Diversity in the person, diversity in the group

advertisement
European Journal of Social Psychology
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
Published online 3 June 2009 in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.647
Agenda
Diversity in the person, diversity in the group: Challenges of identity
complexity for social perception and social interaction
GALEN V. BODENHAUSEN*
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, USA
Abstract
Social psychological research is increasingly coming to grips with the complexity of social identity within the individual,
both from the perspective of perceivers trying to form impressions and make judgments about multiply categorizable
targets, as well as from the perspective of actors using their different self-aspects as a framework for guiding their
interactions with the social world. I review several contributions to the effort to better understand these issues and then
explore some of their possible implications for understanding the nature and consequences of diversity within the group.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
The importance of social categories in guiding social cognition and behavior was recognized decades ago by
seminal figures in social psychology (Allport, 1954; Sherif, 1948; Tajfel, 1974). Since then, research on social
categorization and associated phenomena such as stereotyping and intergroup conflict has flowed in a steadily
increasing stream, attesting to the theoretical centrality and practical significance of these issues. Much of the
social psychological research that has been conducted in this tradition has focused on one identity dimension at a
time (e.g., race or sex or nationality) and has assumed the existence of distinct, discrete categories within a given
dimension (e.g., clearly defined racial groups) rather than overlapping, blended, or ambiguous categories. In addition,
researchers often tend either to adopt the perspective of the observer, asking how social categories are imposed on
target persons in the course of social perception, or they adopt the perspective of the actor, asking how categorical
identities are claimed or disavowed in the course of self-perception, and how these identities influence subsequent selfregulation. These research traditions have yielded undeniably rich and diverse insights into the dynamics of social
categorization, but the picture they have produced has not fully come to terms with some of the inherent complexities of
social categorization.
In recent times, researchers have begun to turn their attention more explicitly toward the multifaceted nature of social
identities. The diversity of identity, within the individual, has been increasingly recognized and investigated, both from the
perceiver’s and the actor’s perspectives. The impact of a group’s diversity on group dynamics and productivity has also
been increasingly recognized in recent years, but the issue becomes more complicated when one recognizes the diversity
that resides within the individual members comprising the group. In this article, I review some of the recent research and
theory bearing on the diversity of identity, considering its implications both for individuals and groups. As will become
clear, many key issues remain ripe for empirical exploration.
*Correspondence to: Galen V. Bodenhausen, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL 60208-2710, USA.
E-mail: galen@northwestern.edu
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Received 1 April 2009
Accepted 9 April 2009
2
Galen V. Bodenhausen
DIVERSITY IN THE PERSON
For fairly obvious reasons, researchers interested in racism often conduct experiments in which a target’s apparent race
(and/or the perceiver’s race) varies, while other aspects of the person are ignored or held constant. By focusing on this
single identity dimension, the overarching role of race can be discerned and evaluated. Similar approaches have been taken
in research on sexism, ageism, and other forms of identity-based prejudice, with many valuable insights accruing.
However, these approaches ignore the inherently multifaceted nature of social identity. By arranging experimental
manipulations so that only one specific social category is salient, researchers bypass fundamental questions about how the
multiplicity of social identity might be spontaneously navigated in the absence of direct experimental cueing. Fortunately,
other research approaches have more to say about this issue, and a picture is emerging of the factors that determine when
and how particular identities become focal and how they function in relation to one another.
The Complexity of Identity
The idea that an actor’s identity is not stable but rather shifts across time was a central feature of the dramaturgical
perspective advanced by the advocates of symbolic interactionism (Burke, 1945; Goffman, 1959). One particular variant
of this approach, role theory (e.g., Stryker, 1968; Stryker & Serpe, 1982), proposed that each social identity an individual
possesses exists in a hierarchy of salience; the actual momentary salience of any given social role is assumed to dictate its
power to guide and constrain behavior, and as salience shifts, behavior shifts in a corresponding manner. From this
perspective, the diversity of the person resides in the repertoire of social roles and identities occupied by the individual.
In more recent decades, the dynamics through which different social categories become self-defining have been
conceptualized most influentially by self-categorization theorists (SCT; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987;
Turner, Oakes, Haslam, & McGarty, 1994). Although framed in terms of how the categorization of the self changes across
situations, the theory assumes corresponding patterns in the categorization of others. Its key insight is that social identities
come spontaneously into focus in a given situation to the degree that they provide a contextually meaningful framework
for defining it as an intergroup situation. The contextual meaningfulness of social categories is postulated to depend on the
degree to which they provide comparative fit (by accounting for patterns of similarity and difference across individuals;
e.g., Wegener & Klauer, 2004) and normative fit (by aligning observed behavior with the norms associated with particular
groups; Oakes, 1987). Other variables that can contextually influence patterns of categorization include the relative
distinctiveness of a given identity (e.g., Nelson & Miller, 1995) and the extent to which activating a categorical identity can
satisfy momentarily influential personal goals and motives (e.g., Mussweiler, Gabriel, & Bodenhausen, 2000). All of these
perspectives emphasize dynamic and flexible shifts in the salience of social categories across time, with their influence on
behavior waxing and waning correspondingly.
A given individual’s identity can shift across distinct dimensions (e.g., sex versus ethnicity versus nationality, etc.) as
well as along a more ‘‘vertical’’ dimension, as emphasized in SCT (i.e., personal level, subgroup level, ‘‘basic’’ group
level, superordinate group level). Consider Barack Obama. He can potentially be categorized as male, a US citizen, a
politician, Christian, etc. Within any of these categories, relatively more inclusive or exclusive bases for categorization
exist (e.g., for the geopolitical identity ‘‘US citizen,’’ we could think of ‘‘Chicagoans’’ as a more exclusive subtype for him
and ‘‘North Americans’’ as a more inclusive one). In theory, all of these different ways of categorizing Obama could be
relatively independent of one another, but in practice they clearly are not. For one thing, any of the seemingly distinct,
independent bases for categorization can serve to define a more exclusive subtype of another category (e.g., ‘‘Black
Americans’’ or ‘‘American Men’’). Penner and Saperstein (2008) reported evidence showing that perceived race—a category
often thought of in biologically essentialist terms (Hirschfeld, 1996)—is determined to a noteworthy degree by a person’s
social status. That is, the racial categories ascribed to others, and the racial categories that are self-ascribed, can change as a
function of changes in an individual’s social status, as marked by membership in categories like ‘‘prisoners’’ and ‘‘the
unemployed.’’ Thus, conceptually orthogonal identity dimensions can interact to determine social categorization. Much
more research needs to address the general psychological principles governing such interactions, although as reviewed below,
many interesting clues are already available. The diversity of the person is greatly expanded beyond his or her basic repertoire
of identities by the many potential interactions among these roles. It is evident not only in shifts in categorization across time
but also in contemporaneous influences and intersections of identities that can modify their basic characteristics.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Identity complexity
3
This idea carries important implications for how social categories are used. Stereotyping and prejudice, too, must be
characterized by a degree of flexibility and context sensitivity, if the categorical representations giving rise to them are so
dynamic and interactive. This idea has been supported by a number of recent studies. Wittenbrink, Judd, and Park (2001)
showed that automatic evaluations of racial minority targets (specifically, African Americans) were moderated by the
context in which these targets were presented (a church versus an urban street corner; see also Barden, Maddux, Petty, &
Brewer, 2004). With respect to stereotyping, Garcia-Marques, Santos, and Mackie (2006) showed that the stereotypes
applied to a given target tend to shift across contexts, because contextually salient information gets incorporated into
active stereotype representations. Along similar lines, Mendoza-Denton, Park, and O’Connor (2008) showed that genderbased stereotyping changes across different contexts. For example, they found that expectations about the differential
assertiveness of men versus women are moderated by the situation in which a target is depicted (e.g., at home versus in the
workplace). There could be a number of reasons why stereotypes shift when category members are encountered in
different contexts. For one, specific contexts can activate specific categorical subtypes, in effect fine-tuning categorization
and stereotyping. Research confirms that subtypes can trigger quite distinct stereotypic associations, compared to the
‘‘parent’’ category (e.g., Devine & Baker, 1991). Another possibility is that context cues can influence the perceived
prototypicality of a target vis-à-vis the category. Prototypicality, in turn, is a noteworthy moderator of stereotyping (e.g.,
Eberhardt, Davies, Purdie-Vaughns, & Johnson, 2006; Maddox, 2004). Because salient (but conceptually orthogonal)
identity dimensions can function as an important kind of context cue, they can modify the expression of stereotyping via
mechanisms such as these.
