10.1177/0093854805282509 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS AND THEIR RELATIONSHIP TO THE EXERCISE OF DISCRETIONARY POWERS CLAUDIA MENDIAS E. JAMES KEHOE The University of New South Wales For four vignettes that described minor criminal incidents, police officers indicated whether they would make an arrest and ranked the importance of four policing ideals in justifying their decision. These ideals were law enforcement, peace maintenance, procedural compliance, and assessing whether the protagonist accepted responsibility. Although an arrest was always warranted, the overall arrest rate was 56%, and only 10% of the officers always indicated an arrest. In justifying an arrest, law enforcement was ranked first on only 31% of occasions. Across arrests and nonarrests, the officers justified 66% of their actions using two combinations of ideals: lawprocedure and peace-procedure. Hence, the officers appeared to exercise their discretionary powers using one of two cognitive schemas to appraise each incident. Keywords: police; discretion; arrest; decision making; schema P olice officers are the gatekeepers of the criminal justice system (Bryett, Harrison, & Shaw, 1994; Friedrich, 1980; Gottfredson & Gottfredson, 1988; Konec#ni & Ebbesen, 1982). They introduce people into the system through their decisions whether or not to arrest a person involved in a criminal incident. Except in a “zero tolerance” environment, police officers are commonly allowed discretion in dealing with low-level criminal incidents by either making an arrest or AUTHOR NOTE: This research was supported by an Australian Postgraduate Research Award to the first author. Please address correspondence to E. James Kehoe, School of Psychology, The University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia; e-mail: j.kehoe@unsw.edu.au. CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR, Vol. 33 No. 1, February 2006 70-92 DOI: 10.1177/0093854805282509 © 2006 American Association for Correctional and Forensic Psychology 70 Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 71 taking an alternative action, such as a warning (Novak, Frank, Smith, & Engel, 2002; Wortley, 2003). In the interest of “community policing,” some officers are being explicitly trained to mediate disputes between two people or refer them to a mediation service, rather than arresting one or both as offenders (Moore, 1992; Novak et al., 2002; Yeats, 1997). However, the availability of alternative actions can create a dilemma when officers are confronted with an ambiguous situation, for example, a domestic dispute in which it is unclear whether or not physical violence has occurred. In the face of ambiguous situations, police officers could legitimately adopt a relatively fixed strategy (cf. Gilboy, 1991; Konec#ni & Ebbesen, 1984; Kunin, Ebbesen, & Konec#ni, 1992). That is, whenever faced with sufficient evidence for an infraction of the law, police officers could justify arresting a suspect and letting a further stage in the criminal justice system make the final disposition. However, as mentioned above, police officers often operate under policies that mix law enforcement with other forms of conflict resolution. Thus, officers are required to exercise their discretion in a way that satisfies a number of ideals1 that originate in departmental policy, training, and broader organizational socialization. The literature reveals three theoretical approaches to the relationship between police officers’ discretionary actions and their ideals (Albonetti, 1991; Brewer, Wilson, & Braithwaite, 1995; Morgan & Smith, 1989; Robinson, 2000; Yarmey, 1990). One approach attempts to predict the actions of officers from objective characteristics of the officers (e.g., Riksheim & Chermak, 1993; Wilson & Braithwaite, 1995). Presumably, these characteristics are associated with underlying attitudes that guide the officers’actions, but this approach does not attempt to directly measure these underlying attitudes. The characteristics most commonly studied have included an officer’s age, years of service, socioeconomic background, gender, and even height (Lundman, 1994; Sherman, 1980; Smith & Klein, 1983). Among these, the only factor consistently found to predict officers’ decisions has been their level of experience (e.g., Finn & Stalans, 2002; Mastrofski, Snipes, Parks, & Maxwell, 2000). More experienced officers tend to resolve incidents more often without making an arrest than less experienced officers (Finckenauer, 1976; Friedrich, 1980; Wilson & Gross, 1994; but see Finn & Stalans, 2002). 72 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR A second approach has more directly addressed the intervening variables between a situation and an officer’s response (Broderick, 1987; Brown, 1988; Skolnick, 1966; Smith & Davidson, 1984; Walsh, 1986; Wortley, 2003). This approach has attempted to explain variability in officers’ actions according to trait-like “operational styles” that combine different ideals about policing. The main ideals commonly identified are (a) adherence to established legal procedures, (b) enforcement of laws, and (c) maintenance of the peace (Avery, 1981; Hancock, 1978; Lipsky, 1980). According to this approach, differences among the officers in their emphasis on different ideals explains why, when responding to similar incidents, one officer makes an arrest whereas another uses an alternative action. At its core, an operational-styles theory predicts that individual officers will show a stable tendency to either arrest or take an alternative action, according to their particular style. However, research has so far not revealed a consistent relationship between the attitudes of officers and their actions across the range of situations they encounter (Meyers, Heeren, & Hingson, 1989; Smith & Klein, 1983; Snipes & Mastrofski, 1990; Stith, 1990; Worden, 1989). Among the clearer results, Mastrofski, Worden, and Snipes (1995) found that officers with a positive attitude toward community policing made fewer arrests (5% in all public encounters) than officers with a negative attitude (17%). The third theoretical approach, interactionist theory, is more cognitive in its flavor. Each officer is thought to have acquired several schemas that can be used as alternative ways to filter and categorize the incoming information about an incident (Finn & Stalans, 2002; Lurigio & Carroll, 1985; Novak et al., 2002; Robinson, 2000; Stalans & Finn, 1995; Wortley, 2003). Each officer’s schemas can be differentially engaged across incidents, which leads to variability in an officer’s actions from one time to another. The variables included in a schema may include both concrete situational features (e.g., the degree of injury to a victim) and psychological variables (including an officer’s policing ideals). This interactionist approach has fostered studies that compared officers’ arrest decisions in specific situations with their answers to questions intended to identify the schemas engaged in those situations. Field investigations adopting this approach have observed offi- Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 73 cers’ actions within particular criminal contexts, including traffic enforcement (Worden, 1989), drunk driving (Meyers et al., 1989), domestic disputes (Robinson, 2000; Worden & Pollitz, 1984), juvenile crime (Hancock, 1978), and peace disturbances (Mastrofski et al., 1995). However, these studies have produced promising but not definitive outcomes. For example, Worden and Pollitz (1984) found that only 17% of the variability in the officers’ actions (arrest versus nonarrest) could be explained by differences among the officers’ attitudes concerning, for example, the degree of enforcement for different laws, adherence to procedure, and perceived support from the community. Laboratory studies, in which officers are presented with written vignettes describing criminal incidents, have had more success than field studies (Finn & Stalans, 2002; Stalans & Finn, 1995; Wortley, 1997). Most notably, Stalans and Finn (1995) presented police officers with a written vignette describing a domestic dispute. The officers were asked what action they would take and to provide an explanation of their action. The results suggested that the officers used one of two schemas. One schema, entitled the “efficiency frame,” entailed pragmatic considerations, including adherence to required procedure. In the other schema, entitled the “normative frame,” the officers focused on whether the protagonists accepted responsibility for the dispute (Finn & Stalans, 1995). Officers who used the efficiency frame tended to make fewer arrests than officers using the normative frame. Although the available field and laboratory studies have yielded some interesting data, they do not distinguish the interactionist theory from the operational-styles theory. In fact, from the wider perspective of a schema theory, the two theories differ in degree rather than in kind. Specifically, in its simplest form, the operational-styles theory essentially contends that each officer has a single predominant schema for responding to criminal incidents. Hence, across incidents, each officer will tend to take the same action and tend to give priority to the same ideals. In contrast, the interactionist theory contends that each officer has several schemas and that, across incidents, each officer will vary in his or her action and in the associated ideals that are given priority. However, the available studies have used between-subject designs, in which each officer is given only one vignette (Finn & 74 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR Stalans, 1995, 2002; Stalans & Finn, 1995; Worden & Pollitz, 1984). From officers’ reactions to a single situation, it is impossible to tell whether these reactions reflect a single dominant schema or just one of several schemas possessed by each officer. Hence, the first aim of this study was to ascertain the degree of variability in both the actions and ideals indicated by officers across a set of criminal incidents. A second aim of this study was to determine the specific ideals given priority by the officers in justifying their actions, and the third aim was to determine the strength of the associations, if any, between those ideals and the actions indicated by the officers. As yet, there is no agreed list of policing ideals, but looking across the operationalstyles and interactionist literature, there are four ideals that are more prominent than others (Brooks, 1993; Mastrofski, Parks, & Worden, 1998; Stalans & Finn, 1995; Worden, 1989; Wortley, 2003). First and perhaps most obviously is the ideal of enforcing the law. On a naive basis, one could hypothesize that officers would use this ideal as ample justification for any arrest. However, there are other plausible hypotheses as to why an officer may make a legal arrest without law enforcement per se being the main justification. For example, in disputes between family members or neighbors, arresting the offender may be seen as a means for restoring peace and preventing potential violence (Kane, 2000; Rigakos, 1997). In addition, some models of community policing advocate differential enforcement of the law—and hence the use of arrest—contingent on community values and norms (Novak et al., 2002). In these terms, an arrest might be justified primarily in terms of community values about keeping the peace. In statistical terms, the conditional probability of using law enforcement as the justification for an arrest may be less than 1.00. Conversely, where officers do have discretion in responding to lowlevel violations of the law, they are not obliged to arrest every offender. Anyone who has ever been “let off” with a warning for a driving offense will appreciate this aspect of police discretion. Hence, the conditional probability of arrest given that law enforcement is the chief priority may also be less than 1.00. A second prominent ideal is keeping the peace. In the context of community policing models, this ideal might be strongly associated with the use of tactics such as mediation and warnings. However, as discussed in the previous paragraph, arrest may also be seen as a Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 75 worthwhile action in some circumstances where keeping the peace is the chief priority. The third ideal is adherence to required procedure and ensuring due process. This ideal was a key component of Stalans and Finn’s (1995) efficiency frame. They reported that officers who used this schema tended to be less likely to make an arrest than officers who used the normative frame. However, to the extent that this ideal reflects an officer’s sense of professionalism (Wortley, 1997), it is not clear whether this ideal would always be associated with a low likelihood of arrest. Stalans and Finn’s (1995) findings were limited to a nonviolent domestic dispute. In other situations, however, adherence to procedure could be associated with a high likelihood of arrest. The fourth ideal derived from the literature is assessing whether the offender accepts responsibility for the incident. This ideal was a key component of Stalans and Finn’s (1995) normative frame, which was associated with a relatively high likelihood of arrest, at least in nonviolent domestic disputes. To achieve our three aims, we adopted a vignette technique. On the basis of information from interviews with police officers and official crime statistics, four types of incidents frequently encountered by local police officers were identified and a vignette for each type of incident was written. All the vignettes entailed incidents of low criminality to allow officers the most freedom to exercise their discretion (Thomas, 1986). Among other things, the incidents had little or no risk of violence, which often triggers an arrest regardless of other considerations (Finn & Stalans, 2002; Kane, 2000; Rigakos, 1997). For each vignette, the officers were asked two key questions. First, they were asked what action they would take in response to the incident. For this question, the officers were given a choice among several alternatives that were legally available to them. Second, the officers were asked which of the four identified ideals they regarded as most important