Acknowledgements We thank Chun Chao, Hans-Peter Hartung andJean Merrill for providing preprints, and the NIH (SM), National Multiple Sclerosis Society (DLF, DJR), DGICYTand the FundacioM. F. Roviralta (LA,A6), NA TO (fellowship to EG)and the Health and Life Insurance Medical Research Fund(MLS) for support. Abstr. 18, 1004 24 Banati, R. B., Gehrmann, J., Schubert, P. and Kreutzberg, G. W. (1993) Gila 7, 111-118 25 Zielasek, J., Tausch, M., Toyka, K. and Hartung, H-P. (1992) Cell. Immunol. 141, 111-120 26 Boje, K. M. and Arora, P. K. (1992) Brain Res. 587, 250-256 27 Chao, C. C. etal. (1992)J. Immunol. 149, 2736-2741 28 Galea, E., Feinstein, D. L. and Reis, D. J. (1992) Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 89, 10945-10949 29 Simmons, M. L. and Murphy, S. (1992) J. Neurochem. 59, 897-905 30 Imai, T., Hirata, Y. and Marumo, F. (1992) Biomed. Res. 13, 371-374 31 Salvemini, D. etal. (1992) Br. J. Pharmacol. 106, 931-936 32 Salter, M., Knowles, R. G. and Moncada, S. (1991) FEBSLeft. 291, 145-149 33 Wallace, M. N. and Fredens, K. (1992) NeuroReport 3, 953-956 34 Koprowski, H. eta/. (1993) Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 90, 3024-3027 35 Murphy, S., Minor R. L., Welk, G. and Harrison, D. G. (1991) J. Cardiovasc. Pharmacol. 17, $265-$268 36 Simmons, M. L. and Murphy, S. Eur. J. Neurosci. (in press) 37 Moilace, V. eta/. (1993) Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 191, 327-332 38 Agullo, L. and Garcia, A. (1991) Eur. J. Pharmacol. 206, 343-346 39 Agullo, L. and Garcia, A. (1992) Biochem. J. 288, 619-624 40 Simmons, M. L. and Murphy, S. (1993)Am. Soc. Neurochem. Abstr. 24, 205 41 Vigne, P., Damais, C. and Frelin, C. (1993) Brain Res. 606, 332-336 42 Feinstein, D. L., Galea, E. and Reis, D. J. (1993) J. Neurochem. 60, 1945-1948 43 Merrill, J. E. et al. J. Immunol. (in press) 44 Corradin, S. B. etal. (1993) Glia 7, 255-262 45 McCarthy, K. D. and Salm, A. K. (1991) Neuroscience 41, 325-333 46 Ding, A. eta/. (1990)J. Immunol. 145, 940-944 47 Radornski, M. W., Palmer, R. M. J. and Moncada, S. (1990) Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 87, 10043-10047 48 Aoki, C. and Pickel, V. (1992) Brain Res. 571, 35-49 49 Nakane, M., Ichikawa, M. and Deguchi, T. (1983) Brain Res. 273, 9-15 50 De Vente, J. and Steinbusch, H. W. M. (1992) Acta Histochem. 92, 13-38 51 Matsuoka, I. eta/. (1992)J. Neurosci. 12, 3350-3360 52 Stamler, J. S., Singel, D. J. and Loscalzo, J. (1992) Science 258, 1898-1902 53 Nathan, C. (1992) FASEBJ. 6, 3051-3064 54 Nozaki, K. eta/. (1993) J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 13, 70-79 55 Kovach, A. G. eta/. (1992) J. Physiol. 449, 183-196 56 ladecola, C. (1992) Am. J. Physiol. 263, Rl156-1161 57 Wang, Q., Paulson, O. B. and Lassen, N. A. (1992) J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 12,947-953 58 Golanov, E. V. etal. (1991) Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 17, 27 59 Dawson, V. L. eta/. (1991) Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 88, 6368-6371 60 Nowicki, J. P., Duval, D., Poignet, H. and Scatton, B. (1991) Eur. J. Pharmacol. 204, 339-340 61 Yamamoto, S., Golanov, E. V., Berger, S. B. and Reis, D. J. (1992) J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 12, 717-726 62 Merrill, J. E. (1987) Immunol. Today 8, 146-150 63 Zielasek, J. et a/. (1992) Immunobiol. 186, 34 64 Boucher, J-L. et al. (1992)Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 187, 880-886 65 Ewing, J. F. and Maines, M. D. (1992) Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 3, 559-570 66 Geller, D. A. et a/. (1993) Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 90, 3491-3495 67 Nakane, M. etal. (1993) FEBS Left. 316, 175-180 Thecontributionof the amygdalato normalandabnormal emotionalstates John P. Aggleton John P. Aggleton is at the Dept of Psychology, Universityof Ourham, South Road,Durham, UK DH13LE. Lesion studies in monkeys have provided some of the most compelling evidence for the involvement of the amygdala in emotional and social behaviour. In spite of this it has proved surprisingly difficult to uncover the precise nature of the role of the amygdala. A number of recent studies now indicate that the amygdala is involved in a specific class of stimulus-reward associations and this discovery, combined with important anatomical findings, has made it possible to gain a much more detailed appreciation of the contribution of the amygdala to emotion in non-human primates. In parallel with