One of the most systematic approaches to understanding the nature of the simultaneous influences of distinct identity
dimensions has been the research on crossed categorization (for a recent review, see Crisp & Hewstone, 2007). The
majority of this research has examined how the extent to which multiple identities are shared between a perceiver and a
target moderates the perceiver’s evaluative reactions to the target. Theorists working in this area have developed many
interesting hypotheses about possible patterns of interactions within the contingency table defined by two cross-cutting
identity dimensions. However, the most common result is an additive pattern, in which the positivity of evaluations is a
straightforward linear function of number of shared identity dimensions, irrespective of which particular dimension(s) are
shared and which are not. This research has offered some important insights, taking us beyond the one-category-at-a-time
approaches that have typified much of the research on social categorization, but it does have some limitations. For one,
with a few exceptions (to be noted), the focus of this work has generally been on the global evaluative consequences of
cross-cutting categories, leaving issues of descriptive stereotyping unaddressed. For another, many cross-categorization
studies involve methods that explicitly direct the perceiver’s attention to both identity dimensions, leaving open questions
about when and whether perceivers are likely to spontaneously notice and make use of more than one category at a time.
Another interesting question about the interplay of different categories concerns the possible interactions among
hierarchically nested identities. When subgroup identities form, it seems likely that their cognitive representation will
emphasize features that set them apart from the more inclusive groups of which they are a subset; otherwise, there would
be little need for a distinct subtype. At the same time, research on the ingroup projection model (Wenzel, Mummendey, &
Waldzus, 2007) has shown that membership in a particular lower-level identity can modify the way a more inclusive
category is construed. Specifically, one’s own subgroup identity is likely to be especially influential in defining the
prototypic characteristics of the superordinate group. Thus, for example, French people and German people may have
different understandings of the typical features that define the superordinate category ‘‘Europeans.’’ If the superordinate
category has different, subgroup-centric meanings for the different subgroups, then the ostensibly shared superordinate
identity may be perceived as being less fully shared than set-theory logic might imply. Moreover, although a shared,
superordinate identity is expected to produce beneficial effects on intergroup attitudes (e.g., Dovidio, Gaertner, & Saguy,
2009), attainment of these benefits can be compromised when members of a subgroup do not feel recognized and respected
within the more inclusive category, and thus do not identify particularly strongly with it (Hornsey & Hogg, 2000). There
are many ways in which different levels of identification within a nested hierarchy can interact with one another.
Yet another context for thinking about category interactions concerns different identities within the same identity
dimension. We typically think of the distinct identities within a given dimension as being mutually exclusive—we are
either men or women, members of one racial group or another, citizens of one country or another, adherents of one religion
or another, etc. In reality, these categories are not mutually exclusive. Even categorization by sex is not as cleanly
dimorphic as most people assume, as the rare but regularly occurring case of intersex persons attests (Fausto-Sterling,
2000). Intersex individuals possess anatomical features that do not permit clear classification of them as either male or
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
4
Galen V. Bodenhausen
female. Such ambiguity is not easily tolerated, and intersex persons typically receive medical interventions that ultimately
force them into one category or the other. Given the low base rate of intersex births and the fact that early ‘‘corrective’’
interventions are commonly pursued, we rarely if ever knowingly encounter intersex adults. However, other kinds of
blended categories are becoming increasingly common, particularly the case of multiracial people. In the most recent
national census in the United States, respondents were permitted for the first time to select more than one category when
reporting their race; many people did so, and demographic projections indicate that steadily increasing numbers will likely
do so in the future.
When people have one Black parent and one White parent, how will they categorize themselves with respect to race?
How will others categorize them? Will both categories apply, or perhaps neither? Might one of the categories trump the
other in defining an individual’s race? If neither category seems to fit, will new categories be created (see, e.g., Travassos &
Williams, 2004, regarding the variety of distinct interracial categories that are commonly in use in Brazil)? There are a
number of findings that provide possible answers to these questions. Regarding perceivers’ categorizations of ambiguous
targets, research on the ingroup over-exclusion effect (Leyens & Yzerbyt, 1992) indicates that perceivers often assign
ambiguous cases to outgroup categories rather than ingroups. One prediction following from this idea is that in the case of
Black/White biracial targets, Black perceivers may be more likely to consider the targets to be White, while White
perceivers may be more likely to consider them to be Black. Another, related possibility is that the criteria for membership
in high status groups are more restrictive than those for membership in lower status groups, such that when their category
membership is in doubt, individuals will be assigned to the lower status group (even by people who are not members of
either group). This notion, known as the rule of hypodescent, was invoked as a legal principle defining the officially
recognized racial identity of mixed-race persons (Banks & Eberhardt, 1998); that is, such persons were considered to be
members of the lower-status ancestral racial group. Recent developments in molecular genetics indicate that ‘‘we are all
multiracial’’ (Chakravarti, 2009, p. 381), so if a strict imposition of the hypodescent rule were to hold in the US context, it
would mean that we are all Black.
Although in fact long abandoned as an official policy, the hypodescent principle may still be active as an implicit
cultural theory regarding race. In support of this possibility, Peery and Bodenhausen (2008) found that when required to
rapidly categorize ambiguous faces as ‘‘Black’’ or ‘‘not Black,’’ White and Asian perceivers were more likely to
categorize the targets as ‘‘Black’’ if they were known (based on information learned during a previous training session) to
have mixed-race ancestry, compared to when nothing was known about their ancestry. They also categorized these same
faces as ‘‘White’’ or ‘‘not White,’’ and in this case, knowledge of a target’s mixed-race ancestry was associated with a
greater likelihood of choosing the ‘‘not White’’ response. Thus, when a target was explicitly known to have both Black and
White ancestry, he or she was more likely to be reflexively categorized by perceivers as Black and not White. Of course,
the ways multiracial people categorize themselves is also a topic of considerable interest. Some evidence suggests that
multiracial youth often feel pressured to choose just one monoracial identity (Herman, 2004). Barack Obama (2004), for
example, wrote poignantly about his reasons for identifying as Black, despite having a White mother. His recent rise to
international prominence raises many interesting questions about how voters construe the race of multiracial candidates
and whether or not these construals correspond to the identities claimed by the candidates themselves (for an overview, see
Peery & Bodenhausen, 2009). Much remains to be learned about the identification patterns of multiracial people, but one
general lesson already seems apparent. Although many people assume that having a multiracial background represents a
burden of identity ambiguity that is likely to undermine well being, this is not inherently the case (Shih & Sanchez, 2005).
The psychological difficulties that have been associated with multiracial identities are better attributed to subtle but
pervasive cultural messages that devalue or fail to recognize the reality of multiracial status (Sanchez, in press; Townsend,
Markus, & Bergsieker, 2009).
The issue of blended categories clearly provides a rich set of questions that add additional complexity to questions
about how different categories interact with one another. Collectively, the issues reviewed above indicate that social
categorization must be viewed with an eye toward possible interactions between different aspects of a person’s identity.
Next I consider the most frequently considered views regarding the interplay of different social categories.
Models of Multiple Category Management
Several different research programs have addressed identity complexity, from both the perceiver’s and the actor’s
perspective. Depending on the approach, particular psychological structures and processes are postulated to characterize
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Identity complexity
5
how multiple identity facets function in relation to one another. Three possibilities emerge with some consistency across
different research programs, so the discussion below is organized around these three primary patterns.
