THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.966248 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 Automatic semantic encoding in verbal short-term memory: Evidence from the concreteness effect Guillermo Campoy1, Judit Castellà2, Violeta Provencio1, Graham J. Hitch3, and Alan D. Baddeley3 1 Faculty of Psychology, University of Murcia, Murcia, Spain Faculty of Psychology, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain 3 Department of Psychology, University of York, York, UK 2 The concreteness effect in verbal short-term memory (STM) tasks is assumed to be a consequence of semantic encoding in STM, with immediate recall of concrete words benefiting from richer semantic representations. We used the concreteness effect to test the hypothesis that semantic encoding in standard verbal STM tasks is a consequence of controlled, attention-demanding mechanisms of strategic semantic retrieval and encoding. Experiment 1 analysed the effect of presentation rate, with slow presentations being assumed to benefit strategic, time-dependent semantic encoding. Experiments 2 and 3 provided a more direct test of the strategic hypothesis by introducing three different concurrent attention-demanding tasks. Although Experiment 1 showed a larger concreteness effect with slow presentations, the following two experiments yielded strong evidence against the strategic hypothesis. Limiting available attention resources by concurrent tasks reduced global memory performance, but the concreteness effect was equivalent to that found in control conditions. We conclude that semantic effects in STM result from automatic semantic encoding and provide tentative explanations for the interaction between the concreteness effect and the presentation rate. Keywords: Verbal short-term memory; Concreteness effect; Immediate serial recall; Presentation rate; Dual task paradigm. Studies of verbal short-term memory (STM) usually involve the immediate serial recall of lists of unrelated words or letters. Memory performance in these situations has been found to be highly affected by the phonological properties of the tobe-remembered stimuli, as occurs in the similarity effect (Conrad & Hull, 1964) and in the word length effect (Baddeley, Thomson, & Buchanan, 1975). As a consequence, it has been traditionally assumed that verbal information is phonologically encoded in STM, with participants’ recall relying on some kind of phonological representations of the stimuli (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). The observation of a number of nonphonological effects in verbal STM tasks, however, suggests the participation of factors beyond the mere maintenance of phonological traces. Some of these operate at the level of the individual item. These include the lexicality effect (better immediate recall of words than nonwords; Hulme, Maughan, & Brown, 1991) and the frequency effect (better immediate recall of high-frequency words than Correspondence should be addressed to Guillermo Campoy, Universidad de Murcia, Facultad de Psicología, Campus de Espinardo, 30100, Murcia, Spain. E-mail: gcampoy@um.es This study was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation [Projects PSI2009-07374 and CSD200800048]. © 2014 The Experimental Psychology Society 1 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 CAMPOY ET AL. low-frequency words; Hulme et al., 1997). There is also abundant evidence that verbal STM may be influenced by semantic factors (Campoy & Baddeley, 2008; Haarmann & Usher, 2001; Martin, 2005; Poirier & Saint-Aubin, 1995; Walker & Hulme, 1999). A crucial finding supporting the participation of semantic codes in STM is the concreteness effect, the observation that immediate serial recall is better for concrete, high-imageability words such as pencil than for abstract, low-imageability words like method (Walker & Hulme, 1999). This effect is assumed to be a direct consequence of the participation of semantic codes in STM tasks, with concrete words benefiting from richer and more distinctive semantic representations (Acheson, Postle, & MacDonald, 2010; Allen & Hulme, 2006; Romani, McAlpine, Martin, 2008; Walker & Hulme, 1999). Although the participation of semantic traces in verbal STM tasks has only been broadly recognized recently, early evidence was already reported in the sixties and early seventies by Baddeley and coworkers (Baddeley, 1966; Baddeley & Ecob, 1970; Baddeley & Levy, 1971). However, this evidence was later overshadowed by extensive data supporting the main role of phonological coding. Baddeley (1966) examined the immediate serial recall of lists of five similar and dissimilar words, with similarity being either phonological (e.g., man, mat, map) or semantic (e.g., huge, big, wide). He found a large effect of phonological similarity (the standard similarity effect), but also a small though significant effect of semantic similarity, suggesting the participation of semantic codes. Likewise, a subsequent study by Baddeley and Levy (1971) found effects of semantic similarity in immediate serial recall of noun–adjective pairs, provided the pairs were semantically compatible (e.g., priest–devout). In another study using semantically compatible or incompatible triplets (e.g., my fine wine vs. wine my fine), Baddeley and Ecob (1970) found effects of both phonological and semantic similarity, with the effects of semantic similarity predominating after a delay. In a recent review of this early evidence, Baddeley (2012) concludes that performance in verbal STM tasks may 2 rely on both phonological and semantic encoding. Phonological encoding is rapid and attentionally undemanding, but phonological traces are fragile and readily forgotten. In contrast, semantic encoding in standard tasks (i.e., immediate serial recall of unrelated words) is harder and takes longer to set up, but traces are more durable. The idea that semantic encoding in standard STM tasks takes longer to be set up is congruent with an early proposal by Shulman (1970, 1971, 1972). According to Shulman, both phonological and semantic encoding are possible in verbal STM tasks, but they have different temporal courses. In standard STM tasks, participants tend to encode information phonologically rather than semantically because phonological encoding is faster and thus more appropriate under the temporal pressure of relatively high presentation rates. A direct prediction of Shulman’s hypothesis is that semantic encoding in STM would benefit from slower presentation rates. To test this prediction, Shulman (1970) analysed the effect of presentation rate in a probe-recognition task. Each trial involved the presentation of a list of words (at a presentation rate of a word every 350, 700, or 1400 ms) followed by an instructing cue and a recognition probe. The instructing cue indicated the kind of response that was required in each particular trial. When the cue was the letter H, participants had to indicate whether the probe was a homonym of any word in the list (homonymprobe condition); when the cue was the letter M, participants had to indicate whether the probe had the same meaning as any presented word (semantic-probe condition); finally, when the cue was the letter I, participants had to indicate whether the probe was identical to any of the list words (identical-probe condition). Results showed that recognition of semantic probes improved with slower presentation rates, whereas a contrary tendency was found in the homonym and identical conditions. Shulman interpreted this as supporting his claim that semantic encoding in STM is time dependent and, thus, benefits from slower presentation. The procedure in this study, however, makes strong conclusions difficult. On the one hand, it seems probable that the task itself THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING induced semantic encoding, since participants knew in advance that they could be prompted to respond on the basis of meaning. This fact could lead to some forms of controlled semantic processing that are not present in standard situations. On the other hand, word lists in Shulman’s experiments involved 10 words, which exceeds the capacity of STM (Cowan, 2005). This fact raises the question of to what extent his results are representative of STM performance. In light of the recent interest in semantic encoding in STM, the idea that presentation rate in STM tasks determines semantic encoding deserves renewed attention. If semantic encoding in STM tasks benefits from a slower presentation rate, this could suggest the participation of mechanisms that go beyond the mere automatic activation of semantic information. Research on the retrieval of word meaning supports the distinction between a fast, automatic activation of semantic representations and a slower, more controlled, and effortful mechanism of strategic retrieval (Badre & Wagner, 2002; Gold et al., 2006; Whitney, Grossman, & Kircher, 2009). There is a range of situations in which automatic activation is not sufficient, and a strategic retrieval is required. It has been suggested that one of these situations is when the meaning of a word has to be retrieved in the absence of the contextual support provided by preceding semantically related words (Whitney et al., 2009). This proposal is congruent with the