Sleep Anxiety 2 - Vanderbilt University

advertisement
NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Published in final edited form as:
Emotion. 2012 October ; 12(5): 1015–1020. doi:10.1037/a0026871.
Sleep Deprivation and Stressors: Evidence for Elevated Negative
Affect in Response to Mild Stressors When Sleep Deprived
Jared D. Minkel,
Department of Psychology and School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania
Siobhan Banks,
School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania and Centre for Sleep Research, University of
South Australia, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia
Oo Htaik,
School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Marisa C. Moreta,
School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania
Christopher W. Jones,
School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania
Eleanor L. McGlinchey,
School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania
Norah S. Simpson, and
School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania
David F. Dinges
School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania
Abstract
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Stress often co-occurs with inadequate sleep duration, and both are believed to impact mood and
emotion. It is not yet known whether inadequate sleep simply increases the intensity of subsequent
stress responses or interacts with stressors in more complicated ways. To address this issue, we
investigated the effects of one night of total sleep deprivation on subjective stress and mood in
response to low-stress and high-stress cognitive testing conditions in healthy adult volunteers in
two separate experiments (total N = 53). Sleep was manipulated in a controlled, laboratory setting
and stressor intensity was manipulated by changing difficulty of cognitive tasks, time pressure,
and feedback about performance. Sleep-deprived participants reported greater subjective stress,
anxiety, and anger than rested controls following exposure to the low-stressor condition, but not in
response to the high-stressor condition, which elevated negative mood and stress about equally for
both sleep conditions. These results suggest that sleep deprivation lowers the psychological
threshold for the perception of stress from cognitive demands but does not selectively increase the
magnitude of negative affect in response to high-stress performance demands.
Keywords
sleep deprivation; stress; stressors; mood
© 2012 American Psychological Association
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jared D. Minkel, University of Pennsylvania, Department of
Psychology, 3720 Walnut Street, Solomon Lab Bldg., Philadelphia, PA 19104-6241. jminkel@psych.upenn.edu.
Minkel et al.
Page 2
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Stress responses are essential for survival, but can also interfere with proper behavioral and
biological functioning. Physiological responses to stressors (such as increased heart rate and
respiration and pupil dilation) facilitate the detection of and escape from threat, but
chronically elevated and excessive stress can damage bodily systems and reduce well-being
and positive affect (Korte, Koolhaas, Wingfield, & McEwen, 2005). Threats to
psychological integrity, such as to one’s social standing or future earning potential, can
activate the same physiological responses as physical threats (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007).
These responses can be maladaptive, interfering with attention (e.g., rapid heart rate during
an important test can be distracting) and decreasing interpersonal effectiveness (e.g., during
public speaking). When chronic, the physiological responses to stressors can lead to poor
sleep quality and maladaptive behaviors that damage the nervous system over time
(McEwen, 2008).
It is believed that the relationship between stress and sleep is bidirectional—that is, stress
can disrupt sleep and sleep loss can increase subsequent stress levels (Vgontzas & Chrousos,
2002; Vgontzas et al., 1998). While there is considerable scientific work on the disruption of
sleep by prior stress (Akerstedt, 2006; Akerstedt et al., 2002; Akerstedt, Kecklund, &
Axelsson, 2007), much less is known about the manner in which sleep loss affects
subsequent responses to stressors.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Animal studies have found evidence that sleep deprivation alters both baseline activity of the
stress system and physiological responses to subsequent stress (see Meerlo, Sgoifo, &
Suchecki, 2008 for review), but data in humans are lacking. To date there are no published
studies that have experimentally manipulated both sleep and stress exposure. Studies that
have measured physiological markers of stress (such as cortisol) during sleep deprivation
experiments have found inconsistent results (see Leproult, Copinschi, Buxton, & Van
Cauter, 1997; Redwine, Hauger, Gillin, & Irwin, 2000). Nevertheless, indirect evidence
from neuroimaging studies supports the hypothesis that sleep deprivation is likely to
decrease the efficiency of top-down inhibition circuitry. Yoo, Gujar, Hu, Jolesz, and Walker
(2007) reported that sleep deprivation was associated with reduced functional connectivity
between the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex, as well as greater overall amygdala
reactivity, in response to passive viewing of negative photographs. Similar deficits in
prefrontal control regions induced by sleep loss have been reported using a go/no-go task
(Chuah, Venkatraman, Dinges, & Chee, 2006), gambling paradigm (Venkatraman, Chuah,
Huettel, & Chee, 2007), working memory task (Chee et al., 2006), and passive viewing of
positive photographs (Gujar, Yoo, Hu, & Walker, 2011). We reasoned that because the
prefrontal control regions that are sensitive to sleep loss and involved in top-down inhibition
are also implicated in stress induction (Wang et al., 2005), sleep deprivation would
potentiate the subjective effects of exposure to a stressor. Furthermore, we predicted that the
effect would be larger for a more intense stressor than for a milder stressor. If this were
found, it would suggest sleep loss potentiates affective responses to stressors in proportion
to the severity of the stressor.