The first pattern is dominance, which occurs when a given identity tends to trump other potential identities, remaining
salient in most circumstances while other identities fall off of the radar screen (Urban & Miller, 1998). To the extent that
other characteristics are noticed, they will tend to function merely as piecemeal, personal features rather than organizing
categorical identities (see Bodenhausen, Macrae, & Sherman, 1999; Roccas & Brewer, 2002). Only the dominant category
retains the power to define the individual’s social identity. From the perceiver’s perspective, category dominance would be
reflected in routinely relying on the same category dimension(s) when construing and reacting to others. A relatively clearcut example might be a White racist, who views every non-White person through the lens of race. For such a person, other
considerations such as sex, occupation, age, or religion matter little in comparison to a target’s race. Even people who do
not necessarily explicitly endorse racist ideologies may allow race to dominate other bases for social categorization. Goff,
Thomas, and Jackson (2008) showed that even such a relatively salient category as gender may be trumped by race in a
sample of participants who were highly unlikely to endorse racist ideologies. They briefly exposed White undergraduates
(from a large public American university) to a series of dark-skinned and light-skinned faces and had them categorize the
faces in terms of race and sex. Black faces were categorized by race with very high accuracy, but when it came to sex,
errors were evident, particularly the error of miscategorizing Black females as males (an error rate in excess of 12% in one
study). In contrast, Black males were very rarely miscategorized as females, and both kinds of sex categorization errors
were very rare among White targets. Goff et al. argue that this pattern of errors reflects the fact that, by default, the
prototypic member of the category Black is a male. When responding to a person primarily in terms of a dominant
category, prototypic features may be automatically inferred, even if they result in misclassifying the person on another
identity dimension.
The categories that hold the power to dominate other possible bases for categorization are likely to be ones that are
essentialized (Medin & Ortony, 1989), meaning that a deep underlying essence that fundamentally defines membership in
the category is ascribed. Rothbart and Taylor (1992) described two key characteristics of essentialized categories: (1)
immutability and (2) inductive richness (see also Haslam, Rothschild, & Ernst, 2000). It makes a good deal of sense that
identities that are perceived to be both unchanging and deeply informative would be ones that tend to dominate social
perception. Race (Hirschfeld, 1996) and sex (Prentice & Miller, 2006) are two identity dimensions that tend to be
essentialized to a substantial degree by many social perceivers. One hypothesis following from this analysis is that the
extent to which racism and sexism are expressed in people’s judgments and behaviors may be a function of the degree to
which they essentialize these categories. Indeed, Haslam, Rothschild, and Ernst (2002) and Keller (2005) provided
evidence consistent with this idea. More pertinent to the present analysis, one could hypothesize that essentialized social
categories will be the ones that ‘‘pop out’’ most inevitably when we are confronted by multiply categorizable targets.
Stangor, Lynch, Duan, and Glass (1992) provided evidence that race and sex are both used spontaneously in the
categorization of multiply categorizable targets. It would be interesting to determine whether the extent to which this is the
case is related to individual differences in essentialist beliefs regarding these categories. Bastian and Haslam (2006)
provided evidence that individuals who endorse essentialist beliefs in a general way tended to endorse stereotypes about a
number of different social groups, suggesting the operation of a general lay theory (see also Molden & Dweck, 2006).
However, there may nevertheless be differences in the extent to which particular identities are essentialized, and these
differences may predict which identities are likely to form the basis for social categorization (and stereotyping) when
multiply categorizable targets are encountered in the absence of experimental cueing toward particular dimensions.
A challenge to the view that essentialized categories (specifically, race) will tend to dominate social perception was
provided by several experiments conducted by Kurzban, Tooby, and Cosmides (2001). They argued that the functionally
most important categories are the ones that track coalitional affiliations. Race might look like a category of particular
importance or dominance because it often covaries with people’s coalitional concerns (i.e., people tend to form coalitions
with racial ingroup members), but in the studies conducted by Kurzban et al., race and coalition status were experimentally
unconfounded. Using the ‘‘who-said-what?’’ paradigm (Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff, & Ruderman, 1978) that has frequently been
employed to examine spontaneous use of social categories, they found that people readily abandon racial categorization
when race no longer marks coalition boundaries. On the other hand, their studies pointed toward the consistent use of sex
as a basis for categorization, regardless of coalition status. Does this result mean that sex inevitably trump other social
categories? A study by Eagly and Kite (1987) paints an interesting picture on this question. Examining the intersections of
national and sex categories, Eagly and Kite found that for male targets, nationality tended to modify the characteristics that
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
6
Galen V. Bodenhausen
were stereotypically applied to them. In contrast, female targets tended to be viewed similarly irrespective of their
nationality. One interpretation of this pattern is that low-status essentialized categories will dominate social perception to a
greater extent than high-status essentialized categories. Clearly, the idea that essentialized categories will inevitably
dominate social categorization is untenable, and more research will be required to address questions about when and for
whom such categories will tend to trump other bases for construing others.
Dominance patterns can also characterize self-categorization processes. In their influential analysis of social identity
complexity, Roccas and Brewer (2002) characterized dominance as one of the less complex forms of managing multiple
social identities. Strict dominance would involve a single identity defining the scope of the person’s ingroup on a crosssituationally stable basis, with other identities being largely irrelevant. A modified, hierarchical dominance pattern would
exist when a person takes other aspects of his or her identity into account when thinking about other people who share
membership in this dominant (primary) category, but not when thinking about others who do not share this identity
(Brewer, Ho, Lee, & Miller, 1987). Brewer and Pierce (2005) documented that these relatively simple forms of social
identification carry implications not just for self-perceptions but also for how others are judged. Specifically, outgroups are
defined in a more rigid and monolithic way, and they are accorded less tolerance, when people have low-complexity social
identities.
Why might a single category come to dominate one’s sense of social identity? In the case of minorities and stigmatized
groups, sometimes the external social world can impose these identities on the individual (Polletta & Jasper, 2001), by
virtue of their salience in the minds of many of the people with whom members of these groups interact (and also in
broader cultural representations). Along these lines, Jetten, Branscombe, Schmitt, and Spears (2001) showed that one
common response to discrimination is to identify more strongly with the relevant group. Multiracial people represent an
interesting case that may be an exception to this rule, because as noted above, even though others may attempt to
pigeonhole them into monoracial categories and subject them to corresponding discrimination and stigmatization, their
own sense of self-definition is likely to be more complex and fluid than that. Along these lines, Pauker and Ambady (2009)
showed that, compared to self-identified monoracial individuals, self-identified multiracial people are less likely to be
influenced by racial category labels when processing faces. A correspondingly lower tendency to essentialize race was
observed in this sample of multiracial people. The importance of psychological essentialism as a moderator of the
dominance pattern was underscored by research showing that the degree to which bicultural individuals hold essentialized
views of race predicts the degree to which their ethnic culture of origin dominates their social identity; moreover, this
essentializing tendency was associated with greater difficulty in psychologically moving between the two cultural frames
of reference (Chao, Chen, Roisman, & Hong, 2007).
Recently, Amiot, de la Sablonnière, Terry, and Smith (2007) proposed a stage model of social identity development,
focusing in particular on what happens when taking on a significant new identity becomes a possibility. In the early
‘‘categorization’’ stage of their model, distinct aspects of identity exist in isolation from one another and one particular
identity (the oldest, most established one) predominates over the others. This dominant identity is likely to be chronically
salient and to be actively bolstered. The expected course of development from this stage is to acquire an ability to deal with
the uncertainty involved in having more than one identity, such that more than one identity can be recognized and claimed
by the self.
The second commonly recognized pattern of multiple-category management is compartmentalization, which occurs
when various social categories exist as encapsulated, non-overlapping representations that take turns dominating social
perception and self definition. No category has chronic dominance, and any of them can be momentarily focal while the
others remain dormant. From the perceiver’s perspective, this means that the immediate context will drive which
categories stand out from among the multiple possible bases for categorization. Racism, sexism, and other specific forms
of prejudice become situated rather than universal phenomena. This idea is at the heart of the activation-inhibition model
of stereotyping proposed by Bodenhausen and Macrae (1998), building on the empirical studies reported in Macrae,
Bodenhausen, and Milne (1995). The model proposes that when multiply categorizable targets are encountered,
those social categories that can be readily discerned will compete with one another for momentary dominance in the
mind of the perceiver. For the winning category, stereotypes associated with the category will be activated and applied
to the target (unless they are unambiguously contradicted by available information). For other initially contending
categories, the activation levels of their associated stereotypes will be actively inhibited, potentially producing lower
accessibility than under baseline conditions (see Macrae et al., 1995). The model proposes a range of variables that can
bias which of the competing categories will win the contest for momentary dominance, including the frequency and
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Identity complexity
7
recency of a category’s prior activation, the situational and general distinctiveness of a given identity, and the degree of fit
between the target’s behavior and category prototypes (i.e., normative fit) among others.
