Burnout and Sleep

advertisement
Burnout and Sleep
Mirjam Ekstedt
Stockholm 2005
Department of Public Health Sciences
Division of Psychosocial Factors and Health
Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm Sweden
Background
Burnout is a form of extreme fatigue related to occupational stress (Maslach et al., 2001) and
which is not relieved during the time normally used for recovery (evening, weekend,
vacation). It has become a major complaint in society and is one cause behind the doubling of
sickness absence in Sweden during the last decade (RFV, 2002). While the effects of stress
may be direct, through the psychobiological stress mechanisms, one should also consider
stress-induced lack of recovery through, for example disturbed sleep. This has not been
studied before. Neither has the pattern of recovery during the day or week been studied. One
also lacks information on how burnout is experienced and for example a qualitative approach
world provide essential understanding of aspects in human life that precedes burnout.
Thus this thesis brings together four studies focusing on the relation between burnout and
sleep, the relation between sleep fragmentation and stress markers, the pattern of
fatigue/sleepiness across the workday and weekend and the lived experiences of time
preceding burnout.
First will be reviewed some of the background literature and previous research on burnout and
sleep and their possible connections. This will be followed by the methodology, a summary of
the results and a discussion.
What is burnout?
Burnout has attracted considerable interest since it was first established in the early seventies
(Freudenberger, 1974) and has lately reached considerable attention in scientific work as well
as in mass media. The number of scientific papers on the topic is extensive and most of them
have a social psychological perspective (Schaufeli & 1998).
One reason for the interest is the remarkable impact on the increase in long-term sick leave in
many industrialized countries (Weber & Jaekel-Reinhard, 2000). In Sweden the total
percentage of mental ill-health has almost doubled from the mid nineties until 2002, with
depression, anxiety and persistent fatigue as dominating symptoms (Socialstyrelsen, 2003a).
This occurred simultaneously with a significant increase in proportion of sleep disturbances,
especially in the youngest age groups (18-24 years). Impaired psychological well-being is
about twice as common in women compared to men (15 % women and 10% men in the age
group 16-44 years) and most remarkable in the working age groups (mainly white-collar
workers and within the school and the health care sector). This supports the notion of
causation by occupational stress, but there is also an increase in sleep problems, anxiety and
fatigue among unemployed and students as well, suggesting that life conditions in general
also has great influence (Hallsten et al., 2002; RFV, 2002; Socialstyrelsen, 2003a). In the
absence of a distinct conceptualization of the increase in ill health, burnout has become an
assembling diagnosis in the daily language (Weber & Jaekel-Reinhard, 2000).
1
Theories
Burnout was first mentioned in the literature by Bradley (1969) as a psychological
phenomenon that occurred in the helping professions, although Freudenberger (1974) is
regarded as the inventor of the term. The term was adopted from empirical research where
respondents used “burnout” as a metaphor to describe a state of exhaustion in which they
gradually were drained of energy (Maslach & Jackson, 1984). However, the term burnout is
not entirely appropriate since it triggers associations to the definite state of a burnt-out candle
or fire, while the phenomenon is better represented by an analogy like an emptied battery.
This reflects the gradual process of more energy expended than what has been provided for.
In Sweden there has been several attempts to arrive at a more “diagnostic” term to suit the
clinical symptoms. “Exhaustion-syndrome” (utmattningssyndrom) proposed by Åsberg et al.,
(2002) is the officially preferred term, and has recently been established as an ICD diagnosis
in the Swedish version (Socialstyrelsen, 1997, 2005). For lack of conceptualization, the term
burnout is used in this thesis as “working name” because of its global acceptance in both
research society and everyday language.
The historical development of the concept took place along two lines, where Freudenberger
(Freudenberger, 1974) represented a more clinical approach and Maslach, (1981b) Pines
(1981) and colleagues placed burnout on the scientific agenda (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998).
Freudenberger (1974) based his first definition of burnout on own experiences in clinical
work; “to fail, wear out, or to become exhausted by making excessive demands on energy,
strengths or resources” (p. 159-160). Later Freudenberger opened up to the conception of
burnout as a syndrome when he expanded the definition to involve personality, work
performance, social relations and life in general. Further, he suggested that the
conceptualization of burnout was a process, including the individual and the organization
(Freudenberger, 1983). The risk of a broad “all-inclusive” definition is that the concept soon
turns out to include almost every kind of social and personal problem and thus looses its
meaning.
The most frequently cited definition of burnout is that by Maslach and co-workers (1986, p
1): “ burnout is a syndrome of emotional exhaustion, depersonalization and reduced personal
accomplishment that can occur among individuals who do ‘people work’ of some kind”. This
established the idea of a syndrome explicitly from the beginning. From the results of
explorative studies a multidimensional measure of burnout was developed, called the Maslach
Burnout Inventory (MBI). It was intended for use with people working in the human services,
health care, and education (Maslach & Jackson, 1981a; Maslach et al., 1996). An adapted
version of the MBI was developed later for use beyond the human services, but in contrast to
Freudenberger, Maslach never expanded burnout to apply outside work (Maslach et al.,
2001). Contemporary burnout research is dominated by the MBI (used in over 90% of the
studies) (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998), which strongly has influenced the conceptualization of
burnout, and has led to the development of theoretical models based on the three MBIdimensions. The hypothesis most used is a sequential progression of burnout over time, which
explicitly directs causality between the different dimensions, but the mechanism has not been
described (Maslach & Leiter, 2005; Maslach et al., 2001).
2
An integrative model that is applicable at both the interpersonal and organizational level, has
been presented by Shaufeli and others (Schaufeli & Buunk, 1996). According to this model a
strong initial motivation and engagement is assumed to be a necessary condition for
developing burnout. This is then associated with an unfavorable job environment and with
inadequate coping strategies as a self-perpetuating force.
Even though the MBI dominates the scientific arena there are several other definitions of
burnout that emphasize other aspects of the concept. For example Pines, Aronson and Kafry
(1981) present a broader approach and include an existential dimension where work is
suggested to become the matter that expects to give meaning to the existence in a secularized
western society. Characteristic symptoms involving feelings of helplessness, hopelessness and
entrapment, are accompanied by low self-esteem (pp 34-35, 202-203). Hallsten (1993) in
contrast, proposes self-esteem to be a prerequisite of burnout and includes “a self-esteem
based on achievements” as a hypothetical contributing factor to account for the “burning-out”
process. The focus in his framework is the gradual process, not the context wherein it appears
(occupational or non-occupational), with regard both to the different symptoms and to the
different dimensions. Cherniss (1980) and his associates constructed a process model in
which burnout is viewed as a series of negative changes in attitude and behavior that occur
over time, associated with a “reality chock”, for new professionals in helping organizations,
(Cherniss, 1980).
Another multidimensional measure of burnout was developed by Melamed and co-workers,
the Shirom-Melamed Burnout Questionnaire (SMBQ) (Kushnir & Melamed, 1992; Melamed
et al., 1992), which, in contrast to the MBI, doesn’t include the antecedents or consequences.
The conceptualization of burnout, based on an extensive review by Shirom (1989), concludes;
“the unique content of burnout has to do with the depletion of an individual’s energetic
resources – a combination of physical fatigue, emotional exhaustion and cognitive
weariness” (p.33). This definition implies a long-term exposure to emotionally charged
demands, in both client related and non-client related occupational settings. Social
commitments as well as major life crisis, are suggested to initiate or exacerbate burnout
(Kushnir & Melamed, 1992). This definition of burnout takes the life conditions into account
and describes three facets of excessive fatigue, which impinges on the person as a whole with
emotions, mind and body.
A synthesis
No definition of burnout has been agreed upon and the existing definitions are contradictory
at least on some points. By looking at commonalities and inconsistencies in the
conceptualizations so far, one tends to arrive at the notion that burnout is excessive fatigue or
exhaustion as a negative affective response to stress in a broad sense. An important but
implicit property in most definition of burnout is that it emphasizes chronicity in that the state
of exhaustion is not relieved by daily or weekly rest (Maslach et al., 2001; Schaufeli &
Buunk, 1996; Shirom, 1989). However, those features are not unique to burnout and some of
the issues that need to be investigated further are discussed below.
3
The main discrepancy in the definitions concerns whether burnout is exclusively job related
or if it can develop in relation to non-occupational settings. Since the MBI questionnaire
dominates the research area and it is restricted exclusively to job-related settings, research on
burnout has mainly focused on burnout and job stress (see for example Kalimo, 1999;
Schaufeli & Buunk, 1996). Instruments that refer to non-occupational settings are scarce
(Hallsten, 1993; Shirom, 1989). There is also a lack of descriptive and explorative studies,
longitudinal approaches, physiological measures and qualitative approaches, but this is
discussed in subsequent sections.
Burnout is often seen as an individual matter. But symptoms developed in relation to others,
whether in a family context, on the societal level or at work in an interpersonal or
organizational level, stress different explanatory perspectives on burnout development.
Further clarifying data is needed however, to explain how burnout is attributed to the
individual, the organization and society. An alternative view is that burnout does not exist as a
syndrome but is a social construct that individualizes societal problems (Söderfeldt, 1997;
Walker, 1986).
Using a “laundry list” of over 100 burnout symptoms Schaufeli and co-workers (1998) found
that the most characteristic core symptoms were clustered into what is called “state
definitions” of burnout, that is a negative mental condition instead of a process that develops
over time. But most assessment does not explain why the concept is limited to certain
dimensions, or why others are excluded. Furthermore, there are inconsistencies regarding
whether prerequisites and sequelae of burnout are to be included or not. In addition, it is not
agreed upon whether burnout is seen as an end state of a long-term unsuccessful struggle with
stressors, or if there is an ongoing process involved in the definition.
With the inconsistencies above in mind, and attempting to avoid a wide-ranging definition of
burnout that includes everything, one might attempt to extract a common denominator of the
conceptualizations above. Thus, the core negative emotional experience in burnout may be
identified as “the depletion of an individual’s energetic resources” (Shirom, 1989, p.33). This
seems to be the dimension for which there is most support in longitudinal research and which
is recognized by most constructors of burnout scales (Shirom, 1989). Thus emotional
exhaustion, together with physical fatigue and cognitive weariness seem to be the core
dimensions of burnout. However, the three fatigue dimensions are poorly defined and poorly
differentiated. And this calls for further evaluation of the concept.
Diagnostic assessment
Burnout is not a formal diagnostic category and it is not included in any of the officially
accepted international diagnostic classification systems: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders 4th ed. (DSM-IV), issued by the American Psychiatric Association (APA,
2000) and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10), issued by the World Health
Organization (WHO, 1992). The diagnostic label commonly used is “Adjustment disorder”
(APA, 2000) or “stress related exhaustion” (Z73.0: WHO, 1992), and if work related, burnout
has been labeled Neurasthenia according to the ICD-10. Depression is another diagnosis used
and other overlapping concepts as Vital Exhaustion (VE), Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS)
4
or Fibromyalgia (FM) have probably been used in diagnosing persons with symptoms of
burnout.
However, recently The National Board of Health and Welfare has added “burnout” or
“exhaustion syndrome” as a supplementary diagnosis to the Swedish version.
(Socialstyrelsen, 2005). The criteria are: physiological or mental symptoms of exhaustion for
at least two weeks, an essential lack of psychological energy, and symptoms as difficulties to
concentrate, decreased ability to cope with stress, irritability or emotional lability, sleep
disturbances and/or ache or pain, dizziness or palpitations having had to occur every day
during a two-week period. The symptoms must cause significant clinical suffering, with
impaired work capacity, and the symptoms must not be related to other psychiatric diagnosis,
substance abuse or medical diagnosis (Socialstyrelsen, 2003b).
Since the diagnostic category is new it has not been used in the present thesis. This is
particularly important since the diagnosis involves disturbed sleep. Sleep was not a part of the
diagnosis in the studies in the present thesis. For scientific work the MBI has been suggested,
with the top third of the Human Service Scale (MBI-HSS), defining “high degree of burnout”
and the bottom-third low degree of burnout. But these criteria are statistical and arbitrary,
rather than based on clinical experiences and should only be used in clinical research
(Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). However, in this thesis the SMBQ has been used for the
selection of study populations because of its applicability also in non-occupational settings.
The scale is positively associated with both episodic stress and chronic work stress (Melamed
et al., 1992; Melamed et al., 1999). The SMBQ shows high correlations with Pines Burnout
Measure and the MBI (Grossi et al., 1999).
Fatigue and related states
In the discussion above fatigue was introduced as the central concept of burnout. It is not a
well-defined concept, however, and there is a need for a brief summary of some of the
discussions of the meaning of fatigue. It may be particularly important since all burnout scales
make use of a number of synonyms of fatigue that may have slightly different meanings.
There is also a need for a discussion of some related concepts such as, Depression, Vital
Exhaustion and The Chronic Fatigue Syndrome.
Fatigue
The definition and assessment of fatigue has been a subject of controversy for many years,
and discussions include whether fatigue is a discrete entity, a continuum or a set of diffuse
symptoms (Beurskens et al., 2000; Kant et al., 2003). In scientific work fatigue is described
either from a biological/neuropsychological (Grandjean, 1970), or a social/psychological
perspective (Cameron, 1973), but the definitions are vague and inconsistent, and the
physiological mechanisms poorly understood. Its use in ordinary language is diffuse and
5
clearly has many different meanings, among which it may be difficult to differentiate
(Aaronson et al., 1999; Piper, 1986; Ream & Richardson, 1996).
Most of the concepts of fatigue assume that it is the outcome of energy loss due to activity
that depletes the resources for further activity. However, if fatigue is a state when energy loss
exceeds energy availability (Piper, 1986), it seems reasonable to believe that imbalance
between catabolic and anabolic processes is involved. Then also dysregulation of the
endocrine or the immune system may yield fatigue (Schedlowski & Tewes, 1999), even
without any excessive activity. This is for example what is suggested in chronic diseases
where fatigue is a common complaint (Ford et al., 1998; Parker et al., 2001). But the energy
drain might also be due to an insufficient anabolic function, for example, a poor restorative
value of sleep.
In research a distinction between chronic and acute fatigue was pointed out at an early stage
(Bartley & Chute, 1947) of which the latter signifies a temporary state that passes after a
resting period, while the former is mainly observed in clinical practice as a state that does not
dissipate easily with rest or sleep (Cahill, 1999; Cameron, 1973; Craig & Cooper, 1992). As
noted above, a key characteristic of burnout is this chronicity. However, it appears that this
temporal pattern of excessive fatigue in burnout has never been verified in longitudinal
studies. This thesis will try to present at least short term longitudinal approach (see below).
Acute fatigue in terms of reduced alertness: sleepiness is used in more limited sense as a
desire for sleep (Dement & Carskadon, 1982; Dinges, 1989).
Fatigue, tiredness, exhaustion are often used as synonyms. However, one may perhaps see
fatigue as a development from low levels to high levels - exhaustion. Tiredness, fatigue and
exhaustion might also be distinct states along the same continuum. Such a model has been
suggested based on explorative studies on elite athletes (fatigue due to excessive physical
activity during leisure time), shift workers, and individuals experiencing “burn-out” (fatigue
due to work), and individuals diagnosed with depression, cancer, or Chronic Fatigue
Syndrome (fatigue due to disease) (Olson, in press). In the latter study each of the tirednessexhaustion dimensions contained components of affective, muscular, somatic, cognitive and
social character. Earlier research on fatigue has often focused on one dimension at a time,
depending on context, but a multidimensional approach may be more fruitful in understanding
the list of symptoms characterizing burnout and may better illustrate how the excessive
fatigue in burnout may develop.
Characteristics of fatigue related to work vary due to work conditions. Studies on physical
fatigue in industrial work (Chambers, 1961; Grandjean, 1979) or decreased performance in
“highly skilled work” have a long history (Bartlett, 1943; Bartley & Chute, 1947). Those
early studies were focused on physical fatigue related to muscular activity (Enoka, 1995) or
fatigue due to extreme physical conditions of noise, exposure to heat, cold, humidity or
chemical substances (Bell, 1978; Gamberale, 1985; Ramsey, 1983). Bartley and Chute (1947)
maintained that the term fatigue to be used only to describe the subjective feeling of lassitude
and disinclination towards activity, and the term impairment to describe the reduction in
physical capacity resulting from accumulated oxygen debt in muscle tissue. Fatigue was
suggested to be a biological warning that the individual’s recourses were overtaxed (Bartley
& Chute, 1947). Bartlett (1953) on the other hand noted that those indicators tended to surface
too late, and that performance could be maintained at an adequate level almost to the point of
6
total exhaustion (Cameron, 1973). This observation is interesting with regard to fatigue and
burnout.
Emotional and physical fatigue are closely intertwined and related to decreased motivation,
feelings of uneasiness (Cameron, 1973; Shirom, 1989), and has in several studies been
suggested as a potent predictor of myocardial infarction (Appels et al., 1993; Appels &
Mulder, 1988). Studies show that prolonged physical training concurrent with inadequate
recovery in athletes, have resulted in the overtraining syndrome (Armstrong & VanHeest,
2002; Kenttä et al., 2001), which shares numerous characteristics with burnout. This
syndrome, however, is also connected to mental stress and striving for achievements,
suggesting that their combined effects could promote excessive physical fatigue.
The cognitive weariness in burnout is associated with fatigue of psychological or affective
character and appears to be of central importance in occupations where continued attention is
required (Shirom, 1989). Psychological fatigue has been associated with stress or emotional
experiences like fear, frustration, anxiety and distress (Aaronson et al., 1999; Cahill, 1999).
The label mental fatigue is used to describe a gradual development of fatigue in relation to
decreased efficacy and motivation, and decreased alertness and performance with time (on
task) as an important ingredient (Cameron, 1973; Grandjean, 1979). Affective fatigue, as
recognized in depression, has elements of sadness, distress and feelings of worthlessness
(Cahill, 1999). Still, those labels are used interchangeably in the literature and there are no
sharp boundaries between them.
A multi-dimensional model of occupational fatigue has been evaluated and tested in different
contexts of working life (Åhsberg, 2000). The work related fatigue due to physical work,
mental work or night work was described as being composed of five dimensions: lack of
energy, lack of motivation, physical exertion, physical discomfort and sleepiness. While these
dimensions were identified via factor analysis there was still a rather strong common
component among them, in particular represented by lack of energy.
It should be emphasized that most burnout instruments usually combine a number of fatiguerelated adjectives to form scales. Thus, it is not possible to determine whether there is any
particular type of fatigue that dominates in burnout. In this thesis we have included the
Åhsberg approach (2000) to explore the possibility of finding a main dimension of fatigue.
In summary, it appears that fatigue is a frequently used, but vague, concept denoting energy
depletion. In the present thesis I will therefore define it operationally (see Methods).
Depression
Depression and burnout share some important characteristics. Depression is characterized by:
depressed mood, weight loss or weight gain, insomnia or hypersomnia, fatigue or loss of
energy, feelings of insufficiency or guilt, thoughts about death and suicide, among other
symptoms (APA, 2000). While many of these (particularly fatigue) may occur in burnout one
distinction derives from differences in their attributional patterns and their context. Burnout is
enmeshed in the social and organizational context while depression essentially reflects
7
personal thoughts and emotions (Leiter & Durup, 1994). Problems at work do not appear in
the etiology of depression to a greater extent than problems in other life domains (Beck, 1970;
Beck & Beamesderfer, 1974). A confirmatory factor analysis of the most widely used
depression (Beck Depression Inventory; BDI) and burnout measure (MBI) suggests that
fatigue is an underlying factor accounting for the correlation between burnout and depression
(Leiter & Durup, 1994).
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome
The major criterion for the Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) is the new onset of severe and
disabling fatigue, present for at least six months and an absence of an identifiable medical and
psychiatric etiology of the fatigue (Fukuda et al., 1994; Holmes et al., 1998). This description
appears to overlap with symptoms of burnout, but the onset of fatigue in CFS is not thought to
result from prolonged stress, as is the case with burnout. The concurrent occurrence of
symptoms like muscle pain, tender lymph nodes, sore throat, headache or other, with a vague
relation to inflammatory response, also distinguish CFS from burnout, since such symptoms
are not included in core definitions of burnout (Fukuda et al., 1994; Krupp et al., 1991).
Vital Exhaustion
Vital Exhaustion, (VE) is a construct characterized by fatigue, irritability and demoralization,
developed to understand the unusual fatigue commonly reported immediately before
myocardial infarction or cardiac death (Appels & Mulder, 1988). VE is suggested to result
from prolonged stress, and there is a high correlation between VE and double (work+ home)
work load (Appels et al., 1993). Exploratory analysis suggests that VE is related to several
stressors in childhood, as well as in family life and at work, which also is what distinguishes
VE from burnout according to Appels (Appels et al., 1993). But when including real life
stress as a prerequisite of burnout the similarities between the two concepts are striking.
However, the measure of VE; the Maastricht Questionnaire (Appels et al., 1987) uses items
on sleep disturbances and depression, apart from fatigue, and doesn’t measure cognitive
impairment, in contrast to the SMBQ, for example. Additionally, the Maastricht
Questionnaire has dichotomous response alternatives, which imposes difficulties to answer in
an early stage when symptoms may be weak, diffuse, and intermittent. Most burnout
measures assess a gradual development of symptoms while the Maastricht Questionnaire is
more applicable in clinical research.
Burnout and stress
Burnout is, by definition, caused by stress and the correlation between the two is high, at least
when measured by questionnaires (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). There are, however, only
limited amounts of stress research in relation to burnout and it is not clear how the
experiences of a stressful situation causes the fatigue and cognitive impairment that
8
characterize burnout (Maslach & Leiter, 2005). Below is summarized some of the views on
stress and its physiological effects.
Stress theory
Stress is a rather ambiguous concept, possibly because it is used to describe four different
phenomena: 1) the stimulus itself 2) the experience of the stimulation 3) the non-specific
general (alarm) stress response and 4) the feedback and the experience of the reaction.
From a general perspective stress refers to the the rate of wear and tear in the organism, and
the biological definition of stress refers to the nonspecific response to any demand (Selye,
1956) to increase the chances of survival of an individual who is facing a life-threatening
situation. According to psychological theories stress is determined by the balance between the
perceived demands from the environment and the individual’s resources to meet those
demands (Frankenhaeuser, 1986; Lundberg, 1995).
Cognitive Activation Theory of Stress, CATS, (Ursin & Eriksen, 2004) emphasizes the
cognitive processes and the neurophysiological activation, and the individual’s appraisal of
the stress situation seems to be the mediating link. Lazarus (1984) proposes a transactional
approach where stress involves both the situation (stimuli) and the individual’s experience
where the notion of appraisal is fundamental. In a phenomenological view background,
meaning and the person’s concerns (that what really matters to a person), defines what counts
as stressful and what the coping possibilities are (Benner & Wrubel, 1989). This is paramount
to the individual differences in the stress response and depends on personality, past
experiences, personal values, goals and expectations ( Benner & Wrubel, 1989; Cooper, 2005;
Parkes, 1994)
Ursin and Eriksen (2004) claim that the level of physiological response depends on the
learned expectancy of being able to cope with the stressful situation. Even though stress
cannot always be alleviated by coping, it might ease tension and provide access to skills that
deliberate energy and allow the person to recognize a new way of being in the situation
(Benner & Wrubel, 1989; Bullington, 1999). Thus adequate coping might reduce the arousal
level, while lack of control over the outcome, ineffective or blocked coping, produces strong
physiological stress response and is suggested to lead to helplessness, frustration and distress
(Frankenhaeuser, 1981; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Ursin & Eriksen, 2004).
A generalized feeling of hopelessness might occur as a response to the enduring stress, when
actions taken to relieve the stress do not help. Hopelessness and helplessness are entities
closely related to depression and might function as a coping strategy that reduces the arousal
(Ursin & Eriksen, 2004). However, there are studies suggesting that psychosocial events may
play a significant role in the onset of episodes of Major Depression (Stanley & Burrows,
2005), and this is also noted in the Diagnostic Statistical Manual of mental disorders (DSMIV) (APA, 2000).
It is important to underline that stress originally is a drive for survival and is not dangerous
per se, but that sustained high tonic activation is maladaptive and may cause damages
9
(Cooper, 2005). It is hypothesized that for each individual there is an optimal level of arousal
(Chrousos & Gold, 1992). This means that if the stress intensity is moderate, the effect on the
brain yields increased alertness and attention, which may improve the cognitive performance
at excessive levels of stress detrimental effects on performance, behavior and emotions may
ensue (Chrousos, 1998b; McEwen, 2004). In fact, several studies show that stress, but only up
to a certain level, improves performance, for example on selective attention tasks (Chajut &
Algom, 2003), and that underload as well as overload related to advanced work processes
induce frustration and stress (Frankenhaeuser & Gardell, 1976).
Among psychological approaches to research on stress and burnout one would have expected
a wide application of qualitative methods. This does not seem to be the case however, and the
present thesis will seek to bring in such a perspective (see below for discussion).
Physiological mechanisms
Contemporary physiological stress models derive from Cannon (1914) and Selye’s (1936,
1956) pioneering work. Selye (1946) proposed a model of stress, the General Adaptation
Syndrome (GAS), comprised of three stages, alarm, resistance, and exhaustion that reflect the
physiological non-specific response to a challenge. The resistance stage of GAS has profound
energy requirements, which, if persistent over time, depletes the person’s capacity and leads
to exhaustion.
Cannon (1914) developed the “fight-flight” concept, which linked the emotional perception of
a “threat” (Dunn & Berridge, 1990; Swanson & Sawchenko, 1983) to physiological changes
in the periphery. Markers of the fight-flight response are the catecholamines epinephrine and
norepinephrine, and other physiological indicators associated with the autonomic nervous
system (Brown, 1991; Dunn & Berridge, 1990). Thus, the Sympathetic Adrenal Medullary
(SAM) system is activated in situations when the individual feels threatened, irrespective of
whether the emergency reaction is to battle or escape (Lundberg, 2000).
The Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenocortical (HPA) axis is also fundamental in the stress
reaction. When the SAM system is activated and neuropeptides like Cortico-ReleasingHormone (CRH) and vasopressin are released they in turn stimulate adrenocorticotropin
(ACTH) release into the general blood circulation within the pituitary (Dunn & Berridge,
1990; Rock et al., 1984). An increase or decrease in the HPA-axis and SAM system produce
abnormal levels of mainly cortisol and catecholamines; epinephrine and norepinephrine into
the blood (Folkow, 1997; Sapolsky et al., 1984). These “stress systems” (i.e. HPA - axis and
SAM system) are interacting with the major endocrine and the gastrointestinal and immune
systems through complex stimulatory and inhibitory feedback pathways (Chrousos & Gold,
1992).
The acute stress response is no problem as long as the stress activation can be shut down
when stress terminates. But chronic stress is much less studied and refers to the resistance and
exhaustion phase in the GAS (Selye, 1946).
10
Recent research in psychoneuroendocrinology and neuroimmunology (Schedlowski & Tewes,
1999) and the theory of allostasis (Sterling & Eyer, 1988) and allostatic load (McEwen,
1998) elucidate how acute and chronic aspects of the stress response might lead to diseases,
and thus also may be the way burnout develops. The allostasis model takes the variability of
the body systems into account, and includes the ability of the body to increase or decrease the
activation level of vital functions to new steady states dependent on the characteristics of the
challenge and the person’s emotions and appraisal of the event (McEwen, 1998; Sterling &
Eyer, 1988). Allostasis has a price - the allostatic load, which refer to the cumulative cost to
the body when the systems overreact, fail to respond adequately or fail to shut down to the
“normal set-point” after a stressful event (McEwen, 2004). It is suggested that serious
pathophysiology can occur if overload is not relieved in some way (McEwen & Stellar,
1993).
Considering the theory of allostatic load it seems reasonable to believe that permanent or
repeated exposure to stress, which one cannot adequately cope with, may cause an allostatic
up regulation of the HPA-axis leading in turn to abnormal elevations in the levels of
circulating glucocorticoids (Chrousos & Gold, 1992; Eriksen et al., 1999; Lundberg &
Frankenhaeuser, 1980). Probably high cortisol levels lead to impaired memory function
through negative effects on neural structures, which have been observed particularly in
hippocampus (Sapolsky et al., 1984; Watanabe et al., 1992). It has been proposed that with
time, longstanding strain on the HPA-axis may lead to hypocortisolism and a blunted HPAaxis regulation in response to stress (McEwen, 1998). This might be one way of explaining
the characteristics of excessive fatigue and exhaustion in burnout. This is supported by
observations of excessively low levels of cortisol in diseases with high fatigue like CFS,
(Demitrack, 1994), Vital Exhaustion (Nicolson & van Diest, 2000) and Fibromyalgia (Parker
et al., 2001) and Post Traumatic Stress Syndrome (Yehuda, 1999), which is caused by high
stress load.
Research on physiological markers in burnout
As suggested above it would seem reasonable to connect exhaustion in burnout to the
exhaustion phase of the General Adapation Syndrome (GAS) after a long term exposure to
repeated stress. Thus, one would expect to find changes in both the HPA and the SAM axis in
individuals with burnout. The relatively few studies on this topic do not give any conclusive
results, however. The few studies on burnout and physiological markers, cortisol in saliva has
been frequently used, due to its role in the HPA-reaction to stress as discussed above, and
since assessment of salivary cortisol is convenient to use in field studies (Wust et al., 2000).
