Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 In association with the Guardian and the Observer Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist The 19 best entries in this year’s competition 1 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Congratulations to our shortlisted writers – exciting new voices in science communication. Winners Stroke survivors – by Patrick Russell (B) The revenge of the Americas – by Katherine Wright (A) Highly commended Echoes in the sand – by Josh Davis (B) Fighting fit – by Laura Dawes (A) We may experience some turbulence – by James Bezer (B) Tactical manoeuvres on the viral battlefield – by Ben Bleasdale (A) Here comes the wild world – by Joseph Bull (A) Not monsters – by Sarah Byrne (A) The rhythm of life – by Abigail Hayward (B) Taming the twinkle – by Michael Hughes (A) What’s so funny? – by Yingying Jiang (B) A critique of sadness – by Pamilla Kaur (B) Blurring the line between life and death – by Fergus McAuliffe (A) Let there be light – by Kate McAllister (A) Termites and intelligent living buildings – by David Parr (B) The skeleton key – by Emma Pewsey (A) The new ‘balanced’ diet – by Maliah Roshan (A) Treating cancer with some help from the brain – by John Wilde (B) Windows into the mind – by Rebecca Winstanley (B) (A = entered in category for professional scientists; B = entered in general category) 2 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Foreword Did I enjoy it? Did I learn something? Was it easy to read? These, broadly speaking, are the judging criteria for the Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize. Our aim is to encourage new voices in the communication of science, to find writers who can explain science in the most engaging way, and to increase the number of people reading, thinking and talking about science. “Science really excites and inspires people,” said Hilary Leevers, Head of Education and Learning at the Trust and one of this year’s judges. “It was great to see so many entries that tackled challenging science in an informative, entertaining and above all well-written manner.” We received almost 600 entries this year. A team of volunteers from the Trust and the Guardian whittled those down to their top 19 – ten in category A (professional scientists) and nine in category B (everybody else). Then our esteemed judges got to work. As well as Hilary, the shortlist was assessed by radio and television presenter Maggie Philbin, psychologist Professor Dorothy Bishop, author and Observer features writer Carole Cadwalladr, physicist and broadcaster Dr Helen Czerski, and Dr James Randerson, environment and science news editor at the Guardian. 3 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist “The calibre of this year’s entries was very high and included some fascinating tales told with great skill, intelligence and passion,” James said. “I hope the winners and all those shortlisted go on to use their talents to keep writing and to enrich the national conversation about science.” The winners – Katherine Wright in the professional scientist category and Patrick Russell in the general category – each received £1000 and saw their work edited and published in the Observer and the Guardian respectively. The entire shortlist has now been edited and you can read all 19 pieces on the following pages. Shortlisted writers from previous years have gone on to write about science in a variety of publications, including several book deals, some even becoming fully fledged science journalists. The Science Writing Prize can lead to fantastic opportunities for those who want to keep writing about science, and we hope to see the work of this year’s shortlist in print and online in the years to come, too. Many congratulations to all! To find out more about the Science Writing Prize, please visit our website: www.wellcome.ac.uk/swp Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Stroke survivors: retraining the brain Patrick Russell Stephen Manning was head chef at a French restaurant in Notting Hill for 25 years. Today, he struggles to make a cup of tea. His wife Joanne intervenes when he pours water into a cup without a tea bag or forgets to add milk to his cereal. But when she is not around, life can be very difficult. It is not that Stephen doesn’t understand what he is trying to do. He knows what a cup of tea looks like. The problem is that he often struggles to remember the steps to make the perfect brew. Last year, Stephen was one of the 150 000 people in the UK who suffered a stroke, caused by a lack of blood getting to parts of the brain. The classic symptoms associated with having a stroke are physical. Patients can end up with paralysed limbs and problems with speech. But for Stephen, something much more subtle underlies his problems – and he is not alone. Of stroke patients, 68 per cent go on to develop apraxia and action disorganisation syndrome (AADS). Sufferers have difficulty in sequencing previously automatic actions, from washing themselves to making the bed. Although the patient’s movement is affected, AADS is primarily a disorder of the mind. Naturally people want to cure what they can see. AADS is hard to identify and although it is common, it has been overlooked in favour of physical stroke rehabilitation. Improved brain-scanning techniques mean it is easier to identify AADS. And now, psychologists and engineers have joined forces in a project that aims to help improve the lives of the thousands of people who suffer from this condition. 4 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist “Patients may have done basic tea-making tasks in hospital, but there is nothing to aid cognitive rehabilitation after that,” says Amy Arnold, a PhD researcher at Birmingham University who is working on the project, called Cogwatch. It aims to restore patients’ independence by developing personalised rehabilitation systems that can be installed into their homes. These systems will silently monitor patients as they go about their daily lives and provide advice to guide them when they make errors. It is hoped that patients will learn to sequence tasks correctly as a consequence. But designing this rehabilitation system has proved a challenge. Ultimately, patients will wear a watch that will monitor their movements. Electronic devices will be installed into everyday objects in their homes, such as a toothbrush or a vest. These will transmit information wirelessly to a central system. This will guide patients if they make errors, through sounds, vibrations or a visual screen. Manish Parekh, a PhD student who is part of the project, explains: “We are incorporating sensors that monitor grip strength or motion into everyday objects. This is the same technology used in mobile phones that detect which way up they are being held.” Another challenge is combining technology with the research carried out by the project’s psychologists. “We are trying to learn how healthy people normally behave and the kind of errors that occur in stroke patients,” explains Amy. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 To monitor how tasks are normally undertaken, the team has studied healthy participants. Sensors that can monitor complex movements were used to examine how they completed several tasks. This information can then be used as a comparison to AADS behaviour. Not everyone will welcome the new technology with open arms. Many stroke patients are above the age of 65 and may struggle to integrate technology into their everyday lives. “It has to be friendly enough to make patients want to use it,” explains Amy. “They don’t want lots of gadgetry and to press lots of buttons, or for it to take over their lives.” This is the reason Cogwatch is working closely with the Stroke Association. “It is great that they are addressing this problem but a system like this will only succeed if it is usable by patients,” explains Dr Clare Walton, the Stroke Association’s Research Communication Officer. “One of the concerns with this project is that the tech group will go nuts, developing all this amazing technology, but that it will be unusable – like developing a vibrating watch for a patient with sensation problems.” With the focus still on physical rehabilitation, this project, though still in its infancy, is quietly tackling AADS head-on for the first time. The disorder affects a massive percentage of stroke survivors, and for people like Stephen Manning, that fight could not have come soon enough. 5 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 The revenge of the Americas Katherine Wright In the 1490s, a gruesome new disease exploded across Europe. It moved with terrifying speed. Within five years of the first reported cases, among the mercenary army hired by Charles VIII of France to conquer Naples, it was all over the continent and reaching into north Africa. The first symptom was a lesion, or chancre, in the genital region. After that, the disease slowly progressed to the increasingly excruciating later stages. The infected watched their bodies disintegrate, with rashes and disfigurements, while they gradually descended into madness. Eventually, deformed and demented, they died. their genitals by 1494. What if Columbus had brought the disease back to Europe with him as an unwelcome stowaway aboard the Pinta or the Niña? Some called it the French disease. To the French, it was the Neapolitan disease. The Russians blamed the Polish. In 1530, an Italian physician penned an epic poem about a young shepherd named Syphilis, who so angered Apollo that the god struck him down with a disfiguring malady to destroy his good looks. It was this fictional shepherd (rather than national rivalries) who donated the name that eventually stuck: the disease, which first ravaged the 16th-century world and continues to affect untold millions today, is now known as syphilis. However, scientists, anthropologists and historians still disagree about the origin of syphilis. Did Columbus and his sailors really transport the bacterium back from the New World? Or was it just coincidental timing that the first cases were recorded soon after the adventurers’ triumphant return to the Old World? Perhaps syphilis was already present in the population, but doctors had only just begun to distinguish between syphilis and other disfiguring illnesses such as leprosy; or perhaps the disease suddenly increased in virulence at the end of the 15th century. The ‘Columbian’ hypothesis insists that Columbus is responsible, and the ‘preColumbian’ hypothesis that he had nothing to do with it. As its many names attest, contemporaries of the first spread of syphilis did not know where this disease had come from. Was it indeed the fault of the French? Was it God’s punishment on earthly sinners? Another school of thought, less xenophobic and less religious, soon gained traction. Columbus’s historic voyage to the New World was in 1492. The Italian soldiers were noticing angry chancres on 6 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Since the 1500s, we have discovered a lot more about syphilis. We know it is caused by a spiralshaped bacterium called Treponema pallidum, and we know that we can destroy this bacterium and cure the disease using antibiotics. (Thankfully we no longer ‘treat’ syphilis with poisonous, potentially deadly mercury, which was used well into the 19th century.) Much of the evidence to distinguish between these two hypotheses comes from the skeletal record. Late-stage syphilis causes significant and identifiable changes in the structure of bone, including abnormal growths. To prove that syphilis was already lurking in Europe before Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Columbus returned, anthropologists would need to identify European skeletons with the characteristic syphilitic lesions, and date those skeletons accurately to a time before 1493. This has proved a tricky exercise in practice. Identifying past syphilis sufferers in the New World is straightforward: ancient graveyards are overflowing with clearly syphilitic corpses, dating back centuries before Columbus was even born. However, in the Old World, a mere scattering of pre-Columbian syphilis candidates have been unearthed. Are these 50-odd skeletons the sought-after evidence of pre-Columbian syphilitics? With such a small sample size, it is difficult to definitely diagnose these skeletons with syphilis. There are only so many ways bone can be damaged, and several diseases produce a bone pattern similar to syphilis. Furthermore, the dating methods used can be inexact, thrown off by hundreds of years because of a fish-rich diet, for example. A study published in 2011 has systematically compared these European skeletons, using rigorous criteria for bone diagnosis and dating. None of the candidate skeletons passed both tests. In all cases, ambiguity in the bone record or the dating made it impossible to say for certain that the skeleton was both syphilitic and preColumbian. In other words, there is very little evidence to support the pre-Columbian hypothesis. It seems increasingly likely that Columbus and his crew were responsible for transporting syphilis from the New World to the Old. 7 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Of course, Treponema pallidum was not the only microbial passenger to hitch a ride across the Atlantic with Columbus. But most of the traffic was going the other way: smallpox, measles and bubonic plague were only some of the Old World diseases which infiltrated the New World, swiftly decimating thousands of Native Americans. Syphilis was not the French disease, or the Polish disease. It was the disease – and the revenge – of the Americas. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Echoes in the sand Josh Davis Spring sunlight shimmers off the wet sand revealed by the ebbing tide, rain patters on my coat as I scan Formby beach one damp Sunday afternoon. I’m with Alison Burns, an archaeologist from Manchester University, and a dozen brave locals. Eventually Burns finds what we’ve been looking for in a patch of brown sediment jutting out from under the sand. We gather round. It is the footprint of a large red deer. As we glance around, we notice many more – a herd. Had we just missed them? Further along the beach, Burns finds something else: a trail of human footprints leading into the dunes. But however carefully we follow them, we’ll never find the people that made them. Not left by damp day-trippers like us, these prints are tangible evidence of someone passing by 6000 years ago. was recording them. He has since documented more than 200 human trails and countless animal tracks, building a unique picture of the environment in which these people lived. The list of animal tracks includes species such as red and roe deer, dogs or wolves, boar, oyster catchers and cranes, as well as the extinct aurochs. During the Mesolithic, this stretch of coast had a large reed marsh protected from the sea by a sand bar. As people and animals walked through the soft mud, their prints were baked hard by the sun and covered in a fine layer of silt from the river flowing into the marsh. Over time the river migrated south, the sand bar disappeared. Millennia later, the sea now washes away the silt, slowly giving up this secret snapshot of ancient life. After a window of time, the tide returns to reclaim them. By determining the age of the sediment, scientists have been able to date the footprints to the late Mesolithic to early Neolithic (4000BC). This information can give us an incredible insight into what Neolithic people were getting up to in this marsh. Adult male human tracks are often found in association with those of red deer: it appears that the men were following, possibly even managing, the herds. There are other tracks which lead directly out to sea where the men may have been fishing. While the men were out hunting, the women and children appear to have been gathering food such as shrimp and shellfish from the marsh itself. There are even prints from children running and playing in the mud. Gordon Roberts, a local resident, has been researching these “ephemeral imprints” since 1989 when he first found tracks himself. He realised that there were many reports of prints but no one 8 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist But it’s the prints of our ancestors who walked this coastline 6000 years ago which are truly special. Rather than simply inferring a lifestyle from artefacts, we can glimpse the life of these people as it happened. From the footprints, Roberts tells me, we can deduce the approximate height and sex of an individual, and by the pace and stride we can calculate their speed of movement. Burns is just as interested in what the tracks don’t show. They span the Mesolithic-Neolithic boundary, when farming and animal husbandry were starting to spread within the British Isles. But these prints show a distinct lack of change. Men still wander out to sea to fish while women Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 continue to patrol the shore looking for shellfish. The footprints seem to show a community in stasis, unchanging while the world around adopts new techniques. Burns believes this is because there was simply no need to start farming or raising livestock here. The marshlands were as productive as ever, the game clearly still plentiful, and quality of life apparently high. So high in fact, that these communities were able to support the disabled. There are imprints made by people with deformities: missing toes, deformed feet and evidence of club foot. We might have thought these people would struggle to survive within a hunter-gatherer society but, on the contrary, the prints tell us they thrived too. These footprints offer us more than just hard facts and data. Standing on the beach on this Sunday afternoon, looking at the track made by a woman 6000 years ago, I immediately feel an intimate connection. I can’t help but wonder where she had been, what she had been doing. Was she returning from a successful afternoon collecting razor clams? Did she have children waiting for her in the forest? We’ll never know. Time and tide wait for no one. Leaning into the rain, heading home, we leave the returning tide to take away forever these amazing echoes of the past. 9 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Fighting fit: how dieticians tested if Britain would be starved into defeat Laura Dawes In December 1939, Britain had been at war with Germany for three months. U-boat attacks threatened incoming food shipments. And, armed with bicycles and walking boots, a group of medical researchers headed to the Lake District to conduct a secret study: if Britain was totally cut off from food imports, would starvation hand victory to Germany? The researchers investigating whether Britain could win the food fight were Cambridge University physiologists Elsie Widdowson and Robert McCance. When war broke out, Elsie and Mac felt they could use their expertise in food and nutrition to answer whether, if German U-boats crippled food imports, would Britain be dieted into defeat? This was an important medical question. Could the public stay fighting fit if food was rationed to what Britain alone could produce? If the ration was too low in protein, people would get ‘famine oedema’ (swelling from fluid build-up). Before the war, Britain imported half its meat, more than half its cheese and a third of its eggs. Much of the protein in the British diet would therefore be lost if a shipping blockade succeeded. Anaemia (insufficient iron) and scurvy (lack of vitamin C) could also become a problem. They decided to experiment on themselves. Four students and Mac’s mother-in-law also volunteered. They would pretend that a German shipping blockade had curtailed imports and they had to eat only British food. Everyone would get equal shares of the available produce. To work out what this might be, Elsie and Mac sought advice from Frank Engledow, a professor of agriculture who later helped set wartime food policy. British food production in 1938 became the basis for the experimental diet: one egg a week (a third of the pre-war consumption); a quarter of a pint of milk a day (half the pre-war consumption); a pound of meat and 4 oz of fish per week, assuming trawlers would be commandeered for patrols. No butter and just 4 oz of margarine. But they could eat as much potato, vegetables, and wholemeal bread as they wanted. The eight guinea pigs would follow this diet for three months. The rationed diet had to provide enough fuel for the long hours in factories and farms needed for the war effort. If people were too weakened by lack of food, infectious diseases would pick them off, just as surely as bullets. Disease played a key part in deciding who won wars. Famously, Napoleon lost his Russian campaign in 1812 after his army was decimated by typhus and dysentery. In total war, it wasn’t just the army who had to stay well to win. The home front also had to remain healthy. Having a sufficient diet was a medical issue that went to the heart of the war effort. 10 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Happily, the gloomy spectres of famine oedema, scurvy and anaemia did not arise. The guinea pigs felt fit and well on the ration and could do their usual work. But there were two main difficulties. One was that meals took a long time to eat. Wholemeal bread without butter took ages to chew. The sheer quantity of potato needed to make up calories also took time to eat. All the fibre in the diet caused 250 per cent bigger poos. They measured it. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 The other problem with eating all that starch was the amount of flatus – gas – that it produced. The consequences could be, in Elsie and Mac’s description, “remarkable”. sugar, meat and fish than Elsie and Mac’s diet. Convoys from America were able to run the U-boat blockade and flesh out British food supplies. To simulate the hardest physical work that might be expected of people during the war, some of the team headed to the Lake District for an intensive fortnight of walking, cycling and mountaineering. It was tough going with snow and ice on the paths. But other than a sore knee for Elsie, the team did well enough that a professional mountaineer rated their performance “distinctly good”. And this was on the diet that might be the lot for all Britain if shipping imports failed. Rationing during the Second World War caused problems – it was hard to cook inventively with limited ingredients, and queuing for supplies burdened housewives. But Elsie and Mac’s study showed that scurvy and starvation would not add to that burden. In 1940, the British government rationed bacon, butter and sugar, just as the team finished their trial. Their report and its conclusion – that Britain could stay fighting fit even if all food imports were lost – was circulated to government departments. But the study was kept secret until after the war. As more foods were rationed, the experiment provided assurance that home front health was secure. Had the conclusion been different, Britain might have had to decide whether to distribute the limited food equitably – and suffer the consequences of widely degraded health – or give more food to workers most important to the war effort. Elsie and Mac’s experiment showed this horrible reckoning was not necessary: Britain could afford to be fair and still be fighting fit. As it turned out, the experiment had been too severe. Rationing was always more generous with butter, 11 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 We may experience some turbulence James Bezer I always make tea in a glass. It makes it so much more exciting. Pouring milk into tea is one of those dull things we do every day without thinking, but looking at the liquid through transparent walls exposes intricate patterns, currents and swirling vortices created by the simple action of your hand. This complex motion is incredibly striking, and