In addition to the evidence reviewed in Bodenhausen and Macrae (1998), more recent studies on cross-cutting social
categories supports the idea that stereotypes tend to be activated and applied in an ‘‘either/or’’ manner. That is, when
stereotypes are activated about one identity, stereotypes about other potential bases of categorization do not appear to be
influential, in keeping with the idea that they are inhibited. For example, Pittinsky, Shih, and Trahan (2006) exposed
perceivers to targets that could be categorized on the basis of their ethnicity and/or their sex, and they looked for evidence
of sex and ethnic stereotyping in reactions to these targets. Participants who were subtly cued to attend to sex applied
gender (but not ethnic) stereotypes to the target, while those who were cued toward ethnicity applied ethnic (but not
gender) stereotypes. Klauer, Ehrenberg, and Wegener (2003) and van Rijswijk and Ellemers (2002) also reported evidence
that stereotyping of multiply categorizable targets follows an either/or (i.e., compartmentalization) pattern.
Research has also examined these dynamics as they apply to self-categorization. In the Roccas and Brewer (2002)
model of social identity complexity, compartmentalization represents a more complex form of identification in which
different identities come to the fore in different circumstances (see also Amiot et al., 2007). This idea clearly resonates
with the main themes of self-categorization theory (Turner et al., 1987). Another research tradition that fits well with the
notion of compartmentalization is work on cultural frame-switching in bicultural persons (Hong, Morris, Chiu, & BenetMartı́nez, 2000). Here the idea is that bicultural individuals often possess two distinct cultural identities that take turns
controlling processes of self-regulation in different contexts, depending on situational cues to the relevance of a given
identity. One such cue may be language, and some research suggests that when using different languages, people appear to
be expressing different personalities, presumably due to the invocation of different, culturally defined norms for behavior
within each group of language users (Ramı́rez-Esparza, Gosling, Benet-Martı́nez, Potter, & Pennebaker, 2004). Chiao,
Heck, Nakayama, and Ambady (2006) showed that even low-level perceptual and attentional processes could be modified
by frame-switching. Compartmentalization effects of this sort are not limited to ethnically defined cultural identities.
Hugenberg and Bodenhausen (2004) studied college students who held memberships in fraternities and sororities, and thus
had a ‘‘Greek system’’ identity as well as a more superordinate ‘‘university student’’ identity. These two identities are
associated with quite different ‘‘cultures,’’ or norms for self-regulation. Hugenberg and Bodenhausen showed that when
one of these identities was salient, its associated norms became more cognitively accessible, while the norms of the rival
identity were actively inhibited.
In Amiot et al.’s (2007) developmental model, the compartmentalization stage is the point at which different identities
gradually come to be seen as complementary rather than oppositional. Compartmentalization of oppositional identities
would be expected to produce different kinds of cognitive consequences, compared to when identities are viewed as
complementary aspects of the self; indeed, Benet-Martı́nez, Leu, Lee, and Morris (2002) showed that standard frameswitching effects like those reviewed above are not found among individuals with multiple identities that seem to them to
work in opposition to one another. In fact, for such people, situational cues that would normally trigger the use of a
particular cultural frame often evoke responses that are more typical of the alternative (subjectively opposed) identity.
This brings us to the third possibility for managing multiple social categories, the idea of category integration. From the
perceiver’s perspective, integration involves taking note of more than one of a target’s social category memberships and
using them in some simultaneous fashion in responding to the target. They could be used additively, as often seen in
research on evaluations of cross-categorized targets (Crisp & Hewstone, 2007). Alternatively, they could interact in some
manner such that the impact of one categorical identity is modified by the presence of other one(s). Exactly this kind of
interactive process is hypothesized in Kunda and Thagard’s (1996) parallel-constraint-satisfaction model of stereotyping
in impression formation. Models of subtyping and subgrouping (Richards & Hewstone, 2001) can also be viewed as
instances of category integration, to the extent that they are defined by conjunctions of categorical identities (see also
Hutter & Crisp, 2005; Kunda, Miller, & Claire, 1990). For example, consider encountering a male hairdresser, Sergio, at
the local salon. Compartmentalization would imply that Sergio is perceived either as a male or as a hairdresser, but
integration would be evident if these two identities form a meaningful subtype having its own stereotypical associations.
Whereas the category interactions proposed in Kunda and Thagard’s model are dynamic and would be expected to change
across contexts, the category interactions involved in subtyping and subgrouping may be much more stable. Indeed, they
may be thought of as creating a new, firmly fused categorical identity that can potentially be activated and used in a
consistent way across contexts (Bodenhausen & Macrae, 1998). Even if encountered in another social situation, such as an
anti-war protest, Sergio might still be perceived and evaluated in terms of his male hairdresser identity.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
8
Galen V. Bodenhausen
Of course, perceivers can also form individuated impressions of multiply categorizable targets (Brewer, 1988; Fiske &
Neuberg, 1990). We might think of individuation as the ultimate in category integration, in that perceivers reconcile the
implications of multiple identities (e.g., male, politician, African American, liberal, sports fan, dog-lover, etc.) in forming
an impression of the target. However, in this situation the various facets of the individual may actually not function as
organizing categories any longer, but instead may serve merely as a set of personal attributes. In this sense, the multiple
category problem is transformed into a matter of multiattribute evaluation (see, e.g., Bodenhausen and Peery, 2009).
The issue of identity integration has been richly explored from the perspective of self-categorization processes. In the
Roccas and Brewer (2002) model of social identity complexity, the most complex form of social identification is called
merger, which involves the simultaneous recognition of multiple categories as being self-defining (e.g., occupation,
ethnicity, sex, etc.). Across different contexts, multiple identities may remain salient and influential. For example, in a
context that makes ethnicity highly salient, a person with a merged social identity would still be likely to feel a sense of
shared identity with an ethnic outgroup member who happened to share the same occupation. Roccas and Brewer review
numerous variables that can increase the probability that social identity merger will occur, including the experience of
societal diversity, the presence of relevant individual needs and motives, variations in stress, and many others.
Identity integration is also a topic of great interest in research on multiracial and bicultural individuals (e.g., BenetMartı́nez & Haritatos, 2005; Cheng & Lee, 2009). The general finding is that greater integration of these identities is
associated with greater well being and more effective social functioning (e.g., Phinney, Horenczyk, Liebkind, & Vedder,
2001). Cheng and Lee argued that multiracial individuals’ degree of identity integration depends on (a) how much
dissimilarity they perceive to exist between the racial groups that constitute their own ancestry and (b) how much conflict
they perceive to exist between these groups. They also showed that the degree of integration is at least somewhat malleable
and subject to experimental manipulation. Within this variability, there may be a general developmental trajectory to
acquire a more stable sense of identity integration over time (Amiot et al., 2007). Strategies for resolving any apparent
inconsistencies or conflict between identities may be discovered and implemented over time, laying the groundwork for a
more integrated, complex social identity (Tadmore & Tetlock, 2006). Achieving an integrated social identity does not
preclude continuing to show evidence of frame-switching across contexts (Benet-Martı́nez et al., 2002). It does not mean
that there will no longer be any contextually driven shifts in momentary self-definition, but it does imply a greater
overarching sense of personal integrity because the different self-elements do not seem to be fundamentally at odds with
one another.
The research reviewed in this section makes it clear that the diversity that exists within the individual can result in a
range of different patterns of social perception and self-perception. In some circumstances a simple focus on a single
identity dimension may be sufficient to capture the relevant psychological dynamics, but in many other cases multiple
categories need to be considered.