Overview of the Present Studies
Two experiments were conducted to investigate the influence of sleep deprivation on
subjective affective responses to cognitive performance stressors in healthy adult volunteers.
To the best of our knowledge, these are the first studies to experimentally manipulate both
exposure to stressors and sleep deprivation in humans. We used a previously validated
stress-induction procedure (Dinges et al., 2005) that altered task difficulty and feedback
about performance to create relatively low-stress and high-stress experimental conditions.
The study design included random assignment to sleep condition and repeated exposure to
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Minkel et al.
Page 3
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
stressors on two consecutive days of testing. This experimental design allowed for both
within- and between-groups comparisons where nuisance variables (such as mood changes
due to experimental conditions) could be evaluated and statistically controlled. Participants
were randomly assigned to a night of total sleep deprivation or a control condition (full night
of sleep), and completed stress-induction procedures in a controlled laboratory setting. The
methods of each study (presented as Study 1 and Study 2) are explained below. The results
from Study 2 independently replicated the results of Study 1 and were therefore combined to
increase statistical power. Results from these combined analyses are reported with Cohen d
effect sizes (Cohen, 1988). Results were inconsistent with our hypotheses, thus alternative
explanations and theoretical models are discussed.
Method—Study 1
Participants and Procedure
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Participants were 30 healthy adults (15 women, 15 men; 13 African American, 14
Caucasian, 3 other or declined to state) between the ages of 22 and 43 years (mean age =
29.0 +/− 7.0) recruited from the community. Volunteers were screened to ensure they had no
medical, psychiatric, or sleep-related disorders, were nonsmokers, and were drug-free. This
was determined by comprehensive self-reported medical history and the Structured Clinical
Interview for DSM–IV Axis I disorders (SCID I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997),
which was conducted by a Masters-level doctoral student in clinical psychology. Interested
participants were not included if they had engaged in any regular night or rotating shift work
within the last 6 months, or had traveled across times zones in the last 3 months. Participants
were also excluded immediately prior to study participation if they were physically ill, had
poor sleep the previous night, indicated suicidal ideation, and/or reported Beck Depression
Inventory (BDI-II; Beck, 1996) scores at or above 14 prior to the first day of testing.
Measures
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Subjective stress was measured before and after each stressor condition by a computeradministered visual analog scale (0–8) with a range of not stressed at all to extremely
stressed. Mood was measured after each condition by a computerized version of the Profile
of Mood States (POMS; McNair, Lorr & Droppleman, 1971; McNair & Heuchert, 2005), a
65-item adjective checklist where each item is rated on a 5-point scale that ranges from not
at all to extremely. The POMS was chosen because it has been used extensively in sleep
deprivation research (see Goel, Rao, Durmer, & Dinges, 2009 for review). To facilitate
comparisons among self-report instruments, raw scores were converted to a 0–100 scale,
which adjusts for differences in the number of items included in each subscale. Analyses of
the affective consequences of stressor exposure focused on Depression-Dejection, TensionAnxiety, and Anger-Hostility subscales of the POMS. Responses to the Fatigue-Inertia
subscale were analyzed as a manipulation check to confirm that sleep deprived participants
reported more fatigue than the rested control group.