The results show groups with stress or increased workload often exhibit increased cortisol
levels, particularly in the morning (Härenstam & Theorell, 1990; Kirschbaum et al., 1992;
Schulz et al., 1998; Steptoe et al., 2000). Similar results have been found in burnout patients
(De Vente et al., 2003) with increased morning salivary cortisol levels in comparison to
healthy controls. This was replicated in females in another study (Grossi et al., in press).
Elevated levels of morning cortisol has been demonstrated also in blue-collar workers with
chronic burnout (Melamed et al., 1999). Other studies have shown contradictory results.
Thus, teachers scoring high on burnout showed a lower overall salivary cortisol secretion
during three consecutive mornings of saliva sampling (Pruessner et al., 1999) and in a study
11
conducted among military personnel high burnout scores was associated with low morning
cortisol and elevated evening levels (Morgan et al., 2002). A reduction of urine free-cortisol
excretion was found in a longitudinal study on female burnout patients compared with healthy
controls (Moch et al., 2003). Single blood samples have failed to demonstrate associations
between cortisol and burnout (Grossi et al., 2003; Grossi et al., 1999).
The inconsistent results may derive from methodological differences (King & Hegadoren,
2003), but also the intensity and chronicity of stress, as well as the individual’s appraisal of
the event and the method of coping, may have had an impact on the results (Eriksen et al.,
1999; Wust et al., 2000).
In addition to responses of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis, one would
expect also a sympathico-adreno-medullary (SAM) mediated stress response in burnout. Thus
elevated heart rate and blood pressure have been found in burnout patients compared to
controls (De Vente et al., 2003). In contrast to an expected reduction in heart rate variability
in soldiers with high burnout scores, an inappropriate level of vagal inhibition of the heart was
seen, which resulted in higher variability (Morgan et al., 2002).
Associations between high burnout and elevated levels of lipids and serum cholesterol were
found in a civilian working force (Melamed et al., 1992; Shirom et al., 1997), while such
changes were not seen in a study in women. In that group no changes in growth hormones or
anabolic variables was found (Moch et al., 2003). The alterations in the HPA-axis, the SNS
system and associated risk factors for CardioVascular Diseases (CVD) and metabolic
syndrome seen in burnout, are not as consistent as are those found in research on stress, job
strain or shift work (Grossi et al., 1999; Knutsson & Bøggild, 2000; Schnall et al., 1998;
Theorell & Karasek, 1996; Yehuda, 1999).
The inflammatory response in relation to burnout has been investigated in a few studies. TNFα was enhanced in a study of burnout among women (Grossi et al., 2003). Differences in the
numbers of NK-cells and an altered NK-cell activity (Bargellini et al., 2000; Nakamura et al.,
1999) and leucocytes (Lerman et al., 1999) have been found. Although important, these
findings are not specific for burnout.
It appears that no physiological models described above are sufficient to explain the process
from the experience of stress, through the CNS into bodily processes (Weiner, 1992). And
burnout does not seem to have been well characterized in terms of stress physiology (Maslach
& Leiter, 2005). One such attempt is made in the present thesis.
Recovery
Above, it was discussed that the energy depletion in burnout may be the “cost” of a long-term
exposure to stress, and a growing body of evidence shows that the ability to shut-down the
regulatory systems to a base-line level after stressful events, is beneficial for health (Chrousos
& Gold, 1992; McEwen, 1998, 2000; Seeman et al., 2001).
12
From the perspective of balance and allostasis, recovery has been defined as “the period of
time that an individual needs to return to a normal or pre-stressor level of functioning
following the termination of a stressor” (Craig & Cooper, 1992). Need for recovery (time
required to return to baseline) has been suggested to be a better measure of severity of stress
than the immediate stress response (Depue & Monroe, 1986; van Amelsvoort et al., 2003). A
prospective, large, cohort study of prolonged fatigue (Bültmann et al., 2000) showed that
fatigue, need for recovery (or unwinding) and burnout were associated to each other (Kant et
al., 2003). This emphasized that recovery has to be considered when prerequisites to burnout
is discussed.
However, as noted earlier the individual’s ability to return to a physiological base-line (i.e
recovery) is also dependent on the interactive process between the person and the
environment, and on the way the situation is experienced (Benner & Wrubel, 1989; Lazarus
& Folkman, 1984). The appraisal of the event, as well as the ability to “unwind”, involves the
whole person: body, mind, thoughts and emotions. Recovery in the meaning of rest has been
defined as “a sense of harmony in feelings, actions and motivation” (Asp, 2002, p. 93) and a
process of maintaining and/or restoring health (Torkildsen, 1999).
Health is in this thesis viewed from a multidimensional perspective, which can vary on a
continuum from health to ill-health along a physiological and behavioral dimension, on a
subjective dimension of wellbeing, and also on an existential dimension where personal
values, motivation and meaning are essential drives for life (Eriksson, 1987; Svensson, 1981,
1990). Knowledge about why and how to rest could lead people to incorporate healthpromoting behavior (Asp, 2002). Therefore, the need for such knowledge is apparent in the
context of burnout.
The energy loss and excessive fatigue found in burnout may not only be an effect of stressor
exposure or insufficient recovery time. It might also be due to insufficient energy
mobilization as discussed above. Unfortunately, very little empirical data is available on the
effects of recovery on stress or burnout. Among the studies needed are descriptions of the
pattern of rest during the day and across days, as well as how such patterns contribute to or
prevent burnout. The present thesis will attempt to provide such descriptive data.
Sleep
The main focus of the present thesis is not on recovery in general, however, but on recovery
through sleep. Since by definition, sleep fails to give sufficient daily restitution to eliminate
fatigue, sleep should be an important aspect of burnout investigation. Sleep is fundamental for
the anabolic processes, intended to reconstruct the damages of the wear and tear (Adam &
Oswald, 1977, 1983). But sleep is also necessary for proper brain functioning, wellbeing and
daily functioning (Benington & Heller, 1995), thus basic for both body and mind. However, if
the restoration through sleep is impaired, it seems reasonable to believe that an “energy debt”
might occur. The arguments for this are discussed below.
13
It has recently been demonstrated that burnout scores are related to reports of disturbed sleep
(Grossi et al., 2003; Melamed et al., 1999) and this is interesting in its own right. However,
no studies of physiological sleep in burnout have been carried out and thus it is not clear if
there is objective support for the reported sleep disturbances. In, for example Primary
Insomnia one often fails to find objective indications of disturbances (Rosa & Bonnet, 2000).
Thus there is a need for studies on physiological sleep in burnout to investigate if sleep is
objectively impaired.
Another reason for studying sleep in burnout is that shortened or fragmented sleep is closely
associated with sleepiness (Bonnet & Arand, 2003; Gillberg, 1995; Härmä et al., 1998;
Stepanski et al., 1984). Also, organic sleep disturbances, such as sleep apnea (Guilleminault
et al., 1993; Mitler, 1993) or restless legs (Rosenthal et al., 1984) increase sleepiness. Thus, if
short or disrupted sleep is substantiated in burnout groups this might be one contributor to the
fatigue symptoms in burnout.
Disturbed sleep, sleep loss, and sleep fragmentation are also associated with impaired
performance, especially on tasks of frontal lobe or executive function, including memory,
creativity and planning skills (Drummond & Brown, 2001; Horne, 1988; Jones & Harrison,
2001). Furthermore sleep disturbances are connected to emotional changes and mood
disorders (Adam et al., 1986) and have been considered a risk factor for episodes of major
depression and anxiety (Ford & Cooper-Patrick, 2001). Since all those features are frequently
seen in burnout (Hallsten et al., 2002; Maslach et al., 2001; Åsberg et al., 2002) it is
important to sort out if disturbed sleep might be a common denominator.
Previous studies on sleep and burnout
The very few studies that reports sleep complaints in burnout (as measured by SMBQ) are
cross sectional, and based on subjective reports of disturbed sleep. Melamed et al (1999)
examined whether chronic burnout was associated with somatic and physiological
hyperarousal (measured by cortisol in saliva and questionnaires) in a sample of blue-collar
workers. Apart from the findings of elevated levels of cortisol in saliva, they found that those
complaining of burnout symptoms for more than 6 months exhibited more difficulties falling
asleep, more early morning awakening, more tiredness on waking up and more difficulties
maintaining sleep than those with non-chronic burnout or without burnout symptoms. They
also complained from more tension and post work irritability.
The other study, by Grossi and coworkers (2003), investigated physiological correlates of
burnout in women in white-collar work and the habitual sleep quality was assessed with the
Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire (KSQ) in conjunction with the physiological assessments.
The high burnout group revealed more sleepiness, impaired sleep quality and awakening than
those scoring low on burnout.
Thus, even though sleep disturbances are common complaints in burnout, it has never been
hypothesized that burnout development may be a matter of impaired sleep or restitution.
Linking the emotional exhaustion and cognitive symptoms to physiological/biological
functioning is a new approach to burnout development that integrates the person as a whole.
14
Therefore, there is a need for objective measures of sleep to determinate the characteristics of
the sleep disturbances, if any. One major purpose of the present thesis was to provide such
measurements of sleep.
An overview of normal sleep
I order to provide a broader background on sleep and its restorative function this section will
present an overview of normal sleep and its restorative function and introduce the standard
methods to record and quantify sleep.
Structure and content of sleep
According to a general behavioral definition, “sleep is a reversible behavioral state of
perceptual disengagement from, and unresponsiveness to, the environment” (Carskadon &
Dement, 2000, p.3). The physiological changes during sleep are well established. Thus, sleep
is a cyclical process, which alternates with wakefulness. The method for physiological
measurement of sleep is based on the recording of electrical brain activity –
electroencephalography (EEG), of at least one EEG-lead (usually C3-A2 or C4-A1 according
to the 10-20 system (Jasper, 1958). There are two main states of sleep, Non-Rapid Eye
Movement (NREM) and Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep. Four stages of NREM sleep are
recognized; stages 1-4. To differentiate between REM sleep from NREM sleep it is necessary
to also record eye movements (electrooculography, EOG) and muscle tone (electromyography,
EMG) (Carskadon & Dement, 2000).
The first stage is Wake (W), or stage 0, characterized by a fast frequency EEG (beta activity,
> 12Hz) with an alpha rhythm (8-12 Hz) if the eyes are closed. After sleep onset, sleep
gradually moves through the stages 1-4, with the EEG showing increased amplitudes in
slower frequency bands. Stage 1-sleep appears when the alpha activity is replaced by a slower
EEG (theta = 4-8 Hz) activity, and a short period of slow eye movements. Stage 1 sleep is
associated with a low arousal threshold and a severely disrupted sleep consists of an increased
percentage of stage 1-sleep. Stage 2-sleep usually constitutes 50 per cent of the total sleep
period and is characterized by moderately slow frequency waves (4-7 Hz) and features slow
waves (K complexes) and rapid so-called sleep spindle (14-16 Hz) activity in the EEG. Delta
activity (0.5-4.0 Hz and an amplitude of > 75 microvolt), dominates Stage 3+4 sleep, and the
slow waves have given this type of sleep the name Slow Wave Sleep (SWS), or Slow Wave
Activity (SWA).
Parallel to the change to deeper sleep, heart rate, blood pressure and ventilation decreases
progressively during NREM sleep (Parmeggiani, 2000). Brain temperature decreases, Mainly
during SWS, which is important for cerebral recovery and the metabolism have slowed down.
The main proportion of Growth Hormone (GH) secretion occurs during SWS. REM sleep is
identified by periodic movement of the eyes (Rapid Eye Movements), increased EEG
frequency and a low muscle tonus. The breathing frequency is increased and so is heart rate
and blood pressure. The temperature regulation centre in hypothalamus is shut off during
REM sleep (Parmeggiani, 2000).
15
The exact onset of sleep is often difficult to identify, since the lighter stages of NREM sleep
often alternate with wakefulness during sleep onset. Stage 1 sleep generally persists for only a
few (1 to 7) minutes and formally sleep starts in the 30s interval that contains more than 50
per cent of stage 1-sleep. As stage 2-sleep progresses, there is a gradual appearance of highvoltage Slow Wave Activity (SWA), delta waves in the EEG. Stage 3 + 4-sleep lasts for 2040 minutes and is suddenly interrupted by an arousal and body movement, whereupon a short
period of REM sleep appears. This marks the end of the first sleep cycle, The NREM and
REM sleep continue to alternate through the night in 4-6 sleep cycles, which vary between 80
-120 minutes. SWS occupy more time in the first cycles while the amount of stage 2-sleep
and REM sleep expands throughout the night (Carskadon & Dement, 2000).
Measuring sleep
Standard criteria for describing sleep were developed by Rechtschaffen and Kales in the late
1960s (Rechtschaffen & Kales, 1968). They divided sleep into stages according to depth and
led to the concept of “sleep architecture”. The data generated by scoring sleep can be
presented in various manners. A sleep hypnogram as in fig 1 provides a visual representation
of the cyclic variation of sleep stages over the course of the night. Except for the sleep stages
(stage 1-4 + REM + awake), Movement time (M), and muscle artifacts are scored. Also brief
transient arousals of central or peripheral character are possible to score. An arousal from
sleep is defined as an EEG shift to at least alpha activity from stages 2-4 or REM, preceded
by at least 10 seconds of uninterrupted sleep (stages 2-4 or REM). Micro-arousals last
between 3 and 15 seconds and are too brief to be identified in the classical sleep staging
system but have been added to the literature later (ASDA, 1992). Frequent arousals rather
result in fragmented sleep than shortened sleep and so can provide valuable information on
the restorative function of sleep. Sleep measures also include Time In Bed (TIB), Total Sleep
Time (TST) and sleep efficiency (TST/TIB).
Figure 1. Progression of sleep stages presented in a hypnogram (below) and the dynamics of delta
power density (mV2) over the course of the night in normal sleep (above).
16
New technologies have provided increased capacities to understand sleep using new
computer-generated methods, like spectral analysis, through, for example, Fast Fourier
Transformation, (FFT) (Borbély et al., 1981). The power spectrum is the result of a
transformation from time to frequency (Aeschbach, 1995). Figure 1 provides a representation
of the delta power density over the course of the night, presented together with a typical
hypnogram. Power density adds information to the sleep analysis. For example SWS can be
regarded as an indicator of sleep intensity, and SWS in the first sleep cycle is more intense
(has higher amplitude) than SWS in the subsequent cycles (Dijk et al., 1990). The progressive
decline of delta power density over sleep cycles (upper part of fig 1), illustrates the gradual
fall of sleep intensity throughout the night. Thus Slow Wave Activity (SWA) shows the
variation of the NREM-REM sleep cycle, in greater detail than the traditional sleep profile
(see for an overview Borbély & Achermann, 1992).
In the spectral analysis all frequencies in the EEG are represented and both the period and the
amplitude of the signal are integrated in the analysis. The peak of the power spectrum reflects
the dominant frequencies in the EEG and, depending on the stage, the peak of the power
spectrum will be different. In SWS delta frequency is dominant and just before sleep (stage
W) the peak is in alpha activity (see fig 1). Power density is presented in 1 Hz bands or
integrated into 0.5-4.5 Hz (delta), 4.51 -7.5 Hz (theta), 7.51 – 12.5 Hz (alpha), 12.51-16.50
(sigma) and 16.51-32.5 Hz (beta) bands.
Sleep/wake regulation
Whether an individual is awake or asleep depends on the balance of forces promoting and
inhibiting each of those two states. The mechanism determining which of the states that will
predominate are poorly understood but a model of sleep/wake regulation suggests mainly
three processes that interacts with each other: the homeostatic (intrinsic) drive for sleep
(process S), the circadian rhythm (process C), (Borbély, 1982; Borbély & Achermann, 2000)
and the level of activation or sleep inertia (Process W) (Folkard & Åkerstedt, 1991; Åkerstedt
& Folkard, 1997).
The homeostatic drive to enter sleep (process S) increases with the duration since the end of
the previous sleep episode. During a usual 16-h wakefulness stable levels of neurobehavioural
function can be maintained, but with sustained wakefulness an increased homeostatic drive
for sleep builds up. When a homeostatic threshold is exceeded, sleep is triggered, and
wakefulness occurs when the sleep drive decreases. Prior time spent awake, and the quality of
prior sleep, is closely related to the need for nocturnal SWS (Åkerstedt & Gillberg, 1986).
Recovery sleep is also usually prolonged and deeper (with a higher arousal threshold and
increased SWS) than basal sleep. Prolongation of wakefulness, sleep debt or sleep
disturbances are shown to produce a rise of SWA during recovery sleep. Delta power density
is thus a sensitive measure of the restorative function of sleep (Carskadon & Dement, 2000).
The circadian aspect of sleep regulation (process C) is distinct from the homeostatic or sleepwake dependent aspects. For instance the increase in sleep propensity after extended
wakefulness is attributed to the homeostatic effect, whereas the propensity in the evening hour
observed even when people have been awake for 16 h or more is attributed to circadian effect
(Kryger et al., 1994). The external light-dark cycle is the primary synchronizer of the
endogenous circadian rhythms (Czeisler, 1995). Circadian rhythm originates in the
17
Suprachiasmatic Nuclei (SCN) of the hypothalamus (Meijer & Rietveld, 1989), and sends
impulses to the pineal gland for melatonin production, normally at, or after the habitual sleep
onset, leading to increased sleepiness. Melatonin is synthesized and secreted during darkness.
Melatonin onset seems to be the hormonal signal timing the rise in blood flow in distal skin
regions, and an increased vasodilatation leads to a decrease in core body temperature (Kräuchi
et al., 1999). The endogenous 24-h rhythm of melatonin and body temperature expresses the
circadian rhythm, and the maximum sleep propensity coincides with the nadir of the rhythm
around 4AM, whereas the lowest sleep propensity is close to the peak of the rhythm 12 hour
later (Borbély, 1982; Borbély & Achermann, 2000).
Sleep intensity and continuity of sleep is also dependent on the arousal level (Perlis et al.,
2001a) and the wake promoting drive (process W) (Åkerstedt & Folkard, 1997). During sleep
the homeostatic sleep drive (process S) decreases until it reaches a certain level and
wakefulness is invoked. The homeostatic and circadian processes interact to determine
neurobehavioral alertness and performance. The circadian system actively promotes
wakefulness (Edgar et al., 1993), which is experienced, for example, as the spontaneously
enhanced alertness in the early evening, even after a sleepless night (Czeisler & Khalsa,
2000).
Sleep can be disrupted spontaneously by an inner or external stimulus, which must be of
higher intensity than the sleep propensity threshold (Staner et al., 2003). The transition into
wakefulness is often characterized by short cortical events, “arousals”. The awakening
stimulus may be various factors like: time spent asleep, intrasleep architecture (NREM-REM
cycles), age, circadian phase, sleep disorders or environmental factors like heat, cold or noise
(Muzet, 2004), as well as anxiety and worries (Morin et al., 2003). Wakefulness is maintained
by a number of structures in the Reticular Activating System (RAS) (Morruzzi & Magoun,
1949) and allows sleep when it is inhibited or inactive (Shneerson, 2000). Furthermore CNS
hyperarousal (intrusion of alpha activity or increase of higher frequencies in the EEG) is
suggested to be involved in sleep disorders like insomnia (Hauri, 2000; Perlis et al., 2001a).
Thus, if the activation level (process W) is on a higher level than the sleep propensity level
(process S), sleep might be inhibited or characterized by frequently arousals.
The restorative function of sleep
The relation between sleep loss and sleepiness/fatigue was discussed above. However, the
physiology of restitution in sleep is not well understood. Below is a summary of some of the
processes that may be involved.
The effects of sleep on restoration and tissue renewal are best studied in animals. The rate of
epidermal mitosis (Bullough, 1948) and myocardial protein synthesis normally increases
during sleep (Rau & Meyer, 1975). However, several indirect indicators link sleep with
synthetic processes in man. Growing infants sleep much of the time and the 24h hormone
pattern is such that anabolic processes would be facilitated during sleep and catabolic
processes during wakefulness (Adam & Oswald, 1983). Thus, body temperature and
metabolism rise with the increased catabolism of the waking period and falls to its nadir
during the nighttime sleep (Mills et al., 1978). Lower body temperature during sleep reduces
the use of cellular energy, and thus conserve energy. The catabolic hormones, cortisol,
epinephrine and norepinephrine that inhibit protein synthesis, are secreted during
18
wakefulness, and the principal secretion of Growth Hormone, GH, testosterone, and other
anabolic hormones have their peak rates of growth and repair during sleep (Weitzman, 1975).
Recent research connects sleep to the immune system (Born, 1999). Under certain conditions
immune activation enhances SWS and intensifies NREM sleep (Mullington et al., 2000;
Pollmächer et al., 1993). On the other hand, sleep deprivation has been associated with
alterations in the expression of cytokines and declines of cellular and natural immunity (Irwin
et al., 2003; Irwin et al., 1996), indicating a health promoting meaning of sleep.
Many argue that the restorative process of sleep (especially SWS) is particularly important for
proper brain functioning (for a review see Horne, 1978). This theory is supported by findings
showing that sleep is involved in reprocessing memory traces and facilitates learning and
memory consolidation (Huber et al., 2004; Maquet, 2001), and to reach optimal performance
(Gosselin et al., 2005; Stickgold et al., 2000).
This earlier research strengthens the theories of the functions of sleep for protection and
detoxification, energy conservation (Berger & Phillips, 1995), brain cooling (McGinty &
Szymusiak, 1990) and tissue restitution (Adam & Oswald, 1977). It might be hypothesized
that if sleep in burnout is characterized by continuous disruptions and shortage of sleep the
restorative and anabolic processes will be interrupted, with detrimental effects for
performance, cognitive functioning and health in burnout.
Disturbed sleep
As reflected in the diagnostic manual for sleep disorders disturbed sleep is mainly a matter of
difficulties initiating or maintaining sleep (AASM, 2001). Below is discussed in more detail
what disturbed sleep is and what characterizes the clinical entity “Primary Insomnia”. Here
will not be discussed the two other clinical entities “Hypersomnia” (sleep apnea, narcolepsy,
etcetera) and “Parasomnia” (night mares, sleep walking etcetera) since they do not seem
relevant in relation to burnout.
Sleep initiation and maintenance
The ultimate sleep disturbance is of course if sleep fails to initiate when it should normally
occur. This is one of the most common complaints in individuals with sleep problems
(Thorpy, 1990). Normal sleep latency is around 10 minutes and wakefulness within sleep
usually accounts for less than 5% of time (Carskadon & Dement, 2000) and a sleep efficiency
(percentage of actual sleep from sleep onset to final awake) below 85% is considered as poor
(Morin, 1993).
Some other variables are also sometimes used as indicators of disturbed sleep. These include
a shortened REM latency or increased SWS latency, although clear criteria do not exist. Large
deviations from normal values of SWS and REM (around 20-25% of sleep respectively, in
young adults) may also be used to indicate disturbances as may deviations from the normal
80-90 minute REM-NREM cycle (Carskadon & Dement, 2000).
19
Sleep, in a number of sleep disorders, is punctuated by frequent, brief arousals of 3 to 15 sec
duration. These arousals are characterized by bursts of EEG alpha activity and, now and then,
transient increases in skeletal muscle tone. These arousals typically do not result in
awakenings, but recur in some conditions, like apnea, leg movement or pain, as often as 1 to 4
times per minute. Even if the arousals or awakenings do not reduce sleep time, the quality of
sleep is deteriorated since sleep may be restricted to stages 1 and 2 and REM might be
reduced or phase delayed (Carskadon & Dement, 2000) (see fig 2).
Figure 2 Fragmented sleep with frequent microarousals, decreased amount of SWS (lower) and
decreased power in delta frequency band (mV2) (upper curve).
Results from studies on sleep fragmentation have suggested that it is the continuity of sleep
that determinates its restorative function (Bonnet, 1986; Stepanski et al., 1984). However it is
difficult to isolate the manipulated effects of sleep fragmentation from effect of changes in
sleep structure, since when the fragmentation rate is high the amount of SWS will be low
(Bonnet & Arand, 2003). Studies on spontaneous sleep disruptions (micro arousals) outside
the laboratory are scarce, and are an important issue for future studies when considering effect
of work stress on sleep and performance.
As indicated above frequent sleep disruption, (or destroyed sleep continuity), strongly affects
sleepiness and daytime functioning (Carskadon & Dement, 2000). Thus, one microawakening
every 1- 4 minutes will severely impair alertness, as demonstrated in experimental studies
(Bonnet, 1986; Bonnet & Arand, 2003), in sleep apnea (Guilleminault et al., 1993; Mitler,
1993) or periodic movements during sleep (Rosenthal et al., 1984). Additionally, sleep
fragmentation has been associated with feelings of distress and “unclear thinking” (Bonnet,
1985, 1986).
20
From a subjective point of view poor sleep quality variation across many sleep episodes is
most closely related to sleep duration (or sleep efficiency) plus SWS (Åkerstedt et al., 1994;
Åkerstedt et al., 1997). However, also fragmentation measures were important as secondary
predictors. As an example, the perception of very poor sleep was given when the sleep
efficiency was down to 65%. Very good sleep quality required > 90%.
Primary Insomnia
Insomnia complaints typically includes problems with falling asleep at bedtime, waking up in
the middle of the night with difficulties going back to sleep, and awakening too early in the
morning. According to the Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) (APA, 2000) and
the International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD) (AASM, 2001) the criteria for
Primary Insomnia is defined as: difficulties initiating and/or maintaining sleep or waking up
too early and complaints of non-recuperative sleep for at least one months. To fulfill the
criteria for an insomnia diagnosis the deficient sleep also must occur several times a week and
result in subjective reports of fatigue, impaired daytime functioning, or mood disturbances.
Often used criteria are sleep latency > 30 minutes, or > 4 awakenings per night, or > 45
minute awake during the night, or > 2 hours of reduced sleep (compared to the normal)
(Morin, 1993).
The etiology of insomnia may differ and there are several main diagnostic subcategories:
Insomnia related to Depression or other mental or medical disorders, Restless legs etcetera.
The primary focus here however is Psychophysiological (AASM, 2001) or Primary Insomnia
(APA, 2000), in which emotional causes are considered to be a primary factor. This
represents about 20% of the insomnia diagnoses (Hauri, 2000; Ohayon, 2002).
Major contributors to sleep disruptions are worries and inability to escape persistent thoughts
often related to stressful life events (Healey et al., 1981). A reasonable hypothesis is that the
arousals result from emotional activation since symptoms that include tension, irritability or
mood disturbances are frequently seen in insomniacs (Hauri, 1983). A combination of
stressful events interfering with sleep and the individual’s attitudes to, and interpretation of
their sleep quality and sleep pattern has been suggested to induce insomnia (Lundh &
Broman, 2000). Primary Insomnia is hypothesized to be a disorder of hyperarousal, which has
been supported by research on the autonomic nervous system and the HPA- axis function
(Born & Fehm, 1998; Huether, 1996). Increased metabolic rate (Bonnet & Arand, 1995a),
body temperature and increased alpha activity on the EEG both awake and during sleep onset
(Freedman, 1986; Monroe, 1967) has been found in insomnia. Poor sleep has also been
associated with increased cathecholamine and cortisol excretion (Adam et al., 1986; Vgontzas
et al., 1999; Vgontzas et al., 1998).
Fatigue and impaired daytime functioning are key characteristics of Primary Insomnia,
(Morin, 1993). It would be expected that sleep loss or disturbed sleep also would produce
sleepiness. Daytime sleepiness has been evaluated in chronic insomniacs and normal sleepers
by the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) (Seidel et al., 1984), but despite sleep loss,
insomnia patients have been shown to be at least as alert as controls (Stepanski et al., 1988).
This seems paradoxical but it is likely that the “masking effect”, of high physical or mental
activity (Carskadon & Dement, 1982; Hockey, 1997), might mask the experience of the
current degree of sleepiness.
21
Thus, insomnia patients seldom report sleepiness, but instead fatigue (lack of energy) and the
impaired daytime functioning has mainly been attributed to concentration difficulties
(Broman, 1994; Hauri, 2000). In addition insomniacs commonly reveal significant elevation
of tension, depression, anger and fatigue on the Profile of Mood States (Bonnet & Arand,
1997; Hauri & Fisher, 1986; Sateia et al., 2000), and in a general population study severe
insomnia in most cases appeared before or at the same time as mood disorders symptoms, or
after anxiety disorders (Ohayon & Roth, 2003). Since both insomnia and impaired
functioning are commonly reported in burnout the need to investigate the co-morbidity of
insomnia in burnout is underscored. Also a need to explore the characteristics of the
subjective sleep complaints in burnout; whether sleep continuity, specific sleep stages or sleep
length is the major problem, and if the sleep complaints are related to sleepiness, fatigue or
both. The present thesis tries to provide such data.
Sleepiness
If sleep is involved in burnout then also sleepiness should be of interest. Interestingly no
studies on burnout and sleepiness are available. The distinction between sleepiness, tiredness
and fatigue is often blurred. It was recently debated whether tiredness and sleepiness are
different dimensions on the same continuum (Dement, 2003; Dement et al., 2003) or if they
are qualitatively distinguishable from each other (Horne, 2003). As discussed earlier, a similar
confusion exists concerning fatigue and sleepiness. However, sleepiness is defined as the
tendency to fall asleep (Carskadon & Dement, 1982) and a period of drowsiness or sleepiness
characterizes the transitional phase between wakefulness and sleep (Pivik, 1991). Since
sleepiness generally increases as a response to sleep reduction (Dement & Carskadon, 1982)
it is a useful indicator of sleep need.
