, 1982). Many adult and adolescent females report that sexual exploitation affected their eventual entrance into prostitution (Silbert & Pines, 1981). Similarly, sexual and physical abuse have been determined to have significant direct effects on lifetime drug use for adolescents, which, in turn, is correlated with increased delinquency for both females and males (Dembo et al., 1989; Dembo et al., 1987; Snyder & Sickmund, 1995). Unfortunately, research linking female delinquency with sexual abuse of adolescent females has been descriptive and plagued with methodological problems, such as a lack of consensus on how to define abuse, poor sampling procedures, and a reliance on official delinquency statistics only (Brown, 1984; Dembo et al., 1987). Research comparing females and males on the association between sexual abuse and delinquency is similarly undeveloped. A self-reported history of having been sexually abused has been associated with comparatively more externalizing behaviors, including delinquency, for males, and comparatively more internalizing behaviors (e.g., suicidality, eating disorders) for females (Chandy, Blum, & Resnick, 1996). However, absent more conclusive research, it is not clear whether sexual abuse, in particular, leads to increased participation in delinquent behavior, or whether other factors, such as destructive parenting in general, are responsible for the appearance of a relationship between sexual abuse and delinquency. Family Factors and Female Delinquency Family correlates of delinquency have taken on special importance in explaining female delinquency because of the presumption that adolescent females evince less autonomy in families. Consequently, female delinquents are thought to be more subject to the effects of family conditions, although the validity of these presumptions is far from certain (Barton & Figueria-McDonough, 1985; Canter, 1982b; Cernkovich & Giordano, 1987; Henggeler et al., 1987; R. Johnson, 1986; Kroupa, 1988; Seydlitz, 1991). Numerous family factors have been correlated with female delinquency (Rosenbaum, 1989), with a greater focus on structural components of the family (e.g., family composition and size, the presence of parental criminality) than family process variables that measure the quality of interaction between family members (e.g., nurturing, parental supervision and monitoring). One common assumption has been that a "broken" home is a major factor in the causation of juvenile delinquency or at least the commission of status offenses (Rosenbaum, 1989; Van Vooris, Cullen, Mathers, & Garner, 1988; Wells & Rankin, 1991; Wright & Wright, 1994). This view was supported by official delinquency statistics, which generally indicated that adolescents from single-parent homes were 96 Hoyt and Scberer more likely to be arrested (Datesman & Scarpitti, 1975; Wells & Rankin, 1991). Studies using self-reported delinquency statistics typically have not found strong evidence of this relationship for females or males (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1987; R. Johnson, 1986; see Canter, 1982a, for contrary findings). Wells and Rankin (1991) concluded from their meta-analytic review of the research that coming from a "broken home" or single-parent family has a modest effect (phi = .05 to .15) on the prevalence of delinquency for both females and males. Moreover, they concluded there are few detectable gender differences in the relationship between coming from a "broken home" and juvenile delinquency. However, delinquents' reports of contacts with police have been correlated with gender and family structure. Specifically, police authorities have been more likely to intervene in the lives of girls from single-mother homes than they have been to intervene in the lives of girls from intact homes (R. Johnson, 1986). The correlation between "broken homes" and delinquency requires a more complex explanation than simply attributing it to the structural constitution of the family. A single-parent family can occur in a variety of ways, some of which may be troubling, others of which may constitute a relief from chronic distress (Wells & Rankin, 1991; Wright & Wright, 1994). Consequently, the structure of the family is likely to have a more indirect effect on delinquency than family process variables (e.g., multiple family disruptions, poor parental supervision and monitoring, marital discord, lack of economic opportunity) that are apt to have a more direct effect on the etiology of delinquency in both males and females. Exposure to family violence and parent criminality or antisocial personality also have been cited as risk factors for the development of conduct disorder and delinquency that vary depending on gender (Kruttschnitt, 1996). Indeed, females and males respond differently to being subjected to family violence, with females exhibiting less externalizing, aggressive, or violent behaviors (Everett & Scherer, in press). Parental criminality may also have a differential effect on daughters and sons, although it is unclear (especially with males) whether this is a consequence of a trait variable, like genetic factors or child temperament, or a parent-child interaction process such as ineffective parenting (Frick, Lahey, Loeber, StouthamerLoeber, Christ, & Hanson, 1992; Rosenbaum, 1989; Zoccolillo, 1993). However, the preliminary empirical evidence suggests that the impact of parental deviance on females is contingent on the degree of identification with the same-sex parent (Kruttschnitt, 1996), suggesting a more process versus trait explanation. Studies of family interactive processes have yielded important empirical observations on the impact of family functioning on female and male delinquency. Most of the following studies of the relationship between