idea that semantic encoding in STM tasks could rely on strategic time-dependent mechanisms, since the absence of contextual semantic support characterizes standard STM tasks involving the presentation of lists of unrelated words. Besides, studies with neuropsychological patients showing significant reduction of semantic effects in STM tasks reveal that these patients have lesions in the left inferior prefrontal cortex (LIPFC; Martin, 2005), a brain region that has been associated with top-down control of semantic memory, including controlled semantic retrieval and postretrieval selection (Badre, Poldrack, PareBlagoev, Insler, & Wagner, 2005; Badre & Wagner, 2002; Wagner, Paré-Blagoev, Clark, & Poldrack, 2001; Whitney, Kirk, O’Sullivan, Lambon Ralph, & Jefferies, 2011). Therefore, semantic STM deficits in this kind of patient could be a consequence of the disruption in mechanisms of control of semantic retrieval (Hoffman, Jefferies, & Lambon Ralph, 2011), consistent with the view that semantic encoding in STM tasks relies on controlled processes. A further step involving the participation of controlled mechanisms in verbal STM tasks is the possibility that participants engage in elaborative strategies of semantic encoding in order to improve their performance. Such semantic strategies could involve, for example, the establishment of semantic links between words and the generation of stories or visual scenes. Evidence for the importance of these strategies of elaborative encoding emerges from studies showing that the adoption of this kind of semantic strategy can eliminate phonological effects usually considered the hallmark of verbal STM, such as the word-length effect or the phonological similarity effect (Campoy & Baddeley, 2008; Logie, Della Sala, Laiacona, Chalmers, & Wynn, 1996). The present study aimed to test the hypothesis that semantic encoding in standard STM tasks relies on controlled and effortful mechanisms of strategic semantic retrieval and encoding. To test this strategic hypothesis, Experiment 1 analysed how a semantic STM effect, the concreteness effect, was affected by the stimulus presentation rate. If semantic encoding depends on effortful, time-dependent mechanisms, the prediction would be greater concreteness effect with slower presentations. Subsequent experiments provided a more direct test of the strategic hypothesis by including secondary attention-demanding tasks together with the STM task. According to the strategic hypothesis, the introduction of secondary tasks would limit the attentional resources available for controlled semantic mechanisms and, thus, would eliminate or at least reduce the concreteness effect. EXPERIMENT 1 Experiment 1 aimed to evaluate the relationship between presentation rate and semantic encoding THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 3 CAMPOY ET AL. Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 in a standard STM task by analysing the effect of presentation rate on the concreteness effect. On the basis of the strategic hypothesis, the prediction was that concrete words would be better recalled with a slower presentation rate, resulting in larger concreteness effect at this rate. Method Participants Forty-eight undergraduate students from the University of Murcia (Spain) took part in the experiment for course credit. All participants in this and the following experiments were native Spanish speakers. Stimuli and apparatus Two sets of 13 Spanish words, one comprising concrete, high-imageability words, the other comprising abstract, low-imageability words, were chosen on the basis of the concreteness and imageability values provided by the LEXESP database (Sebastián, Martí, Carreiras, & Cuetos, 2000). All of the stimuli were trisyllabic nouns with the stress on the penultimate syllable. The sets were matched for word frequency, familiarity, and number of phonemes (see the Appendix). Concrete words were bigote, guitarra, molino, cerveza, corbata, espalda, factura, orquesta, palacio, retrato, alfombra, incendio, and serpiente (moustache, guitar, windmill, beer, necktie, back, invoice, orchestra, palace, portrait, carpet, fire, and snake). Abstract words were jugada, pureza, suceso, tamaño, dominio, esencia, indicio, letargo, mandato, ventaja, ambiente, contexto, and nostalgia (play/move, purity, event, size, dominion, essence, indication, lethargy, mandate, advantage, ambience, context, and nostalgia). All the words, spoken in a neutral tone by a female speaker, were digitally recorded and segmented into individual sound files using audio recorder and editor software. This software was also employed to adjust the duration of the audio files (without altering the pitch), so that all the stimuli lasted 696 ms, which was the mean duration of the original recorded words. A computer programme generated by E-Prime 2 (Schneider, Eschman, & Zuccolotto, 2002) 4 controlled the experiment. A headset with microphone was used to present stimuli and record verbal responses for later processing. Procedure Participants in this and the following experiments were tested individually in soundproof booths. The experiment comprised 52 trials divided into four blocks, one block for each combination of word type (concrete, abstract) and presentation rate (one item per second, one item every two seconds). Two participants were randomly assigned to each of the 24 (4!) possible orders of presentation of these four blocks. The order of presentation of concrete and abstract lists within a block was determined at random. Each trial began with the presentation of a row of dashes, which remained on screen until the participant initiated the trial by pressing the computer mouse button. Two seconds later, a plus sign appeared on the computer screen for one second followed by a five-word list. Words were presented through the headset at a rate of either one item per second or one item every two seconds. After the last stimulus, a question mark was presented on the screen prompting participants to recall the words in serial order. They were instructed to substitute the word espacio (blank) for any word they could not recall. There was a 16-s time limit to complete spoken recall. The 13 lists of each block were constructed for each participant by selecting words from the pertinent set at random, with the constraint that every word appeared in five trials, once in each of the five possible positions within the lists. Experimental trials were preceded by six practice trials, three trials at each presentation rate. These practice trials were constructed so that all the experimental stimuli appeared at least once. A brief rest period was established before each experimental block. Results Participants’ responses were categorized into the following three categories: correct responses (correct words in the right position), order errors THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING Table 1. Percentage of correct responses, order errors, and item errors in Experiment 1 Word type Presentation rate Concrete 1 s/item 2 s/item 1 s/item 2 s/item Abstract Correct responses Order errors Item errors 75.29 (16.30) 79.62 (12.56) 72.24 (16.31) 72.72 (13.54) 14.40 (10.77) 12.29 (8.19) 14.66 (10.24) 13.73 (8.36) 12.95 (10.47) 9.65 (7.84) 16.35 (11.81) 16.15 (10.26) Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 Note: Standard deviations in parentheses. Table 2. Statistical results for the 2 (word type) × 2 (presentation rate) within-subjects ANOVA on the percentage of correct responses, order errors, and item errors in Experiment 1 F df MSE p h2p 17.419 2.408 4.725 1, 47 1, 47 1, 47 68.007 115.211 37.566 ,.001 .127 .035 .270 .049 .091 1.128 1.391 0.725 1, 47 1, 47 1, 47 30.619 80.433 23.173 .294 .244 .399 .023 .029 .015 30.608 3.857 6.839 1, 47 1, 47 1, 47 38.451 37.954 16.957 ,.001 .055 .012 .394 .076 .127 Effect Correct responses Word type Presentation rate Word Type × Rate Order errors Word type Presentation rate Word Type × Rate Item errors Word type Presentation rate Word Type × Rate Note: ANOVA = analysis of variance. (words presented in the current list but recalled in the wrong position), and item errors, with this last category subsuming omissions (blank responses), intraexperimental intrusions (words in the experimental sets but not presented in the current list), and extraexperimental intrusions (words that were not in the experimental sets). Percentages of responses within each category are shown in Table 1. To control for different conditions differing in the number of items recalled, percentages of order errors were calculated with respect to the number of list items recalled, rather than to the total number of responses (Murdock, 1976). Table 2 shows the statistical results of the analyses described below. Percentages of correct responses were submitted to a 2 × 2 within-subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) with word type (concrete, abstract) and presentation rate (one item per second, one item every two seconds) as factors. There was a main effect of word type, revealing that concrete words were better recalled than abstract words (concreteness effect = 4.97%). The main effect of presentation rate was not significant, but there was an interaction between word type and presentation rate. This interaction can be attributed to