Signaled Transitions: Effects of Timeout on Post-Reinforcement Pausing Emily L. Baxter A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of the Arts Department of Psychology University of North Carolina Wilmington 2012 Approved by Advisory Committee Raymond C. Pitts Carole M. VanCamp Mark Galizio Christine E. Hughes Chair Accepted By Digitally signed by Ron Vetter DN: cn=Ron Vetter, o=UNCW, ou=Computer Science, email=vetterr@uncw.edu, c=US Date: 2013.06.12 20:15:27 -04'00' Ron Vetter ______________________________ Dean, Graduate School TABLE OF CONTENTS ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................ iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ....................................................................................................... v DEDICATION ........................................................................................................................ vi LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................................................. vii LIST OF FIGURES .............................................................................................................. viii INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................................... 1 Overview ...................................................................................................................... 1 Fixed-Ratio Schedules and the Associated Pause ........................................................ 3 Aversive Functions of FR Schedules ........................................................................... 6 Rich-to-Lean Transitions ............................................................................................. 9 Statement of the Problem ........................................................................................... 22 METHOD .............................................................................................................................. 24 Subjects ...................................................................................................................... 24 Apparatus ................................................................................................................... 25 Preliminary Training .................................................................................................. 25 General Procedure ...................................................................................................... 27 Phase 1 ........................................................................................................... 27 Phase 2 ........................................................................................................... 31 Data Analysis ............................................................................................................. 33 RESULTS .............................................................................................................................. 35 Phase 1 ....................................................................................................................... 35 Phase 2 ....................................................................................................................... 39 ii DISCUSSION ........................................................................................................................ 50 Phase 1 ....................................................................................................................... 50 Phase 2 ....................................................................................................................... 57 Applied Implications .................................................................................................. 61 Summary .................................................................................................................... 62 REFERENCES ...................................................................................................................... 64 iii ABSTRACT Relatively large post-reinforcement pauses (PRP) are observed during transitions from a rich (e.g., high reinforcer magnitude) environment to a lean (e.g., low reinforcer magnitude) environment compared to other transition types (i.e., rich-rich; lean-rich; lean-lean). Previous literature on transitions have used two discriminative stimuli to indicate the upcoming reinforcer (i.e., large or small).In the Phase 1 of the current study, four pigeons responded on an multiple FR schedule, in which four discriminative stimuli were used to represent each individual transition. The magnitudes of the reinforcers were adjusted until there was extended pausing in the presence of the rich-to-lean transition stimulus compared to the other transitions. The FR and difference between the small and large reinforcer magnitude were adjusted until each subject had longer pauses during the rich-to-lean transition than in the other transitions. As the difference between the small and large reinforcers became more extreme, the PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus increased, while the PRP in the presence of the three other stimuli remained relatively equal for each pigeon. In Phase 2, timeouts (i.e., blackout or stimulus-termination) were added after each reinforcer in probe session to determine if the PRP during transitions would decrease. During blackout timeout, three durations were used: 15, 30, and 60-s. During stimulus-termination timeout only 30-s timeouts were used. During blackout timeout sessions, there was no systematic changes in PRP across durations or transitions. The majority of changes were increases in PRP during timeout sessions. During stimulus-termination timeouts, however, decreases in PRP were observed across all transitions and subjects. The current study replicated the patterns of pausing seen in previous research using four stimuli for each transition and stimulus-termination decreased PRP during rich-to-lean transitions more reliably than did blackout timeout. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS First and foremost I would like to thank my mentor through this process, Dr. Christine Hughes. Over the last 2.5 years, her guidance has helped me become a better student, writer, and researcher. My confidence as a person and an academic has soared because of her and I am forever grateful. I would like to thank all the faculty of the UNCW behavior analysis program, particularly the members of my thesis committee, Dr. Ray Pitts, Dr. Mark Galizio, and Dr. Carole Van Camp. They have been so helpful and insightful during my research process here. I have learned so much from them. I am entering the world of behavior analysis with confidence, knowing that I have learned from the best. I would like to thank all the members of the PPH lab. Without everyone’s hard work daily in the lab my project would not have been completed. I would especially like to thank Kelsey Knight and Cassee Stem who have taken a large role in past and future analysis of my pigeons’ rich-to-lean transitions. I am grateful for the open ears and arms of Shelley Murawsky, Marcelle Medina, and Ashley Aikman, who have listened to and supported me while I have been completing the final steps of my thesis. Many thanks are due to my behavior analysis cohorts: Rachel Eure, Whitney Luffman, Amanda Rickard, Tracy Taylor, and Kristin Yonkers. We went through the trenches together and I am so thankful for their support over the last 2.5 years. Finally, I would like to thank those responsible for raising me to be the person I am today; my mom and dad, Deborah Unangst and Bruce Baxter; my step-dad, Louis Schum; and my brother and sister, Matthew and Talitha Baxter. I would not be where I am today without your love and support. Thank you. v DEDICATION I would like to dedicate this thesis to my late step-brother, Joshua D. Bridenbaker. He inspired me and was the spark for my interest in behavior analysis. I am forever grateful for the impact he has had on my life. vi LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. The Discriminative Stimulus of Each Transition Type for Each Subject ...................... 29 2. Description of Different Conditions as Categorized by the Magnitude of the Lean and Rich Reinforcer Presentations in Terms of How Many Seconds Reinforcer is Presented .................................................................................................. 30 3. Number of Sessions for Each Timeout Duration Subjects Experienced During Blackout Timeout Sessions ........................................................................................................... 32 4. Number of Timeout Sessions Each Subject Experienced During Stimulus-Termination Timeout Sessions ....................................................................... 34 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Post-Reinforcement Pauses During Each Transition Across Reinforcer Magnitudes for Each Subject ......................................................................................... 36 2. Run Rate During Each Transition Across Reinforcer Magnitudes for Each Subject .... 38 3. Average Proportion of Baseline and Absolute Change From Baseline of PostReinforcement Pauses During Blackout Timeout Sessions ........................................... 40 4. Average Proportion of Baseline and Absolute Change from Baseline of Run Rates During Blackout Timeout Sessions.......................................................... 42 5. Average Proportion of Baseline and Absolute Change from Baseline of PostReinforcement Pauses During Stimulus-Termination Timeout Sessions ...................... 44 6. Average Proportion of Baseline and Absolute Change from Baseline of Run Rates During Stimulus-Termination Timeout Sessions ......................................... 46 7. Post-Reinforcement Pauses During Each Timeout Condition for Each Subject ........... 47 8. Run Rate During Each Timeout Condition for Each Subject ........................................ 49 viii INTRODUCTION There are many ways in which behavior can be controlled. Operant contingencies that control behavior include positive and negative reinforcement, and positive and negative punishment. The control of behavior through positive reinforcement is generally viewed as ethical and socially valid in increasing behavior in individuals. Sidman (1989) stated that although it is preferable to change behavior using positive reinforcement, aversive control (coercion) is a natural part of behavior change in our society. The use of aversive control, negative reinforcement or punishment, is commonly thought of as maladaptive, because it may cause fear and anxiety in the individual that experiences it. Perone (2003) discussed how viewing aversive control as maladaptive, might not always be valid. He defined an aversive stimulus in terms of both negative reinforcement and punishment. With respect to negative reinforcement, a stimulus is considered aversive if the contingent removal or postponement of the stimulus increases the behavior that removed or postponed it. With respect to positive punishment, a stimulus is considered aversive if the contingent presentation of it decreases the behavior that produced it. Aversive control, however, should not always be considered maladaptive. An example of an adaptive behavior change in the presence of an aversive stimulus can be observed when a psychology student has failed her previous introduction to psychology test. In order to avoid another failing grade, the student will increase her studying. In order to avoid the aversive stimulus of another failing grade, the behavior of studying increases. Another example of when aversive control is adaptive is when it begins to thunder and lightning while a person is on a walk. The person will move indoors in order to remove this aversive stimulus. If they stay outside they are increasing the likelihood that they will be harmed by the inclement weather. Perone also cited an example in which the lack of aversive control is detrimental. When a frog is put into hot water, it will immediately jump out, responding to the stimulus of the hot water. When the frog is placed into cold water, however, and the temperature is slowly increased, the frog will boil to death because the frog is not able to discriminate the aversive situation. It is important to note that a single stimulus can be considered aversive or nonaversive depending on how it affects behavior. The context in which the stimulus is presented can often determine its effect on the individual’s behavior. For example, attention from others is something that is commonly thought of as a positive stimulus. In a situation in which a person has said or done something embarrassing, however, the attention of others could be considered an aversive stimulus. If after a person has made a comment at which others laughed in a negative manner, that person will be less likely to emit that type of behavior in the future. This example shows how a stimulus, such as laughing, can be considered aversive or nonaversive depending on the context in which it was received. Further, Perone (2003) discussed how there are also aversive effects produced by the use of positive reinforcement. When an individual receives reinforcement contingent on a particular behavior, there might be a time when the reinforcer is no longer available (i.e., the behavior is put on extinction). The deleterious effects of removing the opportunity to receive a reinforcer for a behavior might be considered aversive. For example, a teacher might praise a student for raising her hand and giving the correct answer in class. The teacher may not always call on that particular student, and in that situation no praise is received for hand raising. The transition from receiving praise to not receiving praise could be aversive, because a reinforcer was given previously for the same behavior. Additionally, there often is a time period immediately after a reinforcer is received in which the opportunity to respond for the reinforcer again may not be 2 available. In the previous example, it is only fair for a teacher to call on several students during a class session. Once a student has been called on and has received praise for hand raising, there is a certain period of time that will pass before the teacher will call on that student. This period of time may be aversive to a student because the opportunity to respond, and in turn receive a reinforcer, is not available. That is, the behavior of hand raising is undergoing extinction, and eventually, the student might not continue to raise his or her hand. The aversive qualities of positive reinforcement can be better understood by studying reinforcement schedules and the pattern of behavior that these schedules occasion. Specifically, fixed-ratio (FR) schedules will be discussed in greater detail. Fixed-Ratio Schedules and the Associated Pause Fixed-ratio schedules consist of a response requirement that is held constant and a reinforcer that is received upon completing the response requirement (Crossman, Elliot, Phelps, 1987; Ferster&Skinner,1957). For example, on an FR 50, a subject makes 50 responses before receiving the reinforcer. Once the reinforcer has been received, the response count starts over, and an additional 50 responses are required. This pattern of 50 responses followed by a reinforcer continues until the session is complete. Fixed-ratio schedules produce a reliable pattern of behavior that, when viewed on a cumulative record creates a stair-case pattern. The staircase pattern signifies that once responding begins for the upcoming ratio, there is typically a continuous run of responses until the reinforcer is received. Once the reinforcer is received, there is a generally a pause, in which no responses occur, before the subject begins responding. This pause has been called pre-ratio pause (PreRP), or the post-reinforcement pause (PRP), depending on the context in which the pause occurs. 