Gillberg et.al (1995) have stressed that 1-3 h curtailment of sleep will induce sleepiness.
There are no studies on sleepiness in burnout, but sleep debt (< 5h) and overtime work
(weekly > 60h) have been observed as risks for exhaustion and myocardial infarction in
white-collar workers (Liu & Tanaka, 2002). A number of experimental studies (Bonnet, 1986;
Stepanski et al., 1984) and studies on shift workers (Kecklund et al., 1997), show that
sleepiness is an effect of sleep loss or extended wakefulness (for a review see, for example
Bonnet & Arand, 2003; Gillberg, 1995). A weekly recovery of 36 h off work is normally
sufficient to recover alertness whereas three days or more are needed in shift workers after
periods with severely disturbed circadian rhythmicity (Åkerstedt et al., 2000).
Sleepiness is often measured physiologically as the time taken to fall asleep while lying down
(while EEG, EOG, and EMG is recorded) (Carskadon & Dement, 1982). This constitutes the
so-called Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT). Even if it would be preferable, it is often not
feasible to use physiological methods for measuring sleepiness in many field studies or when
the overall weekly pattern is of interest. Subjective sleepiness is one alternative and is
measured using visual analogue scales (Monk, 1989) or Likert type of scales like the Stanford
Sleepiness Scale (SSS) (Hoddes et al., 1973) or the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS)
(Åkerstedt & Gillberg, 1990).
22
Sleepiness has not been studied in relation to burnout before and it is an interesting question if
sleepiness is increased considering the reported sleep disturbances (Grossi et al., 2003;
Melamed et al., 1999). On the other hand, the same studies found difficulties falling asleep
which suggests a lack of sleepiness.
Also the diurnal pattern might be of interest. Normally sleepiness follows the circadian
rhythm with relatively high levels on awakening and at bedtime and during midday (Hoddes
et al., 1973; Monk, 1989; Åkerstedt & Gillberg, 1990). But if burnout, as hypothesized,
involves a lack of recovery from sleep, it is possible that, the diurnal pattern of sleepiness
might be abnormal.
Another interesting question is whether there is any difference in sleepiness between work
days and weekends. The workday/weekend pattern of sleepiness might provide information
on the chronicity of sleepiness, or if sleepiness in burnout decreases in relation to their
weekend recovery.
One purpose of the present thesis was to provide information on the diurnal and
weekday/weekend pattern of sleepiness. Also (mental) fatigue was added to this study of
patterns since this might provide further information on the acute variation of fatigue across
the day. Since no previous data are available no particular hypothesis can be put forward,
however, other than a difference between high and low burnout groups would be expected.
Qualitative aspects
The tradition of science is dominated by the quantitative approach and so also the discussion
on burnout and sleep. Traditional quantitative data from questionnaires or diaries are usually
based on interviews with patients or others to obtain the dimensions/items of interest for the
scales. Even though these methods give valuable information on the relationships between
variables or on predicting a specific outcome, the core characteristics and nuances of a
phenomenon may be lost. Qualitative research captures experiences in life “from within” the
persons mind; thoughts, feelings, motivation and expectations, and thus provides an
understanding that is different from, but precedes, explanation which in turn is the basis of
our knowledge (Bullington, 1999; Sandelowski, 2004; Sandelowski et al., 1997).
Different qualitative approaches have been applied with success to gain insight into suffering
for example, in living with diseases, like CFS and FM (Åsbring, 2001), stroke (Nilsson et al.,
1999; Sundin & Jansson, 2003), temporarily confusion (Fagerberg & Jönhagen, 2002),
Parkinson’s disease (Sunvisson & Ekman, 2001) and HIV (Barosso & Powell-Cope, 2000).
And phenomenological studies on, for example caring, for suicidal psychiatric inpatients or
patients in the midst of dying have provided important understanding on how to deal with
stress in extreme situations (Talseth et al., 2003), and how to meet persons in crisis (Talseth et
al., 2001).
23
Even though there have been a growing number of qualitative studies in recent years, there
are few on burnout and most of them are in the field of nursing. Out of these, two studies
focus on the experience of work related stress and strategies for reducing stress and burnout
on a personal and organizational level (Farrington, 1997; Hooley, 1997). A few studies
describe antecedents to burnout. A recent study using grounded theory to investigate nurses’
experiences of the patient-nurse relation, own expectations, lack of social support and
powerlessness to influence work was found to precede burnout (Billeter-Koponen & Fredén,
2005). One phenomenological study describes the commitment to nursing and found burnout
to be a strong precursor to reduced commitment (Rinaldi, 1989). Another study on nurses’
leaving their profession did not explicitly concern burnout, but described oppression, a noncaring culture, as a main core bringing the nurses into suffering and exodus, which might lead
to burnout if they had decided to continue (Tinsley & France, 2004).
The only study on how burnout is experienced was a phenomenological study on nursing
students’ experiences of burnout in relation to one situation of overwhelming demands, time
pressure and no outlet (Beck, 1995). In this study burnout was considered as a temporary state
and not a result of prolonged stress and did not describe long-standing problems or a process.
Thus there is clearly a lack of qualitative studies on burnout especially in non-client related
professions.
In reviewing burnout literature it becomes evident that the individual and the situation
wherein the persons live their lives influences burnout development (Cherniss, 1980;
Demerouti et al., 2000; Etzion et al., 1998; Maslach et al., 2001; Schaufeli & Buunk, 1996).
Furthermore, burnout involves experiences and feelings that may not be possible to grasp with
natural science methodology. Therefore a qualitative approach would be suitable in order to
provide an understanding of the meaning of burnout and the experiences of burnout
development. Interestingly, this perspective has never been taken into account. One purpose
of the present thesis was to provide such knowledge. In particular, it is of interest to study
how the time preceding burnout and the manifestations of the state were experienced. The
philosophical basis for the phenomenological life world perspective is described below.
The phenomenological life-world perspective
To provide a background for later discussions the approach to the phenomenological life
world perspective is summarized here. In human science research the life world is essential.
The life-world is the world in which we live our daily lives, often taken for granted in
perceptions and actions. The life world precedes all knowledge in the sense that it exists prior
to any reflection upon it (Dahlberg et al., 2001; Merleau-Ponty,1945), which means that the
body responds to a situation or an event with physiological adjustments prior to any
consciousness reflection upon how or why.
It is through the lived body that humans have access to, and are related to the world. The body
acts as a whole, and it is through the bodily experiences the surroundings become meaningful
to us (Merleau-Ponty, 1995). Through the lived body the physiological and emotional
24
reactions are merged with the conscious mind, and the lived body is experienced as a unified
whole. This implies that an imbalance or dysfunction in one part affects all other parts, and
that feelings of dysfunctions or ill health may occur prior to any consciousness of psychical or
biological facts.
From a phenomenological life world perspective the relations within the human being and
between the organism and its milieu are not relations of linear causality but of circular
causality (Merleau-Ponty, 1995). A circularity between the lived body and the life world
means that if the body undergoes changes, the person’s life world is affected too, and vice
versa. And there is a circularity between the subject and object within the lived body in the
meaning that the lived body can reflect upon itself. This experience of body and soul as
inseparable entities and an ambiguous existence of subject and object, bridges the body-mind
dualism, and means that human experiences are to be viewed as more than the sum of its own
parts (Merleau-Ponty, 1995).
The life world is constituted of both material things and immaterial values like feelings,
intentions and expectations, which have implications for how a phenomenon is experienced.
Furthermore there is no way to step outside of ones own history (Merleau-Ponty, 1995), and
the implication of this is that even though man is free, people are not freely choosing all their
actions all the time; old habits, skills and expectations which have became their “way of being
in the world” (Heidegger, 1982) have to be taken into account. A person’s lived experiences
represent these aspects.
Intentionality is central to phenomenology and refers to the natural way of perceiving the
world. The consciousness is always directed towards an object, a perception or an event and
consciousness can be directed towards the subject itself in self-reflection. Through that act of
directed consciousness knowledge about oneself and the world is attended. This means that
the person is able to understand the meaning of the things that make up our every day’s lives
and that the individual is able to make an active choice of perception (Dahlberg et al., 2001;
Merleau-Ponty, 1995). This view of a self-organizational and “enactive” process of
consciousness, instead of brain areas passively reacting to mere stimulation, is supported by
neuroscience (Ellis, 1999).
From the existing research it is evident that basic conditions for burnout development are
grounded in the human life world, in material facts like circumstances, events at work and in
daily life and in immaterial senses like the person’s expectations, feelings and thoughts. Thus,
in order to grasp the lived experiences of developing burnout, a phenomenological life world
perspective is appropriate to use.
25
Summary and aim
Previous research has shown that burnout is closely related to occupational stress. Energy
depletion, excessive fatigue and emotional exhaustion are the main dimensions in burnout.
Furthermore, burnout is strongly associated with subjectively reported sleep disturbances and
fatigue. Thus, the question arises if there may be a connection between prolonged stress
exposure, impaired recovery or sleep in the burnout syndrome.
Previous studies on burnout have been focused on the psychological responses in relation to
performance and work. Research on physiological measures and burnout are scarce and
inconsistent. And to the best of my knowledge there are no studies available on burnout and
objective sleep. In the present thesis it is attempted to study two “levels” of burnout: those
with high burnout scores but still successfully employed, as well as patients on sick leave
with high burnout scores. Additionally, most of the discussion above has focused on a natural
science based methodology and thus also a qualitative understanding of the concept is needed.
Based on the review above the purpose of the present thesis is to:
• describe the burnout development from a life world perspective.
• describe the physiological characteristics of sleep in persons with burnout on sickleave and in individuals still working but at risk for developing burnout.
• relate burnout and disturbed sleep to a number of established physiological stress
indicators.
• describe the pattern of sleepiness and mental fatigue across the day and week in the
same groups, as well as the chronic pattern of fatigue in the group on sickleave.
Study I
The main purpose of this study was to describe lived experiences of the time preceding
burnout from a life world perspective in order to describe the complex interaction between the
person and the life world, as well as to describe early signs of the burnout development.
Study II
This study aimed to provide information on whether sleep complaints in patients on long-term
sick-leave due to persistent occupational stress are related to disturbed sleep architecture and
if the homeostatic processes are impaired in comparison to a control group of healthy working
individuals. It was also a purpose to investigate if the diurnal pattern of sleepiness and fatigue
differed from that of the control group on weekdays and weekend. Subjective sleep quality
and characteristics of fatigue and work were also included.
26
Study III
This study aimed to investigate physiological and subjective aspects of sleep before a
workday and a day off, in a group of young, working, apparently healthy subjects scoring
high on burnout, compared with a group scoring low on burnout. The purpose was also to
investigate the diurnal pattern of sleepiness on a workday and on a day off in the same groups,
and to describe the groups with respect to work stress, mood, recovery and burnout related
variables.
Study IV
The purpose of the study was to look for a relation between sleep fragmentation and a number
of physiological parameters related to stress, including risk factors for CVD and the metabolic
syndrome. A second purpose was to search for associations between the number of arousals
and mood, burnout, daily stress ratings, work related and other possible predictors in daily
life.
27
Methods
Design of the studies
Table 1 presents an overview of the design, sample and measures used in the studies. Study I
has a descriptive approach using qualitative interviews to reach a deeper understanding of the
experiences of burnout. Study II and III are field studies with both a within and between
groups design. The between group perspective was between groups of burnout patients and
matched controls in study II and between individuals scoring high and low on burnout in
study III. The diurnal pattern of sleepiness (II, II) and Mental Fatigue (MF) (II) across
weekday and weekend was evaluated within each group, as was the weekday and weekend
pattern of sleep, rest and mood in study II and III.
Table 1. Overview of design, sample and measures used in study I-IV.
Study
Design
Analysis
Burnout
inventory
I
Qualitative
interviews
Phenomenology
SMBQ
II
Within and
Between
groups
t-test, 2factor
ANOVA
III
Within and
Between
groups
IV
Correlational
t-test, 2factor
ANOVA
t-test,
single &
multiple
regression
analysis
SMBQ, MBI
Subjective
Objective Subjective
Other
sleepiness/
measures
sleep
measures
fatigue
-
-
-
-
PSG
Work
scoring,
KSS, MF,
KSD, KSQ
stress, BDI,
spectral
SOFI
BAI
analysis
SMBQ
PSG
KSD, KSQ
scoring,
SMBQ
PSG
scoring
blood
measures
-
KSS
Work
stress,
HAD
-
Work stress
Study IV made use of the same sample as in study III and has a correlational design,
evaluating the relationship between physiological measures of sleep, and biological stress
markers as well as possible predictors of disturbed sleep in the daily life (table 1).
28
Participants and setting
The studies are results from field studies in two different settings. The participants in study I
and II were undergoing a treatment program at the stress clinic (Stressmottagningen),
Stockholm. The population in study III- IV was recruited from an information technology
company subjected to a high workload. The control group in study II was recruited to match
the burnout group on gender, age and educational level.
Table 2 presents an overview of participants, and in table 3 the inclusions/exclusions of
participants in study II and III are presented. More details are presented in the result section.
Table 2. Number of persons invited to participate in the studies,
participants included, age and sex
Study
No invited*
No of
to participate participants
I
8
II
173
III, IV
26
* for details se table 3
8
24
24
Men
Women
Age,
Mean±SE
Age,
Range
3
6
10
5
18
14
45±3
39±2
30.5±1
30-56
26-56
24-43
Participants in study I were selected to represent both men and women in different ages,
occupational, marital and social status in order to represent as much experiences as possible. 8
individuals: 5 women and 3 men who, in accordance with the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. (DSM-IV) of the American Psychiatric Association
(APA, 2000), met the criteria for unspecified maladjustment disorder. These persons were
invited through an information letter to participate in the study. All 8 accepted, and received
verbal information before they were included in the study. All had been on sick leave more
than 3 months from their employment as white-collar workers. They also met the criteria of
high burnout score (> 4,75, graded 1-7) on the Shirom - Melamed Burnout Questionnaire,
SMBQ, (Kushnir & Melamed, 1992; Melamed et al., 1992). The sample size was large
enough to achieve a variation of experiences and small enough to permit a deep analysis of
the data (Streubert & Carpenter, 1995).
The burnout group in study II was recruited from the registers of an insurance company
serving white-collar workers and included if: 1) they had a high burnout score (> 4,75, on a
scale of 1-7) on the SMBQ, based on clinical data from the stress clinic (Perski et al., 2002),
2) they were on sickleave (fulltime) since at least three months, 3) their complaints involved
work related burnout. Lacking a formal ICD diagnosis for burnout, stress related exhaustion
(Z73.0 – ICD 10, WHO, 1992) was used as an important additional inclusion criterion. A
control group of white-collar workers was recruited through internal advertisement from the
same insurance company or through personal contacts, and were included if they 1) were fulltime white-collar workers 2) scored < 2,75 on the SMBQ scale.
In order to assess psychiatric co-morbidity, all participants were assessed by a clinical
psychiatrist, using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, (SCID) (First et al., 1997a).
29
Before the interview, participants completed questionnaires to assess previous life events
(Deykin et al., 2001) and the screening questionnaire for personality disorders (SCID-scan)
(First et al., 1997b). This was done to exclude Axis II diagnoses as defined by the DSM-IV
(APA, 2000). 65 % of the patients had a diagnosis of depression at the beginning of the illness
episode, but individuals with ongoing major depression and other Primary Axis I disorders
were also excluded from the study.
The burnout group and controls were medically examined before the study and participants
with infections or a medical history of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS), fibromyalgia,
autoimmune diseases, Cushing’s syndrome, or other chronic diseases that might possibly
explain fatigue were excluded from the study. Other exclusion criteria were: heavy snoring,
sleep apnea (as evidenced in self-reports and oxygen desaturation measurements) and use of
antidepressant medication, hypnotics, beta-receptor blockers and other medication known to
interfere with sleep.
128 individuals on sick leave were contacted, 48 did not respond and 22 declined. Among the
58 volunteers 26 were not included since they fulfilled criteria for an Axis II diagnosis or
ongoing major depression, 18 were not included since they were treated with Selective
Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors (SSRI), hypnotics or other medication interfering with sleep,
one decline after inclusion because of extensive fatigue, and one left the study for family
reasons. The 12 remaining were included in the burnout group. On the basis of the psychiatric
evaluation work related problems was the precipitating factor in all cases, 7 also reported
family stress.
Out of 45 control volunteers 14 scored above 2,75 on SMBQ scale, 8 were not included
because of snoring or sleep apnea and 11 did not match the patients on gender and/or age. The
remaining 12 individuals who met the inclusion criteria described above were included as
controls. None of the control volunteers used hypnotics or antidepressants, and none fulfilled
criteria for Axis I or Axis II diagnosis.
Table 3: Overview over the inclusion/exclusions of participnants in study II and III
Study II
Study III
Low burnout total High burnout
Burnout group
Control group
No excluded
No excluded
group
sample
group
45
invited 128
676
-48 nonresponders
-11 non matchers
-262 nonresponsers
-22 declined
-8 snoring/sleep apnea SMBQ < 2,75
SMBQ > 4,5:
12
-26 Axis II diagnosis
-14 SMBQ >2,75
12
-2 declined
-18 SSRI medicated
+2 replacers
-2 other reasons
participants =12
=12
=12
=12
In study III –IV the participants were selected from a computerized questionnaire distributed
to all employees at the company. 414 employees (out of 676) completed the questionnaires
concerning stress at work, health, sleep and life style. This included the Shirom - Melamed
Burnout Questionnaire (SMBQ) (Kushnir & Melamed, 1992; Melamed et al., 1992) modified
30
from the original range 1-7 to a 4-graded scale, with 4 being almost always. The respondents
were allocated to a high, and low, burnout group based on their total scores on the modified
SMBQ. Inclusion criteria for the high burnout group was set to >2,75 (linear transformation
equaled 4,5 on the original version). Twelve employees scoring above the upper cutoff level
of SMBQ (based on clinical data from the stress clinic) (Perski et al., 2002) were randomly
selected, of which all agreed to participate in the study. Twelve control subjects were
thereafter selected with burnout scores below the lower cut off level (< 1,5), meeting the
matching criteria: sex, age and experience in the company. If more than one individual in the
low burnout group met the matching criteria, selection between these individuals was made
randomly. Two selected subjects in the control group declined participation, and were
replaced by two others, selected from the low burnout group as described above.
Ethical issues and approval.
After recruitment the participants were informed that the participation was voluntary and that
confidentiality would be preserved. They were given verbal information about the procedures
(II & III), and all subjects gave written informed consent to participate. In the interview
situation (I) special care was taken to show an open attitude and to show sensitivity to the
participant’s vulnerability in re-living difficult experiences. The ethical committee of the
Karolinska Institutet approved all studies.
General procedure
In study I data was collected using audio-taped interviews following phenomenological
methods developed by Giorgi (1985, 1997) and Dahlberg (2001). The author conducted all
the interviews in a quiet room at the stress clinic, and they lasted 45 to 90 minutes. The
interviews were tape-recorded and transcribed verbatim. The interviews were in dialogue
form and the participants were encouraged to describe in their own words concrete lived
experiences from their whole life situation during the months preceding the acute phase of
burnout. Follow up questions in order to elucidate the experiences as richly as possible, were
for example: 1) Can you give an example of this? 2) What did you feel? 3) Please tell me
more about that?
In study II the participants were subjected to a polysomnographical recording in their home
(after habituation). Karolinska Sleep Diary (KSD) was filled out every day upon awakening
and a daytime diary, including sleepiness (Karolinska Sleepiness Scale, KSS), and mental
fatigue was completed at bedtime. Additionally, questionnaires raising questions on their
habitual sleep pattern (Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire, KSQ), mood (Beck Depression and
Anxiety Inventory, BDI, BAI), work characteristics, recovery and health were used at the
beginning of the study.
31
The participants in study III and IV were subjected to a two-week protocol with sleep diary
ratings completed upon awakening. Daytime ratings included acute sleepiness (KSS) every
two hours from awakening to bedtime. The diary also included questions about work
performance, health and stress symptoms. Two ambulatory polysomnographic (PSG)
recordings were carried out in their home; one night before a workday and one night before a
day off in a balanced design. Repeated saliva samples were collected during the day after the
PSG-recordings following a schedule described in the methods section. The morning after the
workday PSG, a fasting blood sample was collected between 8 and 9AM. This was done by
the same nurse at the participants’ work place (IV).
After the blood sample a recording of ambulatory blood pressure (ABP) was started and data
were collected during 24 h (only morning sampling reported on here). In conjunction with the
physiological assessment the participants completed a questionnaire to assess the current
sleep pattern (KSQ), health status, mood (Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale, HAD),
medications, work situation and performance (III, IV).
Measures
In table 1 (p. 28) an overview of the measurements used in the different studies are presented.
Polysomnography
Study II-IV included polysomnographically recorded sleep using portable ”Embla” recorders
(Flaga HF). Ag/AgCl electrodes were used with two electroencephalographic (EEG)
derivations C3 –A2 and C4 – A1, one chin electromyographic (EMG) derivation and two
oblique electro-oculogram derivations (EOG). The frequency response for the Embla is
between 0.5 and 100 Hz. The filter settings were 0.5 Hz for high pass filters and 75 Hz for
low pass filters.
The electrodes for the polysomnographic measurement were attached in the laboratory
between 4 and 6AM in study II whereas the electrodes were attached in the participants home
approximately two hours before bedtime in study III and IV.
Sleep stages were scored visually in 30-second epochs according to Rechtschaffen and Kales
(1968). Sleep scoring was blind to group membership. Arousals were scored using the
American Sleep Disorders Association criteria (ASDA, 1992). An arousal was defined as an
EEG shift to at least alpha activity from stages 2-4 or Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep,
preceded by at least 10 seconds of uninterrupted sleep (stages 2-4 or REM). During REM
sleep an increase in EMG-activity was required. For an arousal to be scored it had to last for
more than 3 seconds and for less than 15 sec. Sleep onset latency was scored as time from
‘eyes closed’ to the first epoch of at least three consecutive sleep epochs (Stage 1 or other
sleep stages).
32
Computer-aided methods -spectral analysis
Also spectral analysis (Fast Fourier Transform, FFT) of the EEG was carried out (study II).
The EEG was sampled at a frequency of 100 Hz using conventional filter settings (band pass
filter set to 0.5 -32 Hz). The FFT analysis was based on four-second epochs, and after visual
artifact detection the entire 4-second "sub-epoch" was removed from analysis, and substituted
by the mean of the immediately adjacent epochs. Values were then integrated across each
band and averaged across 1-min intervals of sleep. The bands used were: 0.5-4.5 Hz (delta),
4.51 -7.5 Hz (theta), 7.51 – 12.5 Hz (alpha), 12.51-16.50 (sigma) and 16.51-32.5 Hz (beta).
Physiological measures
Measures of the physiological stress response included different neuroendocrine markers
reflecting the activity within the SAM-system and the HPA-axis respectively (Cox, 1985;
King & Hegadoren, 2003; Seeman et al., 2001). It is known that concomitant with a longdrawn-out activity in the HPA-axis the energy mobilization of glucose and lipids is increased,
with a risk of insulin resistance and other aspects of the metabolic syndrome as well as CVD
(Björntorp, 2002; Peters et al., 2004). Since the metabolic and cardiovascular risk factors are
associated with work stress (Schnall et al., 1998; Theorell & Karasek, 1996) as well as
disturbed sleep (Nilsson et al., 2001; Nilsson et al., 2004) the physiological markers in study
IV were chosen based on those findings. The variables used are briefly presented below.
Blood pressure and heart rate. The acute neural stress response from the Central Nervous
System (CNS) reflected in catecholamine (e.g. norepinephrine and epinephrine) release into
the blood are short-lived and difficult to measure, but the cardiovascular changes with a rise
in blood pressure and heart rate have been used as markers of the intensity of stress related
arousal in several studies (Chrousos, 1998a, 1998b; Folkow, 1997). For the ABP, an
UltraliteTM monitor (90217, Spacelabs medical) was used. The cuff was attached to the
nondominant arm, and the recorder was programmed to operate every hour during waking
hours and every second hour during nighttime sleep (between 12PM and 5AM).
Cortisol is one of the most commonly used biological markers of stress in bio-psycho-social
research, showed to be involved in the final common pathway of the stress response (Wust et
al., 2000). The cortisol response is an integral part of the HPA- activity and receptors are
found in all major organs and bodily tissues (Folkow, 1997). Cortisol release facilitates
energy mobilization but also alters the responsiveness of the immune system. Cortisol was
measured in plasma and in saliva and those measures are normally highly intercorrelated
(King & Hegadoren, 2003). The morning sampling of salivary cortisol followed a schedule
used in previous studies (Pruessner et al., 1997): immediately after awakening, and 15, 30,
and 60 minutes thereafter. This was followed by five additional samples: at 11AM, 3PM, 7PM,
9PM, and at bedtime. For statistical analysis, the awakening value (at 7AM ±1 hour) was used,
as well as the mean values of all four morning samples.
Lipids. In response to acute stress and during prolonged periods of stress, the activation of the
neuroendocrine systems and the subsequent secretion of hormones contribute to the
mobilization of free fatty acids in the circulating blood. The free fatty acids in turn, stimulate
33
the production and release of lipoprotein including cholesterol and triglycerides (Brindley et
al., 1993; Stoney & Finney, 2000).
Triglycerides are produced in the liver and in adipose tissue and are used as energy by the
body. Disturbances in protein synthesis or elimination may lead to increased levels of
triglycerides and subsequently may result in metabolic disturbances, insulin resistance
(Brunner et al., 1997), and increased risk for heart diseases (Carlson & Åberg, 1985).
Elevated levels have been associated with psychosocial arousal (Dimsdale & Herd, 1982)
overload and burnout (Shirom et al., 1997).
Cholesterol is transported in plasma by lipoproteins that consist of proteins, triglycerides and
phospholipids. The main types are the High Density Lipoprotein-Cholesterol, HDL-C, and the
Low Density Lipoprotein-Cholesterol, LDL-C. Low levels of HDL-C with concomitant
elevated levels of LDL-C constitute a risk factor for cardiovascular diseases. Psychosocial
factors have been shown to decrease the HDL-C levels and increase the LDL-C levels and
total cholesterol (Brindley et al., 1993).
Glucose is the main energy source, and the brain is almost exclusively dependent on the
metabolization of glucose. Sleep plays an important role in energy balance and modulates
leptin and carbohydrate regulation (Spiegel et al.), while increased cortisol release in response
to stress stimulates glucose mobilization and lipolysis (Rosmond, 2005).
Insulin (and leptin) are involved in the stimulatory feedback signals in CNS and the LHPAsystem involved in the brain’s prioritization of its own ATP concentration and the allocation
of energy recourses for the rest of the body (Peters et al., 2004). A dysregulation of insulin in
response to stress is one factor involved in the metabolic and cardiovascular diseases
(Høieggen et al., 1998; Kissebah & Krakower, 1994).
Antropometric measures. The Waist to Hip circumference Ratio (WHR) has in a number of
studies been observed as a powerful predictor of CVD, stroke, non-insulin dependent diabetes
(Björntorp, 1993). The Body Mass Index (BMI) is a more generalized measure of body mass
and is often assessed in combination with the WHR. It is hypothesized that increased WHR is
a symptom of chronic hypothalamic arousal as a defeat reaction to exposure to psychosocial
stress (Björntorp, 1991). Taken together, accumulation of visceral fat cells will generate high
levels of free fatty acids, which subsequently may lead to CVD and a state of insulin
resistance and the metabolic syndrome (Björntorp, 1991, 2002; Ottoson et al., 1994). Waist
circumference was measured at the narrowest part around the waistline. Hip circumference
was measured at the widest point between umbilicus and thighs. All measurements were taken
to the nearest 0.5 cm. BMI was calculated as weight (kg) / height (m2) and WHR was
calculated as waist (cm) / hip (cm) (Alberti & Zimmet, 1998).
Laboratory analysis
Serum was separated and frozen within two hours. All blood samples from each participant
were analyzed in the same assay. This sample was analyzed for levels of serum glucose, total
cholesterol, and triglycerides by solid-phase chemistry (Vitros, Johnsson & Johnsson). The
34
average total coefficient of variation was less than 6% for plasma glucose and 4.7% for
cholesterol and triglycerides. LDL-C and HDL-C were measured by colorimetry (Hitachi 911,
Roche) with a total coefficient of variation of less than 6%. Serum insulin and cortisol were
measured by immunofluorescence (Auto Delfia, Wallac), with a limit of sensitivity of 20
pmol/l and a total coefficient of variation of 15% for insulin and 3.5% for cortisol.
Cortisol in saliva samples (using Salivette®, Sarstedt; Rommelsdorf, Germany) was
determined through radioimmunoassay (Orion Diagnostica, Finland). The lower limit of
sensitivity was 20 nmol/L in plasma and 1 nmol/L in saliva and the average inter- and intraassay coefficient of variation never exceeded 10%.