DIVERSITY IN THE GROUP
There has been increasing interest in recent years in understanding the challenges and opportunities that arise from diverse
social groups, such as work teams, juries, and classrooms. In this section I explore the implications of within-person
diversity for addressing these questions about group diversity. I begin by providing a very brief overview of the group
diversity literature.
Sometimes diversity within groups is defined in terms of the extent to which different group members possess different
information or cognitive skills, but most commonly, people think of group diversity in terms of basic demographic
categories—particularly race/ethnicity and sex. The growing interest in how diversity influences a group’s functioning is
timely, because all demographic trends point to the emergence of increasingly diverse societies in which many kinds of
working groups are expected to be characterized by a greater degree of diversity (Golembiewski, 1995). A common
presumption is that diversity should enhance group performance, because people with different backgrounds can be
expected to bring different knowledge and skills to group tasks. Indeed, as the political economist Page (2007) shows, it is
a logically derivable verity that groups having cognitive diversity of this sort will outperform groups selected on a more
singular basis (e.g., selecting the individuals with the highest aptitude test scores). At the same time, the introduction of
diversity into a group could create socioemotional tensions of the sort that have often been observed to arise in intergroup
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Identity complexity
9
situations. If members of different subgroups indeed do subjectively define the situation as an intergroup setting, then
in(sub)group favoritism and related intergroup tendencies can begin to poison the morale and interpersonal relations
within the group. In combination, these two ideas suggest that benefits from group diversity are possible but certainly not
inevitable.
Influential reviews of the empirical literature on the consequences of group diversity (Mannix & Neale, 2005; Milliken
& Martins, 1996; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998) have concluded that the evidence is very mixed. On one hand, there is indeed
evidence that racial diversity can enhance the quality of group task performance (e.g., Antonio, Chang, Hakuta, Kenny,
Levin, & Milem, 2004; Ely & Thomas, 2001), and not just by virtue of minority group members bringing new perspectives
to the table. The presence of a different subgroup can also create competitive motivation resulting in performance
increments (Lount & Phillips, 2007), and the presence of a minority group member can lead majority group members to
think in more complex ways (Sommers, 2006; Sommers, Warp, & Mahoney, 2008). At the same time, there is evidence
that the interpersonal dynamics within a group can suffer after the introduction of diverse group members (e.g., Harrison,
Price, & Bell, 1998). In order to provide an integrative conceptualization of these kinds of findings, van Knippenberg, De
Dreu, and Homan (2004) proposed their categorization/elaboration model of diversity, which recognizes the possibility of
both negative effects arising from intergroup biases but also positive effects arising from the group’s cognitive
diversification. The model specifies key variables that moderate the consequences of group diversity, and the most central
of these is what van Knippenberg et al. term ‘‘diversity beliefs,’’ or lay theories containing assumptions about whether
diversity is a good or a bad thing for a group’s functioning. In support of this idea, van Dick, van Knippenberg, Hägele,
Guillaume, and Brodbeck (2008) showed in two longitudinal studies that, to the extent group members hold pro-diversity
beliefs, diversity increased their degree of cognitive elaboration on group tasks. Homan, van Knippenberg, Van Kleef, and
De Dreu (2007) further showed that it is possible to manipulate diversity beliefs, with the consequence of better group
performance when groups actually were diverse.
Respects for Group Diversity
Once one accepts the fact that people are inherently multifaceted, and that the extent to which any given social category is
spontaneously seen as relevant can vary across contexts, it becomes clear that the level of diversity within a group depends
on which respects for diversity one considers. Consider a Black Christian woman who joins a law firm that is otherwise
populated by White Jewish men. Clearly this represents an increase in the diversity of the group on three demographic
dimensions, but do the majority group members register all of these forms of diversification? Do they all matter, and if so,
do they matter to an equivalent degree? Or might their relative importance change from one context to another? As
reviewed above, there are many possibilities. The problem of determining which respects matter for diversity assessments
is analogous to the general problem of determining the respects that should be used for assessments of (dis)similarity; the
shifting comparative context is likely to matter a lot (Medin, Goldstone, & Gentner, 1993). These considerations point to
the importance of considering subjective or perceived diversity as a causal variable having its own importance—perhaps
having more immediate causal significance in shaping group dynamics and performance outcomes than ‘‘objective’’
diversity; however that is defined (Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Zellmer-Bruhn, Maloney, Bhappu, & Salvador, 2008; see also
Harrison & Klein, 2007).
It may be that some of the inconsistent findings in the prior research on group diversity result from a failure to consider
possible divergences between research participants’ perceptions of diversity and the distribution of ‘‘objective’’ group
characteristics on which researchers choose to focus their attention in defining group diversity. Garcia-Prieto, Erwan, and
Schneider (2003) proposed a model of team diversity that explicitly acknowledges the possibility of shifting respects for
diversity within a given group. They argued that momentary social identity salience is influenced by a number of variables,
including motivational states (such as a desire for distinctiveness or assimilation; see Brewer, 1991), the situational
relevance (normative fit) of an identity, the visibility and contextual distinctiveness of a given identity (see Nelson &
Miller, 1995), others’ reactions and expectations, etc. Once salient, these categories can then determine which goals and
norms are salient as well as guide perceptions of other team members (Early & Mosakowski, 2000; Randel, 2002).
The same basic idea was taken up by Zellmer-Bruhn et al. (2008) in a study that examined changes in the subjective
diversity of groups that unfold over time. They focused on momentary perceptions of (dis)similarity with other team
members as an index of contextually defined subjective diversity. As in other diversity research, they distinguished
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
10
Galen V. Bodenhausen
between ‘‘surface-level’’ diversity, which they defined in terms of social category similarity, and ‘‘deeper’’ diversity,
defined in their study in terms of the perceived similarity of workstyles across members. When a group is newly formed,
Zellmer-Bruhn et al. assume that the most obvious social categories (sex, ethnicity, nationality) will shape perceptions of
social category similarity within a group, and these perceptions will in turn lead to assumptions about the degree of
similarity on less immediately obvious characteristics, such as workstyle. As these other respects for defining diversity
come into focus (in the course of interpersonal observation and information sharing during group interaction), subjective
diversity should change. More generally, any variable that leads people to focus on different aspects of themselves or
others should modify how and whether group diversity is momentarily experienced.
Managing Identity Complexities in Group Diversity
Given that the salience of different social identities can change within a group, it becomes important to consider questions
about how multiple identities can interact and influence one another within the group context. Taking as a starting point a
group that is in fact characterized by heterogeneity, how are these complexities perceived, both by group members
themselves as well as outsiders? Here, we can turn to the same basic possibilities that govern the perception of
multifaceted individuals.
Dominance
If a particular categorical dimension dominates perception of the group, then the crucial question is whether there is
variability within the group on that particular dimension. If sex were the dominant category dimension and all group
members were men, then it would not matter whether some men were Asian and some Caucasian; the group would be
perceived as homogeneous, or low in diversity. On the other hand, if sex were the dominant category dimension and the
group consisted of both women and men, then the group would be perceived as diverse. Dominance of this sort might
conceivably emerge when the group’s activities are strongly associated with a particular social category, for example if the
group’s task is a stereotypically masculine, agentic task. If women are present in the group, then it will be perceived as a
diverse group. However, it may not necessarily be the case that if only men are present in the group, then no diversity will
be perceived. Instead, in such a case, bases for categorization other than sex may suggest themselves to the perceiver
(cf. Eagly & Kite, 1987).
A presumably more frequent basis for category dominance in a workgroup context would be a strong identification of
the group members with the workgroup itself. If there is a sense of shared purpose, then the group’s functional identity
(e.g., ‘‘the computer support team’’) could dominate, leading again to low perceived diversity (we are/they are all ‘‘tech
geeks,’’ and race, sex, nationality, etc., makes little additional difference).
