Effects of E-Textbooks on Student Mental Effort in an ESL Middle School Classroom Yungwei Hao Department of Education National Taiwan Normal University Taiwan hao@ntnu.edu.tw Abstract: As e-textbooks have been increasingly used in K-12 classrooms, teachers are more able to provide students with content materials with multimedia formats and engage students in the learning process. The purpose of this study was to investigate 36 middle school students’ mental effort when students read e-textbooks demonstrated by their teacher in an English-as-a-Second-Language classroom in a middle school. The researcher used the framework of Paas’s survey to develop survey items to measure the mental effort. The results indicated the students took below-medium mental effort for searching for the pages and above-medium mental effort for comprehending the book content. Besides, students’ personal characteristics were examined to search for the association. The findings may shed light on the effect of e-books on students’ cognitive load and provide educators with some implications for e-textbook technology integration in the K-12 school settings. Introduction Because the popularity of e-book reading devices has emerged during the last few years, e-books have been more and more accepted in the K-12 educational settings. Studies (i.e., Rao, 2003) have indicated electronic textbooks (abbreviated as e-textbooks in the following sections) with helpful features may help students learn. E-textbooks can enhance student involvement in class, which will reinforce the learning outcomes (Sun, Flores, and Tanguma, 2012). E-textbooks have been regarded as alternatives to the printed textbooks or even predicted as the main format in the textbook market (Lees, 2011). Because e-textbooks have the advantages of lower prices, convenience, portability (Chu, 2003), flexibility (Annand, 2008), searching the text by keywords (Chu, 2003), hyperlinking, highlighting, and note sharing (Chu, 2003; Guess, 2008), integrating text with multimedia materials (Landoni, 2003; Su, 2005; Sun, Flores, & Tanguma, 2012), publishers have offered support, and researchers have been experimenting its feasibility to explore large adoption. Cognitive load is the burden imposed on the student’s limited working memory in a learning process (Sweller, 1998). Having appropriate cognitive load is important in the learning process (Paas, 1992). Paas proposed cognitive load has two interrelated components: mental load and mental effort. Mental load is the burden caused by tasks, and metal effort is the capacity required by tasks. With the emergence of technology applications in education, students often conduct learning tasks which need unknown mental effort. The purpose of this study was to investigate middle school students’ mental effort when students read e-textbooks demonstrated by their teacher in an English-as-a-Second-Language classroom. The Study Thirty-six students in an English-as-a-Foreign-Language middle school classroom in Taiwan, participated in the study in Spring 2012. During the semester, the teacher demonstrated the students the e-textbook content through the projector and instructed the class with the e-textbook throughout the semester. In a word, the students had their printed textbook at hand, and they had the e-textbook presented overhead. At the end of the semester, Paas’s Mental Effort Scale was used for the students to assess their mental effort in the process. The research participation was voluntary, and the students can withdraw anytime. The scale is a self-rated numerical value based on a 9-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (very very low mental effort) to 9 (very very high mental effort) (Paas, 1992). Studies (i.e., Ayres, 2006; Paas, et al., 2008) have indicated the scale is valid and reliable and have been used in numerous research studies. Findings 15 girls and 21 boys made up the participants. All of them were able to access to computers and to the Internet at home. 53% of the participants read e-books before; 47% had no e-reading experience. 80.6% preferred being taught in e-textbooks. 83.3% disagreed or strongly disagreed that e-textbooks are not helpful for their learning and that printed textbooks and e-textbooks had no difference. The results of this study identified the mental effort when the students read the e-textbook content through the projector. Specifically speaking, 83.3% of the participants indicated they took below-medium mental effort for searching for the pages (Mean=2.56, SD=1.68). 66.7% indicated they took below-medium mental effort for searching for specific content (Mean=3.58, SD=2.09). 47.2% indicated they made below-medium mental effort for reading the digital content (Mean=4.17, SD=2.37). 50% indicated they had below-medium mental effort for comprehending the content (Mean=4.50, SD=2.56). Among the mental efforts, the task of comprehending the content significantly required the most mental effort, and the task of searching for the pages significantly required least. Regarding the association between the mental effort and personal characteristics, there was some significant difference that girls needed lower mental effort than boys when searching for the pages, t(34)=2.70, p<.05. No significant differences were found for other tasks. As for other characteristics, for example, students’ foreign language beliefs (Horwitz, 1987), they are being analyzed and will be presented together later. Conclusions The results of this pilot study indicated the mental effort imposed by e-textbooks in a middle school classroom. Searching for pages required the least mental effort, but comprehending the content required most. Besides, students’ personal characteristics are being examined to search for the association. It is expected that the findings shed light on the effect of e-books on students’ cognitive load and provide educators with knowledge of the individual differences and with some implications for e-book technology integration in the K-12 learning environments. References Ayres, P. (2006). Using subjective measures to detect variations of intrinsic cognitive load within problems. Learning and Instruction, 16(5), 389–400. Chu, H. (2003). Electronic books: Viewpoints from users and potential users. Library Hi Tech, 21(3), 340–346. Fialkoff, F. (2008). Beyond the Kindle. Library Journal, 133(12), 8. Horwitz, E. K. (1987). Surveying student beliefs about language learning. In A. Wenden & J. Rubin (Eds.), Learner strategies in language learning (pp. 119-129). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice/Hall International. Landoni, M. (2003). Electronic books. In J. Feather & P. Sturges (Eds.), Routledge international encyclopedia of information and library science, 2nd ed. London: Routledge, 168–171. Paas, F. G. W. C. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(4), 429–434. Paas, F. G. W. C., Ayres, P., & Pachman, M. (2008). Assessment of cognitive load in multimedia learning: Theory, methods and applications. In D. H. Robinson & G. Schraw (Eds.), Recent innovations in educational technology that facilitate student learning (pp. 11–35). Charlotte, NC: Information Age. Rubin (Eds.), Learner strategies in language learning (pp. 119-129). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice/Hall International. Rainie, L., Zickuhr, K., Purcell, K., Madden, M., & Brenner, J. (2012). The rise of e-reading. The Pew Research Center’s Internet & American Life Project. Rao, S. S. (2003). Electronic books: A review and evaluation. Library Hi Tech, 21(1), 85-93. Su, S- F. (2005). Desirable search features of web-based scholarly e-book systems. The Electronic Library, 23(1), 64–71. Sun, J., Flores, J., & Tanguma, J. (2012). E-textbooks and students’ learning experiences. Journal of Innovative Education, 10(1), 63-77. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12(2), 257–285. Tennant, R. & O’Reilly, T. (2008). Does Amazon’s Kindle have a future? Library Journal, 133(12), 13.