this, it appears increasingly likely that amygdala dysfunction contributes to the emotional changes that accompany certain neurological disorders, including dementia and schizophrenia. The involvement of the amygdala in emotion was first highlighted by the extraordinary loss of social and affective behaviour that follows bilateral removal of this structure in monkeys 1. While the importance of the amygdala has been repeatedly confirmed since, its precise role remains elusive. However, recent anatomical and behavioural findings have significantly advanced our understanding of the functions of the primate amygdala. In addition, there is growing evidence that the amygdala contributes to various clinical conditions characterized by prominent changes in emotion. By integrating findings from these areas of 328 © 1993,ElsevierScience Publishers Ltd, (UK) research, new ideas concerning the contribution of the amygdala to both normal and abnormal emotional states are emerging. Anatomical considerations The amygdala, named after its fanciful resemblance to an almond, lies in the anterior medial portion of each temporal lobe. It is composed of approximately a dozen nuclei, each with a distinctive set of cytological, histochemical and connectional features 2. Many of its cells closely resemble those in adjacent cortical regions 3. The structure is further characterized by a complex array of intrinsic connections 2-4 and by the enormous range of neuroactive substances found within its boundaries. It was originally thought that the principal connections of the amygdala were with the hypothalamus, but it is now clear that the primate amygdala has many other dense, subcortical and cortical connections (Fig. 1). Studies of the monkey brain have shown that the amygdala receives direct projections from unimodal sensory cortex (visual, auditory and somatosensory), the afferents arising from relatively late stages in the hierarchy of sensory processing"~'5. In addition, it receives afferents from polysensory and 'limbic' association areas including the cingulate gyrus, temporal pole, superior temporal sulcus, insula, perirhinal cortex and frontal cortex a'5. Thus, the amygdala TINS, Vol. 16, No. 8, 1993 sponse TM. Thus, the animals are able to acquire and maintain general strategies for getting food as long as they do not depend on linking the memory of food rewards with specific items. More remarkably, amygdalectomized monkeys show normal learning rates for some pattern and object discriminations 13'18'19, even though such tasks should tax specific stimulus-reward associations. To account for this it is argued that normal monkeys can solve foodrewarded discriminations in several ways 2°. While they may choose the correct stimulus because it is associated with the intrinsic value of the reward (how good it tastes), they may select the correct item because it is linked with the external aspects of the reward, i.e. the monkey chooses the stimulus that leads to the sight of the food reward. It is supposed that the former mechanism is impaired by amygdalectomy while the latter is left intact ~5. Evidence for this distinction comes from the finding that amygdalectomized monkeys can perform well on visual discrimination tasks when they are required to displace the correct item to find a reward, Behavioural studies of monkeys i.e. they can use the sight of the food reward to signal Bilateral removal of the amygdala produces a a correct choice. In contrast, amygdalectomized permanent disruption of social and emotional be- monkeys show abnormal preferences among novel haviour (part of the 'Kliiver-Bucy syndrome'). From foods21, and are impaired on discrimination tasks studying its anatomical connections, the simplest when the reward cannot be seen 15'21 (Fig. 3). In both explanation is that amygdalectomy disconnects in- situations the animal must link the sensory features of coming sensory information from subcortical affective a specific stimulus with the palatability of food. Recent and autonomic centres 7'8. However, this account is findings indicate that this amygdala function depends insufficient since it overlooks the extensive amygdala- on its direct and indirect connections with the cortical projections and the particular patterns of ventromedial prefrontal cortex 22. learning and behavioural deficits associated with Although there is much evidence that amygdala amygdala damage. It has, for example, been reported lesions disrupt