Sleep Deprivation
Participants completed a 48-hr laboratory-based protocol that included two continuous days
of testing. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of
Pennsylvania and all participants provided informed consent and were compensated for their
participation. Participants entered the laboratory at 9:00 a.m. (Day 1) and completed
consenting procedures. Testing was completed throughout the day (procedures described
below), and participants remained in the laboratory and either received no sleep opportunity
(sleep deprivation group) or 9-hr of sleep opportunity (control group) from 11:00 p.m. to
8:00 a.m. Fourteen participants were randomized to the sleep deprivation protocol (6
women) and 16 participants (9 women) were randomized to the control condition. In order
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Minkel et al.
Page 4
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
to prevent bias between sleep deprivation and control groups, participants were not informed
of their condition until 8:00 p.m. on Day 1 (after low and high-stressor testing conditions
had been completed). At night, participants in the sleep-deprived condition completed 10
minute reaction time (RT) tasks every two hours. Outside of testing, sleep-deprived
participants were allowed to read magazines, complete puzzles, and interact with staff to
maintain wakefulness, but no TV or electronics were available. Although two subjects at a
time were enrolled in the study (always in the same sleep condition), contact between
participants was strictly prohibited. A second day of testing (Day 2) was then completed
followed by 10 hours of recovery sleep opportunity for all participants.
At all scheduled wake times, participants were kept awake in the laboratory under
continuous behavioral monitoring and were exposed to artificial lighting only (levels were
not strictly controlled during wake times). During scheduled sleep times, all lights were
turned off (less than 1 lux) and trained staff members monitored participants using infrared
cameras from an adjacent room. Adherence to sleep condition was further validated by
actigraphy, an objective method for estimating sleep based on movement (Ancoli-Israel et
al., 2003). Participants in both conditions remained in the laboratory for the full duration of
the experiment.
Stress Induction
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Participants completed cognitive performance test batteries under conditions designed and
previously validated to induce relatively low and high subjective stress (Dinges et al., 2005).
Participants completed both high-stressor and low-stressor conditions on both days of the
experiment. Testing lasted approximately 20 minutes and occurred at the same time on both
days (between 4:00 p.m. and 6:00 p.m.) to control for circadian effects on mood and
alertness. All participants first completed the low-stressor condition, then returned to their
rooms to complete questionnaires. Approximately 1 hour later, they completed the highstressor condition. The lack of counterbalancing of order of stressor condition was deliberate
to prevent responses to the more intense stressors from contaminating subsequent responses
to mild stressors.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Stressor tasks—Mental arithmetic tasks were used as the primary stressor. In the highstressor condition, participants responded orally to a serial subtraction task (i.e., repeated
subtraction of 13 from a four-digit number, Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, 1993; Wang
et al., 2005) and a difficult descending subtraction task (i.e., repeated subtraction of 9, then
8, then 7 etc. from a four-digit number; Dinges, Orne, Evans, & Orne, 1981). In the lowstressor condition, participants counted backward by 2. The high-stressor condition also
included a difficult version of the Stroop color-naming task (i.e., names and colors were
discordant; Stroop, 1935) and a difficult divided attention task involving the concurrent
presentation of memory, arithmetic, visual monitoring and auditory monitoring tasks
(Synwork; Elsmore, 1994), while the low-stressor condition included an easy version of the
Stroop (i.e., font colors and names were concordant) and no divided attention task. The
high-stressor condition also included negative feedback about performance given both orally
and by computer, and greater time pressure than the low-stressor condition. Negative
feedback included presentation on the participant’s monitor of his or her performance as a
Testing environment—Participants completed all stressor tasks alone in a brightly lit
room that contained minimal furniture, giving it an austere appearance. A trained staff
member, unfamiliar to the participants, delivered instructions for the tasks by intercom.
Cameras were clearly visible in front of and above the participant. Participants were not told
that the tasks were intended to produce stress, but rather that they were tests of cognition and
performance that were very important for the study.
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Minkel et al.
