The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification This is an interactive Adobe Acrobat PDF 9 document. It is best viewed on screen as it contains embedded video and an interactive menu. The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Introduction Counting begins with the task of reproducing a sequence of number words starting from one. Over time, children’s familiarity with the sequence of counting words develops to the point where they are able to identify the number word before or after any number without needing to start the count from one. That is, the process of oral counting starts as a fixed recitation but moves to a ‘breakable chain’ of number words. Students can use counting as the basis of a range of strategies, including various kinds of additions and subtractions. Within these strategies, counting refers to more than producing the forward sequence of number words, sometimes called rote counting. If you ask a student what number comes after nine, he or she will often initially count from one to find the answer. To be able to use the strategy of counting-on, students need to be able to know the sequence of number words well enough to continue counting from any number. That is, you cannot count on from seven if you do not know the number word that follows seven. Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements This student counts from one to find the next number word in a counting sequence. > 2 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Challenges in the ‘teens’ When children learn to say the counting words, it is quite common for them to experience some problems with the ‘teens’. The teen number words often sound very similar to other number words. For example, ‘thirteen’ sounds similar to ‘thirty’, ‘fourteen’ to ‘forty’, ‘fifteen’ to ‘fifty’ and so on. Although it can be surprising to hear the response ‘ninety-one’ when you ask a child what number comes after ‘nineteen’, this is a logical answer if the child treats the number words ‘nineteen’ and ‘ninety’ as being the same. Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements This problem of understanding the naming of numbers between ten and twenty also includes the two number words eleven and twelve that occur before the ‘teens’. The old English or Germanic origins of eleven and twelve suggest that they originally meant “one left over” and “two left over”, respectively. However, the etymology of the pre-teen number words eleven and twelve does little to support a child’s search for consistent structure in the counting word sequences. As well as reversals in the ‘teens’ where the smaller value is named before the larger value (e.g. the four comes before the ten in fourteen) students need to interpret two different modifications of “ten”; ‘teen’ and ‘ty’, neither of which clearly says “ten”. Since the ten and ones structure of the ‘teens’ is not transparent in English, more learning time must be devoted to developing an understanding that the numbers from 11 to 19 express one group of ten and a particular number of ones. Knowledge of a student’s use of the sequence of number words is a powerful indicator of the efficiency of addition and subtraction methods. Wright (1989) distinguished between the development of the forward sequence of number words and the backward sequence of number words, describing five levels in each as well as an emergent level. The levels are summarised in the following tables. > 3 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements Forward number word sequences L0 Emergent L1 Initial (10) L2 Intermediate (10) L3 Facile (10) L4 Facile (30) L5 Facile (100) Cannot count to 10 Counts to 10 but cannot give the number after. Counts to 10 and gives the number after, but counts from one. Counts to 10 and gives the number after. Counts to 30 and gives the number after. Counts to 100 and gives the number after. Level 0: Emergent The emergent level characterises children who have not yet developed knowledge of the sequence of number words from one to ten. They may know some of the counting words in correct sequence but cannot consistently count from one to ten. Level 1: Initial (10) At the level of the initial sequence up to ten the forward sequence of numbers from one to ten can be produced but not the word for a number immediately after one in this range. Level 2: Intermediate (10) At the intermediate number word sequence to ten the number word just after a given number word can be produced within this range but only by dropping back to count from one when doing so. Level 3: Facile (10) At the facile number word sequence to ten the number word just after a given number word can be produced within this range without dropping back to count from one. Level 4: Facile (30) When students are facile with number word sequences to thirty the forward sequence of number words from one to thirty can be produced and the number word just after a given number word can be identified within this range without dropping back. Level 5: Facile (100) When students are facile with number word sequences to one hundred, the forward number word sequences can be produced in the range from one to one hundred (or beyond) and the number word just after a given number word can be identified within this range without dropping back. > 4 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements Backward number word sequences L0 Emergent L1 Initial (10) L2 Intermediate (10) L3 Facile (10) L4 Facile (30) L5 Facile (100) Cannot count backwards from 10 to 1. Counts backwards from 10 to 1 but cannot give the number immediately before a given number. Counts backwards from 10 to 1 and gives the number before, but counts from one to do so. Counts backwards from 10 to 1 and gives the number before without dropping back. Counts backwards from 30 and gives the number before without dropping back. Counts backwards in the range to 100 and gives the number before. Level 0: Emergent The emergent level characterises children who have not yet developed knowledge of the backward sequence of number words from ten to one. Level 1: Initial (10) At the level of the initial backward number word sequence up to ten the backward sequence of numbers from ten to one can be produced but not the word for a number immediately before one in this range. Level 2: Intermediate (10) At the intermediate backward number word sequence to ten, the number word just before a given number word can be produced within this range but only by dropping back to count from one when doing so. Level 3: Facile (10) At the facile backward number word sequence to ten the number word just before a given number word can be produced within this range without dropping back to count from one. Level 4: Facile (30) When students are facile with backward number word sequences up to thirty the backward sequence of number words from thirty to one can be produced and the number word just before a given number word can be identified within this range without dropping back. Level 5: Facile (100) When students are facile