in deciding their action. To do so, the officers were asked to rank a set of propositions that reflected these ideals. The forcedchoice nature of the ranking task presented the officers with a dilemma similar to the one that they face when deciding what action to take with regard to a criminal incident (Brooks, 1993; Robinson, 2000). That is, if policing ideals do influence officers’actions, or at least their after-the-fact justifications, then officers may need to give one or more 76 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR of the ideals priority over the rest, even though all could have a positive value if rated individually. Hence, by defining the relative importance of the underlying ideals, rather than their absolute importance, the ranking procedure was intended to capture any conflict among policing ideals that officers must resolve. To reduce any demand characteristics, an attempt was made to divert the attention of the participants away from the linkage between their actions and their ideals by inserting two additional questions between the two key questions. These inserted questions concerned features of the suspect’s behavior that could plausibly influence the officers’ actions (e.g., Finn & Stalans, 2002; Lundman, 1994; Riksheim & Chermak, 1993; Sherman, 1980; Wortley, 2003). To give these questions face validity across the four criminal vignettes read by each officer, one or more features of the suspect were randomly varied within each vignette (namely, their demeanor, their statement to the police, and/or their criminal history). METHOD PARTICIPANTS The participants were 173 officers from 12 police stations in Sydney, Australia. The officers’ rank varied from probationary constable to senior constable, and their years of service ranged from 4 months to 35 years (M = 8.5 years, SD = 8.2). However, 20% of the officers failed to answer this question. Agreements with both the police service and police union prevented the collection of demographic information, including the officers’ gender and location of their station. PROCEDURE Police officers were recruited in their stations. The officers were asked to read five vignettes in a randomized order and answer the questions as if they were responding to a real incident. Data collection sessions lasted about 20 minutes. The Appendix shows each of the five vignettes, including the variations in phrasing with regard to the suspects’ behavior. Manipulations Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 77 of the suspect variables were randomized across the vignettes given to each officer. Four vignettes described incidents of low-level but definite criminality, namely, a minor assault during a dispute between patrons in a public bar, malicious damage to a neighbor’s property, shoplifting in a record shop, and a nonviolent dispute between a husband and wife. The fifth vignette described disruptive but noncriminal behavior at a football game. This fifth vignette was included for two methodological reasons. First, it allowed us to determine whether or not the vignette method produced any unrealistic bias to indicate an arrest. In fact, only 1% of the officers indicated an arrest for this vignette. Second, intermixing this noncriminal vignette among the criminal vignettes was intended to reduce any tendency by the participants either to indicate an arrest without paying attention to the vignette or to vary their response to the criminal vignettes if they felt that it was socially desirable to do so. After reading each vignette, the officers were given the following choice of actions: (a) caution only, (b) caution and log a report, (c) take to station for inquiries, (d) arrest with conditional bail, (e) arrest with unconditional bail, and (f) “other,” which allowed officers to detail an alternative action. For the statistical analyses, these indicated actions were grouped into two categories: “arrest” versus “nonarrest.” The arrest category included “arrest with conditional bail” and “arrest with unconditional bail.” The category of nonarrest included all other specified actions. For 217 of the original 865 responses, the officers used the “other” option. Based on the details provided by the officers, 130 of these responses were reclassified by two independent raters. Specifically, 38% were recoded as indicated arrests and 62% as nonarrests. The remaining 87 “other” statements were not included in subsequent analyses because the observers failed to agree on a classification. The second and third questions concerned two of the suspect variables. These items asked officers to rate the degree to which they agreed, on a 5-point scale, with statements using details of the vignette. For example, for the assault vignette, the question concerning the suspect’s demeanor was, “The man who swung at the other cooperated with the police officers.” Analyses of the officers’ answers revealed that they attended to the details of the vignettes. For example, when presented with vignettes describing a suspect as cooperative, the officers agreed more strongly with the relevant question (Ms ! 3.6) than 78 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR when presented with a vignette in which the suspect was uncooperative (Ms " 1.8). A similar difference was found for all the manipulationcheck questions (ps < .01). Log-linear analyses were conducted to determine whether the suspect variables influenced the likelihood of officers’ indicating an arrest (Elliott, 1988; Everitt, 1977; Norusis, 1993). The only manipulation that had a significant effect in more than one vignette was the suspect’s statement, namely, whether or not the suspect confessed guilt. Specifically, in the malicious damage vignette, a confession attracted an indicated arrest from 75% of the officers, but a nonconfession attracted an indicated arrest from only 9% of the officers, t(113) = 6.22, p < .01. In the assault vignette, a far smaller difference was seen, with a confession attracting arrests from 41% of the officers versus 20% when there was no confession, t(123) = 2.50, p < .01. The only other significant effect was criminal history in the shoplifting vignette, in which suspects with a reported criminal involvement attracted an indicated arrest from 80% of the officers versus 61% when no prior criminal involvement was reported, t(127) = 2.32, p < .01. The fourth and last question asked the officers to rank four propositions with regard to the importance of each of them in deciding their intended action. The specific propositions were as follows (in order of presentation): (a) “Strictly enforce the law regulating this type of incident,” (b) “Follow the appropriate procedures for this type of incident,” (c) “Resolve the dispute and maintain the peace,” and (d) “Verify whether suspects accept responsibility for their actions.” For descriptive purposes, these four propositions were labeled as “law,” “procedure,” “peace,” and “responsibility,” respectively. To conduct a coherent analysis of the officers’ indicated actions, especially their consistency across vignettes and the relationship of the indicated actions to the officers’ ranking of the