Sleep diary
Subjective sleep quality was measured in study II to IV using the Karolinska Sleep Diary,
KSD (Åkerstedt et al., 1994; Åkerstedt et al., 1997). The KSD was filled in every day upon
awakening (Appendix II). It contained questions about “bed times”, “rise times”, “sleep
latency”, “sleep quality”, “ease of falling asleep”, “calm sleep”, “sleep throughout the allotted
time”, “number of awakenings”, “ease awakening”, “sufficient sleep” and “well rested”. A
Sleep Quality Index (SQI) was used consisting of the following items: “sleep quality?” “ease
falling asleep”, “calmness of sleep” and “sleep throughout”. The response alternatives ranged
from 1 (great problems/very poor) to 5 (no problems at all/very good).
Also questions about activation at bedtime and “sleep anticipation” was used, range 1=very
relaxed to 9=very activated/very good (III) (see Appendix II).
Wake diary
Daytime ratings included sleepiness (II and III) and mental fatigue (II) and were carried out
at awakening (around 7AM) and approximately at 10AM, 2PM, 8PM and at bedtime. Sleepiness
was rated on the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) (Åkerstedt & Gillberg, 1990) ranging
from 1 to 9 (very alert-extremely sleepy, fighting sleep, an effort to remain awake). It has
been validated against electrophysiological indices of sleepiness (Åkerstedt & Gillberg,
1990). Mental fatigue was rated on a 9-point scale ranging from 1=very fresh to 9= totally
exhausted, with “5” meaning neither fresh, nor exhausted (see Appendix III-IV).
A second part of the wake diary included items describing “time pressure”, “sufficient rest”,
“thoughts about work during leisure”, “mood” and “global health” (III). (Appendix III-IV)
Questionnaires and indices
Burnout
Burnout was measured in all studies using the Shirom – Melamed Burnout Questionnaire,
SMBQ (Melamed et al., 1992; Shirom, 1989). It consists of a list of 22 symptom sentences
that measure different aspects of energetic exhaustion. Each item was scored on a seven-point
35
scale graded from 1=almost never to 7=almost always (see Apendix V). The main subscale:
emotional exhaustion and physical fatigue was formed from 8 items, the tension and
listlessness subscales consisted of 4 item each and the cognitive weariness subscale of 6
items. A total index with the four subscales was calculated for each participant with a
reliability coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha) of 0.90. This index correlates highly with the
emotional exhaustion subscale of Maslachs Burnout Inventory, MBI (Maslach et al., 1996)
and with Pines Burnout measure (Pines et al., 1981) in a recent study of burnout women
(Grossi et al., 2003).
In addition, the general version of MBI- General Survey, MBI-GS (Maslach et al., 1996) was
used in study II. The MBI-GS assesses the same three general aspects as the original measure
(emotional exhaustion, depersonalization and reduced personal accomplishment), using
slightly revised items, and the suitable labels are; emotional exhaustion, cynicism (an
alienated attitude towards one’s job) and professional efficacy (Maslach et al., 1996, p.20-21).
The subscales are different from the original version in that they are generic, without
emphasis on emotions and without direct reference to service recipients. The inventory
consists of 16 items scored on a 7-point scale from 0=never to 6=always.
Sleep
Habitual sleep quality was assessed with the Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire (KSQ)
(Åkerstedt et al., 2002). From this was derived the Sleep Quality Index (SQI) which includes:
initiation and maintenance of sleep, with the items; “sleep quality”, “calmness of sleep”,
“ease of falling asleep” and “sleep throughout the allotted time”. The response alternatives
were 6= always/almost every day, 5=very often/> 4 days per week, 4=mostly/several days per
week, 3=sometimes/several times per month, 2=seldom/a few times per year and 1=never in
study II and varied from 1never to 5 always, in study III (see Appendix I). The SQI has been
validated against polysomnography and show good correlations with objective sleep
parameters (Kecklund & Åkerstedt, 1997). A Sleep Sufficiency Index (SSI = sleep
time/habitual sleep need x 100) was calculated from the habitual sleep need.
Depression and anxiety
The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961), and the Beck Anxiety Inventory
(BAI) (Beck et al., 1988) were used to assess depression and anxiety in study II. Those
inventories are developed to discriminate groups with anxiety and depression in clinical
research in psychiatric populations.
The Hospital Anxiety (HAD-A) and Depression scale (HAD-D) (Zigmond & Snaith, 1983)
was used in study III. HAD has been tested and evaluated in different groups as a useful
instrument because of its brevity, simplicity and lack of effect of somatic conditions
(Hopwood et al., 1991; Zigmond & Snaith, 1983). It shows strong correlations with the BDI
and Spielberger’s state Trait Anxiety Inventory in a Swedish sample (Lisspers et al., 1997).
36
Health and mood
The health and mood assessment (IV) consisted of 22 items, used in the clinical work at the
stress clinic. The checklist is similar to the Symptom Checlist-90 (Derogatis, 1977) and the
General Health Questionnaire (Goldberg & Hillier, 1979) and concern to what extent
complaints like, irritability, pain, worries etcetera had occurred during the last three months.
The response alternatives varied from 1 = never to 5 = always/almost every day. The 22 items
were factor analyzed (n=414, varimax rotation) for purposes of data reduction (unpublished).
The orthogonal factor solution yielded five factors with an eigenvalue above one: “cognitive
symptoms” (5 item), “ache/pain” (4 item), “tension” (3item), “passivity” (3 item) and
“fatigue” (3 item). The remaining four items had a factor loading below 0.4 and were not
accepted. The indices: “tension” and “ache/pain” were used in study IV. The remaining
indices overlap with SMBQ and were not suitable for inclusion in the analysis. The tension
index consisted of “irritability”, “anger” and “tension/restlessness” (Cronbach’s α =0.85),
and the ache/pain index included the items: “pain from neck and shoulders”, “upper back
pain”, “lower back pain”, and “aching hands and joints” (Cronbach’s α =0.88) see appendix I.
Occupational fatigue
To differentiate specific dimensions of fatigue related to work in burnout (study II) we
selected the Swedish Occupational Fatigue Inventory (SOFI), which has been used and
validated in different occupational settings (Åhsberg, 2000). SOFI consists of 25 expressions
with a response scale ranging from 0 to 6, verbally defined at the endpoints: 0 = not at all and
6 = to a very high degree. The 25 items form five dimensions: “lack of energy”, which
indicates a general character, “physical exertion”, “physical discomfort”, ”lack of motivation”
and “sleepiness”, consisting of four item each.
Work stress
Work characteristics were assessed in the questionnaire and consisted of psychosocial work
factors, inspired by the two dominating models concerning job stress over recent years: the
demand-control-support model (Karasek, 1979; Toomingas et al., 1997) and the effort-reward
model (Siegrist, 1996). The original demand-control-support model deals primarily with work
content that can be changed by means of organizational redesign. The effort reward imbalance
model deals with the framework around the job situation, such as the balance between effort
and reward (money, esteem, job security). The items varied partly between studies to suit
different conditions. The questionnaire was generally shorter in study II since the participants
suffered from difficulties to concentrate on paperwork. In study II two indices were formed;
demands at work (4 items, Cronbach’s α = 0.85) and decision latitude (3 items, Cronbach’s α
= 0.76) (see Appendix I). The participants were asked to remember the work conditions six
moths before sick leave. The response alternatives were: 1 = totally disagree to 4 =totally
agree. Psychosocial work factors in study III and IV derives from a factor analysis of the
whole sample (n=414) (unpublished), and included job demands (10 items, Cronbach’s α
=0.85), control (i.e. decision latitude, 3 items, Cronbach’s α =0.88), support from managers
(4 items, Cronbach’s α =0.86), support from colleagues (5 items, Cronbach’s α =0.83). Also
an index concerning thoughts about work during leisure time (3 items, Cronbach’s α =0.82),
and single items like: “work interferes with leisure time”, “working hours per week”, “time
37
pressure”, and frequency of “bringing work home” were used to various extent in study IIIIV. The response alternatives were; 1 (totally disagree) to 4 (totally agree), except for
“bringing work home”, the scale of which ranged from 1 = never to 5 = always/almost every
day (see appendix I).
Statistics and analysis
Phenomenological analysis - study I
A phenomenological life world approach based on Husserl’s (1998) phenomenology was used
in study I to describe the lived experiences of time preceding burnout. The phenomenological
method implies a rigorous, step-by-step analysis of concrete life descriptions. In order to stay
faithful to data and avoid being biased during the analysis the researcher has to set aside any
pre-understanding or taken for granted assumptions of the phenomenon: bracketing (Giorgi,
1997).
The analysis has four steps:
1. Naive reading in order to grasp a sense of the whole.
2. Discrimination of meaning units within the chosen perspective, focusing on the
phenomenon under study.
3. Every meaning unit was reflected on with free imaginative variation and transformed
into a statement expressing its most invariant meaning - a careful transformation of
each meaning unit into everyday language in an attempt to shed light on, and better
understand the participants’ lived experience of the phenomenon.
4. When all meaning units had been transformed and in reflecting on the variations of
meanings, the analysis brought out an essence of the phenomenon and the core that
did not vary between the individual’s unique meanings emerged (Dahlberg et al.,
2001; Giorgi, 1985, 1997).
The insights reached were synthesized and integrated into a descriptive structure containing
eight closely interrelated constituents. The general structures emerging from the analysis
illuminates the essential meaning of the phenomenon “the lived experiences during the time
preceding burnout”.
Statistics – study II-IV
Differences between groups were analyzed using t tests or χ2 tests (II -IV). For measures
within groups and for analysis between groups for several days or across days an Analysis of
Variance (ANOVA) for repeated-measures was applied. The latter included correction for
violation of the assumptions of equal variances and covariances (Winer, 1971). The between
group factor used in the analysis was group (burnout patients/high burnout group and healthy
controls/low burnout group) and the within group factors were: time of day (awakening to
bedtime) and day (weekday/workday and weekends/days off) (II, III). When appropriate, and
after a significant main effect, pairwise t-tests were carried out to investigate the simple
38
effects. (II, III). Background variables were included as covariates to rule out their influence
on the dependent variables.
Correlations (Pearson’s r) were calculated to describe the magnitude and direction of a
relationship between variables (II-IV) and also to assess the stability of different measures
(i.e. cortisol in blood and saliva in study IV, between MBQ and SMBQ study II or test –retest
reliability of the SMBQ in study III). A set of stepwise regression analyses were carried out
in order to understand the relation between disturbed sleep and the physiological parameters,
and also to find the best predictor of high frequency of arousal (IV) (Winer, 1971).
Variables with a skewness >± 2.0 were log transformed (IV). The internal consistency of the
indices used in the analysis (II-IV) was tested by computing the alpha coefficient (Cronbach,
1951). An alpha level of 0.05 was considered to be of statistical significance. All values in the
analyses are expressed as mean ± SE. All calculations were carried out using Statview
software (version 5.0.1; SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC) and SPSS 10.0 (11.0 in study II) for
Macintosh (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Ill.).
39
Summary of studies
In the following the major findings from each study are presented briefly.
Lived experiences of the time preceding burnout (study I)
This study aimed to elucidate lived experiences of the time preceding burnout and identify
early signs of burnout in the development of burnout. The study has a phenomenological life
world approach and the analysis followed methods developed by Giorgi and Dahlberg
(Dahlberg et al., 2001; Giorgi, 1997).
The essential meaning of the time preceding the manifestation of burnout was experienced as
being trapped in an ambiguous struggle to live up to the never-ending demands within and
outside working life on the one hand and to the stimulating challenges as a self-nourishing
drive on the other. By gradually cutting off everything that interfered with their focus on the
“struggle”, like personal needs of regular meals, physical training, social activities and time
for sleep and rest, a variety of bodily and psychological manifestations emerged. Symptoms
like pain, frequent infections, emotional instability, sleep disturbances, cognitive impairment
appeared, with an overwhelming fatigue as a common feature. The leisure time became either
consumed by illness and fatigue, or by an inability to unwind that made it hard to sleep and
recuperate.
In focusing even harder the “cutting off” became a barrier that prevented help from others and
they became alienated from their own feelings and from others. The physiological and
psychological manifestations progressed and the participants described how a terrifying
feeling emerged of being trapped in a vicious circle without means to struggle against it. In
that stage they were too drained of energy to be able to change the situation.
The description of the time preceding burnout shows that the participants came to critical
points and events that were crucial for the outcome. Acceptance of, and reconciliation with
the situation, was what could stop the struggle and that marked the turn of the tide. From there
a new reconstruction of life could emerge.
The essential meaning of the time preceding burnout was illuminated by eight constituents;
“inner incentive”, “feeling responsible”, “threatened self-image”, “cutting off”, “bodily
manifestations”, “psychological manifestations”, “fatigue’ and “reaching the bottom line”
(see fig 3).
40
Figure 3. General structure of lived experiences of the time preceding burnout.
The results show that there is a need for better understanding of the early signs in order to
prevent the damaging effects of burnout at an early stage. Furthermore, the study suggests that
a better understanding of how to reach behind the defense of “cutting off” is an essential skill
for nurses, healthcare professionals and others encountering the burnout sufferers.
Sleep, sleepiness and fatigue in burnout patients (study II)
This study recorded polysomnography in white-collar workers with a high level of burnout.
Sleep and the time of day pattern of sleepiness and mental fatigue on weekdays and weekend
were described, and precipitating factors and different dimensions of occupational fatigue
were also measured. 12 patients on long-term sick leave (> 90 days) because of occupational
burnout, and a control group with 12 healthy fulltime white-collar workers participated.
The PSG findings showed that sleep was impaired in burnout patients compared to controls
for: sleep efficiency (lower), SWS (lower), stage 1-sleep (higher), arousals (higher), and sleep
fragmentation index (# arousals + # awakenings to stage 0 and stage1) was higher in the
burnout group. But also REM sleep (lower), Wake time After Sleep Onset, WASO (higher)
and SWA (lower) showed that sleep was affected, compared with healthy controls. TST,
bedtime and rise time, sleep latency, SWS latency, REM sleep latency, number of awakenings
and stage 2-sleep did not differ between groups.
This subjective perception of sleep was characterized by significantly more awakenings,
lower sleep quality, and less sufficient sleep in the burnout group. The groups did not differ
on TST, bed times or rise times. Most variables showed a clear weekend effect – later sleep
hours being associated with longer and more sufficient sleep. Sleep quality and number of
awakenings were not affected, however. No interactions were seen.
Results from the 2-way ANOVA for repeated measures showed a significant main effect for
sleepiness with respect to group, day and time of day. The burnout group was sleepier than
41
the controls and there was a U-shaped diurnal pattern with the nadir at midday. Several
interaction effects were seen, indicating that the difference between the groups was larger
during the weekend and the diurnal pattern was shallower in the burnout group. Post-hoc
comparisons between groups showed higher sleepiness for the burnout groups for each point
in time, both weekdays and weekends, except for the workweek afternoon when the controls
were as sleepy as the burnout group (fig 4).
KSS, 1very alert -9 extremely sleepy
9
weekday
weekend
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
Mental fatigue, 1 fresh - 9 exhausted
The main finding from the within group ANOVAs on the different days in each group showed
that the burnout group was as sleepy on weekends as on weekdays, while the healthy controls
perceived more alertness during days off compared to weekdays.
9
weekend
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
n.s.
aw 10 15 20 bt
aw 10 15 20 bt
Time of day
weekday
1
n.s.
aw 10 15 20 bt
aw 10 15 20 bt
Time of day
Figure 4. Mean±SE for rated sleepiness and mental fatigue at different points of day and for a day
of work and a day off. Dashed line indicates significant pairwise differences.
The same analysis for mental fatigue (fig 4) showed a significant main effect of group, with
higher levels of mental fatigue at virtually all points in time (except for weekday evenings) in
the burnout group compared to controls. The u-shaped time of day pattern in both groups
indicated higher levels of mental fatigue in the mornings and evenings but the diurnal pattern
was shallower in the burnout group. The within group ANOVAs showed no significant effects
except for time of day, suggesting a consistent level of higher mental fatigue across weekdays
in both groups.
The total SMBQ-score for the whole sample was positively correlated with the total MBI-GI
score showing that the selection criteria are consistent with the burnout measure most used in
research. Among the burnout patients 7 (58%) met the criteria for insomnia according to
DSM-IV. The BDI and BAI-scores confirmed that the burnout group contained elements of
depression and anxiety even though patients with an ongoing major depression were not
included in the study.
42
The work situation for burnout patients before sick leave was characterized by higher work
stress and more interference with leisure time. The burnout group also had more difficulties
unwinding after work. The five dimensions of fatigue measures on the SOFI, were all
markedly elevated in the burnout group compared with the controls indicating a general lack
of energy without any particular type of fatigue dominating.
This study clearly showed that burnout patients were characterized by impaired sleep and
alertness.
Sleep and sleepiness in young individuals with high and low
burnout scores (study III)
This study sought to investigate PSG measured sleep in persons at risk of developing burnout
that is, with high burnout scores but still successfully working. The aim was also to study the
diurnal pattern of sleepiness, as well as self-rated work stress and mood in groups with
different burnout scores. Sleep was measured in the participants own home one night before
work and one night before a day off. Diary ratings of sleep, sleepiness and work performance
were carried out during two weeks.
The main result from the PSG measures in the high vs low burnout group was a significant
main effects of group with respect to the total number of arousals and number of arousals per
hour, which both were higher for the high burnout group. Sleep efficiency tended to be poorer
in the high burnout group, but did not reach statistical significance. Significant effects of day
were found for bedtime, rise time, total sleep time, stage 2 sleep and REM-sleep, which
showed that both groups went to bed later before the day off, rose earlier and slept shorter
before the workday. Both groups showed more stage 2 and REM-sleep before the day off. No
significant interaction effects were found.
The sleep diary ratings for the two PSG nights showed a significant difference between
groups in: sleepiness (KSS) at rise time, impaired awakening, and sufficient sleep, with
poorer values in the high burnout group. There was an interaction effect in KSS at rise time
and impaired awakening, indicating that the high burnout group did not feel as refreshed after
sleep as the low burnout group on days off. The diary ratings showed that perceived sleep
quality, wake time after sleep onset and number of awakenings did not differ between groups,
or between weekday/day off.
The main findings from the diurnal pattern of sleepiness on the workday and on the day off in
high vs low burnout groups was a significant interaction effect (day x group) indicating that
the groups were equally sleepy during workdays, but differed during the days off, with higher
sleepiness in the high-burnout group in the morning and afternoon.
Questionnaire data showed that the groups did not differ in perceived support from managers
or co-workers, decision latitude or working hours. But those with high burnout scores felt
43
higher demands at work, worked more at weekends and during leisure time, and described
more frequently that they brought work home than did the low burnout scorers. The work
characteristics were confirmed in the diary ratings. The high burnout group had more thoughts
about work during leisure time than the low-burnout group and felt less satisfaction with work
performance, even though the groups did not differ in time pressure, work pace, work load or
working hours.
This study shows a moderate sleep fragmentation and impaired recovery on days off in
individuals with high burnout scores. Possibly this could be an early sign of risk for
developing burnout.
Arousals from sleep and physiological correlates (study IV)
This study sought to investigate if the burnout and sleep parameters, mainly arousals would
be correlated to physiological markers that in the long run could have adverse effects on
health. Also the correlation between fragmentation and work stress, real life stress and mood
was tested.
A stepwise regression analysis using the main sleep parameters (frequency of arousals, TST
and sleep efficiency) and burnout groups as predictors brought out number of arousals as the
best predictor of morning cortisol (serum & saliva), heart rate, systolic and diastolic blood
pressure, total cholesterol, HDL-, LDL-Cholesterol and LDL/HDL-ratio. In addition to the
number of arousals, TST was positively related to the LDL/HDL-ratio and negatively to
cortisol in saliva. For the latter also the burnout group entered the regression (higher cortisol
in burnout subjects). Sleep efficiency was negatively related to heart rate.
Burnout group and gender were tested in the same analysis and gender was the only predictor
of WHR (higher in women) and was accompanied by burnout group and sleep efficiency as
significant predictor of higher HDL-C. No relations were found between fragmentation and
any of the other predictors and triglycerides, P-glucose, or the insulin/glucose ratio.
Work related stress (“bringing work home”) and tension index was the best predictor of
arousals among the subjective stress variables tested. Those variables were closely correlated
to each other and to burnout. Neither of the control variables tested – ache/pain index,
snoring, having small children, anxiety, or depression - entered any of the stepwise multiple
regression analyses,
In summary, this study demonstrated that a subtle sleep fragmentation in otherwise normal
sleep was related to physiological changes that in the long run may affect health. Also
“unclear boundaries between work and leisure time” and “tension/irritability” were associated
with the sleep fragmentation, suggesting that inability to unwind the stress system may be a
mediating factor.
44
Discussion
Introduction
Before discussing the results a summary of the main findings is presented below. Then the
discussion will concern sleep in burnout, which was the main objective of the investigation.
Thereafter the findings on stress physiology, sleepiness and fatigue are discussed, and the
process of burning out will be discussed from a life world perspective. After methodological
considerations and a tentative integration of the results, implications for further research will
be discussed.
The main findings of this thesis are:
In individuals on sickleave for burnout/stress related exhaustion:
• Experiences of time preceding burnout were described as being trapped in a pitfall
where the stimulating challenges had become a self-nourishing drive to protect a
threatened self-image and balancing never-ending demands. “Cutting off” recovery
led to increasing amounts of physiological, cognitive and emotional symptoms.
Acceptance of, and reconciliation with, the situation, was the turning point.
• Sleep was impaired on all essential sleep variables in the burnout group, indicating an
impaired recuperation. The subjective ratings of sleep were in agreement with the PSG
recordings.
• The diurnal pattern of sleepiness was different in weekends and weekdays in burnout
group and controls. The compensatory restorative effect of prolonged sleep during
days off had an impact on alertness in controls but not in the burnout group.
• Work stress was clearly pronounced in the burnout group, supporting its key role in
the development of burnout.
• The fatigue was generalized and of an unspecific character and extended across
physical and mental dimensions.
In persons at risk for developing burnout:
• PSG recorded sleep in young individuals, were characterized by a higher frequency of
arousals compared to those with low burnout scores.
• The sleep fragmentation, rather than burnout scores, was associated with elevated
levels of cortisol, blood pressure, heart rate and lipids, suggesting a link between
disturbed sleep and metabolic and cardiovascular risk indicators.
• Arousals from sleep were related to workload and stress/tension.
• The sustained sleepiness pattern on workdays and weekend suggests an early sign of
impaired recovery from sleep during days off.
• Working more at weekends and during leisure time, bringing work home more
frequently, combined with more thoughts about work at leisure, indicate a prolonged
work load and wound-up stress system in the high burnout group.
45
In order to avoid confusion about the study groups, the individuals at sick leave for
burnout/stress related exhaustion in study II are referred to as burnout patients, and the young
group scoring high on burnout in study III and IV is referred to as pre-burnouts. When the
groups are discussed together they will be labeled burnout groups. And the healthy controls
(II) and low burnout group (III) will be labeled control groups when they are discussed
together.
Sleep
Burnout and sleep
Sleep problems was a common theme in the descriptions of the time preceding burnout and
sleep was voluntary curtailed in order to cope with work demands (I). It was also obvious in
the sleep diaries of the burnout groups in both sleep studies (II, III) as well as in the
questionnaires, similar to other questionnaire observations by Melamed (1999), Grossi (2003)
and coworkers.
The two sleep studies in this thesis provide support for the subjective complaints of disturbed
sleep in burnout. The polysmomnographical findings showed that sleep in young individuals
at risk of developing burnout (III) were characterized by a higher frequency of arousals
compared to colleagues with low burnout scores. The level was higher than the cutoff point
used for diagnosing sleep apnea (Guilleminault et al., 1993), which suggests that the effects
are not marginal. Since a higher frequency of arousals was significant for both recordings
(one night in-between two workdays and one night at the end of the working week – after a
workday and before a day off) the result seems relatively robust.
The burnout patients (II) showed an even stronger sleep impairment. Thus, beyond arousals,
they also showed fragmentation in terms of real awakenings, as well as more stage 1 sleep
and wake time after sleep onset. Sleep in the group of burnout individuals on sickleave also
contained a lower sleep efficiency, less SWS and REM sleep and a lower delta power density
than in controls (II). There are no other PSG studies on burnout available to compare with
but the polysomnograhy is at least as impaired (with less SWS, REM and reduced SWA) as
in insomnia, even though the subjective perception was similar (AASM, 2001; Morin et al.,
1993).
Vital Exhaustion (VE) may be a state similar to burnout and in the only sleep study available
it was demonstrated a reduction in SWS similar to that in the present burnout group (van
Diest & Appels, 1994). Patients with Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) appear to exhibit
reduced sleep efficiency, comparable to what was found in the burnout group, but those
patients tended to extend their sleep (Sharpley et al., 1997). Taken together the two studies
suggest that burnout is associated with impaired sleep.
46
Sleep and stress
The two sleep studies are descriptive and do not provide any clear explanation of the reason
for the sleep disturbances. However, long-term stress seems to be the most likely cause. Thus,
the burnout groups reported increased levels of work stress in the questionnaires (II, III),
mainly as high work demands, as well as reports of not being able to avoid thinking of work.
Furthermore, in the interview study the participants described how they began to more or less
voluntary shorten their sleep due to work demands. To this was added frequent awakenings at
night or in the early morning (I). The process also involved alternating states of
sleepiness/fatigue and of difficulties sleeping.
The notion of stress as a cause of disturbed sleep in the pre-burnout group is also supported
by study IV, which showed that a high frequency of arousals was related to higher subjective
tension/irritability, which is a common correlate of stress (Chrousos & Gold, 1992), as well as
a predictor of disturbed sleep (Healey et al., 1981; Rosa & Bonnet, 2000). Thus, one might
hypothesize that a reduced capacity to relax mentally because of stress or tension might be a
contributor to the sleep disturbances seen in this thesis since “persistent thoughts” and an
inability to unwind were found in both burnout groups (II, III). This is consistent with
findings of a stronger tendency to experience stress-related intrusive thoughts being
associated with greater sleep complaints (Hall et al., 2000).
Incidentally, also the diagnosis Primary Insomnia is closely linked to long term exposure to
stress (Freudenberger, 1983; Hauri, 2000; Morin, 1993) and the established etiology is clearly
stress (Healey et al., 1981; Morin et al., 2003). Primary Insomnia is also associated with
physiological and CSN mediated hyper arousal (Adam et al., 1986; Bonnet & Arand, 1998;
Perlis et al., 2001b; Vgontzas et al., 2001), and impaired daytime functioning and fatigue are
part of the diagnostic criteria of insomnia (AASM, 2001; APA, 2000). In the study on burnout
patients (II) it was estimated that about 60% fulfilled the criteria for insomnia according to
DSM-IV (APA, 2000) and the ICSD (AASM, 2001). This raises the question of the relation
between burnout and Primary Insomnia.
Stress physiology and sleep
The study on the groups with high and low burnout scores (IV) brought out arousals from
sleep as the best predictor of a number of cardiovascular and metabolic risk factors. The
arousals were associated with increased heart rate and blood pressure (i.e. increased CNS
activation), as well as increased levels of cortisol, and lipids (IV). This indicates a link to the
HPA-axis and the metabolic system. The findings are consistent with studies showing that
sleep fragmentation is associated with an increased metabolic rate, and cortisol (and
catecholamines) is usually increased by arousals from sleep (Spiegel et al., 1999; SpäthSchwalbe et al., 1991).
As already mentioned, the link with stress was brought out in the prediction of arousals by
tension index (IV). This may be interpreted as an indication of not feeling well. Possibly, this
indicates a “pre-phase” of burnout related to what was reported in the interview study that
“something feels very wrong” (I), which was experienced before any consciousness of illness
47
appeared. Thus, illness seemed to be experienced at a prereflective level before any explicit
manifestation of measurable biological facts (Husserl, 1970; Merleau-Ponty, 1995). This is in
concordance with a cluster of illness with subjective health complaints (Ursin & Eriksen,
2001), and emphasizes the role of emotions as a possible link in the chain between
psychosocial stress and diseases (Bullington, 1999). This is highly speculative though and
need to be investigated further.
Although the transient disruptions of sleep (IV) observed in the present studies could be
harmless, they signify an elevated physiological arousal, and it is possible that over time they
could interfere with the normal anabolic dominance in sleep. It is possible that the metabolic
and hemodynamic changes might be linked to subsequent pathology since it has been
demonstrated that disturbed sleep predicts cardiovascular disease (Nilsson et al., 2001), and
diabetes type II (Nilsson et al., 2002). Other studies have suggested extreme fatigue and sleep
loss as precursors of early heart events (Appels & Mulder, 1988; van Diest & Appels, 1994).
In addition, diabetics (Vgontzas & Chrousos, 2002) and patients with CVD often suffer from
sleep loss, fatigue and stress (Edéll-Gustafsson & Hetta, 2001). However, the physiological
pathways between stress, sleep and burnout or other diseases are largely unknown.
It should be emphasized that in the present work high morning cortisol in blood and saliva, as
well as the metabolic and hemodynamic variables were predicted by arousal from sleep rather
than burnout (IV). It is not clear what this means other than the metabolic and related effects
may “need” an acute load of tension/anxiety in order to show a link to burnout. Rosa et al
(2000) put forward similar suggestions with regard to Primary Insomnia, that is, sleep
fragmentation and other impairment appeared in insomniacs only under acute stages of
tension/anxiety. Otherwise sleep was relatively normal. It might be the case that the duration
of the exposure to stress needs to be longer for a clear effect to be seen.