Page 5
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
percentile score relative to other participants. All participants were shown a percentile score
that was in the bottom 25th percentile (regardless of their actual performance) and an
experimenter read a script over the intercom stating that their performance was “well below
average.” Participants were informed of the true nature of the experiment and the
performance feedback immediately following the final stress induction procedure (on Day
2). Debrief interviews confirmed that participants were unaware that the procedures were
primarily intended to elicit stress.
Additional Procedures
Although the primary aim of this experiment was to evaluate the influence of sleep
deprivation on stress responses, additional procedures were completed when time allowed.
These secondary measures included additional questionnaires and tests of executive function
and other cognitive abilities (results not reported here).
Study 2
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Study 2 was designed as a replication of Study 1 but it also included a higher probability of
randomization to the sleep deprivation condition to maximize power for within-subjects
comparisons. Methods are reported with an emphasis on ways in which the two studies
differed. One participant in the sleep deprivation condition failed to complete questionnaires
following exposure to the high-stressor condition on Day 2.
Method
Participants and Procedure—Participants were 23 healthy adults (9 women, 14 men;
13 Caucasian, 8 African American, 2 other or declined to state) between the ages of 22 and
45 years (mean age = 30.8 +/− 6.8) recruited from the community. The same inclusion
criteria were used as in Study 1, but psychological problems were assessed by self-report
only (i.e., no clinical interviews were conducted). Participants reporting suicidal ideation
and/or BDI-II scores greater than 14 were disqualified from the study and referred to the
study psychiatrist.
Measures—The same self-report measures were used as in Study 1. Subjective stress was
measured before and after each induction procedure; mood was measured after each
induction procedure.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Sleep Deprivation—The same sleep deprivation procedures were used in Study 2 as in
Study 1 (see Sleep Deprivation section above). Fifteen participants were randomized to the
sleep deprivation protocol (6 women) and 8 participants (3 women) were randomized to the
control condition.
Stress Induction—Study 2 included the same basic approach to stress induction as Study
1 (see Stress Induction section above). All tasks were the same, with the exception of the
difficult divided attention task (i.e., Synwork), which was removed from the high-stressor
condition. The conditions differentiating the low-stressor from high-stressor conditions were
also identical between Study 1 and Study 2, with the exception of negative feedback about
performance, which was given by computer only, rather than both by computer and by the
experimenter. These changes were made based on feedback from participants and staff about
which tasks were most effective in eliciting subjective stress.
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Minkel et al.
Page 6
Results (Combined Sample)
Participants
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The combined sample included 53 healthy adults (29 men and 24 women; 21 African
American, 26 Caucasian, and 6 other or declined to state) between the ages of 22 and 45
years (mean age 29.8 +/− 6.9). The combined sample included 29 sleep-deprived
participants, 12 female, 15 African American, 10 Caucasian, and 4 other) and 24 control
participants (12 female, 6 African American, 17 Caucasian, 1 other).
Manipulation Checks
The effects of the high-stressor versus low-stressor condition were compared using withinsubjects analyses of affective responses on Day 1, prior to the sleep manipulation. In the
combined sample, the high-stressor condition elicited significantly greater subjective stress
than the low-stressor condition, t(52) = 6.44, p = .001, d = 0.93. The manipulation also had
significant effects on negative affect. The Anger-Hostility, t(52) = 2.51, p = .02, d = 0.35;
Tension-Anxiety, t(52) = 4.73, p < .001, d = 0.65; and Depression-Dejection, t(52) = 2.83, p
= .007, d = 0.50, subscale scores were significantly higher following the high-stressor
condition compared to the low-stressor condition.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The effects of sleep deprivation on subjective feelings of fatigue were assessed using
between-subjects analyses of mood responses on Day 2, following the sleep manipulation.
Sleep deprivation was associated with significantly greater Fatigue-Inertia subscale scores
after both the low-stressor condition, F(1, 51) = 54.01, p < .001, d = 2.33, and high-stressor
condition, F(1, 50) = 32.56, p < .001, d = 1.67.