stimulus-reward associations, it is that while amygdalectomy can produce socioemotional quite possible that damage to this structure has some changes in monkeys only two months old9, these additional effects brought about by a corticalchanges become increasingly evident with age 9'1°. subcortical disconnection. A consideration of the Such findings imply a role in learning about social neuroanatomy of the amygdala (Fig. 1) strengthens information rather than simply a global loss of af[ective this possibility. Furthermore, the apparent lack of behaviour brought about by a lack of sensory inputs. responsiveness to stimuli that might be regarded as A more explicit proposal concerning the contri- innate, such as an air puff or being handled 23, along bution of this region to learning is that the amygdala with the abnormal galvanic skin responses to novel enables the formation of stimulus-reward associ- tones 24 or mild shock is consistent with a disconations 8'1~. These associations then help to establish nection rather than an associative deficit. While it is the emotional significance of external events, includ- undesirable to propose multiple impairments following ing social actions. Evidence comes from the disruptive amygdalectomy (i. e. a loss of associative abilities and effects of amygdala lesions on tests that tax stimulus- a sensory-affective disconnection), this may prove a reward associations. These include learning-set 12, fairer reflection of the complexity of the region. discrimination reversals 11-13, and win-stay loseRecent research has shown that the rhinal cortex shift 14'15 tests. The fact that amygdalectomized plays a critical role in anterograde memory25'26. This monkeys are unresponsive to stimuli that evoked is important as virtually all of the amygdalectomized emotion prior to surgery suggests that this impair- animals described so far received aspiration lesions ment includes a failure to use previously learnt via a rostral and medial approach that involves stimulus-reward associations. removing rhinal tissue. This additional damage may In an important refinement Gaffan 15 has proposed alter the interpretation of previous findings. Indeed, that the amygdala is involved in a specific class of there is good evidence that the apparent contribution stimulus-reward associations - those between dis- of the amygdala to memory tasks such as delayed crete stimuli and their intrinsic, reward value. nonmatching-to-sample and delayed response was a Examples include the precise association between consequence of such damage TM. Only a handful of how a particular food item appears and how pleasant it studies have used a dorsal, stereotaxic route to lesion tastes, and the link between a specific animal in a the amygdala, thereby sparing the rhinal cortex. social troop and its level of agonistic behaviour. The These stereotaxic studies have confirmed that lesions need for such a refinement stems from the fact that of the amygdala, but not the rhinal cortex, disrupt extensive amygdalectomized monkeys still appear sensitive to emotional responses 13'27, although reward or punishment 16'17, and are able to perform amygdala damage is required to produce the full normally on a number of food-rewarded tasks such as hypoemotionality~3. Stereotaxic amygdala lesions also delayed nonmatching-to-sample and delayed re- impair discrimination reversals ~3, so strengthening receives an enormous array of convergent sensory information (Fig. 1A). Given its numerous subcortical projections to regions such as the hypothalamus, substantia innominata, ventral striatum, and various autonomic centres in the midbrain 3 (Fig. 1), the amygdala represents an important route by which external stimuli could influence and activate emotions. This view had to be modified with the discovery that the amygdala has extensive projections back upon the cortex 3'5. These projections not only reciprocate the afferent connections but reach many other regions of association cortex (Fig. 1B). This is most striking in the visual system where amygdala efferents terminate in almost every visual region in the temporal and occipital lobes, including the striate cortex 5 (Fig. 2). In association with its many subcortical afferents, the amygdala may therefore be involved in the modulation of sensory processing by affective states. Finally, the lack of amygdala-parietal connections highlights the affinity of the structure with stimulus identification rather than location 6. TINS, Vol. 16, No. 8, 1993 329 A ,, // . Posterior thalamus SPf/VPMpc SG/PO/MGmc L~ Midline thalamus _LHA ,PB OLT VMH PB NTS/DNX RF AH OLT CS / IP ~°°°.