Compartmentalization
Compartmentalization, as applied to a group, would imply perceiving meaningful subcategories within the group that
would be salient to different degrees, depending on the context. If there is no strongly dominant category, then the respects
that are used for determining a group’s diversity should be quite malleable. Depending on the actual compositional
properties of the group, it could be perceived as diverse in some contexts (e.g., when sex is salient) but as homogeneous in
others (e.g., when race is salient). At the group level, however, circumstances may work against the dynamic malleability
of perceived diversity. In particular, the presence of multiple basis for diversity within the group can result in the potential
development of ‘‘faultlines’’ within a group (Lau & Murnighan, 1998). Faultlines emerge to the extent that multiple bases
for categorization coincide, such that an intergroup situation exists on more than one dimension concurrently. For
example, if it were the case that all of the women in a mixed-sex work group also differed from the men in terms of their
age (e.g., the women are younger than the men) and their ethnicity (e.g., the women are not White and the men are), then a
serious psychological faultline could emerge, and the tendency to see the group as being composed of two distinct
subgroups would be harder to resist. Compartmentalization would likely not result in dynamically shifting perceptions in
such a case, because any salient basis for categorizing group members would result in the same sorting of individuals into
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Identity complexity
11
subgroups. As previously noted, the research on crossed categorizations indicates that outgroup memberships tend to be
additive; thus, simultaneous recognition of multiple bases for outgroup status should enhance the tendency toward
evaluative differentiation between the two factions (Crisp & Hewstone, 2007). Available research in the context of
organizational faultlines suggests a possibly more complex set of evaluative consequences (Thatcher, Jehn, & Zanutto, 2003).
In particular, Thatcher et al. found a curvilinear pattern such that the presence of ‘‘medium’’ faultlines resulted in better group
morale and productivity than either strong faultlines or a lack of faultlines in heterogeneous groups (i.e., groups that are high in
diversity, but for which the various bases for categorization do not align in a manner that creates faultlines).
If the dynamics of intergroup evaluation are not necessarily as straightforward as relevant theory would suggest, the
possible consequences for stereotyping processes remain even more uncertain. As previously discussed, currently
available research indicates that stereotyping of multiply categorizable targets is often based on just one of the possible
categorical identities. If a faultline exists, which of the intergroup-defining variables will be the focus of stereotyping? Will
one of the categorical dimensions dominate the others in a stable manner, or will this shift across different contexts? In a
workgroup context, it may be that a particular dimension will stand out as being most relevant and may thus tend to
dominate the other dimensions in defining the relevant stereotypes. Perceived relevance may often be a function of
stereotypic expectations about the group’s task (e.g., seeing it as an agentic, ‘‘male’’ task results in an emphasis on gender,
while seeing it as a task requiring extensive experience results in an emphasis on age). Many important questions arise
about the malleability of the stereotypic impressions each subgroup may have of the other, and how potential contextual
shifts in these intergroup images might influence salient diversity beliefs (i.e., whether diversity is seen as valuable or
problematic).
Integration
Subjective diversity and diversity-related beliefs within a group should also vary as a function of group members’ social
identity complexity. When individuals see their own identities in a more complex and integrated way, they are likely to
perceive—and be more accepting of—diversity within the group. Individuals with more complex social identities may be
less likely to perceive a rigid subgroup structure within a group, because they are better able to appreciate shared identity
facets even when they differ from others in some seemingly fundamental way (Roccas & Brewer, 2002). Even if they do
end up construing the situation as one involving distinct subgroups, the consequences may not be as problematic among
people with more complex social identities. One of the critical factors determining whether diversity within a group is
problematic or advantageous is the extent to which members of different subgroups feel respected and recognized within
the overarching group (Hornsey & Hogg, 2000). As Brewer and Pierce (2005) have shown, individuals with highly
complex social identities are more accepting of ostensible outgroup members and thus less prone to fall into a negative
intergroup dynamic with them.
Research on identity integration in the case of bicultural identities is also relevant. Particularly in cases that
change longstanding precedents (e.g., cases where women and minorities enter into arenas that were historically the
exclusive purview of white males), the situation can be perceived as a ‘‘culture clash’’ and the individuals involved need to
negotiate a new, common culture in which all parties can participate. Until identity integration occurs, the distinct
identities may be perceived as oppositional, and individuals may feel that their social identities are not valued. Swann,
Polzer, Seyle, and Ko (2004) argued that people in groups seek verification of both their personal and their social identities.
They report evidence indicating that verification of personal self-views results in better interpersonal dynamics within a
group, while verification of social self-views results in greater commitment to the group as a whole. When an oppositional
structure is perceived to characterize a group setting (e.g., ‘‘I am a woman, but women are not taken seriously in this
group’’), individuals may perceive themselves to face a dilemma. Either they can attempt to distance themselves from the
undervalued identity, or they can maintain their social identity but feel alienated from the group. The prospects for
managing this dilemma effectively may depend on the individual’s level of social identity complexity. With relatively noncomplex identities, the situation may indeed be experienced as a difficult dilemma arising from oppositional aspects of the
self. Individuals with a compartmentalized social identity may be better able to navigate such situations effectively, because
they can temporarily de-emphasize particular identities while in the group setting and still feel a measure of social selfverification based on other aspects of their identity. Individuals with integrated social identities (‘‘merger,’’ in the terminology
of Roccas and Brewer, 2002) may be in the best position to deal with group diversity, because they can find bases for social
self-verification in many diverse ways and may not experience any sense of inherent oppositionality in the situation.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
12
Galen V. Bodenhausen
Along these lines, Cheng et al. (2008) recently argued that individuals with high levels of identity integration are best
positioned to realize the hypothesized benefits of diverse group settings. In terms of social-emotional processes, such
individuals should have the greatest comfort with diversity and should be much less inclined to see different social
identities as necessarily reflecting conflict or incompatibility. In terms of task performance, individuals with highly
integrated social identities have been shown to have better access to disparate knowledge and skill sets, making them better
able to contribute to problem solving and other aspects of group productivity (Cheng, Sanchez-Burks, & Lee, 2007).
Obviously the effectiveness of teams and groups is not strictly a function of the characteristics of individual members.
Even if a group is composed of many individuals with highly integrated social identities, psychological and socialstructural features of the task environment are still relevant in either affording the opportunity for successfully exploiting
the potential value of these individual assets or, alternatively, blocking their expression (Hornsey & Hogg, 2000; SanchezBurks & Lee, 2007). This suggests that in order to manage team diversity effectively, the working environment should be
engineered in ways that promote the recognition and valuing of complex social identities, allowing for group members to
experience social self-verification within the group. Such experiences may actually have the beneficial consequence of
promoting the development of more integrated, complex social identities within group members, for example through the
developmental mechanisms described by Amiot et al. (2007). One thing does appear to be clear—experiencing diverse
environments can carry important benefits for the individual. Antonio et al. (2004) provided evidence that greater racial
diversity within students’ social networks is associated with higher levels of integrative complexity in their reasoning, and
Leung, Maddux, Galinsky, and Chiu (2008) showed that the extent which individuals have been exposed to multiple
cultures predicts many important cognitive skills associated with creative thinking and problem solving. To the extent that
individuals feel valued by (and committed to) a group, they are best positioned to offer these skills in the service of
pursuing the group’s goals.
CONCLUSION
Social psychological research is increasingly coming to grips with the complexity of social identity within the individual,
both from the perspective of perceivers trying to form impressions and make judgments about multiply categorizable
targets, as well as from the perspective of actors using their different self-aspects as a framework for guiding their
interactions with the social world. I have reviewed numerous, disparate contributions to the effort to better understand
these issues, and I explored some of their possible implications for understanding the nature and consequences of diversity
within the group. One general conclusion suggested by the available evidence is that there can be reciprocal beneficial
relationships between social identity complexity within the individual and diversity within the group. Individuals with a
more complex sense of self may be better able to offer diverse cognitive skills and knowledge in a group task environment,
and it is this cognitive diversity that lies at the heart of the benefits of team diversity for group performance (Page, 2007).
They may also be able to navigate the group environment with greater facility and thus contribute to positive interpersonal
dynamics within the group. Conversely, experiencing diverse group environments provides individuals with opportunities
to develop greater creativity and more integratively complex self-understandings. In short, diversity in the person and
diversity in the group can potentially complement one another, resulting in better functioning at both levels.