3 Crossman (1968) conducted a study to investigate how the ratio in FR schedules affected pausing in two White Carneau pigeons. The first phase consisted of a multiple FR 10 FR 100 schedule of food presentation. A multiple schedule consists of two or more components and a different discriminative stimulus is associated with each component. In this study, the FR 10 component was associated with a green key light, and the FR 100 component was associated with a red key light. Upon the completion of each ratio requirement, the pigeons received 3-s access to grain. Crossman found that the PreRP before the FR 10 was shorter than the PreRP before the FR 100. In this context the pause would be considered a PreRP, because it was not controlled by the amount of work just completed (i.e., the FR), but rather was controlled by the upcoming work/FR. The longer pause might indicate that the upcoming large ratio was aversive to the pigeon, and therefore the pigeon did not initiate responding as quickly as before a small ratio. In the next part of the experiment, the subjects experienced a chained FR 10 FR 100 schedule. Similar to a multiple schedule, a chained schedule consists of two or more components and a different discriminative stimulus is associated with each component; however, in a chained schedule, all components must be completed before the primary reinforcer is presented. Once the first component is completed, the only signal that the next component is in effect is a change in the discriminative stimulus. The change in the discriminative stimulus can be considered a conditioned reinforcer. There was a change in the relative reinforcer rate from the multiple schedule to the chained schedule. The reinforcer magnitude after each FR had changed from being equal during the multiple schedule, to only a conditioned reinforcer after the FR 10 and a 3-s presentation of food after the FR 100. In the chained schedule, the pauses were shorter before the FR 100 than before the FR 10. This suggests that ratio size is not the only factor that is responsible for pausing, but the delay to the reinforcer affects the PreRP duration. That is, the 4 PreRP might have been longer before the FR 10 in the chained schedule, because the first component was more delayed from the primary reinforcer and there was only a conditioned reinforcer presented contingent on completion of the FR 10 unlike completion of the FR 100. It might be less aversive to begin responding on a large ratio when the signaled delay to reinforcement in the large ratio component is shorter than the delay to reinforcement in the small ratio component. In a follow-up study, Crossman (1971) studied PRPs in both multiple and mixed FR schedules. A mixed schedule is similar to a multiple schedule in that it consists of two or more components. Unlike a multiple schedule, the components are not signaled by different discriminative stimuli; that is, a single stimulus is used for all components. The pause during a mixed schedule is considered a PRP because the pause is controlled by the previously experienced ratio. In this study, both the multiple and mixed schedules consisted of two FR components. The small ratio remained the same (FR10), whereas the large ratio was adjusted across conditions. In the multiple schedule, similar to the previous study, the pauses were shorter before the FR 10, and longer before the larger FR (15, 25, or 55), and in the component with the large FR, the PRP increased as the FR increased. Once the large ratio was increased to FR 25, there was no longer an overlap between the PRPs of the small and large ratios. As the difference between the small and large ratio increased, so did the PRP before the large ratio. This suggests that the context in which the large ratio occurs determines how aversive it is to the subject. The transition from an FR 10 to an FR 15 is not as aversive as the transition from an FR 10 to an FR 55, as evidenced by the longer PRP in the latter. The results of the mixed schedule were similar to those in the multiple schedule; however, the large ratio had to be an FR 40 before there was no overlap in the PRPs between the small and large ratios. Therefore, the difference between the 5 small and large ratios was larger in the mixed schedule when there was no signal of the upcoming ratio. Because there was no signal for the upcoming ratio, the pigeons could not discriminate between the differences in the ratios to the extent that they were able to in the multiple schedule. The previously experienced ratio is the only part of the component that could have been controlling the pigeons’ PRP in the mixed schedule. Aversive Functions of Fixed-Ratio Schedules There is evidence to support that the pause during fixed-ratio responding during could be induced by the aversive nature of the time immediately following reinforcement. There are other observable behaviors that occur following reinforcement during FR schedules such as escape and aggression. Azrin (1961) found that pigeons will respond in order to escape from an FR schedule by responding on a timeout operandum in order to self-impose timeout (extinction). Although timeout is typically thought of as punishing, Azrin suggested that in a condition in which a subject could put itself into a period of timeout during FR responding, timeout would be considered reinforcing. Pigeons were used as subjects in this study. For each pigeon the FR requirement was raised from 65 to 200 across sessions. During a 60-min session, pigeons’ key pecking was reinforced according to an FR schedule that remained constant within a session; the reinforcer magnitude (i.e., food) remained constant. There was a separate timeout key that was continuously available to the subject on which it could respond at any time. When there was a response on the timeout key, there was a decrease in the intensity of the transillumination on both the FR and timeout keys. The lights did not turn off, but the change in intensity signaled that there were no programmed consequences for responding on the FR key. At any point, the subject could respond a second time on the timeout key. The transillumination would return to its normal intensity, and responses on the FR key once again were reinforced. Azrin found that as 6 the FR increased, time spent in timeout increased. In addition, the pigeons tended to escape almost exclusively during the latter part of the PRP. Self-imposed timeout almost never occurred immediately after a reinforcer was received. This might indicate that there are specific times during the PRP that are particularly aversive. Additionally, the longer the pigeon is in a PRP before it responds on the timeout key, and the longer the pigeon self-imposes timeout, the more aversive the upcoming ratio is considered. Thompson (1964) conducted a systematic replication of Azrin’s (1961) study, in which he studied the aversiveness of increasing FR schedules and the reinforcing properties of selfimposed timeout, or escape, in lever pressing in rats. There were a few adjustments made to the procedure. In Azrin’s study, FR requirements were only observed in ascending order. Thompson examined if the effect of increasing the FR was reversible by first increasing the FR requirement, and then decreasing it. Thompson also wanted to evaluate if responding on a timeout lever was maintained by escape from the current FR. In all sessions, a timeout lever was available concurrently with the FR lever. Three responses on the timeout lever initiated a 30-s timeout. Timeout was signaled either by a change of the discriminative stimulus from light to dark, or dark to light depending on the condition. During timeout there were no programmed consequences for responding on either lever. Time spent in timeout and the frequency of timeout responses were measured. In one experiment, the FR was increased in increments of 25 across sessions, starting at FR 25, until there was a dramatic increase in the PRP duration. Once a subject reached their peak FR requirement, the ratio was then decreased in increments of 25 responses across sessions, until an FR 25 was reached. The frequency of responding on the timeout lever increased as the FR increased and then decreased as the FR decreased. This 7 suggests that the greater the FR was, the more aversive it may have been to the subject, as evidenced by the increased number of responses on the timeout lever. An additional experiment in Thompson’s (1964) study was conducted in order to see if escape from the food lever was reinforcing responding on the timeout lever. First, the FR requirement was increased in increments of 25 responses over each session until a breaking point was reached and the subject no longer responded. Once this FR was reached, it was held constant, and response requirements on the timeout lever increased from an FR 1 to an FR 3 and continued to increase across sessions until responding on the timeout lever was extinguished. Once the FR requirement for the timeout key had reached a certain size, the typical pattern of a run of responses and a PRP occurred. This is further evidence that FR schedules have aversive functions, because lengthy PRP durations occurred during responding for both timeout and water. Another behavior that occurs during FR responding is aggression toward other subjects (e.g., Gentry, 1968; Knutson, 1970). Gentry demonstrated that pigeons were more likely to attack a target pigeon when they were responding on an FR 50 schedule than in an extinction condition. There were two types of subjects: the experimental pigeons whose key pecking was reinforced on an FR 50, and the target pigeons, which were stationed in the back of the chamber enclosed in an apparatus that measured the frequency of attacks. This study used an ABAB design. Condition A was a no reinforcement component for responding, in which the food key was inoperative. This provided a baseline for how often the pigeon attacked when there was no response requirement or reinforcement provided. Condition B was an FR 50 schedule. Gentry found that virtually no attacks were made during the no-reinforcement condition, but attacks were frequently made in the FR 50. One subject emitted up to 2000 attack responses in a single 8 session. The range of attacks across subjects during the FR 50 was about 200-2000. Another piece of evidence of the aversive qualities of FR schedules is that most of the attacks occurred during the PRP. The subjects were more aggressive when they had to work to receive a reinforcer (FR 50), than when they did not receive a reinforcer at all. The studies by Crossman (1968, 1971), Azrin (1961), Thompson (1964), and Gentry (1968) demonstrate that particular aspects of an FR schedule can function aversively, such as the time immediately after a reinforcer is received. The time period in which extended pausing is most likely can be considered a transition from one schedule to the next schedule. The duration of the PRP during the transition from one schedule to the next schedule can be an especially effective measure of the aversiveness of any type of transition. The aversiveness of the transitions can further be manipulated by the arranging different contingencies between which the subject is transitioning. Rich-to-Lean Transitions Transitions are ubiquitous and should be considered a part of all contingencies and behaviors. Transitions are present in each change in reinforcement or workload from one contingency to the next, not just during FR schedules. Behavior can be affected in different ways depending on the context in which a transition occurs. In certain transitions, the change in the contingency may be a loss of reinforcement. In other transitions the change in the contingency may be a gain of reinforcement. It is important to know how they function in different circumstances. For example, when a behavior therapist is working with a client, it is helpful to know what activities are more preferred versus less preferred. If the therapist is trying to make a smooth transition from one activity to the next, it would not be beneficial to transition from an activity that is highly reinforcing with a low work load, to an activity that is much less 9 reinforcing with a heavy work load. This is a very specific example, but as previously stated transitions are present in every type of contingency. Transitions from stimuli signaling one activity to stimuli signaling the next activity, depending on the change in the contingency, can be considered aversive. The analysis of the aversive function of transitions in contingencies will be given further consideration. Transitions from one FR schedule to another FR schedule are one way to assess the aversive functions of these schedules. The aversive qualities of FR schedules can be observed during transitions from situations that are “rich” to situations that are “lean,” relative to each other. For example, in a rich condition a subject might receive more reinforcers, have a lighter work load, or a mix of both. In a lean condition, a subject might receive fewer reinforcers, have a heavier work load, or a mix of both. In situations in which both rich and lean environments are available, there are four types of transitions: rich to rich, rich to lean, lean to rich, and lean to lean. A reinforcer or work load may be valued at an “x” amount, but that value can change depending on what reinforcer or workload is presented before or after. This is observed during transitions as the establishing operations are in constant flux. For example, a small reinforcer is potentially worth less when preceded by a large reinforcer, than when it is preceded by a small reinforcer. Perone and Courtney (1992) used an animal model to study the effects of rich-to-lean transitions on behavior. The study was conducted in order to determine what was functionally controlling pausing during transitions. The authors hoped to elucidate the controlling variables of the pause; that is whether the pause between transitions was controlled by the past reinforcer (PRP) or the upcoming reinforcer (PreRP). The authors hypothesized that pausing during transitions might be a function of both the previously experienced ratio and the signaled 10 upcoming ratio, depending on the context of the condition (mixed or multiple schedule). They used both mixed and multiple schedules to compare the patterns of pausing across the four transitions types. In the mixed schedule, there was a single stimulus during all transitions (e.g., white). In the multiple schedule, there was one stimulus that indicated whether the upcoming reinforcer magnitude was small (e.g., red) and another stimulus that indicated whether the upcoming reinforcer was large (e.g., green). Perone and Courtney hypothesized that in a mixed schedule, the pause would be controlled only by the previous reinforcer and not the upcoming reinforcer; in a multiple schedule the pause would be controlled by both the previous reinforcer and the signaled upcoming reinforcer. In Perone and Courtney’s (1992) study, four male White Carneau pigeons were the subjects. All four pigeons were studied in sessions using a mixed schedule, and two of these pigeons were also studied using a multiple schedule. For both schedules, the pigeons’ key pecking was maintained by an FR 80 schedule, and the reinforcers used were either a small or a large magnitude of grain. Presentation of the components (small and large) varied throughout the session such that there were the four transitions described above. There were 41 reinforcers presented in a quasi-random order, such that each type of transition occurred 10 times in each session. Within a session no more than four large or four small reinforcers were presented in succession. In half of the sessions, the first reinforcer received was large and in the other half of the sessions the first reinforcer was small. Therefore, the first reinforcer (e.g., small) received in each session was presented 21 times and the other reinforcer (e.g., large) was presented 20 times. In the mixed schedule, the small reinforcer ranged from 0.5 s to 4 s; the large reinforcer ranged from 5.25 s to 20 s. In the multiple schedule, the small reinforcer ranged from 0.5 s to 4 s; the large reinforcer ranged from 4.5 s to 20 s. The houselight remained lit during sessions. 