°o ,,,.°o° 330 ~N~ Vo1.16, No. 8,1993 Fig. 1. (opposite). Summary diagram showing (A) the main afferent and (B) efferent connections of the monkey amygdala. The cortical connections are shown from a medial (left), lateral (middle), and ventral (right) perspective. The density of the cortical shading corresponds to the relative density of the various amygdala-cortical connections (black being the densest). The sylvian fissure (SF) has been opened to expose the insula. The amygdala, which lies in the centre of each illustration, is shown using a standard coronal section. Due to their complexity the termination sites of the cortical afferents to the amygdala are not depicted. Abbreviations of amygdala nuclei: AB, accessory basal; AHA, amygdalo-hippocampal area; B, basal; CE, central; L, lateral; M, medial. Other abbreviations: CC, corpus callosum; CIN, cingulate sulcus; CS, central sulcus; DNX, dorsal motor nucleus of vagus; INS, insula; IP, inferior parietal sulcus; IT, inferior temporal sulcus; LHA, lateral hypothalamic area; L TN, lateral tuberal nucleus; MGmc, magnocellular part of medial geniculate nucleus of thalamus;/vlD, nucleus medialis dorsalis; NA, nucleus accumbens; NBM, basal nucleus of Meynert; NTS, nucleus of the solitary tract; OL T, olfactory tubercle; OS, orbital sulcus; OT, occipito-temporal sulcus; OS, orbital sulcus; P, putamen; PAG, periaqueductal gray; PB, parabrachial nucleus; PO, posterior nuclear complex of thalamus; PP, peripenduncular nucleus; RF, reticular formation; RS, rhinal sulcus; SF, sylvian fissure; SG, suprageniculate nudeus of thalamus; SN, substantia nigra; SP, sulcus principalis; SPf, subparafascicular nucleus; STS, superior temporal sulcus; TC, tail of caudate; VA4H, ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus; VPMpc, ventroposteromedial nucleus (parvicellular part); VTA, ventral tegmental area. (Modified, with permission, from Ref. 5.) the link between stimulus-reward learning and emotionality. Clearly, further selective lesion studies are required, and it may prove particularly advantageous to use cytotoxins that spare fibre pathways2a. It will also be necessary to consider the impact of rhinal damage when describing the effects of amygdala pathology in humans. Clinical reports of amygdala damage in humans Selective amygdala damage is very rare in humans. The large majority of cases concern surgeries for epilepsy, behavioural disturbances, or both. Many of these used stereotaxic methods, often producing unilateral, subtotal lesions of an amygdaloid region that may well have been abnormal prior to surgery. It is therefore not surprising that some cases show no overt change in emotion28 . Other reports stress a decrease in aggressionzs'z9 or an increase in placidity and indifference 2s'3°. In one detailed report a woman described how her emotions felt dissociated following amygdala surgery31. Clearly amygdala damage can alter emotionality in humans, but it need not produce the dramatic changes observed in monkeys. One explanation for this species difference concerns the possible existence of additional corticallimbic routes in humans, so reducing the impact of amygdala damage. Indirect support comes from the finding that bilateral amygdala lesions in humans can leave galvanic skin responses intact3z'33, unlike the case in monkeys24. Furthermore, the extreme hypoemotionality of the Kltiver-Bucy syndrome is only observed in humans when there is a combination of cortical and subcortical temporal damage28. It is also the case that the extreme emotional changes observed in amygdalectomized monkeys can diminish quite markedly with timez3, so further reducing any species difference. As a consequence it is quite possible that the human amygdala, in concert with the prefrontal cortex, enables associations between discrete stimuli and their incentive reward in a manner similar to that presumed in other primates. While this remains to be tested, there is recent evidence that the amygdala may not be necessary for covert affective behaviour 34 (which may rely on striatal function). Most neuropsychological studies have concentrated on memory, revealing that selective amygdala damage has little or no effect on most tests 28. This fits with recent discoveries concerning the rhinal region and memory. However, amygdala