strangely beautiful. But what makes it all the more amazing is that, in peering into your tea, you’re gazing into one of the greatest mysteries in physics. Since the 19th century, we have had a pretty good theoretical description of how liquids and gases move around. By simply applying Newton’s laws describing the motion of solid objects to a continuum of particles you reach a few short lines of maths that describe the behaviour of fluids. These are the Navier-Stokes equations. In principle, these equations should apply in any situation. If you’re interested in a slow-moving or viscous fluid, they can easily be used to make accurate predictions about how it will behave. Under these conditions, flows are smooth and steady; a stream of dye injected into a current like this would remain together in a line throughout its length, without spreading about and breaking apart. Nice and easy. However, as with many things in science, it’s not quite as simple as that. Outside a narrow range of conditions, the system becomes rather uncooperative. Seemingly random disturbances are created in the flow, forming swirling vortices (called eddies), and making different parts of the fluid mix together. These disturbances, known as 12 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist turbulence, are why, if you injected a narrow stream of red dye into a river, it would quickly spread and turn the fish a lovely shade of pink. Turbulence affects almost all fluids in nature, to at least some extent. It forms the motion of the atmosphere. It’s why your heartbeat makes a noise. And it stirs up your tea. It’s also extremely complicated. The eddies that make up turbulent flows happen at all scales within the fluid: big vortices are made up of small vortices, which are made up of even smaller vortices and so on. But, unlike in some areas of science, we can’t just ignore what’s going on at scales much too small to see because we’re more interested in the big picture. All those little perturbations, growing out of immeasurably small variations in the initial conditions, add up and contribute to significant changes in the large-scale structure of the flow. This unimaginable complexity in almost every real-world situation means that the Navier-Stokes equations could only be solved by supercomputers many orders of magnitude more powerful than anything we have today. In fact, mathematicians still don’t have a clue whether there are some circumstances in which they simply don’t have a solution. In 2000, this was named one of the most important unsolved problems in mathematics, and if you happen to stumble upon a case where they don’t work, or prove that they always do, you would be lavishly rewarded with the milliondollar Millennium prize. So if the Navier-Stokes equations are too hard to solve in almost any real situation, what can we use Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 to model turbulence? Simplified forms are widely used in engineering and applied sciences, as they make the maths slightly less impossible in real situations. Another approach is to work backwards from experimental data about a flow to a mathematical framework that sticks all the numbers together. Both of these approaches are very useful, helping to design everything from jet engines to hair dryers, but in some circumstances, the predictions they make are not just wrong, but physically impossible. This inability to make perfectly accurate predictions about the real world remains hugely frustrating, and means engineers still rely on experimental data to see how the object in their computers would behave in the real world. You can rest assured that wind tunnels are still of vital importance in aeroplane design. This is what marks fluid dynamics out from many other areas of physics. Solid objects, for instance, can be precisely modelled using simple maths and fundamental laws. If you threw a ball in the air, a school student could predict, using its initial speed and angle to the ground, exactly where and when it would land. Yet turbulence remains a mystery; not only is the maths fiendishly difficult, but we still don’t really know what causes this weird phenomenon to occur in the first place. It is still the case, therefore, that we know more about relativity, quantum mechanics and the first nanosecond after the Big Bang than the movement of the liquids and gases that constantly surround us. Still using teacups? You’re really missing out. 13 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Tactical manoeuvres on the viral battlefield Ben Bleasdale “Let’s make them work for us,” Professor Laura Kasman tells me. She’s talking about viruses, the tiny replicating machines that infect everything on the planet – from minuscule bacteria to mighty elephants. Scientists have studied these remarkable replicators for more than a century, learning how to avoid them and the diseases they cause. Yet they remain one of the biggest threats to our survival as a species. Research into understanding and combating this threat is a global effort. This is war on a grand scale, and every advantage must be exploited. The fact is that we’re all jam-packed with viruses; when someone tells you that they’re full of cold, they’re actually just slightly more full than usual. The average, healthy human being carries around trillions of viruses with them every day. Mostly, we’re blissfully unaware of this ecosystem of competing, infectious predators within our bodies. Only when one gets out of control do we notice. The traditional, laborious approach for researching such pathogens has been to purify each virus and study it in isolation. Laura was trained in the same way. “That makes sense for finding out basic information about a virus,” she tells me, “but it was apparent to me that it had very little to do with real-life infections”. She thinks we’re missing part of the story. 14 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist In the crowded battlefield, it’s not just how viruses interact with their hosts – us – that matters, it’s also how they interact with each other. Yet this aspect of infection isn’t getting the attention it deserves: “The search terms ‘virus– host interaction’ found thousands of published articles [on PubMed], whilst ‘virus–virus interaction’ found zero,” Laura tells me. Not content to let this stand, she is pioneering a movement to harness this hidden aspect of infection, for our own benefit. Working at the Medical University of South Carolina in the USA, Laura has been spearheading a team that seeks to draw together the early findings within this underappreciated field. They hope this will empower researchers across the globe, spurring on new ideas and experiments that could ultimately lead to better antiviral treatments. Based on the same concept as Wikipedia, they’ve established the Virus–Virus Interactions Database, an open and free online hub for scientists to consult and contribute to. The encyclopedia, with its increasing number of entries, is already revealing exciting new links between research that was previously scattered far and wide. It shows, for example, how one virus can dramatically change the way another virus replicates, creating huge differences in the spread of disease across the world. Understanding the powerful effects of competition and interaction between viruses may hold the secret to new treatments, potentially changing the lives of millions. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 And Laura’s bold move is already being vindicated. After the Database was established, a ground-breaking study from Australia has shown that, surprisingly, getting sick might actually stop you getting sicker. The research revealed that patients who’d recently been infected with a common cold virus were protected from more severe viruses, like influenza, even after the cold had disappeared. This temporary protective effect seems to stem from the ‘smash-and-grab’ approach used by cold viruses, which leaves the body’s immune system on high alert, giving it a head start against incoming flu viruses. This was also an excellent example of one virus influencing another, proof for Laura that her work could make a difference for patients. “That was a really great feeling that we were able to predict it, and then it was there,” she says. Such results are spurring a new wave of research, aimed at protecting patients using nature’s existing tools – viruses themselves. This movement is starting small but, having planted the seed, Laura is optimistic that it can help other scientists uncover further exciting discoveries. “I hope that it will inspire virologists to move away from artificial one-virus systems and look for these interactions,” she says. This change won’t happen overnight, but Laura’s pioneering work has got researchers thinking and talking – the first step on a long road that could benefit everyone. 15 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist At the moment, only the tip of the iceberg has been glimpsed. The idea of getting a cold to ward off influenza is far from ideal, but we already know that our bodies are full of countless other viruses that give us no noticeable symptoms at all. It might be that, by helping the right side in this hidden battlefield of infection, we can harness these viruses to fight back against other dangerous diseases. As Laura succinctly puts it, “If we are going to be infected with all of these symptom-less viruses anyway, let’s make them work for us.” And why not, they’ve been fighting this war much longer than we have. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Here comes the wild world Joseph Bull The far northwest of Uzbekistan is a place of dust and wind and fermented camel’s milk, where there is never anything between you and the horizon. The locals joke (sometimes through gritted teeth) that it is not the middle of nowhere, it is the end of nowhere. This remote, semi-arid scrubland is an ancient arena in which tigers and eagles used to chase an ice-age relic: the saiga antelope. Now that the hungry herbivorous antelope are all but gone, poached to the brink of extinction, you might think there would be a lot more vegetation around. But the opposite is true: the wilderness is turning into a desert. “Beyond the Wild Wood, comes the wide world,” Ratty warns in The Wind in the Willows. How many children have read that and immediately wanted to explore the wild woods and the world beyond? But the Earth is not as wild as it once was and scientists are beginning to ask whether we have tamed the wilderness too much. Do we need wildernesses? And if so, can we rebuild them? Desertification is affecting many of the drylands in the world, which cover 41 per cent of global land surface and are where some 38 per cent of all people live. In Uzbekistan, the saiga antelope don’t only eat the shrubs, they also break up the soil with their hooves and fertilise it with their dung. This maintains soil structure and allows the vegetation to reseed itself, preventing the entire landscape from becoming a wasteland. So one way to help solve the desertification problem in this part of the world might be ‘re-wilding’: bring back the saiga and they’ll do the rest. 16 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Re-wilding is not limited to remote corners of Central Asia. There are far too many deer in the UK, for instance, because we have removed their natural predators. In such high numbers, deer undermine and overwhelm native woodlands that we need for flood control, carbon sequestration and more. We could carry on spending a large amount of money culling the deer every year, or we could consider re-wilding parts of the British countryside. Scientists have simulated what would happen if originally native predators, like wolves or lynx, were reintroduced into large fenced wilderness reserves. Get the area and the fence right, and they predict that the deer could return to manageable levels within a few decades. How far can we go with this