a larger concreteness effect with the slow presentation rate (concreteness effect = 6.89%) than with the fast rate (concreteness effect = 3.04%). The analysis of simple effects revealed that the concreteness effect was significant with slow presentations, F(1, 47) = 22.360, MSE = 50.966, p , .001, h2p = .322, and fast presentations, F(1, 47) = 4.074, MSE = 54.608, p = .049, h2p = .080. On the other hand, the presentation rate had no effect on abstract words (F , 1), whereas concrete words were better recalled with slow presentations than with fast presentations, F(1, 47) = 6.101, THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 5 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 CAMPOY ET AL. MSE = 73,657, p = .017, h2p = .115. The larger concreteness effect at the slow rate, therefore, can be specifically attributed to the effect of presentation rate on the recall of concrete words.1 An equivalent ANOVA on the percentages of order errors showed no significant main effects or interaction. In contrast, an ANOVA on the percentages of item errors showed a main effect of word type (more item errors with abstract words) and a significant interaction between word type and presentation rate (greater effect of word type with slow presentations). Resembling the pattern found for correct responses, the analysis of simple effects revealed that the effect of word type was significant with both slow presentations, F(1, 47) = 39.377, MSE = 25.799, p , .001, h2p = .456, and fast presentations, F(1, 47) = 9.355, MSE = 29.609, p = .004, h2p = .116. In turn, the effect of presentation rate was significant for concrete words, F(1, 42) = 11.543, MSE = 22.653, p = .001, h2p = .197, but there was no effect for abstract words (F , 1). Discussion Results showed a straightforward pattern. Concrete words were better recalled at the slow rate (a word every two seconds) than at the fast presentation rate (one word per second), whereas the recall of abstract words was not affected by the presentation rate. This pattern resulted in a larger concreteness effect with slow presentations, congruent with Shulman’s proposal that semantic encoding in STM benefits from a slow presentation rate (Shulman, 1970, 1971, 1972). Memory for item identity and serial order in verbal STM tasks is generally assumed to rely on mechanisms that are to some extent independent (Burgess & Hitch, 1999; Murdock, 1976; Nairne & Kelley, 2004). In the present experiment, the concreteness effect was specifically a consequence of more item errors with abstract words than with concrete words, suggesting that word concreteness had an effect on memory for item identity rather than for order information. This fact is congruent with the widespread opinion that lexical–semantic factors affect item memory rather than order memory in STM tasks (Majerus, 2009). Results of Experiment 1 have diverse implications. On the one hand, they show the importance of taking presentation rate into consideration whenever semantic encoding in STM can play a role, with the use of different rates limiting the comparison between studies. On the other hand, and more importantly, results are congruent with the strategic hypothesis according to which semantic encoding in standard STM tasks relies on controlled, time-dependent mechanisms of strategic semantic retrieval and encoding. It is important to note, however, that the effect of presentation rate constitutes only indirect evidence for the strategic hypothesis, with alternative interpretations being possible (see General Discussion). To provide a more direct test of the strategic hypothesis, the following two experiments investigated the effect of introducing secondary attention-demanding tasks on the immediate serial recall of concrete and abstract words. EXPERIMENT 2 In Experiment 2, the availability of attentional resources for strategic semantic processing was limited by introducing an attention-demanding random-generation task (Towse & Neil, 1998). 1 In this and the following two experiments, we performed additional ANOVAs on the percentages of correct responses after including serial position as a within-subject factor. The standard serial position effect, consisting of better recall of the initial (primacy effect) and last (recency effect) items, was found in the three experiments. In Experiment 1, there was also a significant interaction between presentation rate and serial position, revealing that presentation rate had a greater effect on the recall of items presented in central positions. The three-way interaction between word type, presentation rate, and serial position was not significant. In Experiments 2 and 3, there was a significant interaction between concurrent task and serial position, which was a consequence of greater effect of the concurrent tasks for items presented in central serial positions. Importantly, however, the interaction between word type, concurrent task, and serial position was not significant in either experiment. For the sake of brevity, and given that these results do not contribute additional relevant information, they are not further discussed. 6 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING More specifically, we introduced a random time interval generation task (Vandierendonck, De Vooght, & Van der Goten, 1998), in which participants had to press a button at variable intervals, trying to generate the most irregular and unpredictable patterns they could produce. In this randomtapping condition, participants presumably had to allocate attention to monitor the sequence they were generating, inhibiting repetitions and trying to maximize variability (Vandierendonck et al., 1998). Results in this condition were compared to those in a simple-tapping control condition in which participants were asked to click the mouse button at a steady, uniform pace throughout the presentation of to-be-remembered items. On the basis of the strategic hypothesis, random tapping was expected to eliminate or reduce the concreteness effect by limiting the attention resources available for controlled semantic processes. Method Participants Forty-eight undergraduate students from the University of Murcia participated for course credit. Stimuli and apparatus These were the same as those in Experiment 1. Procedure Experiment 2 consisted of 52 trials divided into four blocks, one block for each combination of word type (concrete, abstract) and concurrent task (random tapping, simple tapping). Two participants were randomly assigned to each of the 24 possible orders of presentation of the four blocks. Before the experimental task, participants were instructed on how to perform the two kinds of tapping. For the simple-tapping condition, they were asked to click the mouse button at a steady pace of about two clicks per second. For the random-tapping condition, participants were instructed to click the mouse button at variable intervals, trying to generate the most unpredictable and haphazard sequence they could produce. Each trial began with a message indicating the kind of tapping (simple or random) that was Table 3. Percentage of correct responses, order errors, and item errors in Experiment 2 Word type Concrete Abstract Concurrent task Correct responses Order errors Item errors Random tapping Simple tapping Random tapping Simple tapping 67.02 18.73 18.14 74.68 61.67 14.42 21.01 13.17 22.37 69.36 16.67 17.37 Note: Standard deviations in parentheses. required in that trial, which remained on the screen until the first participant’s click of the tapping sequence. After two seconds, a plus sign appeared on the computer screen for one second followed by a five-word list. Words were presented at a rate of one word every two seconds with participants being instructed to perform the tapping task throughout the entire list presentation (but not during recall). Other aspects of the procedure were the same as those in Experiment 1. Results As in Experiment 1, participants’ responses in the memory task were classified into correct responses, order errors, and item errors (Table 3). Table 4 Table 4. Statistical results for the 2 (word type) × 2 (concurrent task) within-subjects ANOVA on the percentage of correct responses, order errors, and item errors in Experiment 2 Effect Correct responses Word type Concurrent task Word Type × Task Order errors Word type Concurrent task Word Type × Task Item errors Word type Concurrent task Word Type × Task F df MSE p h2p 17.839 1, 47 76.619 ,.001 .275 44.939 1, 47 62.942 ,.001 .489 0.000 1, 47 37.555 .986 .000 5.314 1, 47 46.447 .026 .102 19.818 1, 47 45.333 ,.001 .297 0.000 1, 47 33.791 .992 .000 22.297 1, 47 38.246 ,.001 .322 37.167 1, 47 32.086 ,.001 .442 0.001 1, 47 17.355 .979 .000 Note: ANOVA = analysis of variance. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 7 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 CAMPOY ET AL. shows the statistical results of the ANOVAs detailed below. Percentages of correct responses were submitted to a 2 × 2 within-subjects ANOVA with word type (concrete, abstract) and concurrent task (simple tapping, random tapping) as factors. There was a main effect of word type, showing that concrete words were better recalled than abstract words (concreteness effect = 5.34%). The main effect of concurrent task was also significant, revealing better memory performance in the simple-tapping condition than in the random-tapping condition (tapping effect = 7.68%). Importantly, however, there was no interaction between word type and concurrent task, showing that the concreteness effect did not differ between tapping conditions. An ANOVA on the percentages of order errors revealed significant main effects of both word type (more order errors for abstract words) and concurrent task (more order errors with random tapping). In turn, an ANOVA on the percentages of item errors also showed significant main effects of word type (more item errors with abstract words) and concurrent task (more item errors with random tapping). In both cases, however, there was no significant interaction between the two factors. Additional analyses were conducted to examine participants’ performance on the secondary task. Specifically, our interest focused on the level of randomness in the tapping patterns generated by the participants. We used the variability of the between-tap intervals as an index of randomness, with higher variability being assumed to reflect more random patterns. Interval variability in a trial was quantified as the average proportional difference between all intervals generated by the participant in that trial (for a detailed description of these calculations, see Heath, 2006, Formulae 1, 2, and 4). Intervals generated before the onset of the first to-be-remembered word were not considered. Values of interval variability were .40 for both concrete and abstract trials in the randomtapping condition and .17 for both concrete and abstract trials in the simple-tapping condition. A 2 × 2 within-subjects ANOVA with word type and tapping type as factors showed a main effect 8 of tapping type, F(1, 47) = 330.183, MSE = 85.386, p , .001, h2p = .875, revealing greater interval variability in the random-tapping condition than in the simple-tapping condition. Neither the main effect of word type nor the interaction was significant (both Fs , 1). Discussion Results showed that introducing a concurrent attention-demanding task (random tapping) affected memory performance negatively, with poorer immediate serial recall being a consequence of both order errors and item errors. Contrary to the strategic hypothesis, however, limiting available attentional resources by a concurrent task had parallel negative effects on the recall of concrete and abstract words, so that the concreteness effect in the random-tapping condition was equivalent to that obtained in a control, simple-tapping condition. In turn, the level of randomness in the tapping sequences (estimated from the variability of the generated intervals) was numerically identical in concrete and abstract trials. This seems to rule out the possibility that better memory performance for concrete words was a consequence of neglecting the tapping task during the presentation of concrete lists. These results are clearly inconsistent with the idea that the concreteness effect in STM is a consequence of controlled attention-dependent processes, as suggested by the strategic hypothesis. One additional aspect of the results that deserves consideration is the fact that, in contrast to Experiment 1, there was a significant effect of word concreteness in terms of order, as occasionally found in previous studies (Acheson et al., 2010, Experiment 1; Allen & Hulme, 2006, Experiment 1). An inspection of Table 4, however, reveals that the size of the concreteness effect (h2p ) in terms of item errors was three times larger than the effect in terms of order errors. It seems safe, therefore, to conclude that the concreteness effect was mostly a consequence of differences in item recall. In any case, it is important to note that percentage of order and item errors represents only a rough, indirect estimation of order and item THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING memory, respectively, and should be interpreted with caution. Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 EXPERIMENT 3 At odds with the strategic hypothesis, Experiment 2 found that limiting available attention resources by a concurrent task did not eliminate or reduce the concreteness effect. Experiment 3 aimed to further investigate the consequences of introducing concurrent attention-demanding tasks by a procedure that was presumed to maximize the opportunity for the concurrent task to detectably affect the concreteness effect. Main differences with respect to Experiment 2 were twofold. On the one hand, the closed sets of stimuli used in the previous experiment were replaced by open sets of words, so that each experimental stimulus appeared only once during the experiment. As found previously in this and other studies (e.g., Walker & Hulme, 1999), the concreteness effect in STM seems to be mainly a consequence of differences in item recall. Consequently, a procedure with open sets of stimuli was expected to be more sensitive because performance would depend on item memory to a greater extent. On the other hand, we introduced concurrent tasks involving visuospatial interference. We employed two different concurrent tasks, one predominantly visual and the other predominantly spatial. In one condition, three irregular polygons were presented simultaneously with the auditory presentation of each to-be-remembered word, and participants had to determine which polygon was different from the other two by pressing the corresponding key (see Figure 1). This task was assumed to involve high levels of visual interference (cf. Logie, 1995), although spatial interference was probably present because of the fact that there Figure 1. Schematic representation of the experimental procedure in the polygon condition of Experiment 3. Participants responded to each polygon triad, though only the first response is depicted as an example (represented response is correct). Depicted polygons correspond to stimuli actually used in the experiment. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 9 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 CAMPOY ET AL. Figure 2. Schematic representation of the experimental procedure in the arrow condition of Experiment 3. Participants responded to each arrow triad, though only the first response is depicted (represented example corresponds to an incorrect response). was a spatial stimuli–response mapping (see below). In another condition, three arrows were presented with each word, and participants had to reproduce the sequence by pressing the corresponding arrow keys (see Figure 2). Interference in this condition was assumed to be mainly spatial, though the visual presentation of the arrows probably generated considerable levels of visual interference. Apart from limiting the availability of attentional resources, the fact that these concurrent tasks involved high levels of visuospatial interference was expected to hinder a potentially important semantic elaborative strategy: that based on the generation of mental images and visual scenes (Paivio, 1986). Because concrete words are more imageable than abstract words, it seems reasonable to presume that such strategy would benefit the recall of concrete words to a greater extent. Evidence for the fact that the presentation of visual inputs interferes with the generation of mental images has been found in a wide range of 10 studies employing different approaches (Baddeley & Andrade, 2000; Dean, Dewhurst, Morris, & Whittaker, 2005; Logie, 1986; Quinn & McConnell, 2006). According to Borst, Niven, and Logie (2012), the presentation of visual inputs interferes with the generation of mental images because the mechanisms underlying visual mental imagery overlap to a great extent to those involved in visual perception. Congruent with this notion, visual mental imagery and visual perception have been found to recruit overlapping sensory regions of the brain (Kosslyn et al., 1993). Concurrent task was manipulated between participants to guarantee sufficient number of observations per condition without increasing the duration of the task and the complexity of the design. Participants’ performance in the two concurrent-task conditions was compared to that in a control condition without concurrent task. The prediction from the strategic hypothesis was that the concreteness effect would be eliminated or THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING reduced in the concurrent-tasks conditions. No specific prediction was made about differences between the predominantly visual and the spatial concurrent tasks. Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 Method Participants One hundred and seventeen undergraduate students from the University of Murcia participated in the experiment. Thirty-nine participants were randomly assigned to each of the two concurrenttask conditions (polygons and arrows). The remaining 39 participants were assigned to a control group with no concurrent task. Stimuli and apparatus We selected two sets of Spanish words from the LEXESP database (Sebastián et al., 2000), one comprising 70 concrete, high-imageability words, and the other comprising 70 abstract, low-imageability words. All of the words were trisyllabic nouns with the stress on the penultimate syllable. The two word sets were globally matched for number of phonemes and individually matched for word frequency and familiarity (see Appendix). An additional group of 30 trisyllabic nouns with similar values of frequency and familiarity were chosen for practice trials. All the words were recorded by a female speaker in a neutral tone of voice and were segmented into individual sound files using audio editing software. The experiment was controlled by experimental software written in E-Prime (Schneider et al., 2002). Additionally, we used custom-built software written in Visual Basic (Microsoft Co., Redmond, WA, USA) to generate word lists for each participant prior to the experimental session. To minimize the phonological similarity of the words selected for a given list, no words within a list were allowed to share a syllable in the same position or to start with the same phoneme. Apart from this restriction, the programme created the lists by selecting words from the pertinent set at random without replacement. A headset with microphone was used to present stimuli and record participants’ responses. For the polygons condition, we constructed 168 pairs of eight-sided irregular polygons by using a program written in Matlab (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA) by Collin and McMullen (2002). This programme was designed to generate families of polygons, with the members of a family being similar to each other (see Collin & McMullen, 2002, for an operational definition of family similarity). With the exception of the number of sides, we used the default values for all the customizable parameters of the programme to generate 168 families of polygons. We then took two instances from each family to obtain the 168 pairs. Procedure The experiment consisted of 28 trials, 14 trials for each type of word. The experimental procedure in the polygons condition is depicted in Figure 1. Each trial began with the presentation of a row of dashes, indicating that a key had to be pressed to initiate the trial. After 1.5 seconds following the key press, three polygons were presented in a horizontal row, and participants had to indicate which polygon differed from the other two by pressing the pertinent key. The polygon triad consisted of the two polygons composing one of the 168 pairs of similar polygons (see above), with one of the polygons of the pair appearing twice. Participants were instructed to press the key 1, 2, or 3 on the numeric keypad according to the locus of the differing polygon (key 1, 2, and 3 for left, middle, and right positions, respectively). The polygon triad remained on the screen for 2 s regardless of the participant’s response. After the first polygon triad, a five-word list was presented auditorily at a rate of one word every two seconds, with each word being accompanied by the visual presentation of a new polygon triad. The locus of the differing polygons within the triads was determined at random with the constraint that, throughout each trial, these polygons appeared twice in each of the three possible positions. After the last stimulus of the list, a row of five question marks was presented to prompt participants for spoken recall of the five words in serial order. They were asked to substitute the word espacio (blank) for any word they could not THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 11 CAMPOY ET AL. Table 5. Percentage of correct responses, order errors, and item errors in Experiment 3 Word type Concurrent task Concrete Polygons Arrows Control Polygons Arrows Control Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 Abstract Correct responses Order errors Item errors 59.01 (13.09) 61.06 (14.02) 73.55 (14.81) 51.79 (14.97) 51.61 (16.34) 64.95 (14.98) 19.23 (9.56) 17.68 (8.75) 9.73 (10.09) 19.91 (10.77) 20.80 (13.33) 11.94 (8.96) 27.47 (10.99) 26.37 (12.18) 19.08 (10.52) 36.30 (13.28) 35.71 (14.09) 26.81 (12.10) Note: Standard deviations in parentheses. recall. There was a 16-s time limit to complete recall. The procedure in the arrows condition was the same as that in the polygons condition, but polygon triads were replaced by three arrows pointing to different directions (see Figure 2). For each arrow presentation, participants had to press the corresponding arrow keys on the keyboard, starting from the arrow presented on the left of the screen. In the control condition, no polygons or arrows were presented, with other aspects of the procedure being the same. In the three conditions, six practice trials preceded experimental trials. Results Participants’ responses in the memory task were classified into correct responses, order errors, and item errors as in previous experiments (Table 5). Table 6 presents the statistical results of the ANOVAs described below. Percentages of correct responses were submitted to a 2 × 3 mixed ANOVA with word type (concrete, abstract) as the within-subject factor and concurrent task (polygons, arrows, and control) as the between-subject factor. There was a main effect of word type, with concrete words being better recalled than abstract words (concreteness effect = 8.43%). There was also a main effect of concurrent task, revealing that the percentage of correct responses differed between groups. Post hoc Tukey HSD tests (MSE = 381.48, df = 114) revealed better memory performance in the control group that in the other two groups (both ps , .001; polygon effect = 13.85%; arrow effect = 12.91%), whereas there was no difference between the polygon group and the arrow group Table 6. Statistical results for the 2 (word type) × 3 (concurrent task) mixed ANOVA on the percentage of correct responses, order errors, and item errors in Experiment 3 F df MSE p h2p 78.522 12.245 0.469 1, 114 1, 114 2, 114 52.884 381.475 52.884 ,.001 ,.001 .627 .408 .177 .008 5.534 11.119 0.693 1, 114 1, 114 2, 114 42.638 171.949 42.638 .020 ,.001 .502 .046 .163 .012 131.664 7.107 0.399 1, 114 1, 114 2, 114 33.111 267.268 33.111 ,.001 .001 .672 .536 .111 .007 Effect Correct responses Word type Concurrent task Word Type × Task Order errors Word type Concurrent task Word Type × Task Item errors Word type Concurrent task Word Type × Task Note: ANOVA = analysis of variance. 12 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING (p = .952). Importantly, there was no interaction between word type and concurrent tasks, revealing that the concreteness effect did not differ across groups. An ANOVA on the percentages of order errors showed a significant main effect of word type (more order error with abstract words). The main effect of concurrent task was also significant, with post hoc Tukey HSD tests (MSE = 171.95, df = 114) revealing significant differences between the control group and the other two groups (fewer order errors in the control groups; both ps , .001). There was no difference between the polygons group and the arrows group (p = .987). The interaction between word type and concurrent task was not significant. Similarly, an ANOVA on the percentages of item errors revealed a main effect of both word type (more item errors with abstract words) and concurrent task. Post hoc Tukey HDS tests (MSE = 267.27, df = 114) showed significant differences between the control group and the other two groups (fewer item errors in the control group, both ps , .01), with no difference between the polygons group and the arrows group (p = .945). Again, there was no interaction between word type and concurrent task. In order to analyse performance in the concurrent tasks, we calculated the proportion of correct responses and the mean reaction time (RT) on concrete and abstract trials for each participant. Responses generated prior to presentation of the first word in the list (responses to the first polygon/ arrow triad in each trial) were not considered. Mean proportion of correct responses was .72 in both the polygon and arrow groups and for both concrete-word and abstract-word trials. As expected from this numerical coincidence, a 2 × 2 ANOVA on the proportion of correct responses with word type and concurrent task as factors showed no significant main effects or interaction (all Fs , 1). In turn, an ANOVA on the mean RTs revealed a main effect of concurrent task (longer RTs in the arrow group), F(1, 76) = 33.864, MSE = 18,575.557, p , .001, h2p = .308. However, neither the main effect of word type nor the interaction was significant (both Fs , 1). Mean RT for concrete and abstract trials were, respectively, 1300 ms and 1290 ms in the polygon group, and 1378 ms and 1375 ms in the arrow group. Discussion Results were equivalent to those in Experiment 2. In comparison with the control condition, the introduction of concurrent demanding tasks had a detrimental effect on memory performance, involving more item errors and order errors. Importantly, however, this detrimental effect of the concurrent tasks on recall was equivalent for concrete and abstract lists, so that the concreteness effect on the conditions with concurrent tasks was comparable to that in the control condition. Performance in the concurrent tasks was equivalent for concrete and abstract trials, ruling out the possibility that better recall of concrete words was due to selective neglect of the concurrent task during the presentation of concrete lists. As in Experiment 2, therefore, results in this experiment were incompatible with the strategic hypothesis