11 In Phase 1, the small and large reinforcer magnitudes were changed across conditions in both the mixed and multiple schedules. In the first condition of both schedules, the magnitude of the reinforcer in both components was equal. Changes to the conditions were made when pausing across each of the four transitions was stable for 10 sessions, or after a maximum of 50 sessions, and there could be no increasing or decreasing trends. Across the previous 10 sessions, the differences between the mean of the first 5 medians and the mean of the last 5 medians had to be within 10%of the grand mean, or within 1s. When criteria were met to move on to the next sequence, the small reinforcer duration was decreased by half and the large reinforcer duration was increased by half. In Phase 2, only two of the birds were studied, both under mixed and multiple schedules. The difference between Phase 1 and Phase 2 was that instead of adjusting both the large and the small reinforcers, the experimenters adjusted only the magnitude of the large reinforcer. The small reinforcer remained at 4 s during Phase 2, while the large reinforcer magnitudes were 4-s, 12-s, and 20-s access to grain. Perone and Courtney (1992) found that during the mixed schedule, PRPs were longer after a large reinforcer was received than after a small reinforcer was received. There was no interaction between the past reinforcer and the upcoming reinforcer in respect to pause length. The PRP increased as a function of the difference between the small and the large reinforcer durations. The results from the multiple-schedule condition suggested that there was an interaction between the past and upcoming reinforcers such that when the past reinforcer was large and the signaled upcoming reinforcer was small, there was a longer PRP than in any of the other transition types. Similar to in the mixed schedule, the PRP increased as a function of the difference between the small and the large reinforcers. The interaction between the past and the upcoming reinforcers became more evident as the difference between the small and large 12 reinforcers became more extreme. Across the two pigeons, the average difference in PRP duration between a rich-to-lean transition and any of the other three types of transitions was 25 s. When the upcoming reinforcer was large, pause duration was as short as 0.18 s regardless of whether the previous reinforcer was small or large. The results from sessions in which multiple schedules were used made it evident that pausing was controlled by both the past and the upcoming reinforcers. The lengthy pause following a rich to lean transition suggests an aversive quality to that particular type of transition. This study led to several follow-up studies in which various ways of determining the aversiveness of rich-to-lean transitions have been investigated. Wade-Galuska, Perone, and Wirth (2004) investigated how force requirements on responding could influence the PRP. The study was arranged similarly to Perone and Courtney’s (1992) study of rich to lean transitions. The difference was that instead of adjusting the reinforcer magnitudes, the experimenters adjusted the force requirement. Four male Sprague-Dawley rats were used in this experiment. Lever pressing was maintained by a multiple schedule of food delivery in which a different lever (left/right) was used in each component of the multiple schedule. Pressing on either lever was reinforced with a food pellet, according to an FR 30 schedule. During baseline, both levers had a force requirement of 0.25 N in order for lever presses to be counted as responses. Across phases, the force requirement for one of the levers remained at 0.25 N, while the force requirement for the other lever increased in four increments; 0.40 N, 0.55 N, 0.70 N, and 0.85 N. Similar to Perone and Courtney’s study, there were 41 components presented in a semi-random order in each session. The sequence of the components allowed for each of the four transitions of force requirements to occur 10 times: low force to low 13 force (rich to rich), high force to high force (lean to lean), low force to high force (rich to lean), and high force to low force (lean to rich). In general, Wade-Galuska et al. (2004) found that pausing was the longest when the upcoming force requirement was high, which is comparable to the lean reinforcer magnitude. The harder the upcoming workload, the less favorable it was and the pause increased. The longest PRP occurred before a high-force requirement that followed a low-force requirement (rich to lean). Run rates also were analyzed in this study. Generally, run rates were the lowest in the low to high force requirement transition (rich to lean). These results are consistent with the findings in the Perone and Courtney (1992) study. The long PRP and low run rate during the rich-to-lean transition suggests that this type of transition is aversive. In addition, this study supported Perone and Courtney’s account of how both the previous and upcoming ratios affect the PRP. The study of rich-to-lean transition in reinforcement schedules has been studied with other reinforcers, such as drugs. Galuska, Wade-Galuska, Woods, and Winger (2007) studied self-administration of cocaine in rhesus monkeys using the methods of Perone and Courtney (1992). They were interested in i.v. self-administration of cocaine. Most of the previous accounts of self-administration claimed that pausing on FR schedules is maintained by the previous dose received (e.g., Skjoldager et al., 1991; Winger and Woods, 2001). Galuska et al. studied lever pressing maintained on a multiple schedule. Both the dose size of cocaine and the FR varied across conditions. When the small and large drug doses used for self-administration were 0.003 mg/kg and 0.03 mg/kg, respectively, the FRs used were 10, 30, 50, 70, 90, 150, and 210. When the small and large drug doses used for self-administration were 0.0056 mg/kg and 0.056 mg/kg, respectively, the FRs used in each condition were 30, 90, 150, and 210. Each condition lasted 20 14 sessions. The large and small doses were arranged quasi-randomly so that each transition was experienced either 25 times (smaller dose condition) or 12 times (larger dose condition). The four types of transitions in this study were large dose to small dose (rich to lean), small dose to large dose (lean to rich), large dose to large dose (rich to rich), and small dose to small dose (lean to lean). Once the monkeys completed the FR, the center key light turned green and the cocaine infusion began. Once the infusion was complete, a 2-s timeout started in which the green light turned off. Every response made during this timeout started the timer over again. The pause timer started once the 2-s timeout was over. In general, the PRP was the longest after a large dose was received and the upcoming dose was small. This effect was more pronounced the higher the FR requirement. Galuska et al. (2007) did not find any major differences in the rich to lean pause duration between the two dose size conditions (0.003 mg/kg and 0.03 mg/kg versus 0.0056 mg/kg and 0.056 mg/kg). The run rates were the highest in the transition from a small dose to a large dose (lean to rich), and lowest in the transition from a large dose to a small dose (rich to lean). These results are in line with those of Perone and Courtney (1992). Transitioning from a favorable condition to an unfavorable condition disrupted responding in the form of longer pausing before a run of responding began, and a lower rate of responding during the run of responding. Applied Implications The aversive functions of rich-to-lean transitions also have been studied in humans. It is beneficial to study the effects of transitions in all populations, but it is particularly helpful for populations that might display more challenging behaviors. Certain individuals with developmental disabilities might self-injure, aggress, or display other types of destructive behaviors. Studying these individuals in the context of rich-to-lean transition contingencies 15 would be helpful in identifying when these behaviors occur most frequently. This could lead to adjustments in the environment that would decrease the probability of these behaviors occurring. Bejarano, Williams, and Perone (2003) used similar techniques as in the previous studies (Perone & Courtney, 1992; Wade-Galuska et al., 2005; Galuska et al., 2007) to investigate richto-lean transitions in humans. Their participant was a man with mild mental retardation. The apparatus used to study his responding was a touch screen computer that had a dispenser by its side that provided reinforcers. The reinforcers consisted of either a quarter (large reinforcer) or a penny (small reinforcer). Responding was maintained on a multiple schedule using a match-tosample procedure. In both components, an observing response to a single object on the screen was required initially. Once the observing response was made, the object disappeared and a 0.25s feedback tone occurred. Two objects then appeared on the screen, and if the comparison that matched the sample was selected, the participant received one coin (either a quarter or a penny) from the dispenser. The rich component, which consisted of an FR 10 schedule and a quarter as a reinforcer, had black greater- or less-than signs as the stimuli for the match-to-sample task and a red background. The lean component, which consisted of an FR 60 and a penny as a reinforcer, had a black sideways “U” shapes as stimuli for the match-to-sample task and a yellow background. Therefore, the rich component consisted of a lower response effort and a larger reinforcer, and the lean component consisted of a higher response effort and a smaller reinforcer. There was no difference in the difficulty of matching between the rich and lean components. Berajano et al. (2003) found results similar to those in the animal literature. The PRP was the longest when there was a transition from the rich-to-lean component. Berajano et al. also plotted the pause durations in 1-s bins. This showed frequency distributions of each pause time for each type of transition. The longest bin was 19-s, and during the rich-to-lean transition, the 16 highest proportion of PRPs were in this bin. Berajano et al. illustrated that the effects seen in the basic research are also relevant in applied research with humans. When a subject has just experienced a rich environment and transitions to a lean environment, the PRP will be at its longest. This might be an indication that the rich-to-lean transition functions more aversively compared to the other types of transitions. Williams, Saunders, and Perone (2011) extended the applied research with individuals with mild intellectual disabilities. The study consisted of three experiments. The first experiment was similar to the previously discussed study. That is, both the magnitude of the reinforcer and the work requirement were different in the rich and lean conditions. This was done in order to create ideal conditions to observe the effect of the previous reinforcer and upcoming work requirement on the PRP. The rich and lean components were presented according to a multiple schedule. The stimuli for both the rich and lean components were displayed at the same time, however, only one of the schedules was active. In this experiment they were able to replicate the results of Perone and Courtney (1992); that is the longest PRP occurred during the rich-to-lean transition. The second experiment was an attempt to replicate the results from Perone and Courtney ’s Experiment 2 in which mixed and multiple schedules were used. During a mixed schedule pausing would be under control of the previous reinforcer regardless of the upcoming ratio and during a multiple schedule pausing would be under control of both the previous reinforcer and the upcoming ratio. The results were consistent with Perone and Courtney’s findings. In the mixed schedule, pausing was extended following a rich component, regardless of the upcoming component. In the multiple schedule, there was an interaction between the past reinforcer and workload and upcoming reinforcer and workload. When the past ratio was rich and the signaled upcoming ratio was lean, pausing was the longest. In the third experiment, the 17 sensitivity to changes in the ratio and the reinforcement in the rich and lean components were examined. In some of the sessions the FRs were different while the reinforcers were the same, the FRs were the same while the reinforcers were different, or both were different from each other creating a double discrepancy in the rich and lean components. It was found that sensitivity to reinforcers or ratio varied depending on the individual. The study of the PRP during rich-to-lean transitions has been the focus in the animal and human literature. There are additional studies, however, that focus on other behaviors that may increase or decrease during rich-to-lean transitions. Additional Evidence of the Aversive Function of Rich-to-Lean Transitions A situation is considered aversive, if a subject engages in behavior to escape from and/or avoid it (Azrin, 1961; Thompson, 1964). Therefore, if rich-to-lean transitions are aversive, it would be expected that individuals would engage in an escape response to avoid the transition in place. Perone (2003) reported a study that illustrated this point by showing that pigeons’ escape responses increased during rich-to-lean transitions. The general method was similar to that of Perone and Courtney (1992); however, an escape key was available during 5 out of 10 presentations of each type of transition during a session. The pigeons’ key pecking was reinforced according to a multiple schedule of food presentation. There were six conditions in which the FR was held constant (FR 20, FR40, FR60, FR80, FR 100, FR 120). During half of the transitions, a side key was lit simultaneously with the center food keys, and the pigeon could respond on it in order to turn off the food key light and the house light. During