new science of re-wilding? Once we begin to understand the roles that various species have to play in maintaining the landscape, the potential is staggering. Take the Oostvaardersplassen wetlands in the Netherlands. Here, animals have been introduced where they never existed naturally, in order to play a part that other creatures used to fulfil. Freeroaming ponies and cattle now take the place of absent wild horses and aurochs. Wetlands are an important component of the human water supply but the new, semi-wild species maintain wetlands better than human management ever could. It is only a short conceptual leap to the really groundbreaking idea of ‘Pleistocene re-wilding’. This involves reintroducing species to large reserves in places where they haven’t lived abundantly since the last ice age. Some scientists are exploring the concept of bringing leopards and Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 hippos back to Europe and introducing lions and elephants to the USA in place of long-extinct dire wolves and ground sloths. Others wouldn’t stop there, and there is a growing body of research into the possibilities for ‘de-extinction’, whereby DNA samples could be used to resurrect species such as the sabre-toothed tiger and woolly mammoth. Controversial? Yes, and not without challenges. But the argument here is the same as for saiga antelope and wolves: wildernesses, and the beasts they contain, play roles that support the economy in ways that people simply cannot feasibly replace with technology. We need wildernesses. That is not to say we shouldn’t heed Ratty’s warning about the potential dangers that lurk in the wild. But we should also consider what Badger had to say in that same story: “They built to last, for they thought their city would last for ever. People come…and they go. But we remain.” If we clear all the wild spaces, the cities we build in their place will be undermined and, eventually, will fall. 17 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Not monsters: neurodiversity, acceptance and changing perspectives on autism Sarah Byrne “She looked at it, screamed aloud, hit her hands together above her head, and cried out in despair, that this was not her child: It howled in such an inhuman manner that it was nothing like the child she knew.” (‘A Changeling is Beaten with a Switch’, Jacob and Wilhelm Grimm) European folklore is full of stories about changelings – children and babies supposedly stolen by fairies or demons and replaced with an alien version. These tales may actually have been about children with developmental disorders such as autism, in which a seemingly normal child can seem to suddenly regress and lose abilities such as speech. April 2013 was Autism Acceptance Month, the third since its founding in the US in 2011 as a response to traditional ‘awareness’ campaigns. We’ve clearly come a long way since the days of folklore, but we are still far from fully understanding autism. There are two big questions: what causes it, and what can we do about it? It is now generally accepted that autism – characterised by language and social difficulties and repetitive behaviours – is heritable. Other proposed causes, including parenting styles, environmental pollution and childhood vaccinations, probably contain about as much truth as the changeling legends. Tracking down the genetic causes of such a complex disorder is difficult though. There isn’t a single ‘gene for autism’. And even when we know 18 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist that changes in certain genes are associated with the condition, it is not straightforward to map these changes to actual behaviours or changes in the brain’s workings. Finding the genes – and we’re far from finding all of them – is only the beginning. Next we need to understand the complex interplay between them, and between these genes and our environment. We are a long way from a genetic test (and further still from any gene-related therapy) for autism. The other way of trying to understand what’s going on is to look at the brain itself. Brain scans of autistic and non-autistic people show some significant differences – and the more severe the symptoms, the bigger the differences. Although this is interesting, it doesn’t help us much with potential treatments. “The nobleman said to her: ‘Woman, if you think that this is not your child, then do this one thing. Take it out to the meadow where you left your previous child and beat it hard with a switch.’” The child in this fairytale got off lightly. Common advice in such stories was to put the ‘changeling’ in a pot of boiling water or on a fire, or outside to freeze or starve to death. The idea was that the mythical kidnappers would appear just in time to stop the sacrifice, and return the rightful child. Autism has no magical cures, and despite their promises, today’s alternative treatments – from diets to electric shocks – lack evidence, and can do more harm than good. Early diagnosis and intervention with behavioural therapies may work Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 better, but although this is a widely used and mainstream approach, the evidence is still surprisingly tentative. There have been some studies of hormone treatment, with the so-called ‘cuddle hormone’ oxytocin appearing to improve empathy and sociability. But now some are asking not just can we find a cure, but should we? A recent research study showed that autistic children actually reasoned in a more logical manner than their normally developing peers. The children watched an adult solving a simple puzzle, with some unnecessary steps included, and then were asked to repeat the process themselves. Most children copy the adult exactly, even when they know their actions are ‘silly’. The autistic children, however, simply cut out the irrelevant steps and performed the task efficiently – to which the only response can be: sensible children! Acceptance goes a step further than traditional awareness campaigns. Some talk about ‘neurodiversity’: the idea that autistic traits are just another way of being human, more comparable to racial or gender differences than a disorder to be pitied or fixed. According to this perspective, championed by many autistic people themselves, it is society that needs to change to overcome its prejudices and accommodate these differences. Others are more cautious, however, voicing concerns that such normalisation may stifle research, and disadvantage those who are severely disabled by their condition. 19 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Awareness and acceptance are needed, because too often the stereotypes haven’t moved on from the days of folklore. The screaming, unreachable, mindlessly violent monster of a child still persists in the popular imagination. So does the hope of a miracle cure that will restore the ‘real’ person stolen by the disease. Maybe now is the time to leave those fairytales in the past and realise that people on the autistic spectrum are not changelings or aliens, but valuable and contributing members of society. The human experience is varied and diverse. And that is not necessarily a bad thing. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 The rhythm of life: a powerful beat Abigail Hayward Birds sing in the trees. Whales sing in the depths. Humans sing in concert halls. Music is a quality that makes our species unique. Even if you haven’t deliberately gone to a concert, or plugged yourself into your iPod, music blares from cars, shops, bars and houses. Music is virtually inescapable. The world is never on mute. Practically all humans have the ability to perceive tones, harmony, pitch and rhythm – the fundamental building bricks of what we define as music. But where does this capacity come from? Are humans evolutionarily adapted to listening to music? What, when it comes down to it, is the point of music? This question has been asked many times, famously in Arthur C Clarke’s novel Childhood’s End, where aliens come down to Earth and are quite perturbed at man’s obsession with making and listening to music. They might have a point. Even Darwin wrote that “neither the enjoyment nor the capacity of producing musical notes are faculties of the least use to man”. For many years, the evolution of music was a murky subject, but within the last 50 years there has been a surge of interest. We are starting to identify where the mechanisms involved in music perception have come from, and possibly even the reasons why music perception evolved. A study conducted by Ava R Chase in 2001 found that goldfish had the potential to tell the difference between blues and baroque music. This suggests that an apparatus which evolved tens of thousands of years ago in these fish has, many 20 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist years on, been adapted to make music perception possible. For example, our middle ear bones, which now function in hearing, acted as jaw supports. Our lungs, which acted as a floatation control system in fish (the swim bladder), now enable us to vocalise and breathe. Sometimes people forget that current function doesn’t necessarily match ancestral function. The evolution of music didn’t start with sound perception. Instead, it developed as it became necessary, as tetrapods advanced onto land. Thanks to these mechanisms, humans have always had the ability to perceive and distinguish between different sounds. However, as is true today, this occurs to different degrees – some people are just more naturally musical than others. If music is an adaptation, what aspects of it allowed more musical people to outcompete people with lesser musical abilities? Firstly, there is Steven Pinker’s “auditory cheesecake” hypothesis. The idea was that music stimulated our pleasure centres, “an exquisite confection crafted to tickle the sensitive spots of... our mental faculties”. Although it attracted a lot of criticism at the time of its proposal, recent research has given this hypothesis more credibility. It has been shown that listening to music triggers the brain to release the chemical dopamine, which is part of a reward-driven learning process (in other words, learning is ‘rewarded’ with dopamine). In this way, music may actuate and actually develop our mental capacities. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Music could also be a factor in mate choice. Darwin himself mused that people “endeavoured to charm each other with musical notes and rhythm”. Music doesn’t seem evolutionary helpful, as it may attract predators. However, musical aptitude requires a large mental capacity, or ease of learning, which would have allowed ancient man to evade predators. Learning music also requires a large amount of patience, which is an attractive trait in terms of establishing a longlasting relationship. Patience can also be applied to the skills needed to raise a family. Singing lullabies could be considered to be one of these skills and is this is actually the only genre of music that can be considered universal. Lullabies can be found across all cultures, ranging from Sweden’s Mors Lilla Olle, to Kenya’s Rock, Rock, Rock. Mother–infant song has roots in ancient times, and is another possible reason for the evolution of music. Firstly, lullabies are a means of communication between mother and child – the language is simple, making them easy to learn and understand. Lullabies also stimulate the area of the brain responsible for perception, which could be important for the development of the mind. Given that an infant’s cries could attract unwanted attention, being able to soothe them would surely be adaptive to save the family from predation. 