and rather support the idea that semantic encoding in standard STM tasks relies on automatic mechanisms. Experiment 3 again showed reliable concreteness effects in terms of both item and order errors. However, an inspection of effect sizes in Table 6 revealed that the concreteness effect in terms of item errors was more than 11 times the size of the effect in terms of order errors, consistent with the idea that lexical–semantic factors in verbal STM tasks principally affect item memory rather than order memory (Majerus, 2009). GENERAL DISCUSSION The concreteness effect in STM is broadly assumed to be a consequence of semantic traces being richer and more distinctive for concrete than for abstract words (Walker & Hulme, 1999). From this point of view, the concreteness effect can be taken as an index of the degree to which the to-be-remembered words are semantically encoded in STM. We adopted this approach to evaluate the strategic hypothesis: the idea that the participation of semantic codes in standard STM tasks (those THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 13 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 CAMPOY ET AL. involving the immediate serial recall of lists of unrelated words) relies on controlled, strategic mechanisms of semantic retrieval and elaborative encoding. Since controlled semantic retrieval and elaborative encoding are most probably time dependent, the strategic hypothesis would predict larger semantic effects when to-be-remembered words were presented at slower presentation rates. Congruently with this prediction, Experiment 1 showed a larger concreteness effect with a presentation rate of one word every two seconds in comparison with a rate of one word per second. The concreteness effect in this experiment was specifically a consequence of differences between concrete and abstract words in terms of item errors, with the presentation rate affecting exclusively the percentage of item errors with concrete words. Neither order errors nor memory performance for abstract words was modulated by the presentation rate. Although the results of Experiment 1 were compatible with the strategic hypothesis, they only constitute indirect evidence, with alternative interpretations being also possible (see below). Experiments 2 and 3 provided a more direct test of the strategic hypothesis by introducing concurrent attention-demanding tasks: a random time interval generation task in Experiment 2 and two different visuospatial tasks in Experiment 3. In contrast with the random time interval generation task, which was originally conceived to demand executive resources with no verbal or visuospatial interference (Vandierendonck et al., 1998), concurrent tasks in Experiment 3 involved extensive visuospatial interference. We expected that this visuospatial interference would hinder potentially relevant semantic strategies based on the generation of mental images or scenes. Another important difference between Experiments 2 and 3 is that Experiment 3 included open sets of stimuli (with each to-be-remembered word appearing only once throughout the experiment), instead of the closed sets used in Experiment 2. We assumed that memory performance would rely on item memory to a greater extent with open sets of stimuli, so that the procedure would be more sensitive to variations in the concreteness effect. Despite these differences, Experiments 2 and 3 yielded a 14 qualitatively identical pattern. Reducing available attentional resources by concurrent, attentiondemanding tasks resulted in a marked decrement in memory performance, expressed in worse item and order recall. However, the concreteness effect in conditions with concurrent demanding tasks was equivalent to that found in control conditions. These results constitute strong evidence against the strategy hypothesis and, instead, support the idea that the concreteness effect in standard STM tasks emerges from automatic encoding of semantic information. If semantic encoding in STM tasks does not depend on a controlled, time-dependent mechanism, an alternative interpretation is required for the effect of presentation rate in Experiment 1. One possibility is to evoke the levels of processing (LoP) principle, whereby the more deeply and elaborately the stimulus is processed, the better it is retained (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). It might be argued that a slow presentation rate could favour deeper (semantic) processing during word presentation, which would benefit especially the subsequent recall of concrete words. However, previous research has shown that the LoP only has an effect on immediate recall when the memory task includes a highly interfering secondary task that entails massive displacement of information from STM to long-term memory (Loaiza, McCabe, Youngblood, Rose, & Myerson, 2011; Rose & Craik, 2012; Rose, Myerson, Roediger, & Hale, 2010). An explanation based on the LoP, thus, seems implausible. Alternatively, if we assume that semantic encoding is automatic but involves spreading activation (Collins & Loftus, 1975) throughout the semantic network, then more time will lead to broader and deeper activation, better encoding, and better recall. In the case of phonological encoding, the associated network is much less rich and hence gains less from the added time. Otherwise, within the framework of the multicomponent working memory model (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974), it has been recently suggested that a slower presentation rate might promote the participation of the episodic buffer as the main storage device for the immediate recall of verbal information (Barrouillet, Plancher, THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING Guida, & Camos, 2013). The episodic buffer is assumed to store multimodal representations that combine features of different nature (for example, verbal, visual, and semantic) in a multidimensional code, integrating information from diverse modalities and memory systems (Baddeley, 2000). Constructing this kind of multidimensional representations requires taking and binding information from different sources into a single representation, with these processes probably benefiting from a slower presentation rate. From this perspective, the effect of presentation rate in Experiment 1 might be due to the greater involvement of the episodic buffer with slow presentations, with concrete words benefiting from richer and more distinctive multidimensional representation in this buffer. An additional aspect that deserves consideration is the global decrement of memory performance yielded by the introduction of attention-demanding tasks in Experiments 2 and 3. In our opinion, this result may be a consequence, at least in part, of the effect of reducing available attentional resources on the maintenance mechanisms in STM. At present, maintenance of verbal information in the short term is usually assumed to rely on two different mechanisms: articulatory rehearsal and attentional refreshing (Camos, Lagner, & Barrouillet, 2009; Mora & Camos, 2013; Oberauer & Lewandowsky, 2008, 2013; Raye, Johnson, Mitchell, Greene, & Johnson, 2007). The reason why introducing attentiondemanding concurrent tasks could have interfered with the attention-based refreshing mechanism seems obvious because this mechanism is assumed to rely on domain-general attentional resources, with the same limited resources being required for both refreshing STM traces and performing the concurrent tasks (Barrouillet, Bernardin, & Camos, 2004; Oberauer & Lewandowsky, 2008, 2013). Using concurrent tasks to temporally hinder attentional refreshing constitutes the basic procedure of the studies on the time-based resource-sharing model of working memory, which have repeatedly shown how memory performance decreases as the concurrent task distracts attention for a greater proportion of time (Barrouillet et al., 2004; Barrouillet, Bernardin, Portrat, Vergauwe, & Camos, 2007). Regarding articulatory rehearsal, it has been generally assumed that this maintenance mechanism operates with no extensive attentional demand, but that attention is necessary during the very initial phase in which the rehearsal loop is set up (Naveh-Benjamin & Jonides, 1984). Therefore, limiting attentional resources could have impaired memory performance by both restricting the participation of the attentional refreshing mechanism and disrupting the construction of the rehearsal loops. Our final considerations concern the question of what might be the nature of the semantic processing underlying the concreteness effect in STM. One possibility is that this effect was a consequence of participants encoding and actively maintaining semantic representations. Initial proposals about the contribution of semantic maintenance in verbal STM tasks emerged from neuropsychological patients showing what was claimed to be a specific deficit in the short-term retention of semantic representations (Martin, Shelton, & Yaffee, 1994). Subsequent research suggests that STM performance of these patients is a consequence of the impairment of semantic control mechanisms rather than a specific semantic STM deficit (Hoffman et al., 2011). Either way, it has been alleged that memory performance of these patients in verbal STM task is not