transitions in which the escape key was available and the pigeon initiated responding on the center key first (i.e., made a single peck on the FR key), the escape key was turned off. If the pigeon pecked the escape key first, however, the center key light and house light were turned off and the escape key 18 dimmed until the pigeon pecked the escape key a second time. Once the pigeon pecked the escape key a second time, the center key light and house light turned on, the escape key light turned off, and the pigeon had to finish the FR currently in place. As the FR increased across sessions, pausing and number of escape responses in the rich to lean component increased. This suggests that it was aversive for the pigeon to respond after a large reinforcer had been received when the upcoming signaled reinforcer was small. Responding in order to terminate the escape period, however, might suggest that the aversiveness of the rich-to-lean transition decreases over time. The pigeon did not completely avoid the current FR, but only postponed its completion by responding to escape. As the duration since the last reinforcer received increased, the aversiveness of responding for the smaller reinforcer decreased and the pigeon responded to terminate the escape contingency. Holtyn (2010) supported the claim that escape responses indicate that the rich-to-lean transitions have aversive functions. It was unclear in Perone (2003) whether the escape response was maintained by escape from the FR schedule in place or escape from the stimulus indicating the upcoming reinforcer; that is, a stimulus change. In order to determine what was functionally maintaining escape responses, Holtyn conducted an experiment in which a response on the escape key changed the schedule in place from a multiple schedule to a mixed schedule. Therefore, the FR schedule remained in effect and responses on the response key satisfied the FR requirement. Thus, responding on the escape key presumably would be maintained by the escape of the stimulus that indicated the upcoming reinforcer. During preliminary training, four White Carneau pigeons’ key pecking was maintained by an FR 100 schedule with two alternating components consisting of a small (1 s) or large (7 s) reinforcer. The training phase consisted of sessions in which the schedule in place alternated daily between a mixed schedule (center key 19 light transilluminated by white) one day and a multiple schedule (center key light transilluminated by one of two colors depending on the upcoming reinforcer) the other day. In the experimental conditions, a multiple schedule was initially in place in which reinforcers were received by responding on the center key. In half of the components, the left key light, called the stimulus-termination key (i.e., escape key) was transilluminated by the same color as the center key. If the initial response was to the stimulus-termination key, the stimulus on the center key changed to white, and the termination key turned off. Once the pigeon completed the FR requirement, the mixed schedule would change back into the multiple schedule. If the initial response was made to the center key, the stimulus for the upcoming reinforcer remained, the termination key turned off, and the pigeon was required to complete the FR. There was no condition in which responding on the termination key allowed the subject to escape the FR in place. Responding on the termination key would only change the multiple schedule into a mixed schedule. The FR requirement was adjusted across conditions, ranging from 20 to 200. Each condition lasted at least 20 sessions. In general, Holtyn (2010) found that the pause duration was the longest in the rich-to-lean transition, and the pause during the rich-to-lean transitions increased as the FR requirement increased during experimental conditions. The number of responses on the stimulus-termination key also increased as a function of increasing the FR requirement. Though it was variable across pigeons, generally the number of responses on the termination key was highest when the upcoming reinforcer was lean regardless of the previous reinforcer. The proportion of time spent in the mixed schedule was variable across birds as well. The pigeons spent the most time in the mixed schedule when the stimulus indicated an upcoming lean reinforcer. The FR requirement did not appear to affect the proportion of time spent in the mixed schedule. It was demonstrated 20 in this study that the stimulus indicating the lean upcoming reinforcer has an aversive function regardless of the past reinforcer, as evidenced by the responses made to escape the stimulus that represented the upcoming lean reinforcer. Long (2005) also studied the aversive function of the lean stimulus, in the context of richto-lean transitions. An observing response was used in this study, in order to determine if pigeons would respond to change the schedule in place from a mixed schedule to a multiple schedule. Four White Carneau pigeons were trained using the same procedures that were used in Perone and Courtney (1992). During training, a multiple schedule was in effect on the center key, in which one color indicated an upcoming small reinforcer and a different color indicated an upcoming large reinforcer. In a different phase of training, pigeons were trained to make observing responses by pecking one of the two side keys. During the experimental conditions, a mixed schedule with an FR 100 was in place on the center key; in one component the reinforcer was small (1 s) and in the other component, the reinforcer was large (6 s or 7 s). Across phases, the left and/or right key also would be lit on half of the trials. A peck to that key, an observing response, changed the mixed schedule on the center key to a multiple schedule. The stimulus signaling the upcoming reinforcer was presented and the pigeon was required to finish the current FR under the multiple schedule. Once the FR was completed, the mixed schedule stimulus would turn back on. Depending on the condition, an observing response on one of the side keys produced the rich or lean stimulus, only the rich stimulus, only the lean stimulus, or neither stimulus. These observing conditions were counterbalanced across the two side keys. Long (2005) calculated the probability of responding on the observing key as a function of the observing conditions; that is, whether a response on the observing key turned on the either the rich or lean stimulus, only the rich stimulus, or only the lean stimulus. Responding was 21 maintained on the observing key when the observing response produced the rich or lean stimulus or when the observing response produced the rich stimulus only. Responding was not maintained on the observing key when the observing response produced only the lean stimulus. Long suggested that the stimulus that signals an upcoming rich reinforcer is a conditioned reinforcer; its production maintained responding on the observing response key in any condition in which the rich stimulus was produced. The results also suggest that the stimulus signaling an upcoming lean reinforcer is neither a conditioned reinforcer nor a conditioned punisher. It is not a conditioned reinforcer because the production of the upcoming lean stimulus did not maintain observing. It is not a conditioned punisher because the presence of the upcoming lean stimulus did not punish responses on the observing key that produced either the upcoming rich or lean stimulus. Long (2005) might have made an incorrect assumption when she stated that the stimulus associated with the lean upcoming reinforcer was not a conditioned punisher. An issue that needs to be considered with respect to the aversive function of the lean stimulus is that in the Perone and Courtney (1992) procedure, the stimulus that indicates an upcoming lean reinforcer represents two different types of transitions (rich to lean and lean to lean). This makes it difficult to tease apart its function as a punisher. In contrasting the two types of transitions involved, the transition from a rich to a lean reinforcer is different than the transition from a lean to a lean reinforcer. Holtyn (2010) demonstrated that escape responding was maintained by termination of the lean stimulus, but it is not clear if responding is maintained due to the aversive function of the stimulus or the aversive function of the transition itself. Statement of the Problem 22 In the previous literature published on rich-to-lean transitions (Perone and Courtney, 1992; Wade et al., 2004, 2007; Berajano et al., 2003; Perone, 2003; Holtyn, 2010; Long, 2005) the transitions in all of the studies have been signaled by only two stimuli: the stimulus that signals whether the upcoming reinforcer was small or large. Much emphasis has been placed on the role of the stimulus that indicates an upcoming lean reinforcer (Holtyn, 2010; Long, 2005). It might be more beneficial, however, to focus on the aversive function of individual transitions. It is not possible to do this by observing transitions in which only the upcoming rich or lean reinforcers are indicated by discriminative stimuli. If each transition was indicated by an individual signal, then each transition could be assessed separately and the PRP for each transition could be compared to the PRP of other transitions. There is applied importance in studying the effects of rich-to-lean transitions (Berajano, Williams, & Perone, 2003; Williams, Saunders, & Perone, 2011). All individuals can have trouble transition from one activity to the next, but for individuals with developmental disabilities the negative effects of transitions can include aggression, self-injurious behaviors, and escape (Sainato, Strain, Lefebvre, & Rapp, 1987; Waters, Lerman, & Hovanetz, 2009). These aversive effects of transitioning could be thought of as being functionally the same as the extended PRP seen in the animal literature during rich-to-lean transitions. If this statement is true, it would be beneficial to study a way that could decrease the aversiveness of the transition as evidenced by a decrease in the length of the PRP between transitions. Mazur and Hyslop (1982) studied the effects of adding a timeout following reinforcement on the PRP. Pigeons responded on an FR 50, 100, or 150. During a session, half of the ratios began with a 30-s timeout. During timeout, the house light remained on, but the key on which the pigeons responded was turned off for 30 s. Once the time out was complete, the key was lit 23 again, and the FR was completed. The PRP following a timeout was less than when there was no timeout between ratios. Therefore, it was proposed that adding a timeout between each of the transitions would decrease the PRP, particularly during the rich-to-lean transition, possibly decreasing the aversive function of each transition type. Phase 1.In Phase 1, small and large reinforcer magnitudes were manipulated in order to observe a difference in PRP during each type of transition. This phase established the transitions of large to large (rich to rich), large to small (rich to lean), small to large (lean to rich), and small to small (lean to lean). Phase 2.Once control of each transition was established, two timeout conditions (i.e., blackout and stimulus-termination) were conducted to examine if adding a timeout in between transitions would decrease PRP. The current study helps to answer the following two experimental questions: (1) Will stimulus control form for each transitions if four discriminative stimuli are used to signal each of the four transition types using a multiple schedule. (2) Will adding a timeout between each transition decrease the PRP between each transition type, therefore decreasing the aversive function of the transitions? Method Subjects The subjects were four Racing Homer pigeons (Pigeons 190, 192, 8381,and 10312 were born in 2007, 2008, 2006, and 2008, respectively).Two pigeons (Pigeons 190 and 8381) had previous experience responding on random interval (RI) schedules and received acute administration of d-amphetamine more than 1 year before the current experiment. Two pigeons were experimentally naïve (Pigeons 192 and 10312). Pigeons 192, 8381, and 10312 were 24 maintained at 85% free feeding weight, and Pigeon 190 was maintained at 80% free feeding weight. Subjects received Milo grain during experimental sessions and, if necessary, were fed a supplementary amount of Purina Pigeon Checkers after experimental sessions to maintain their specified weight. Fresh water and grit were provided at all times while the pigeons were in the colony room cages. The colony is located in a temperature and humidity regulated room on a 12:12 hr light/dark time schedule, starting at 7 a.m. Apparatus Experimental sessions were conducted in four identical sound-attenuating Med Associates, Inc. modular operant chambers, ENV-007, with exhaust fans that ran throughout all sessions. To further block extraneous noise, white-noise was played on multiple speakers within the running room. The dimensions of the inside of the operant chambers were 30.5 cm L x 24.1 cm W x 29.2 cm H. On the front wall there are three response keys, each 2.5 cm in diameter. All response keys were equidistant from each other (6.6 cm from each center), with the middle key in the center of the wall. Each key was 21.5 cm above the floor of the operant chamber. In this experiment, only the center key was used, and it was transilluminated by white, green, or red. A fourth color light was required for this experiment. To accomplish this both the green and red light were transilluminted at the same time to in order to produce an orange color light. The force required for a key peck to register ranged from .18-0.38 N across boxes. When enough force was applied there was a single audible click of a relay within the chamber. There was a light (1.5 cm in diameter), on the center of the back wall 2.5 cm from the top that provided general illumination. The reinforcer of milo grain was delivered with a hopper that was accessed through a 6.5 cm x 5.5 cm opening centered on the front wall, 12.8 cm below the center key. Preliminary Training. 