with backward number word sequences to one hundred the backward number word sequences can be produced in the range from one hundred to one and the number word just before a given number word can be identified within this range without dropping back. > 5 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification This student counts from one to find the number word before thirteen. Introductiona Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Number word before Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements > 6 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Zero and the counting numbers When children count backwards from 10, some learn to finish the backward count with “zero” and others with “blast off”. Although there is value in counting down from 10 to 0, a problem arises when some children begin a forward count from zero. In developing a one-to-one match between objects and counting words, children can allocate the word “zero” to the first object counted. For this reason, the descriptions of the levels of the counting words do not include zero. Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification Numeral identification Our system of numerals has a long history of development, with the symbols derived from the Indian Brahmi numerals, then adopted and popularised by the Arab empire. Initially the place value system used nine numerals and a blank space called sunya in India and sifr in the Arab world, both words meaning empty. The tenth symbol for zero appeared later. There are two uses of zero that are both important but are somewhat different. One use is as an empty place indicator in our place-value system and the second use of zero is as a number in its own right. There are also different aspects of zero (and other numbers), namely the concept or quantity, the notation and the name. In assessing numeral identification we seek to establish the link between the verbal and the symbolic. References Acknowledgements In using number, children must integrate many layers of verbal, procedural, symbolic and conceptual meaning. To illustrate how these layers can coexist, consider the following video showing some of the different meanings attributed to “five”. > 7 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Get me 5 counters… Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements Teacher: Could you get me 5 counters from this pile, please? Student: (Grabs a handful of blue counters and then a handful of yellow counters) Teacher: OK. We will leave the yellow ones there – put the yellow ones back. Now, would you like to tell me, how many blue ones have you got here? Student: (Pointing at different counters, leaving some out) One, two, three, four, five. Teacher: How many? Student: (Repeats the count again leaving out some counters) One, two, three, four, five. Teacher: How many was that? Student: (Holding up 5 fingers) Five. Teacher: Five. Um hmm. > 8 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences This child has learnt what five fingers are, and in that limited context, could be said to know five. He has knowledge of the forward sequence of number words to five and recognition that the last word in a count has special meaning. His counting procedure starts with the first item in a row and ends on the final item in the row, but he does not match his counting words one-to-one with each and every item. As well as the spoken word (e.g. “five”), number can be represented symbolically as a written word (e.g. five) or as a written numeral (e.g. 5). Although numerals are the written and read symbols for numbers, they can also play a similar role to letters in forming part of a name, as in licence plates and telephone numbers. At its most basic level, numeral identification is a form of shape recognition, which can result in a simple association of the word “two” with the symbol ‘2’ without a cardinal meaning (Mix, Sandhofer, & Baroody, 2005). This means that numeral identification can develop at a different rate to number knowledge. Learning to identify, recognise and write numerals is an important part of early arithmetical development. When a young child learns the name of a numeral it sows the idea that a symbol can stand for a whole word (Mix, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 2002). Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements Although this student knows that the symbols may identify numbers, he is uncertain as to which numerals match with which words. Numeral identification > 9 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements Numeral identification refers to being able to state the name of a displayed numeral. L0 Emergent L1 1-10 L2 1-20 L3 1-100 L4 1-1000 L5 1-10 000 May identify some but not all numerals 1–10. Identifies all the numerals 1–10. Identifies all the numerals 1–20. Identifies numerals in the range 1–100. Identifies numerals in the range 1–1000. Identifies numerals in the range 1–10 000. Level 0: Emergent At the emergent numeral identification level the student may identify some, but not all numerals in the range 1–10. Level 1: 1-10 At the 1–10 numeral identification level the student can identify all numerals in the range 1–10. Level 2: 1-20 At the 1–20 numeral identification level the student can identify all numerals in the range 1–20. Level 3: Facile (10) At the 1–100 numeral identification level the student can identify all numerals in the range 1–100. Level 4: Facile (30) At the 1–1000 numeral identification level the student can identify one–, two– and three–digit numbers. Level 5: Facile (100) At the 1–10 000 numeral identification level the student can identify one–, two–, three– and four–digit numbers. > 10 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification References Mix, K. S., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (2002). Quantitative development in infancy and early childhood. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Mix, K. S., Sandhofer, C. M., & Baroody, A. J. (2005). Number words and number concepts: The interplay of verbal and nonverbal quantification in early childhood. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol. 33). New York: Academic Press. Wright, R. J. (1989). Numerical development in the kindergarten year: A teaching experiment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia. Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements > 11 The Numeracy Continuum Aspect 1: Counting sequences and numeral identification Acknowledgements The descriptions of the levels of the number word sequences, forwards and backwards, and the levels of numeral identification are derived from the work of Professor Bob Wright. His contribution and the contributions of his colleagues to our growing understanding of the role played by counting in developing a rich sense of number are gratefully acknowledged. Introduction Challenges in the ‘teens’ Forward number word sequences Backward number word sequences Zero and the counting numbers Numeral identification References Acknowledgements > 12