Influence of Sleep Deprivation on Subjective Responses to Stressors
Sleep deprivation and low-stressor condition—ANCOVA analyses were used to
compare subjective stress and negative affect (i.e., anger, anxiety, and depression) between
sleep-deprived participants and rested controls on Day 2 (after the sleep manipulation) while
controlling for these responses on Day 1 (before the sleep manipulation). Following the lowstressor condition, sleep-deprived participants reported significantly higher levels of
subjective stress, F(1, 52) = 5.48, p = .02, d = 0.61 than control participants. Negative mood
states were also elevated. Anger-Hostility, F(1, 52) = 16.63, p < .001, d = 0.91, and TensionAnxiety, F(1, 52) = 10.80, p = .002, d = 0.81, subscale scores were greater for sleep
deprived participants than for rested participants. There was also a marginally significant
trend toward sleep-deprived participants reporting greater Depression-Dejection subscale
scores, F(1, 52) = 3.34, p = .07, d = 0.56. These results are reported in Table 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Sleep deprivation and high-stressor condition—Following the high-stressor
induction procedure, there were no significant differences between sleep-deprived and
control participants on subjective stress, F(1, 50) = 0.78, p = .38. Similarly, there were no
differences between sleep deprived and rested control participants on Anger-Hostility,
Tension-Anxiety, or Depression-Dejection, all F(1, 51) < 1.19, all p values > .28, all d
values <0.27 following the high-stressor condition.
Within-subjects analyses, postmanipulation—Paired-samples t tests comparing
subjective stress and mood responses between the low-stressor and high-stressor conditions
on Day 2 revealed that participants in the control condition reported greater Subjective
Stress, t(23) = 4.40, p < .001, following the high-stressor condition than following the lowstressor condition. Negative mood states were also elevated. Tension-Anxiety, t(23) = 3.10,
p = .005, and Anger-Hostility, t(23) = 2.48, p = .02, were significantly greater following the
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Minkel et al.
Page 7
high-stressor condition, but the Depression-Dejection subscale was not significantly
affected, t(23) = 1.44, p = .17.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Participants in the sleep deprivation condition reported significantly greater subjective stress
following the high-stressor task than following the low-stressor task as well, t(28) = 3.77, p
= .001. Analyses of mood subscales revealed a nonsignificant trend toward greater TensionAnxiety scores, t(27) = 1.93, p = .06, but differences between the low-stressor and highstressor condition were not significant for Anger-Hostility, t(27) = 1.00, p = .33, or
Depression-Dejection, t(27) = 0.27, p = .79.
Interactions of sleep-condition and stressors—A repeated measures ANOVA was
conducted where stress and mood responses on Day 2 to the low stressor condition and high
stressor condition were entered as repeated measures and sleep condition (deprivation vs.
control) was entered as a between-groups measure. Subjective stress and mood responses to
stressors on Day 1 were entered as covariates in these analyses. The interaction of sleep
condition and stressor level was not significant for subjective stress, F(1, 48) = 0.42, p = .52.
A significant interaction of affective response by sleep condition was found for the AngerHostility subscale, F(1, 48) = 6.08, p = .02, but not for the Tension-Anxiety, F(1, 48) = 2.10,
p = .15, or Depression-Dejection F(1, 48) = 1.40, p = .24.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Discussion
The purpose of the experiments reported was to investigate the effects of sleep deprivation
on subjective responses to cognitive performance stressors in healthy adult volunteers.
Based on previous findings that sleep deprivation is associated with impaired inhibitory
control during both cognitive (Chuah et al., 2006) and emotional (Yoo et al., 2007) tasks, we
expected to find an interaction where the effect of sleep deprivation increased as the stressor
increased (i.e., there would be a small difference between groups in response to low-stressor
conditions, but a larger difference in response to the high-stressor condition). In fact, our
findings suggest the opposite occurred. Relative to participants who had a full night’s sleep,
sleep-deprived participants reported significantly greater subjective stress, anger, and
anxiety in response to the low-stressor condition, but differences between the sleep-deprived
and control groups in response to the high-stressor condition were not significant.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
There are two important limitations to keep in mind when interpreting these results. First, all
participants completed the low-stressor condition prior to the high-stressor condition. It is
therefore possible that order effects influenced responses such that there may not be a
unique relationship between mild stressors and sleep deprivation. Additional studies are
needed to more fully disentangle these effects, but we believe the manipulation check
provided by the control group (namely that they reported greater subjective stress in
response to the high-stressor condition than the low-stress condition on Day 2) partially
attenuates this concern. Second, the group-by-stressor-condition interactions were mostly
not significant (with the exception of anger). As recently emphasized by Nieuwenhuis,
Forstmann, & Wagenmakers. (2011), it would be incorrect to conclude that the differences
between the responses to the low- and high-stressor conditions were significantly different
between sleep-deprived and control participants. Nevertheless, we believe the failure to
detect significant interactions likely reflects a relative lack of statistical power and
cautiously suggest that there is probably a unique relationship between sleep deprivation and
relatively minor stressors.