damage can disrupt TINS, Vol. 16, No. 8, 1993 face recognition zs'31. This has been studied in particular detail in a woman with bilateral amygdala damage following surgery for epilepsy (Broks, pers. commun.). Although she shows no obvious loss of emotionality and is able to identify famous faces and match unfamiliar faces, she is impaired in her ability to recognize unfamiliar faces, to label the affective meaning of different facial expressions, and to identify eye-gaze directions. These findings may be linked with the discovery of amygdala neurones sensitive to faces3'~. The same subject also shows marked difficulties in judging those aspects of speech associated with attitude or emotions (prosody), indicating that her problems are not restricted to vision. These deficits, which have obvious links with the changes in social behaviour observed in amygdalectomized monkeys1, highlight the involvement of the structure with affective stimuli. The involvement of the amygdala in emotion may have an important bearing on several forms of dementia. In Alzheimer's disease the amygdala consistently shows neuronal loss, numerous neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques 36. These changes are severe and may occur early in the course of the v Fig. 2. The anatomical relationship between the amygdala and visually related cortical areas in the temporal and occipital lobes (letters correspond to cortical areas described by van Bonin and Bailey52). The amygdala receives a substantial input from the highest levels of the cortical processing hierarchy (area TEL while efferents from the basal amygdala nucleus terminate in all levels of visually related cortex in the temporal and occipital lobes. Intrinsic connections within the amygdala may permit a processing loop back to the visual cortices. Abbreviations: AB, accessorybasal nucleus; CE, central nucleus; L, lateral nucleus;/Pl, medial nucleus. (Taken, with permission, from Ref. 5.) 331 psychotic behaviour, often resembling paranoid schizophrenia42. Besides the presence of neurofibrillary tangles in the cortex, amygdala and nucleus basalis, the only commonly reported change was extensive gliosis in the amygdala (Fig. 4). 100 90 80 70 - ] I Acquisition ~ Reversal 60 o iii 50 \l 40 \ \ \ \ \ 30 20 2~ 10 i 0 1 2 3 4 5 Normal control 6 2 3 AM Fig. 3. Acquisition (Acquis.) and reversal deficit shown by monkeys with amygdalectomy (AM) on the performance of an object discrimination task for an unseen reward. The open bar represents errors made in 80 trials of initial acquisition, and the hatched bar represents errors made in a subsequent 80 trials with the reward values of the two objects reversed• (Taken, with permission,from Ref. 21 .) disease 36. In Pick's disease the amygdala shows intense gliosis and atrophy36'37, while in Huntingdon's chorea the amygdala can be considerably shrunken38. One of the diagnostic features of these dementias is a change in emotion. The most frequent change in Alzheimer's disease is an increase in passivity, and other common changes including an increase in agitated and self-centred behaviour as well as a loss of spontaneity and aesthetic appreciationa9-~1. In Pick's disease, apathy, irritability, or depression are often first symptoms37, while in Huntingdon's chorea changes such as euphoria, depression, or paranoia are prominent. In all three disorders pathology is present in multiple sites capable of influencing emotion. Nevertheless, amygdala dysfunction is a common feature and obvious similarities can be drawn between the hypoemotionality of monkeys with amygdala damage and the more negative mood changes associated with these dementias. Furthermore, in Alzheimer's and Pick's disease the emotional changes can be unrelated to the severity of the accompanying memory loss 37'4°. This finding echoes the differential effects of selective amygdala and hippocampal-rhinal damage in monkeys. Clearly the challenge is now to determine whether amygdala pathology is directly linked to these disruptions in emotion. A relatively high incidence of psychotic symptoms is associated with dementia. Of particular interest is the recent report of a familial presenile dementia in which an early symptom was prominent antisocial and 332 The amygdala and schizophrenia The possible involvement of amygdala dysfunction in schizophrenia has often been ignored; yet there is sufficient evidence to warrant a closer look. It is known, for example, that the lateral