However, much remains to be explored when it comes to these issues. The perceived and experienced sense of identity
remains one of the key explanatory variables for social psychological theory (e.g., Markus, 2005; Tajfel, 1974), and there
are still many issues to be empirically investigated concerning the ways whereby different self-aspects influence how
identifications arise, particularly in diverse group settings that afford many distinct opportunities for categorizing the self
and others.
REFERENCES
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Amiot, C. E., de la Sablonnière, R., Terry, D. J., & Smith, J. R. (2007). Integration of social identities in the self: Toward a cognitivedevelopmental model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 364–388.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Identity complexity
13
Antonio, A. L., Chang, M. J., Hakuta, K., Kenny, D. A., Levin, S., & Milem, J. F. (2004). Effects of racial diversity on complex thinking
in college students. Psychological Science, 15, 507–510.
Ashforth, B. E., & Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory and the organization. Academy of Management Review, 14, 20–39.
Banks, R. R., & Eberhardt, J. L. (1998). Social psychological processes and the legal bases of racial categorization. In J. L. Eberhardt, &
S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Confronting racism: The problem and the response (pp. 54–75). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Barden, J., Maddux, W. W., Petty, R. E., & Brewer, M. B. (2004). Contextual moderation of racial bias: The impact of social roles on
controlled and automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 5–22.
Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
42, 228–235.
Benet-Martı́nez, V., & Haritatos, J. (2005). Bicultural identity integration (BII): Components and psychological antecedents. Journal of
Personality, 73, 1015–1050.
Benet-Martı́nez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., & Morris, M. W. (2002). Negotiating biculturalism: Cultural frame switching in biculturals with
oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 492–516.
Bodenhausen, G. V., & Macrae, C. N. (1998). Stereotype activation and inhibition. In R. S. Wyer, Jr (Ed.), Stereotype activation and
inhibition: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 11, pp. 1–52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bodenhausen, G. V., Macrae, C. N., & Sherman, J. W. (1999). On the dialectics of discrimination: Dual processes in social stereotyping.
In S. Chaiken, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 271–290). New York: Guilford.
Bodenhausen, G. V., & Peery, D. (2009). Social categorization and stereotyping in vivo: The VUCA challenge. Social & Personality
Psychology Compass, 3, 133–151.
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. K. Srull, & R. S. Wyer, Jr (Eds.), A dual process model of
impression formation: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17,
475–482.
Brewer, M. B., Ho, H., Lee, J., & Miller, N. (1987). Social identity and social distance among Hong Kong school children. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 156–165.
Brewer, M. B., & Pierce, K. P. (2005). Social identity complexity and outgroup tolerance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
31, 428–437.
Burke, K. (1945). A grammar of motives. New York: Prentice-Hall.
Chakravarti, A. (2009). Kinship: Race relations. Nature, 457, 380–381.
Chao, M. M., Chen, J., Roisman, G. I., & Hong, Y-y. (2007). Essentializing race: Implications for bicultural individuals’ cognition and
physiological reactivity. Psychological Science, 18, 341–348.
Cheng, C.-Y., & Lee, F. (2009). Multiracial identity integration: Perceptions of conflict and distance among multiracial individuals.
Journal of Social Issues, 65, 51–68.
Cheng, C.-Y., Sanchez-Burks, J., & Lee, F. (2007). Increasing individual-level innovation through identity-integration. Academy of
Management Best Paper Proceedings.
Cheng, C.-Y., Sanders, M., Sanchez-Burks, J., Molina, K., Lee, F., Darling, E., et al. (2008). Reaping the rewards of diversity: The role of
identity integration. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00103.x
Chiao, J. Y., Heck, H. E., Nakayama, K., & Ambady, N. (2006). Priming race in biracial observers affects visual search for Black and
White faces. Psychological Science, 17, 387–392.
Crisp, R. J., & Hewstone, M. (2007). Multiple social categorization. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(Vol. 39, pp. 163–254). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Devine, P. G., & Baker, S. M. (1991). Measurement of racial stereotype subtyping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 44–
50.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S., & Taguy, S. (2009). Commonality and the complexity of ‘‘we’’: Social attitudes and social change.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 3–20.
Eagly, A. H., & Kite, M. E. (1987). Are stereotypes of nationalities applied to both women and men? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 53, 451–462.
Early, P. C., & Mosakowski, E. (2000). Creating hybrid team culture: An empirical test of international team functioning. Academy of
Management Journal, 43, 26–49.
Eberhardt, J. L., Davies, P. G., Purdie-Vaughns, V. J., & Johnson, S. L. (2006). Looking deathworthy: Perceived stereotypicality of Black
defendants predicts capital-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 17, 383–386.
Ely, R. J., & Thomas, D. A. (2001). Cultural diversity at work: The effects of diversity perspectives on work group processes and
outcomes. Administrative Science Quarterly, 46, 229–273.
Fausto-Sterling, A. (2000). Sexing the body: Gender politics and the construction of sexuality. New York: Basic Books.
Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum model of impression formation from category-based to individuation processes:
Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). New York: Academic Press.
Garcia-Marques, L., Santos, A. S. C., & Mackie, D. M. (2006). Stereotypes: Static abstractions or dynamic knowledge structures?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 814–831.
Garcia-Prieto, P., Bellard, E., & Schneider, S. C. (2003). Experiencing diversity, conflict, and emotions in teams. Applied Psychology,
52, 413–440.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
14
Galen V. Bodenhausen
Goff, P. A., Thomas, M. A., & Jackson, M. C. (2008). ‘‘Ain’t I a woman?’’: Towards an intersectional approach to person perception and
group-based harms. Sex Roles, 59, 392–403.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Anchor Books.
Golembiewski, R. T. (1995). Managing diversity in organizations. Tuscaloosa, AL: University of Alabama Press.
Harrison, D. A., & Klein, K. J. (2007). What’s the difference? Diversity constructs as separation, variety, or disparity in organizations.
Academy of Management Review, 32, 1199–1228.
Harrison, D. A., Price, K. H., & Bell, M. P. (1998). Beyond relational demography: Time and the effects of surface- and deep-level
diversity on work group cohesion. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 96–107.
Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39,
113–127.
Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2002). Are essentialist beliefs associated with prejudice? British Journal of Social Psychology,
41, 87–100.
Herman, M. (2004). Forced to choose: Some determinants of racial identification in multi-racial adolescents. Child Development, 75,
730–748.
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making: Cognition, culture, and the child’s construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Homan, A. C., van Knippenberg, D., Van Kleef, G. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2007). Bridging faultlines by valuing diversity: Diversity
beliefs, information elaboration, and performance in diverse work groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1189–1199.
Hong, Y-y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C-y., & Benet-Martı́nez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture
and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709–720.
Hornsey, M. J., & Hogg, M. A. (2000). Assimilation and diversity: An integrative model of subgroup relations. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 4, 143–156.
Hugenberg, K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2004). Category membership moderates the inhibition of social identities. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 233–238.
Hutter, R. R. C., & Crisp, R. J. (2005). The composition of category conjunctions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 647–
657.
Jetten, J., Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Spears, R. (2001). Rebels with a cause: Group identification as a response to perceived
discrimination from the mainstream. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1204–1213.
Keller, J. (2005). In genes we trust: The biological component of psychological essentialism and its relationship to mechanisms of
motivated social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 686–702.
Klauer, K. C., Ehrenberg, K., & Wegener, I. (2003). Crossed categorization and stereotyping: Structural analyses, effects patterns, and
dissociative effects of context relevance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 332–354.
Kunda, Z., Miller, D. T., & Claire, T. (1990). Combining social concepts: The role of causal reasoning. Cognitive Science, 14, 551–577.
Kunda, Z., & Thagard, P. (1996). Forming impressions from stereotypes, traits, and behaviors: A parallel-constraint-satisfaction theory.
Psychological Review, 103, 284–308.
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Science USA, 98, 15387–15392.
Lau, D., & Murnighan, J. K. (1998). Demographic diversity and faultlines: The compositional dynamics of organizational groups.
Academy of Management Review, 23, 325–340.
Leung, A. K-y., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C-y. (2008). Multicultural experience enhances creativity: The when and how.