We interpret our results as suggesting that sleep loss does not necessarily potentiate
subjective responses to cognitive performance stressors in proportion to the severity of the
stressor. Instead, an alternative hypothesis was supported by both experiments. Rather than
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Minkel et al.
Page 8
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
increasing subjective responses to more intense stressors, it appears that sleep loss primarily
lowers the threshold at which a person experiences an event as a stressful. Additional studies
are needed before a satisfactory mechanism for this effect can be identified, but we believe
that cognitive appraisals are likely to be involved. A simple explanation for these findings is
that sleep-deprived participants experienced unexpected difficulty on relatively easy tasks
(e.g., counting by 2) during the low-stressor condition. In contrast, the high-stressor
condition included tasks that were so challenging (e.g., subtracting by 13) that all
participants experienced similar levels of subjective stress. We recommend that future
studies in this area also assess cognitive variables (such as attributions for poor
performance) that might mediate associations between sleep loss and negative affective
responses to stressors.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
An alternative explanation for these findings is that sleep-deprived participants would have
hyper-responded to both low-stressor and high-stressor conditions, but the responses to the
high-stressor condition were masked by ceiling effects. We believe this explanation is
unlikely for two reasons. First, responses to the high-stressor condition were not near the top
of the scale, suggesting that participants were at least able to report greater stress and mood
changes if they chose. Second, and perhaps more important, responses to the high-stressor
condition showed relatively more variability than responses in the low-stressor condition,
even in the sleep-deprivation condition. Ceiling effects are associated with a restriction in
range that was not observed here. Nevertheless, future studies could address this alternative
explanation more directly by quantifying responses in other domains (such as behavioral or
neural responses) and/or by exposing participants to a greater variety of stressor intensities.
The unexpected finding that sleep loss was associated with increased negative affective
responses to relatively mild cognitive performance stressors represents an important
contribution to understanding the relationship between stress and sleep in real-world
settings. Previous research has established that subjectively perceived stress during the day
interferes with sleep at night (Akerstedt, 2006). Our findings complement this research by
showing that inadequate sleep at night may then increase subjective stress and negative
mood in response to relatively minor stressors encountered the following day. These
findings may also explain how sleep deprivation may contribute to the experience of people
feeling overwhelmed (e.g., “I can’t take it anymore”) or overreacting (e.g., “flying off the
handle”) in the presence of relatively modest cognitive demands.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Our findings appear robust (given that they were largely replicated in two independent
samples) for the effects of one night of total sleep deprivation, which can be experienced in
real world settings, such as extended-duty shift work and students “pulling all nighters.” It
remains uncertain however, whether the results can be generalized to the even more
common experience of chronic partial sleep deprivation, which occurs in association with
many medical and psychological disorders that shorten sleep, as well as in lifestyles of
increased work and commute times (Basner et al., 2007). There is ample evidence that
chronic partial sleep deprivation produces cumulative cognitive deficits of the same type and
degree as those found for total sleep deprivation (Dinges et al., 1997; Van Dongen, Maislin,
Mullington, & Dinges, 2003; Belenky et al., 2003; see Banks & Dinges, 2007 for review),
therefore, it is likely that chronic partial sleep deprivation also lowers the threshold for stress
reactions to relatively mild performance demands. Nevertheless, studies using chronic
partial sleep restriction and recovery sleep opportunities (e.g., Banks, Van Dongen, Maislin,
& Dinges, 2010) must be expanded to include evaluation of responses to stressors before the
findings presented here can be generalized with confidence beyond total acute sleep
deprivation.