four propositions, the final sample included only officers who provided a complete set of data (i.e., coded actions and rankings for all five vignettes). From the original sample of 173 officers, the data from 91 (53%) met the inclusion criteria. Among the excluded participants, the data from 30 officers were discarded because they failed to rank the propositions but instead gave absolute ratings. A further 52 participants were excluded because the officers either failed to provide any indicated action or Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 79 gave an unclassifiable “other” response for one or more vignettes. The final sample and excluded participants did not appear to differ. Specifically, their respective levels of indicated arrests and their length of service failed to differ significantly (ps > .15). RESULTS According to the operational-styles approach, the distribution of actions and ideals should be relatively constant across criminal incidents for a given group of officers. Conversely, the interactionist approach predicts that officers may vary in their actions and in which ideals they view as most important from one incident to another. In fact, the officers showed significant variability in their indicated actions across the criminal vignettes. Only 10% of the officers indicated an arrest in all four vignettes, and conversely, only 5% never indicated an arrest. The full distribution was as follows: 10%, 32%, 35%, 18%, and 5% of the officers made 4, 3, 2, 1, and 0 indicated arrests, respectively. Although this distribution differed significantly from a uniform distribution, #2(4, N = 91) = 17.03, p < .01, it shows no evidence of bimodality that would indicate two groups of officers with distinct arrest strategies. Conversely, among the separate vignettes, none of them elicited an indicated arrest from all the officers. Specifically, for the assault, malicious damage, shoplifting, and domestic dispute vignettes, the proportion of officers indicating an arrest was 32%, 44%, 73%, and 75%, respectively, #2(3, N = 364) = 49.84, p < .01. The overall likelihood of an indicated arrest was 56%, and a subsidiary analysis did not reveal any correlation with the officers’ length of service (r = –.12, n = 75, p > .10). The officers also varied in the range of propositions they used to justify their actions. Only 16% of the officers gave any proposition the first rank in all four vignettes. The relationship between indicated actions and the rankings of the propositions was examined using spectral analysis (Diaconis, 1988, 1989). There were 24 possible rank-ordered combinations formed by the four propositions. Spectral analysis decomposes a data set into its principal components and estimates the proportion of variability explained by each component. In the case of rank-ordered data, this analysis identifies the relative contribution made by different-sized 80 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR groupings of the propositions, which are analogous to the main effects and interactions in an analysis of variance (Speed, 1987). For the 24 possible rank-orderings, the spectral analysis yields a maximum of four components of variability: (a) first order, (b) unordered pairs, (c) ordered pairs, and (d) unordered triplets. The first-order component represents how frequently the officers ranked each individual proposition in a given position. The unordered-pairs component reflects the number of officers who assigned any two propositions a given pair of ranks, regardless of the order. The ordered-pairs component assumes a fixed order. Finally, the unordered triplets include propositions in the top three ranks in any order. Spectral analysis does not have a provision for repeated measures. Hence, a separate analysis was conducted for each vignette: One subanalysis described the distribution of rank-orders regardless of the action, and a second subanalysis described the association of rankorders with the indicated action of arrest versus nonarrest. In the subanalysis for the ranking of the propositions, the first-order component explained an average of 71% of the variance in the rankings across vignettes (range = 64-78%). The two second-order components—the unordered pairs and ordered pairs—together explained virtually all of the remaining variance. Across vignettes, the unorderedpairs explained an average of 13% of the variance (range = 7-20%), and the ordered-pairs explained an average of 15% of the variance (range = 10-24%). Together, the first- and second-order components explained 99% of the variance. The second subanalysis yielded a strong first-order component in the association of indicated actions with the rankings of the propositions. An average 64% of the variance in the officers’ actions could be associated with their rankings of individual propositions (range = 5476%), the unordered pairs explained an additional 14% of the variance (range = 8-26%), and the ordered pairs explained another 21% of the variance (range = 13-32%). In total, 99% of the total variance was explained by the first- and second-order components together. Table 1 shows three sets of likelihoods, expressed as whole percentages. The left column shows the likelihoods that the officers ranked each of the propositions (or pair of propositions) in the top ranks. For example, the peace proposition was ranked first on 33% of all occasions regardless of the action the officers indicated. Examina- Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 81 TABLE 1: Proportion of Propositions in Top Ranks (in percentages) Proposition Peace Procedure Law Responsibility Peace-Law Peace-Procedure Peace-Responsibility Law-Procedure Law-Responsibility Procedure-Responsibility N All Actions 33 35 20 13 9 33 10 33 2 12 364 Arrest 20 40 31 9 11 22 3 49 3 12 203 Nonarrest 50 28 6 17 7 47 19 14 2 12 161 tion of the table reveals that the officers used procedure (35%) slightly more often than peace (33%) as the primary justification for their actions. The law proposition was only used as the primary justification on 20% of all occasions, and the responsibility proposition was used infrequently (13%). This overall pattern of likelihoods differed significantly from a uniform distribution, #2(3, N = 364) = 27.14, p < .001. Examination of the center and right columns reveals that the relative frequencies with which different propositions received the first rank differed significantly depending on which action had been indicated by officers, #2(3, N = 364) = 59.76, p < .001. Specifically, when an arrest was indicated, procedure was most often used as the primary justification (40%), whereas about a third of the indicated arrests (31%) were justified by law in the first rank. The propositions for peace (20%) and responsibility (9%) were of lesser importance to the officers in justifying an arrest. This pattern of likelihoods differed significantly from a uniform distribution, #2(3, N = 203) = 23.60, p < .001. The pattern of rankings for indicated nonarrests differed dramatically from that generated by arrests. Overall, peace was ranked first in 50% of all instances of nonarrest, and procedure was ranked first for 28%. The propositions for law (6%) and responsibility (17%) were ranked first only infrequently. This pattern of likelihoods differed significantly from a uniform distribution, #2(3, N = 161) = 35.84, p < .001. 