ventricles can be enlarged in schizophrenia and that this change is often most pronounced adjacent to the arnygdala43. Furthermore, post mortem studies have shown that the amygdala can appear shrunken 44'45, a change unrelated to medication. Further evidence comes from the connectivity between the amygdala and those other brain regions that show pathological or activity changes in schizophrenia, i.e. the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, cingulate gyrus and frontal cortex4s. Neurochemical studies also implicate the amygdala in the aefiology of schizophrenia. It is accepted that the therapeutic actions of neurolepfic drugs depend on their antagonism of dopamine D2 receptors. It is therefore relevant that the amygdala both receives a dopaminergic innervation from the mesolimbic pathway40 and has direct projections to other dopaminergic sites, including the ventral striatum and nucleus accumbens2. Furthermore, an increase in dopamine and its metabolite homovanillic acid has been found in the left amygdala of schizophrenics45. While this may be a consequence of drug treatment it would imply a somewhat surprising asymmetrical effect. Other Fig. 4. Coronal section showing striking atrophy of the amygdala (arrowed) in a woman who suffered from a familial presenile dementia associated with prominent psychotic behaviour. (Luxol fast blue-periodic acid-Schiff reaction-hematoxylin.) (Taken, with permission, from Ref. 42.) TINS, Vol. 16, No. 8, 1993 reported neurochemical changes in the amygdala include a decrease in GABA, a decrease in cholecystokinin and an increase in vasoactive intestinal polypeptide 4~. While it is unlikely that the malfunctioning of any one brain site will prove sufficient to produce the array of cognitive, emotional and attentional disorders associated with schizophrenia, the link between the amygdala and emotional changes in schizophrenia seems worthy of further study. Not only do schizophrenics often display inappropriate mood or a lack of affect, they also have difficulty identifying the emotional status of other people 47'48. These problems are reminiscent of those that follow amygdala damage, and it is tempting to postulate that they involve a dysfunction of stimulus-reward associations. This proposal seems all the more plausible as this associative function depends on amygdala-prefrontal connections 15'22, and it has been argued that abnormalities in frontal lobe function are a central feature of schizophrenia 49'5°. A further proposal that can be derived from animal studies is that the changes in affect and memory 51 observed in schizophrenia reflect pathologies in different regions (amygdala-frontal cortex and the rhinal-hippocampal region, respectively). If so they may prove dissociable. Finally, a number of similarities have been noted between childhood autism and the negative features of schizophrenia 5°. Indeed, it has been argued that common cognitive abnormalities may occur in both conditions~°. Furthermore, the behavioural effects of amygdala lesions in the infant monkey bear a similarity to those socioemotional changes observed in autism9, leading to the speculation that amygdala dysfunction contributes to autism9. From this it can be seen that there are a number of recent proposals concerning the involvement of the amygdala in a variety of disorders, all characterized by changes in emotion. The task now is to test for the involvement of the amygdala in a more direct manner. Selected references 1 Kling, A. S. and Brothers, L. A. (1992) in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J. P., ed.), pp. 353-377, Wiley-Liss 2 Price, J. L., Russchen, F. T. and Amaral, D. G. (1987) in Integrated Systems of the CNS (Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, Vol. 5) (Bjorklund, A., Hokfelt, T. and Swanson, L. W., eds), pp. 279-381, Elsevier Scientific Publishers 3 McDonald, A. J. (1992) in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J. P., ed.), pp. 67-96, Wiley-Liss 4 Aggleton, J. P. (1985) Exp. Brain Res. 57, 390-399 5 Amaral, D. G., Price, D. L., Pitk~nen, A. and Carmichael, S. T. (1992) in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J. P., ed.), pp. 1-66, Wiley-Liss 6 Ungerleider, L. G. and Mishkin, M. (1982) in Analysis of Visual Behavior (Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A. and Mansfield, R. W. J., eds), pp. 549-586, MIT Press 7 Downer, J. L. de C. (1961) Nature 191, 50-51 8 Aggleton, J. P. and Mishkin, M. (1986) in Biological Foundations of Emotion (Plutchik, R. and Kellerman, H., eds), pp. 281-300, Academic Press 9 Bachevalier, J. and Merjanian, P. M. in Innovations in Autism (Bauman, M. and Kemper, T., eds), Johns Hopkins Press (in press) 10 Thompson, C. I., Bergland, R. M. and Towfighi, J. T. (1977) J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 91,533-548 11 Jones, B. and Mishkin, M. (1972) Exp. Neurol. 