Thus, even though no conclusions about causality can be drawn from the present results (IIV) it seems reasonable to believe that a prolonged period of overload and stress could be a
significant contributor to disease, especially since sleep, assumed to repair the wear and tear
was impaired. This is a conclusion that Selye made already in the early 20ties (Selye, 1936,
1946).
And according to the model of allostatic load (McEwen & Stellar, 1993; McEwen, 2004;
Seeman et al., 2001), extended stress or tension may result in an up-regulation in the HPAaxis and SAM systems resulting in a more or less permanent hyperarousal, mediating frequent
microdisturbances of sleep (III). Evidence for sleep interfering with the same physiological
systems (Moldofsky, 1997; Vgontzas & Chrousos, 2002; Weitzman, 1975) imply that a
regular return to a pre-stressor level is necessary to avoid the physiological state of balance to
be pathologically displaced (McEwen, 1998, 2004). Further studies are needed however, but
some studies show alteration in the above-mentioned systems in insomnia (Adam et al., 1986;
Bonnet & Arand, 1995a; Bonnet & Arand, 1998) and Vgontzas (2001) reported significant
association between the degree of nocturnal hypersecretion of cortisol and sleep disturbances,
which supports this suggestion. Some recent research (Alderling et al., 2005) shows a
decreased cortisol levels in patients with clinical depression, insomnia and anxiety, suggesting
a “blunted” HPA-axis response due to long-term stress exposure.
48
Sleepiness
Sleepiness was included as a variable in the sleep studies because it is a natural complement
to sleep. Even though excessive fatigue and complaints of sleep disturbances are common in
burnout, the daily and weekly pattern of sleepiness has never been investigated before.
The diurnal pattern of sleepiness was quite pronounced in both studies (II, III). Sleepiness
was high in the morning and in the late evening, with high levels during the midday hours.
The pre-burnout group (III) showed higher levels of sleepiness at the time of rising than their
colleagues. Still the sleepiness levels throughout the work day were comparable in the two
groups. However, during the weekend the low burnout group showed a clear drop in
sleepiness (and increased alertness) that must be interpreted as recovery. This effect was not
present in the pre-burnout group (III). This, again, suggests a lack of recovery through
weekend rest, which seems a pervasive phenomenon in burnout (Freudenberger, 1974;
Maslach & Jackson, 1981a). The same pattern of non-recuperation was seen in the burnout
patients (II) and the levels were considerably higher than what was seen in the pre-burnout
group.
Sleepiness, KSS, 1 very alert - 9 very sleepy
9
Study II
Study III
Skiftworkers
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
weekday
aw 15
weekend
bt aw 15
bt
weekday
aw 12 16
morning
day
night
weekend
bt
aw 12
16
bt
20 24 04
day off
08 12
16 22
Time of day
Figure 5. Mean for rated sleepiness at different points of day and weekend, in the burnout groups
(study II filled black circles study III filled grey circles) and controls (open circles). To compare with
sleepiness in shift workers on night shift (triangles) (Axelsson et al., 2004), train drivers on early
morning shifts (open squares), day shift (filled squares) and a day off (open squares with broken lines)
(Söderström et al., 2001).
49
The levels of sleepiness were comparable with what is seen in, for example shift workers
during a night shift (Kecklund et al., 1997) or after early rising (Ingre et al., 2004; Söderström
et al., 2001) (See fig 5). Thus the levels of sleepiness in burnout must be considered far above
normal levels.
Sleepiness and performance in relation to disturbed sleep
The disturbed sleep in the burnout groups is a problem in its own right. However, it is an
interesting question whether the observed sleep disturbances, as discussed above (II, III)
could contribute to the observed sleepiness/fatigue and cognitive weariness. The crosssectional nature of the present study does not permit any conclusions in this regard, but there
are other data that may strengthen the argument.
Thus, even if field studies on arousals are scarce and most of them have been made in relation
to respiratory obstruction, several studies show clear associations between sleep
fragmentation and high sleepiness levels, as well as impaired daytime functioning
(Carskadon et al., 1982; Guilleminault et al., 1991; Stepanski et al., 1984). Furthermore,
experimental sleep fragmentation clearly impairs alertness (Bonnet, 1986; Bonnet & Arand,
2003), performance and cognitive functioning (Gosselin et al., 2005; Vgontzas et al., 2004).
Interestingly the sleep fragmentation in the pre-burnout group (III) was not accompanied by
any concomitant reduction of total sleep time. Even though some studies suggest that daytime
alertness is impaired as a function of sleep continuity rather than total sleep time in both
“normal” sleepers and in sleep disorders (Carskadon et al., 1982; Rosenthal et al., 1984;
Stepanski et al., 1984), no studies have been able to successfully separate sleep fragmentation
per se from sleep loss or reduction in specific sleep stages. However, it seems reasonable to
assume that a repeated sleep fragmentation might reduce the recuperative value of sleep and
lead to reduced alertness and impaired performance (III).
In the burnout patients (II) the higher level of stage 1 sleep might have contributed further to
the accumulation of fatigue and cognitive impairment, since it has been shown in a
metaanalysis that the relation between fragmentation and sleepiness increases if also increases
in stage 1 are included (Wesensten et al., 1999).
Additionally, it may be of importance that sleep duration in the burnout patients (II), was
considerably less than 7 hours. In the light of recent observations of 7 hours of sleep as a level
below which fatigue/sleepiness starts to accumulate (Belenky et al., 2003; Van Dongen et al.,
2004) it seems a distinct possibility that individuals with burnout get insufficient amounts of
sleep to maintain alertness. Furthermore, the Sleep Sufficiency Index (SSI) was clearly below
80% during weekdays (II), indicating a persistent, insufficient sleep (Broman et al., 1996). It
is plausible to believe that simply too little time for sleep, due to work-related reasons, might
have started the sleep insufficiency, like what has been seen in a Swedish population study
(Broman et al., 1996).
50
Voluntary sleep reduction during the workweek in, for example, shift workers is generally
compensated by extended recovery sleep in weekends (Åkerstedt et al., 2000), and normally,
recovery sleep is suggested to compensate for the SWS reduction and impairment due to sleep
loss (Carskadon & Dement, 1985; Dijk et al., 1991; Dijk & Beersma, 1989). But even though
sleep was extended during days off in all participants (II, III) the compensatory restorative
effect had an impact on alertness only in the control groups. The SSI remained on a low level
during the weekend (86%) in burnout patients (II), and it seems conceivable that the effect of
the fragmented and insufficient amounts of sleep in burnout causes a sleep deficit to
accumulate across time. It is not clear whether the effects are due to fragmentation, sleep
length or content of specific stages, however.
Sleepiness and SWS
One argument for the importance of SWS is that it is a sensitive measure of the restorative
function and an increased delta power or SWS is normally seen after experimentally
shortened sleep (Borbély et al., 1981; Webb & Agnew, 1971). Notably, the amount of SWS
was lower in both high and low burnout groups in study III than what is seen in “normal”
sleepers in this age group (Williams et al., 1974) (table 4) and the reason for this is unclear.
One might speculate that these study groups of IT professionals were subjected to a high
work pressure, which might have suppressed the SWS, although the low burnout group did
not complain of tension or intrusive thoughts about work (Hall et al., 2000; Kecklund &
Åkerstedt, 2004).
One might also consider the significance of the reduced amount of SWS and SWA in the
burnout patients (II). Both have been considered restorative although there are no data on
whether SWS adds any restorative value above that of, for example sleep continuity (Bonnet
& Arand, 2003). The reason for the reduced SWS/SWA in the present study may be the sleep
fragmentation in burnout. Both fragmentation and stress/tension may reduce SWS (Kecklund
& Åkerstedt, 2004). However, the present design does not permit any conclusion on causes.
As outlined in the background, SWS is viewed as the most restorative sleep stage and perhaps
most important for brain repair (Horne, 1978). Growing evidence suggests that the prefrontal
cortex is particularly vulnerable to both stress (Lupien et al., 1998) and sleep deprivation
(Harrison & Horne, 2000; Jones & Harrison, 2001) and sleep is necessary for procedural
memory and executive functioning (Gais & Born, 2004; Maquet, 2000; Nilsson et al., 2005).
Thus, the self-reported dissatisfaction with work in the pre-burnout group (III) might be a
combined effect of the high stress and impaired sleep. It might also be hypothesized that the
decreased cognitive function reported in burnout in the present studies (I, II) might be
associated with a reduction of SWS, sleep efficiency and delta power density in this group.
No studies on executive functioning and sleep in burnout are available however, to confirm
this suggestion.
51
Table 4. Means (SD), from PSG measured sleep in study II and III and from *comparison groups
of the same age (Williams, 1974)
40-49 old
women*
n=11
High BO
n=12
Low BO
n=12
453 (62)
432 (24)
463 (41)
418 (26)
30-39 yrs
old
women*
n=10
433 (19)
408 (53)
425 (23)a
401 (48)
377 (27)
426 (31)
Sleep efficiency (%)
85 (0.06) 91 (0.02) 96 (0.02)a
87 (7.0)
90 (4.1)
96 (0.06)
Awakenings stage 0 (#)
16 (4.3)
13 (4.1)
3.1 (2.1)
6.9(4.2)
6.3(3.5)
1.4 (1.15)
20 (9)
12 (4)
-
12(6)
8(4)
-
Study II
Burnout
group
n=12
Control
group
n=12
SPT (min)
455 (23)
TST (min)
373 (73)
Arousals/h (#)
Study III
WASO (% of SPT)
13.4 (7.6) 6.9 (2.5)
1.6 (1.3)b
9.6 (7.1)
8.8 (8.1)
1.8 (4.0)
Stage 1 (%)
5.4 (3.0)
3.2 (1.4)
5.6 (2.1)
3.6 (3.3)
4.2 (2.5)
4.2 (1.6)
Stage 2 (%)
43 (11)
43 (3)
54.0 (8.6)
50.7(6.5) 56.0(8.3) 53.8 (7.7)
SWS (%)
10 (4)
15 (5)
12.0 (8.3)
7.8 (6.4)
REM (%)
19 (6)
14 (3)
26.7(4.1)a 24.4(5.8) 21.4(6.7) 26.2 (5.3)
Sleep onset latency (min)
21 (22)
10 (6)
7.8(6.0)
19.3(24)
15.8(8.0)
9.8 (9.5)
Latency stage 2 (min)
5.3 (9)
3.1 (4)
7.6(6.1)
4.7 (5.4)
3.0 (1.5)
6.7 (4.0)
SWS latency (min)
18 (10)
14 (6)
35.0 (18.3)
29 (18)
27 (18)
23.1 (7.2)
Stage 1 to REM latency (min)
78 (35)
65 (16)
82.3 (34)
80 (34)
74 (24)
78.7 (19.5)
8.0 (8.5)
14.0 (7.3)
a=significantly higher and b=lower levels compared to men in the same age group.
WASO=Wake Time after Sleep Onset, REM= Rapid Eye Movement sleep,
SWA= Slow Wave Activity, NREM=non REM (stage 2+3+4)
A possible alternative interpretation of the increased sleepiness is the presence of a reduced
sleep debt (or weakened drive for sleep) (Borbély, 1982; Gillberg & Åkerstedt, 1994) due to
inactivity and distress in the patient group. Thus a similar lack of SWS and assumed lower
sleep drive (process S) has been suggested to be present in depression (Staner et al., 2003),
representing an impaired “ability” to accumulate effects of sleep loss (Borbély, 1982;
Borbély & Achermann, 2000). Staner (2003) has contrasted this to a hypothesized increase in
a wake promoting drive (process W), which would cause a lower increase in SWA in
insomniacs, while the homeostatic drive (accumulation of sleep debt) is intact. This is rather
speculative, however.
Fatigue
Excessive fatigue is the core dimension of burnout (Maslach & Jackson, 1981b; Shirom,
1989) and since the burnout scale used to select participants essentially was a fatigue scale the
findings of high levels of fatigue or burnout using such or similar scales (II, III) are trivial,
but may counter an impression of validity and reliability. However, the repeated ratings of
mental fatigue during the week (II) showed sustained differences between the burnout groups
and the controls during workdays and weekend. Thus fatigue was not reduced during the time
52
for recovery presumably provided by the weekend rest and sleep. This suggests an established
long term fatigue level that is more than a temporary state.
As discussed above, the disturbed sleep may at least partly be responsible for the severe
fatigue/exhaustion in burnout (I, II). Thus it was of interest to examine whether there was any
diurnal variation of fatigue throughout the day. Such a diurnal pattern of mental fatigue (II),
with lower levels in the middle of the day was shown, although the pattern was shallower than
in sleepiness. There are no other studies to compare with, so the implications of this diurnal
pattern are not clear. However, they may simply reflect the circadian rhythm in temperature
metabolism (Buysse et al., 2004; Van Cauter et al., 1997) that is also seen in sleepiness, as
discussed above.
The differences in temporal pattern of fatigue and sleepiness emphasizes that the two
represent different states. Sleepiness has been defined as a drive towards sleep, due to lack of
sleep (Dement & Carskadon, 1982; Dinges, 1989), whereas fatigue is more a disinclination
towards activity, usually because of excessive exposure to that activity and/or frustration at
having to do it (Grandjean, 1970). At a moderate level of sleep debt (or an adaptation to sleep
deprivation), the individual often fails to recognize the subjective indication of being sleepy
(Dement et al., 2003), and since the lay person’s use of the terms often are blurred, it might be
that at low and intermediate levels both scales reflect the same phenomenon. Therefore it is an
interesting question whether the persistent pattern of severe sleepiness in the burnout groups
is due to sleepiness per se or to an misinterpretation of mental fatigue as sleepiness.
In the present case (II) it seems plausible that if sleep for a long period of time does not
provide adequate recovery, an accumulation of sleepiness may lead to permanently high
fatigue levels. In addition, the high engagement (I) and tension (III) might have masked the
sleepiness and interfered with recovery for a long time, thus leading to the chronic state of
fatigue (Cameron, 1973). This should be followed up in experimental studies.
Fatigue and mood
A complementary and more traditional view of the causation of fatigue is that extreme fatigue
emerges as an emotional and psychological response to a stressful situation where all efforts
to get rid of the “threat” in a traditional manner (i.e working harder), have failed. The
experience of fatigue is also closely related to decreased motivation, feelings of uneasiness,
anxiety and distress (Cahill, 1999; Cameron, 1973), and is in line with most theories of stress
where a depressive behavior is one way to adapt and manage a fearful situation (Lazarus &
Folkman, 1984; Selye, 1946; Ursin & Eriksen, 2004).
In study I fatigue was described as being paralyzing, and feelings of numbness like living in a
vacuum or in a state of non-existence were reported (I). Such feelings of emptiness and loss
of meaning, deteriorated mood and decreased performance found in this thesis (I-III) are
shared with features of major depression as well (APA, 2000). Even though patients with an
ongoing major depression were excluded from study II, 58% (7 persons) exceeded the clinical
limit (>22) for depression on the BDI. This is in line with the overlap with depression found
in other studies on burnout (Hallsten, 1993; Åsberg et al., 2002). Although burnout and
53
depression share several features such as feelings of worthlessness, concentration difficulty
and fatigue (Lloyd et al., 1994; Martin et al., 1997), they are considered to be distinguished
concepts (Glass et al., 1993; Leiter & Durup, 1994; McKnight & Glass, 1995). But since
sleep disturbances is a common complaint in both depression and burnout (Borbély & WirtzJustice, 1982; Ohayon & Roth, 2003), and fatigue is a feature of insomnia (AASM, 2001;
APA, 2000), it is a plausible to believe that the excessive fatigue in burnout is linked to
disturbed sleep and depressive mood as well. However this is an issue for further research,
and the direction of the relationship is not understood.
The concept of fatigue
The fatigue in burnout, as indicated in the introduction, is a poorly defined concept and in the
every-day meaning its use is broad and blurry. The one-dimensional scale; mental fatigue was
used (II) to assess the state that occurs as a result of prolonged mental strain; a feeling of
inadequacy and inability to think. The interviews added a dimension through its description of
fatigue as a “dense fog” that enveloped them (I) and was described to accumulated as the
process of burnout progressed.
The complex nature of fatigue, with impact on both physical and mental functioning, was not
extensively explored in this work. When the occupational fatigue inventory (SOFI) was
applied in diverse job context different profiles of fatigue emerged (Åhsberg, 2000). Thus,
firemen in action had a peak in physical fatigue, train drivers in sleepiness etcetera. However,
in the burnout patients (II), fatigue was significantly increased on all five dimensions, even if
the F-ratio was largest (most significant) for “lack of energy”. This indicates that the fatigue
in burnout is more general than specific, with a severity of fatigue that exceeds the fatigue
that normally is experienced after a day’s work (Åhsberg, 2000). It might also be that the
SOFI measure is insufficient to discriminate different aspects of fatigue in conditions of ill
health since it was developed for use in a healthy population and the fatigue dimensions
correlated more than expected when it was applied in a patient group (Åhsberg & Furst,
2001). The fatigue in cancer patients during radiotherapy was generalized and unspecific like
in the present study (II), although on a lower level.
Possibly, fatigue is experienced differently in illness and in health (Cella et al., 2002; Ream &
Richardson, 1996). This suggestion is for example supported by studies where the meaning of
fatigue in women with fibromyalgia (FM), was a quite different experience from the tiredness
narrated by healthy women (Söderberg et al., 2002). In that study the descriptions of
“trötthet” in healthy women signified tiredness, described as a temporary feeling relieved by
rest, while the FM women communicated “trötthet” as the heavy burden of something always
present that was hard to relieve. In the interview study (I) the participants distinguished their
experiences of fatigue from that of most people at the end of the day, and reported that they
were “dead tired” and had “gone beyond all tiredness” (I p. 64). Further they expressed their
feelings of fatigue as being “not sleepy-tired” (I), which represents a fatigue that does not
withdraw with rest or sleep, in agreement with definitions of burnout (Maslach & Leiter,
2005) and observed in chronic diseases (Cella et al., 2002; Olson et al., 2002; Söderberg et
al.. , 2002). This suggests that the fatigue has characteristics distinguished from tiredness or
54
sleepiness and may be a path to the exhaustion in burnout (Olson, in press; Ream &
Richardson, 1996).
The same confusion exists about the concepts “tiredness” and “sleepiness” as mentioned
earlier, and it has been suggested that tiredness is a sub-optimal state of sleepiness (Dement,
2003; Dement et al., 2003) while others claims that both are qualitatively distinguished from
each other (Horne, 2003).
Taken together, the descriptions of the experienced tiredness/fatigue represent specific and
separate sides of the phenomenon throughout the process of burning out, and suggest that
there is a distinction between sleepiness, tiredness, fatigue and exhaustion. This hypothesis
needs to be further evaluated and confirmed in qualitative studies however, to provide a new
understanding about those concepts in relation to burnout. If tiredness is a precursor of
fatigue, and fatigue a precursor of exhaustion, the transition from one state to another is the
prime target for health care providers (Olson, in press). Tiredness might be alleviated through
providing sufficient sleep, while fatigue might require other interventions. A careful listening
to the descriptions of the unusual sensations (Toombs, 1992, p.70), might imply specific
interventions in different stages of the process, and this has also to be further understood.
Another question that needs further qualitative studies to understand, is if there is a specific
“sickness- fatigue” that is non-comparable with any other description of fatigue, and if this
fatigue has the same features in different diseases. Since fatigue is the most common
symptom of illness (Aaronson et al., 1999) these research questions are crucial for
recognizing and preventing not only burnout but also other illnesses and diseases.
The process of burning out
One aim of this thesis was to elucidate how the process of burning out was experienced in
order to reach a better understanding of how to prevent and alleviate burnout in today’s
working life. From the descriptions of lived experiences a general structure of the person’s
transition throughout the burnout process emerged (figure 3). This will be discussed below.
Responsibilities towards work and real life
In most conceptualizations, burnout is considered as a result of prolonged occupational stress.
Self-reports of a sustained mental strain, never-ending demands and ‘bringing work home’
(II-IV) indicated an untenable work situation. Furthermore the psychiatric assessment in
study II revealed that work related problems were a precipitating factor in burnout, in line
with the mainstream of theories of burnout (Shirom, 1989), and with the growing body of
research in the area (Bakker et al., 2000; Demerouti et al., 2000; Kalimo, 1999; McVicar,
2003; Stordeur et al., 2001). On the other hand, the participants described how they were
absorbed not only by responsibilities towards work, but also responsibilities towards the
family and/or other commitments in their life world (I). Even though there is a lack of
55
research on “real life stress” and burnout, a few studies suggesting that stress outside work
promotes or exacerbates burnout (Etzion, 1984; Kushnir & Melamed, 1992) support these
findings.
Another interesting question is to what extent the “fuzzy borders” between work and leisure
time indicated in these studies (I-III) may contribute to the sleep disturbances, and to the
dissatisfaction and disengagement from work. Connections between organizational stress and
job satisfaction (Adams et al., 1998; Upenieks, 2002), and between high work stress and
emotional exhaustion (Demerouti et al., 2000; Koustelios, 2001; Stordeur et al., 2001) are
supported in several studies. Moreover, job dissatisfaction and disengagement are both related
to burnout (Demerouti et al., 2000; Kalliath & Morris, 2002; Pines et al., 1981). On the other
hand, sleep disturbances have been connected to job dissatisfaction and a need for recovery,
suggesting a link to sleep (Axelsson et al., 2003; Axelsson et al., 2004).
However burnout is commonly considered an individual matter, a result of ineffective
coping with prolonged stress (Maslach et al., 2001). A positive attitude has been viewed as an
effective buffer against disease (Antonovsky, 1979; Thoresen et al., 2003). By working
“excessive overtime”, “bringing work home” and “working during leisure time” (I-IV), the
burnout group made an active choice of action to solve the work demands, instead of a
passive withdrawal (Jones, 1993; Lee & Ashforth, 1996). But in contrast with other studies
(Maslach et al., 2001), active and confronting coping did not seem to be enough to buffer
against burnout (Demerouti et al., 2001; Thoresen et al., 2003). This illustrates that there is a
certain limit above which more efforts are impossible and it emphasizes a fundamental need
for recovery (Grandjean, 1970).
The inner incentive in the interview study (I) corresponds to recent development of
engagement as the positive antithesis of burnout (Maslach & Leiter, 2005; Maslach et al.,
2001). A high degree of commitment and initial engagement has been suggested to be a
characteristic that distinguishes burnout from depression, job stress or other overlapping
concepts (Hallsten, 1993; Hallsten et al., 2002; Maslach et al., 2001). To succeed with a
difficult task or to create something new might be a creative and “self-nourishing” drive as
long as “a sense of harmony” (Asp, 2002, p. 200) with respect to other responsibilities in life
is maintained. But being constrained by time and being trapped between different
responsibilities created a sense of guilt or “disharmony in feelings, actions and motivation”
(Asp, 2002, p.200) and the participants found themselves like “trapped in a pitfall” (I).
According to CATS (Ursin & Eriksen, 2004) hopelessness emerges when efforts and
recourses available does not change the situation and this feeling is associated with guilt and
depression.
Maslach and Leiter (Maslach & Leiter, 1997) rephrased burnout as “an erosion of
engagement” and Hallsten (1993, 2002) hypothesized that the “absorbing engagement” turns
into “frustration”, which involves a negative attitude and decreased motivation when the
goals are not reached. The descriptions of how the all-absorbing focus on work, piece by
piece, deflated the rest of their lives to the extent that the leisure time was not sufficient to
restore their strength (I), illustrate their difficulty to maintain a balance in life. Interestingly,
intervention programs usually focus on teaching the individual how to manage stress, often
with poor results (Maslach et al., 2001), while little attention has been paid to how to recover
and balance life.
56
To conclude, the findings in this thesis (I-IV) of high work pressure and excessive overtime,
is in line with the mainstream of research on burnout (Adams et al., 1998; Brown & Pranger,
1992; Jenkins & Elliott, 2004; Maslach et al., 2001; Stordeur et al., 2001). But the findings
also showed that they were squeezed between different responsibilities in life (I) supporting
the notion of “real life stress” as an important condition for the burnout development (Etzion,
1984; Kushnir & Melamed, 1992).
This is in line with the life-world perspective implying a circularity between the person and
his/her milieu (Merleau-Ponty, 1995), meaning that what happens in the work organization
for example and in the interrelationship with others affects the individual and vice versa
(Dahlberg et al., 2001). Thus the causality is rather circular than linear (Merleau-Ponty, 1995)
implying responsibilities in relation to others, irrespective of whether it is at work or “in real
life”, understood as affecting the person.
“Cutting off” - and neglect of essential needs
In the descriptions in study I it was found that the reason for focusing even harder on
fulfilling responsibilities, even when strength began to dissipate, was a struggle to shelter the
threatened self-image as skilled and competent professionals (I). This reflects that a person’s
existential significance often is rooted in “doing”, instead of “being” (Cherniss, 1980;
Heidegger, 1962; Pines, 1993). One’s self-image is greatly influenced by self-esteem
(Lyttkens, 1989) and self-esteem primarily built on accomplishment and “doing” has been
called performance based self-esteem (Hallsten et al., 2002; Hallsten et al., 2005), which
recently has been recognized as a plausible predictor of burnout (Hallsten et al., 2005). This
thesis did not intend to provide for any causal conclusions of relations to personality or
whether self-esteem is an antecedent or consequence of burnout but further studies are
needed.
The self-defense of “cutting off” served to bottle up emotions and everything that interfered
with their focus to fulfill their responsibility towards work (I). The emerging fatigue and
emotional exhaustion was a suffering that became a threat against the person’s self, or his/her
sense of identity (Lindholm & Eriksson, 1993; Norberg et al., 2001).
In suffering, different “façades” are often constructed for two purposes: to keep the person’s
suffering concealed from others, as well as from her/himself (Fredriksson & Lindström,
2002). But even though the “cutting off” sheltered the self-image and protected from
suffering to a certain degree, it was also part of the suffering. The “cutting off” included
neglect of essential needs such as: regular meals, sleep, physical training, and time for
recovery and rest (I-IV). The façade of “cutting off” also implied withdrawal from deep
relations with others and feelings of alienation and “non-existence” emerged (I). Self-care, is
the basis for health and all human life (Eriksson, 1987; Halldorsdottir, 1999), but the “cutting
off” was a hindrance for self-care and created a barrier that disabled them from accepting care
from others (I) that may have promoted the gradual development of physical and mental
illness.
57
The turning point
It is debated whether burnout is a process or a state (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998) and in this
thesis the results are commensurate with the idea that burnout is both (Hallsten et al., 2002):
an ongoing development of emerging symptoms (I-IV) but also critical points throughout the
process. Such a point was described when the participants reached the bottom-line and in
acceptance gave up their strong protection of self (I). Their façade of cutting off cracked in
different ways, but the act of acceptance became the turning point from where a new
reconstruction of life could be created (I). Acceptance is a way “to expand oneself” (Marcel,
1979, p 116-29), to get reconciled with one’s vulnerability and aware of the interdependence
in relation to others (Lindseth et al., 1993). This is in line with findings of how sufferers’
facades were abandoned in other studies (Fredriksson & Lindström, 2002).
Hallsten (2002) also discusses that critical events seem to be crucial for whether the
frustration over an unsolvable situation turns into reorganization or if the process turns into
depression or burnout. Thus, to reach behind the façade of cutting of seems to be crucial for
bringing the burnout development into a turning point and thus avoid that the process
progresses into the state of burning-out (see fig. 3).
In this study (I) the “cutting off” was characterized by withdrawal and non-presence, which
made the participants “blind and deaf to warning signals from their own body and from
others” (I p.63). This became a hindrance to communicate their needs (Marcel, 1967), which
complicates the possibilities for managers or others to intervene at an early stage. How to
respond to this unspoken ethical demand, without insulting the integrity of the person, is an
ethical dilemma. Lögstrup (1971) argues that the responsibility of the provider of care never
is to take over the responsibility of “the other”. Thus the ethical demand cannot be met until
the sufferer opens up and invites “the other” (Lögstrup, 1971).
Being met in the suffering has opened up in other crisis situations (Rasmussen, 2000; Sundin
& Jansson, 2003; Talseth et al., 2001; Talseth et al., 2003). But this implies openness,
availability, presence (Norberg et al., 2001; Younger, 1995) and an intensive attentiveness to
understand the most constructive way to meet “the other”, in a nonjudgmental way (Buber,
1954; Pettigrew, 1990).
Although the experiences of the time preceding burnout took place outside the health care
system, the interviewees (I) were in need of care, in order to reduce the risk for burnout.
Those who first recognized the burnout symptoms were: family, colleagues, managers, human
resource personnel or company health service. This study emphasizes the importance of the
organizational culture, climate and ethics, and calls for further studies addressing leadership
training and organizational healthiness (Cox et al., 1993; Karasek & Theorell, 1990;
Winnubst, 1993). In reality this means openness and shared responsibility between managers
and employees to communicate organizational incongruities and fuzziness that might be
embedded in the culture of the workplace. This study is limited to the process of “burning
out” and does not provide data for the reconstruction of a balance in life. Thus, further
research is needed to describe the recovery process as well, important for wellbeing and
rehabilitation of burnout personnel.