21 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Music may be an adaptive trait. You may choose the logic that everything happens for a reason. That music must have given people an evolutionary advantage, as it still persists today. But what we may not be able to conclusively explain is this: after a bird serenades his potential partner, he can just fly away. After a whale’s last song, he sinks back to the depths. But at the end of a musical performance, a person can sit with tears in their eyes, unable to explain why. Music has done something to us emotionally as a species. And it’s something we may never be able to truly fathom. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Taming the twinkle Michael Hughes For poets and composers of nursery rhymes, the twinkling of the stars is a delight. For astronomers, it’s a disaster. Try to take a photograph of a star through a telescope and you’ll find out why. The constant motion and distortion of the light corrupts the image, blurring out small features. We don’t even need a telescope to see this twinkling, just find a break in the clouds and take a look at the night sky. The stars seem to dance around, left to right, up and down, sometimes fainter and sometimes brighter, never quite seeming to rest in one place. If you were to travel a few hundred miles straight up, and sit just outside the Earth’s atmosphere, the twinkling would stop. Now the stars would be fixed points: steady, constant and unmoving. Their light has travelled this way across the vastness of space, undisturbed and undistorted. It’s only in the final moments of its journey, passing through our turbulent atmosphere, that it is bent and twisted out of shape. What scientists call the resolution of an image – the minimum separation needed between two objects before they blur together – is limited by the atmosphere. No matter how expensive the telescope, or how large its mirrors and lenses, the limit doesn’t change. This is one reason why billions of dollars were spent putting the Hubble Space Telescope into orbit, safely above the influence of the atmosphere. Much to the relief of government bean-counters, there is another way. It’s called adaptive optics. The idea is to ‘undo’ the effect of the atmosphere 22 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist by reversing the distortions it causes. A ‘deformable’ mirror is added to the telescope. The surface of this kind of mirror can be reshaped under computer control. So, if the distortions from the atmosphere can be measured, then the opposite distortion can be simulated by the deformable mirror, and so much of the effect of the atmosphere can be cancelled out. If it sounds simple, then think again. Firstly, because the atmosphere is constantly shifting, so are the distortions. This means that a new shape for the deformable mirror has to be calculated and applied many times a second. Secondly, the instruments used to measure the distortions, called wavefront sensors, need a relatively bright light source to work on. The objects that the astronomers actually want to study usually aren’t suitable, so they need to find a second, brighter star in the same region of the sky. The distortions measured using this ‘guide-star’ are then used to correct the interesting image. Unfortunately, this all relies on there being a bright-enough star close by. The further away the guide-star, the poorer the correction will be. Not content to live with this limitation, astronomers have resorted to making their own guide-stars by bouncing an intense laser beam off the upper atmosphere. This allows them to correct images from any point in the sky. While adaptive optics systems are allowing us to probe ever deeper into space, they can also be used to deal with troublesome optical distortions closer to home. Modern microscopy techniques allow Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 scientists to look several millimetres into biological tissue. But, like the atmosphere, the tissue distorts the focus of the light, limiting the penetration and resolution of the microscope. Scientists around the world are now using adaptive optics techniques to improve the quality of their images. Some retinal imaging cameras now include systems to correct for distortions caused by the eye, allowing individual photo-receptors – the light-sensitive cells on our retinas – to be seen. And as costs fall, we are even beginning to see simple adaptive optics built into consumer electronics devices such as DVD players. While we might not notice adaptive optics in these everyday devices, there is something on the horizon that will be difficult to miss. The European Southern Observatory, a 15-nation organisation that funds observatories in Chile, has approved construction of the European Extremely Large Telescope. When it becomes operational in the early 2020s, it will be the largest optical telescope in the world, boasting a primary mirror nearly 40 metres across. Just as important will be its adaptive optics system, which is likely to include two deformable mirrors and multiple laser guide-stars. Without these, the telescope would be an expensive folly. With them, it will be able to track down Earth-like planets orbiting nearby stars, and study galaxies at the edge of the known universe. We don’t yet know what these stars and galaxies will tell us about our place in the universe. But one thing is for sure: thanks to adaptive optics, nothing will be twinkling. 23 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 What’s so funny? Yingying Jiang What causes irregular breathing, is highly contagious and can start an epidemic? No, not a new strain of bird flu, but something much more pleasant: laughter. Whether it’s the chortling of a baby in a pushchair, a group of giggling teenagers on the bus or a hearty belly laugh in the office as someone cracks a joke, we laugh and hear laughter every day. But for a behaviour that is so easy and natural, it raises some intriguing questions that scientists don’t yet fully understand: Why do we laugh? Are laughing individuals ‘fitter’ for survival? Why does the sight of strangers in hysterics set us giggling too? Just what is so funny? Why we laugh seems obvious at first – it’s because we find something amusing. However, the next time you are out among groups of people, listen carefully to when they actually laugh, and you might notice that laughter and humour are not inseparable. In a study of laughter, Robert Provine, professor of psychology and neuroscience at the University of Maryland, Baltimore County, recorded hours of real conversations in public places. From 1200 ‘laugh episodes’, he found that only 10 to 20 per cent of laughs followed what seemed like attempts at humour. The majority of chuckles were generated by completely banal statements like ‘I’ll see you guys later’ and ‘I think I’m done’. Are we just being polite when we titter at these non-witticisms? After his initial study, Provine soon realised that the essential ingredient of laughter is not a punch line but another person. Laughter is a social 24 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist activity. In the absence of anything that tries to make us laugh, like a film comedy or a stand-up routine, we are 30 times more likely to laugh when we’re in company than when alone. You might think that your laughter is solely a form of self-expression, but in fact it serves a more fundamental purpose. It elicits positive feelings in other people. Brain scans have revealed that the sound of laughter triggers a response in the part of our brain that is activated when we smile, thus priming our facial muscles for laughter. Just as we yawn when others yawn, we also feel the urge to laugh when we see other people laugh. Laughter is highly contagious. Just search for ‘laughing weatherwoman’ on YouTube and try to keep a straight face. A common misconception about laughter is that it is unique to humans, but a look at our ape relatives would suggest that the human laugh might be an echo of the chimpanzee chuckle. To investigate the origins of laughter, psychologist Marina Davila-Ross and her team at the University of Portsmouth conducted what might just be the greatest experiment in the world: tickling primates. Juvenile orang-utans, chimps, gorillas and bonobos, as well as humans, were tickled in the armpits, palms and feet for their reaction. DavilaRoss found that the apes responded in the same way as human children when being tickled: squirming and vocalising laugh-like sounds. For example, a chimp’s laugh is rapid and breathy, which has been dubbed ‘play-panting’. Though the vocalisation was different between species, Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 probably due to anatomical differences, this variability matched the well-established genetic relationship between them, suggesting that our laughter comes from the last common ancestor of great apes and humans. It’s easy to understand why natural selection might favour individuals with a strong fight-orflight response, but what might be the evolutionary advantage of laughing? According to Jaak Panksepp, a neuroscientist and psychologist at Washington State University, primal laughter could have evolved as a ‘signalling device’, giving an honest indication of an individual’s emotional health, just as a peacock’s tail feathers indicate physical health. Laughter can be used to show readiness for friendly interaction, as well as to defuse tension. Panksepp also believes that laughing animals may have appeared more attractive to the opposite sex, since laughter indicates a positive temperament, leading to interaction and ultimately to reproduction. On a less positive note, mocking, jeering laughter towards outsiders can be used to reinforce a group’s solidarity – the difference between laughing with and laughing at someone. Laughter is therefore a vital social lubricant, fostering a sense of group unity while maintaining a subtle status hierarchy, both of which could have been especially important for small groups of early humans. 25 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist The human brain is still a complex puzzle. With the development of higher cognitive functions, the repertoire of things that trigger our laughter has increased since our ancestors’ time. Although humans may have the last laugh in the evolutionary sense, we still have a long way to go to fully understand the mystery of laughter. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 A critique of sadness Pamilla Kaur Signs of heartbreak, loss or any other form of emotional pain that sends your lacrimal glands into overdrive, emotional tears are unique to humans. You won’t come home to find your dog having a breakdown to a Celine Dion CD, or turn to see your cat’s eyeballs secreting tears when Rose lets go of Jack in Titanic. Even though animals can experience sadness, they don’t produce tears like we can, in response to films or music for example, and that’s pretty crucial. Charles Darwin was one of the first scientists to investigate crying by studying animals such as Indian elephants, but he never once saw them emotionally tearing or met anyone who could testify to such an event. Like us, animals produce tears as a reflex to foreign bodies that enter the eye, such as dust particles, which makes sense: an enzyme in tears is essentially the Pac-Man of the biology world, engulfing bacteria to maintain the health of the eye. However, why should sad situations from films to funerals cause humans to reach for the tissues too? What is it about sadness that can make us produce the same teary response as to a physical irritant? Research in this field is sadly lacking. It could provide us with some fundamental information about the behaviour of humans but, considering the complex neurological stimulation involved, it’s no wonder scientists still find the process of crying to be elusive. Nevertheless, scientists are trying, and some of the most interesting research is the result of seemingly bizarre methods. A study by Israeli scientists from 26 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist the Weizmann Institute of Science involved bottling tears produced by women welling up over a sad film, then asking male test subjects to inhale the vapour. The men’s testosterone levels