36, 362-377 12 Schwartzbaum, J. S. and Poulos, D. A. (1965) J. Comp. Physiol. PsychoL 60, 320-328 TINS, VoL 16, No. 8, 1993 13 Aggleton, J. P. and Passingham, R. E. (1981) J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 95, 961-977 14 Spiegler, B. J. and Mishkin, M. (1981) Behav. Brain Res. 3, 303-317 15 Gaffan, D. (1992) in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J. P., ed.), pp. 471--483, Wiley-Liss 16 Aggleton, J. P. and Passingham, R. E. (1982) J. Comp. PhysioL Psychol. 96, 71-77 17 Hearst, E. and Pribram, K. H. (1964) Psych. Rep. 14, 39-42 18 Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R. and Amaral, D. G. (1989) J. Neurosci. 9, 1922-1936 19 Schwartzbaum, J. S. (1965) J. Comp. Physiol. PsychoL 60, 314-319 20 Gaffan, D. and Bolton, J. (1983) Quart. J. Exp. PsychoL 35B, 149-155 21 Baylis, L. L. and Gaffan, D. (1991) Exp. Brain Res. 86, 617-622 22 Gaffan, D. and Murray, E. M. (1990) J. Neurosci. 10, 3479-3493 23 Weiskrantz, L. W. (1956) J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 49, 381-391 24 Bagshaw, M. H. and Benzies, S. (1968) Exp. NeuroL 20, 175-187 25 Murray, E. M. (1992) in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Acknowledgements I wouldlike to thank And)/Young, David 6affan, Shirley Whiteleyand Paul Broksform their help. Partsof the researchcitedin this paper weresupported by grants from the MRC Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J. P., ed.), pp. 453-470, Wiley-Liss 26 Squire, L. R. and Zola-Morgan, S. (1991) Science 253, 1380-1386 27 Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R., Alvarez-Royo, P. and Clower, R. P. (1991) Hippocampus 1,207-220 28 Aggleton, J. P. (1992) in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J. P., ed.), pp. 485-503, Wiley-Liss 29 Narabayashi, H., Nagao, T., Saito, Y., Yoshida, M. and Naghata, M. (1963) Arch. Neurol. 9, 1-16 30 Balasubramaniam, V. and Kanaka, T. S. (1975) Confin. NeuroL 37, 195-201 31 Jacobson, R. (1986) Psych. Med. 16, 439-450 32 Tranel, D. and Hyman, B. T. (1990) Arch. Neurol. 47, 349-355 33 Tranel, D. and Damasio, H. (1989) Neuropsychologia 27, 381-390 34 Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. (1993) J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 79-88 35 Leonard, C. M., Roils, E. T., Wilson, F. A. W. and Baylis, G. C. (1985) Behav. Brain Res. 15, 159-176 36 Mann, D. M. A, (1992) in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J. P., ed.), pp. 575-593, Wiley-Liss 37 Cummings, J. L. and Duchen, L. W. (1981) Neurology 31, 141 5-1422 38 Zech, M., Roberts, G. W., Bogerts, B., Crow, T. J. and Polak, J. M. (1986) Acta NeuropathoL 71,259-266 39 Petry, S., Cummings, J. L., Hill, M. A. and Shapira, J. (1989) J. Geriat. Psychiatry Neurol. 2, 203-207 40 Chatterjee, A., Milton, E. S., Smyth, K. A. and Whitehouse, P. J. (1992) Arch. Neurol. 49, 486-491 41 Rubin, E. H., Morris, J. C. and Berg, L. (1987) J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 35, 721-725 42 Sumi, S. M., Bird, T. D., Nochlin, D. and Raskind, M. A. (1992) Neurology 42, 120-127 43 Shelton, R. C. and Weinberger, D. R. (1986) in The Neurology of Schizophrenia (Nasrallah, H. A. and Weinberger, D. R., eds), pp. 207-250, Elsevier Scientific Publishers 44 Bogerts, B., Meetz, C. and Schonfeldt-Bausch, R. (1985) Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 42,784-791 45 Reynolds, G. P. (1992) in The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J. P., ed.), pp. 561-574, Wiley-Liss 46 Kilts, C. D., Anderson, C. M., Ely, T. D. and Mailman, R. B. (1988) Ann. N.Y. Acad. 5ci. 537, 173-187 47 Cramer, P., Bowen, J. and O'Neill, M. (1992) Br. J. Psychiatry 160, 481-487 48 Bellack, A. S., Mueser, K. T., Wade, J., Sayers, S. and Morrison, R. (1992) Br. J. Psychiatry 160, 473-480 49 Buchsbaum, M. S. (1990) 5chizophr. Bull. 16, 379-389 50 Frith, C. D. (1992) The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 51 Saykin, A. J. etal. (1991) Arch. Gen. Psychiatry48, 618-624 52 Bonin, von G. and Bailey, P. (1947) The Neocortex of Macaca mulatta, University of Illinois Press 333