25 Pigeons 190 and 8381 did not require preliminary training as they already key pecked. Pigeons 192 and 10312 were both experimentally naïve and therefore required initial training. First, Pigeons 192 and 10312 were placed in the chamber with the houselight on for 30-min sessions over 5 days to habituate them to being in an enclosed chamber. Then hopper training was necessary in order to teach them to eat from the hopper. During the initial sessions of hopper training the houselight remained on, and the hopper light was on while the hopper was raised. The food hopper was initially raised, and milo was available to the pigeons. Using a remote from outside of the chamber, the experimenter quickly dropped the hopper and then raised it once again. This oriented the pigeon to the hopper and allowed habituation to the noise the hopper makes. There was a maximum of 50 hopper presentations, or until the pigeon was eating from the hopper reliably, within each training session. Initially, each pigeon was required to eat continuously from the hopper for 10 s without pulling its head out. Once the pigeon ate continuously for 10 s, the hopper was dropped and immediately raised. This pattern repeated, but the requirement decreased by 1 s, until the pigeons continuously ate from the hopper. Once the pigeon completed this initial hopper training, the process repeated once again, except that the houselight was off while the hopper was raised with the hopper light on. Next, key pecking was shaped by differentially reinforcing closer and closer approximations of the target behavior of key pecking. The center light was transilluminated white during the shaping sessions. The house light was on, but the side keys were dark. Each presentation of milo lasted 4 s. This process took about five sessions for each pigeon. Once all of the pigeons were hopper trained and key pecking, the next step was to train all pigeons to peck the center key when it is tranilluminated white, green, red, and orange. Over the course of 12 days, daily sessions were held in which a single color (white, green, red, or 26 orange) was transilluminated on the center key, the houselight was on, and pecking was reinforced on a fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule, until 50 reinforcers were received. A 4-s presentation of the reinforcer was provided. A single key light color was experienced for 2 days in a row. The pattern of the key lights during training was red, white, orange, and green. Once the pigeons reliably pecked to all color stimuli on the center key, the process repeated one more time. General Procedure The general procedures of this experiment were based on that of Perone and Courtney (1992). Experimental sessions were held 7 days a week at approximately the same of time of day. In each session, there was a sequence of 40 transitions and 41 reinforcers received. There were 40 sequences available, and the order in which they were presented daily was chosen semirandomly using a random sequence generator. Half of the sequences included one extra large reinforcer, and the other half of the sequences included one extra small reinforcer (i.e., the one delivered at the start of the session). Each transition type (lean to lean, lean to rich, rich to lean, and rich to rich) was presented 10 times within a session. Each transition type was signaled by its own colored discriminative stimulus (four total discriminative stimuli). In a session, no more than four small or large reinforcers could be received consecutively. In the beginning of each session, the only light illuminated was the houselight for 3 s, and then a response-independent presentation of a small or large reinforcer occurred. Following the initial presentation, the first discriminative stimulus turned on. Phase 1.A multiple schedule was in place in which the FR remained the same for each component and the reinforcer magnitude was identical (4 s/4 s), but the key light changed color indicating the four components. Because there was no difference in the reinforcer amount in, there were no transitions from small to small, small to large, large to small, or large to large. In 27 this phase, milo was presented for 4 s after the completion of each ratio. There was no intercomponent interval. The FR requirement gradually raised from FR 1 to FR 50 (Pigeons 190, 192, 10312) or FR 80 (Pigeon 8381) over the course of 17-20 sessions This process determined if each of the pigeons paused more or less in the presence of a particular stimulus. The hierarchy of the PRP for the key light colors determined what stimulus would be used for each type of transition for each pigeon. To control for color bias, the stimulus that produced the longest PRP when reinforcers were equal was used as the small to large (lean to rich) discriminative stimulus when the magnitudes changed. Likewise, the stimulus that produced the shortest PRP was used as the large to small (rich to lean) discriminative stimulus when the magnitudes changed. The second longest PRP stimulus was the large to large (rich to rich) transition, and the third longest PRP stimulus was designated to the small to small (lean to lean) transition. Table 1 provides the discriminative stimuli for each transition type for each subject. The reinforcer magnitudes were adjusted until the ideal conditions for pausing were in place (i.e., the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition was the longest compared to the other transitions). Once a pigeon demonstrated extended pausing during the rich-to-lean transitions, the small and large reinforcers were no longer adjusted. The conditions were labeled with the small reinforcer on the left and the large reinforcer on the right. All four pigeons experienced the 1.25 s/ 4.25 s condition. Three of the pigeons (Pigeons 192, 8381, 10312) experienced the 1.25 s/ 6.25 s condition. One pigeon (Pigeon 8381) experienced the 1.25 s/ 8.25 s condition. The 0.25 s was added into each of the times in order to both allow for the hopper to come up and the pigeon to move to the hopper. In the conditions that follow, the small magnitude remained at 1.25 s, while the large magnitude increased by 2 s for each manipulation. Table 2 provides the number 28 Table 1 The Discriminative Stimulus of Each Transition Type for Each Subject Pigeon Lean to Rich Rich to Lean Rich to Rich Lean to Lean 190 Green White Red Orange 192 Green White Orange Red 8381 Red White Orange Green 10312 Green Red Orange White Note: Discriminative stimulus colorbased on the initial PRP length in the presence of each stimulus. There was a reversal of longest to shortest and its associated stimulus for experimental conditions (i.e., the stimulus that occasioned the longest PRP became the lean-to-rich stimulus and the stimulus that occasion the shortest PRP became the rich-to-lean stimulus). 29 Table 2 Description of Different Conditions as Categorized by the Magnitudes of the Lean and Rich Reinforcer Presentations in Terms of How Many Seconds Reinforcer is Presented Pigeon Small Magnitude Large Magnitude Sessions 190 4s 4s 50 1.25 s 4.25 223 4s 4s 50 1.25 s 4.25 s 55 1.25 s 6.25 s 170 4s 4s 58 1.25 s 4.25 s 60 1.25 s 6.25 s 140 1.25 s 8.25 s 27 4s 4s 50 1.25 s 4.25 s 55 1.25 s 6.25 s 144 192 8381 10312 30 of sessions for each of the conditions the pigeons experienced during Phase 1. Additional manipulations to the FR were required for Pigeons 8381 (FR 150), 10312 (FR 40), and 190 (FR 35). For Pigeons 190 and 10312, a decrease in FR was required as the subjects were not completing sessions. For Pigeon 8381 an increase in FR was required as the subject did not have extended pausing during the rich-to-lean transition with smaller FR. The magnitudes of the reinforcers were adjusted in order to observe the ideal conditions for extended pausing in the rich-to-lean component for each pigeon. The stability criterion to move on to the next condition was that all four pause duration measures lacked increasing or decreasing trends and were stable over the last 10 sessions. The stability criterion to move on to Phase 2 was a clear distinction in pause duration between the rich-to-lean transition and the other three transition types. That is, the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition should have been be 20 s greater than the PRP during the other three transitions, as demonstrated in the Perone and Courtney (1992) study. Phase 2. The same schedule as in Phase 1 was in effect during this phase, however, a timeout was added following each reinforcer during probe sessions. These probe session occurred based on stability; that is there was no increasing or decreasing trend in PRPs for each transition. Two types of timeouts were examined; blackout and stimulus termination timeouts. Blackout timeouts consisted of a blackout in the chamber following each reinforcer. Three timeout durations (15 s, 30 s, and 60 s) were tested in a quasi-random order. All four pigeons were included in this condition. There was a 1.5-hr time limit during the probe sessions. Table 3 lists the number of timeout sessions at each timeout duration each pigeon experienced during blackout timeout. Once all of the blackout timeout sessions were completed, stimulus-termination timeout sessions began. Pigeon 8381 was not included in this condition due to the instability of his 31 Table 3 Number of Sessions for Each Timeout Duration Subjects Experienced During Blackout Timeout Sessions Pigeon 15 s 30 s 60 s Total 190 4 3 2 9 192 3 6 2 11 8381 2 1 1 4 10312 2 5 2 9 32 baseline sessions. Stimulus-termination timeout sessions consisted of stimulus-termination (i.e., center keylight remained off, houselight turned on) following each reinforcer. One timeout duration was used during this condition (30 s). The 30-s timeout was chosen as that was the condition in which the most decreases were seen in PRP during the blackout timeout sessions. There was a 1.5-hr time limit during the probe sessions. Table 4 lists the number of timeout sessions for each pigeon during stimulus-termination timeout. Data Analysis Data from each session was recorded using MedPC behavioral software. In Phase 1, the number of responses, PRP duration between transitions, and time per ratio was recorded. The PRP and run rate of each transition type were of primary interest. Recording of responses and time was done in 0.01 s intervals. For each session, the median PRP duration and run rate was analyzed for each transition type. The median of the median PRP for each transition over the last 10 days was analyzed in Microsoft Excel daily in order to compare the transition types. In Phase 2, the number of responses, PRP duration following the timeout, and time per ratio was recorded. The PRP and run rate of each transition were still of primary interest. Each timeout condition (i.e., each duration during blackout timeout sessions; stimulus-termination sessions) was analyzed separately within subjects. Proportion of baseline of PRP (run rate) was analyzed by dividing the mean of the timeout sessions for every transition by the mean of baseline sessions (sessions immediately before the timeout session) for every transition. Absolute change of PRP (run rate) was analyzed by subtracting the mean of baseline sessions from the mean of timeout sessions. 33 Table 4 Number of Timeout Sessions Each Subject Experienced During Stimulus-Termination Timeout Sessions Pigeon 30 s 190 2 192 5 10312 3 34 Results Phase 1 Figure 1 shows the median (inter-quartile range) PRP (s) for each pigeon when the upcoming reinforcer was small (white circles) and when the upcoming reinforcer was large (black circles) as a function of the past reinforcer. The size of the reinforcers (s) is indicated on the x-axis; the small reinforcer amount is on the left and the large reinforcer amount is on the right. Therefore, each transition type is represented on each individual graph (rich-rich, rich-lean, lean-rich, lean-lean). Every condition that the pigeons experienced is represented across graphs (i.e., 4 s /4 s, 1.25 s /4.25 s, 1.25 s /6.25 s, 1.25 s /8.25 s). Figure 1 shows that during the 4 s /4 s condition (left column) for three of the four pigeons (Pigeon 190, 8381, and 10312), the PRP in the presence of all stimuli was relatively equal. The median PRP (s) in the presence of each of the stimuli for Pigeon 8381, 10312, and 190 was 6.1, 5.03, 8.15 s, respectively. The median PRP in the presence of the green stimulus that represented the future lean-to-rich transition for Pigeon 192 was slightly elevated relative to the PRP in the presence of the other stimuli. The average PRP (s) of the other three transition stimuli was 8.0 s; whereas, the PRP (s) in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus was 12.2 s. In general, as the difference between the small and large reinforcers became more extreme, the PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus increased, while the PRP in the presence of the three other stimuli remained relatively equal for each pigeon. For Pigeon 8381, there was relatively no change in PRP for any of the transitions in the 1.25 s/ 4.25 s condition. In the 1.25 s/ 6.25 s condition, the PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (19.95 s) was 10 s longer than that of the PRP in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (9.4 s). In the 1.25 s/ 8.25 s condition, the PRP in the presence of both the rich-to-lean and lean-to-lean stimuli 35 Figure 1 Post-Reinforcement Pauses During Each Transition Across Reinforcer Magnitudes for Each Subject 45 8381 30 15 0 4.00 75 4.00 1.25 4.25 1.25 6.25 4.00 1.25 4.25 1.25 6.25 4.00 1.25 4.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 8.25 10312 50 Pause (s) 25 0 4.00 30 192 20 10 0 4.00 80 60 190 Upcoming Small Upcoming Large 40 20 0 4.00 4.00 1.25 4.25 Past Reinforcer (s) Figure 1.Median (IQR) post-reinforcement pause (s) for the last 10 days of each condition for each of the four pigeons. Past reinforcer is labeled on the x-axis, and the upcoming reinforcers are indicated by the white circles (small) and black circles (large). Note the different scales on the y-axis. 36 decreased. The PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (17.6 s), however, remained 10 s longer than that of the PRP in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (7.7 s). For Pigeon 10312, the PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (21.7 s) was 10 s longer than that of the PRP in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (11.6 s) in the 1.25 s/4.25 s condition. In the 1.25 s/6.25 s condition, the PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (23.5 s) was 15 s longer than that of the PRP in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (8.3 s). For Pigeon 192, the PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (18.9 s) was 14.6 s longer than that of the PRP in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (4.3 s) during the 1.25 s/4.25 s condition. The PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (20.6 s) was 18.7 s longer than that of the PRP in the presence of the lean-to-lean transition (1.9 s). For Pigeon 190, the PRP in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (48.8 s) was 42 s longer than that of the PRP in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (6.8 s) in the 1.25 s/4.25 s condition. Figure 2 shows the median run rates (responses/min) during the subsequent ratio of each transition type over the last 10 stable days of each condition for each pigeon. The data are graphed in the same way as in Figure 1. During the 4 s/4 s condition, the run rates were relatively equal in the presence of each stimulus. The average run rate for Pigeons 8381, 10312, 192, and 190 was 216, 215.5, 125.5, and 242.2 responses/min, respectively. For Pigeon 8381, there was relatively no change in run rate for any of the transitions in the 1.25 s/ 4.25 s condition. In the 1.25 s/ 6.25 s condition, the run rate in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (179) was 43 responses/min slower than that of the run rate in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (222). In the 1.25 s/ 8.25 s condition, the run rates in the presence of all four stimuli decreased; however, the run rate in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (141) remained the lowest and 37 Figure 2 Run Rates During Each Transition Across Reinforcer Magnitudes for Each Subject 400 300 200 100 8381 0 4.0 4.0 1.25 4.25 1.25 6.25 4.00 1.25 4.25 1.25 6.25 4.00 1.25 4.