82 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR On the basis of the first ranking, it appears that procedure rather than law was the modal justification for an indicated arrest, whereas peace was the predominate justification for a nonarrest. However, when unordered pairs are considered, a different picture emerges, particularly with regard to the usage of procedure. Examination of the left column reveals that peace-procedure and law-procedure combinations each accounted for 33% of all justifications. Among the remaining pairs, none accounted for more than 12% of the justifications. Among the final sample of 91 officers, only 14 (15%) consistently used a single pair of propositions to justify their indicated action. However, 36 of the officers (40%) used a combination of the peaceprocedure and law-procedure pairs to justify their actions. Further examination revealed that these officers did not neatly divide their indicated actions between these two pairs. Among the 36 officers, only 16 (44%) justified all their indicated arrests using the law-procedure pair and all their nonarrests with the peace-procedure pair. The relative frequencies with which the different pairs were used to justify an arrest versus nonarrest differed significantly, #2(5, N = 364) = 73.86, p < .001. Further examination of Table 1 reveals two complementary features. Nearly half of the indicated arrests were justified by the law-procedure pair (49%), but it was used only infrequently to justify a nonarrest (14%). Conversely, the peace-procedure pair predominated in the justification of nonarrests (47%) and was only moderately prominent in justifying arrests (22%). The likelihoods shown in Table 1 can be viewed as the predictive value of arrest versus nonarrest for alternative justifications. If, however, the propositions do in fact reflect ideals that are a part of the schemas used by officers to decide on their action, the ability of the proposition rankings to postdict the action may provide some insight—albeit correlational in nature—into how these schemas are structured. Hence, Table 2 shows the conditional likelihoods of arrest given that individual propositions were placed in the first rank or that a pair of propositions appeared in the first two ranks. In Table 2, the left column lists the propositions, the middle column lists the number of times that the propositions appeared in the top ranks (i.e., first rank for individual propositions and top two ranks for pairs), and the right column lists the proportion of times the propositions were accompanied by an indicated arrest.2 Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 83 TABLE 2: Conditional Likelihood of an Arrest Given Top Ranking of Individual and Pairs of Propositions Proposition Peace Procedure Law Responsibility Peace-Law Peace-Procedure Peace-Responsibility Law-Procedure Law-Responsibility Procedure-Responsibility N P(Arrest) 121 126 71 46 33 120 36 121 9 45 34 64 87 41 67 38 17 82 67 56 NOTE: N = Number of occasions on which the proposition(s) occupied the first rank or top two ranks. P(Arrest) is the proportion of arrests, in percentages. Examination of Table 2 reveals that ranking law in the first position was highly postdictive of an arrest. Specifically, in 87% of the instances in which law was ranked first, there was an indicated arrest. The procedure proposition in the first rank also postdicted an arrest in 64% of occasions. In contrast, peace (34%) and responsibility (41%) were less postdictive of arrest and hence were postdictive of nonarrest. A chi-square test confirmed that the distribution of arrests versus nonarrests differed significantly as a function of first-ranked propositions, #2(3, N = 364) = 59.76, p < .001. Post-hoc tests indicated that the relative frequency of arrests versus nonarrests for each proposition differed significantly for the peace, procedure, and law propositions (ps < .01), but not for responsibility (p > .10). Examination of the lower portion of Table 2 suggests that the postdictive value of the propositions moderated each other when they were paired in the first and second ranks. In fact, the postdictive likelihood of an indicated arrest for a pair could be explained by an additive combination based on the conditional likelihoods for the individual propositions. Specifically, the conditional likelihoods for peace, procedure, law, and responsibility deviated from the overall likelihood of arrest (56%) by –22%, 8%, 31%, and –15%, respectively. The postdictive likelihood for a given pair of propositions could be well approximated by the algebraic sum of the two appropriate deviations plus the overall likelihood of an arrest. For example, for the peace-law 84 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR combination, the computed likelihood was equal to 65% = –22% + 31% + 56%. This computed likelihood compares favorably with the actual conditional likelihood of an arrest for the peace-law combination (67%). For the peace-procedure, peace-responsibility, law-procedure, law-responsibility, and procedure-responsibility combinations, the computed likelihoods were 42%, 19%, 96%, 73%, and 50%, respectively. The corresponding actual likelihoods were 38%, 17%, 82%, 67%, and 56%. The overall agreement between computed and actual conditional likelihoods was excellent. Across the six pairs, the correlation coefficient (df = 4) was .98, p < .01. When the computed likelihoods were regressed on the actual likelihoods, the slope was 1.06 and the intercept was .01. DISCUSSION This study had three empirical aims: (a) to determine the degree of variability in both the indicated actions and rankings of ideals by officers across a set of low-level criminal incidents, (b) to determine the specific ideals given priority by the officers in justifying their actions, and (c) to determine the associations, if any, between those ideals and the actions indicated by the officers. At a theoretical level, this study was aimed at assessing whether the pattern of variability in actions, ideals, and their associations agreed more closely with the expectations from an operational-styles theory or from an interactionist theory. In more quantitative terms, the theoretical aim was to identify the number and type of schemas that the individual officers used across criminal incidents. For all four criminal vignettes, the officers would have been completely entitled to indicate an arrest and to justify it in terms of law enforcement. In fact, the officers exercised their discretion in a more complex way. There was considerable variability in both the choice of actions by the officers and their justification for those actions, individually and across incidents. However, the officers were not entirely random. They used a small set of schemas as revealed by the association between the officers’ actions and their justifications. Specifically, the major findings of this study are as follows: Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 85 1. With regard to the first aim, individual officers commonly varied in their indicated actions across the criminal vignettes, with only 10% indicating an arrest in all four vignettes. The officers also varied individually in the range of propositions they used to justify their actions. Across all propositions, only 16% of the officers gave the same proposition the first rank in all four vignettes. Likewise, only 15% of the officers consistently used a single pair of propositions to justify all their indicated actions. 