58
Methodological considerations
In this thesis focus has been on how the development of burnout is experienced (I), subjective
and objective measures of sleep (II, III), diurnal and weekly pattern of sleepiness (II, III) and
fatigue (II), and how the impaired sleep may relate to physiological measures of health (IV).
The phenomena under investigation affect the person—from the cellular biological level to
the mental, emotional and cognitive level, and contains the persons history, actual life
situation, purposes and expectations. Both quantitative and qualitative approaches have been
applied. The quantitative and qualitative paradigms are complementary to each other and in
combining those methods a deeper understanding of burnout has been attained. Thus, the aims
of the studies have determined the choice of method (Clark, 1998; Sandelowski & Barroso,
2002), and a fruitful approach has been to use the hypothetic-deductive methods to measure
the psycho-physiological responses, like EEG, hormones and blood pressure, and the
established measures of burnout and sleep, whereas the phenomenological life world
approach was appropriate to grasp the meaning of the lived experiences of burnout.
Qualitative methods have large components of subjectivity and context dependence. On
the other hand, all human research deals with the dilemma of subjectivity irrespective of
methodological approach, since the subjective influence of the scientist is always there
whether it is traditional empirical science or not. Qualitative studies emphasize the
understanding of lived human experiences, and aim to describe a deeper underlying meaning
of data. The researcher’s contribution in the process of data collection and analysis belongs to
the data as well (Dahlberg et al., 2001). With a qualitative approach validity and reliability are
an ongoing process throughout the whole study. In striving for objectivity and validity of data
in study I the necessity of self-awareness, the ability to affect one’s pre-understanding and
being conscious about the limits of self-awareness (Dahlberg et al., 2001), were crucial. Selfreflection was used to put the researcher’s presumptions and pre-understanding into light. The
interviewer made diary notes on feelings, reactions and assumptions throughout the whole
process, in order to distance one-self and set aside any pre-understanding or taken for granted
assumptions of the phenomenon (Giorgi, 1997). The co-author analyzed the transcripts
independently and there was also a dialectic encounter between the two during the research
process. Co-assessment of other professionals in seminaries was another way the results were
validated.
In the quantitative studies (II-IV) awareness of subjective influences (i.e. hypothesis and
background knowledge) of the scientist were needed in the reflections upon the meaning of
statistical data, and the qualitative understanding and interpretation to translate numbers into
words (Föllesdal et al., 1984; Streubert & Carpenter, 1995). Efforts were made to restrain the
preunderstanding and to detach the researchers from the interpretation of data to the greatest
extent possible. The questionnaires and many of the subjective variables used in study II-IV
have been validated, and in our hands diaries seem to provide reliable data (Åkerstedt et al.,
1997). Compliance in diary ratings was high, partly due to close and frequent interaction
between the experimenter and participant. No data loss occurred other than as exceptions.
All studies were suffering from small sample sizes which one has to bear in mind when
interpreting the results. However, looking at the results it does not seem likely that study II
59
and III would have yielded much different conclusions with larger sample sizes. However,
larger samples would have made subgroup analyses possible, for example with respect to
gender. According to Giorgi, (1985, 1997) and Dahlberg (2001) the sample size in qualitative
research should be large enough to achieve a variation of experiences and small enough to
permit deep analysis of the data. It is also important to choose information-rich participants
with a variety of backgrounds such that the phenomenon of interest might be covered
(Streubert & Carpenter, 1995). In study I the participants were both men and women with a
wide age range and in different marital status. They also came from different work places.
Since the interviews provided rich information the researchers determined that the number of
participants was adequate.
In the sleep studies (II, III) it was important to describe sleep as realistically as possible.
Thus, it was of particular importance to obtain measures of sleep in the home environment.
On the other hand, field studies suffer from influences of numerous confounding variables
not easy to control. Example of such variables in the sleep environment is temperature, light
exposure, presence of small children or a snoring partner etcetera. Other variables like age,
sex, social responsibilities, may vary considerably between individuals and confound the
results. In study IV possible control factors: “self-reported snoring”, “having small children”
(< 7 yrs) and ache/pain were tested in the stepwise regression analysis. Furthermore the
comparison groups (study II and III) were selected with respect to age, sex, educational
level and occupation in order to grasp at least some of those confounders. The groups did not
differ on most background variables, like number of children, marital status, lifestyle, BMI, or
medication.
Sleeping with the EEG equipment might have disturbed the sleep even if the sleep
environment was familiar. Although the first night effect is most investigated in laboratory
settings (Agnew et al., 1966; Dement et al., 1965), some studies have found such effects even
in home environment (Walsh & Sugerman, 1986; Wauquier et al., 1991). On the other hand
other studies did not find any first night effect, especially with ambulatory equipment (Coates
et al., 1981; Kader & Griffin, 1983). However, one night of habituation was performed to get
the participants familiar with the method and the electrodes (II). The problems of changes
across time were handled through a balanced design in study III.
The selection procedure may have had confounding effects in all studies and care must be
exercised when generalizing. Possibly, for example, another pre-burnout group than the ITcompany employees might have presented another picture of sleep, sleepiness and stress. On
the other hand, questionnaire studies of larger groups seem to show conclusively that at least
sleep and stress problems are universal. Study II had a broader population from which the
participants were sampled, but still mainly represents burnout in white collar workers. In
study IV data represent a secondary analysis carried out on a sample of young individuals
scoring high and low on burnout. The variable “burnout group” was tried in all the stepwise
multiple regression analyses but did not enter any of them, suggesting that the selection
procedure may not have had any confounding effects.
Possibly, the large number of analyses carried out in groups with a small sample size may
have inflated the risk of type 1 errors, yielding too many significant results. On the other hand
the results in study II-IV formed a coherent pattern, which suggests that the results are
relatively robust.
60
A main purpose of scientific work is to generalize the findings to other settings. In the present
studies the cross-sectional designs and correlative approaches limit the conclusions that can
be drawn about causality. Prospective studies, with a longitudinal approach would give better
information about possible pathways between sleep, fatigue and burnout. However, despite
those limitations the findings give descriptions of how sleep, fatigue and physiological
markers may be linked to each other and they generate new questions, to be followed up in
further studies.
Generalization is limited in study I since qualitative research in general is understood as very
context dependent. Phenomenological research is contextual and never understood as
universal. The goal is to illuminate and raise the understanding of the phenomenon. The
general structure or essence that emerged means that the result is raised above the concrete
level, but still it is within the chosen context: the participants on sick-leave from their whitecollar employment for burnout. Dahlberg et. al (2001) argue that the possibility to transfer and
apply the results in other similar contexts depend on: 1) whether a new understanding has
emerged 2) the quality of data, (i.e if data are rich of naïve descriptions of lived experiences
devoid from interpretation, construction or explanation), 3) the degree to which the researcher
has been aware of and able to hold the pre-understanding. The scientific value of a lifeworld
research study depends on the thoroughness in reasoning, labeled a coherence criterion
(Kvale, 1997). The coherence criterion involves a research result that presents an inner logic,
that is, it should be possible to follow the research process throughout the study. Further, the
reasoning should be visible throughout, without contradictions, if the study is to be judged as
valid (Dahlberg et al., 2001; Sandelowski & Barroso, 2002). Since the aim of study I was to
describe lived experiences of time preceding burnout, healthy white-collar workers was not
included in the study. However, it would have been interesting to investigate why colleagues
with similar work conditions did NOT develop burnout. Those informants possibly would
have added understanding on how a balance between different responsibilities in life can be
created and this is an issue for further studies.
One problem that has been discussed elsewhere in this thesis is the presence of co-morbidity.
This is particularly complicated with respect to depression (persons with chronic fatigue were
excluded). Fatigue is a key characteristic of both depression and burnout. Even if ongoing
major depression was a criterion for exclusion sub clinical levels were clearly present among
those on sickleave due to burnout. However, this thesis was not focused on differentiating
burnout from other states but future research will have to deal with this problem of comorbidity.
Tentative integration and some hypotheses
The main findings in this thesis suggest that impaired sleep and recovery is involved in the
burnout process and might be a link to the excessive fatigue that characterizes burnout. This
adds a physiological dimension to the main stream of research, which has focused on
psychosocial processes, and the erosion of energy (Maslach, 2005).
61
Figure 6 illustrates a working hypothesis on the process of burning out based on the findings
in this thesis. The process describes the dynamics within the physiological, emotional and
cognitive systems (the spiral in the figure), and the interaction between the person and his/her
situation. The process might move back and forth and might vary between individuals
dependent on the situation.
Without drawing any conclusions about causality one might speculate on a “vicious circle”
where high work demands and other commitments in life lead to a restricted time for recovery,
and to begin with, a voluntary shortening of sleep to cope with the responsabilities. This is a
common situation in today’s society (Bonnet & Arand, 1995b), and recovery sleep (during
weekends or leisure) normally compensates and restore the balance. Even if sleep is not
shortened, “thoughts of work” and tension to perform well might elevate the physiological
arousal and transient microarousals migt disturb sleep and result in impaired recovery. A
third possibility is that high work demands (stress) cause an increased need for sleep, which
most individuals ignores. An “inner incentive” might be the “drive” that compensates for the
moderate sleep debt and makes possible a further increase of effort, if needed (the first loop in
the process).
If the situation persists and becomes chronic the fatigue starts to accumulate since the energy
drain is higher than the restitution. The engagement and tension still masques the fatigue and
“bringing work home”, and “work at weekends” might be a way to cope with the work
pressure. Thus essential needs for recovery and repair are “cut off” and feelings of
dissatisfaction with the situation emerge. Impaired cognitive functioning promotes anxiety
and fear of failing and even more effort may be needed to keep up the same standard at work.
At this stage there is clearly a need to be attentive to the accumulation of energy drain / lack
of recovery and to take steps that breaks the “vicious circle”. Managers or health care
providers are those who in the first line would observe the complaints of fatigue and sense of
illness that occur before any biological measures of ill health appear (second loop).
If the process continues the physiological arousal might interfere with the restorative function
of sleep resulting in the severe fragmented sleep with disturbed restorative quality as was seen
in the patients. In this state, disturbed sleep has become chronic and may be regarded as
stress-related insomnia. The overwhelming fatigue makes it difficult to fulfill the demands
and feelings of guilt and worthlessness add more energy drain to the process. Alienation from
everything that interferes with work implies that also activities that generate energy are
withdrawn. Thus actions, thoughts and emotions are closely intertwined with the
physiological systems and the vicious circle of ill health, fatigue, alienation and distress
becomes self-nourishing, and may be difficult to turn around (third loop). At this stage
professional treatment is needed in particular to return to normal good quality sleep.
This hypothetical reasoning on burnout development, emphasizes the personal appraisal of
the situation and the total load on the individual. Clearly work stress might be the major
contributor, but lack of recovery (where sleep is essential) might be equally important.
However, this is a working hypothesis that integrates bodily and psychological processes with
the individual’s life world but needs to be confirmed in further research. Also studies on how
the vicious circle can be stopped at different stages in the process are needed
62
Figure 6 A working hypothesis on how the burnout process might progress through physiological and
psychological processes - impaired recovery and energy depletion.
Practical implications and further research
The findings of the present thesis have several implications for practice and for further
research. The most important finding is that burnout was associated with disturbed sleep, even
at an early stage, and that the disruption seems to be a part of the fatigue/sleepiness in
burnout, independent of sleep length. These observations emphasize the need for effective
treatment of disturbed sleep in order to prevent or alleviate burnout. For example,
psychological/behavioral treatment of sleep (Morin et al., 1999) but also pharmacological
intervention might provide relief. Similarly, information/interventions for improved sleep
should be part of preventive work on company and population level. Inferences can also be
made to the context of nursing care, in inspiring to innovative thinking to implement effective
routines that promote good sleep in hospital wards, and in public health care.
There is also a need for clinical intervention studies of different types of treatment for sleep
disorders to establish whether this could be part of evidence based medical treatment. Such
studies are presently under way. The observed sleep disturbances also raise the question of
63
whether burnout and insomnia are overlapping states. As discussed previously the level of
disturbed sleep, the fatigue, and the etiology are similar. This issue needs to be studied in
large studies specifically designed for this purpose.
However, there is also a need for other lines of research. One should be focused on following
large risk groups before burnout appears to obtain better information on risk indicators and
physiological and psychological mechanisms. If sleep turns out to be important in burnout
development it would also be of interest to study the possible role of sleep apnea, Primary
Insomnia, restless legs, etcetera, in the development of burnout. As was discussed previously,
one could perhaps cause burnout without any increase in stress, but instead through lack of
recovery.
The findings also emphasize that experiments are needed. In particular, the effects of partial
sleep loss on the endocrine and immune system may give clues as to the mechanism of
burnout. This would need to focus on partial sleep loss across considerable periods of time
since this is the way sleep loss normally occurs (rather than periods of total sleep loss).
Further research where individual differences are evaluated on an individual level is needed to
give a deeper and more nuanced view of the phenomenon. This requires use of new statistical
tools such as, for example, mixed models analysis (cf. Ingre et al., 2004). Such research is
important for example to clarify if some individuals are more vulnerable to developing
burnout than others.
This thesis also emphasizes that there is a need for a deeper understanding of the nature of
fatigue – as a symptom of disturbed or insufficient sleep and the core dimensions of burnout
have to be distinguished and evaluated. Future studies should focus on the “illness-fatigue”
observed in the present studies - to understand what fatigue is, if fatigue is experienced
differently in health and illness, how and to what extent sleep disturbances are involved.
Furthermore, we need information on how fatigue affects the perception of health, wellbeing
and illness, and how illness and diseases affect fatigue. Since fatigue primarily is a subjective
experience affecting emotions, motivation, performance and wellbeing, qualitative studies are
needed to provide such understanding.
Finally, the findings in this thesis also illustrate how illness might be experienced through
subtle bodily sensations before any explicit manifestation of measurable biological facts. This
enlightens the need of further research on how knowledge on a prereflective level is
communicated. It is still not known how acute or chronic stressful experiences, or the failure
to cope with them, contribute to the onset of a disease or illness (Ader, 1980). Thus, also
multidimensional approaches would be valuable in order to bridge experiences to physiopsychological processes and bring science a step forward in this regard. But such thinking
challenges traditional methods and calls for new perspectives, where the natural science
methodology and qualitative studies are integrated and used simultaneously.
64
References
Aaronson, L. S., Teel, C. S., Cassmeyer, V., Neuberger, G. B., Pallikkathayil, L., Pierce, J., et al.
(1999). Defining and measuring fatigue. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 31, 45-50.
AASM. (2001). ICSD - International Classification of Sleep Disorders, revised: Diagnostic and
coding manual. Chicago, Illinois: American Academy of Sleep Medicin.
Adam, K., & Oswald, I. (1977). Sleep is for tissue restoration. J R Coll Physicians Lond, 11(4), 376388.
Adam, K., & Oswald, I. (1983). Protein synthesis, bodily renewal and the sleep-wake cycle. Clinical
Science, 65, 561-567.
Adam, K., Tomeny, M., & Oswald, I. (1986). Physiological and psychological differences between
good and poor sleepers. Journal of psychiatric research, 20, 301-316.
Adams, A., Bond, S., & Hale, C. A. (1998). Nursing organizational practice and its relationship with
other features of ward organization and job satisfaction. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 27(6),
1212-1222.
Ader, R. (1980). Psychosomatic and psychoimmunologic research - presidental adress. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 42(3).
Aeschbach, D. (1995). Dynamics of the human sleep electroencephalogram: Effects of hypnotics,
sleep deprivation, and habitual sleep length. Unpublished PhD thesis, Swiss Federal Institute
of Technology, Zürich.
Agnew, H. W., Webb, W. B., & Williams, R. L. (1966). The first night effect: An EEG study of sleep.
Psychophysiology, 2, 263-266.
Alberti, K. G. M. M., & Zimmet, P. Z. (1998). Definition, diagnosis and classification of diabetes
mellitus and its complications. Part 1: Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus.
Provisonal report of a who consultation. Diabetic medicine, 15, 539-553.
Alderling, M., Theorell, T., de la Torre, B., & Lundberg, I. (2005). Job strain and saliva cortisol
secretion in swedish population-based study. Paper presented at the 2nd ICOH International
Conference on Psychosocial Factors at Work, Okahama, Japan.
Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health, stress and coping. San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass.
APA. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. (DSM-IV) Washington
DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Appels, A., Falger, P. R. J., & Schouten, E. G. W. (1993). Vital exhaustion as risk indicator for
myocardial infarction in women. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 37, 881-890.
Appels, A., Höppener, P., & Mulder, P. A. (1987). A questionnaire to assess premonitory symptoms of
myocardial infarction. Int J Cardiol, 17, 15-24.
Appels, A., & Mulder, P. (1988). Excess fatigue as a precursor of myocardial infarction. European
Heart Journal, 9, 758-764.
Armstrong, L. E., & VanHeest, J. L. (2002). The unknown mechanism of the overtraining syndrome:
Clues from depression and psychoneuroimmunology. Sports Med, 32(3), 185-209.
ASDA. (1992). EEG arousals: Scoring rules and examples. Sleep, 15, 173-184.
Asp, M. (2002). Vila och lärande om vila. En studie på livsvärldsfenomenologisk grund. Phd thesis.
Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis, Göteborg.
Axelsson, J., Åkerstedt, T., Kecklund, G., Lindqvist, A., & Attefors, R. (2003). Hormonal changes in
satisfied and dissatisfied shift workers across a shift cycle. Journal of Applied Physiology, 95,
2099-2105.
Axelsson, J., Åkerstedt, T., Kecklund, G., & Lowden, A. (2004). Tolerance to shift work - how does it
relate to sleep and wakefullness? International Archives of Occupational and Environmental
Health, 77, 121-129.
65
Bakker, A. B., Killmer, C. H., Siegrist, J., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2000). Effort-reward imbalance and
burnout among nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 31, 884-891.
Bargellini, A., Barbieri, A., Rovesti, S., Vivoli, R., Rongcaglia, R., & Borella, P. (2000). Relation
between immune variables and burnout in a sample of physicians. Occupational and
Environmental Medicine, 57, 453-457.
Barosso, J., & Powell-Cope, G. M. (2000). Metasynthesis of qualitative research in living with hiv
infection. Qualitative Health Research, 10, 340-353.
Bartlett, F. C. (1943). Fatigue following highly skilled work. Proceedings of the Royal Society (B),
131, 147-257.
Bartlett, F. C. (1953). Psychological criteria of fatigue. In W. F. Floyd & A. T. Welford (Eds.),
Symposium on fatigue. London: Lewis, H.K.
Bartley, S. H., & Chute, E. (1947). Fatigue and impairment in man. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Beck, A. T. (1970). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
Press.
Beck, A. T., & Beamesderfer, A. (1974). Assessment of depression: The depression inventory.
Modern problems of pharmacopsychiatry, 7, 151-169.
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical
anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893897.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring
depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Beck, C. (1995). Burnout in undergraduate nursing students. Nurce Educ, 20(4), 19-23.
Belenky, G., Wesensten, N. J., Thorne, D. R., Thomas, M. L., Sing, H. C., Redmond, D. P., et al.
(2003). Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and
subsequent recovery: A sleep dose-response study. J Sleep Res, 12(1), 1-12.
Bell, P. A. (1978). Effects of noise and heat stress on primary and subsidiary task performance.
Human Factors, 20, 749-752.
Benington, J. H., & Heller, H. C. (1995). Restoration of brain energy metabolism as the function of
sleep. Progress in Neurobiology, 45, 347-360.
Benner, P., & Wrubel, J. (1989). The primacy of caring. Stress and coping in helath and illness.
Menlo Park: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, Inc.
Berger, R., & Phillips, N. (1995). Energy conservation and sleep. Behavioural Brain Research, 69, 6573.
Beurskens, A. J., Bültmann, U., Kant, I. J., Vercoulen, J. H., Bleijenberg, G., & Swaen, G., M. (2000).
Fatigue among working people: Validity of a questionnaire measure. Occupational and
Environmental Medicine, 57, 353-357.
Billeter-Koponen, S., & Fredén, L. (2005). Long-term stress, burnout and patient-nurse relations:
Qualitative interview study about nurses' experiences. Scand J Caring Sci, 19, 20-27.
Björntorp, P. (1991). Visceral fat accumulation: The missing link between psychosocial factors and
cardiovascular disease? Journal of Internal Medicine, 230, 195-201.
Björntorp, P. (1993). Visceral obesity: A "civilization syndrome". Obesity Research, 1, 206-222.
Björntorp, P. (2002). Det metabola syndromet. In B. A. Rolf Ekman (Ed.), Stress (pp. 161-172).
Stockholm: Liber.
Bonnet, M. H. (1985). Effect of sleep disruption on sleep, performance, and mood. Sleep, 8(1), 11-19.
Bonnet, M. H. (1986). Performance and sleepiness as a function of frequency and placement of sleep
disruption. Psychophysiology, 23(3), 263-271.
Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1995a). 24-hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal
sleepers. Sleep, 18(7), 581-588.
Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1995b). We are chronically sleep deprived. Sleep, 18, 908-911.
Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1997). Physiological activation in patients with sleep state
misperception. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 533-540.
66
Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1998). Heart rate variability in insomniacs and matched normal
sleepers. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 610-615.
Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (2003). Clinical effects of sleep fragmentation versus sleep
deprivation. Sleep Med Rev, 7(4), 297-310.
Borbély, A. A. (1982). A two-process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol, 1, 195-204.
Borbély, A. A., & Achermann, P. (1992). Concepts and models of sleep regulation: An overview.
Journal of Sleep Research, 1, 63-79.
Borbély, A. A., & Achermann, P. (2000). Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. In M. H.
Kryger, T. Roth & W. C. Dement (Eds.), Prinicples and practice of sleep medicine (3 ed., pp.
377-390). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company.
Borbély, A. A., Baumann, F., Brandeis, D., Strauch, I., & Lehmann, D. (1981). Sleep deprivation:
Effects on sleep stages and eeg power density in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol,
51, 483-493.
Borbély, A. A., & Wirtz-Justice, A. (1982). Sleep, sleep deprivation and depression. Human
Neurobiology, 1, 205-210.
Born, J. (1999). Sleep and immune functions. In M. S. a. U. Tewes (Ed.), Psychoneuroimmunology
(pp. 417-442). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publischers.
Born, J., & Fehm, H. (1998). Hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal activity during human sleep: A
coordinating role for the limbic hippocampal system. Experimental and Clinical
Endocrinology & Diabetes, 106, 153-163.
Bradley, H. B. (1969). Community-based treatment for young adult offenders. Crime and
Delinquency, 15, 359-370.
Brindley, D. N., McCann, B. S., Niaura, R., Stoney, C. M., & Suarez, E. C. (1993). Stress and
lipoprotein metabolism: Modulators and mechanisms. Metabolism, 42, 3-15.
Broman, J. E. (1994). Persistent insomnia. Uppsala: Uppsala University, Dept Psychiatry.
Broman, J. E., Lundh, L. G., & Hetta, J. (1996). Insufficient sleep in the general population.
Neurophysiol Clin, 26, 30-39.
Brown, G. T., & Pranger, T. (1992). Predictors of burnout for psychiatric occupational therapy
personnel. Can J Occup Ther, 59(5), 258-267.
Brown, M. R. (1991). Neuropeptide-mediated regulatin of the neuroendocrine and autonomic
responses to stress. In J. McCubbin, P. Kaufman & C. Nemeroff (Eds.), Stress, neuropeptides,
and systemic disease (pp. 73-93).
Brunner, E. J., Marmot, M. G., Nanchahal, K., Shipley, M. J., Stansfeld, S. A., Juneja, M., et al.
(1997). Social inequality in coronary risk: Central obesity and the metabolic syndrome.
Evidence from the whitehall ii study. Diabetologia, 40, 1341-1349.
Buber, M. (1954). Det mellanmänskliga. Ludvika: Dualis Förlag AB.
Bullington, J. (1999). The mysterious life of the body: A new look at psychosomatics. Motala:
Almqvist & Wiksell International.
Bullough, W. S. (1948). The effects of experimentally induced rest and exercise on the epidermal
mitotic activity of the adult male mouse, mus musculus l. Proceeding of the Royal Society.
Series B, 135, 233-242.
Buysse, D. J., Nofzinger, E. A., Germain, A., Meltzer, C. C., Wood, A., Ombao, H., et al. (2004).
Regional brain glucose metabolism during morning and evening wakefulness in humans:
Preliminary findings. Sleep, 27(7), 1245-1254.
Bültmann, U., de Vries, M., Beurskens, A. J. H. M., Bleijenberg, G., Vercoulen, J. H. M. M., & Kant,
I. (2000). Measurement of prolonged fatigue in the working population. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 411-416.
Cahill, C. A. (1999). Diffferential diagnosis of fatigue in women. JOGNN, 28, 81-86.
Cameron, C. (1973). A theory of fatigue. Ergonomics, 16, 633-648.
Cannon, W. B. (1914). The emergency function of the adrenal medulla in pain and the major
emotions. American Journal of Physiology, 33, 356-372.
67
Carlson, L. A., & Åberg, A. (1985). Serum triglycerides - an independent risk factor for myocardial
infarction but not for angina pectoris. New England Journal of Medicine, 312, 1127.
Carskadon, M., & Dement, W. (1982). The multiple sleep latency test: What does it measure? Sleep,
5, 67-72.
Carskadon, M. A., Brown, E. D., & Dement, W. C. (1982). Sleep fragmentation in the elderly:
Relationship to daytime sleep tendency. Neurobiol Aging, 3, 321-327.
Carskadon, M. A., & Dement, W. C. (1985). Sleep loss in elderly volunteers. Sleep, 8(3), 207-221.
Carskadon, M. A., & Dement, W. C. (2000). Normal human sleep: An overview. In M. H. Kryger, T.
Roth & W. C. Dement (Eds.), Prinicples and practice of sleep medicine (Third ed., pp. 15-25).
Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company.
Cella, D., Lai, J. S., Chang, C. H., Peterman, A., & Slavin, M. (2002). Fatigue in cancer patients
compared with fatigue in the general united states population. Cancer, 94(2), 528-538.
Chajut, E., & Algom, D. (2003). Selective improves under stress: Implications for theories of social
cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Cognition, 85, 231-248.
Chambers, E. A. (1961). Industrial fatigue. Occupational psychlogy, 35, 44-57.
Cherniss, C. (1980). New public professionals and the problem of burnout. In Professional burnout in
human service organizations (pp. 1-17). New York: Praeger Publishers.
Chrousos, G. P. (1998a). Stress as a medical and scientific idea and its implications. Advances in
Pharmacology, 42, 552-556.
Chrousos, G. P. (1998b). Stressors, stress, and neuroendocrine integration of the adaptive response.
Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 851, 311-335.
Chrousos, G. P., & Gold, P. W. (1992). The concepts of stress and stress system disorders. JAMA,
267, 1244-1252.
Clark, A. M. (1998). The qualitative-quantitative debate: Moving from positivism and confrontation to
post-positivism and reconciliation. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 27(6), 1242-1249.
Coates, T. J., George, J. M., Killen, J. D., Marchini, E., Hamilton, S., & Thorensen, C. E. (1981). First
night effect in good and sleep-maintenance insomniacs when recorded at home. Sleep, 4, 293298.
Cooper, C. L. (2005). Handbook of stress medicine and health (Vol. 2). Lancaster, UK: CRC Press.
Cox, T. (1985). The nature and measurement of stress. Ergonomics, 28, 1155-1163.
Cox, T., Kuk, G., & Leiter, M. (1993). Burnout, health, work stress and organizational healthiness. In
W. Schaufeli, C. Maslach & M. Tadeusz (Eds.), Professional burnout. Recent developments in
theory and research (pp. 177-193). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.
Craig, A., & Cooper, R. E. (1992). Symptoms of acute and chronic fatigue. In A. P. Smith & D. M.
Jones (Eds.), Handbook of human performance (Vol. 3, pp. 289-339). London: Harcourt
Brace Jovanovich.
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psykometrika, 16, 297333.
Czeisler, C. A. (1995). The effect of light on the human circadian pacemaker. In D. Chadwick & K.
Ackrill (Eds.), Circadian clocks and their adjustment (pp. 254-302). Chichester: Ciba
Foundation.
Czeisler, C. A., & Khalsa, S. B. S. (2000). The human circadian timing system and sleep-wake
regulation. In M. H. Kryeger, T. Roth & W. C. Dement (Eds.), Prinicples and practice of
sleep medicine (pp. 353-376). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company.
Dahlberg, K., Drew, N., & Nyström, M. (2001). Reflective lifeworld research. Lund: Studentlitteratur.
De Vente, W., Olff, M., Van Amsterdam, J. G. C., Kamphuis, J. H., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (2003).
Physiological differences between burnout patients and healthy controls: Blood pressure, heart
rate, and cortisol responses. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60, i54-i61.
Dement, W., Kahn, E., & Roffwarg, H. S. (1965). The influence of the laboratory situations on the
dreams of the experimental subject. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 140, 119-131.
Dement, W. C. (2003). More on feeling tired: "response" to Jim Horne. Sleep, 26(6), 764.
68
Dement, W. C., & Carskadon, M. A. (1982). Current perspectives on daytime sleepiness: The issues.
Sleep, 5, 56-66.
Dement, W. C., Hall, J., & Walsh, J. K. (2003). Tiredness versus sleepiness: Semantics or a target for
public education? Sleep, 26(4), 485-486.