American Psychologist, 63, 169–181.
Leyens, J.-P., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (1992). The ingroup overexclusion effect: Impact of valence and confirmation on stereotypical
information search. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 549–569.
Lount, R. B., Jr., & Phillips, K. W. (2007). Working harder with the out-group: The impact of social category diversity on motivation
gains. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103, 214–224.
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Milne, A. B. (1995). The dissection of selection in social perception: Inhibitory processes in
social stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 397–407.
Maddox, K. B. (2004). Perspectives on racial phenotypicality bias. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 383–401.
Mannix, E., & Neale, M. A. (2005). What differences make a difference? The promise and reality of diverse teams and organizations.
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 6, 31–55.
Markus, H. R. (2005). On telling less than we can know: The too tacit wisdom of social psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 16, 180–184.
Medin, D. L., Goldstone, R. L., & Gentner, D. (1993). Respects for similarity. Psychological Review, 100, 254–278.
Medin, D. L., & Ortony, A. (1989). Psychological essentialism. In S. Vosniadou, & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical
reasoning (pp. 179–195). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Mendoza-Denton, R., Park, S. H., & O’Connor, A. (2008). Gender stereotypes as situation-behavior profiles. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 44, 971–982.
Milliken, F. J., & Martins, L. L. (1996). Searching for common threads: Understanding the multiple effects of diversity on organizational
groups. Academy of Management Review, 21, 402–433.
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding ‘‘meaning’’ in psychology: A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception,
and social development. American Psychologist, 61, 192–203.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Identity complexity
15
Mussweiler, T., Gabriel, S., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Shifting social identities as a strategy for deflecting threatening social
comparisons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 398–409.
Nelson, L. J., & Miller, D. T. (1995). The distinctiveness effect in social categorization: You are what makes you unusual. Psychological
Science, 6, 246–249.
Oakes, P. J. (1987). The salience of social categories. In J. C. Turner, M. A. Hogg, P. J. Oakes, S. D. Reicher, & M. S. Wetherell (Eds.),
Rediscovering the social group (pp. 117–141). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Obama, B. (2004). Dreams from my father: A story of race and inheritance. New York: Three Rivers Press.
Page, S. E. (2007). The difference: How the power of diversity creates better groups, firms, schools, and societies. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Pauker, K., & Ambady, N. (2009). Multiracial faces: How categorization affects memory at the boundaries of race. Journal of Social
Issues, 65, 69–86.
Peery, D., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2008). Black þ White ¼ Black: Hypodescent in reflexive categorization of racially ambiguous faces.
Psychological Science, 19, 973–977.
Peery, D., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2009). Ambiguity and ambivalence in the voting booth and beyond: A social-psychological
perspective on racial attitudes and behavior in the Obama era. Du Bois Review, 6, DOI: 10.1017/S1742058X09090067.
Penner, A. M., & Saperstein, A. (2008). How social status shapes race. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 105,
19628–19630.
Phinney, J. S., Horenczyk, G., Liebkind, K., & Vedder, P. (2001). Ethnic identity, immigration, and well-being: An interactional
perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 493–510.
Pittinsky, T. L., Shih, M. J., & Trahan, A. (2006). Identity cues: Evidence from and for intra-individual perspectives on positive and
negative stereotyping. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 2215–2239.
Polletta, F., & Jasper, J. M. (2001). Collective identity and social movements. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 283–305.
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2006). Essentializing differences between women and men. Psychological Science, 17, 129–135.
Ramı́rez-Esparza, N., Gosling, S. D., Benet-Martı́nez, V., Potter, J. P., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2006). Do bilinguals have two personalities?
A special case of cultural frame switching. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 99–120.
Randel, A. E., (2002). Identity salience: A moderator of the relationship between group gender composition and work group conflict.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23, 749–766.
Richards, Z., & Hewstone, M. (2001). Subtyping and subgrouping: Processes for the prevention and promotion of stereotype change.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 52–73.
Roccas, S., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Social identity complexity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 88–106.
Rothbart, M., & Taylor, M. (1992). Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds? In G. Semin, & K.
Fiedler (Eds.), Language, interaction and social cognition (pp. 11–36). London: Sage.
Sanchez, D. T. (in press). How do forced-choice dilemmas affect multiracial people? The role of identity autonomy and public regard in
depressive symptoms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology.
Sanchez-Burks, J., & Lee, F. (2007). Culture and workways. In S. Kitayama, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology
(Vol. 1, pp. 346–369). New York: Guilford.
Sherif, M. (1948). An outline of social psychology. New York: Harper & Brothers.
Shih, M., & Sanchez, D. T. (2005). Perspectives and research on the positive and negative implications of having multiple racial
identities. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 569–591.
Sommers, S. R. (2006). On racial diversity and group decision making: Identifying multiple effects of racial composition on jury
deliberations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 597–612.
Sommers, S. R., Warp, L. S., & Mahoney, C. C. (2008). Cognitive effects of racial diversity: White individuals’ information processing
in heterogeneous groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1129–1136.
Stangor, C., Lynch, L., Duan, C., & Glass, B. (1992). Categorization of individuals on the basis of multiple social features. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 207–218.
Stryker, S. (1968). Identity salience and role performance: The relevance of symbolic interaction theory for family research. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 30, 558–564.
Stryker, S., & Serpe, R. T. (1982). Commitment, identity salience, and role behavior: Theory and research. In W. Ickes, & E. Knowles
(Eds.), Personality, roles, and social behavior (pp. 199–218). New York: Springer.
Swann, W. B., Jr., Polzer, J. T., Seyle, D. C., & Ko, S. J. (2004). Finding value in diversity: Verification of personal and social self-views
in diverse groups. Academy of Management Review, 29, 9–27.
Tadmore, C. T., & Tetlock, P. E. (2006). Biculturalism: A model of the effects of second-culture exposure on acculturation and
integrative complexity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 173–190.
Tajfel, H. (1974). Social identity and intergroup behavior. Social Science Information, 14, 65–93.
Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N., & Ruderman, A. (1978). The categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 778–793.
Thatcher, S. M. B., Jehn, K. A., & Zanutto, E. (2003). Cracks in diversity research: The effects of diversity faultlines on conflict and
performance. Group Decision and Negotiation, 12, 217–241.
Townsend, S. S. M., Markus, H. R., & Bergsieker, H. B. (2009). My choice, your categories: The denial of multiracial identities. Journal
of Social Issues, 65, 185–204.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
16
Galen V. Bodenhausen
Travassos, C., & Williams, D. R. (2004). The concept and measurement of race and their relationship to public health: A review focused
on Brazil and the United States. Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 20, 660–678.
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group. Oxford, UK:
Blackwell.
Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & McGarty, C. (1994). Self and collective: Cognition and social context. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 20, 454–463.
Urban, L. M., & Miller, N. M. (1998). A meta-analysis of crossed categorization effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74, 894–908.
van Dick, R., van Knippenberg, D., Hägele, S., Guillaume, Y. R. F., & Brodbeck, F. C. (2008). Group diversity and group identification:
The moderating role of diversity beliefs. Human Relations, 61, 1463–1492.
van Knippenberg, D., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Homan, A. C. (2004). Work group diversity and group performance: An integrative model
and research agenda. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1008–1022.
van Rijswijk, W., & Ellemers, N. (2002). Context effects on the application of stereotype content to multiple categorizable targets.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 90–101.
Wegener, I., & Klauer, K. C. (2004). Inter-category versus intra-category fit: When social categories match social context. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 567–593.
Wenzel, M., Mummendey, A., & Waldzus, S. (2007). Superordinate identities and intergroup conflict: The ingroup projection model.
European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 331–372.
Williams, K. Y., & O’Reilly, C. A. (1998). Demography and diversity in organizations: A review of 40 years of research. In B. M. Staw,
& L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 77–140). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (2001). Spontaneous prejudice in context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 815–827.
Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E., Maloney, M. M., Bhappu, A. D., & Salvador, R. (2008). When and how do differences matter? An exploration of
perceived similarity in teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 107, 41–59.
Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1–16 (2010)
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp
Download