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 8.25 Median Run Rates (responses/min) 300 200 100 10312 0 4.00 200 100 192 0 4.00 300 Upcoming Small Upcoming Large 200 100 190 0 4.00 4.00 1.25 4.25 Past Reinforcer (s) Figure 2. Median (IQR) run rates (responses/minute). Shown are the last 10 days of each condition for each of the four pigeons. Past reinforcer is labeled on the x-axis, and the upcoming reinforcers are indicated by the white circles (small) and black circles (large). Note the different scales on the y-axis. 38 was 48 response/min slower than that of the run rate in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (141). For Pigeon 10312, the run rates in the presence of all stimuli remained relatively equal within and across conditions. The average run rates in the 4 s/4 s, 1.25 s/4.25 s, and 1.25 s/6.25 s conditions were 215, 221, and 221 responses/min, respectively. For Pigeon 192, the run rate in the presence of the rich-to-lean (102) and the lean-to-lean (100) stimuli were on average 13 responses/min slower than the lean-to-rich (118) and rich-to-rich (113) stimuli in the 1.25 s/4.25 s condition. In the 1.25 s/6.25 s condition, the run rate in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (151) was 18 responses/min slower than that of the run rate in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (169). For Pigeon 190, the run rate in the presence of the rich-to-lean stimulus (134) was 50 responses/min slower than that of the run rate in the presence of the lean-to-lean stimulus (184) in the 1.25 s/4.25 s condition. It should be noted in the first column in Figures 1 and 2 each symbol is not representative of the actual transition it symbolizes. Changes to discriminative stimuli were made simultaneously to the start of the reinforcer magnitude adjustments. Phase 2 Blackout Timeout. Figure 3 shows the average proportion of baseline (left column) and absolute change from baseline (right column) of the average PRP during each transition (R = Rich; L= Lean) when 15-s (black bars), 30-s (light-gray bars), and 60-s (dark-gray bars) timeouts occurred. For the proportion of baseline graphs, a dashed line at 1 represents no change from baseline. For the absolute change graphs, a dashed line at 0 represents no change from baseline. If a bar is above the dashed line, the PRP increased during these conditions; if a bar is below the dashed line, the PRP decreased during these conditions. The average absolute changes from 39 Figure 3 Average Proportion of Baseline and Absolute Change from Baseline of Post-Reinforcement Pause During Blackout Timeout for Each Subject 20 6 8381 10 4 0 2 -10 0 Absolute Change from Baseline (s) 6 20 10312 Proportion of Baseline 4 2 0 6 192 4 10 0 -10 50 40 30 20 10 2 0 -10 0 6 190 15 s 30 s 60 s 200 150 4 100 50 2 0 -50 0 RL LR RR RL LL LR RR LL Transition Figure 3. Average proportion of baseline (left column) and average absolute change from baseline (right column) of PRP (s) during blackout timeout. Shown are each transition (R = Richreinforcer and L = Lean reinforcer) on the x-axis for each pigeon across timeout durations. The dashed line represents baseline. Note the different scales on the y-axis on right column graphs. 40 baseline data are included with the proportion because although the proportion of baseline may appear to be large, the change in absolute pause in some cases may only be a couple seconds. For example, if a change in proportion is 0.5, the absolute change could either be 50 s (100 s during baseline) or 2 s (4 s during baseline). It is also beneficial to have the visual aid of average absolute change as it gives a more complete picture of how PRP was affected by timeout. There was no systematic effect of increasing timeout duration within or across pigeons. The majority of changes in PRP compared to baseline were increases. There were only 14 decreases in PRP relative to baseline out of 48 opportunities (between pigeons and timeout durations). Pigeon 8381 is the only subject in which the proportion of baseline decreased during all three timeout in the rich-to-lean transition. The PRP decreased to 0.88, 0.79, and 0.42 of baseline for the 15, 30, and 60-s timeouts, respectively. For Pigeon 10312, PRP decreased to 0.84 of baseline during the 30-s timeout. For Pigeon 192, PRP decreased to 0.79 and 0.96 of baseline during the 15 and 30-s timeouts, respectively. The decrease during the 30-s timeout, however, was only 0.54 s. For Pigeon 190, PRP decreased to 0.72 of baseline during the 30-s timeout. There in one notable decrease in PRP during the lean-to-lean transition. For Pigeon 8381, PRP decreased to 0.52 of baseline, a 7.9-s decrease, during the 30-s timeout. Figure 4 shows the average proportion of baseline (left column) and absolute change from baseline (right column) for run rate (responses/min). The graphs are arranged identical to those in Figure 3. The majority of changes in run rate compared to baseline were decreases. There were only 14 increases in run rate relative to baseline, out of 48 opportunities (between pigeons and timeout durations). The largest decreases in proportion of baseline were observed during the rich-to-lean transition. In the 15-s timeout, run rate decreased to 0.69, 0.73, and 0.73 of baseline, for Pigeons 8381, 10312, and 192, respectively. In the 30-s timeout, run rate 41 Figure 4 Average Proportion of Baseline and Absolute Change from Baseline of Run Rates During Blackout Timeout for Each Subject 1.5 75 8381 0 1.0 -75 0.5 -150 0.0 50 1.5 Change from Baseline (responses/min) 10312 Proportion of Baseline 1.0 0.5 0.0 1.5 192 1.0 0.5 0 -50 30 0 -30 -60 0.0 1.5 190 30 0 1.0 -30 -60 0.5 15 s 30 s 60 s -90 -120 0.0 RL LR RR RL LL LR RR LL Transition Figure 4. Average proportion of baseline (left column) and average absolute change from baseline (right column) of run rate (responses/min) during blackout timeout. Shown are each transition (R = Richreinforcer and L = Lean reinforcer) on the x-axis for each pigeon across timeout durations. The dashed line represents baseline. Note the different scales on the y-axis on right column graphs. 42 decreased to 0.36, 0.93, 0.59, and 0.16 of baseline for Pigeons 8381, 10312, 192, and 190, respectively. Although there was a large difference in the proportion of change between Pigeon 8381 (0.36) and Pigeon 192 (0.59), the absolute changes from baseline were relatively equal; the decreases were 74.8 and 64.5 responses/min, respectively. In the 60-s timeout, run rate decreased to 0.6, 0.85, and 0.9 of baseline for Pigeons 8381, 10312, and 192, respectively. For Pigeon 8381, there were also large decreases in run rate in the lean-to-lean transition during the 15 and 60-s timeouts. In the 15-s timeout, run rate decreased to 0.41 of baseline. In the 60-s timeout, run rate decreased to 0.29 of baseline. Stimulus-Termination Timeout. Figure 5 shows the average proportion of baseline of PRP (top graph) and absolute change (bottom graph) for PRP when a 30-s timeout occurred after each reinforcer during which time only the key light was turned off. The graph is arranged similar to Figure 3. The vertical bars now, however, represent the following individual subjects; Pigeon 10312 (black bars), 192 (light-gray bars), and 190 (dark-gray bars). Pigeon 8381 was excluded from this part of the study due to instability of daily PRP durations in each transition. Overall, the PRP during timeout sessions decreased across all pigeons and transition types. There is one exception during the lean-to-rich transition for Pigeon 192, in which the PRP remained at baseline. Decreases were most notable during the rich-to-lean and the rich-to-rich transitions. For Pigeons 10312, 192, and 190, PRP decreased to 0.71, 0.98, and 0.68 of baseline, respectively, during the rich-to-lean transition. Although the proportion of change for Pigeon 190 was 0.68, it was only a 1.7-s decrease. During the rich-to-rich transition, the PRP decreased to 0.41, 0.39, and 0.16 of baseline, for Pigeons 10312, 192, and 190, respectively. The decreases for Pigeons 10312 and 192, however, were only 2.75 and 0.85 s, respectively. 43 Figure 5 Average Proportion of Baseline and Absolute Change from Baseline of Post-Reinforcement Pause during Stimulus-Termination Timeout for Each Subject 1.2 Proportion of Baseline 30-s TO 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 Change from Baseline (s) 5 0 -5 -10 -15 -20 Pigeon 10312 Pigeon 192 Pigeon 190 -25 RL LR RR LL Transition Figure 5. Change in proportion (top graph) and the median absolute change from baseline (bottom graph) in PRP (s) during the second timeout condition. Shown are each transition (R = Rich reinforcer and L = Lean reinforcer) on the x-axis for each pigeon. The dashed line represents baseline. 44 Figure 6 shows the average proportion of baseline (top graph) and absolute change (bottom graph) in run rate. The graphs are set up identical to that of Figure 5, except the y-axis for the bottom graph now represents responses/min. Overall, there are no systematic effects of timeout on run rate across transition types and the proportions of change remained relatively close to 1. There were effects of timeout, however, seen within subjects. The run rates for Pigeon 10312 remained relatively close to baseline across all transitions. The run rates only slightly decreased to 0.98, 0.98, 0.96, and 0.99 of baseline during the rich-tolean, lean-to-rich, rich-to-rich, and lean-to-lean transitions, respectively. The run rates for Pigeon 192 increased during all transitions. The run rates increased to 1.05, 1.15, 1.3, and 1.1 of baseline during the rich-to-lean, lean-to-rich, rich-to-rich, and lean-to-lean transitions, respectively. The second largest increase across subjects was observed during the rich-to-rich transition for Pigeon 192. The run rate increased by 47.4 responses/min. For Pigeon 190, run rate increased to 1.55 of baseline during the rich-to-lean transition, a 59 response/min increase. This was the largest increase in run rate across all subjects and transitions. Figure 7 shows the median (inter-quartile range) PRP (s) for each pigeon when the upcoming reinforcer was small (white circles) and when the upcoming reinforcer was large (black circles) as a function of the past reinforcer. The size of the reinforcers (s) is indicated on the x-axis; the small reinforce amount is on the left and the large reinforce amount is on the right. Therefore, similar to Figure 1, each transition type is represented on each individual graph (richrich, rich-lean, lean-rich, lean-lean). For all pigeons, the first column represents the last 10 days of baseline before any timeout was introduced into sessions. The second, third, forth, and fifth columns represent the median PRPs for 15-s blackout timeout, 30-s blackout timeout, 60-s blackout timeout, and 30-s stimulus termination timeout, respectively. 45 Figure 6 Average Proportion of Baseline and Absolute Change from Baseline of Run Rates During Stimulus-Termination Timeout for Each Subject 1.8 30-s TO Proportion of Baseline 1.6 1.4 1.2 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 Change from Baseline (responses/min) 0.0 80 Pigeon 10312 Pigeon 192 Pigeon 190 60 40 20 0 -20 RL LR RR LL Transition Figure 6. Change in proportion (top graph) and the median absolute change from baseline (bottom graph) in run rate (responses/min) during the second timeout condition. Shown are each transition (R = Rich reinforcer and L = Lean reinforcer) on the x-axis for each pigeon. The dashed line represents baseline. 46 Figure 7 Post-Reinforcement Pauses During Each Timeout Condition for Each Subject 300 225 Baseline Stimulus Termination 30 s Blackout 15 s Blackout 30 s Blackout 60 s 20 190 15 150 10 75 5 0 0 1.25 4.25 1.25 4.25 1.25 4.25 1.25 4.25 1.25 4.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 8.25 1.25 1.25 8.25 1.25 75 10312 50 Pause (s) 25 0 1.25 20 192 10 0 1.25 30 8381 20 10 0 1.25 8.25 Past Reinforcer (s) 8.25 Upcoming Small Upcoming Large Figure 7. Median (IQR) post-reinforcement pause (seconds). Shown are the medians of the last 10 days of baseline and median of all sessions for each timeout condition for each of the four pigeons. Past reinforcer is labeled on the x-axis, and the upcoming reinforcers are indicated by the white circles (small) and black circles (large). Note the different scales on the y-axis. 47 Compared to baseline, there is no orderly change for the rich-to-lean transition across timeout durations during blackout timeout condition for all pigeons. During the stimulus termination timeout condition, however, the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition decreased for all pigeons. For Pigeons 190, 10312, and 192 PRP across the rich-to-rich, lean-to-lean and leanto-rich transition remained in the same range across all timeout conditions. For Pigeon 190, the median PRP during the rich-to-lean transitions, compared to baseline, increased by 100 s and 200 s during the blackout 15-s timeout and blackout 60-s timeout, respectively. Compared to baseline, the PRP for all transitions during the blackout 30-s timeout condition remained in the same range as baseline. During the stimulus termination timeout condition, the rich-to-lean PRP decreased by 40 s. For Pigeon 10312, all PRPs remained relatively in the same range. The exceptions are the blackout 30-s timeout and the stimulus termination timeout conditions, in which PRP during the rich-to-lean transition decreased by 35 s and 42 s, respectively. For Pigeon 192, the median rich-to-lean PRP during the blackout 30-s timeout and stimulus termination timeout conditions decreased by 10 s and 9 s, respectively. The median rich-to-lean PRP during the blackout 60-s timeout, however, increased by 50 s. For Pigeon 8381, the median PRP of the rich-to-lean transition remained in the same range, with the exception of the 60-s blackout timeout. In the 60-s blackout timeout the PRP decreased by 7 s. Figure 8 shows the median run rates (responses/min) for each transition type. For all pigeons, the first column represents the last 10 days of baseline before any timeout was introduced into sessions. The second, third, forth, and fifth columns represent the median PRPs for 15-s blackout timeout, 30-s blackout timeout, 60-s blackout timeout, and 30-s stimulus termination timeout, respectively. In general, there was no difference or orderly change in run rate during the rich-to-lean transition compared to baseline. 48 Figure 8 Run Rates During Each Timeout Condition for Each Subject Baseline Stimulus Termination 30 s Blackout 15 s Blackout 30 s Blackout 60 s 225 150 75 0 190 Run rate (responses per minute) 1.25 4.25 1.25 4.25 1.25 4.25 1.25 4.25 1.25 4.