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Minkel et al.
Page 9
Acknowledgments
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
This research was supported by the National Space and Biomedical Research Institute through NASA NCC 9-58
and the National Institutes of Health through National Research Service Award F31 MH079604.
References
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Akerstedt T. Psychosocial stress and impaired sleep. Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment and
Health. 2006; 32:493–501.
Akerstedt T, Kecklund G, Axelsson J. Impaired sleep after bedtime stress and worries. Biological
Psychology. 2007; 76:170–173. [PubMed: 17884278]
Akerstedt T, Knutsson A, Westerholm P, Theorell T, Alfredsson L, Kecklund G. Sleep disturbances,
work stress and work hours: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2002;
53:741–748. [PubMed: 12217447]
Ancoli-Israel S, Cole R, Alessi C, Chambers M, Moorcroft W, Pollak CP. The role of actigraphy in the
study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep. 2003; 26:342–392. [PubMed: 12749557]
Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. Journal of
Clinical Sleep Medicine. 2007; 3:519–528. [PubMed: 17803017]
Banks S, Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Neurobehavioral dynamics following chronic sleep
restriction: Dose-response effects of one night for recovery. Sleep. 2010; 33:1013–1026. [PubMed:
20815182]
Basner M, Fomberstein KM, Razavi FM, Banks S, William JH, Rosa RR, Dinges DF. American time
use survey: Sleep time and its relationship to waking activities. Sleep. 2007; 30:1085–1095.
[PubMed: 17910380]
Beck, AT. Beck Depression Inventory. 2nd ed.. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996.
Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR, Thomas ML, Sing HC, Redmond DP, Balkin TJ. Patterns of
performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: A sleep
dose-response study. Journal of Sleep Research. 2003; 12:1–12. [PubMed: 12603781]
Chee MWL, Chuah LYM, Venkatraman V, Chan WY, Philip P, Dinges DF. Functional imaging of
working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 h of sleep deprivation: Correlations
of fronto-parietal activation with performance. Neuroimage. 2006; 31:419–428. [PubMed:
16427321]
Chuah YML, Venkatraman V, Dinges DF, Chee MWL. The neural basis of interindividual variability
in inhibitory efficiency after sleep deprivation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006; 26:7156–7162.
[PubMed: 16822972]
Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988.
Dinges, DF.; Orne, EC.; Evans, FJ.; Orne, MT. Performance after naps in sleep-conductive and
alerting environments. In: Johnson, D.; Tepas, W.; Colquohoun, W.; Colligan, M., editors.
Biological rhythms: Sleep and shiftwork (Advances in sleep research). New York, NY: Spectrum;
1981.
Dinges DF, Pack F, Williams K, Gillen KA, Powell JW, Ott GE, Pack AI. Cumulative sleepiness,
mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep
restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep. 1997; 20:267–277. [PubMed: 9231952]
Dinges DF, Rider RL, Dorrian J, McGlinchey EL, Rogers NL, Cizman Z, Metaxas DN. Optical
computer recognition of facial expressions associated with stress induced by performance
demands. Aviation Space Environmental Medicine. 2005; 76:B172–B182.
Elsmore TF. Synwork1: A PC-based tool for assessment of performance in a simulated work
environment. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers. 1994; 26:421–426.
First, MB.; Spitzer, RL.; Gibbon, M.; Williams, JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM–IV AxisI disorders administration booklet. New York, NY: American Psychiatric Press; 1997.
Goel N, Rao H, Durmer JS, Dinges DF. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Seminars
in Neurology. 2009; 29:320–339. [PubMed: 19742409]
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Minkel et al.
Page 10
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Gujar N, Yoo SS, Hu P, Walker MP. Sleep deprivation amplifies reactivity of brain reward networks,
biasing the appraisal of positive emotional experiences. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;
31:4466. [PubMed: 21430147]
Gunnar M, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology.
2007; 58:145–173.
Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–A tool for investigating
psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993; 28:76–81.