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., de Jonge, J., Janssen, P., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2001). Burnout and
engagement at work as a function of demands and control. Scand J Work Environ health,
27(4), 279-286.
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2000). A model of burnout and life
satisfaction amongst nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 32, 454-464.
Demitrack, M. A. (1994). Chronic fatigue syndrome: A disease of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
axis? Annals of Medicine, 26, 1-5.
Depue, R. A., & Monroe, S. M. (1986). Conceptualization and measurement of human disorder in life
stress research: The problem of chronic disturbance. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 36-51.
Derogatis, L. R. (1977). Symptom checklist-90 manual - II (2 ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins
University Press.
Deykin, E. Y., Keane, T. M., Kaloupek, D., Fincke, G., Rothendler, J., Siegfried, M., et al. (2001).
Posttraumatic stress disorder and the use of health services. Psychosom Med, 63(5), 835-841.
Dijk, D.-J., Brunner, D. P., & Borbély, A. (1991). Eeg power density during recovery sleep in the
morning. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 78, 203-214.
Dijk, D. J., & Beersma, D. G. M. (1989). Effects of sws deprivation on subsequent eeg power density
and spontaneous sleep duration. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 72, 312-320.
Dijk, D. J., Brunner, D. P., & Borbély, A. A. (1990). Time course of eeg power density during long
sleep in humans. Am J Physiol, 258, R650-R661.
Dimsdale, J. E., & Herd, J. A. (1982). Variability of plasma lipids in response to emotional arousal.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 44, 413-427.
Dinges, D. F. (1989). The nature of sleepiness: Causes, contexts, and consequences. In A. Stunkard &
A. Baum (Eds.), Perspectives in behavioral medicine: Eating, sleeping, and sex (pp. 147-179).
Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Drummond, S. P., & Brown, G. G. (2001). The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses
to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(5 Suppl), S68-73.
Dunn, A. J., & Berridge, C. W. (1990). Physiological and behavioral responses to corticotropinreleasing factor administration: Is crf a mediator of anxiety or stress responses? Brain
Research Reviews, 15, 71-100.
Edgar, D., Dement, W., & Fuller, C. (1993). Effect of scn lesions on sleep in squirrel monkeys:
Evidence for opponent processes in sleep-wake regulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 13,
1065-1079.
Edéll-Gustafsson, U. M., & Hetta, J. E. (2001). Fragmented sleep and tiredness in males and females
one year after percutaneous transluminal coronary angioplasty (ptca). Journal of Advanced
Nursing, 34, 203-211.
Ellis, R. D. (1999). Integrating neuroscience and phenomenology in th estudy of consciousness.
Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 30(1), 18-47.
Enoka, R. M. (1995). Mechanisms of muscle fatigue: Central facors and task dependency. J
Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 5(3), 141-149.
Eriksen, H. R., Olff, M., Murison, R., & Ursin, H. (1999). The time dimension in stress responses:
Relevance for survival and health. Psychiatry Research, 85, 39-50.
Eriksson, K. (1987). Vårdandets idé (the idea of caring). Stockholm, Sweden: Almqvist & Wiksell.
Etzion, D. (1984). Moderating effect of social support on the stress-burnout relationship. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 69, 615-622.
Etzion, D., Eden, D., & Lapidot, Y. (1998). Relief from job stressors and burnout: Reserve service as a
respite. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 577-585.
Fagerberg, I., & Jönhagen, M. E. (2002). Temporary confusion: A fearful experience. J Psychiatr
Ment Health Nurs, 9(3), 339-346.
69
Farrington, A. (1997). Strategies for reducing stress and burnout in nursing. Br J Nurs, 6(1), 44-50.
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997a). Structured clinical interview
for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (Scid-I), Clinical Version. Arlington: American Psychiatric Press,
Inc.
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Smith Benjamin, L. (1997b). Structured
Clinical Interview for DSM-IV axis II personality disorders (Scid-II). Washington DC:
American Psychiatric Press, Inc.
Folkard, S., & Åkerstedt, T. (1991). A three process model of the regulation of alertness and
sleepiness. In R. Ogilvie & R. Broughton (Eds.), Sleep, arousal and performance: Problems
and promises (pp. 11-26). Boston: Birkhäuser.
Folkow, B. (1997). Physiological aspects of the "defence" and "defeat" reactions. Acta Physiologica
Scandinavica, 640, 34-37.
Ford, D. E., & Cooper-Patrick, L. (2001). Sleep disturbances and mood disorders: An epidemiologic
perspective. Depress Anxiety, 14(1), 3-6.
Ford, H., Trigwell, P., & Johnson, M. (1998). The nature of fatigue in multiple sclerosis. J Psychosom
Res, 45(1 Spec No), 33-38.
Frankenhaeuser, M. (1981). Coping with job stress - a psychobiological approach. In G. J. B Gardell
(Ed.), Working life (pp. 213-233): John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Frankenhaeuser, M. (1986). A psychobiological framework for research on human stress and coping.
In H. H. Appley & R. Trumbull (Eds.), Dynamics of stress: Physiological, psychological and
social perspective (pp. 101-116). New York: Plenum Press.
Frankenhaeuser, M., & Gardell, B. (1976). Underload and overload in working life: Outline of a
multidisciplinary approach. Journal of Human Stress, 2(September), 35-46.
Fredriksson, L., & Lindstrom, U. A. (2002). Caring conversations - psychiatric patients' narratives
about suffering. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 40(4), 396-404.
Freedman, R. R. (1986). EEG power spectra in sleep-onset insomnia. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol, 63, 408-413.
Freudenberger, H. J. (1974). Staff burn-out. Journal of Social Issues, 30, 159-165.
Freudenberger, H. J. (1983). Burnout: Contemporary issues, trends, and concerns. In B. A. Farber
(Ed.), Stress and burnout (pp. 23-28). New York: Anchor Press/Doubleday.
Fukuda, K., Straus, S. E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M. C., Dobbins, J. G., & Komaroff, A. (1994). The
chronic fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International
chronic fatigue syndrome study group. Ann Intern Med, 121(12), 953-959.
Föllesdal, D., Elster, J., & Wallöe, L. (1984). Argumentatsjonsteori, sprok og videnskabsfilosofi. Oslo,
Bergen, Stavanger och Tromsö: Universitetsforlaget.
Gais, S., & Born, J. (2004). Declarative memory consolidation: Mechanisms acting during human
sleep. Learn Mem, 11(6), 679-685.
Gamberale, F. (1985). Use of behavioral performance tests in the assessment of solvent toxicity.
Scand J Work Environ Health, 11(suppl 1), 65-74.
Gillberg, M. (1995). Sleepiness and its relation to the length, content, and continuity of sleep. Journal
of Sleep Research, 4 (suppl 2), 37-40.
Gillberg, M., & Åkerstedt, T. (1994). Sleep restriction and SWS-suppression: Effects on daytime
alertness and night-time recovery. J Sleep Res(3), 144-151.
Giorgi, A. (1985). Sketch of a psychological phenomenological method. In a. Giorgi (ed.):
Phenomenology and psychological research (pp.8-22): Pittsburg, PA: Duquesne University
Press.
Giorgi, A. (1997). The theory, practice and evaluation of the phenomenological method as a
qualitative research procedure. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 28(2), 119-135.
Glass, D. C., McKnight, J. D., & Valdimarsdottir, H. (1993). Depression, burnout, and perceptions of
control in hospital nurses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 147-155.
Goldberg, D. P., & Hillier, V. F. (1979). A scaled version of the general health questionnaire.
Psychological Medicine, 9, 139-145.
70
Gosselin, A., De Koninck, J., & Campbell, K. B. (2005). Total sleep deprivation and novelty
processing: Implications for frontal lobe functioning. Clin Neurophysiol, 116(1), 211-222.
Grandjean, E. (1979). Fatigue in industry. Br J Ind Med, 36, 175-186.
Grandjean, E. P. (1970). Fatigue. American Industrial Hygiene Association Journal (July-August).
Grossi, G., Perski, A., Ekstedt, M., Johansson, T., Lindström, M., & Holm, K. (In press). The morning
salivary cortisol response in burnout. Journal of Psychosomatic Research.
Grossi, G., Perski, A., Evengård, B., Blomkvist, V., & Orth-Gomér, K. (2003). Physiological
correlates of burnout among women. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55, 309-316.
Grossi, G., Theorell, T., Jürisoo, M., & Setterlind, S. (1999). Psychophysiological correlates of
organizational change and threat of unemployment among police inspectors. Integrative
Physiological and Behavioral Science, 34, 30-42.
Guilleminault, C., Stoohs, R., Clerk, A., Cetel, M., & Maistros, P. (1993). A cause of excessive
daytime sleepiness. Chest, 104(3), 781-787.
Guilleminault, C., Stoohs, R., & Duncan, S. (1991). Daytime sleepiness in regular heavy snorers.
Chest, 99, 40-48.
Hall, M., Buysse, D. J., Nowell, P. D., Nofzinger, E. A., Houck, P., Reynolds, C. F., et al. (2000).
Symptoms of stress and depression as correlates of sleep in primary insomnia. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 62, 227-230.
Halldorsdottir, S. (1999). Suffering- reflection-caring. InternaTional Journal for Human Caring, 1520.
Hallsten, L. (1993). Burning out: A framework. In W. Schaufeli, C. Maslach & T. Marek (Eds.),
Professional burnout: Recent developments in theory and research (pp. 95-113). Washington
DC: Taylor & Francis.
Hallsten, L., Bellaagh, K., & Gustavsson, K. (2002). Utbränning i Sverige- en populationsstudie.
Stockholm: Arbetslivsinstitutet.
Hallsten, L., Josephson, M., & Torgén, M. (2005). Performance-based self-esteem: A driving force in
burnout processes and its assessment. Arbete och Hälsa, 4, 1-39.
Harrison, Y., & Horne, J. A. (2000). The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: A review. J
Exp Psychol Appl, 6(3), 236-249.
Hauri, P., & Fisher, J. (1986). Persistent psychophysiologic (learned) insomnia. Sleep, 9(1), 38-53.
Hauri, P. J. (1983). A cluster analysis of insomnia. Sleep, 6(4), 326-338.
Hauri, P. J. (2000). Primary insomnia. In M. H. Kryger, T. Roth & W. C. Dement (Eds.), Prinicples
and practice of sleep medicine (Third ed., pp. 633-639). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders
Company.
Healey, E. S., Kales, A., Monroe, L. J., Bixler, E. O., Chamberlin, K., & Soldatos, C. R. (1981). Onset
of insomnia: Role of life-stress events. Psychosom Med, 43(5), 439-451.
Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time. New York: Harper & Row.
Heidegger, M. (1982). The basic problems of phenomenology. (A. Hofstader, Trans.). Bloomington:
Indiana University Press.
Hockey, R. G. J. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress
and high workload: A congnitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45, 73-93.
Hoddes, E., Zarcone, V., Smythe, H., Phillips, R., & Dement, W. (1973). Quantification of sleepiness:
A new approach. Psychophysiology, 10, 431-436.
Holmes, G. P., Kaplan, J. E., Ganz, N. M., Komaroff, S. L., Schonberger, L. B., Straus, S. E., et al.
(1998). Chronic fatigue syndrome: A working case definition. Annals of Internal Medicine,
108, 387-389.
Hooley, L. (1997). Circumventing burnout in aids care. Am J Occup Ther, 51(9), 759-766.
Hopwood, P., Howell, A., & Maguire, P. (1991). Screening fror psychiatric morbidity in patients with
advanced breast cancer: Validation of two self-report questionnaires. Br. J. Cancer, 64, 353356.
Horne, J. (2003). The semantics of sleepiness. Sleep, 26(6), 763; author reply 764.
71
Horne, J. A. (1978). A review of the biological effects of total sleep deprivation on man. Biol Psychol,
7, 55-102.
Horne, J. A. (1988). Sleep loss and "divergent" thinking ability. Sleep, 11(6), 528-536.
Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., & Tononi, G. (2004). Local sleep and learning. Nature,
430(6995), 78-81.
Huether, G. (1996). The central adaptation syndrome: Psychosocial stress as a trigger for adaptive
modifications of brain structure and brain function. Progress in Neurobiology, 48, 569-612.
Husserl, E. (1970). The crisis of european sciences and trancendental phenomenology. Evanston, IL:
North Western University Press.
Husserl, E. (1998). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy.
Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publ.
Härenstam, A., & Theorell, T. (1990). Cortisol elevation and serum gamma-glutamyl transpeptidase in
response to adverse job conditions: How are they interrelated? Biological Psychology, 31,
157-171.
Härmä, M., Suvanto, S., Popkin, S., Pulli, K., Mulder, M., & Hirvonen, K. (1998). A dose-response
study of total sleep time and the ability to maintain wakefulness. Journal of Sleep Research, 7,
167-174.
Høieggen, A., Fossum, E., Moan, A., Eide, I. K., & Kjeldsen, S. E. (1998). Insulin modifies the
glucose response to mental stress independently of forearm blood flow. Blood Pressure, 7,
291-298.
Ingre, M., Kecklund, G., Åkerstedt, T., & Kecklund, L. (2004). Variation in sleepiness during early
morning shifts: A mixed model approach to an experimental field study of train drivers.
Chronobiology International, 21, 973-990.
Irwin, M., Clark, C., Kennedy, B., Christian Gillin, J., & Ziegler, M. (2003). Nocturnal
catecholamines and immune function in insomniacs, depressed patients, and control subjects.
Brain Behav Immun, 17(5), 365-372.
Irwin, M., McClintick, J., Costlow, C., Fortner, M., White, J., & Gillin, J. C. (1996). Partial night
sleep deprivation reduces natural killer and cellular immune responses in humans. FASEB J,
10, 643-653.
Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten twenty electrode system of the international federation.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371-375.
Jenkins, R., & Elliott, P. (2004). Stressors, burnout and social support: Nurses in acute mental health
settings. J Adv Nurs, 48(6), 622-631.
Jones, K., & Harrison, Y. (2001). Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Med
Rev, 5(6), 463-475.
Jones, M. L. (1993). Role conflict: Cause of burnout or energizer? Soc Work, 38(2), 136-141.
Kader, G. A., & Griffin, P. T. (1983). Reevaluation of the phenomenon of the first night effect. Sleep,
6, 67-71.
Kalimo, R. (1999). Knowledge jobs - how to manage without burnout? Scandinavian Journal of Work
Environment and Health, 25(special), 605-609.
Kalliath, T., & Morris, R. (2002). Job satisfaction among nurses. JONA, 32, 648-654.
Kant, I. J., Bültmann, U., Schröer, K. A. P., Beurskens, A. J. H. M., van Amelsvoort, L. G. P. M., &
Swaen, G. M. H. (2003). An epidemiological approach to study fatigue in the working
population: The maastrich cohort study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60(Suppl
1), i32-i39.
Karasek, R., & Theorell, T. (1990). Healthy work: Stress, productivity, and the reconstruction of
working life. New York: Basic Book.
Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude and mental strain. Implications for job
redesign. Adm Sci Q, 24, 285-308.
Kecklund, G., & Åkerstedt, T. (1997). Objective components of individual differences in subjective
sleep quality. Journal of Sleep Research, 6, 217-220.
72
Kecklund, G., & Åkerstedt, T. (2004). Apprehension of the subsequent working day is associated with
a low amount of slow wave sleep. Biol Psychol, 66(2), 169-176.
Kecklund, G., Åkerstedt, T., & Lowden, A. (1997). Morning work: Effects of early rising on sleep and
alertness. Sleep, 20(3), 215-223.
Kenttä, G., Hassmen, P., & Raglin, J. S. (2001). Training practices and overtraining syndrome in
swedish age-group athletes. Int J Sports Med, 22(6), 460-465.
King, S. L., & Hegadoren, K. M. (2003). Stress hormones: How do they measure up? Biological
research for nursing, 4, 92-103.
Kirschbaum, C., Bartussek, D., & Strasburger, C. J. (1992). Cortisol responses to psychological stress
and correlations with personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 13531357.
Kissebah, A. H., & Krakower, G. R. (1994). Regional adiposity and moridity. Physiological Reviews,
74, 761-811.
Knutsson, A., & Bøggild, H. (2000). Shiftwork and cardiovascular disease: Review of disease
mechanisms. Review on Environmental Health, 15, 359-372.
Koustelios, A. (2001). Organizational factors as predictors of teachers' burnout. Psychological
Reports, 88, 627-634.
Krupp, L. B., Mendelson, W. B., & Friedman, R. (1991). An overview of chronic fatigue syndrome. J
Clin Psychiatry, 52, 403-410.
Kryger, M. H., Roth, T., & Carskadon, M. (1994). Circadian rhythms in humans: An overview. In K.
M., R. T. & D. W. (Eds.), Principles and practice of sleep medicine (pp. 301-308).
Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company.
Kräuchi, K., Cajochen, C., Werth, E., & Wirz-Justice, A. (1999). Warm feet promote the rapid onset
of sleep. Nature, 401, 36-37.
Kushnir, T., & Melamed, S. (1992). The gulf war and its impact on burnout and well-being of working
civilians. Psychological Medicine, 22, 987-995.
Kvale, S. (1997). Den kvalitativa forskningsintervjun. Lund: Studentlitteratur.
Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer.
Lee, R., & Ashforth, B. (1996). A meta-analytic examination of the correlates of the three dimensions
of job burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 123-133.
Leiter, M. P., & Durup, J. (1994). The discriminant validity of burnout and depression: A
confirmatory factor analytic study. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 7, 357-373.
Lerman, Y., Melamed, S., Shragin, Y., Kushnir, T., Rotgoltz, Y., Shirom, A., et al. (1999).
Association between burnout at work and leukocyte adhesiveness/aggregation. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 61, 828-833.
Lindholm, L., & Eriksson, K. (1993). To understand and alleviate suffering in a caring culture.
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 18(9), 1354-1361.
Lindseth, A., Marhaug, V., Norberg, A., & Udén, G. (1993). Registrated nurses' and physicians'
reflections on their narratives about ethically difficult care episodes. Journal of Advanced
Nursing, 20, 245-250.
Lisspers, J., Nygren, A., & Söderman, E. (1997). Hospital anxiety and depression scaale (had): Some
psychometric data for a Swedish sample. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 96, 281-286.
Liu, Y., & Tanaka, H. (2002). Overtime work, insufficient sleep, and risk of non-fatal acute
myocardial infarction in Japanese men. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 59, 447451.
Lloyd, S., Streiner, D., & Shannon, S. (1994). Burnout, depression, life and job satisfaction among
canadian emergency physicians. The Journal of Emergency Medicine, 12, 559-565.
Lundberg, U. (1995). Methods and applications of stress research. Technology and Health Care, 3, 39.
Lundberg, U. (2000). Catecholamines. In G. Fink (Ed.), Encyclopedia of stress (Vol. 1, pp. 408-413).
New York: Academic Press.
73
Lundberg, U., & Frankenhaeuser, M. (1980). Pituitary-adrenal and sympathetic-adrenal correlates of
distress and effort. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 24, 125-130.
Lundh, L.-G., & Broman, J.-E. (2000). Insomnia as an interaction between sleep-interfering and sleepinterpreting processes. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 49, 299-310.
Lupien, S. J., de Leon, M., de Santi, S., Convit, A., Tarshish, C., Nair, N. P. V., et al. (1998). Cortisol
levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. nature
neuroscience, 1, 69-73.
Lyttkens, L. (1989). Disciplined man (swe). Stockholm: Allmänna Förlaget.
Lögstrup, K. E. (1971/1956). The ethical demand. Philadelphia: Fortress Press.
Maquet, P. (2000). Functional neuroimaging of normal human sleep by positron emission
tomography. Journal of Sleep Research, 9, 207-231.
Maquet, P. (2001). The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science, 294, 1048-1052.
Marcel, G. (1967). Presence and immortality. Pittsburg, KA: Duquesne University Press.
Marcel, G. (1979). The mystery of being II. In: Reflection and mystery, vol II (french original). New
York: University Press of America.
Martin, F., Poyen, D., Bouderlique, E., Gouvernet, J., Rivet, B., Disdier, P., et al. (1997). Depression
and burnout in hospital health care professionals. International Journal of Occupational and
Environmental Health, 3, 204-209.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. (1981a). The measurement of experienced burnout. Journal of
Occupational Behavior, 2, 99-113.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981b). Burnout in helath professions: A social psychological analysis.
In G. Sanders & J. Suls (Eds.), Social psychology of health and illness (pp. 227-251):
Erlbaum.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1984). Burnout in organizational settings. Beverly Hills CA: Sage.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1986). Maslach burnout inventory. Manual (2nd ed.). Palo Alto CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press.
Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., & Leiter, M. P. (1996). Maslach burnout inventory manual (Third ed.).
Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
Maslach, C., & Leiter, M. (1997). The thruth about burnout. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Maslach, C., & Leiter, M. P. (2005). Stress and burnout: The critical research. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.),
Handbook of stress medicine and helath (Vol. 2, pp. 155-172). Lancaster, UK: CRC Press.
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of Psychology, 52,
397-422.
McEwen, B., & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the individual: Mechanismns leading to disease.
Archives of Internal Medicine, 153, 2093-2101.
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Protective and demaging effects of stress mediators. The New England Journal
of Medicine, 338(3), 171-179.
McEwen, B. S. (2000). Effects of adverse experiences for brain structure and function. Biological
Psychiatry, 48, 721-731.
McEwen, B. S. (2004). Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress: Allostasis and allostatic
overload and relevance to the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Ann N Y Acad Sci,
1032, 1-7.
McGinty, D., & Szymusiak, R. (1990). Keeping cool: A hypothesis about the mechanisms and
functions of slow wave sleep. Trends Neurosci, 13, 480-487.
McKnight, J. D., & Glass, D. C. (1995). Perceptions of control, burnout, and depressive
symptomatology: A replication and extension. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
63, 490-494.
McVicar, A. (2003). Workplace stress in nursing: A literature review. Journal of Advanced Nursing,
44(6), 633-642.
Meijer, J., & Rietveld, W. (1989). Neurophysiology of the suprachiasmatic circadian pacemaker in
rodents. Physiological Reviews, 69, 671-707.
74
Melamed, S., Kushnir, T., & Shirom, A. (1992). Burnout and risk factors for cardiovascular diseases.
Behavioral Medicine, 18, 53-60.
Melamed, S., Ugarten, U., Shirom, A., Kahana, L., Lerman, Y., & Froom, P. (1999). Chronic burnout,
somatic arousal and elevated salivary cortisol levels. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 46,
591-598.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1995/1945). Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge.
Mills, J. N., Minors, D. S., & Waterhouse, J. M. (1978). Adaptation to abrupt time shifts of the
oscillator(s) controlling human circadian rhythms. J Physiol, 285, 567-594.
Mitler, M. M. (1993). Daytime sleepiness and cognitive functioning in sleep apnea. Sleep, 16, 68-70.
Moch, S. L., Panz, V. R., Joffe, B. I., Havlik, I., & Moch, J. D. (2003). Longitudinal changes in
pituitary-adrenal hormones in south african women with burnout. Endocrine, 21(3), 267-272.
Moldofsky, H. (1997). Sleep-wakefulness and immune-neuroendocrine functions in humans. In H.
Osamu (Ed.), Sleep and sleep disorders (pp. 239-260): Academic press 1997.
Monk, T. H. (1989). A visual analogue scale technique to measure global vigor and affect. Psychiatr
Res, 27, 89-99.
Monroe, L. (1967). Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor sleepers.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 255-264.
Morgan, C. A., Cho, T., Hazlett, G., Coric, V., & Morgan, J. (2002). The impact of burnout on human
physiology and on operational performance: A prospective study of soldiers enrolled in the
combat driver qualification course. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 75, 199-205.
Morin, C. M. (1993). Insomnia - psychological assessment and management. New York: The Guilford
Press.
Morin, C. M., Hauri, P. J., Espie, C. A., Spielman, A. J., Buysse, D. J., & Bootzin, R. R. (1999).
Nonpharmacological treatment of chronic insomnia. Sleep, 8(22), 1134-1156.
Morin, C. M., Rodrigue, S., & Ivers, H. (2003). Role of stress, arousal, and coping skills in primary
insomnia. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 259-267.
Morin, C. M., Stone, J., Trinkle, D., Mercer, J., & Remsberg, S. (1993). Dysfunctional beliefs and
attitudes about sleep among older adults with and without insomnia complaints. Psychology
and Aging, 8, 463-467.
Morruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the eeg.
Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology, 1, 445-473.
Mullington, J., Korth, C., Hermann, D. M., Orth, A., Galanos, C., Holsboer, F., et al. (2000). Dosedependent effects of endotoxin on human sleep. Am J Pysiol Regulatory Integrative Comp
Physiol, 278, R947-R955.
Muzet, A. (2004). Does the physical environment interact with the sleep process? Sleep Medicine
Reviews, 8, 77-79.
Nakamura, H., Nagase, H., Yoshida, M., & Ogino, K. (1999). Natural killer (nk) cell activity and nk
cell subsets in workers with a tendency of burnout. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 46,
569-578.
Nicolson, N. A., & van Diest, R. (2000). Salivary cortisol patterns in vital exhaustion. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 49, 335-342.
Nilsson, I., Jansson, L., & Norberg, A. (1999). Crisis phenomena after stroke reflected in an existential
perspective. Intl J Aging and human development, 49(4), 259-277.
Nilsson, J. P., Söderstrom, M., Karlsson, A. U., Lekander, M., Akerstedt, T., Lindroth, N. E., et al.
(2005). Less effective executive functioning after one night's sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res,
14(1), 1-6.
Nilsson, P., Nilsson, J.-Å., Hedblad, B., & Berglund, G. (2001). Sleep disturbances in association with
elevated pulse rate for the prediction ofmortality - consequences of mental strain? Journal of
Internal Medicine, 250, 521-529.
Nilsson, P., Rööst, M., Engström, G., Hedblad, B., Janzon, L., & Berglund, G. (2002). Incidence of
diabetes in middle-aged men is related to resting heart rate and difficulties to fall asleep.
75
(abstract). Paper presented at the 7th International Congress of Behavioural Medcine,
Helsinki, Finland.
Nilsson, P. M., Rööst, M., Engström, G., Hedblad, B., & Berglund, G. (2004). Incidence of diabetes in
middle-aged men is related to sleep disturbances. Diabetes Care, 27, 2464-2469.
Norberg, A., Bergsten, M., & Lundman, B. (2001). A model of consolation. Nursing Ethics, 8(6), 544553.
Ohayon, M. M. (2002). Epidemiology of insomnia: what we know and what we still need to learn.
Sleep Medicine Reviews, 6, 97-111.
Ohayon, M. M., & Roth, T. (2003). Place of chronic insomnia in the course of depressive and anxiety
disorders. Journal of psychiatric research, 37, 9-15
Olson, K. (in press). A new way of thinking about fatigue, Edmonton, Canada.
Olson, K., Tom, B., Hewitt, J., Whittingham, J., Buchanan, L., & Ganton, G. (2002). Evolving
routines: Preventing fatigue associated with lung and colorectal cancer. Qual Health Res,
12(5), 655-670.
Ottoson, M., Vikman-Adolfsson, K., Enerbäck, S., Olivecrona, G., & Björntorp, P. (1994). The effects
of cortisol on the regulation of lipoprotein lipase activity in human adipose tissue. Journal of
Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 79, 820-825.
Parker, A. J., Wessely, S., & Cleare, A. J. (2001). The neuroendocrinology of chronic fatigue
syndrome and fibromyalgia. Psychol Med, 31(8), 1331-1345.
Parkes, K. R. (1994). Personality and coping as moderators of work stress processes: Models, methods
and measures. Work and Stress, 8, 110-129.
Parmeggiani, P. L. (2000). Physiological regulation in sleep. In M. H. Kryger, T. Roth & W. C.
Dement (Eds.), Prinicples and practice of sleep medicine (3 ed., pp. 169-178). Philadelphia:
W.B. Saunders Company.
Perlis, M. L., Kehr, E. L., Smith, M. T., Andrews, P. J., Orff, H., & Giles, D. E. (2001a). Temporal
and stagewise distribution of high frequency eeg activity in patients with primary and
secondary insomnia and in good sleeper controls. Journal of Sleep Research, 10, 93-104.
Perlis, M. L., Smith, M. T., Andrews, P. J., Orff, H., & Giles, D. E. (2001b). Beta/gamma EEG
activity in patients with primary and secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep,
24(1), 110-117.
Perski, A., Grossi, G., Evengård, B., Blomkvist, V., Ylbar, B., & Orth-Gomér, K. (2002). Emotional
exhaustion common among women in the public sector. Läkartidningen, 99, 2047-2052.
Peters, A., Schweiger, U., Pellerin, L., Hubold, C., Oltmanns, K. M.,Conrad, M., et al. (2004). The
selfish brain: Competition for energy resources. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 28(2), 143-180.
Pettigrew, J. (1990). Intensive nursing care- the minestry of presence. Crit Care Nurs Clin North Am,
2, 503-508.
Pines, A. (1993). Burnout: An existential perspective. In W. Schaufeli, C. Maslach & M. Tadeusz
(Eds.), Professional burnout: Recent developments in theory and research. Philadelphia:
Taylor & Francis.
Pines, A. M., Aronson, E., & Kafry, D. (1981). The research. In Burnout from tedium to personal
growth (pp. 202-205). New York: The Free Press.