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 1.25 6.25 8.25 1.25 1.25 8.25 1.25 300 200 100 10312 0 1.25 225 150 75 192 0 1.25 300 200 100 0 8381 1.25 8.25 Past Reinforcer (s) 8.25 Upcoming Small Upcoming Large Figure 8. Median (IQR) run rates (responses/minute). Shown are the medians of the last 10 days of baseline and median of all sessions for each timeout condition for each of the four pigeons. Past reinforcer is labeled on the x-axis, and the upcoming reinforcers are indicated by the white circles (small) and black circles (large). Note the different scales on the y-axis. 49 For Pigeon 190, the median run rate during the rich-to-lean transition remained in the same range across all timeout conditions, with the exception of the 30-s blackout timout in which it decreased by 130 responses/min compared to baseline. For Pigeon 192, run rate during the 15s and 30-s blackout timeout decreased compared to baseline by 50 responses/min. For Pigeon 8381, run rate decreased during the 15-s, 30-s, and 60-s timeout durations in the blackout timeout condition by 56, 100, and 59 responses/min, respectively. Discussion Phase 1 The purpose of Phase 1 was to systematically replicate Perone and Courtney’s (1992) study that focused on PRPs during different types of transitions (i.e., rich-to-lean, lean-to-rich, lean-to-lean, and rich-to-rich), particularly the extended pausing that was observed during the rich-to-lean transition. Additional studies also have replicated Perone and Courtney, using different subject types, reinforcers, and work requirements (Bejarano et al., 2003; Wade-Galuska et al., 2004;Long, 2005; Galuska et al., 2007; Holtyn, 2010; Williams et al., 2011). The previous studies have only used two discriminative stimuli to signal the upcoming component (e.g. reinforcer magnitude, work load). The actual transition, therefore, was not explicitly signaled. In order to assess individual differences in the aversiveness of each transition, as defined by the PRP, the current study used four unique stimuli to signal the current transition. For all four pigeons, relatively longer pausing (15-40 s) were observed when the signaled upcoming reinforcer was small compared to when the signaled upcoming reinforcer was large. There was an interaction between the past and upcoming reinforcers such that when the past reinforcer was large and the signaled upcoming reinforcer was small, there was a longer PRP than in any of the other transitions. This response pattern is similar to what was found using 50 only two stimuli for the upcoming reinforcer in Perone and Courtney (1992) and related literature. Manipulations to FR and reinforcer size were made in order to observe extended pausing in the rich-to-lean transition. It should be noted that across both types of manipulations, changes in the PRP during the other three types of transitions (lean-to-rich, rich-to-rich, lean-to-lean) were relatively small. The FR requirement was adjusted for Pigeons 8381, 10312, and 190. In order to observe the extended pauses during the rich-to-lean transition the FR ranged from a 35 (Pigeon 190) to 150 (Pigeon 8381). Previous literature points out that as FR increases, typically PRP increases (Crossman, 1971; Galuska et al., 2007; Thompson, 1964). For Pigeon 8381, PRP increased as the FR increased. Initially, PRP was relatively equal across transition types during FR manipulations. Once the FR was adjusted from 100 to 150, however, the PRP during the richto-lean transition exceeded the PRPs of the other three transitions. For Pigeons 10312 and 190, PRP decreased as FR decreased. Initially, the PRPs (particularly during the rich-to-lean transition) were so lengthy that the session time limit would occur before all 40 transitions were completed. In this case, decreases to the ratio stopped once each subject was completing every session. The relation between FR and PRP in the current study is that PRP decreased as FR increased across pigeons. Pigeon 8381 had the largest FR, but the shortest PRPs, particularly during the rich-to-lean transition. Pigeons 10312 and 190 had the two smallest FRs, but the longest PRPs, particularly during the rich-to-lean transition. The reinforcer amount also was also adjusted until extended pausing during the rich-tolean transitions was observed. Manipulations to reinforcer size were also done in previous richto-lean literature (Perone and Courtney, 1992; Bejarano et al., 2003; Williams et al., 2011). In these studies, as the difference between the two reinforcer amounts increased, the PRP increased. 51 With the exception of Pigeon 8381, PRP increased as the difference between the small and large reinforcers increased. Even though Pigeon 8381 required the largest FR and largest difference between the small and large reinforcers, this subject’s pauses were still shorter compared to the other three pigeons. In fact, pauses during the rich-to-lean transition for Pigeon 8381 were shorter when there was a 7-s difference than when there was a 5-s difference between the reinforcers. Additional increases in FR were not made, because the subject would not complete sessions when the FR was greater. Additional increases in the difference between the reinforcers were not made as it is unclear if grain would still available in the hopper following more than 8.25 s of consumption. Even though Pigeon 190 required the smallest FR and smallest difference between small and large reinforcers, this subject’s pauses were the longest compared to the other three pigeons. It might be important to note that these two subjects are the two that were in previous experiments in which their key pecking was reinforced according to an RI schedule. Thus, the experimental history of the subjects did not appear to influence the relations between FR and reinforcer amount and pausing. It was necessary to manipulate the FR and reinforcer amounts until extended pausing was seen in the rich-to-lean transition because an important aspect of this study was to see if using four stimuli could occasion extended pausing as was observed in Perone and Courtney (1992) using only two stimuli. If the manipulations had not been made, it would not have appeared that using four stimuli had the same effect on pausing. Similar manipulations to FR size and reinforcer amount were made across subjects in previous subjects in order to see extended pausing during the rich-to-lean transition (Bejarano et al., 2003; Galuska et al., 2007; Perone and Courtney, 1992; Williams et al., 2011). 52 In the current study, three out of the four pigeons’ (Pigeons 8381, 192, 190) run rates (responses/min) differed between transitions and across conditions. Of the three pigeons whose run rates were different, there was a main effect of the upcoming reinforcer; that is, when the upcoming reinforcer was small, run rates were slower than when the upcoming reinforcer was large. For Pigeons 8381 and 190, an interaction between the past and upcoming reinforcers was observed in the condition with the largest discrepancy between the large and small reinforcers. That is, when the previous reinforcer was large and the upcoming reinforcer was small, the run rate was slower than in the other three transitions. For Pigeon 10312, there were no systematic differences in run rates between transition types and across conditions. The pattern of run rate observed in Perone and Courtney (1992) differed slightly than those observed in the current study. In the Perone and Courtney study, run rates did not differ greatly between transitions and across conditions. A possible explanation for the difference in run rates could be that in the current study the transition that was in place remained signaled throughout the entire FR, and there was a constant “reminder” while responding in the presence of the transition stimuli. When the rich-to-lean transition was signaled, the discriminative stimulus might have been occasioning longer pauses throughout responding, and therefore decreased the overall run rate. One purpose of the current study was to see if using a unique stimulus for each transition would produce a change in the sensitivity to the rich-to-lean transition compared to Perone and Courtney’s (1992) study (i.e., pauses would be longer or shorter during the rich-to-lean transition). Regardless of the upcoming reinforcer, the when the past reinforcer was small PRP tended to be longer in duration in both studies. The longest PRP observed in both studies was during the rich-to-lean transition. In Perone and Courtney, the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition for Pigeons 3280, 3611, and 5112 ranged between 30.17 s and 36.5 s. In the current 53 study, the PRP for all pigeons ranged between 20.6 s and 48.7 s. The current study has a larger range and the highest PRP during the rich-to-lean transition. The ranges overlap, however, and both data sets are variable when compared across subjects. Comparing the proportion of the richto-lean transition over the lean-to-lean transition is another analysis that helps compare using two stimuli versus four stimuli. In Perone and Courtney, the proportion of change between the richto-lean and lean-to-lean transition for two of the three subjects is 2.68 and 5.6. The proportion of change for the third subject is 177.4, though the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition is only 30.2 s. In the current study, the proportions of change for Pigeons 10312, 192, 190, and 8381 are 3.7, 17.2, 12.3, and 11, respectively. Overall, with the exception of the outlier in Perone and Courtney, the proportion of change in the current study is higher than that of Perone and Courtney. The larger proportion of change observed in the current study suggests that using four unique stimuli for each transition increases the sensitivity to the rich-to-lean transition, versus using only two stimuli to indicate the upcoming reinforcer. The extended pause and decreased run rate during the rich-to-lean transition might be explained by contrast effects. Contrast effects occur when one component of a multiple schedule is preceded or precedes another component with a differing schedule of reinforcement (Nevin and Shettleworth, 1966). Positive contrast is observed when transitioning from a lean reinforcer to a rich reinforcer. The PRP was slightly shorter and the run rate (in three out of the four pigeons) was higher in the lean-to-rich transition than in the rich-to-rich transition. Even though overall reinforcement was higher in a rich-to-rich transition, the contrast between the lean and the rich reinforcer occasioned an increase in responding in the presence of the rich-to-lean transition stimulus. Negative contrast is observed when transitioning from a rich reinforcer to a lean reinforcer. Even though overall reinforcement was less during the lean-to-lean transition 54 than during the rich-to-lean transition, the contrast between the rich and the lean reinforcer occasioned a greater decrease in responding in the presence of the rich-to-lean transition stimulus. Contrast effects on PRP and run rate became more pronounced in the current study during the rich-to-lean transition as the difference between the reinforcers increased. There was an interaction between the previous reinforcer and upcoming reinforcer, such that when the previous reinforcer was large and the upcoming reinforcer was small the PRP was longer than it was in the other three transitions. This suggests that there was joint control by the previous and upcoming reinforcer of the PRP. If just the previous reinforcer or the upcoming reinforcer controlled pausing, it would be expected that the PRP during the rich-to-rich and richto-lean transitions or rich-to-lean and lean-to-lean transitions would be similar. The PRP during a rich-to-lean transition, however, was longer than the PRP during the other three transition types. Perone and Courtney (1992) focused on the joint control by the previously experienced reinforcer and the signaled upcoming reinforcer. Joint control of the previous and upcoming reinforcer was conceptualized differently in the current study, as the upcoming reinforcer was not the only reinforcer that was signaled. By using four individual stimuli for the transitions, each combination of previous and upcoming reinforcer pairs was signaled. It is difficult in the current study, therefore, to determine if the PRP was under joint control of the previous and upcoming reinforcer or if the stimuli signaling the transition were controlling the pauses. There is a possibility that the two stimuli, for example, that signal the rich-to-lean and lean-to-lean transition functioned in the same way. That is, they signaled an upcoming lean reinforcer, and the previously experienced reinforcer has joint control in differentiating the PRP between the two stimuli. 55 Future directions: Phase 1. Although the longest PRP was seen in the presence of the rich-to-lean transition stimulus, it is still unclear whether the stimulus itself was controlling the pause, or if the joint control of the previously experienced reinforcer and the upcoming reinforcer was controlling the pause. A procedure to test for stimulus control of the transition stimulus should be conducted in order to observe what is controlling the pause. A procedure that could test for stimulus control would be to present the rich-to-lean stimulus during an incompatible transition. For example, during a lean-to-rich transition, in which the PRP is usually short, the rich-to-lean stimulus could be substituted for the lean-to-rich stimulus. In a typical session, this presentation would be impossible because a rich-to-lean stimulus would only be presented following a rich reinforcer. If extended pausing is observed, this would be evidence that the specific stimulus does have control over the pause that is seen during the rich-to-lean transition. Although the proportion of change in the current study was greater than the proportion of change in Perone and Courtney, it did not demonstrate that using four stimuli necessarily occasions longer pauses than using only two stimuli. There should be a direct comparison, using the same pigeons, of the PRP using only two stimuli to signal whether the upcoming reinforcer is large or small. Once this replication is done, then the PRPs during the rich-to-lean transition can be compared across studies. The following comparisons between the two studies would be needed: the differences between the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition in each study and the difference between the PRP of the rich-to-lean transition and the other three transitions in each study. It might be predicted that shorter pauses, particularly during the rich-to-lean transition would be observed using only two stimuli. Using only two stimuli requires “remembering” what was previously experienced, and there is no “reminder” (i.e., individual transition stimulus) of 56 the current transition. The joint control by the previous and upcoming reinforcer of the PRP would not be as strong as it was in the current study. Phase 2 The purpose of Phase 2 was to decrease the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition. Mazur and Hyslop (1982) successfully decreased the PRP during FR responding by adding a 30-s timeout following half of the reinforcers received in a session. In the current study, two timeout conditions were arranged. The first timeout condition consisted of a blackout timeout with durations of 15, 30, and 60 s; whereas the second timeout condition consisted of the key light turning off for 30 s. During blackout and stimulus-termination timeout sessions, the timeouts were added following each reinforcer. It was hypothesized that the addition of a timeout between transitions would decrease the PRP during all transition types. If there was joint control by the previous and upcoming reinforcer, a timeout would degrade the control of the previous reinforcer, shifting the control to the upcoming reinforcer. Therefore, pauses in the rich-to-lean transition would be similar to those in the lean-to-lean transition, as the lean upcoming reinforcer would be controlling the pause. The results from the blackout timeout within and between pigeons were variable across transition types and timeout durations. Overall, the addition of the timeouts increased the PRP and decreased the run rate across all transition types compared to baseline for three of the four pigeons (10312, 192, and 190). Pigeon 8381 is the only subject in which there was a decrease in the average PRP with all timeout durations during