[PubMed: 8255414]
Korte SM, Koolhaas JM, Wingfield JC, McEwen BS. The Darwinian concept of stress: Benefits of
allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews. 2005; 29:3–38. [PubMed: 15652252]
Leproult R, Copinschi G, Buxton O, Van Cauter E. Sleep loss results in an elevation of cortisol levels
the next evening. Sleep. 1997; 20:865–870. [PubMed: 9415946]
McEwen BS. Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: Understanding the protective
and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2008;
583:174–185. [PubMed: 18282566]
McNair, DM.; Heuchert, JP. Profile of mood states: Technical update. North Tonawanda, NY: MultiHealth Systems Inc.; 2005.
McNair, DM.; Lorr, M.; Droppleman, LF. Manual: Profile of mood states. San Diego, CA: EdITS;
1971.
Meerlo P, Sgoifo A, Suchecki D. Restricted and disrupted sleep: Effects on autonomic function,
neuroendocrine stress systems and stress responsivity. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2008; 12:197–
210. [PubMed: 18222099]
Nieuwenhuis S, Forstmann BU, Wagenmakers EJ. Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience:
A problem of significance. Nature Neuroscience. 2011; 14:1105–1107.
Redwine L, Hauger RL, Gillin JC, Irwin M. Effects of sleep and sleep deprivation on interleukin-6,
growth hormone, cortisol, and melatonin levels in humans. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology &
Metabolism: Clinical and Experimental. 2000; 85:3597–3603.
Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
1935; 18:643–662.
Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional
wakefulness: Dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from
chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003; 26:117–126. [PubMed:
12683469]
Venkatraman V, Chuah YM, Huettel SA, Chee MW. Sleep deprivation elevates expectation of gains
and attenuates response to losses following risky decisions. Sleep. 2007; 30:603–609. [PubMed:
17552375]
Vgontzas AN, Chrousos GP. Sleep, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and cytokines: Multiple
interactions and disturbances in sleep disorders. Endocrinology and Metabolism Clinics of North
America. 2002; 31:15–36. [PubMed: 12055986]
Vgontzas AN, Tsigos C, Bixler EO, Stratakis CA, Zachman K, Kales A, Chrousos GP. Chronic
insomnia and activity of the stress system: A preliminary study. Journal of Psychosomatic
Research. 1998; 45:21–31. [PubMed: 9720852]
Wang J, Rao H, Wetmore GS, Furlan PM, Korczykowski M, Dinges DF, Detre JA. Perfusion
functional MRI reveals cerebral blood flow pattern under psychological stress. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2005; 102:17804–17809.
Yoo SS, Gujar N, Hu P, Jolesz FA, Walker MP. The human emotional brain without sleep–A
prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Current Biology. 2007; 17:R877–R878. [PubMed: 17956744]
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
8.5 (10.3)
Tension-Anxiety
Fatigue-Inertia
31.6 (17.9)
16.7 (12.2)
3.0 (5.0)
5.2 (8.0)
33.8 (31.5)
32.4 (18.2)
14.0 (7.8)
2.8 (4.0)
4.0 (4.7)
17.6 (24.3)
Sleep-deprived (n = 28)
1.67
0.26
−0.10
−0.09
0.17
2.33
0.81
0.56
0.91
0.61
Cohen d
p
<.001
.28
.80
.45
.38
<.001
<.01
.07
<.01
.02
Note. A negative effect size indicates less negative mood in the sleep-deprived group than the rested control group. To facilitate comparisons, subscale scores were converted to a 0–100 scale.
3.5 (8.6)
14.4 (9.0)
Depression-Dejection
5.8 (9.8)
4.8 (6.3)
Fatigue-Inertia
29.2 (25.2)
8.6 (5.4)
Tension-Anxiety
Anger-Hostility
1.0 (1.8)
Depression-Dejection
Subjective Stress
0.9 (1.9)
Anger-Hostility
High Stressor
8.3 (10.9)
Subjective Stress
Low Stressor
Rested (n = 24)
Mood subscale
Condition
Self-Reported Stress and Mood After Stressors for Sleep-Deprived and Control Participants (Both Experiments Combined)
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Table 1
Minkel et al.
Page 11
Emotion. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 25.
Download