Piper, B. F. (1986). Fatigue. In V. Carrieri, A. Lindsay & C. West (Eds.), Pathophysiological
phenomena in nursing: Human responses to illness. (pp. 219-234). Philadelphia: W. B.
Sanders & Co.
Pollmächer, T., Schreiber, W., Gudewill, S., Vedder, H., Fassbinder, K., Wiedemann, K., et al. (1993).
Influence of endotoxin on nocturnal sleep in humans. American Journal of Physiology, 264,
R1077-R1083.
Pruessner, J. C., Helhammar, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (1999). Burnout, perceived stress, and cortisol
responses to awakening. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 197-204.
Pruessner, J. C., Wolf, O. T., Helhammar, D. H., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., von Auer, K., Jobst, S., et al.
(1997). Free cortisol levels after awakening: A reliable biological marker for the assessment
of adrenocortical activity. Life Sciences, 61, 2539-2549.
76
Ramsey, J. D. (1983). Heat and cold. In G. R. J. Hockey (Ed.), Stress and fatigue in human
performance (pp. 33-60). London: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Rasmussen, B. (2000). Striving for becoming at-home in the midst of dying. American Journal of
Hospice & Palliative Care, 17(1), 31-43.
Rau, E., & Meyer, D. K. (1975). Recent advances in the study of cardiac structure and metabolism.
Metabolism, 7(105).
Ream, E., & Richardson, A. (1996). Fatigue: A concept analysis. International Journal of Nursing
Studies, 33, 519-529.
Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (1968). A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring
system for sleep stages of human subjects. Bethesda: US Department of Health, Education and
Welfare, Public Health Service.
RFV. (2002). Långtidssjukskrivningar för psykisk sjukdom och utbrändhet. - vilka egenskaper och
förhållanden är utmärkande för de drabbade? Stockholm: Riksförsäkringsverket.
Rinaldi, D. (1989). The lived experience of commitment to nursing: As percieved by nurses in specific
nursing environment. Colombia University.
Rock, J. P., Oldfield, E. H., Schulte, H. M., Gold, P. W., Kornblith, P. L., Loriaux, L., et al. (1984).
Corticotropin releasing factor administered into the ventricular csf stimulates the pituitaryadrenal axis. Brain Res, 323(2), 365-368.
Rosa, R. R., & Bonnet, M. H. (2000). Reported chronic insomnia is independent of poor sleep as
measured by electroencephalography. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 474-482.
Rosenthal, L., Roehrs, T., Sicklesteel, J., Zorick, F., Wittig, R., & Roth, T. (1984). Periodic
movements during sleep, sleep fragmentation, and sleep-wake complaints. Sleep, 7(4), 326330.
Rosmond, R. (2005). Role of stress in the pathogenesis of the metabolic syndrome.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 1-10.
Sandelowski, M. (2004). Using qualitative research. Qualitative Health Research, 14(10), 1366-1386.
Sandelowski, M., & Barroso, J. (2002). Finding the findings in qualitative studies. J Nurs Scholarsh,
34(3), 213-219.
Sandelowski, M., Docherty, S., & Emden, C. (1997). Qualitative metasynthesis: Issues and
technigues. Research in nursing & Health, 20, 365-371.
Sapolsky, R. M., Krey, L. C., & McEwen, B. S. (1984). Stress down-regulates corticosterone receptors
in a site-specific manner in the brain. Endocrinology, 114, 287-292.
Sateia, M., Doghramji, K., Hauri, P. J., & Morin, C. M. (2000). Evaluation of chronic insomnia. Sleep,
23, 243-263.
Schaufeli, W., & D, E. (1998). The burnout companion to study & practice. A critical analysis.
London: Taylor & Francis Ldt.
Schaufeli, W. B., & Buunk, B. P. (1996). Professional burnout. In M. J. Schabracq, J. A. M. Winnubst
& C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Handbook of work and health psychology (pp. 311-346). Chichester:
John Wiley & Sons.
Schedlowski, M., & Tewes, U. (1999). Psychoneuroimmunology. An interdisciplinary introduction.
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Schnall, P. L., Schwartz, J. E., Landsbergis, P. A., Warren, K., & Pickering, T. G. (1998). A
longitudinal study of job strain and ambulatory blood pressure: Results from a three-year
follow-up. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60, 697-706.
Schulz, P., Kirschbaum, C., Prüssner, J., & Hellhammer, D. (1998). Increased free cortisol secretion
after awakening in chronically stressed individuals due to work overload. Stress Medicine, 14,
91-97.
Seeman, T. E., McEwen, B. S., Rowe, J. W., & Singer, B. H. (2001). Allostatic load as a marker of
cumulative biological risk: Macartur studies of successful aging. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 98, 4770-4775.
77
Seidel, W. F., Ball, S., Cohen, S., Patterson, N., Yost, D., & Dement, W. C. (1984). Daytime alertness
in relation to mood, perfomance, and nocturnal sleep in chronic insomniacs and
noncomplaining sleepers. Sleep, 7, 230-238.
Selye, H. (1936). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature, 138, 32.
Selye, H. (1946). The general adaptation syndrome and the diseases of adaptation. The Journal of
Clinical Endocrinology, 6, 117-231.
Selye, H. (1956). Tre stress of life. New York: McGraw Hill.
Sharpley, A.,Clements,A., Hawton,K.,&Sharpe,M. (1997). Do patients with "pure" chronic fatigue
syndrome (neurasthenia) have abnormal sleep? Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 592-596.
Shirom, A. (1989). Burnout in work organizations. In C. L. C. a. I. Robertson (Ed.), International
review of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 25-48). New York: John Wiley &
Sons Ltd.
Shirom, A., Westman, M., Shamai, O., & S., C. R. (1997). Effects of work overload and burnout on
cholesterol and triglycerides levels: The moderating effects of emotional reactivity among
male and female employees. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 2, 275-288.
Shneerson, J. M. (2000). Handbook of sleep medicine. Oxfod: Blackwell Science Ltd.
Siegrist, J. (1996). Adverse health effects of high-effort/low-reward conditions. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 1, 27-41.
Socialstyrelsen. (1997). Klassifikation av sjukdomar och hälsoproblem 1997. Stockholm: Almqvist &
Wiksell Tryckeri.
Socialstyrelsen. (2003a). Folkhälsa och sociala förhållanden. Lägesrapport 2003 (No. 2004-131-1).
Stockholm.
Socialstyrelsen. (2003b). Utmattningssyndrom. Stressrelaterad psykisk ohälsa (No. 2003-123-18).
Stockholm: Socialstyrelsen.
Socialstyrelsen. (2005). Ändringar i och tillägg till klassifikation av sjukdomar och hälsoproblem 1997
(ksh97) - systematisk förteckning. http://www.sos.se/epc/klassifi/KSHandr.htm.
Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., L'Hermite-Baleriaux, M., Copinschi, G., Penev, P. D., & Van Cauter, E.
(2004). Leptin levels are dependent on sleep duration: Relationships with sympathovagal
balance, carbohydrate regulation, cortisol, and thyrotropin. J Clin Endocrinol Metab, 89(11),
5762-5771.
Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., & Van Cauter, E. (1999). Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine
function. The Lancet, 354, 1435-1439.
Späth-Schwalbe, E., Gofferje, M., Born, J., & Fehm, H. L. (1991). Sleep disruption alters nocturnal
acth and cortisol secretory patterns. Biological Psychiatry, 29, 575-584.
Staner, L., Cornette, F., Maurice, D., Viardot, G., Le Bon, O., Haba, J., et al. (2003). Sleep
microstructure around sleep onset differentiates major depressive insomnia from primary
insomnia. Journal of Sleep Research, 12, 319-330.
Stanley, R. O., & Burrows, G. D. (2005). The role of stress in mental illness: The practice. In C. L.
Cooper (Ed.), Handbook of stress medicine and health (Vol. 2, pp. 87-100). Lancaster, UK:
CRC Press.
Stepanski, E., Lamphere, J., Badia, P., Zorick, F., & Roth, T. (1984). Sleep fragmentation and daytime
sleepiness. Sleep, 7(1), 18-26.
Stepanski, E., Zorick, F., Roehrs, T., Young, D., & Roth, T. (1988). Daytime alertness in patients with
chronic insomnia compared with asymptomatic control subjects. Sleep, 11(1), 54-60.
Steptoe, A., Cropley, M., Griffith, J., & Kirschbaum, C. (2000). Job strain and anger expression
predict early morning elevations in salivary cortisol. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 286-292.
Sterling, P., & Eyer, J. (1988). Allostasis: A new paradigm to explain arousal pathology. In S. Fisher
& J. Reason (Eds.), Handbook of life stress, cognition and health (pp. 631-651): John Wiley
and Sons Ltd.
Stickgold, R., James, L., & Hobson, J. A. (2000). Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after
training. Nature, 3, 1237-1238.
78
Stoney, C. M., & Finney, M. (2000). Cholesterol and lipoproteins. In G. Flink (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
stress (Vol. 1, pp. 454-459). San Diego: Academic Press.
Stordeur, S., D'hoore, W., & Vandenberghe, C. (2001). Leadership, organizational stress, and
emotional exhaustion among hospital nursing staff. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35(4), 533542.
Streubert, H. J., & Carpenter, D. J. (1995). Qualitative research in nursing. Advancing the humanistic
imperative. Philadelphia: J.B.Lippingcott Company.
Sundin, K., & Jansson, L. (2003). "Understanding and being understood" as a creative caring
phenomenon - in care of patients with stroke and aphasia. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 12,
107-116.
Sunvisson, H., & Ekman, S.-L. (2001). Environmental influences on the experiences of people with
parkinson´s disease. Nursing inquiry, 8, 41-50.
Svensson, B. E. (1981). Psychological integrity and the concept of biofile., Akademisk avhandling:
Linköpings univeristet. Institutionen för preventiv- och social medicin, Linköping.
Svensson, B. E. (1990). Identification and elucidation of fields of research within health and social
training courses of medium lenght. Interdisciplinaria, 9(1), 41-53.
Swanson, L. W., & Sawchenko, P. E. (1983). Hypothalamic integration: Organization of the
paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei. Annu Rev Neurosci, 6, 269-324.
Söderberg, S., Lundman, B., & Norberg, A. (2002). The meaning of fatigue and tiredness as narrated
by women with fibromyalgia and healthy women. J Clin Nurs, 11(2), 247-255.
Söderfeldt, M. (1997). Burnout? University of lund, Lund.
Söderström, M., Ingre, M., Kecklund, G., Åkerstedt, T., & Kecklund, L. (2001). Lokförarens
arbetsbelastning - påverkan på sömn, trötthet och prestation. Stockholm: Institutet för
Psykosocial Medicin (IPM).
Talseth, A.-G., Gilje, F., & Norberg, A. (2001). Being met- a passageway to hope for relatives of
patients at risk of comitting suicide. A phenomenological hermeneutic study. Archives of
Psychiatric Nursing, Vol XV(6), 249-256.
Talseth, A. G., Gilje, F., & Norberg, A. (2003). Struggling to become ready for consolation:
Experiences of suicidal patients. International Journal of Nursing Ethics.
Theorell, T., & Karasek, R. A. (1996). Current issues relating to psychosocial job strain and
cardiovascular disease research. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1, 9-26.
Thoresen, C. J., Kaplan, S. A., Barsky, A. P., Warren, C. R., & de Chermont, K. (2003). The affective
underpinnings of job perceptions and attitudes: A meta-analytic review and integration.
Psychol Bull, 129(6), 914-945.
Thorpy, M. J. (1990). Handbook of sleep disorders (Vol. 6). New York: Marcel Dekker Inc.
Tinsley, C., & France, N. (2004). The trajetory of registrated nurses' exodus from the profession: A
phenomenological study of the lived experiences of oppression. Int J Hum Caring, 8(1), 8-12.
Toombs, S. K. (1992). The meaning of illness. A phenomenological account of the different
perspectives of physician and patient. London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Toomingas, A., Theorell, T., Michélsen, H., & Nordemar, R. (1997). Associations between self-rated
psychosocial work conditions and musculoskeletal symptoms and signs. Scandinavian
Journal of Work Environment and Health, 23, 130-139.
Torkildsen, G. (1999). Leisure and recreation management. London: F & FN Spon, Taylor and
Francis Group.
Upenieks, V. V. (2002). What constitutes successful nurse leadership? JONA, 32, 622-632.
Ursin, H., & Eriksen, H. R. (2001). Sensitization, subjective health complaints, and sustained arousal.
Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 933, 119-129.
Ursin, H., & Eriksen, H. R. (2004). The cognitive activation theory of stress.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(5), 567-592.
van Amelsvoort, L. G., Kant, I. J., Bultmann, U., & Swaen, G. M. (2003). Need for recovery after
work and the subsequent risk of cardiovascular disease in a working population. Occup
Environ Med, 60 Suppl 1, i83-87.
79
Van Cauter, E., Polonsky, K. S., & Scheen, A. J. (1997). Roles of circadian rhythmicity and sleep in
human glucose regulation. Endocrine Reviews, 18, 716-738.
van Diest, R., & Appels, A. W. P. M. (1994). Sleep physiological characteristics of exhausted men.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 56, 28-35.
Van Dongen, H. P. A., Baynard, M. D., Maislin, G., & Dinges, D. F. (2004). Systematic
interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: Evidence of traitlike differential vulnerability. Sleep, 27, 423-433.
Vgontzas, A. N., Bixler, E. O., Lin, H.-M., Prolo, P., Mastorakos, G., Vela-Bueno, A., et al. (2001).
Chronic insomnia is associated with nyctohemeral acativation of the hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal axis: Clinical implication. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 86,
3787-3794.
Vgontzas, A. N., & Chrousos, G. P. (2002). Sleep, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and
cytokines: Multiple interactions and disturbances in sleep disorders. Endocrinology and
Metabolism Clinics of North America, 31, 15-36.
Vgontzas, A. N., Mastorakos, G., Bixler, E. O., Kales, A., Gold, P. W., & Chrousos, G. P. (1999).
Sleep deprivation effects on the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal and growth
axes: Potential clinical implications. Clinical Endocrinology, 51, 205-215.
Vgontzas, A. N., Tsigos, C., Bixler, E. O., Stratakis, C. A., Zachman, K., Kales, A., et al. (1998).
Chronic insomnia and activity of the stress system: A preliminary study. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 45, 21-31.
Vgontzas, A. N., Zoumakis, E., Bixler, E. O., Lin, H. M., Follett, H., Kales, A., et al. (2004). Adverse
effects of modest sleep restriction on sleepiness, performance, and inflammatory cytokines. J
Clin Endocrinol Metab, 89(5), 2119-2126.
Walker, G. A. (1986). Burnout: From metaphor to ideology. Canadian Journal of Sociology, 11, 3555.
Walsh, J. K., & Sugerman, J. (1986). Daytime sleepiness and the first-night effect: A model of
transient insomnia. Sleep Research, 15, 181.
Watanabe, Y., Gould, E., & McEwen, B. (1992). Stress induces atrophy of apical dendrites of
hippocampal ca3 pyramidal neurons. Brain Research, 588, 341-345.
Wauquier, A., van Sweden, B., Kerkhof, G. A., & Kamphuisen, H. A. C. (1991). Ambulatory first
night sleep effect recording in the elderly. Behavioural Brain Research, 42, 7-11.
Webb, W. B., & Agnew, J., H.W. (1971). Stage 4 sleep: Influence of time course variables. Science,
174, 1354-1356.
Weber, A., & Jaekel-Reinhard, A. (2000). Bournout syndrome: A disease of modern societies?
Occupational Medicine, 50, 512-517.
Weiner, H. (1992). Perturbing the organism: The biology of stressful experience. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press.
Weitzman, E. D. (1975). Neuro-endocrine pattern of secretion during the sleep-wake cycle of man.
Progress in Brain Research, 42, 93-102.
Wesensten, N. J., Balkin, T. J., & Belenky, G. (1999). Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation?
A review and reanalysis. Journal of Sleep Research, 8, 237-245.
WHO, W. H. O. (1992). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems
(Vol. Tenth revision). Geneva: World Health Organization.
Williams, R. L., Karacan, I., & Hursch, C. J. (1974). Eeg of human sleep. New York: John Wiley and
Sons.
Winer, B. J. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Winnubst, J. (1993). Organizational structure, social support and burnout. In W. Schaufeli, C. Maslach
& M. Tadeusz (Eds.), Professional burnout. Recent developments in theory and research.
Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.
Wust, S., Wolf, J., Hellhammer, D. H., Federenko, I., Schommer, N., & Kirschbaum, C. (2000). The
cortisol awakening response - normal values and confounds. Noise Health, 2(7), 79-88.
80
Yehuda, R. (1999). Linking the neuroendocrinology of posttraumatic stress disorder woth recent
neuroanatomic findings. Neuroendocrinology, 4, 256-265.
Younger, J. B. (1995). The alienation of the sufferer. ANS Adv Nurs Sci, 17(4), 53-72.
Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatry
Scandinavia, 67, 361-370.
Åhsberg, E. (2000). Dimensions of fatigue in different working populations. Scand J Psychol, 41(3),
231-241.
Åhsberg, E., & Furst, C. J. (2001). Dimensions of fatigue during radiotherapy--an application of the
swedish occupational fatigue inventory (sofi) on cancer patients. Acta Oncol, 40(1), 37-43.
Åkerstedt, T., & Folkard, S. (1997). The three-process model of alertness and its extension to
performance, sleep latency, and sleep length. Chronobiology International, 14(2), 115-123.
Åkerstedt, T., & Gillberg, M. (1986). Sleep duration and the power spectral density of the eeg.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 64, 119-122.
Åkerstedt, T., & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual.
International Journal of Neuroscience, 52, 29-37.
Åkerstedt, T., Hume, K., Minors, D., & Waterhouse, J. (1994). The meaning of good sleep: A
longitudinal study of polysomnography and subjective sleep quality. J Sleep Res, 3, 152-158.
Åkerstedt, T., Hume, K., Minors, D., & Waterhouse, J. (1997). Good sleep - its timing and
physiological sleep characteristics. Journal of Sleep Research, 6, 221-229.
Åkerstedt, T., Kecklund, G., Gillberg, M., Lowden, A., & Axelsson, J. (2000). Sleepiness and days of
recovery. Transportation Research Part F, 3, 251-261.
Åkerstedt, T., Knutsson, A., Westerholm, P., Theorell, T., Alfredsson, L., & Kecklund, G. (2002).
Sleep disturbances, work stress and work hours. A cross-sectional study. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 53, 741-748.
Åsberg, M., Nygren, Å., Rylander, G., & Rydmark, I. (2002). Stress och utmattningsdepression. In B.
A. Rolf Ekman (Ed.), Stress (pp. 224-232). Stockholm: Liber.
Åsbring, P. (2001). Chronic illness - a disruption in life: Identity-transformation among women with
chronic fatigue syndrome and fibromyalgia. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 34(3), 312-319.
81
Cover picture © 2005 by Christian Portin
Published and printed by Karolinska University Press
Box 200, SE-171 77 Stockholm, Sweden
 2005 by Mirjam Ekstedt
ISBN 91-7140- 466-X
Abstract
Burnout and Sleep
The overall aim of this thesis was to describe the physiological characteristics of sleep in persons
with burnout and the relation between sleep and a number of physiological stress markers. The
aim was also to evaluate the diurnal pattern of subjective sleepiness and fatigue across workday
and weekend, and to describe the experiences of time preceding burnout from a life world
perspective.
This thesis focuses on burnout in white-collar workers; one group on sick-leave (I, II) and one
with high burnout scores (pre-burnout) but still at work (III, IV). The Shirom- Melamed Burnout
Questionnaire (SMBQ) was used for selection of participants with a mean of >4,75 on the total
score (range 1-7) as upper limit and <2,5 as cut off for the two control groups.
A combination of methods was used – interviews, sleep and wake diaries, questionnaires, blood
and saliva samples, and polysomnograpic (EEG, EOG, EMG) measures of sleep. The analyses
included; t tests, χ2, analysis of variance, ANOVA, and covariance, ANCOVA, correlations,
Pearson’s r, stepwise regression analyses (study II-IV), and phenomenological analysis (study I).
Study I describes the complex interaction between the person and his/her life world during the
process of burning out. Study II evaluated whether subjective sleep complaints in burnout
individuals on sick leave was related to disturbed sleep architecture and impaired homeostatic
processes. Physiological and subjective aspects of sleep before a workday and a day off were
investigated in study III. The diurnal pattern of sleepiness on workday and weekend was
evaluated in study II and III, and the diurnal pattern of mental fatigue was evaluated in study II
as well as occupational fatigue. Additionally, the burnout groups (II III) were described with
respect to work stress, mood, recovery and burnout related variables. Study IV was a correlation
study where the relation between sleep fragmentation and a number of physiological stress
parameters, as well as possible predictors in daily life was evaluated.
The most important finding is that sleep was impaired on all essential sleep variables in the
burnout group on sick-leave (II). In the pre-burnout group (III) the main findings was an
increased frequency of arousals, and the relatively moderate sleep fragmentation was related to a
number of risk factors for CVD and the metabolic syndrome (IV). Also unclear boundaries
between work and leisure time and tension/irritability were associated with the sleep
fragmentation. The burnout group on sick leave was sleepier than the controls for most point in
time, with levels comparable to night or early morning shifts, and mental fatigue was equally
elevated (except for weekday evenings). The pattern of sleepiness and mental fatigue was
consistent across days. The high and low burnout groups were equally sleepy during workdays
but differed during days off, indicating impaired recovery in the high burnout group. The time
preceding burnout (I) was experienced as being trapped between never-ending demands on the
one hand and stimulating challenges on the other. “Cutting off” important areas of life enhanced
the strong focus on responsibilities, protected their ‘self-images’ and impeded sleep and recovery.
Acceptance of the situation was the turning point from where a new reconstruction of life could
emerge.
These findings suggest that sleep disruptions and impaired recovery seem to be characteristics of
burnout, resulting in fatigue, and that inability to unwind the stress system may be a mediating
factor. With this knowledge interventions for sleep improvement on company or population level
seems important, and effective treatment of sleep disorders in order to prevent or relieve burnout
is needed, as is a deeper understanding of how fatigue is experienced in health and illness.
List of original papers
This thesis is based on following papers, which will be referred to in the text by their
Roman numerals.
I
Ekstedt, M, Fagerberg, I.
Lived experiences of time preceding burnout.
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 2005 (49), 59-67
II
Ekstedt, M., Söderström, M., Åkerstedt, T., Nilsson, J., Søndergaard, H-P., Perski, A.
Disturbed sleep in occupational burnout. (Submitted)
III
Söderström, M., Ekstedt, M., Åkerstedt, T., Nilsson, J., Axelsson, J.
Sleep and sleepiness in young individuals with high burnout scores
Sleep, 2004 (17), 1369-1377
IV
Ekstedt, M., Åkerstedt, T., Södeström, M.
Microarousals during sleep are associated with increased levels of lipids, cortisol and
blood pressure.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 2004 (66), 925-931
Reprints have been made with kind permission of the publishers
This work has been supported by:
The National Institute for Psychosocial Medicine, IPM.
The National Institute for Working Life, NIWL
Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research, FAS
The Alecta Insurance Company.
The R & D-unit Sörmland County Council.
List of Abbreviations
ABP
ACTH
ANOVA
BAI
BDI
BMI
CATS
CFS
CNS
CRH
CVD
DSM-IV
EEG
EMG
EOG
FFT
HAD
HDL-C
HPA
ICSD
KSD
KSS
KSQ
LDL-C
MBI
NREM
ICD-10
LHPA
MSLT
PSG
RAS
REM
SAM
SCID
SCN
SMBQ
SOFI
SSI
SSS
SWA
SWS
SQI
TIB
TST
VE
W
WASO
WHR
Ambulatory Blood Pressure
Adrenocorticotropin
Analysis of variance
Beck Anxiety Inventory
Beck Depression Inventory
Body Mass Index
Cognitive Activation Theory of Stress
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome
Central Nervous System
Cortico-Releasing-Hormone
Cardiovascular diseases
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th ed.
Electroencephalography
Electromyography
Electrooculography
Fast Fourier Transformation
Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale
High Density Lipoprotein- Cholesterol
Hypothalamo-Puitary-Adrenal
International Classification of Sleep Disorders
Karolinska Sleep Diary
Karolinska Sleepiness Scale
Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire
Low Density Lipoprotein-Cholesterol
Maslach Burnout Inventory
Non-Rapid Eye Movement
International Classification Diseases
Limbic-Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenocortical
Multiple Sleep Latency Test
Polysomniographic
Reticular Activating System
Rapid Eye Movement
Symphato-Adrenal-Medullary
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV
Suprachiasmatic nuclei
Shirom-Melamed Burnout Questionnaire
Swedish Occupational Fatigue Inventory
Sleep Sufficiency Index
Stanford Sleepiness Scale
Slow Wave Activity
Slow Wave Sleep
Sleep Quality Index
Time In Bed
Total Sleep Time
Vital Exhaustion
Wake
Wake Time After Sleep Onset
Waist to Hip circumference Ratio
Table of contents
BACKGROUND .................................................................................................................................................... 1
WHAT IS BURNOUT? ............................................................................................................................................ 1
Theories.......................................................................................................................................................... 2
A synthesis...................................................................................................................................................... 3
Diagnostic assessment................................................................................................................................... 4
FATIGUE AND RELATED STATES .......................................................................................................................... 5
Fatigue ........................................................................................................................................................... 5
Depression ..................................................................................................................................................... 7
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome ........................................................................................................................... 8
Vital Exhaustion............................................................................................................................................. 8
BURNOUT AND STRESS ........................................................................................................................................ 8
Stress theory................................................................................................................................................... 9
Physiological mechanisms ..........................................................................................................................10
Research on physiological markers in burnout .........................................................................................11
RECOVERY .........................................................................................................................................................12
SLEEP .................................................................................................................................................................13
Previous studies on sleep and burnout.......................................................................................................14
An overview of normal sleep.......................................................................................................................15
Disturbed sleep ............................................................................................................................................19
SLEEPINESS ........................................................................................................................................................22
QUALITATIVE ASPECTS .....................................................................................................................................23
THE PHENOMENOLOGICAL LIFE- WORLD PERSPECTIVE ....................................................................................24
SUMMARY AND AIM ..........................................................................................................................................26
Study I...........................................................................................................................................................26
Study II .........................................................................................................................................................26
Study III ........................................................................................................................................................27
Study IV ........................................................................................................................................................27
METHODS ...........................................................................................................................................................28
DESIGN OF THE STUDIES ....................................................................................................................................28
PARTICIPANTS AND SETTING .............................................................................................................................29
ETHICAL ISSUES AND APPROVAL. .....................................................................................................................31
GENERAL PROCEDURE .......................................................................................................................................31
MEASURES .........................................................................................................................................................32
Polysomnography ........................................................................................................................................32
Computer-aided methods -spectral analysis.............................................................................................33
Physiological measures...............................................................................................................................33
Laboratory analysis.....................................................................................................................................34
Sleep diary....................................................................................................................................................35
Wake diary ...................................................................................................................................................35
Questionnaires and indices.........................................................................................................................35
STATISTICS AND ANALYSIS ...............................................................................................................................38
Phenomenological analysis - study I ..........................................................................................................38
Statistics – study II-IV .................................................................................................................................38
SUMMARY OF STUDIES.................................................................................................................................40
LIVED EXPERIENCES OF THE TIME PRECEDING BURNOUT (STUDY I) ...............................................................40
SLEEP, SLEEPINESS AND FATIGUE IN BURNOUT PATIENTS (STUDY II).............................................................41
SLEEP AND SLEEPINESS IN YOUNG INDIVIDUALS WITH HIGH AND LOW BURNOUT SCORES ( STUDY III) ........43
AROUSALS FROM SLEEP AND PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATES ( STUDY IV) ......................................................44
DISCUSSION .......................................................................................................................................................45
INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................................................................45
SLEEP .................................................................................................................................................................46
Burnout and sleep ........................................................................................................................................46
Sleep and stress............................................................................................................................................47
Stress physiology and sleep.........................................................................................................................47
SLEEPINESS ........................................................................................................................................................49
SLEEPINESS AND PERFORMANCE IN RELATION TO DISTURBED SLEEP .............................................................50
SLEEPINESS AND SWS ......................................................................................................................................51
FATIGUE .............................................................................................................................................................52
Fatigue and mood ........................................................................................................................................53
The concept of fatigue .................................................................................................................................54
THE PROCESS OF BURNING OUT ........................................................................................................................55
Responsibilities towards work and real life...............................................................................................55
“Cutting off” - and neglect of essential needs...........................................................................................57
The turning point .........................................................................................................................................58
METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS ..............................................................................................................59
TENTATIVE INTEGRATION AND SOME HYPOTHESES .........................................................................................61
PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS AND FURTHER RESEARCH ......................................................................................63
REFERENCES ....................................................................................................................................................65
APPENDIX
I.
INDICES
II. SLEEP DIARY – SÖMNDAGBOKEN
III. WAKE DIARY STUDY II- VAKENHETSDAGBOKEN
IV. WAKE DIARY STUDY III- VAKENHETSDAGBOKEN
V.
SHIROM- MELAMED BURNOUT QUESTIONNAIRE
I
III
IV
V
VI
Download