Animal Behavior : An overview The term “Ethology” is used to describe the study of animal behaviors that occur in natural environments. Behaviors are innate (instinctive) or learned. There are many behaviors with a genetic component, but a stimulus is required for the behavior to be manifested. Such behaviors are called fixed action patterns. For example, a male bird will do some kind of dance or display to attract females. The dude was never taught this behavior, and most likely has no clue as to why he’s doing it – it is, therefore, innate. What is the stimulus? It’s the presence of a female of his species. Imagine a bunch of male birds of some species that have been separated from females. Somewhere in their pea-sized brains is a programmed courtship dance, but they’ve never done it. Then a female is introduced. All the young dudes start doing essentially the same dance that their daddies did, without ever having been instructed. There are 100’s of examples. One that I like is when some baby birds see a red spot (birds like red) on mommy’s beak. They peck on the spot and mommy regurgitates some yummy food. In this case, both chick and mommy are engaging in fixed action patterns. The stimulus for the baby is the presence of the red spot. The stimulus for the mom is having her beak pecked. Chicks will peck a red spot on a fake mommy, and moms without chicks will puke if you peck their beak. Proximate and Ultimate behavioral causes: Proximate causes are the immediate manifestation of an innate behavior. The ultimate cause is increased reproductive success. This is the link between behavior and survival of the fittest (evolution). If the animal in question (insect, mammal, whatever) does not engage in the proximate behavior, it will ultimately be less likely to survive and reproduce. An example would be ducklings that learn shortly after hatching to follow their mommy (imprinting). Those that do are rewarded with food and protection, and thus increase their chances of surviving and reproducing. Those that don’t follow mommy might get eaten instead of fed. Imprinting: For the first few days after ducks and geese hatch, they identify “mommy” and follow “her” around. If there is no real mommy, a surrogate will do. This can be a human, as I learned once when I got some baby geese to raise for Christmas dinner. That was a one time experience, because it’s pretty cute having these little goslings following you around in single file every where you go. I knocked them off and ate them anyway, but after that experience I went back to chickens and turkeys, which do not imprint. Cranes imprint too. Some whooping cranes raised in captivity imprinted on some dude in a whooping crane outfit. When the time came, said dude climbed into an ultralight plane with his crane suit on and taught them their migration route. Such is the power of imprinting. Innate behaviors: (Instinctive) These are the ones that are part of the genetic programming. Different species of birds build nests in a certain way and in a certain place. No one taught them how to do it. For example, movement, following environmental cues (chemical, magnetism, etc.), migration, hibernation, reproductive behaviors, communication (auditory, visual, chemical), aggression, fleeing from danger, etc. There are many innate behaviors in the animal world. Learned behaviors: (As opposed to innate) Habituation is a learned behavior that does not require a teacher. For example, if a rat is exposed to a hip hop music for the first time, he might flip out. If he hears hip hop about the same time every day, it learns to take it in stride…no big deal. He has become habituated to this particular stimulus. Spatial learning: Animals see patterns and landmarks in their environment. This makes it easy to find their nest (or whatever) after an excursion to find food. If environments were completely uniform, spatial learning could not occur. This is similar to what are called “cognitive maps.” Associative learning: Classical and Operant conditioning: Animals learn to associate a behavior with either a positive or negative experience. If a bird eats an orange monarch butterfly larva, it will taste nasty (it’s toxic too). The bird associates orange (not the species) with nasty, and avoids it. If a certain type of food is eaten and an electric shock (ouch) is administered, then no matter how tasty the food is, the animal will leave it alone. This is classical conditioning, and a type of associative learning. Operant conditioning involves “trial and error” learning and can be associated with either positive or negative consequences. I had a dog one time that tried to take a bite out of a porcupine. I had to take him to the vet to get the quills removed from his mouth. I don’t think the slow-moving porcupines left the area, but that dog never tried to bite one again. Others: Agonistic behaviors: Usually males. The classic example is fighting over a female. This happens all the time. The tough guy gets the female…most of the time. However, sometimes the nice guy gets the female. In a study of orangutans, it was observed that the dominant (most aggressive) male would not always get the female. Some males who were inclined to avoid or walk away from a fight would retreat to the periphery of the group, where he would find a receptive female to mate with, while the macho male was showing off by banging on his chest. It appears that such behaviors have made it all the way into the human population. Altruism is selflessness. Altruistic behaviors are those that have survival value to others within the population. Such as alerting others of impending danger, like predators. Picking ticks off your buddy’s back is altruistic. Insight (reasoning): This is mostly a human thing. We have the mental ability to adjust our behavior as a result of thinking ahead of the consequences. Some humans learn this earlier in life than others.