family functioning and delinquency (Barton & Figueria-McDonough, 1985; Canter, 1982a; Cernkovich & Giordano, 1987; R. Johnson, 1986; Seydlitz, 1991) have been conducted with robust samples and have used self-report delinquency data. The use of idiosyncratic measures of family functioning, however, has made it difficult to draw comparisons between the studies. Nonetheless a variety of conclusions stand out. In general, parenting style and discipline practices with young children are gender-differentiated (Mulhern & Passman, 1981; Starrels, 1994). However, there do not appear to be substantial gender differences in parenting styles and discipline Female Juvenile 97 of young children with conduct disorder, except that mothers tend to resort to more physical punishment with boys (Webster-Stratton, 1996). Consequently, parents of conduct-disordered girls (especially those with early onset of conduct disorder) may be using parenting practices with their daughters that are unlike those used by parents of daughters without conduct disorder (Webster-Stratton, 1996). With adolescents, the effect of parenting style varies depending on the age and gender of the juvenile. In general, parental influence and control have been found to have an inhibiting effect on delinquency. However, parental controls are more effective for females in later adolescence, whereas for males they are more effective in mid-adolescence (Seydlitz, 1991). Similarly, Cernkovich and Giordano (1987) concluded from their large factor analytic study that the family interaction patterns that were predictive of female delinquency were different from those predictive of male delinquency. Identity support (parental acceptance and respect for the youth), conflict with parents, and parental disapproval of peers were the strongest predictors of delinquency among females. The degree of parental supervision and monitoring and the quality of intimate and instrumental communication were the strongest predictors of delinquency for males. Furthermore, female delinquents apparently have a more negative view of their parents than do nondelinquent females (Kroupa, 1988). Henggeler et al. (1987), in a small sample study using official delinquency data, also found that delinquent females have more conflict in their relationships with their parents than similarly situated delinquent males. On the other hand, gender does not appear to be related to the strength of parental attachment, nor does family bonding and parental attachment appear to explain the differences between male and female offending (Barton & Figueria-McDonough, 1985; Canter, 1982a; Kruttschnitt, 1996). Both males and females report being closer to their mothers than to their fathers, although the degree of closeness to father has been shown to be a better predictor of delinquent behavior than attachment to mother (R. Johnson, 1987; Kroupa, 1988). In short, the emphasis on family structure in studies of female delinquency is unlikely to provide a detailed picture of the etiology of female delinquency. Problematic forms of family structure are frequently side effects of dysfunctional family processes. A more fruitful tactic for clarifying the family influences on the etiology of female delinquency lies in studying family functioning. Studies of delinquency in general have emphasized parenting style, family attachment patterns, and parental monitoring and supervision (Tolan, 1988). For female delinquents, the quality of parental influence and control seem to be key issues. In particular, there appear to be strong correlations between female delinquency and a lack of parental acceptance and a high degree of conflict with parents; however, it is uncertain whether this is a cause or effect of female delinquency. The Peer Context of Female Delinquency The delinquent orientation of peers has been related consistently to delinquency for both genders, although there appear to be distinct patterns of relation- 98 Hoyt and Scherer ship that differ in female and male delinquent peer groups (Campbell, 1987; Giordano, Cernkovich, & Pugh, 1986; Kruttschnitt, 1996; Morash, 1986). Giordano et al. (1986) and Morash (1986), in particular, conducted well-designed studies using large, stratified, community samples and self-reported delinquency as the outcome measure. On the basis of these studies, it appears that delinquent females are more likely than males to have engaged in intimate self-disclosure, to have characterized their friendships as caring and trusting, and to have reported significantly less conflict and imbalance in their relationships. Female peer groups engaged in less delinquency were less likely to have a regular meeting place outside of their homes and were less exclusive than males (Morash, 1986). These results cast doubt on the stereotype of the female delinquent as a lonely, social misfit (Short & Strodbeck, 1965) and the assumption that the gender difference in delinquency is due to the fact that females are not as peer-oriented as males (Giordano et al., 1986). In fact, peer relationships may mitigate delinquent behavior in females, in contrast to their amplifying affect in males (Giordano et al., 1986; Kruttschnitt, 1996). Similarly, female gangs appear to have developmental and functional processes that differ from male gangs. A female gang typically emerges after a male gang has been established and often takes a feminized version of the male name (Campbell, 1990). The stereotypical gang role for girls is to conceal and carry weapons for the boys, provide sexual favors, and sometimes fight against girls who are connected with enemy male gangs (Chesney-Lind & Shelden, 1992). The most consistent empirical research finding is that friendship and bonding between gang members (sometimes sealed through initiation