the rich-to-lean transition. The stimulustermination timeout procedure was conducted with three of the four pigeons (10312, 192, and 190). For all three subjects the stimulus-termination timeout procedure decreased the average 57 PRP during the rich-to-lean transition. Run rates varied across pigeons, but overall remained relatively close to baseline. An explanation for the decreases seen in both timeout conditions might be that perhaps after many sessions the PRP was controlled in part by how much time had passed since the previous reinforcer was received and not solely by the illumination of the key light after the reinforcer. For example, if a pigeon on average paused for 20 s following a reinforcer, and the timeout was 15 s, the measured PRP would have been only 5 s. In this example, temporal control is not being considered as a mechanism that degraded the control of the previous reinforcer, but instead as a sort of “counting” mechanism. During the blackout timeout, each timeout (15, 30, and 60 s) decreased the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition for Pigeon 8381. The explanation of temporal control could apply to this subject, as his pauses during the rich-to-lean transition were already relatively low. With the addition of a timeout, the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition would have decreased even more if the timeout was included as part of his pausing time. The different effects on PRP observed across pigeons during the blackout timeout suggest that this type of timeout did not degrade the control of the previous reinforcer. If the timeout had degraded the control by the past reinforcer, thus breaking the joint control by the past and upcoming reinforcers, it would be expected that the PRP would have decreased across all conditions. Decreases in the PRP might not have been observed across all pigeons during the rich-to-lean transition due to the effect of the long history the subjects had with the stimuli. Though the timeout put temporal distance between the previously experienced reinforcer and the upcoming reinforcer, the discriminative stimulus for the transition was still present. These data suggest that the stimuli came to function as discriminative stimuli for the actual transition; 58 therefore, timeout would not degrade the control of the past reinforcer. Perhaps if the timeouts occurred daily, not just during occasional probe sessions, the strength of the control by each transition stimulus would decrease. Might the decreases in PRP seen during the blackout timeouts be explained by contrast effects? When the pigeon was in timeout, there was no opportunity for reinforcement. A transition from a timeout to a key-light signaling a rich-to-lean transition was an overall increase in reinforcement rate, even though previously the rich-to-lean stimulus had occasioned an extended PRP. Sadowsky (1973) showed that when pigeons were responding on a signaled variable-interval schedule, response rates increased following a black-out timeout period. In the current study, however, the majority of changes in PRP were increases and in run rate were decreases across all transitions. Consequently, though there were some decreases seen in PRP and increases in run rate, it is not likely that these changes were solely due to contrast between the transitions and blackout timeout. During the stimulus-termination timeout, the rich-to-lean transition PRP decreased and run rate increased more reliably than during the blackout sessions. Two previously discussed explanations could apply to the decreases in PRP seen across all birds during the stimulustermination timeout. First, stimulus-termination timeout might have degraded the control of the previous reinforcer. The history with the individual transition stimuli, however, is the same as that of the blackout timeout; so why would the blackout not have also degraded the control of the previous reinforcer? Second, contrast effects could explain the decreases in PRP and increases in run rate during the stimulus-termination. It is difficult, however, to say that contrast effects were only present in the stimulus-termination timeout. After all, there was also contrast between the 59 blacked-out chamber and a transition stimulus that signaled the opportunity for reinforcement was once again available. Why was there such a discrepancy in the number of decreases seen in PRP a between blackout and stimulus termination? An interpretation of the inconsistency in decreases during the blackout timeouts is that stimulus termination was not as much of a disruptor as blackout was during timeout. Baseline sessions began with the house light on and the keylight off, which is identical to how timeouts were presented during stimulus-termination timeout sessions. A chamber blackout was not something that was common in a session, therefore the distraction provided by blackouts following each reinforcer may have increased PRP and decreased run rate. Galuska and Yadon (2011) studied how prefeeding affected the PRP during rich-to-lean transitions. They found that pre-feeding increased the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition. If a blackout was a disruptor similar to prefeeding that would explain why the PRP increased during the black-out timeout sessions. There is another interpretation of blackout as a disruptor that should be considered. During baseline sessions, the only time that a blackout in the chamber would occur was once the session was complete. Per anecdotal observation, the pigeons would turn away from the keys and towards the door at the end of sessions in preparation to leave the chamber. If this behavior occurred during the blackout timeout sessions, it would explain why the PRP increased overall during these sessions. Future Directions: Phase 2. Similar to Phase 1, Phase 2 should be replicated using only two stimuli to signal the upcoming reinforcer. This replication should be done in order to determine if using a timeout during transitions can break the joint control of the previous and upcoming reinforcer when only the upcoming reinforcer is explicitly signaled. Stimulustermination timeout sessions should be run in order to compare the difference between using two 60 stimuli versus four stimuli. It might be expected that if joint control by the previous and upcoming reinforcer is more easily broken using only two stimuli, the PRP during timeout would be shorter than what was observed when four stimuli were used. This replication would not be done with blackout timeouts, as there were few decreases in PRP observed during the current study. Another direction is to present each timeout type over consecutive days in order to see what happens to the PRP all transitions when timeout is no longer just a probe. One prediction might be that after many sessions timeout would no longer degrade the control of the previous reinforcer. Joint control by the previous and upcoming reinforcer would form once again. PRP, therefore, would increase over time to the duration that was observed during baseline sessions of the current study. Applied Implications Transitions, particularly those going from a highly preferred activity to a less preferred activity, can be difficult for all people. More challenging behaviors during these transitions can be seen in the developmentally delayed populations. During rich-to-lean transitions behaviors such as aggression, self-injury, and general disruptive behavior can occur which threaten harm to the individual and to others (Tustin, 1995; Waters et al., 2007; Wilder, Chen, Atwell, Pritchard, Weinstein, 2006). Studies have been conducted looking for ways in which these problem behaviors during transitions could be reduced. Waters et al. used visual schedules to attempt to reduce problem behaviors. Visual schedules were pictures that were presented to the subject that represented the current activity and the upcoming activity. Once the current activity was complete, the subject would put the picture of the current activity into a bag, and then bring the picture of the upcoming activity to the designated activity area. They found that providing the 61 pictures preceding transitions helped to decrease aggressive and disruptive behaviors, but only when used in conjunction with differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO) and extinction. Another method to decrease problem behaviors during transitions was to use a warning prior to the transition (Tustin, 1995; Wilder et al., 2006). The results varied, as sometimes behaviors decreased with a warning of the upcoming transition, while other times behaviors increased with a warning of the upcoming transition. Overall, there has not been a method that consistently decreases problem behaviors during rich-to-lean transitions in the applied field. In the current study, the “problem” behavior of pigeons (i.e., extended pausing) during rich-to-lean transitions was reduced using a stimulus-termination timeout. A similar behavior that might be seen in an applied setting is noncompliance or deviance (e.g., engaging in behaviors that are incompatible to a behavior that has been requested; not engaging in a behavior that has been requested). These behaviors are similar to PRP in the current study, as all are measures of latency to start responding to a requested task. An important next step would be to conduct a similar study with human subjects who display these behaviors during transitions. Using a stimulus-termination timeout during transitions in an applied setting might include a period in which the preferred activity is not being engaged before transitioning to the nonpreferred activity. For example, if a subject is watching television and it is time to set the table before dinner, the television would first be turned off for a specified amount of time before the request to set the table was made. In general, using timeouts during rich-to-lean transitions should be studied to determine whether it would be a useful tool for decreasing problem behaviors. Summary 62 During signaled rich-to-lean transition, for animal subjects and humans alike, aberrant behaviors can occur. The behavior of interest in the current study was the PRP. Adding a blackout timeout between each transition did not reliably decrease the PRP during the rich-tolean transition. Adding a stimulus-termination between each transition, however, did decrease the PRP during the rich-to-lean transition. Moving forward it will be important to translate this research into the applied setting. If the right type of transitional timeout in the applied setting can be studied, dangerous and disruptive behaviors could be decreased and prevent harm to individuals. 63 References Azrin, N. H. (1961). Time-out from positive reinforcement.Science, 133, 382-383. Bejarano, R., Williams, D. C., & Perone, M. (2003).Pausing on multiple schedule: Toward a laboratory model of escape motivated behavior. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 21, 18-22. Crossman, E. K. (1968). Pause relationships in multiple and chained FR schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 117-126. Crossman, E. K. (1971). The effectos of fixed-ratio size in multiple and mixed fixed-ratio schedules.The Psychological Record, 21, 535-544. Crossman, E. K., Bonem, E. J., & Phelps, B. J. (1987). A comparison of response patterns on fixed-, variable-, and random-ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 395-406. Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957).Schedules of Reinforcement.New York: AppletonCentury-Crofts. Findley, J. D. (1958). Preference and switching under concurrent scheduling.The Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 123-144. Galuska, C. M., Wade-Galuska, T., Woods, J. H., & Winger, G. (2007). Fixed-ratio schedules of cocaine self-administration in rhesus monkeys: joint control of responding by past and upcoming doses. Behavioral Pharmacology, 18, 171-175. Galuska, C.M., & Yadon, K.A. (2011). The effect of prefeeding on fixed-ratio pausing is jointly determined by past and upcoming reinforcer magnitudes. Behavioural Processes, 86, 156-159. 64 Gentry, D. G. (1968).Fixed-ratio schedule-induced aggression.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 813-817. Holtyn, A. F. (2010). Escape from stimuli correlated with transitions across lean and rich schedules of reinforcement (Unpublished master’s thesis). West Virginia University, Perone, Morantown, West Virginia. Knutson, J. F. (1970). Aggression during the fixed-ratio and extinction components of a multiple schedule of reinforcement.The Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 221-231. Long, J. B. (2005). The behavioral functions of stimuli signaling transitions across rich and lean schedules of reinforcement (Unpublished master’s thesis).West Virginia University, Perone, Morgantown, West Virginia. Mazur, J. E., & Hyslop, M. E. (1982).Fixed-ratio performance with and without a postreinforcement timeout.The Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 143155. Nevin, J.A., & Shettleworth, S.J. (1966). Analysis of contrast effects in multiple schedules. The Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 305-315. Perone, M., &Courtney, K. (1992). Fixed-ratio pausing: Joint effects of past reinforcer magnitude and stimuli correlated with upcoming magnitude. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 33-46. Perone, M. (2003).Negative effects of positive reinforcement.The Behavior Analyst, 26, 1-14. Sadowsky, S. (1973).Behavioral contrast with timeout, blackout, or extinction as the negative condition.The Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 499-507. 65 Sainato, D. M., Strain, P. S., Lefebvre, D., & Rapp, N. (1987).Facilitating transition times with handicapped preschool children: A comparison between peer-mediated and antecedent prompt procedures.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 285-291. Skjoldager, P., Winger, D., & Woods, J. H. (1991).Analysis of fixed-ratio behavior maintained by drug reinforcers.The Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 331-343. Thompson, D. M. (1964). Escape from SD associated with fixed-ratio reinforcement. The Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,7, 1-8. Tustin, R.D. (1995). The effects of advanced notice of activity transitions on stereotypic behavior.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 91-92. Wade-Galuska, T., Perone, M., & Wirth, O. (2005).Effects of past and upcoming response-force requirements on fixed-ratio pausing. Behavioural Processes, 68, 91-95. Waters, M. B., Lerman, D. C., &Hovanetz, A. N. (2009). Separate and combined effects of visual schedules and extinction plus differential reinforcement on problem behavior occasioned by transitions.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 309-313. Wilder, D.A., Chen, L., Atwell, J., Pritchard, J., & Weinstein, P. (2006). Brief functional analysis and treatment of tantrums associated with transitions in preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 103-107. Williams, D. C., Saunders, K. J., & Perone, M. (2011). Extended pausing by humans on multiple fixed-ratio schedules with varied reinforcer magnitude and response requirements. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 203-220. Winger, G., Woods, J. H. (2001). The effects of chronic morphine on behavior reinforced by several opioids or by cocaine in rhesus monkeys. Drug Alcohol Dependence, 62, 181189. 66