necessarily related to the active maintenance of semantic representations in the short term (Shivde & Anderson, 2011). Alternative accounts include, for example, a poorer use of long-term memory at recall, resulting in less efficient reconstruction of degraded phonological traces (Shivde & Anderson, 2011). From a different approach, Shivde and Anderson (2011) found evidence of semantic maintenance by using what they called the concurrent probe paradigm. This procedure included two apparently independent tasks that participants had to perform simultaneously. On the one hand, participants were asked to keep in mind the meaning of a target word in order to indicate whether this meaning matched the meaning of a probe word that was presented THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 15 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 CAMPOY ET AL. after a retention interval. Concurrently, participants performed a lexical decision task, which could occur either during the retention interval or after the participants’ response to the memory probe. Analysis of the reaction times in the lexical decision task revealed slower responses for words semantically related to the target item that they were asked to maintain. Other aspects being equal, this semantic relatedness effect vanished if participants had already provided a response to the probe word. There was also no effect in equivalent conditions in which the relevant dimension in the memory task was the phonological or visual (word form) characteristics of the target and probe words rather than their meaning. The effect also disappeared when participants were instructed to focus on the lexical decision task and stop maintaining in mind the meaning of the target word. These findings were interpreted as a proof that the relatedness effect was actually a consequence of active semantic maintenance. In principle, it could be alleged that a similar form of active semantic maintenance might underlie the concreteness effect found in the present study. However, it is important to note that, unlike our study, both the instructions provided to the participants and the nature of the memory task itself oriented participants to engage in semantic maintenance. This contrasts with what occurs in standard verbal STM procedures, in which it has been broadly demonstrated that participants mainly rely on the maintenance of phonological traces. The same argument can be raised with regard to a number of neuroimaging studies that seem to identify the neural basis of short-term semantic maintenance: a frontotemporal circuit in which prefrontal areas (mainly, the LIPFC) would be in charge of maintaining the activation of neural semantic representations in the temporal cortex (Fiebach, Friederici, Smith, & Swinney, 2007; Shivde & Thompson, 2004). Again, memory tasks in these neuroimaging studies were designed to boost semantic encoding and maintenance. The fact that there is no evidence of active semantic maintenance with a standard immediate serial recall procedure casts doubt about the possibility that such active 16 maintenance mechanisms underlie semantic effects in this kind of tasks, with alternative accounts being possible. It could be alleged, for example, that semantic effects in standard tasks are a consequence of the initial semantic activation generated by the presentation of a to-be-remembered words facilitating the reconstruction of degraded phonological traces. This facilitating effect could be seen as a form of semantic priming (Shivde & Anderson, 2011) and would operate at recall but also during the retention interval, contributing to the reactivation and maintenance of phonological traces in this interval. In its turn, reactivation of phonological traces during the retention interval might involve the subsidiary automatic reactivation of semantic representations leading to a snowball effect, with semantic activation facilitating the reactivation of phonological traces and reactivation of phonological traces feeding back the activation of semantic representation. We began with a simple question, whether the semantic contribution to immediate serial verbal recall depended on the application of a controlled semantic strategy. Our results suggest that this is not the case, and, as such, they resemble findings with more complex sentence stimuli. Baddeley, Hitch, and Allen (2009) studied the effects of concurrent disruption of the visuospatial, phonological, and executive subcomponents of working memory on the capacity to take advantage of sentence structure in immediate serial recall, comparing retention of constrained sentences with that of the equivalent words in random order. They found clear effects of visuospatial and articulatory suppression, which were magnified when combined with an attentional load. Each concurrent task had a greater or lesser impact on overall performance, but this did not interact with the advantage gained from sentence form. This is also consistent with the conclusions drawn by Caplan and Waters (1999) who present substantial evidence that the use of grammatical structure in language processing is automatic and does not require additional executive processes. To summarize, the present study shows that semantic encoding in standard verbal STM THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING tasks (immediate serial recall of unrelated words) does not depend on the participation of controlled semantic strategies. It is important to note, however, that this does not mean that participants cannot implement this kind of strategy in certain circumstances, as suggested by the fact that the standard phonological effect in STM is abolished when participants are explicitly instructed to use a semantic strategy (Campoy & Baddeley, 2008). Original manuscript received 14 May 2013 Accepted revision received 18 August 2014 REFERENCES Acheson, D. J., Postle, B. R., & MacDonald, M. C. (2010). The interaction of concreteness and phonological similarity in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 17–36. Allen, R., & Hulme, C. (2006). Speech and language processing mechanisms in verbal serial recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 64–88. Baddeley, A. D. (1966). Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 362–365. Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417–423. Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. Baddeley, A. D., & Andrade, J. (2000). Working memory and the vividness of imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 126–145. Baddeley, A. D., & Ecob, J. R. (1970). Simultaneous acoustic and semantic coding in short-term memory. Nature, 227, 288–289. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 8 (pp. 47–90). New York: Academic. Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J., & Allen, R. J. (2009). Working memory and binding in sentence recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 438–456. Baddeley, A. D., & Levy, B. A. (1971). Semantic coding and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 132–136. Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589. Badre, D., Poldrack, R. A., Pare-Blagoev, E. J., Insler, R. Z., & Wagner, A. D. (2005). Dissociable controlled retrieval and generalized selection mechanisms in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 47, 907–918. Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2002). Semantic retrieval, mnemonic control, and prefrontal cortex. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1, 206–218. Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., & Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 83–100. Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., Portrat, S., Vergauwe, E., & Camos, V. (2007). Time and cognitive load in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 570–585. Barrouillet, P., Plancher, G., Guida, A., & Camos, V. (2013). Forgetting at short term: When do eventbased interference and temporal factors have an effect? Acta Psychologica, 142, 155–167. Borst, G., Niven, E., & Logie, R. H. (2012). Visual mental image generation does not overlap with visual short-term memory: A dual-task interference study. Memory and Cognition, 40, 360–372. Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551– 581. Camos, V., Lagner, P., & Barrouillet, P. (2009). Two maintenance mechanisms of verbal information in working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 457–469. Campoy, G., & Baddeley, A. (2008). Phonological and semantic strategies in immediate serial recall. Memory, 16, 329–340. Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 77–94. Collin, C. A., & McMullen, P. (2002). Using Matlab to generate families of similar Attneave shapes. Behaviour Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 34, 55–68. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreadingactivation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 17 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 CAMPOY ET AL. Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion, and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 429–432. Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. New York, NY: Psychology Press. Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684. Dean, G. M., Dewhurst, S. A., Morris, P. E., & Whittaker, A. (2005). Selective interference with the use of visual images in the symbolic distance paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1043–1068. Fiebach, C. J., Friederici, A. D., Smith, E. E., & Swinney, D. (2007). Lateral inferotemporal cortex maintains conceptual–semantic representations in verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 2035–2049. Gold, B. T., Balota, D. A., Jones, S. J., Powell, D. K., Smith, C. D., & Andersen, A. H. (2006). Dissociation of automatic and strategic lexicalsemantics: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for differing roles of multiple frontotemporal regions. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 6523–6532. Haarmann, H. J., & Usher, M. (2001). Maintenance of semantic information in capacity-limited item short term memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 568–578. Heath, J. P. (2006). Quantifying temporal variability in population abundances. Oikos, 115, 573–581. Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., & Ralph, M. A. (2011). Explaining semantic short-term memory deficits: Evidence for the critical role of semantic control. Neuropsychologia, 49, 368–381. Hulme, C. Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 685–701. Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G. D. A., Martin, S., & Stuart, G. (1997). Word frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: Evidence for a redintegration proces in immediate serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1217–1232. Kosslyn, S. M., Alpert, N. M., Thompson, W. L., Maljkovic, V., Weise, S. B., Chabris, C. F., … Buonnano, F. S. (1993). Visual mental imagery activates topographically organized visual cortex: PET 18 investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 263–287. Loaiza, V., McCabe, D., Youngblood, J., Rose, N. S., & Myerson, J. (2011). The influence of levels of processing on recall from working memory and delayed recall tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1258–1263. Logie, R. H. (1986). Visuo-spatial processing in working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 229–247. Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum. Logie, R. H., Della Sala, S., Laiacona, M., Chalmers, P., & Wynn, V. (1996). Group aggregates and individual reliability: The case of verbal short-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 24, 305–321. Majerus, S. (2009). Verbal short-term memory and temporary activation of language representations: The importance of distinguishing item and order information. In A. S. C. Thorn & M. P. A. Page (Eds.), Interactions between short-term and long-term memory in the verbal domain (pp. 244–276). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Martin, R. C. (2005). Components of short-term memory and their relation to language processing. Evidence from neuropsychology and neuroimaging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 204–208. Martin, R. C., Shelton, J., & Yaffee, L. (1994). Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 83–111. Mora, G., & Camos, V. (2013). Two systems of maintenance in verbal working memory: Evidence from the word length effect. PLoS One, 8, Article no. e70026. Murdock, B. B. (1976). Item and order information in short-term serial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 191–216. Nairne, J. S., & Kelley, M. R. (2004). Separating item and order information through process dissociation. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 113–133. Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Jonides, J. (1984). Maintenance rehearsal: A two-component analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 369–385. Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008). Forgetting in immediate serial recall: Decay, temporal distinctiveness, or interference? Psychological Review, 115, 544–576. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 AUTOMATIC SEMANTIC ENCODING Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2013). Evidence against decay in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 380–411. Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Poirier, M., & Saint-Aubin, J. (1995). Memory for related and unrelated words: Further evidence on the influence of semantic factors in immediate serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 384–404. Quinn, J. G., & McConnell, J. (2006). The interval for interference in conscious visual imagery. Memory, 14, 241–252. Raye, C. L., Johnson, M. K., Mitchell, K. J., Greene, E. J., & Johnson, M. R. (2007). Refreshing: A minimal executive function. Cortex, 43, 135–145. Romani, C., McAlpine, S., & Martin, R. C. (2008). Concreteness effects in different tasks: Implications for models of short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 292–323. Rose, N. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (2012). A processing approach to the working memory/long-term memory distinction: Evidence form the levels-of-processing span task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1019–1029. Rose, N. S., Myerson, J., Roediger, H. L., & Hale, S. (2010). Similarities and differences between working memory and long-term memory: Evidence from the levels-ofprocessing span task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 471–483. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools, Inc. Sebastián, N., Martí, M. A., Carreiras, M., & Cuetos, F. (2000). LEXESP. Léxico informatizado del español. Barcelona: Ediciones de la Universidad de Barcelona. Shivde, G. S., & Anderson, M. C. (2011). On the existence of semantic working memory: Evidence for direct semantic maintenance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 37, 1342–1370. Shivde, G., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2004). Dissociating semantic and phonological maintenance using fMRI. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 10–19. Shulman, H. G. (1970). Encoding and retention of semantic and phonemic information in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 499–508. Shulman, H. G. (1971). Similarity effects in short-term memory. Psychological Bulletin, 75, 399–415. Shulman, H. G. (1972). Semantic confusion errors in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 221–227. Towse, J. N., & Neil, D. (1998). Analyzing human random generation behavior: A review of methods used and a computer program for describing performance. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 30, 583–591. Vandierendonck, A., De Vooght, G., & Van der Goten, K. (1998). Does random time interval generation interfere with working-memory executive functions?. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 10, 413–442. Wagner, A. D., Pare-Blagoev, E. J., Clark, J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2001). Recovering meaning: Left prefrontal cortex guides controlled semantic retrieval. Neuron, 31, 329–338. Walker, I., & Hulme, C. (1999). Concrete words are easier to recall than abstract: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short-term serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1256–1271. Whitney, C., Grossman, M., & Kircher, T. T. (2009). The influence of multiple primes on bottom-up and top-down regulation during meaning retrieval: Evidence for 2 distinct neural networks. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2548–2560. Whitney, C., Kirk, M., O’Sullivan, J., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Jefferies, E. (2011). The neural organization of semantic control: TMS evidence for a distributed network in left inferior frontal and posterior middle temporal gyrus. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 1066–1075. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 19 CAMPOY ET AL. APPENDIX Description of the word sets and comparison between concrete and abstract sets Table A1. Word sets in Experiments 1 and 2 Downloaded by [the Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford] at 08:54 11 November 2014 Concrete words Abstract words Word property Mean SD Range Mean SD Range t(24) p Concreteness Imageability Frequency Familiarity Phonemes 6.0 6.2 22.7 5.4 7.1 0.4 0.3 17.4 0.9 0.9 5.5–6.6 5.6–6.7 8.6–65.7 3.0–6.5 6–9 3.5 3.2 26.1 5.3 7.1 0.3 0.4 18.3 0.8 1.0 3.0–3.9 2.5–3.7 6.1–65.5 3.6–6.5 6–9 17.70 21.18 0.48 0.34 0.00 .00 .00 .63 .74 1.00 Note: Values of concreteness, imageability, word frequency, and familiarity were taken from the LEXESP database (Sebastián et al., 2000). Concreteness, imageability, and familiarity ranged from 1 to 7. Word frequency is expressed in number of occurrences per million words. Table A2. Word sets in Experiment 3 Concrete words Abstract words Word property Mean SD Range Mean SD Range t(138) p Concreteness Imageability Frequency Familiarity Phonemes 5.9 5.9 11.4 5.1 6.8 0.4 0.4 7.9 0.8 0.6 5.4–6.7 5.4–6.8 2.5–34.5 3.1–6.7 6–8 3.7 3.6 11.4 5.1 6.9 0.4 0.6 7.6 0.8 0.7 2.6–4.3 1.8–4.4 2.7–30.9 3.1–6.6 6–8 33.56 28.66 0.05 0.02 0.37 .00 .00 .96 .98 .71 Note: Values of concreteness, imageability, word frequency, and familiarity were taken from the LEXESP database (Sebastián et al., 2000). Concreteness, imageability, and familiarity ranged from 1 to 7. Word frequency is expressed in number of occurrences per million words. 20 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014