rituals) is an important factor in accounting for female participation in gangs (Brown, 1977; Bowker & Klein, 1983; Giordano, 1978). Academic Correlates of Female Delinquency Academic difficulties have been one of the single best predictors of delinquent behavior, particularly for boys (Empey, 1982; Tremblay et al., 1992). For girls, the association is less clear, with some studies finding no significant correlation until adolescence (Stattin & Magnusson, 1989; Tremblay et al., 1992) and others reporting poor school achievement at age 8 years predicting later criminality in women (Hamalainen & Pulkkinen, 1995). How academic attitudes and success relate or contribute to female delinquency remains unclear even with the use of large, community samples (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992; Hamalainen & Pulkkinen, 1995; Rosenbaum & Lashley, 1990; Torstensson, 1990) and longitudinal data (Hamalainen & Pulkkinen, 1995; Torstensson, 1990). An array of ecological covariates including ethnicity and school composition, community context, socioeconomic status, and institutional bonding have complicated efforts to draw hard conclusions about the contribution of academic correlates to female delinquency. Researchers studying the academic correlates of female delinquency consistently have used strain and control theories as a foundation (Jarjoura, 1996; 'Iorstensson, 1990). Strain theory has predicted that unpleasant school experiences and school failures lead to the frustration of long-range ambitions, reducing students' Female Juvenile 99 commitment to conformity, and thus increasing the probability of delinquency. Theoretically, the salience of blocked ambitions and restricted vocational opportunities is greater for males and older adolescents than it is for females and younger adolescents (Cohen, 1955). However, empirical research on how age and gender affect the impact of blocked ambitions and restricted vocational opportunity on delinquency have been inconclusive (Rankin, 1980). Control theory has postulated that school failure and consequent delinquent behavior are due to a lack of attachment to parents and teachers and a weak commitment to educational and occupational goals. Similar to strain theory, empirical efforts have found limited support for the premises of control theory, but researchers generally caution that the effect may be weak and inextricably intertwined with other socio-ecological variables (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992; Farnworth, 1984; Torstensson, 1990). Power-Control Theory and Female Delinquency The power-control theory of delinquency (Hagan, Gillis, & Simpson, 1985; Hagan & Kay, 1990; Hagan, Simpson, & Gillis, 1979, 1987, 1988) was developed to explain why gender differences in common forms of delinquency decline with each step down the class structure. Class is defined by the power the mother or father wields at work. Parents in the command class supervise or manage others at work, while parents in the obey class do not. When there is no difference between the parents' class at work or the family is headed by the mother, the family is defined as egalitarian. When the father has a higher class position than the mother, the family is classified patriarchal. This family type is assumed to determine who controls the children and the extensiveness of that control, which, in turn, is presumed to account for differences in the levels of male and female delinquency (Hagan et al., 1979, 1985, 1987, 1988). The theory postulates that girls will be less delinquent than boys because of girls' greater identification with the mother (relational control) and mothers' greater monitoring of girls (instrumental control). Moreover, the theory suggests that girls in egalitarian families (because mothers are engaged in less parenting) and boys in patriarchal families (because they are being prepared for greater occupational risktaking) are assumed to be the most delinquent-prone. In support of power-control theory, female adolescents' identification with their mothers and mothers' monitoring of daughters have been found to lead to a reduction in risk-taking and delinquent behavior particularly in patriarchal families (Hagan et al., 1987, 1988). In egalitarian families, daughters gained a kind of freedom which was reflected in reduced control by mothers and an increased openness to risk-taking, resulting in more delinquency (Hagan et al., 1988). Also, gender differences in delinquency in the underprivileged classes have been found to be smaller, presumably because neither lower socioeconomic status males or females are being prepared for occupations that involve risk-taking (Hagan et al., 1985, 1987, 1988). However, questions have been raised in empirical studies about several of the key premises to power-control theory (Hill & Atkinson, 1988; Jensen & Thompson, 1990; Morash & Chesney-Lind, 1991; Singer & Levine, 1988). For example, research 100 Hoyt and Scherer has shown that fathers also exert control over their children. The quality of that control, however, is probably different from mothers' and may be more focused on sons (Hill & Atkinson, 1988; Morash & Chesney-Lind, 1991). Other research has found gender differences in delinquency to be greater in egalitarian families than in patriarchal families or found less delinquency in females in every type of family (Morash & Chesney-Lind, 1991; Singer & Levine, 1988). Moreover, family social class rather than socialization for risk-taking was found to be a better predictor for delinquent behavior in both females and males (Morash & Chesney-Lind, 1991). In short, it remains unclear to what extent social class accounts for the differences between female and male delinquency. The contradictory findings suggest that, while