decreased as a direct result of inhaling the tear vapour, suggesting tears are a chemical signal of sadness. Subsequently, other scientists have theorised about the evolutionary significance of this. Lower levels of testosterone reduce the amount of anger and arousal a male feels. Perhaps, as humans evolved to live in social groups, the chemical signal released by crying allowed other humans, especially men, to protect those who were more vulnerable. As well as a chemical signal, tears are physical signs of sadness. Studies at the University of Maryland showed that viewing images of crying faces with the tears Photoshopped out didn’t generate the same level of empathy in their test subjects as images with the tears in. This shows it is the actual tears that are perceived as a signal of sadness, not just the crumpled, dissolving facial expressions that act as a fleshy canvas for your salty tears. Probably the most familiar role of tears is to provide a sense of relief. There is a lot of scientific debate on whether crying provides a feeling of calmness. On the one hand, nervous activity has been shown to increase with tears. However, anyone who’s ever sobbed their heart out after a bad day will know the comforting feeling of contentment after having a good cry. So some Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 scientists (maybe those who have had a bad day recently) do believe tears can be interpreted as an evolutionary coping mechanism to expel pent-up emotions through stress hormones and toxins, as well as to express our feelings to others. It’s not hard to see how this could have been particularly useful before the evolution of human language: watching someone cry is powerful in itself. What is clear, though, is that research in this field is helping scientists understand more about how we behave. So the next time Jack and Rose reduce you to a snivelling, slobbering mess, remember that it’s because complex and fundamental biological processes are at play, that potentially evolved to protect you from danger. 27 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Blurring the line between life and death Fergus McAuliffe How clear is the line between life and death? At first glance, the distinction seems quite clear. We all have a beating heart – it is what keeps us alive. If your heart stopped beating, you’d be in cardiac arrest. This would, left untreated, lead to your death. When spring begins and the temperature rises, something remarkable happens. The frogs begin to thaw out. Their hearts start to beat again, and their lungs start to breathe. In a matter of hours the wood frog is hopping around like nothing happened. But what if an animal didn’t need a beating heart to be alive? So, if a wood frog can do this, why can’t we? When the cells in our bodies begin to freeze they dehydrate. Water is sucked out of the cells and freezes in the area around them. With less water inside them to provide shape and structure, the cells collapse, split and die. Even if these cells thaw out they can no longer function. It is this mechanism that kills human fingers and toes in severe cases of frostbite. To explore this question we must go on a journey all the way to the cold, dark forests of northern Canada. This isn’t a story of fantasy or of fairytale, but it does involve a frog. Each winter the wood frog (Rana sylvaica) blurs the line between life and death as it hibernates. Unlike humans, frogs are cold-blooded. Their body temperature closely follows the temperature of their environment. The wood frog is the only species of North American frog that lives north of the Arctic Circle. It has evolved in such a way that it can survive this freezing environment. As the temperature drops below zero degrees Centigrade at the onset of winter, the wood frog starts to freeze. First its skin cells freeze, then its legs. Next is the chest, head and – finally – the heart. The wood frog has no heartbeat or pulse and exists in cardiac arrest. It stays in this deathlike state for weeks at a time. With up to 70 per cent of their bodies frozen solid, these amphibians are so inanimate that they don’t even respond to the kiss of a princess. 28 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Rather than migrating to avoid the cold weather, like so many other animals do, the wood frog has evolved a safety mechanism to elude the lethal effects of freezing. This mechanism has become the focus of research by Professor Ken Storey and Janet Storey of Carleton University, Ottawa, Canada. Through years of observation and experimentation, the Storeys have elucidated many of the biological pathways that the wood frogs use to enable them to freeze and thaw. When frogs begin to freeze, their organs start to dehydrate. Water lost in this way freezes in the frog’s internal spaces such as the abdominal cavity. By ensuring that water freezes in these spaces and not around the cells (as in humans), the frogs limit the damage to their organs caused by ice crystals. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 But why don’t the dehydrated cells in the frog die? As water leaves the cells it is replaced with a simple sugar, glucose, that the wood frog makes in its liver. This glucose combines with the remaining water inside the cells to form a natural sugary antifreeze, or cryo-protectant, which keeps the cell’s vital machinery from freezing. While up to 70 per cent of the water in the wood frog is frozen solid in cavities, the crucial parts in the inside of the cells are protected and retain their function. While preparing to hibernate, the wood frog starts making extra glucose in its liver. Its forward planning is so good that glucose levels can be up to 40 times higher at the moment of freezing onset than during the summer. While these high levels of glucose would be toxic to human cells, this is not the case with the wood frog. In the spring, when the frog thaws out, water re-enters the cells, which then resume their normal function. The glucose is removed and excreted from the body in sugary urine. This freeze–thaw adaptation allows the wood frog to survive, not by avoiding the cold but by embracing it. This small creature – no longer than two inches in length and found only in North America – challenges our understanding of what it means to be alive: the beating heart. This is how the wood frog is blurring the line between life and death. Not by freezing to death, but freezing to live. 29 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Let there be light Kate McAllister The most scientific thing most people would associate with flickering office lights would perhaps be the direct correlation between flickering and expletives uttered. As it turns out, however, the strip lights barely illuminating my workspace might also be having some sinister effects on my physiology. A growing area of science known as chronobiology is beginning to understand how light is involved in almost everything, from how we feel to how we age. We are evolved to sleep when it is dark and to be alert in the day. Scientists have known for decades that our daily cycle, or circadian rhythm, is carefully controlled by internal timers. The master clock, the suprachiasmatic nuclei or SCN, is found deep in the ancient parts of our brains. The ticking over of this internal clock is entwined with external cues, which scientists refer to as zeitgebers, or time givers. For almost all animals, the main zeitgeber is light – bright sunshine is our cue to leap out of bed and get to it. If we don’t get enough light our physiology is knocked out of whack, impacting on everything from urine production to body temperature. Families of people with dementias and other degenerative diseases won’t be surprised to hear that disrupted sleep is one the primary reasons that patients have to be taken in to residential care. Daytime napping and disrupted sleep through the night are distressing and disorienting for families and patients alike. Circadian rhythm disruption has also been associated with ‘sundowning’, the tendency for people with dementia to experience confusion, mood swings and agitation through the evening. 30 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Our circadian rhythms change as we age, with sleep abnormalities common in the elderly population. In older life we are less likely to spend time outside in natural sunlight (for a number of reasons such as less social opportunities and increasing frailty). For some, this is aggravated by age-related deterioration in the eye. Cataracts and glaucoma, which are common in the elderly and even more so in dementia patients, further obscure light getting to photosensitive cells. Visiting an elderly relative with Alzheimer’s disease in a residential care home I was always struck at the dimly lit corridors, the dark rooms, the tiny windows. I had suspected that the lack of natural light didn’t do much for his spirits, but it didn’t occur to me that the constant time indoors with just the faint glow of the television may actually have been doing harm. Speaking at the 2012 Cambridge Science Festival, Professor Russell Foster from the University of Oxford explained that nursing homes typically have a light level of around 20 lux during the day (a bright sunny day would be around 100 000 lux). Add ocular problems, such as cataracts, and you can see why people in residential homes struggle to maintain healthy sleep–wake rhythms. Considering that disrupted sleep can lead to cognitive problems, reduce wellbeing and increase irritability, the consequences of low light become clear. In recent years, bright light therapy – sitting with a box emanating very bright light – has been trialled with Alzheimer’s disease patients. Light therapy has been shown to positively influence Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 fragmented sleep–wake cycles in some participants. And there is theoretical science to support it. The light box increases the intensity of stimulation to the SCN, via the retinohypothalamic tract, a corridor of brain cells linking the eye to the hypothalamus. Strikingly, bright light therapy has also been shown to reduce agitated behaviours in dementia. Such steps, although not disease-modifying, are crucial to lessening the burden of the disease on both patient and carer. Encouragingly, it appears that the importance of chronobiology is being taken more seriously in a wide range of areas, including architecture. Better lighting in hospitals has been shown to impact on recovery rates of patients and further research studies are investigating how substantial this effect is. Electronics giant Philips have recently created an in-hospital lighting system to improve the levels of daytime light, incorporating a two-hour light boost in the mornings that they claim supports healthy sleep. How this will impact patients in the long term remains to be seen. Chronobiology is a growing field in science, but like much of biomedicine, there is much yet to be understood. One thing is for certain, though: the average light level in an office is around 400 lux, and that’s not high enough to entrain my SCN. This strip light will have to go… 31 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Termites and intelligent living buildings David Parr Life can be hard to define. A traditional biological definition is structures of organised cells that eat in order to grow, adapt, react, maintain and regulate their internal state, with the capacity to reproduce. Sometimes, though, organisms need to maintain and regulate the climate outside of their own bodies. Termites in Nigeria, for example, have trouble surviving when exposed to the normal conditions of their native land. For a species famed for its animal architecture, it is often overlooked that the grand form actually follows vital function. Termites even build tunnels to move through when foraging outside the nest in order to maintain their own requirements for temperature and humidity. that the structure promotes gas