2. When an arrest was indicated, the law proposition (“Strictly enforce the law regulating this type of incident”) was only invoked as the primary justification about one third of the time (31%). If all the unordered pairs from Table 1 in which law was in either of the top two ranks are added together—namely, peace-law (11%), law-procedure (49%), and law-responsibility (3%)—the law proposition was used in the justification of an arrest on only about two thirds of occasions (63%). However, law was highly diagnostic of arrest. That is, it was almost never used as the primary justification for a nonarrest (6%) and was used only moderately in the top two ranks to justify a nonarrest (22%). On a postdictive basis, the use of law in the first rank was associated with an 87% likelihood of an arrest. 3. The peace proposition (“Resolve the dispute and maintain the peace”) was not a mirror image of the law proposition. Whereas law was used almost exclusively as a justification for arrest, peace appeared as a justification for both arrest and nonarrest, albeit not in a completely symmetric fashion. Specifically, peace was used moderately often as the justification for an arrest in either the first rank (20%) or the top two ranks (36%). With regard to nonarrest, peace appeared frequently as the justification in the first rank (50%) and in the top two ranks (72%). On a postdictive basis, the peace proposition was negatively diagnostic of arrest (34%), which was well below the overall arrest rate (56%). 4. The procedure proposition (“Follow the appropriate procedures for this type of incident”) was used as the prime justification for an arrest more often than law in both the first rank (40%) and the top two ranks (83%). Nevertheless, the procedure proposition was not particularly diagnostic of an arrest. That is, procedure was also used moderately often as the prime justification for a nonarrest (28%) and appeared almost as often in the top two ranks for nonarrest (73%) as it did for arrest. On a postdictive basis, the use of procedure was associated with a 64% likelihood of arrest, which was only slightly higher than the overall arrest rate (56%). 5. The proposition for responsibility (“Verify whether suspects accept responsibility for their actions”) was rarely used as the primary justification for either an arrest (9%) or nonarrrest (17%). Responsibility appeared infrequently in the first two ranks for an arrest (18%) but 86 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR moderately often for nonarrest (33%). On a postdictive basis, responsibility was moderately diagnostic of nonarrest, being associated with a 41% likelihood of arrest, which was below the base rate for arrest (56%). As may now be apparent, these results did not reveal evidence for trait-like operational styles. That is, the officers did not cluster into groups that showed distinct patterns of actions and justifications for them. Instead, the results revealed evidence for an interactionist theory. Specifically, officers appear to have two main overlapping schemas. As Table 1 shows, the law-procedure pair was used to justify the officers’ actions on 33% of all occasions, and the peace-procedure pair was used on another 33%. Among the individual officers, 40% exclusively used these two pairs to justify their indicated actions. In these two possible schemas, the adherence to appropriate procedure is the common denominator. This adherence to procedure was clearly not a mere mechanical compliance with some recipe that led inevitably to the same action. The attention to procedure was combined with the officer’s judgment as to whether the ideals reflected in the law and peace propositions were more salient in deciding what action to take. In this respect, adherence to procedure may be an expression of the officer’s overall professionalism. The schemas identified in this research refine the picture described by Stalans and Finn (1995). In their research using vignettes describing nonviolent domestic disputes, they found that an arrest action was indicated less often by officers adopting an efficiency frame, and arrest was indicated more often by officers adopting a normative frame. In Stalans and Finn’s depiction of the efficiency frame, they emphasized procedure and due process. In their depiction of the normative frame, they emphasized ascertaining whether the protagonists accepted responsibility for the dispute. In these findings, the idea of the efficiency frame is perhaps best captured by the peace-procedure pair. On a postdictive basis, this pair was associated with a moderately low likelihood of arrest (38%), whereas procedure by itself was associated with a moderately high likelihood of arrest (64%). In contrast, the idea of the normative frame is best captured by the law-responsibility pair that, postdictively, was associated with a 67% likelihood of arrest. The responsibility proposition by itself was associated with only a 41% Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 87 likelihood of arrest. However, in our overall results, responsibility was used rarely to justify either an arrest (9%) or nonarrest (17%). Even when considering only our domestic dispute vignette, responsibility was also used rarely as the first-rank justification for an arrest (6%) or nonarrest (13%). Hence, the efficiency frame as conceptualized by Stalans and Finn may exist but was not engaged in response to our particular vignettes. These results flesh out past and recent thinking with regard to the role of different ideals in policing schemas (Novak et al., 2002; Robinson, 2000; Wortley, 2003). The high rankings given by the officers to the law, procedure, and peace propositions lend support to hypotheses that policing ideals include not only law enforcement but also adherence to due process and the maintenance of peace (Avery, 1981; Broderick, 1987; Brown, 1988; Hancock, 1978; Lipsky, 1980; Skolnick, 1966; Smith, 1984). The pattern of linkages between the propositions and indicated arrests is consistent with the main thrust of schema theory as used in the interactionist approach (Finn & Stalans, 2002; Lurigio & Carroll, 1985; Robinson, 2000; Stalans & Finn, 1995), namely, that officers have multiple cognitive structures for interpreting incoming information and deciding on a course of action. In this case, the ideals reflected in the propositions may provide superstructures that transcend situation-specific schemas and help the police officers frame their tactical options to the complexities of both familiar and novel incidents. APPENDIX Text of Vignettes VIGNETTE 