some effect for social class and family style may exist, the postulates of power and control theory have yet to be validated. It may be that family social status is only indirectly related to female delinquency through the more direct effects of family process variables that may covary with family social status. The Women's Movement and Female Delinquency Misconceptions and stereotypes about females have been incorporated into research that attributed increases in female delinquency to the rise of feminism. This section briefly highlights this empirical research, which was conducted from the mid1970s to the early 1980s. In general, however, this line of research is no longer considered to be a viable explanatory factor in the causation of female delinquency. Originally, it was assumed that the women's liberation movement radically altered women's perceptions of their personal needs and abilities (Adler, 1975). Women and female adolescents were thought to be committing more crimes either because they perceived less legitimate access to opportunities on the basis of gender or because changing gender roles gave young women more incentive to abandon traditional roles and become more "masculinized." Two results seem to lend some support to these ideas. Black and White female delinquents were found to regard their opportunities less positively than nondelinquent females (Datesman, Scarpitti, & Stephenson, 1975), and the perception of general blocked opportunity was predictive of delinquency (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1979). However, the perception of gender-based blocked opportunities was not correlated with female delinquency (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1979). Furthermore, a masculine sex role, whether in terms of self or others' expectation, has not been associated with an increased frequency of any category of delinquency in females (Norland, Wessel, & Shover, 1981; Shover et al., 1979; Thornton & James, 1979). In fact, females with more-masculine traits were found to be less involved in delinquency than those with less masculine traits. Similarly, the hypothesized association between liberated attitudes among females and actual involvement in delinquent behavior has been found to be minimal (Austin, 1982). In fact, adolescent females with more traditional gender attitudes tended to be more delinquent than those with liberated attitudes (FigueriaMcDonough, 1984; Giordano & Cernkovich, 1979). Taken together, these results suggest that the women's movement has not served to increase female delinquency by altering women's perceptions of their Female Juvenile 101 needs and abilities. Alternately, some commentators have suggested that the women's movement may have precipitated a societal backlash, which is reflected in a greater likelihood of the justice system to arrest, convict, and punish women offenders (Simon & Landis, 1991; J. W. White & Kowalski, 1994). However, there is no empirical support for this supposition. Instead, it is more likely that the slight rise that occurred in female delinquency during the 1970s was primarily a function of the legal reclassification of juvenile offenses and the introduction of new procedural standards (Curran, 1984). THE STATE OF THE SCIENCE OF FEMALE DELINQUENCY AND ITS IMPLICATIONS The most prevalent impressions left from a review of the female delinquency literature are the ambiguities and piecemeal nature of the research. As a result, we have little conclusive understanding of the important etiological factors contributing to female delinquency. While some social scientists have determined that female delinquency follows a unique developmental course (Chamberlain & Reid, 1994; Dembo et al., 1993; Robins, 1986; Stattin & Magnusson, 1989; J. L. White et al., 1989), other researchers have concluded that the underlying causes, evolution, and personality factors that are associated with female delinquency and conduct disorder are perhaps subtler than, but essentially the same as those for male delinquency (Hamalainen & Pulkkinen, 1995; Krueger et al., 1994; Zoccolillo, 1993). Researchers have observed a high correlation between female delinquency and a history of having been physically and sexually abused, yet it remains unknown whether this is a consequence of the abuse or some other concomitant family dysfunction. Family structure has been investigated, but the more etiologically promising family process variables have yet to be thoroughly examined. We know that female and male delinquent peer relationship patterns are different and suspect that peer relationships may buffer females from involvement in delinquency, however, empirical evidence is lacking. Studies of school correlates of female delinquency and the effects of social class have been contradictory. Even the women's movement has been implicated in generating an increase in female delinquency, but it is more likely that the women's movement has simply drawn attention to the travails that young women encounter in the juvenile justice system. Some social scientists suggest that "relative deviance" accounts for bias in the juvenile justice system's response to female delinquency. In essence, because the threshold for arrest and conviction of females is higher, those females who are arrested and convicted are expected to be more deviant and therefore are sanctioned more severely (Dembo et al., 1993). Others contend that female aggression and conduct problems are judged more harshly because of women's status in society. Namely, females who are aggressive are deemed more deviant from society's patriarchal norms, particularly relative to males whose aggression is expected, tolerated, and condoned (J. W White & Kowalski, 1994). A renewal of theoretical and empirical efforts is needed before the phenomenon of female delinquency can be understood and adequately prevented or treated. 