exchange through enhanced diffusion in the finest tunnels, not just as a bulk flow of buoyant air, in the same way that gas movement occurs in the alveolar ducts. He told Pearce: “It was only when I began to see things as a process... and not an object that I began to understand how it worked. It is all physiology.” Pearce himself cites this as a turning point in his outlook: “We should try to see things in nature not as objects and copy their form, but as processes and systems”. Mick Pearce watched this story of design and survival unfold through a David Attenborough documentary. The iconic chimneys and galleries of termite nests led him to design the Eastgate Centre, a large retail/office structure built in Harare, Zimbabwe, in 1996. By learning from nature, a technique now called biomimicry, his design team built one of the most sophisticated green buildings of the time. The design links thermal mass and ventilation, and responds to the local climate. It maintains and regulates its internal state and reacts to stimulus, core principles of life. Turner and Soar’s resulting paper, ‘Beyond Biomimicry’, proposes that the mound is not a static, isolated structure, but the lungs themselves of a macro-organism; the termites are analogous to cells in a larger, cohesive whole, surrounded by the nest as a second skin. Current building design, they argue, sees the building fabric as a barrier separating internal spaces from the outside world. However, as the internal space needs air, light and heat to function, these things are then ‘bolted on’ in air conditioning and electric lights. Living systems, however, resolve this paradox by seeing membranes as “adaptive interfaces, where fluxes of matter and energy across the wall are not blocked, but managed by the [structure] itself”, a statement they see as true for termite mounds as well as for human skin. Three years ago, Scott Turner and Rupert Soar made contact with Pearce. They were also learning from termites and were intrigued by Eastgate. As the conversation developed, Turner shared his view of termite nests: that the central structure may operate not just as a chimney, but in a way much closer to human lungs. He believes Soar works in 3D printing and material science, and his current project is seeking to develop ‘intelligent’ buildings. The ability for a building to grow and adapt is yet to be realised; yet again termites have got there first. They are constantly ‘tuning’ their structure – by acting as the senses of the wider macro-organism, they continually 32 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 assess the conditions of the nest, adding and removing material in an attempt to balance the internal requirements against a range of everchanging external conditions. This is the growth and adaptation portion of defining a living building. It could be argued that our buildings already achieve this. We add extensions if we want more space, we open windows for more air, but in reality we are very bad at this, and especially at doing this for the lowest energy costs. The Eastgate Centre was extensively tuned over a period of years by the design team in order to achieve the best balance between energy usage and the required internal conditions. Soar’s new challenge is to create a building that grows and adapts itself, without relying on human decision-making and action, and he is looking to the final part of the definition of life to solve this: genetics and reproduction. Together with Julian Vincent, Soar is developing a framework based on DNA that can ‘evolve’ a building from information on the intended usage and location. The building form and materials would be derived from computer algorithms, and manufactured using new 3D printing tools. Further, the building would be adaptive, forever refining itself through multiple iterations by tuning its structure to a minute scale in response to its self-monitoring processes. If successful, this project could be the next step to a living building, an achievement already 20 years in the making, but one which termites have been doing for centuries. 33 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 The skeleton key Emma Pewsey I love you to your bones. That might be a bit forward, but I make no apologies. Bones are brilliant. Unlike buildings, which are over-engineered to cope with extreme events, bones react to the forces that you impose on them to give you exactly the strength you need. Every time you go for a walk, you make your bones stronger. As your foot hits the ground, the stress sent through the bones supporting you stimulates cells called osteoblasts. These then lay down the foundations for new bits of the hard, supportive outer shell of calcium-containing minerals that coat your bones. Swap your walk for a week in bed and your bones will become measurably weaker. With bone, it’s use it or lose it. But why do bones weaken? Well, your skeleton doesn’t only support your body physically. Bones are also the body’s 24-hour one-stop calcium shop. Calcium is crucial not just for keeping bones strong, but also for generating the electrical signals that keep the heart beating and your muscles moving. So drinking that glass of milk won’t just keep your bones strong, it will keep your heart beating too! To release the calcium, another group of cells called osteoclasts swing into action. These break down the hard surface of the bone, expelling the minerals into the bloodstream. This also has benefits for the strength of your skeleton. Tiny cracks, shorter than the width of a hair, form in your bones as part of everyday activities. A sudden 34 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist shock can turn these cracks into fractures. The work of the osteoclasts therefore cleans up these fragile regions so the osteoblasts can replace them with fresh, strong bone. So the magic of bones comes from this careful balance of destruction and renewal processes, which ensures your skeleton is the most perfectly adapted structure on Earth. Unfortunately, trips into space present new problems. Astronauts lose 1 to 2 per cent of their bone density every month they’re in space. This bone loss can be partially reversed when they’re back on Earth, but even a year after they’ve returned, their bones still won’t be back to full strength. You might think that losing bone density in space would be an expected consequence of being in microgravity. With fewer forces acting on the skeleton there will be less stimulation of the bone-forming osteoblasts. However, in space bone formation continues more or less normally. Instead, bone destruction speeds up. Weaker bones aren’t the only consequence. The calcium released from bones has to go somewhere – astronaut pee. Processing this extra calcium puts astronauts at an increased risk of kidney stones – a particularly painful prospect halfway to Mars. It damages equipment too. The drink of ‘choice’ in space is processed urine. However, the Urine Processing Assembly on the International Space Station needed emergency repairs in 2009 after the high calcium concentrations flowing through it formed a scale which blocked it. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 So what can be done to keep astronaut bones strong, and space toilets flowing freely? This is where life on Earth and life in space help each other out. Osteoporosis has a similar boneweakening effect as space travel, though six times less severe. Research into preventing osteoporosis can therefore help astronaut bones, and vice versa. There are currently two big weapons used in the war against weak bones. The first is exercise. Weight-bearing exercise stimulates osteoblasts to form new bone, and has been shown to be an effective treatment for osteoporosis. Space agencies are well aware of this, and have developed a range of complicated-looking exercise contraptions for astronauts to strap themselves in to. However, despite years of research these aren’t quite perfect, and astronauts still return to Earth with weaker bones. The second weapon is another straightforward piece of health advice. Health’s current bad boy is sodium chloride – common table salt. Eating too much salt may cause a grim-sounding condition called acidosis, where the fluids in your body become more acidic. To neutralise this acidity, more calcium is released from your bones, weakening them. 35 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist It turns out that the highly processed nature of space food means the average astronaut consumes twice their recommended daily allowance of salt. So is cutting down on salt the key to lessening the misery of brittle bones in space (and of cleaning blocked-up space toilets)? It’s a possible solution that’s being investigated, but since your sense of taste is dampened in space, reducing salt risks making astronauts unhappy as without it, food becomes even blander. On Earth we have more choice over what we eat. So support your bones by reducing your salt intake. After all, they support you in more ways than you might think. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 The new ‘balanced’ diet Maliah Roshan Imagine being able to guzzle just exactly what you want, and as much as you fancy, with no negative consequences. No more limiting yourself to a small pack of crinkle-cut cheddar crisps that vanishes in a few seconds: now you could scoff a family-sized pack, no worries. All you’d have to do to achieve this calorific nirvana is override those problematic calories with some more beneficial ones. It sounds like wishful thinking, yet a recent spate of nutritional research publications is showing how it might be done. Groups of researchers from the US to China have fed rats, mice and hamsters the sort of high-fat, high-sugar diet guaranteed to lead to obesity, clogged arteries and diabetes. However, they found that the animals could largely be saved from such a fate if they were supplied with some other specially selected nutrient that appeared to counter the damage. Extracts of chokeberries, peanuts, turmeric and chickpeas are just some of these, and the list of nutrients capable of offsetting unhealthy foods or even lifestyle is constantly expanding. Coffee, for example. American researchers conducted one of the largest surveys on the effects of coffee consumption on mortality, by following over 400 000 US citizens aged over 50 across 13 years. The coffee drinkers in the study were generally an unhealthy bunch of couch potatoes compared to non-coffee drinkers. They were more likely to smoke, take no vigorous physical exercise, consume a lot of alcohol and eat more red meat, while eating very few fresh fruits or vegetables. They also had lower levels of education, which is known to lead to poorer health. 36 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist The coffee drinkers had higher rates of mortality, but once these relevant lifestyle details were taken into account the scientists found something surprising: coffee was actually protecting people against a broad range of illnesses ranging from stroke and lung diseases to infections and diabetes. And the more coffee they consumed the more protection they got: 10–15 per cent lower mortality in coffee drinkers who downed six cups a day. This counters our usual assumptions about caffeine – all that caffeine surely can’t be good for us? In fact, the caffeine in coffee does cause a spike in blood sugar levels and therefore also insulin – definitely unhealthy over the long term. And yet coffee drinkers are less likely to develop diabetes than non-drinkers. How? It would appear there is a sort of offsetting effect at work here. The clues behind this lie in the components found in coffee, and in the selected nutrients used in the animal studies mentioned earlier. Coffee is rich in chlorogenic acids, which are potent antioxidants (molecules that mop up unstable oxygen and other such molecules). These have been shown to lower blood pressure in both healthy and mildly hypertensive people. Roasted coffee extracts activate antioxidant elements in a number of genes, protecting cells from the sort of oxidative stress associated with inflammatory processes such as those found in diabetes. In fact, one of the key players in that process, NF-κB, is blocked by caffeic acid, formed when chlorogenic acids are broken down in the intestine. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Chokeberries are also very high in antioxidant molecules, such as anthocyanins and flavonoids, and, like coffee, these appear to counter an unhealthy diet. When rats were fed a diabetespromoting high-fructose diet, as well as gaining weight they showed the metabolic markers typically seen in the run-up to developing diabetes and heart disease. However, those rats who were given an additional chokeberry extract gained less weight and showed fewer of the pre-diabetes signals. Analysis of the rats’ gene expression showed that the chokeberry extract was blocking the expression of genes involved in inflammation and insulin signalling. Another study using peanuts echoed some of the results seen with the chokeberries. Hamsters on a high-fat, atherosclerosis-inducing diet fed a peanut extract were saved from the worst effects of the diet. This can be explained by peanuts’ long list of heart-friendly ingredients, from mono- and polyunsaturated fats to high levels of ß-sitosterol, known to reduce serum cholesterol levels, and resveratrol, a broadly acting antioxidant that results in improved insulin sensitivity and lessens the risk of heart disease. What coffee, peanuts and chokeberry extract have in common is they all contain a cocktail of ingredients including antioxidants that put a brake at various stages in the processes leading to obesity, insulin resistance and cardiovascular disease. 37 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist This suggests that if you love your food too much and want to be able to have your cake and eat it, you need to look further than having your five a day. By selecting the right nutrients in the right amounts you may be able to tip the balance of healthy versus damaging foods in your favour. A ‘balanced’ diet may soon mean something quite different. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Treating cancer with some help from the brain John Wilde Just a sniff is all it takes. It’s that distinctive aroma of tequila which, thanks to a foolish night in my youth, leaves me feeling ill at even the slightest whiff. My brain has gone through a process called associative learning in which the smell and taste of tequila has become linked with the presence of alcohol, triggering a physiological response which leaves me feeling rather off-colour. This isn’t a problem for me – thankfully I don’t need tequila. But what about cancer patients and chemotherapy? Anticipatory nausea and vomiting (ANV) is a well-documented phenomenon in oncology and can be incredibly debilitating. Around 25–30 per cent of patients experience severe nausea and vomiting when they enter the chemotherapy clinic following just one treatment session. It can be so severe that some patients will opt to end their therapy before they have had sufficient doses to have any beneficial effect, while those who do continue with their treatment must endure several horrible hours each time they return for a round. But how and why does this happen? The side-effects of chemotherapy occur due to the defence mechanisms of the body: chemotherapy is a cocktail of poisons which the body tries to avoid by inducing nausea and vomiting. These unpleasant side-effects still arise despite drugs used to combat them. In associative theory this is an ‘unconditioned response’ as it is a direct cause of the chemotherapy treatment, the ‘unconditioned stimulus’. In ANV the entire 38 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist clinical context (the smell of the clinic, the location, the décor and even the nurses themselves) becomes associated with the unpleasant effects of chemotherapy. The result of this is that exposure to the clinical context alone is enough to generate the feeling of illness. One potential intervention is ‘stimulus scapegoating’. This uses associative theory to prevent the association from forming. A key element in associative learning is that an event (e.g. chemotherapy-induced illness) becomes linked with the stimulus most easily identified from a group of stimuli. Scapegoating uses this principle by introducing a new, attentiongrabbing stimulus to overshadow the clinical context. This is usually done using a strong flavour that is new to the individual. This idea has proven effective in animal studies conducted by Michelle Symonds and Geoffrey Hall at the University of York. Rats were given either a sour flavour or water while being trained to associate nausea and a distinct context. The rats given the sour flavour were happier to drink a sucrose flavour, compared to control rats given water, when they were returned to the training context. This indicates that the rats associated nausea with the sour flavour as opposed to the context. This effect has also been demonstrated in humans. Ursula Stockhurst and her team at the University of Düsseldorf found that when cancer patients were given a novel fruit juice as an overshadowing stimulus, there were no cases of Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 ANV, whereas the control group given water produced two. Scapegoating and its potential uses are still being investigated, with studies by Friedemann Geiger and Levke Wolfgram in Kiel, Germany, due to begin in 2013. Their study will use 52 newly diagnosed paediatric cancer patients to examine the effectiveness of the scapegoating procedure using flavoured or tasteless sweets. The results could further prove how simply adding a distinctive flavour could vastly improve a patient’s quality of life by making their time before sessions more palatable. Moreover, there is some evidence that the benefits of scapegoating may extend even further. Stockhurst and colleagues’ study also found a beneficial effect in reducing nausea and vomiting after chemotherapy treatment, as well as before it. Stockhurst thinks this may be because the patient records nausea and vomiting in the same context as that of the treatment, causing an interaction between the conditioned side-effects and those actually caused by the drugs. By removing the conditioned nausea using the scapegoating intervention, the patient has a reduction in overall feelings of nausea and illness. If this proves replicable and usable in the clinic then it could lead to a reduction in the number and dosage of anti-nausea drugs, which have their own side-effects, that patients need to take. 39 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Cancer is on the rise in the developed world; 1 in 3 people will be affected by it at some point in their lives. Thankfully our growing understanding of psychology should allow us to make its treatment less unpleasant. Maybe one day all chemotherapy treatments will come with drink, although be sure it’s not something you’re partial to. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 Windows into the mind Rebecca Winstanley Throughout history, people have imagined that the eyes are the windows into the soul. Most of these people could not have envisaged the modern methods putting this philosophy to the test. Over the past few decades, advances in eye-tracking technology have transformed psychology: tiny changes in the speed and direction of eye movements can reveal profound insights into the inner workings of the brain. For example, observing eye movements can reveal the extent to which we are aware of other people’s perspectives. To investigate this, a team of researchers at the University of Queensland used the famous Sally–Anne paradigm: participants see Sally placing a ball in a box and leaving the room. Following this, Anne (the villain of the story) hides the ball in a different box. While we know the ball is no longer there, most of us recognise that Sally, when she comes back into the room, will look for it in the original box. Even when the participants were distracted by another task and not consciously paying attention to the actors, their eye movements still identified with the belief state of the Sally character – participants looked sympathetically towards the first box. The researchers dubbed this ‘implicit mentalising’. The same test has helped shape our understanding of when young children learn to deduce the views of those around them. Children under the age of three typically fail in tasks that test for a ‘theory of mind’ – that is, the ability to recognise that the views of others are different to your own. However, recent studies have 40 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist demonstrated that when children between 6 and 18 months view a Sally–Anne video, they preferentially look towards Sally’s box even when the ball is no longer there. The finding that a theory of mind may exist in an implicit form earlier than believed not only brings exciting new challenges to the field of developmental psychology, but suggests eye movements could potentially be used as an early-age diagnostic tool for autism spectrum disorders, where theory of mind abilities are impaired. When we see other people moving, our brains unconsciously generate ‘motor plans’ to perform the same action, but we generally inhibit these plans and prevent any actual movement. A study published earlier this year revealed that watching videos of dancers improved the control of eye movements in individuals with Parkinson’s disease. The study’s authors suggest that by watching the dancers, participants formed and inhibited dance-related motor plans. Inhibiting this movement then strengthened their ability to control more automatic movements in subsequent tasks. If true, this may be used to develop therapies to help people with Parkinson’s disease maintain more control over the movement of their bodies. Studying eye movements has also shed light on how memory is organised in the brain. It is well documented that generating sequences of rapid eye movements before trying to remember something considerably enhances recall for things like lists of words, childhood memories, details of stories and visual landmarks. Wellcome Trust Science Writing Prize 2013 The left and right hemispheres of the brain are connected by a bundle of nerve fibres called the corpus callosum – this is crucial for communication between the two sides of the brain. Cognitive psychologists have confirmed that activities which stimulate the corpus callosum can, in turn, bolster memory. Because the right hemisphere controls movement in the left eye and the left hemisphere the right eye, sequences of rapid eye movements actively stimulate the corpus callosum, which may help the successful encoding and retrieval of information. Whether it’s in the processing of everyday information or in simple ways to improve day-today functioning, when it comes to the inner workings of the brain, our eyes are surprisingly telling. 41 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist Wellcome Trust We are a global charitable foundation dedicated to achieving extraordinary improvements in human and animal health. We support the brightest minds in biomedical research and the medical humanities. Our breadth of support includes public engagement, education and the application of research to improve health. We are independent of both political and commercial interests. Wellcome Trust Gibbs Building 215 Euston Road London NW1 2BE, UK T +44 (0)20 7611 8888 F +44 (0)20 7611 8545 E contact@wellcome.ac.uk wellcome.ac.uk The Wellcome Trust is a charity registered in England and Wales, no. 210183. Its sole trustee is The Wellcome Trust Limited, a company registered in England and Wales, no. 2711000 (whose registered office is at 215 Euston Road, London NW1 2BE, UK). MC-5686.18/12-2013/JS 42 | Science Writing Prize 2013: The shortlist