1: COMMON ASSAULT You and your partner are walking by the local hotel on Friday night and hear loud shouting coming from inside. You go in and see a man take a swing at another and push him onto the floor. You approach the men and ask them to tell you why they were arguing. At this point, the man who had swung at the other says, Uncooperative: “It’s none of your business, can’t we just sit and talk without the police butting in?” Cooperative: “I was just sitting here and he came up to me and told me to leave.” The other man calmly starts explaining how he had come back from the bar and found this man sitting with his date. He had asked him to leave her alone, but 88 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR he got angry and started swinging at him. You turn to the other man and ask him if this was true. Still agitated, he says, Confession: “I guess I might’ve hit him, but you can’t just come up and tell someone to leave like that.” No confession: “I didn’t do anything, she was just sitting alone and I kept her company. Nothing else happened!!” VIGNETTE 2: MALICIOUS DAMAGE You are called to a dispute between neighbors. The dispatch tells you that Prior: the police had been called to that address before for similar disputes. No prior: there’s no prior record of problems at that address. When you arrive, you are greeted by a man who introduces himself as Mr. Russell. He tells you about a series of confrontations with his neighbor, Mr. Peterson. He says, “I’ve never been particularly friendly with him. We’re basically polite, but he’s always complaining about my kids playing in the front yard. The real trouble started last month when we put up a basketball hoop at the front of the house. Peterson started harassing my kids when they played. He complains that they make too much noise. Now, around an hour ago I heard a loud crash from the front. I came out, saw the hoop lying on the ground, and Peterson going into his house.” You then walk next door to Mr. Peterson’s home. He answers the door and after identifying yourself you tell him of the complaint against him. You ask if he was responsible for the damage to Mr. Russell’s property. He says, Confession: “Yes, I did it, got sick of the noise and knew he wasn’t going to make the kids be quiet.” No confession: “I don’t know what happened. I’ve been inside all day.” VIGNETTE 3: SHOPLIFTING You are walking with your partner by the local mall when the owner of the record shop comes up to you and tells you that a woman just walked out of his store without paying for a couple of CDs. He also tells you that he got a good look at her and gives you a very good description of the woman. You walk around the mall and see a woman matching the description. You approach her, identify yourself, and ask her if she could come back to the store with you. Uncooperative: She gets agitated and says, “I’m not going anywhere with you. Who do you think you are telling me what to do?” Cooperative: She is polite and says, “Yes, whatever you say. Let’s just clear this up.” You take her back to the store and the owner immediately identifies the woman. Your partner also recognizes her as Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 89 Prior: the person he arrested sometime back after a complaint from the mall’s security guard. No prior: a local he knows has never been in trouble with the police before. VIGNETTE 4: DOMESTIC DISPUTE You are dispatched to a block of apartments. The neighbors were worried about the loud shouting coming from the apartment next to them and called the police. You arrive at the apartment and are greeted by a man. After identifying yourself, you inform him about the neighbor’s complaint and ask to come into the apartment. He says, Uncooperative: “Why do you need to come in? Nobody here asked for your help. Go bother someone else,” and he goes to close the door. Cooperative: “Yes, I guess so. If you really have to.” You tell him that you need to come in and talk to his wife. He opens the door and lets you in. As you and your partner come in, you see the wife sitting at the kitchen table. She looks like she’s been crying and is still somewhat upset. You approach her and ask if she’s OK. She nods, but doesn’t say anything. You turn to the husband and ask him if he knows why his wife was upset. He says, “I got back from work and she started on about how I’m never here.” Confession: I guess I lost it and I might’ve grabbed her a bit too hard.” No confession: I didn’t hit her or anything. I just told her to leave me alone.” The wife then says, “I was just mad at him for always staying back. He didn’t have to push me so hard.” You ask her if this has ever happened before. She answers, Prior: “Every now and then he loses it. The police have been here before.” No prior: “We fight like normal, but the police have never been here before.” VIGNETTE 5: DISRUPTIVE BEHAVIOR You are working at a football game. After the first half of the game, you notice a group of males being extremely loud and shouting obscenities at the opposing side. You approach them and ask them to get down from their seats and not yell so loud. They do as you said. A short time later, you see them again shouting and making it difficult for the people next to them to enjoy the game. 90 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR NOTES 1. The use of ideal is intended to be a generic label that encompasses a large set of psychological constructs that have been used by researchers to denote, among other things, the values, goals, and attitudes of police officers. Hence, ideal is used here to mean the ultimate aim of an endeavor, rather than meaning a model of perfection. 2. The conditional likelihood of a nonarrest in each case is not shown, because it is equal to 1 minus the conditional likelihood of arrest. REFERENCES Albonetti, C. A. (1991). An integration of theories to explain judicial discretion. Social Problems, 38, 247-266. Avery, J. (1981). Police: Force or service? Sydney, Victoria, Australia: Butterworths. Brewer, N., Wilson, C., & Braithwaite, H. (1995). Psychological research and policing. In N. Brewer & C. Wilson (Eds.), Psychology and policing (pp. 395-412). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Broderick, J. J. (1987). Police in a time of change. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press. Brooks, L. W. (1993). Police discretionary behavior: A study of style. In R. G. Dunham & G. P. Alpert (Eds.), Critical issues in policing (pp. 140-164). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press. Brown, M. K. (1988). Working the street: Police discretion and the dilemmas of reform. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Bryett, K., Harrison, A., & Shaw, J. (1994). The role and functions of police in Australia (Vol. 2). Sydney, Victoria, Australia: Butterworths. Diaconis, P. (1988). Group representations in probability and statistics (Vol. 11). Hayward, CA: Institute of Mathematical Statistics. Diaconis, P. (1989). A generalization of spectral analysis with application to ranked data. The Annals of Statistics, 17, 949-979. Elliott, G. C. (1988). Interpreting higher order interactions in log-linear analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 121-130. Everitt, B. S. (1977). The analysis of contingency tables. London: Chapman and Hall. Finckenauer, J. O. (1976). Some factors in police discretion and decision making. Journal of Criminal Justice, 4, 29-46. Finn, M. A., & Stalans, L. J. (1995). Police referrals to shelters and mental health treatment: Examining their decisions in domestic assault cases. Crime and Delinquency, 41, 467-480. Finn, M. A., & Stalans, L. J. (2002). Police handling of the mentally ill in domestic violence situations. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 29, 278-307. Friedrich, R. J. (1980). Police use of force: Individuals, situations and organizations. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 452, 82-97. Gilboy, J. A. (1991). Deciding who gets in: Decision making by immigration inspectors. Law and Society Review, 25, 571-599. Gottfredson, M. R., & Gottfredson, D. M. (1988). Decision making in criminal justice: Toward the rational exercise of discretion. Cambridge, UK: Ballinger. Hancock, L. (1978). Police discretion in Victoria: The police decision to prosecute. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Sociology, 14, 33-40. Mendias, Kehoe / ENGAGEMENT OF POLICING IDEALS 91 Kane, R. J. (2000). Police responses to restraining orders in domestic violence incidents: Identifying the custody-threshold thesis. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27, 560-580. Konec#ni, V. J., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1982). An analysis of the sentencing system. In V. J. Konec#ni & E. B. Ebbesen (Eds.), The criminal justice system: A social-psychological analysis (1st ed., pp. 293-442). San Francisco: Freeman & Co. Konec#ni, V. J., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1984). The mythology of legal decision making. International Journal of Law & Psychiatry, 7, 5-18. Kunin, C. C., Ebbesen, E. B., & Konec#ni, V. J. (1992). An archival study of decision-making in child custody disputes. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 564-573. Lipsky, R. (1980). Street-level bureaucracy. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Lundman, R. J. (1994). Demeanor or crime? The Midwest city police-citizen encounters study. Criminology, 32, 631-656. Lurigio, A. J., & Carroll, J. S. (1985). Probation officers’ schemata of offenders: Content, development, and impact on treatment decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1112-1126. Mastrofski, S. D., Parks, R. B., & Worden, R. E. (1998). Community policing in action: Lessons from an observational study. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, National Institute of Justice. Mastrofski, S. D., Snipes, J. B., Parks, R. B., & Maxwell, C. D. (2000). The helping hand of the law: Police control of citizens on request. Criminology, 38, 307-342. Mastrofski, S. D., Worden, R. E., & Snipes, J. B. (1995). Law enforcement in a time of community policing. Criminology, 33, 539-563. Meyers, A. R., Heeren, T., & Hingson, R. (1989). Discretionary leniency in police enforcement of laws against drinking and driving: Two examples from the state of Maine, USA. Journal of Criminal Justice, 17, 179-186. Moore, M. H. (1992). Problem-solving and community policing. In M. Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.), Modern policing (Vol. 15, pp. 99-158). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Morgan, R., & Smith, D. J. (Eds.). (1989). Coming to terms with policing. London: Routledge. Norusis, M. J. (1993). SPSS for Windows Advanced Statistics Release 6.0 [Computer software]. Chicago: Microsoft Corporation. Novak, K. J., Frank, J., Smith, B. W., & Engel, R. S. (2002). Revisiting the decision to arrest: Comparing beat and community officers. Crime & Delinquency, 48, 70-98. Rigakos, G. (1997). Situational determinants of police responses to civil and criminal injunctions for battered women. Violence Against Women, 3, 204-216. Riksheim, E. C., & Chermak, S. M. (1993). Causes of police behavior revisited. Journal of Criminal Justice, 21, 353-382. Robinson, A. L. (2000). The effect of a domestic violence policy change on police officers’schemata. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27, 600-624. Sherman, L. W. (1980). Causes of police behavior: The current state of quantitative research. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 17, 69-100. Skolnick, J. H. (1966). Justice without trial: Law enforcement in democratic society. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Smith, D. (1984). The organizational context of legal control. Criminology, 22, 19-38. Smith, D. A., & Davidson, L. A. (1984). Equity and discretionary justice: The influence of race on police arrest decisions. Journal of Criminology and Law, 75, 234-249. Smith, D. A., & Klein, J. R. (1983). Police agency characteristics and arrest decisions. In G. P. Whitaker & C. D. Phillips (Eds.), Evaluating performance of criminal justice agencies (Vol. 19, pp. 63-98). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 92 CRIMINAL JUSTICE AND BEHAVIOR Snipes, J. B., & Mastrofski, S. D. (1990). An empirical test of Muir’s typology of police officers. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 14, 268-296. Speed, T. (1987). What is an analysis of variance? The Annals of Statistics, 15, 885-994. Stalans, L. J., & Finn, M. A. (1995). How novice and experienced officers interpret wife assaults: Normative and efficiency frames. Law and Society Review, 29, 287-321. Stith, S. M. (1990). Police response to domestic violence: The influence of individual and familial factors. Violence and Victims, 5, 37-49. Thomas, J. M. (1986). The interpretive method in the study of legal decision-making. Washington and Lee Law Review, 46, 1267-1289. Walsh, W. (1986). Patrol officer arrest rates: A study in the social organization of police work. Justice Quarterly, 3, 271-290. Wilson, C., & Braithwaite, H. (1995). Police patroling, resistance, and conflict resolution. In N. Brewer & C. Wilson (Eds.), Psychology and policing (pp. 5-29). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Wilson, C., & Gross, P. (1994). Police-public interactions: The impact of conflict resolution tactics. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 159-175. Worden, R. E. (1989). Situational and attitudinal explanations of police behavior: A theoretical reappraisal and empirical assessment. Law and Society Review, 23, 667-709. Worden, R. E., & Pollitz, A. A. (1984). Police arrests in domestic disturbances: A further look. Law and Society Review, 18, 105-119. Wortley, R. (1997). Attributions as a function of expertise: The case of the police decision to arrest. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 525-538. Wortley, R. K. (2003). Measuring police attitudes toward discretion. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30, 538-558. Yarmey, A. D. (1990). Understanding police and police work: Psychosocial issues. New York: New York University Press. Yeats, M. A. (1997). “Three Strikes” and restorative justice: Dealing with young repeat burglars in Western Australia. Criminal Law Forum, 8, 369-385.