102 Hoyt and Scherer Clearly, female delinquency is a multivariate phenomenon with a variety of determinants and diverse etiological paths. It is quite likely that a subset of female delinquents manifest delinquent offenses that are every bit as serious and antisocial as male delinquents and acquire their behavioral problems through similar etiological processes to many male delinquents. On the other hand, it is equally likely that many female delinquents acquire their antisocial conduct through gender-specific developmental processes, gender-related socialization, and a vulnerability to sexual abuse that are not a part of the pathway to male delinquency. In other words, female delinquents are both similar to male delinquents and different than male delinquents. Three strategies have been identified for expanding our knowledge about female delinquency: add-and-stir research, female-only focused research, and integrated universal models of research (Figueria-McDonough, 1992). The add-and-stir strategy of including females in studies of male delinquents has been useful for highlighting gender stereotypes and examining behavioral similarities and differences across gender. Researchers employing this tactic believe that existing theories should be systematically tested with females before deciding that they do not explain female delinquency (Rosenbaum & Lashley, 1990). Others argue that gender differences and the absence of gender sensitivity in delinquency theory and research necessitates a separate theory to explain female delinquency (Chesney-Lind, 1989). We believe that both same-sex and cross-gender studies are needed. It is very likely that the substantial developmental and social differences between females and males alter the etiology of delinquency. Female delinquents report different qualities of family process, different patterns of relating to peers, different attitudes toward school, and more victimization. Yet, rejecting analyses known to contribute to male delinquency is an exercise in "throwing the baby out with the bath water." A recognition and integration of both these realities will lead to the development of a unified theory of delinquency that is gender-sensitive and comprehensive in the selection of predictor variables (Figueria-McDonough, 1992; FigueriaMcDonough, Barton, & Sarri, 1981). More comprehensive and gender-integrated studies of delinquency are possible, but require attention to the diversity of biopsychosocial influences on human nature. Developmental and individual factors such as age, endocrinological effects, identity development, and history of victimization are vital considerations. More detailed observations of family process (rather than family structure) will yield invaluable data on etiology and potential objectives for prevention and intervention. Larger socio-ecological influences such as school context, peer relationships, community resources, and cultural background are essential if we are to distinguish between the various etiologies and types of delinquency (Elliott, Wilson, Huizinga, Sampson, Elliot, & Rankin, 1996; Figueira-McDonough, 1992; Kruttschnitt, 1996; Sommers & Baskin, 1993; J. W White & Kowalski, 1994). Some delinquency-related studies have approximated this comprehensiveness through inclusion of various theories, multivariate strategies, and longitudinal designs (Caspi et al, 1993; Robins, 1986; Simons, Miller, & Aigner, 1980; Smith & Patternoster, 1987). These studies and the preceding review clearly indicate a need for comprehensive and multisystemic studies of female delinquency that will enhance our understanding of female Female Juvenile 103 and male delinquents by drawing attention to the diversity in delinquency and clarifying the relationships between the wide variety of etiological factors that contribute to delinquency. Ultimately, this could lead to a gender-integrated model of delinquency that will result in efficient, impartial, and effective delinquency prevention and juvenile justice intervention efforts. REFERENCES Adler, F. (1975). Sisters in crime. New York: McGraw-Hill. Austin, R. (1982). Women's liberation and increases in minor, major, and occupational offenses. Criminology, 20, 407-430. Barton, W, & Figueira-McDonough, J. (1985). Attachments, gender, and delinquency. Deviant Behavior, 6, 119-144. Bergsmann, I. R. (1989). The forgotten few: Juvenile female offenders. Federal Probation, 53, 73-78. Bowker, L., & Klein, M. (1983). The etiology of female juvenile delinquency and gang membership: A test of psychological and social structure explanations. Adolescence, 18, 739-751. Brown, S. (1984). Social class, child maltreatment, and delinquent behavior. Criminology, 22, 259-278. Brown, W K. (1977). Black female gangs in Philadelphia. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 21, 221-228. Butts, J. A., Snyder, H. N., Finnegan, T A., Augenbaugh, A. L, & Poole, R, S. (1996). Juvenile court statistics 1993. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Calhoun, G., Jurgens, J., & Chen, F. (1993). The neophyte female delinquent: A review of the literature. Adolescence, 28, 461-471. Campbell, A. (1987). Self definition by rejection: The case of gang girls. Social Problems, 34, 451-466. Campbell, A. (1990). On the invisibility of the female delinquent peer group. Criminal Justice, 2, 41-62. Canter, R. (1982a). Sex differences in self-report delinquency. Criminology, 20, 373-393. Canter, R. (1982b). Family correlates of male and female delinquency. Criminology, 20, 149-167. Caspi, A., Lynam, D., Moffitt, T, & Silva, P. (1993). Unraveling girls' delinquency: Biological, dispositional, and contextual contributions to adolescent misbehavior. Developmental Psychology, 29,19-30. Cernkovich, S., & Giordano, P (1979). Delinquency, opportunity, and gender. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 70, 145-151. Cernkovich, S., & Giordano, P. (1987). Family relationships and delinquency. Criminology, 25, 295-319. Cernkovich, S., & Giordano, E (1992). School bonding, race, and delinquency. Criminology, 30, 261-291. Cernkovich, S., Giordano, P., & Pugh, M. (1985). Chronic offenders: The missing cases in self-report delinquency research. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 76, 705-732. Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (1994). Differences in risk factors and adjustment for male and female delinquents in treatment foster care. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 3, 23-39. Chandy, J. M., Blum, R. W., & Resnick, M. D. (1996). Gender-specific outcomes for sexually abused adolescents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 20, 1219-1231. Chesney-Lind, M. (1973). Judicial enforcement of the female sex role: The family court and the female delinquent. Issues in Criminology, 8, 51-59. Chesney-Lind, M. (1977). Judicial paternalism and the female status offender: Training women to know their place. Crime and Delinquency, 23, 121-130. Chesney-Lind, M. (1988). Girls in jail. Crime and Delinquency, 34, 150-168. Chesney-Lind, M. (1989). Girl's crime and woman's place: Toward a feminist model of female delinquency. Crime and Delinquency, 35, 5-29. Chesney-Lind, M., & Shelden, R. (1992). Girls: Delinquency and juvenile justice. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Cohen, A. (1955). Delinquent boys: The culture of the gang. New York: Free Press. Cowie, J., Cowie, V, & Slater, E. (1968). Delinquency in girts. London: Heinemann. Curran, D. (1984). The myth of the "new" female delinquent. Crime and Delinquency, 30, 386-399. Datesman, S., & Scarpitti, F. (1975). Female delinquency and broken homes. Criminology, 13, 33-55. Datesman, S., & Scarpitti, F. (1977). Unequal protection for males and females in the juvenile court. In T. N. Ferdinand (Ed.), Juvenile delinquency: Little brother grows up (pp. 59-77) Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Datesman, S,, Scarpitti, F, & Stephenson, R. (1975). Female delinquency: An application of self and opportunity theories. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 12, 107-123. 104 Hoyt and Scherer Dembo, R., Dertke, M., La Voie, L., Borders, S., Washburn, M., & Schmeidler, J. (1987). Physical abuse, sexual victimization, and illicit drug use: A structural analysis among high risk adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 10, 13-34. Dembo, R., Williams, L, La Voie, L., Berry, E., Getreu, A., Wish, E. D., Schmeidler, J., & Washburn, M. (1989). Physical abuse, sexual victimization, and illicit drug use: Replication of a structure analysis among high risk adolescents. Violence and Victims, 4, 121-138. Dembo, R., Williams, L., & Schmeidler, J. (1993). Gender differences in mental health service needs among youths entering a juvenile detention center. Journal of Prison and Jail Health, 12, 73-101. Elliott, D. S. (1993). National youth survey (United States): Wave VI, 1983. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research. Elliott, D. S., Wilson, W. J., Huizinga, D., Sampson, R. J., Elliott, A., & Rankin, B. (1996). The effects of neighborhood disadvantage on adolescent development. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 33, 389-426. Empey, L. T (1982). American delinquency. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press. Everett, T, & Scherer, D. G. (in press). Children who witness interparental violence: Empirical data and theoretical implications. Aggression and Violent Behavior. Farnworth, M. (1984). Male-female differences in delinquency in a minority group sample. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 21, 191-212. Federal Bureau of Investigation. (1993). Crime in the United States. Washington, DC: US Department of Justice. Figueria-McDonough, J. (1984). Feminism and delinquency. British Journal of Criminology, 24, 325-342. Figueria-McDonough, J. (1992). Community structure and female delinquency rates. Youth and Society, 24, 3-30. Figueria-McDonough, J., Barton, W H., & Sarri, R. C. (1981). Normal deviance: Gender similarities in adolescent subcultures. In M. Q. Warren (Ed.), Comparing female and male offenders (pp. 17-45). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Christ, M. A. G., & Hanson, K. (1992). Familial risk factors to oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: Parental psychopathology and maternal parenting. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 49-55. Giordano, P.(1978). Girls, guys, and gangs: The changing social context of female delinquency. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 69, 126-132. Giordano, P., & Cernkovich, S. (1979). On complicating the relationship between liberation and delinquency. Social Problems, 26, 467-481. Giordano, P., Cernkovich, S., & Pugh, M. (1986). Friendships and delinquency. American Journal of Sociology, 5, 1170-1202. Girls Incorporated (1996). Prevention and parity: Girls in juvenile justice. Indianapolis, IN: Girls Incorporated National Resource Center. Gray, E. (1988). The link between child abuse and juvenile delinquency: What we know and recommendations for policy and research. In G. Hotaling, D. Finkelhor, & M. Strauss (Eds.), Family abuse and it consequences (pp. 109-123). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hagan, J., Gillis, A., & Simpson, J. (1985). The class structure of gender and delinquency: Toward a power-control theory of common delinquent behavior. American Journal of Sociology, 90,1151-1178. Hagan, J., & Kay, F. (1990). Gender and delinquency in white-collar families: A power-control perspective. Crime and Delinquency, 36, 391-407. Hagan, J., Simpson, J., & Gillis, A. (1979). The sexual stratification of social control: A gender based perspective on crime and delinquency. British Journal of Sociology, 30, 25-38. Hagan J., Simpson, J., & Gillis, A. (1987). Class in the household: A power-control theory of gender and delinquency. American Journal of Sociology, 92, 788-816. Hagan J., Simpson, J., & Gillis, A. (1988). Feminist scholarship, relational and instrumental control, and a power-control theory of gender and delinquency. British Journal of Sociology, 39, 301-336. Hamalainen, M., & Pulkkinen, L. (1995). Aggressive and non-prosocial behaviour as precursors of criminality. Studies on Crime and Crime Prevention, 4, 6-21. Hancock, L., & Chesney-Lind, M. (1982). Female status offenders and justice reforms: An international perspective. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 15, 109-122. Henggeler, S., Edwards, J., & Borduin, C. (1987). The family relations of female juvenile delinquents. The Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 199-209. Hill, G., & Atkinson, M. (1988). Gender, familial control, and delinquency. Criminology, 26, 127-147. Hindelang, M. J., Hirschi, T, & Weis, J. G. (1981). Measuring delinquency. Beverly Hills: Sage. Hirschi, T (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Horowitz, R., & Pottieger, A. E. (1991). Gender bias in juvenile justice handling of serious crime-involved youths. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 28, 75-100. Female Juvenile 105 Jarjoura, G. R. (1996). The conditional effect of social class on the dropout-delinquency relationship. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 33, 232-255. Jensen, G., & Thompson, K. (1990). What's class got to do with it? A further examination of powercontrol theory. American Journal of Sociology, 95, 1009-1023. Johnson, D. R., & Scheuble, L. K. (1991). Gender bias in the disposition of juvenile court referrals: The effects of time and location. Criminology, 29, 677-698. Johnson, R. (1986). Family structure and delinquency: General patterns and gender differences. Criminology, 24, 65-84. Johnson, R. (1987). Mother's versus father's role in causing delinquency. Adolescence, 22, 305-315. Konopka, G. (1966). The adolescent girl in conflict. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Koroki, J., & Chesney-Lind, M. (1985). Everything just going down the drain: Interviews with female delinquents in Hawaii Report 319. Honolulu, HI: Youth Development and Research Center. Krohn, M., Curry, J., & Nelson-Kilger, S. (1983). Is chivalry dead? An analysis of changes in police dispositions of males and females. Criminology, 21, 417-437. Kroupa, S. (1988). Perceived parental acceptance and female juvenile delinquency. Adolescence, 23,171185. Krueger, R. F., Schmutte, P S., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T E., Campbell, K., & Silva, P. A. (1994). Personality traits are linked to crime among men and women: Evidence from a birth cohort. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 328-338. Kruttschnitt, C. (1996). Contributions of quantitative methods to the study of gender and crime, or bootstrapping our way into the theoretical thicket. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 12,135-161. Lombroso, C., & Ferrero, W. (1895). The female offender. New York: Philosophical Library. McCormack, A., Janus, M., & Burgess, A. (1986). Runaway youths and sexual victimization: Gender differences in an adolescent runaway population. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10, 387-395. Morash, M. (1986). Gender, peer group experiences, and seriousness of delinquency. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 23, 43-67. Morash, M., & Chesney-Lind, M. (1991). A reformulation and partial test of the power control theory of delinquency. Justice Quarterly, 8, 347-376. Mulhern, R. K., & Passman, R. H. (1981). Parental discipline as affected by the sex of the parent, the sex of the child, and the child's apparent responsiveness to discipline. Developmental Psychology, 17, 604-613. Norland, S., Wessel, R., & Shover, N. (1981). Masculinity and delinquency. Criminology, 19, 421-433. Phelps, R. J., McIntosh, M., Jesudason, V, Warner, P., & Pohlkamp, J. (1982). Wisconsin juvenile female offender project. Madison, WI: Youth Policy and Law Center, Wisconsin Council on Juvenile Justice. Poe-Yamagata, E., & Butts, J. A. (1996). Female offenders in the juvenile justice system. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Pollak, O. (1950). The criminality of women. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Pollak, O., & Freidman, A. S. (Eds.). (1969). Family dynamics and female sexual delinquency. Palo Alto, CA: Science and Behavior Books. Pope, C, & Feyerherm, W (1982). Gender bias in juvenile court dispositions. Social Service Research, 6, 1-16. Rankin, J. (1980). School factors and delinquency: Interactions by age and sex. Sociology and Social Research, 64, 420-434. Rhodes, J. E., & Fischer, K. (1993). Spanning the gender gap: Gender differences in delinquency among inner-city adolescents. Adolescence, 28, 879-889 Robins, L N. (1986). The consequences of conduct disorder in girls. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior: Research, theories, and issues. New York: Academic Press. Rosenbaum, J. L. (1989). Family dysfunction and female delinquency. Crime and Delinquency, 35, 31-44. Rosenbaum, J. L., & Chesney-Lind, M. (1994). Appearance and delinquency: A research note. Crime and Delinquency, 40, 250-261. Rosenbaum, J., & Lashley, J. R. (1990). School, community context, and delinquency: Rethinking the gender gap. Justice Quarterly, 7, 493-513. Rush, F. (1980). The best kept secret: Sexual abuse of children. New York: McGraw-Hill. Schlossman, S., & Wallach, S. (1978). The crime of precocious sexuality: Female delinquency in the progressive era. Harvard Educational Review, 48, 65-94. Schwartz, 1. M., Steketee, M. W, & Schneider, V W. (1990). Federal juvenile justice policy and the incarceration of girls. Crime and Delinquency, 36, 503-520. Seydlitz, R. (1991). The effects of age and gender on parental control and delinquency. Youth and Society, 23, 175-201. 106 Hoyt and Scherer Shelden, R. G, (1981). Sex discrimination in the juvenile justice system: Memphis, Tennessee, 1900-1917. In M. Q. Warren (Ed.), Comparing male and female offenders (pp. 55-72). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Short, J. F., & Strodbeck, F. L. (1965). Group process and gang delinquency. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Shover, N., Norland, S., James, J., & Thornton, W. (1979). Gender roles and delinquency. Social Forces, 58, 162-175. Silbert, M., & Pines, M. (1981). Sexual child abuse as an antecedent to prostitution. Child Abuse and Neglect, 5, 407-411. Simon, R. J., & Landis, J. (1991). The crimes women commit, the punishments they receive. Lexington, MA: Heath. Simons, R., Miller, M., & Aigner, S. (1980). Contemporary theories of deviance and female delinquency: An empirical test. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 17, 42-57. Simons, R., & Whitbeck, L. (1991). Sexual abuse as a precursor to prostitution and victimization among adolescent and adult homeless women. Journal of Family Issues, 12, 361-379. Singer, S., & Levine, M. (1988). Power-control theory, gender, and delinquency: A partial replication with additional evidence on the effects of peers. Criminology, 26, 627-647. Smith, D. L. (1980). Young female offenders: Analysis of differential handling based on sex. Pittsburgh, PA: National Center for Juvenile Justice. Smith, D., & Patternoster, R. (1987). The gender gap in theories of deviance: Issues and evidence. Justice of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 24, 140-172. Snyder, H. N. (1996). Juvenile arrests, 1995. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Snyder, H. N., & Sickmund, M. (1995). Juvenile offenders and victims: A national report. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Snyder, H. N., Sickmund, M., & Poe-Yamagata, E. (1996). Juvenile offenders and victims: 1996 update on violence. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Sommers, L, & Baskin, D. R. (1993). The situational context of violent female offending. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 30, 136-162. Starrels, M. E. (1994). Gender differences in parent-child relations. Journal of Family Issues, 15,148-165. Stattin, H., & Magnusson, D. (1989). The role of early aggressive behavior in the frequency, seriousness, and types of later crime. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 710-718. Steffensmeier, D. J., & Steffensmeier, R. (1980). Trends in female delinquency: An examination of arrest, juvenile court, self-report, and field data. Criminology, 18, 62-85. Sussman, A. (1977). Sex-based discrimination and the PINS jurisdiction. In L. E. Teitelbaum & A. R. Gough (Eds.), Beyond control: Status offenders in the juvenile court (pp. 179-191). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger. Teitelbaum, L. E., & Gough, A. R. (Eds.). (1977). Beyond control: Status offenders in the juvenile court. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger. Thomas, W. I. (1928). The unadjusted girl. Boston: Little Brown. Thornton, W, & James, J. (1979). Masculinity and delinquency revisited. British Journal of Criminology, 19, 225-241. Tolan, P. (1988). Socioeconomic, family, and social stress correlates of adolescent antisocial and delinquent behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 317-331. Torstensson, M. (1990). Female delinquents in a birth cohort: Tests of some aspects of control theory. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 6, 101-115. Tremblay, R. E., Masse, B., Perron, D., Leblanc, M., Schwartzman, A. E., & Ledingham, J. E. (1992). Early disruptive behavior, poor school achievement, delinquent behavior, and delinquent personality: Longitudinal analyses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 64-72. Van Vooris, P., Cullen, F., Mathers, R., & Garner, C. (1988). The impact of family structure and quality on delinquency: A comparative assessment of structural and functional factors. Criminology, 26, 235-261. Vedder, C. B., & Somerville, D. B. (1970), The delinquent girl. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Webster-Stratton, C. (1996). Early-onset conduct problems: Does gender make a difference? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 540-551. Weithorn, L. A. (1988). Mental hospitalization of troublesome youth: An analysis of skyrocketing admission rates. Stanford Law Review, 40, 773-838. Wells, L. E., & Rankin, J. H. (1991). Families and delinquency: A meta-analysis of the impact of broken homes. Social Problems, 38, 71-93. White, J. L, Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1989). A prospective replication of the protective effects of IQ in subjects at high risk for juvenile delinquency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 719-724. Female Juvenile 107 White, J. W., & Kowalski, R. M. (1994). Deconstructing the myth of the nonaggressive woman: A feminist analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 487-508. Wright, K., & Wright, K. (1994). Family life, delinquency, and crime: A policymakers guide. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Zoccolillo, M. (1993). Gender and the development of conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 65-78.